ArticlePDF Available

The Influence of Presentation Frames of Visualization Information for Safety on Situational Awareness under a Three-Level User-Interface Design

MDPI
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health (IJERPH)
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To explore the influence of the construction and presentation frames of visualization information for safety (VIS) on people’s situation awareness (SA), we designed a three-level user interface (UI) of VIS based on the three-stage SA theory, including perception (SA1), comprehension (SA2), and projection (SA3). Then, 166 subjects were recruited and divided into three groups to participate in the experiment, in which SA was measured by the situation-present-assessment method (SPAM) and situation-awareness-rating technique (SART), and eye-movement data were recorded. The results show that the level−3 UI design could effectively improve the subjects’ SA levels. Although the increase in VIS displayed caused by the higher UI level led to a decrease in the perception-stage score of SA, the level−3 UI fully considered the three stages of human information processing, and helped improve the SA of the subjects; the overall SA score measured using the SART method was not significant, but the result was consistent with the SPAM. There was a framing effect on the presentation of VIS, and subjects perceived different degrees of risk under different presentation frames; that is, less risk under the positive frame, more risk under the negative frame, and a higher level of SA under the positive frame compared with the negative frame. To some extent, the nearest-neighbor-index (NNI) algorithm could be utilized to quantify subjects’ eye-tracking fixation mode. While the frames were guided by the high-level interface and the positive presentation frame, the distribution of the subjects’ gaze points was more discrete; they could grasp the relevant information more comprehensively and had a relatively high level of SA. To some extent, this study can provide a reference for the design and optimization of the VIS presentation interface.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Citation: Yuan, X.; Yan, J.; Sun, L.;
Cheng, F.; Guo, Z.; Yu, H. The
Influence of Presentation Frames of
Visualization Information for Safety
on Situational Awareness under a
Three-Level User-Interface Design.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,
20, 3325. https://doi.org/
10.3390/ijerph20043325
Academic Editors: Paul B.
Tchounwou and Lingxin Chen
Received: 4 January 2023
Revised: 9 February 2023
Accepted: 12 February 2023
Published: 14 February 2023
Copyright: © 2023 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
International Journal of
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Article
The Influence of Presentation Frames of Visualization
Information for Safety on Situational Awareness under a
Three-Level User-Interface Design
Xiaofang Yuan 1,2, Jing Yan 1,2,*, Linhui Sun 1,2, Fangming Cheng 3, Zigu Guo 1,2 and Hongzhi Yu 1,2
1School of Management, Xi’an University of Science and Technology, Xi’an 710054, China
2Research Center for Human Factors and Management Ergonomics, Xi’an University of Science and
Technology, Xi’an 710054, China
3School of Safety Science and Engineering, Xi’an University of Science and Technology, Xi’an 710054, China
*Correspondence: yanjing@stu.xust.edu.cn; Tel.: +86-158-9127-5836
Abstract:
To explore the influence of the construction and presentation frames of visualization
information for safety (VIS) on people’s situation awareness (SA), we designed a three-level user
interface (UI) of VIS based on the three-stage SA theory, including perception (SA1), comprehension
(SA2), and projection (SA3). Then, 166 subjects were recruited and divided into three groups
to participate in the experiment, in which SA was measured by the situation-present-assessment
method (SPAM) and situation-awareness-rating technique (SART), and eye-movement data were
recorded. The results show that the level
3 UI design could effectively improve the subjects’ SA
levels. Although the increase in VIS displayed caused by the higher UI level led to a decrease in the
perception-stage score of SA, the level
3 UI fully considered the three stages of human information
processing, and helped improve the SA of the subjects; the overall SA score measured using the
SART method was not significant, but the result was consistent with the SPAM. There was a framing
effect on the presentation of VIS, and subjects perceived different degrees of risk under different
presentation frames; that is, less risk under the positive frame, more risk under the negative frame,
and a higher level of SA under the positive frame compared with the negative frame. To some extent,
the nearest-neighbor-index (NNI) algorithm could be utilized to quantify subjects’ eye-tracking
fixation mode. While the frames were guided by the high-level interface and the positive presentation
frame, the distribution of the subjects’ gaze points was more discrete; they could grasp the relevant
information more comprehensively and had a relatively high level of SA. To some extent, this study
can provide a reference for the design and optimization of the VIS presentation interface.
