ArticlePDF Available

Aesthetic chills cause an emotional drift in valence and arousal

Frontiers
Frontiers in Neuroscience
Authors:
  • Institute for Advanced Consciousness Studies

Abstract and Figures

Aesthetic chills are an embodied peak emotional experience induced by stimuli such as music, films, and speeches and characterized by dopaminergic release. The emotional consequences of chills in terms of valence and arousal are still debated and the existing empirical data is conflicting. In this study, we tested the effects of ChillsDB, an open-source repository of chills-inducing stimuli, on the emotional ratings of 600+ participants. We found that participants experiencing chills reported significantly more positive valence and greater arousal during the experience, compared to participants who did not experience chills. This suggests that the embodied experience of chills may influence one’s perception and affective evaluation of the context, in favor of theoretical models emphasizing the role of interoceptive signals such as chills in the process of perception and decision-making. We also found an interesting pattern in the valence ratings of participants, which tended to harmonize toward a similar mean after the experiment, though initially disparately distributed. We discuss the significance of these results for the diagnosis and treatment of dopaminergic disorders such as Parkinson’s, schizophrenia, and depression.
This content is subject to copyright.
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 1
TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 07 March 2023
DOI 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Mariella Pazzaglia,
Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
REVIEWED BY
Alexander Toet,
Netherlands Organisation for Applied
Scientific Research, Netherlands
Erik Leemhuis,
Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy
*CORRESPONDENCE
Abhinandan Jain
abyjain@mit.edu
Felix Schoeller
felixsch@mit.edu
PRESENT ADDRESS
Xiaoxiao Hu,
Wellesley College, Wellesley, MA,
United States
These authors share first authorship
SPECIALTY SECTION
This article was submitted to
Perception Science,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Neuroscience
RECEIVED 06 August 2022
ACCEPTED 09 December 2022
PUBLISHED 07 March 2023
CITATION
Jain A, Schoeller F, Horowitz A, Hu X,
Yan G, Salomon R and Maes P (2023)
Aesthetic chills cause an emotional
drift in valence and arousal.
Front. Neurosci. 16:1013117.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
COPYRIGHT
© 2023 Jain, Schoeller, Horowitz, Hu,
Yan, Salomon and Maes. This is an
open-access article distributed under
the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use,
distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) and the copyright
owner(s) are credited and that the
original publication in this journal is
cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution
or reproduction is permitted which
does not comply with these terms.
Aesthetic chills cause an
emotional drift in valence and
arousal
Abhinandan Jain1*, Felix Schoeller1,2,3*, Adam Horowitz1,
Xiaoxiao Hu1, Grace Yan1, Roy Salomon2and Pattie Maes1
1MIT Media Lab, Cambridge, MA, United States, 2The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Centre,
Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel, 3Institute for Advanced Consciousness Studies, Santa Monica,
CA, United States
Aesthetic chills are an embodied peak emotional experience induced by
stimuli such as music, films, and speeches and characterized by dopaminergic
release. The emotional consequences of chills in terms of valence and
arousal are still debated and the existing empirical data is conflicting. In this
study, we tested the effects of ChillsDB, an open-source repository of chills-
inducing stimuli, on the emotional ratings of 600+ participants. We found that
participants experiencing chills reported significantly more positive valence
and greater arousal during the experience, compared to participants who
did not experience chills. This suggests that the embodied experience of
chills may influence one’s perception and affective evaluation of the context,
in favor of theoretical models emphasizing the role of interoceptive signals
such as chills in the process of perception and decision-making. We also
found an interesting pattern in the valence ratings of participants, which
tended to harmonize toward a similar mean after the experiment, though
initially disparately distributed. We discuss the significance of these results for
the diagnosis and treatment of dopaminergic disorders such as Parkinson’s,
schizophrenia, and depression.
KEYWORDS
chills, emotion, valence, arousal, emotional drift, ChillsDB, interindividual
differences, synchronization
1. Introduction
Aesthetic chills (thereafter “chills”) are a peak emotional response characterized by
a feeling of cold down the spine, sometimes accompanied by goosebumps (e.g., on the
arms). While chills are a growing topic of study, evidence is still conflicting in regards to
the emotional consequences of chills and interindividual differences (see e.g., Panksepp,
1995;Grewe et al.,2011;Zickfeld et al.,2020;de Fleurian and Pearce,2021). Specifically,
the causes and consequences of chills in terms of valence (i.e., hedonic tone or affective
quality) and arousal (i.e., the level of autonomic activation) are still unclear (de Fleurian
and Pearce,2021). Chills have been linked to a wide range of emotions, both positive and
negative, as well as general arousal response (Maruskin et al.,2012). Brain studies suggest
Frontiers in Neuroscience 01 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 2
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
chills engage reward-related brain regions, in the striatum
and prefrontal cortex (Blood and Zatorre,2001) specifically
rewarding dopamine release in the caudate nucleus and nucleus
accumbens (Salimpoor et al.,2011). While chills tend to produce
physiological arousal and reward, the underlying mechanism,
consequences and downstream effects are still unclear. In order
to address these questions, we tested the emotional reaction of
600+ participants to ChillsDB, an open-source database of chills
stimuli, searching for interindividual differences and variations
in the ratings of valence and arousal based on whether or not the
participants experienced chills.
Chills seem to be a universal emotional phenomenon found
across human cultures and languages (McCrae,2007). As a
marker of human peak experiences across the arts, sciences, and
world religions (Schoeller,2015a), chills can be generated by a
wide range of media: music, films, paintings, poetry, science,
mathematics, religion, and rituals (Schoeller,2015b). Among
others, they have psychological consequences on pleasure and
reward (Blood and Zatorre,2001;Schoeller and Perlovsky,
2016), prosocial tendencies and altruism (Fukui and Toyoshima,
2014), meaning-making (Schoeller,2015a), attention, memory,
and cognitive function (Sarasso et al.,2020). Their relationship
to physiologic factors includes heart rate (Sumpf et al.,2015),
pupil dilatation (Laeng et al.,2016), skin conductance (Grewe
et al.,2009), and indeed muscle contractions (McPhetres and
Zickfeld,2022). Research has investigated chills’ relationship
to emotional valence and arousal. For example, the emotional
features of chill-eliciting music have been examined (Panksepp,
1995;Grewe et al.,2011;de Fleurian and Pearce,2021). When
analyzing approximately 1,000 musical stimuli, de Fleurian and
Pearce found that chills music was on average more negative
in valence, in accordance with previous findings that chills are
more frequently associated with perceived sadness (Panksepp,
1995). Salimpoor et al. (2011) and Laeng et al. (2016) found
that physiological markers of arousal predicted chills when
listening to music. The study of the adjacent emotion of “being
moved” by Zickfeld et al. (2020) found that experiences rated as
“very moving” resulted in less arousal compared to emotional
experiences rated as “sad.” The evidence further suggests that
there may be interindividual differences in chills induced by
personality and gender. It seems that females experience musical
chills more often than males (Panksepp,1995;Kunkel et al.,
2008). Here again, existing evidence is conflicting as Goldstein’s
(1980), Rickard’s (2004), and Grewe et al.’s (2009) found no
significant difference in terms of gender. To our knowledge, no
age differences have been reported thus far (Goldstein,1980;
Panksepp,1995;Grewe et al.,2009). In terms of personality,
chills have been found to be a good predictor of the personality
trait Openness to Experience (McCrae,2007), specifically item
188 of NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI), which measures
chills in response to music and the arts (McCrae,2007).
This accumulated evidence calls for large scale studies of the
chills phenomenon, mapping stimuli, participants’ states and
traits, and context. The present study is a first attempt to
better characterize the emotion of chills by examining a large
corpus of chills stimuli (ChillsDB) constituted by parsing social
media (YouTube and Reddit). Based on the prior research,
we expect that (1) chills should have an effect on valence
and arousal, (2) there should be interindividual differences
in chills demographics (genre, ethnicity, and age), and (3)
the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and Item
188, in particular, should predict for chills (McCrae,2007;
Krishnakumar and Schoeller,2019).
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Stimuli
We used the top 50 videos of ChillsDB, an open-source
database of validated audiovisual stimuli eliciting aesthetic
chills (goosebumps, psychogenic shivers) in a US population
(Schoeller et al.,2022b). The database consists of 204 chills-
eliciting videos in three categories: music, film, and speech
(see Table 1). ChillsDB was built using an ecologically-valid
method for harnessing chills stimuli “in the wild” by searching
for mentions of somatic markers in user comments using
algorithms to parse social media platforms (YouTube and
Reddit).
2.2. Procedure
We conducted an online study on Prolific crowdsourcing
platform1to evaluate the emotional effect of chill-inducing
videos. Participants were first screened for neurologic disorders
and randomly assigned to one of ChillsDB top 50 videos.
Before the video, they were given a definition of “the feeling
of emotional chills and shivers” as “the feeling of cold down
your spine that are NOT related to temperature or sickness but
that are caused by some strong emotions.” Participants were
asked to report their age, gender, ethnicity, frequency of daily
chills (1 - never; 5 - always), NEO item 188 score (“Sometimes
when I am reading poetry or looking at a work of art, I feel a
chill or wave of excitement”) (1 - Strongly Disagree; 5- Strongly
Agree). Following a circumplex model of emotion (Russell,
1980), participants were asked to report their current mood
in feeling “Extremely Unpleasant” to “Extremely Pleasant” for
the valence rating, “Extremely Calm” to “Extremely Excited”
for the arousal rating, “Extremely Sleepy” to “Fully Awake” for
sleepiness rating on a 10 point Likert scale before the video.
Participants were then exposed to the audiovisual stimulus from
the database and asked to report whenever they experienced
1www.prolific.co
Frontiers in Neuroscience 02 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 3
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
TABLE 1 A gold standard for aesthetic chills: the top 10 validated videos from our study, including the top three for each category (film, music,
and speech).
Video title Type Video description Average chills
reported
Jurassic World Movie The final battle between Indominus Rex, Tyrannosaurus Rex, and Velociraptors as it
appeared in the movie “Jurassic World” (2015).
3.90
The Hunger Games Movie Fan made video of “The Hanging Tree” from the score of “The Hunger Games.” The
music was composed by James Newton Howard.
4.18
Naruto Movie An important battle in the widely popular Japanese manga series written and
illustrated by Masashi Kishimoto.
2.90
Miserere Music Allegri’s Miserere is written for two choirs, who alternate phrases and then unite for a
final resolution.
2.60
Dream Speech A medley of motivational speeches by speakers such as Les Brown, Eric Thomas, and
Will Smith.
3.00
Dropout Wisdom Speech A speech by Dr. Rick Rigsby about lessons learned from his father. 3.10
Interstellar Movie Fan made video with Interstellar’s Hans Zimmer theme. Chills occur at the line
“Because my dad promised me.”
3.20
Unbroken Speech Fan made video with motivational speeches by Les Brown, Eric Brown, and Steve
Jobs.
2.50
Disney Heroes Movie Fan made video of Disney heroes singing in their native languages, with medley
images of their stories.
3.90
Giving Movie “Giving” is a 3 min Thai TV commercial by TrueMove mobile company. Concept by
Panu Meepaibul.
3.06
All stimuli have a probability 0.8 to elicit chills in a US population (from Schoeller et al.,2022a).
chills by pressing a large button on the right side of the
screen. Only once the stimulus is over are they allowed to
continue to the next part of the study. After the exposure
they were asked to report the intensity of the chills, and rate
their experience in terms of valence and arousal all on a five
point Likert scale. Finally, we conclude by asking the final five
items of the NEO questionnaire (“Do you feel you do not
have good imagination, “Are you quick to understand things,
“Do you use difficult words, “Do you spend time reflecting
things, and “Are you full of ideas.”) on a five point Likert
scale.
To ensure the quality of data we implemented two
attention checks, one before the stimulus and one after, to
determine if participants were paying attention to questions
(Oppenheimer et al.,2009). The attention checks constituted
selecting the correct response to a described question, e.g.,
“Please select ‘strongly agree for this question.” Participants
were compensated based on time spent in the study at an
TABLE 2 Independent samples T-test for age, daily occurrences of
chills and NEO item 188.
Statistic df P
Age Student’s t2.41 558 0.016
Daily chills Student’s t2.83 558 0.005
NEO Item 188 Student’s t4.28 a 558 <0.001
aLevene’s test is significant (p<0.05), suggesting a violation of the assumption of
equal variances.
approximately hourly rate of USD 11.72. Each experiment lasted
approximately 10 min and was well-received by the participant.
Some of the participants even wrote to the authors a personal
email to thank them for the experience (e.g., “I just wanted to
take a moment and tell you that I thoroughly enjoyed this study
and found it also to be a unique experiment”).
2.3. Participants
Participants were recruited on the Prolific platform and were
screened for psychiatric conditions or neurologic disorders.
660 subjects participated in the experiment (Mean age = 33.6,
50% males, 49.5% female, and 0.5% = other). We removed
100 participants who reported an aberrant proportion of chills
(N>10) and did not fulfill the two attention checks. All
the participants reside in the United States of America and
practice the English language as their first language. 75.7 White,
7.9 Multiracial, 8.4 Asian, 3.6 African American, 3.9 Hispanic,
and 0.5% Others.
2.4. Ethics
The experiment is in compliance with the Helsinki
Declaration. The study was approved by the Committee on
the Use of Humans as Experimental Subjects at MIT. All
participants gave their voluntary informed consent and we
followed the Ethics Code of the American Psychological
Frontiers in Neuroscience 03 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 4
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
FIGURE 1
Arousal and valence ratings prior to exposure to the stimulus are normalized across participants, independently of whether they reported chills.
Hence, prior valence and arousal do not seem to influence the probability of participants to experience chills.
FIGURE 2
Participants rating of the experience depending on whether they experienced chills or not. We found that participants who experienced chills
report significantly greater valence and arousal than those who did not.
TABLE 3 Independent samples T-test for valence and arousal ratings.
Statistic df P Effect size
Experience rating Valence Student’s t8.324 558 <0.001 Cohen’s d 0.7420
Experience rating Arousal Student’s t3.082 558 0.002 Cohen’s d 0.2748
Association. All participants were informed about the purpose
of the research, their right to decline to participate and to
withdraw from the experiment, and the limits of confidentiality.