Keywords:
framing effect; situation awareness (SA); visualization information for safety (VIS); situation-
present-assessment method (SPAM); situation-awareness-rating technique (SART); eye-movement
1. Introduction
With the development of the Internet, big data, cloud computing, and other technolo-
gies and urbanization, information visualization has played an increasingly crucial role in
the field of safety and emergency management. Visualization information for safety (VIS)
is the transformation of information and knowledge related to safety risks into a visual
representation, using the human ability to quickly identify views to improve cognitive
efficiency and make high-quality decisions [
1
]. In recent years, the application of various
safety-risk-monitoring technologies has gradually become popular. When VIS is presented
on the platform interface, people can perceive, understand, and predict the safety-risk
status of the target object promptly. For example, during the outbreak of COVID-19, a large
amount of VIS emerged to help the public quickly understand the real-time development of
the epidemic and predict the future evolutionary trend by promptly conveying the quantity
of information and spatial distribution of the outbreak to the public [
2
]. However, with the
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043325 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 2 of 26
increasing complexity and diversification of VIS, VIS has brought abundant information
and increased people’s cognitive load. Thus, minimizing the cognitive load of the interface
design is one aspect of improving its usability [
3
5
]. Weiser argued that information tech-
nology is often the enemy of calm, with countless pieces of information trying to attract
attention, and, owing to the limitation of an individual’s attention resources, the usability
of interface design can be achieved by minimizing user attention, improving the user’s
situation awareness (SA), and minimizing their input [
6
]. Blackwell’s proposed framework
of cognitive dimensions of notations (CDs) predicts the usability of interface design in
terms of the structural properties of a notation, its properties, and the resources of an
environment, providing an account of information construction that respects the value
of user activity [
7
,
8
]. A good visualization of safety information can help people better
obtain, store, and process information so that they can make effective decisions. Within
the general context of visualization for safety information, as the actual safety status of the
target object is invisible, the relevant context needs to be constructed and maintained in the
brain, and the way information is constructed and represented affects people’s construction
and maintenance of SA [912].
SA is the perception of the elements in the environment, the comprehension of their
meaning, and the projection of their status in the near future within a given period of
time and area of space [
13
]. It can be explained through the classical three-stage theory
of information processing [
13
]. The first stage is perception (SA1); attention is the key
link in this stage, and one makes a simple perception of task-relevant elements and their
current state in the surrounding environment. The second stage is comprehension (SA2),
where one understands how they affect the goal by integrating the elements of stage one.
The third stage is projection (SA3), where one integrates the information from the first
two stages to predict the future behavior and state of these elements within the context. The
concept of SA first originated in the 1990s with the study of fighter pilots [
14
], and has since
been extended to other fields [
15
]. It is frequently used to evaluate human–machine-interface
design [
16
,
17
]. Jones and Endsley reported that 76.3% of errors were related to the perception
of information, 20.3% of errors were related to the comprehension of information, and 3.4%
of errors were related to “failure to project the situation into the future” [
18
]. Researchers
have found that presenting more information can make participants feel overconfident and
lead to less accurate decisions [
19
]. However, some studies have concluded that people’s
decisions depend on limited information, and that “less is more” [
20
]. Thus, it can be
concluded that the construction of information has an impact on SA. As the VIS elements
presented become more complex, it is worth investigating how to construct them to better
accommodate people’s cognitive processing.
In addition, how information is presented via the interface largely influences SA by
determining how much information can be acquired, how accurately it can be acquired,
and to what degree it is compatible with people’s SA needs [
13
]. Recently, Rakhra [
21
]
and Fu [
22
] found that different displaying formats for the same visual elements could
have an impact on people’s SA and cognitive efficiency. Some scholars have also studied
the effects of information presentation on individual cognition and decision making from
the perspective of information-representation frames, typically through the framing ef-
fect [
23
26
]. The framing effect refers to the phenomenon that equivalent descriptions of the
same issue lead to different decision preferences. The traditional verbal framing effect was
first demonstrated by Tversky and Kahneman, based on the Asian disease problem [
23
].