We also provided them with a contact for any questions
concerning the research and with the opportunity to ask any
questions regarding the phenomenon under study (aesthetic
chills) and receive appropriate answers. All participants reacted
positively to the experiment and were thankful for the
opportunity to learn about the phenomenon.
2.5. Reviewer disclosure
Following the standard reviewer disclosure request
endorsed by the Center for Open Science (Lilienfeld,2017).
We confirm to have reported all measures, conditions, data
exclusions, and how we determined our sample sizes.
3. Results
3.1. Chills participants
We then examined interindividual responses in chills
(Table 2). A total of 369 participants reported chills (66%). These
participants who reported more chills (M= 35.7, SD = 13.5),
compared to those who did not (M= 38.7, SD = 14.7) were
significantly younger [t(558) = 2.41, p<0.01]. Participants who
reported chills experienced chills more frequently in everyday
life (M= 1.3, SD = 0.7) as compared to those who did not
Frontiers in Neuroscience 04 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 5
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
FIGURE 3
Distribution of experience rating for participants depending on whether they experienced chills or not. Participants who reported chills tend to
cluster toward higher arousal and valence ratings than participants who did not.
report chills during the experiment (M= 1.1, SD = 0.7), this
difference is statistically significant [t(558) = 2.83, p= 0.005].
Furthermore, participants who reported chills (M= 2.1,
SD = 1.1) had significantly higher scores on the NEO Item 188
[t(558) = 4.28, p<0.001] than participants who did not
report chills (M= 1.6, SD = 1.3). Prior subjective ratings of
valence and arousal did not seem to influence the probability
of participants to experience chills (Figure 1).
ANOVA was performed to test the difference in chills
between ethnicities. ANOVA revealed that there was not a
statistically significant difference in chills between the groups
[F(5) = 1.23, p= 0.29]. We also performed an ANOVA to
search for a difference in chills across gender, the test revealed
that there was not a significant difference between genders
[F(3) = 0.55, p= 0.64]. We did not find any statistical difference
in terms of post cumulative NEO score between participants
who experienced chills and those who did not [t(558) = 0.6,
p>0.55]. We found that prior exposure to the stimulus did not
affect chills prevalence [t(558) = 1.15, p>0.25].
3.2. Effects on valence and arousal
Chills participants reported a change in valence and arousal
toward the experience (Figure 2). Compared to the participants
who did not report chills (M= 2.19, SD = 0.78), participants who
reported chills (M= 2.77, SD = 0.78) showed a significantly more
positive valence after the experience [t(558) = 8.3, p<0.001].
We also found a change in arousal, whereby participants who
reported chills (M= 2.41, SD = 0.94) compared to those who did
not (M= 2.15, SD = 0.96), were more aroused [t(558) = 3.1,
p= 0.002]. Hence, we reject the null hypothesis of independence
between chills report and either valence or arousal (Table 3).
Figure 3 shows the distribution of ratings of arousal and valence
of the participants who reported experiencing chills versus who
did not.
We further found the number of chills experienced by
the participants to be positively correlated with the reported
experience arousal (r= 0.104, p= 0.014) and valence (r= 0.274,
p∗∗∗ <0.001) ratings. Similarly reported chills intensity was
also positively correlated with the reported experience arousal
(r= 0.162, p∗∗∗ <0.001) and valence (r= 0.417, p∗∗∗ <0.001)
ratings. This suggests a positive impact of the number of
chills and the intensity of chills on emotional appraisal of the
stimuli. Figure 4 shows the drift observed in mean arousal and
valence ratings of the stimulus when participants experience
chills concurrently with the stimulus versus when they do
not.
When examining differences across ethnicities, we found
that, though initially different, the valence and arousal
ratings tended toward a similar mean after the experience
in chills participants (Figure 5). We note however, that
Frontiers in Neuroscience 05 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 6
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
FIGURE 4
Ratings of the experience in terms of valence and arousal for each stimulus (ratings were normalized from –0.5 to 0.5). The blue dots indicate
mean rating in valence and arousal for stimuli which induced chills in participants while the orange dots indicate the mean valence and arousal
ratings of stimuli which did not induce chills. We see that the majority of stimuli’s ratings tend to cluster in the top right quadrant, whereas the
bottom left quadrant is mostly populated by ratings of stimuli who did not cause the chills. Essentially, this suggests that the chills response
influences the participant’s perception of the stimulus.
this harmonization observed may be an artifact of the
limited resolution of the scales and should be further
investigated.
4. Discussion and conclusion
We examined the emotional consequences of aesthetic
chills in terms of valence and arousal in a large sample
of participants and across many different stimuli including
music, films, and speech. We found that participants who
experienced chills during the experiment reported significantly
more positive emotional valence and greater arousal for their
experience as compared to the participants who did not
report chills. This indicates that the embodied emotion of
chills causes participants to experience stimuli with greater
emotional intensity. It is interesting to consider these results
in light of prior research on the role of dopamine in salience
signaling and so-called precision encoding (Diederen and
Fletcher,2021;Haarsma et al.,2021). Dopamine release has
been linked to improved emotion recognition (Schuster et al.,
2022), which may partially explain some of the results reported
and call for further investigation of the chills phenomenon
in dopaminergic-related pathologies (e.g., Parkinson’s disease,
schizophrenia, and depression), specifically investigating the
role of bodily signals in shaping the perception of the subject’s
context and rewarding stimuli (Nguyen et al.,2015;Schoeller
et al.,2022a).
Our findings extend previous studies by Salimpoor et al.
(2011) and Laeng et al. (2016) who found that—when listening
to music—physiological markers of arousal predicted chills. It is
interesting to consider that Fleurian and Pearce found that chills
music was on average lower in valence, and Panksepp’s found
that chills were associated with perceived sadness (Panksepp,
1995), insofar as our results seem to suggest that for the same
given stimulus, participants who experience chills will tend
to rate it as greater in valence and arousal. The emotional
drift induced by chills may explain the popularity of these
stimuli in popular culture and in the web media that they were
extracted from. In accordance with the existing literature, we
found demographic differences in chills frequency. We found
a significant difference in terms of age, as younger participants
had a greater chance of experiencing chills. As a limitation, we
note that this finding may be due to the age distribution in
our sample, as the use of YouTube is not equally distributed in
society (Auxier and Anderson,2021) and some of the content
is culture and age-specific (e.g., anime, motivation speech, and
films). Our results also confirm that Item 188 of the Revised
Frontiers in Neuroscience 06 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 7
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
FIGURE 5
Differences in experience arousal and valence ratings across ethnicities depending on whether participants experienced chills or not. It seems
that participants who reported chills show greater similarity in emotional ratings of the experience as compared to those who did not.
NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) is a good predictor of
chills (McCrae,2007). However, we did not find a significant
difference in terms of evaluating NEO score post exposure
to chills. Furthermore, we were not able to replicate prior
findings that chills vary with gender, nor did we find any ethnic
difference in chills. Interestingly, when examining differences
in emotion amongst various ethnicities, we found that the
means for each individual ethnicity, though initially significantly
different, tended to harmonize toward the same mean value.
The distribution observed in Figure 5 is likely due to the five
points scoring, however, we report this interesting pattern, as
worthy of further research into the emotional harmonization
of chills, i.e., their ability to synchronize emotional valence in
a group with initial disparate ratings—putting everyone on a
similar emotional level.
The capacity of chills stimuli to induce positive emotional
valence and greater arousal for the same stimulus regardless
of prior emotional states may be relevant for mental health
intervention (Khalsa et al.,2018), e.g., to influence negatively
valenced rumination in depression (Schoeller et al.,2022c), or
negative affective bias in schizophrenics. The neural correlates
of chills discussed in Blood and Zatorre (2001) and Salimpoor
et al. (2011) suggests a pattern of activity reminding of
euphoria in drug research, whereby ventral tegmental area
(VTA) neurons project to the nucleus accumbens and the
hippocampus in the limbic system while correlating with a
deactivation of the amygdala and orbito and ventromedial
prefrontal cortex (Blood and Zatorre,2001). A future research
question would be to assess whether the drift in arousal and
valence also correlates with reward-driven improvement in
Frontiers in Neuroscience 07 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 8
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
memory encoding, as suggested by studies on the role of VTA
dopaminergic projections to the hippocampus in encoding
emotional memories (Shohamy and Adcock,2010;Ripollés
et al.,2018). Our results mirror those of Ferreri and colleagues
on music and reward and the role of dopamine in the process
(Ferreri et al.,2013,2021;Ferreri and Rodriguez-Fornells,
2017,2022). Note that activity in the insular cortex during
the chills response speaks to the importance of interoception
(and peripheral signals) in the chills response. Hence, while this
study carried the inherent limitations of online experiments,
it would be of interest to replicate the present results using
peripheral measures as well as a wearable device to enhance
feeling of chills by simulating the somatic markers of chills
(Schoeller et al.,2019;Haar et al.,2020;Jain et al.,2022).
Following the embodied model of emotions (Pezzulo,2013),
we hypothesize that such manipulation of interoceptive signals
should influence further the emotional drift in valence and
arousal reported by participants in this study, in line with the
recent framework outlined in Schoeller et al. (2022a). Such
controlled manipulations could be useful for mood disorders
such as depression, specifically to address anhedonia symptoms
and reward sensitivity (Webb et al.,2022).
As the first study testing ChillsDB stimuli, a number of
limitations should be noted. We chose the top 50 videos of
ChillsDB based on how many mentions of dictionary elements.
Though specifically designed for this purpose ChillsDB is
indeed not exhaustive (or even representative of the gigantic
sum of data available on YouTube). Hence the results
reported here, though convenient for experimental purposes,
can and should be enriched by further studies. A further
limitation of this study is that all measures are obtained
through self-report. As emphasized previously, future studies
should include physiological measures [e.g., pupil size galvanic
skin response (GSR), heart rate (HR), etc.] to validate the
degree to which chilling experiences are actually induced by
these stimuli and confirm the present findings in terms of
change in arousal.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this study can be found in
online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories
and accession number(s) can be found below: https://doi.org/
10.7910/DVN/ADLSZE.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were
reviewed and approved by the Committee on the Use
of Humans as Experimental Subjects at MIT. The
patients/participants provided their written informed consent
to participate in this study.
Author contributions
AJ and FS conceptualized the study and designed the
experiments and analyzed the results. GY and XH built
the code under the supervision of AJ and FS. All authors
participated equally in writing the manuscript and approved the
submitted version.
Funding
This research was partly supported by the Joy
Ventures Research Grant.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated
organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the
reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or
claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed
or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Auxier, B., and Anderson, M (2021). Social Media Use in 2021. Washington, DC:
Pew Research Center: Internet, Science & Tech.
Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music
correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898
de Fleurian, R., and Pearce, M. T. (2021). The relationship between valence
and chills in music: a corpus analysis. I-Perception 12, 1–11. doi: 10.1177/
20416695211024680
Diederen, K. M. J., and Fletcher, P. C. (2021). Dopamine, prediction error and
beyond. Neuroscientist 27, 30–46. doi: 10.1177/1073858420907591
Frontiers in Neuroscience 08 frontiersin.org
fnins-16-1013117 March 7, 2023 Time: 13:28 # 9
Jain et al. 10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117
Ferreri, L., Aucouturier, J., Muthalib, M., Bigand, E., and Bugaiska, A. (2013).
Music improves verbal memory encoding while decreasing prefrontal cortex
activity: An fNIRS study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:779. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.
00779
Ferreri, L., Mas-Herrero, E., Cardona, G., Zatorre, R. J., Antonijoan, R. M.,
Valle, M., et al. (2021). Dopamine modulations of reward-driven music memory
consolidation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1502, 85–98. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14656
Ferreri, L., Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2017). Music-related reward responses
predict episodic memory performance. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 3721–3731. doi: 10.
1007/s00221-017- 5095-0
Ferreri, L., and Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2022). Memory modulations through
musical pleasure. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1516, 5–10. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14867
Fukui, H., and Toyoshima, K. (2014). Chill-inducing music enhances altruism
in humans. Front. Psychol. 5:1215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01215
Goldstein, A. (1980). Thrills in response to music and other stimuli. Physiol.
Psychol. 8, 126–129.
Grewe, O., Katzur, B., Kopiez, R., and Altenmüller, E. (2011). Chills in different
sensory domains: Frisson elicited by acoustical, visual, tactile and gustatory
stimuli. Psychol. Music 39, 220–239.
Grewe, O., Kopiez, R., and Altenmüller, E. (2009). The chill parameter: goose
bumps and shivers as promising measures in emotion research. Music Percept 27,
61–74. doi: 10.1525/mp.2009.27.1.61
Haar, A. J. H., Jain, A., Schoeller, F., and Maes, P. (2020). Augmenting aesthetic
chills using a wearable prosthesis improves their downstream effects on reward
and social cognition. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020- 77951-w
Haarsma, J., Fletcher,P. C., Griffin, J. D., Taverne, H. J., Ziauddeen, H., Spencer,
T. J., et al. (2021). Precision weighting of cortical unsigned prediction error signals
benefits learning, is mediated by dopamine, and is impaired in psychosis. Mol.
Psychiatry 26, 5320–5333. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0803- 8
Jain, A., Schoeller, F., Zhang, E., and Maes, P. (2022). “Frisson: Leveraging
metasomatic interactions for generating aesthetic chills, in Proceedings of the 2022
International Conference on Multimodal Interaction (ICMI ’22) (New York, NY:
Association for Computing Machinery), 148–158.
Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W.,
Feinstein, J. S., et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol.
Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 3, 501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.
12.004
Krishnakumar, A., and Schoeller, F. (2019). Investigating the role of psychogenic
shivers in mental health. Psychol. Mental Health Care 5:8tzh5. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/
8tzh5
Kunkel, M., Pramstaller, C., Bücher, S., and von Georgi, R. (2008). A Construct-
Psychological Approach to the Measurement of Chill-Sensations.
Laeng, B., Eidet, L. M., Sulutvedt, U., and Panksepp, J. (2016). Music chills:
The eye pupil as a mirror to music’s soul. Conscious Cogn. 44, 161–178. doi:
10.1016/j.concog.2016.07.009
Lilienfeld, S. O. ed (2017). “Maximizing the reproducibility of your research, in
Psychological science under scrutiny: Recent challenges and proposed solutions, Vol.
25 (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell), 371, 3–21.