Subsequent scholars have explored its prevalence and influencing factors [
24
,
25
]. Among
the types of framing effects, the attribute-framing effect [
26
] occurs when a single attribute
of an object or event is carried over in the positive or negative description, resulting in
a different perception of quality for consumers. For example, research has shown that
when beef is described as “75% lean” and “25% fat”, the result is a different perception of
quality for consumers, with more people preferring “75% lean” in the positive frame [
26
].
However, few scholars have studied the framing effect of visualization-information repre-
sentation and explored it in conjunction with the concept of SA. In conjunction with the
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 3 of 26
objective of this paper, frames can also be understood as different presentations of the same
visualization information. When VIS elements are represented in different frames, it is
worth investigating whether people perceive risks differently at the level of perception,
comprehension, and projection of the information.
Thus, we designed a three-level interface for VIS presentation based on the three-
stage SA theory, and, based on this, we manipulated the representation frames of VIS
and adopted the objective, subjective, and process-integrated measurement method to
investigate whether and how the construction and representation of VIS under the three-
level UI design influence people’s SA. We also aimed to provide a theoretical basis for
the design and optimization of VIS presentation interfaces, to some extent. The following
hypotheses are proposed: (1) The construction of VIS elements in the interface has an effect
on an individual’s SA, and different UI levels have different effects on the three stages of
information processing, that is, perception, comprehension, and prediction, which in turn
affect an individual’s overall SA. (2) There is a framing effect in the presentation of VIS, and
different presentation frames affect individual’s cognitive processing of the three stages of
perception, comprehension, and prediction of information, which in turn affects their SA.
2. Design of VIS Elements and the Three-Level UI
2.1. Design of VIS Elements
By investigating the existing VIS presentation interface, we found that the common
VIS included information such as the basic information of hidden risks, the number of
hidden risks and their rectification, and the degree of risk warning. Combined with the
three-stage SA theory, the main VIS elements used in this study were selected, including
the statistical chart for the number of hidden risks and their rectification, the statistical chart of
the hazard-rectification rate, and the risk-warning chart in the jurisdiction, which are presented
in this paper in a vertical bar chart, horizontal bar chart, and dashboard chart, respectively
(Figures 1and 2). Combined with the theory of framing effect, the attribute frames were
manipulated using the “number of rectified hazards and “number of unrectified hazards,”
“rectification rate of hazards,” and unrectified rate of hazards” (Figure 1). To reproduce the VIS-
presentation-interface reality, we show the corresponding bar charts in green and red colors to
represent “safe” and “danger”, respectively. Previous studies revealed that red was recognized
globally as the color for danger, and this was consistent across cultures and occupations; green,
although not recognized globally as the color for “safe,” represented “low risk in most cultures
and “safe” in the Chinese cultural context [
27
]. Therefore, in this study, green and red colors
were chosen to represent the “safe” and danger” messages, respectively.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 4 of 27
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
Figure 1. Main VIS elements after manipulating the attribute frames: (a,c) are positive frames; (b,d)
are negative frames.
2.2. Three-Level UI Design for VIS Presentation
Based on the three-stage-SA theory and the actual situation, a three-level UI for the
VIS presentation was designed (see Figure 2).
Level-1 UI (UI1): this level of interface presents a small number of VISs. At this level,
one gets an intuitive sense of the total number of hazards currently in the jurisdiction and
the number of hazards that have been rectified.
Level-2 UI (UI2): based on UI1, certain ratio information is added to help people un-
derstand the status of the rectification of hidden hazards in each jurisdiction.
Level-3 UI (UI3): based on UI1 and UI2, certain probabilistic risk information is
added to help people better predict the future state of risks in each jurisdiction.
Figure 1.
Main VIS elements after manipulating the attribute frames: (
a
,
c
) are positive frames;
(b,d) are negative frames.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 4 of 26
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 5 of 27
Figure 2. Three-level UI design for VIS presentation (English version). Note. This figure shows the
UIs employed in the study. The green-line box is the level-1 UI, the orange-line box is the level-2 UI,
and the red-line box is the level-3 UI.