Maruskin, L., Thrash, T., and Elliot, A. (2012). The chills as a psychological
construct: Content universe, factor structure, affective composition, elicitors, trait
antecedents, and consequences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 135–157.
McCrae, R. R. (2007). Aesthetic chills as a universal marker of openness to
experience. Motiv. Emot. 31, 5–11. doi: 10.1007/s11031-007-9053-1
McPhetres, J., and Zickfeld, J. H. (2022). The physiological study of emotional
piloerection: A systematic review and guide for future research. Int. J.
Psychophysiol. 179, 6–20. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.06.010
Nguyen, V. T., Breakspear, M., Hu, X., and Guo, C. C. (2015). Hierarchical
integration of interoception and exteroception in the anterior insula
during naturalistic emotional experience. Autonomic Neurosci. Basic Clin.
192:81.
Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T., and Davidenko, N. (2009). Instructional
manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. J. Exp. Soc.
Psychol. 45, 867–872.
Panksepp, J. (1995). The emotional sources of “chills” induced by music. Music
Percept. 13, 171–207. doi: 10.2307/40285693
Pezzulo, G. (2013). Why do you fear the Bogeyman? An embodied predictive
coding model of perceptual inference. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 902–911.
doi: 10.3758/s13415-013- 0227-x
Rickard, N. S. (2004). Intense emotional responses to music: A test of the
physiological arousal hypothesis. Psychol. Music 32, 371–388. doi: 10.1177/
0305735604046096
Ripollés, P., Ferreri, L., Mas-Herrero, E., Alicart, H., Gómez-Andrés, A., Marco-
Pallares, J., et al. (2018). Intrinsically regulated learning is modulated by synaptic
dopamine signaling. Elife 7:e38113. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38113
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39,
1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714
Salimpoor, V., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J.
(2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and
experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–262. doi: 10.1038
/nn.2726
Sarasso, P., Neppi-Modona, M., Sacco, K., and Ronga, I. (2020). ‘Stopping for
knowledge’: The sense of beauty in the perception-action cycle. Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev. 118, 723–738.
Schoeller, F. (2015a). Knowledge, curiosity, and aesthetic chills. Front Psychol.
6:1546. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01546
Schoeller, F. (2015b). The Shivers of knowledge. Hum. Soc. Stud. 4, 44–55.
doi: 10.1515/hssr-2015-0022
Schoeller, F., Haar, A. J. H., Jain, A., and Maes, P. (2019). Enhancing human
emotions with interoceptive technologies. Phys. Life Rev. 31, 310–319. doi: 10.
1016/j.plrev.2019.10.008
Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Horowitz, A. H., Yan, G., Hu, X., Maes, P., et al. (2022b).
ChillsDB, A gold standard for aesthetic chills stimuli. PsyArXiv [Preprint] doi:
10.31234/osf.io/9wrmq
Schoeller, F., Horowitz, A. H., Maes, P., Jain,A., Reggente, N., Moore, L. C., et al.
(2022a). Interoceptive technologies for clinical neuroscience. PsyArXiv [Preprint]
doi: 10.31234/osf.io/sqr6z
Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Adrien, V., and Maes, P. (2022c). Aesthetic chills foster
self-acceptance and emotional breakthrough in depression. PsyArXiv [Preprint].
doi: 10.31234/osf.io/rhftq
Schoeller, F., and Perlovsky, L. (2016). Aesthetic chills: knowledge-acquisition,
meaning-making, and aesthetic emotions. Front. Psychol. 7:1093. doi: 10.3389/
fpsyg.2016.01093
Schuster, B. A., Sowden, S., Rybicki, A. J., Fraser, D. S., Press, C., Holland,
P., et al. (2022). Dopaminergic modulation of dynamic emotion perception.
J. Neurosci. 42, 4394–4400.
Shohamy, D., and Adcock, R. (2010). Dopamine and adaptive memory. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 14, 464–472.
Sumpf, M., Jentschke, S., and Koelsch, S. (2015). Effects of aesthetic chills on
a cardiac signature of emotionality. PLoS One 10:e0130117. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pone.0130117
Webb, C. A., Murray, L., Tierney, A. O., Forbes, E. E., and Pizzagalli,
D. A. (2022). Reward-related predictors of symptom change in behavioral
activation therapy for anhedonic adolescents: a multimodal approach.
Neuropsychopharmacology [Online ahead of print] doi: 10.1038/s41386-022-
01481-4
Zickfeld, J. H., Arriaga, P., Santos, S. V., Schubert, T. W., and Seibt, B. (2020).
Tears of joy, aesthetic chills and heartwarming feelings: Physiological correlates of
Kama Muta. Psychophysiology 57:e13662. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13662
Frontiers in Neuroscience 09 frontiersin.org
... Studies in which a combination of positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used have shown that dopamine is released in the striatum during anticipation and peak experience of musical pleasure, which induces chills [6]. Here, "chills" represent an embodied peak emotional experience characterized by goosebumps and dopamine release [6,7]. Chills are widely regarded as a marker of the highest pleasure levels [8] and can be induced not only by music but also by other stimuli, such as films and speeches [7]. ...
... Here, "chills" represent an embodied peak emotional experience characterized by goosebumps and dopamine release [6,7]. Chills are widely regarded as a marker of the highest pleasure levels [8] and can be induced not only by music but also by other stimuli, such as films and speeches [7]. Given that chills are positively correlated with both valence and arousal [7], we define "musical pleasure" in this study as a state of high valence and to eliminate the potential influence of varying degrees of familiarity among the participants, using music that the participants have never heard is necessary. ...
... Chills are widely regarded as a marker of the highest pleasure levels [8] and can be induced not only by music but also by other stimuli, such as films and speeches [7]. Given that chills are positively correlated with both valence and arousal [7], we define "musical pleasure" in this study as a state of high valence and to eliminate the potential influence of varying degrees of familiarity among the participants, using music that the participants have never heard is necessary. This approach allows the investigation of pleasure induced purely by surprise owing to prediction error. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background/Objectives: Musical pleasure is considered to be induced by prediction errors (surprise), as suggested in neuroimaging studies. However, the role of temporal changes in musical features in reward processing remains unclear. Utilizing the Information Dynamics of Music (IDyOM) model, a statistical model that calculates musical surprise based on prediction errors in melody and harmony, we investigated whether brain activities associated with musical pleasure, particularly in the θ, β, and γ bands, are induced by prediction errors, similar to those observed during monetary rewards. Methods: We used the IDyOM model to calculate the information content (IC) of surprise for melody and harmony in 70 musical pieces across six genres; eight pieces with varying IC values were selected. Electroencephalographic data were recorded during listening to the pieces, continuously evaluating the participants’ subjective pleasure on a 1–4 scale. Time–frequency analysis of electroencephalographic data was conducted, followed by general linear model analysis to fit the power-value time course in each frequency band to the time courses of subjective pleasure and IC for melody and harmony. Results: Significant positive fits were observed in the β and γ bands in the frontal region with both subjective pleasure and IC for melody and harmony. No significant fit was observed in the θ band. Both subjective pleasure and IC are associated with increased β and γ band power in the frontal regions. Conclusions: β and γ oscillatory activities in the frontal regions are strongly associated with musical rewards induced by prediction errors, similar to brain activity observed during monetary rewards.