3. Methods
3.1. The Measurement of SA
3.1.1. Three-Level UI Design for VIS Presentation
Five SA-measurement methods are commonly used: subjective measures (SART), ob-
jective measures (SAGAT and SPAM), process measures (such as eye-tracking), and per-
formance measures [28]. In this study, the situation-present-assessment method (SPAM),
situation-awareness-rating technique (SART), and eye-tracking technique were used to
measure the subjects’ level of SA in each presentation frame, based on the three-stage SA
theory. Among them, SPAM is a real-time probe technique that measures participants’ SA
using online-probe questions related to the task without freezing the scenario, which rec-
ords response accuracy and latency to measure SA [29]. As SPAM focuses on the ability
to locate information in the situation and does not require freezing the task performed by
the subject, compared with the situation-awareness-global-assessment technique
(SAGAT), which reduces the intrusiveness of the task [30], it was chosen as the primary
SA-measurement method because of the purpose of this study and the complexity of VIS
presented. SART [31], a subjective measurement method of SA conducted after the exper-
iment, is a multidimensional scale, and the most widely used is the 3D-SART, which
mainly includes three dimensions: demand for attentional resources (AD), supply of at-
tentional resources (AS), and understanding of the current situation (SU). To make the
research results more reliable, we further adopted the subjective SART to assess the sub-
jects’ SA. Furthermore, visualization information is a form of visual–spatial information,
and eye tracking is an effective method for studying a person’s cognition, because it cap-
tures the vision of the individual. Eye-movement patterns reflect different information-
processing processes, among which SA-process measures have been validated in pilot
studies [32–34]. Moreover, for the framing effect, different representations may activate
different adaptors, and accordingly, different factors, such as context, may activate differ-
ent domain-specific adaptors [35]. Accordingly, this study also combined eye-tracking
techniques to measure people’s visual processing of interface information and investigate
how the presentation frame of VIS affects their attention allocation under the three-level
UI design.
Figure 2.
Three-level UI design for VIS presentation (English version). Note. This figure shows the
UIs employed in the study. The green-line box is the level-1 UI, the orange-line box is the level-2 UI,
and the red-line box is the level-3 UI.
2.2. Three-Level UI Design for VIS Presentation
Based on the three-stage-SA theory and the actual situation, a three-level UI for the
VIS presentation was designed (see Figure 2).
Level-1 UI (UI1): this level of interface presents a small number of VISs. At this level,
one gets an intuitive sense of the total number of hazards currently in the jurisdiction and
the number of hazards that have been rectified.
Level-2 UI (UI2): based on UI1, certain ratio information is added to help people
understand the status of the rectification of hidden hazards in each jurisdiction.
Level-3 UI (UI3): based on UI1 and UI2, certain probabilistic risk information is added
to help people better predict the future state of risks in each jurisdiction.
3. Methods
3.1. The Measurement of SA
3.1.1. Three-Level UI Design for VIS Presentation
Five SA-measurement methods are commonly used: subjective measures (SART),
objective measures (SAGAT and SPAM), process measures (such as eye-tracking), and
performance measures [
28
]. In this study, the situation-present-assessment method (SPAM),
situation-awareness-rating technique (SART), and eye-tracking technique were used to
measure the subjects’ level of SA in each presentation frame, based on the three-stage
SA theory. Among them, SPAM is a real-time probe technique that measures partici-
pants’ SA using online-probe questions related to the task without freezing the scenario,
which records response accuracy and latency to measure SA [
29
]. As SPAM focuses on
the ability to locate information in the situation and does not require freezing the task
performed by the subject, compared with the situation-awareness-global-assessment tech-
nique (SAGAT), which reduces the intrusiveness of the task [
30
], it was chosen as the
primary SA-measurement method because of the purpose of this study and the complexity
of VIS presented. SART [
31
], a subjective measurement method of SA conducted after
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 5 of 26
the experiment, is a multidimensional scale, and the most widely used is the 3D-SART,
which mainly includes three dimensions: demand for attentional resources (AD), supply of
attentional resources (AS), and understanding of the current situation (SU). To make the
research results more reliable, we further adopted the subjective SART to assess the subjects’
SA. Furthermore, visualization information is a form of visual–spatial information, and eye
tracking is an effective method for studying a person’s cognition, because it captures the
vision of the individual. Eye-movement patterns reflect different information-processing
processes, among which SA-process measures have been validated in pilot studies [
32
34
].
Moreover, for the framing effect, different representations may activate different adaptors,
and accordingly, different factors, such as context, may activate different domain-specific
adaptors [
35
]. Accordingly, this study also combined eye-tracking techniques to measure
people’s visual processing of interface information and investigate how the presentation
frame of VIS affects their attention allocation under the three-level UI design.
3.1.2. Measurement Material of SA
(1)
Design the Probe Questions of SPAM
The SPAM questionnaire was designed based on the study by Jones et al. [
36
] and the
context of this study, with a total of 10 items. Of these, items Q1–Q6 and Q8–Q10 were
measured by accuracy, which was obtained based on the VIS presented in the interface,
while Q7 was measured by a 7-point Likert scale, and Q10 was a ranking of the likelihood
of an accident occurring in the area (e.g., BCDAE). That is, area B is the most likely to see
an accident, followed by areas C, D, A, and E (Table 1).
Table 1. SPAM question library.
Stage of SA NO. Questions Answer
SA1
1 Which area has the highest number of hidden risks? B
2 Which area has the lowest number of hidden risks? E
3 Which area has the lowest number of hidden risks rectified? C
4 Which area has the highest number of hidden risks rectified? A
SA2
5 Which area has the lowest rectification rate of hidden risks? A
6 Which area has the highest rectification rate of hidden risks? B
7 What is the overall risk profile of the whole jurisdiction? 1 (very dangerous)–7 (very safe)
SA3
8 Which area is most likely to see a safety-risk accident? B
9 Which area is least likely to see a safety-risk accident? E
10
What is the ranking of the likelihood of a safety accident in the
jurisdiction? (Ranked from most to least prone to see a
safety-risk accident)
BCDAE
(2)
Design the Subjective Scales of SART
Three-dimensional SART scales were designed based on the studies of Taylor [
31
]
and Su et al. [
37
], containing a total of 10 questions, all measured using the 7-point Likert
scale (Table 2).
Table 2. SART scale.
Dimension NO. Sub-Dimension Items
AD
1 Degree of situation instability How stable is the context?
2 The complexity of the situation What is the complexity of the situation?
3 The degree of variability in the situation How many variables are present in the situation?
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 6 of 26
Table 2. Cont.
Dimension NO. Sub-Dimension Items
AS
4 Level of mental arousal How aroused are you in this situation?
5 Level of attention concentration To what extent are you able to focus your attention
on the situation?
6 Level of attention division Are you able to pay attention to a lot of information
at one time?
7 Level of spare capacity How much spare capacity do you have left in
the situation?
SU
8 Quantity of information obtained How much of the information in the situation are
you able to access and understand?
9 Quality of information obtained What is the quality of the information you receive
and understand?
10 Familiarity with the context How familiar are you with the situation?
Note. AD: attention demand; AS: attention supply; SU: situation understanding.
3.2. Experimental Design
3.2.1. Independent Variables
In this study, a two-factor mixed experiment was adopted, with factor 1 being the
UI level (between-subject variable), containing three levels: UI1, UI2, and UI3. Factor
2 is the presentation frame of VIS (within-subject variable) and consists of four levels:
the positive–positive frame (PP), the positive–negative frame (PN), the negative–positive
frame (NP), and the negative–negative frame (NN).
By manipulating the presentation frame of VIS at each UI level, we incorporated a
total of 10 relevant contexts into the experiment. In UI1, the presentation frame of the total
number of rectified hazards in the jurisdiction was manipulated, and only two relevant
contexts existed at this level, namely PP and NN. The influence of the framing effect is
mainly focused on the perception and comprehension stages, which can be illustrated by
the dual-system theory; that is, there are two systems of intuitive and inferential judg-
ments involved in the process of people making judgments about problems, where system
1 adopts a heuristic processing mode, similar to perception, while system 2 requires more
computation, and adopts an analytical processing mode [
38
,
39
]. Thus, further, manipu-
lating the presentation frame for the rectification rate of potential hazards in the UI2, and
with the combination of UI1, four relevant contexts exist at this level, namely PP, PN, NP,
and NN. UI3 adds the prediction module to UI2, with four relevant contexts, namely PP,
PN, NP, and NN.