... To test the effects of chills videos on depression and self-schema, we used two stimuli from ChillsDB, a recently constituted chills stimuli database [22]: one motivational video specifically targeting self-schema through emotional invectives to the listener and a viral commercial video targeting pro-social feelings and known to elicit chills and tears. Both have been shown in past studies to elicit chills in 80% of subjects as well as powerful emotional reactions [37]. To estimate the subjective effects of chills and assess to what extent they may resemble psychedelic or psychotherapeutic experiences, we used the recently constituted Emotional Breakthrough Inventory (EBI; [18]). ...
... Furthermore, we found that participants who reported chills reported a greater emotional breakthrough, a measure commonly used in psychedelic research to assess the patient's propensity to experience difficult emotions during the session. Similar to prior studies [37], we also found that chills were significantly correlated with a change in valence and arousal, a positive outcome for depressed patients who ordinarily struggle with anhedonia and lower reward sensitivity [44,45]. Interestingly, the change in valence score was mediated by the level of emotional breakthrough. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Depression is a major global health challenge, affecting over 300 million people worldwide. Current pharmacological and psychotherapeutic interventions have limited efficacy, underscoring the need for novel approaches. Emerging evidence suggests that peak emotional experiences characterized by awe, transcendence, and meaning hold promise for rapidly shifting maladaptive cognitive patterns in depression. Aesthetic chills, a peak positive emotion characterized by physical sensations such as shivers and goosebumps, may influence reward-related neural pathways and hold promise for modifying core maladaptive beliefs rooted in early adverse experiences. Methods We enrolled 96 patients diagnosed with major depressive disorder. A validated database of multimedia known to elicit chills responses (ChillsDB) was used for stimulus presentation. Participants’ emotional responses were assessed using the Emotional Breakthrough Inventory (EBI), while shifts in self-schema were measured via the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire (YSPQ). Results The study found that chill-inducing stimuli have the potential to positively influence the core schema of individuals with depression, impacting areas of self-related beliefs. The associated phenomenology triggered by chills appears to share similarities with the altered states of consciousness induced by psychedelic substances like psilocybin. Conclusions These preliminary results suggest that the biological processes involved in aesthetic chills could be harnessed as a non-pharmacological intervention for depression. However, further investigation is necessary to comprehensively understand the neurophysiological responses to chills and to evaluate the practicality, effectiveness, and safety of utilizing aesthetic chills as a preventive measure in mental health care.
... Aesthetic chills (henceforth"chills") have been reported in response to a wide variety of content, including art (music, literature, poetry), science (lectures, documentaries), and religious content (choral pieces, ritual) [43,46]. Individuals vary substantially in their susceptibility to chills and the intensity with which they experience them, with recent studies relating the likelihood of experiencing chills to openness to experience, absorption, extroversion, and positive arousal [42][43][44][45][46]. Importantly, chills seem to enhance affect, awe, meaning, and prosociality [47][48][49][50], outcomes consistent with psychedelic, mystical, or ST experiences. In summary, circumstantial evidence suggests that aesthetic chills may be a marker of aesthetically-evoked ST that is: (1) replicable [43,44], (2) correlated with recognizable physical markers such as tingling sensations, piloerection (goosebumps), and shivering [51,52], and (3) linked to neurophysiological markers [42]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Self-transcendence (ST) is a state of consciousness associated with feelings of ego-dissolution, connectedness, and moral elevation, which mediates well-being, meaning-making, and prosociality. Conventional paths to ST, like religious practice, meditation, and psychedelics, pose nontrivial barriers to entry, limiting ST’s study and application. Aesthetic chills (henceforth “chills”) are a psychophysiological response characterized by a pleasurable, cold sensation, with subjective qualities and downstream effects similar to ST. However, evidence is lacking directly relating chills and ST. In the summer of 2023, we exposed a diverse sample of 2937 participants in Southern California to chills-eliciting stimuli, then assayed chills, mood and ST. Even after controlling for differences in demographics, traits, and prior affective state, both chills likelihood and intensity were positively associated with measures ST. Parametric and non-parametric analyses of variance, mutual information, and correlation structure found that chills occurrence and intensity, and ST measures are reliably interrelated across a variety of audiovisual stimuli. These findings suggest aesthetic chills may denote sufficiently intense feelings of self-transcendence. Further study is necessary to demonstrate the generalizability of these results to non-WEIRD populations, and the precise direction of causal relationships between self-transcendent feelings and aesthetic chills.
... Being aesthetically moved creates a drift in emotional arousal and the induction of strong affective valence (Jain et al., 2023b), which have repeatedly been shown to impact memory (Kensinger, 2004). Traumatic autobiographical memories have a profound influence on individuals' well-being and have been suggested to be a transdiagnostic feature of multiple mental health difficulties, including Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (Dalgleish & Hitchcock, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aesthetic experiences, emerging saliently in the arts, play a pivotal role in transformative learning and creative processes that elicit physiological, affective, and cognitive responses associated with mental health indices. Interactions between subjects and aesthetic objects (e.g., visual artwork, music, and moving bodies) often entail elements of surprise and uncertainty that drive the inference of the hidden causes in the subject’s internal and external environment. These generate dynamics that align with the action-oriented Predictive Processing framework of brain function. Creative Arts Therapies (CATs) harness these dynamics by cultivating relational engagement using the arts modalities, prompting affective and cognitive processing. In this manuscript, we offer a review and conceptual analysis of recent empirical findings and theoretical premises that underpin aesthetic experiences and their relation to the psychotherapeutic use of the arts with a broad spectrum of populations and mental health conditions. We present a neuroscience-based approach to aesthetic intra- and inter-personal experiences, integrating therapeutic change factors of externalization-concretization, embodiment, and symbolization with functional network configurations, and interpersonal brain-to-brain coupling, to support predictive processing, learning, and creativity. Present and future interdisciplinary collaborations are underlined to elucidate the neurodynamic mechanisms driving psychological transformations, bridging neuroaesthetics and CATs.
... In fact, rice pudding can actually be considered a very mundane food. Furthermore, studies that have assessed the stimuli that give rise to 'chills' (frisson manifested as goose bumps or shivers) in different sensory modalities find that it is acidic fruits like biting into a slice of lemon or drinking grapefruit juice (Grewe et al., 2011; see also Jain et al., 2023). ...
... bodily sensations that act as emotional, conditioned responses representing complex reinforcer values related to prior experiences (7,20). Chills have been a research topic of growing interest in the past decade (7,21), notably for their effects on positive affect (22,23), reward learning (6,13,22,24), memory and attentional processes (25,26), prosocial tendencies (27)(28)(29), and as a nonpharmacological substitute for dopaminergic-related illnesses, and mood disorders (4,7,22). Substantial individual differences exist in both propensity for and intensity of chills (24,(30)(31)(32)(33)(34)(35)(36)(37)(38)(39)(40), which hinders progress in understanding and utilizing chills as therapeutic tools for mental health conditions and enhancing positive emotional experiences broadly. ...