3.2.2. Dependent Variable
The dependent variable in this study is the SA of subjects toward different situations,
which was comprehensively measured by objective, subjective, and process methods,
as follows.
(1) The three-stage and total scores obtained by SPAM. Based on the E-prime experimental
platform, the proportion of correct responses to the probe questions was recorded to
measure the SPAM score of SA, that is, the total SPAM score of SA = the number of
correct responses/total number of probe questions
×
100, and the stage scores (SSA1,
SSA2, SSA3) were also calculated.
(2)
The three-dimensional and total scores obtained by SART (post-test score of SA).
The subjective SART score was calculated based on the three-dimensional scores,
including the score for attention demand (SAD), the score for attention supply (SAS),
and the score for situational understanding (SSU), that is, SSA = SSU
(SAD
SAS).
(3)
Eye-movement indexes: the nearest neighbor index (NNI), the average regression
count in the area of interest (RCAOI), the average fixation count in the area of interest
(FCAOI), and the average fixation duration in the area of interest (FDAOI) were se-
lected as eye-movement indexes. Among them, the NNI-fixation index is a commonly
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 7 of 26
used clustering algorithm, based on the distance between gaze points within a region,
which is influenced by the subjects’ SA level and visual-search strategy, measuring the
spatial dispersion of their gaze points [
40
]. When NNI < 1, the points are aggregated;
when NNI = 1, the points are randomly dispersed; when NNI > 1, the points are
regularly dispersed. The gaze and look-back behavior for a visual element reflect the
attention allocation, acquisition, and storage [
34
,
41
]. The regression count reflects the
individual’s level of reprocessing of information [
42
], while the fixation count reflects
the subject’s ability to extract information during the task; the higher the number
of gaze points, the more attention the subject pays to the area and the more useful
information the subject can extract [
43
]. The average fixation duration is generally
used for coding tasks. The longer the fixation duration, the more difficult it is to ex-
tract information, the greater the cognitive load, or the more appealing the target [
44
].
Thus, the abovementioned eye-movement indicators were chosen to measure the
overall and local distribution of attention and its correlation with the level of SA in
each UI level and presentation frame.
3.2.3. Control Variables
To make the experimental situation more realistic, we added three control variables to
the three-level UI design, as relevant auxiliary information (Figure 3). It was verified that
the auxiliary control variables did not have a significant effect on the experimental data in
the following analysis, and that subjects’ attention was significantly less allocated to these
areas than to the main task areas.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 8 of 27
that the auxiliary control variables did not have a significant effect on the experimental
data in the following analysis, and that subjects’ attention was significantly less allocated
to these areas than to the main task areas.
The size and position of the VIS modules were controlled. Some studies have pro-
posed that the layout of the visualization information in the interface influences the de-
gree of information understandability and the accuracy and efficiency of people’s cogni-
tion process toward the human–machine interface, and that users are prone to pay more
attention to the information on the left and the upper area [45]. To balance the impact of
the layout of the main VIS-element presentation, we presented them at the bottom left,
top center, and top right of the figure, and the size and position of the same modules were
kept consistent across all UI levels.
The saturation of the red and green colors that represent “safe and “danger” infor-
mation in each UI level was controlled, and low saturation red and green was used to
keep the colors of the stimulus consistent and avoid the strong visual impact of high sat-
uration [17,46].
Figure 3. Control-variable modules in the UI design. Note. The yellow-line box shows the corre-
sponding control modules.
3.3. Experimental Design
Before the experiment, the specific content of the 3D-SART scale and the procedure
of this experiment were briefly introduced to the subjects, who then entered the formal
experiment. The procedures were as follows.
(1) The subject provided his/her informed consent.
(2) The subject sat in front of the screen with the eye tracker placed within an effective
range of 4575 cm, adjusted the seat and posture, and calibrated the gaze point, using
the 5-point calibration method. When the subject entered the actual experiment, the
eye-movement data were recorded simultaneously.