Article
Full-text available
Why does the same experience elicit strong emotional responses in some individuals while leaving others largely indifferent? Is the variance influenced by who people are (personality traits), how they feel (emotional state), where they come from (demographics), or a unique combination of these? In this 2,900+ participants study, we disentangle the factors that underlie individual variations in the universal experience of aesthetic chills, the feeling of cold and shivers down the spine during peak experiences. Here, we unravel the interplay of psychological and sociocultural dynamics influencing self-reported chills reactions. A novel technique harnessing mass data mining of social media platforms curates the first large database of ecologically sourced chills-evoking stimuli. A combination of machine learning techniques (LASSO and SVM) and multilevel modeling analysis elucidates the interacting roles of demographics, traits, and states factors in the experience of aesthetic chills. These findings highlight a tractable set of features predicting the occurrence and intensity of chills—age, sex, pre-exposure arousal, predisposition to Kama Muta (KAMF), and absorption (modified tellegen absorption scale [MODTAS]), with 73.5% accuracy in predicting the occurrence of chills and accounting for 48% of the variance in chills intensity. While traditional methods typically suffer from a lack of control over the stimuli and their effects, this approach allows for the assignment of stimuli tailored to individual biopsychosocial profiles, thereby, increasing experimental control and decreasing unexplained variability. Further, they elucidate how hidden sociocultural factors, psychological traits, and contextual states shape seemingly “subjective” phenomena.
... Hence, identifying the relationship between aesthetic chills, the precision-weighting of prediction errors, and its dopaminergic substrate could help to understand the role of dopamine in these conditions. Using a recently constituted database of CS, some preliminary studies have investigated the effects of aesthetic chills on subjects diagnosed with Major Depressive Disorder (Jain et al., 2023a;Schoeller et al., 2023c). Preliminary data suggest that chills have an effect in mood disorders (Jain et al., 2023a, b;Schoeller et al., 2023c). ...
Article
Full-text available
The phenomenon of aesthetic chills—shivers and goosebumps associated with either rewarding or threatening stimuli—offers a unique window into the brain basis of conscious reward because of their universal nature and simultaneous subjective and physical counterparts. Elucidating the neural mechanisms underlying aesthetic chills can reveal fundamental insights about emotion, consciousness, and the embodied mind. What is the precise timing and mechanism of bodily feedback in emotional experience? How are conscious feelings and motivations generated from interoceptive predictions? What is the role of uncertainty and precision signaling in shaping emotions? How does the brain distinguish and balance processing of rewards versus threats? We review neuroimaging evidence and highlight key questions for understanding how bodily sensations shape conscious feelings. This research stands to advance models of brain-body interactions shaping affect and may lead to novel nonpharmacological interventions for disorders of motivation and pleasure.
Thesis
Full-text available
Only two decades ago scholars started becoming interested in an emerging intersemiotic translation modality: audio description. This PhD dissertation revolves around museum audio description, an accessibility service on the rise, though still less widespread than screen audio description. According to guidelines and practitioners, the main goal of an audio descriptive guide is that blind and partially sighted people can see the audio described exhibit in their mind’s eye. Knowing how we build mental images or representations drawing on linguistic input alone and how sensory experiences could shape these images or representations is pivotal to choose the right audio descriptive style, which might be distinct from the most widespread one. Here, we review findings from cognitive psychology and neuroscience on language processing, mental image creation and spatial cognition. Considering these findings, we advance that an audio description tailored to how blind and partially sighted people conceptualize and perceive the world can paint more accurate pictures in their minds and enhance the resulting aesthetic experience. This overarching hypothesis is put to the test in a study with 53 blind and partially sighted participants. Conceived as an online audio descriptive visit, this study compares two audio descriptive styles differing in the adopted perspective. The first audio descriptive style, which is the most widespread in practice, takes the perspective of an observer. Conversely, the embodied audio descriptive style adopts an inner perspective and invites the listener into the painting. Overall, participants did not build more vivid mental images or representations after listening to the embodied audio descriptions. In contrast, the paintings described with this inner perspective elicited a more intense aesthetic experience, as predicted. In addition, this study has shown that a single audio descriptive style is unlikely to accommodate for blind and partially sighted people’s preferences. It has also unearthed that picturing described information can be challenging for this audience. Given these results, we encourage further research into how visual information can be intersemiotically translated so that blind and partially sighted people can picture the exhibits in their minds satisfactorily. We also underline the importance of conducting longitudinal studies which allow for assessing not only the short-term influence of the variable or variables under study, but also their impact in the long run. These studies could reveal whether an effect which goes undetected emerges weeks or months after the first study session. Furthermore, these studies would open the door to researching the transformative power of the visual arts in the lives of blind and partially sighted people.
Preprint
Full-text available
Interoception—the perception of internal bodily signals—has recently emerged as an area of significant interest due to its potential implications in emotion and the prevalence of dysfunctional interoceptive processes across psychopathological conditions. Despite the importance of interoception in cognitive neuroscience and psychiatry, its experimental manipulation remains technically challenging. This is due to the invasive nature of existing methods, the limitation of self-report and unimodal measures of interoception, and the absence of standardized approaches across disparate fields. This article integrates diverse research efforts from psychology, physiology, psychiatry, and engineering to address this oversight. The aim is to provide a detailed cross-disciplinary overview, suggesting a consistent terminology for multidisciplinary research into interoceptive processes, their underlying mechanisms, and potential implications. Following a general introduction to the neurophysiology of interoception as hierarchical predictive processing, we review the existing paradigms for manipulating interoception the laboratory (e.g., interoceptive modulation), their underlying mechanisms (e.g., interoceptive conditioning), and clinical applications (e.g., interoceptive exposure). We suggest a general classification for interoceptive technologies reflecting interoception as a hierarchical system of bodily information processing, and discuss their potential for diagnosing and treating mental health disorders. Despite promising results, considerable work is still needed to develop standardized, validated measures of interoceptive function across domains and before these technologies can translate safely and effectively to clinical settings.
Preprint
Full-text available
We introduce ChillsDB, the first validated database of audiovisual stimuli eliciting aesthetic chills (goosebumps, psychogenic shivers) in a US population. To discover chills stimuli “in the wild”, we devised a bottom-up, ecologically-valid method consisting in searching for mentions of the emotion's somatic markers in user comments throughout social media platforms (YouTube and Reddit). We successfully captured 204 chills-eliciting videos of three categories: music, film, and speech. We then tested the top 50 videos in the database on 600+ participants and validated a gold standard of 10 stimuli with a .9 probability of generating chills. All ChillsDB tools and data are fully available on GitHub and PhysioNet for researchers to be able to contribute and perform further analysis.
Article
Full-text available
Music, thanks to its strong evocative power, is considered a powerful mnemonic tool for both normal and clinical populations. However, the mechanisms underpinning the music‐driven benefits on memory remain unclear. In memory research, reward dopaminergic signals have been highlighted as a major modulator of memory traces consolidation. Over the last years, via behavioral and pharmacological approaches, we have investigated the hypothesis that dopaminergic‐dependent musical pleasure is a crucial mechanism underpinning music‐driven memory benefits. Our results show that the pleasure felt during music listening, modulated by both the dopaminergic transmission and participants’ sensitivity to music reward, can increase episodic memory performance for the music itself as well as for nonmusical‐associated information. In this commentary paper, we aim to review the main findings obtained from three different studies, in order to discuss current advances and future directions in this research area.
Article
Full-text available
Chills experienced in response to music listening have been linked to both happiness and sadness expressed by music. To investigate these conflicting effects of valence on chills, we conducted a computational analysis on a corpus of 988 tracks previously reported to elicit chills, by comparing them with a control set of tracks matched by artist, duration, and popularity. We analysed track-level audio features obtained with the Spotify Web API across the two sets of tracks, resulting in confirmatory findings that tracks which cause chills were sadder than matched tracks and exploratory findings that they were also slower, less intense, and more instrumental than matched tracks on average. We also found that the audio characteristics of chills tracks were related to the direction and magnitude of the difference in valence between the two sets of tracks. We discuss these results in light of the current literature on valence and chills in music, provide a new interpretation in terms of personality correlates of musical preference, and review the advantages and limitations of our computational approach.
Article
Full-text available
Music listening provides one of the most significant abstract rewards for humans because hearing music activates the dopaminergic mesolimbic system. Given the strong link between reward, dopamine, and memory, we aimed here to investigate the hypothesis that dopamine‐dependent musical reward can drive memory improvements. Twenty‐nine healthy participants of both sexes provided reward ratings of unfamiliar musical excerpts that had to be remembered following a consolidation period under three separate conditions: after the ingestion of a dopaminergic antagonist, a dopaminergic precursor, or a placebo. Linear mixed modeling of the intervention data showed that the effect of reward on memory—i.e., the greater the reward experienced while listening to the musical excerpts, the better the memory recollection performance—was modulated by both dopaminergic signaling and individual differences in reward processing. Greater pleasure was consistently associated with better memory outcomes in participants with high sensitivity to musical reward, but this effect was lost when dopaminergic signaling was disrupted in participants with average or low musical hedonia. Our work highlights the flexibility of the human dopaminergic system, which can enhance memory formation not only through explicit and/or primary reinforcers but also via abstract and aesthetic rewards such as music.
Preprint
Aesthetic chills, a strong emotional reaction characterized by a specific bodily response of thermoregulatory mechanisms such as shivers and goosebumps, may hold scientific and clinical potential for reward-related or dopaminergic illnesses. In this first exploratory study, we examined the effects of chills stimulation on subjects clinically diagnosed with depression. Our results suggest that chill-inducing stimuli may have the potential to affect the core schema of depressed patients, specifically in terms of shame and self-acceptance. These results suggest that the mechanism of action during the chills response may resemble the form of problem resolution induced by the psychedelic and psychotherapeutic experience, leading to similar positive outcomes for the subject. Further research is needed to fully understand the effects of chills on mental health and to determine the feasibility and safety of using aesthetic chills as a therapeutic intervention.
Article
Anhedonia is a cardinal characteristic of depression which predicts worse treatment outcome and is among the most common residual symptoms following treatment. Behavioral Activation (BA) has been shown to be an effective treatment for depressed adults, and more recently, depressed adolescents. Given its emphasis on systematically and gradually increasing exposure to and engagement with rewarding activities and experiences, BA may be a particularly effective intervention for adolescents experiencing anhedonia and associated reward system dysfunction. In the present study, anhedonic adolescents (AA; n = 39) received 12 weekly sessions of BA and completed a multimodal (i.e., neural, behavioral, and self-report [ecological momentary assessment]) assessment of reward function at pre-treatment and post-treatment (as well as weekly self-report assessments of anhedonia). Typically developing adolescents (TDA; n = 41) completed the same measures at corresponding timepoints. Multilevel models tested pre-treatment reward-related predictors of anhedonia improvement, as well as change in reward measures over the course of BA. Analyses revealed significant reductions in anhedonia following BA treatment. Enhanced pre-treatment neural (striatal) reward responsiveness predicted greater anhedonia improvement. In contrast, baseline self-report and behavioral reward measures did not predict treatment outcome. A group x time interaction revealed greater increases in both reward- and loss-related neural responsiveness among AA relative to TDA adolescents. Consistent with a capitalization (rather than compensatory) model, pre-treatment neural – but not self-report or behavioral – measures of relatively enhanced reward responsiveness predicted better BA outcome. In addition to alleviating anhedonia, successful BA may also increase neural sensitivity to affectively salient (e.g., reward- and loss-related) stimuli among anhedonic youth.
Article
This paper provides an accessible review of the biological and psychological evidence to guide new and experienced researchers in the study of emotional piloerection in humans. A limited number of studies have attempted to examine the physiological and emotional correlates of piloerection in humans. However, no review has attempted to collate this evidence to guide the field as it moves forward. We first discuss the mechanisms and function of non-emotional and emotional piloerection in humans and animals. We discuss the biological foundations of piloerection as a means to understand the similarities and differences between emotional and non-emotional piloerection. We then present a systematic qualitative review (k = 24) in which we examine the physiological correlates of emotional piloerection. The analysis revealed that indices of sympathetic activation are abundant, suggesting emotional piloerection occurs with increased (phasic) skin conductance and heart rate. Measures of parasympathetic activation are lacking and no definite conclusions can be drawn. Additionally, several studies examined self-reported emotional correlates, and these correlates are discussed in light of several possible theoretical explanations for emotional piloerection. Finally, we provide an overview of the methodological possibilities available for the study of piloerection and we highlight some pressing questions researchers may wish to answer in future studies.
Article
Emotion recognition abilities are fundamental to our everyday social interaction. A large number of clinical populations show impairments in this domain, with emotion recognition atypicalities being particularly prevalent among disorders exhibiting a dopamine system disruption (e.g., Parkinson's disease). Although this suggests a role for dopamine in emotion recognition, studies employing dopamine manipulation in healthy volunteers have exhibited mixed neural findings and no behavioural modulation. Interestingly, whilst a dependence of dopaminergic drug effects on individual baseline dopamine function has been well established in other cognitive domains, the emotion recognition literature so far has failed to account for these possible interindividual differences. The present within-subjects study therefore tested the effects of the dopamine D2 antagonist haloperidol on emotion recognition from dynamic, whole-body stimuli while accounting for interindividual differences in baseline dopamine. 33 healthy male and female adults rated emotional point-light walkers (PLWs) once after ingestion of 2.5 mg haloperidol and once after placebo. To evaluate potential mechanistic pathways of the dopaminergic modulation of emotion recognition, participants also performed motoric and counting-based indices of temporal processing. Confirming our hypotheses, effects of haloperidol on emotion recognition depended on baseline dopamine function, where individuals with low baseline dopamine showed enhanced, and those with high baseline dopamine decreased emotion recognition. Drug effects on emotion recognition were related to drug effects on movement-based and explicit timing mechanisms, indicating possible mediating effects of temporal processing. Results highlight the need for future studies to account for baseline dopamine and suggest putative mechanisms underlying the dopaminergic modulation of emotion recognition.Significance statementA high prevalence of emotion recognition difficulties amongst clinical conditions where the dopamine system is affected suggests an involvement of dopamine in emotion recognition processes. However, previous psychopharmacological studies seeking to confirm this role in healthy volunteers thus far have failed to establish whether dopamine affects emotion recognition and lack mechanistic insights. The present study uncovered effects of dopamine on emotion recognition in healthy individuals by controlling for interindividual differences in baseline dopamine function and investigated potential mechanistic pathways via which dopamine may modulate emotion recognition. Our findings suggest that dopamine may influence emotion recognition via its effects on temporal processing, providing new directions for future research on typical and atypical emotion recognition.