(3) The experiment involving a specific UI level (the between-subject variable) was ran-
domly presented to the subject, who participated in only one level of interface,
among which UI1 contained a total of two blocks, that is, two contextual frames, each
block containing 10 trials. To distribute the learning effect, we randomly presented
the contexts within the UI level, and the subject responded with a mouse click or key
press, according to the corresponding probe questions. Each trial was preceded by a
Figure 3.
Control-variable modules in the UI design. Note. The yellow-line box shows the corre-
sponding control modules.
The size and position of the VIS modules were controlled. Some studies have proposed
that the layout of the visualization information in the interface influences the degree of
information understandability and the accuracy and efficiency of people’s cognition process
toward the human–machine interface, and that users are prone to pay more attention to
the information on the left and the upper area [
45
]. To balance the impact of the layout of
the main VIS-element presentation, we presented them at the bottom left, top center, and
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 8 of 26
top right of the figure, and the size and position of the same modules were kept consistent
across all UI levels.
The saturation of the red and green colors that represent “safe and “danger information
in each UI level was controlled, and low saturation red and green was used to keep the colors
of the stimulus consistent and avoid the strong visual impact of high saturation [17,46].
3.3. Experimental Design
Before the experiment, the specific content of the 3D-SART scale and the procedure
of this experiment were briefly introduced to the subjects, who then entered the formal
experiment. The procedures were as follows.
(1)
The subject provided his/her informed consent.
(2)
The subject sat in front of the screen with the eye tracker placed within an effective
range of 45–75 cm, adjusted the seat and posture, and calibrated the gaze point, using
the 5-point calibration method. When the subject entered the actual experiment, the
eye-movement data were recorded simultaneously.
(3)
The experiment involving a specific UI level (the between-subject variable) was
randomly presented to the subject, who participated in only one level of interface,
among which UI1 contained a total of two blocks, that is, two contextual frames, each
block containing 10 trials. To distribute the learning effect, we randomly presented
the contexts within the UI level, and the subject responded with a mouse click or
key press, according to the corresponding probe questions. Each trial was preceded
by a 1000-ms gaze point, “+”, to eliminate differences in the first-fixation position
of subjects. After each situation, the subject would rest for 5 min and immediately
complete the 3D-SART scale under the guidance of the experimenter, keeping the
posture as still as possible during the process (Figure 4a). Both experiment UI2 and
experiment UI3 consisted of four blocks, and the specific procedure was similar to
that of UI1 (Figure 4b,c).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 9 of 28
1000-ms gaze point,+, to eliminate differences in the first-fixation position of sub-
jects. After each situation, the subject would rest for 5 min and immediately complete
the 3D-SART scale under the guidance of the experimenter, keeping the posture as
still as possible during the process (Figure 4a). Both experiment UI2 and experiment
UI3 consisted of four blocks, and the specific procedure was similar to that of UI1
(Figure 4b,c).
(4) At the end of the experiment, the subject was thanked and given a small gift.
(a)
Figure 4. Cont.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 9 of 26
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 10 of 28
(b)
(c)
Figure 4. Experiment procedure: (a) The procedure of experiment UI1 (contains two contexts); (b)
the procedure of experiment UI2 (contains four contexts); (c) the procedure of experiment UI3 (con-
tains four contexts).
3.4. Experimental Subjects
The sample size required for this study was calculated according to the G*power
software, and the total sample size required to predict a level of statistical power of 80%
Figure 4.
Experiment procedure: (
a
) The procedure of experiment UI1 (contains two contexts); (
b
) the
procedure of experiment UI2 (contains four contexts); (
c
) the procedure of experiment UI3 (contains
four contexts).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 10 of 26
(4)
At the end of the experiment, the subject was thanked and given a small gift.
3.4. Experimental Subjects
The sample size required for this study was calculated according to the G*power
software, and the total sample size required to predict a level of statistical power of 80%
at a significance level of
α
= 0.05 and a medium effect (f = 0.25) was obtained to be at
least 159. We recruited 166 students from Xi’an University of Science and Technology and
divided them into three groups to participate in this experiment. Based on Kihberger’s
study of the framing effect, it was found that there was no significant difference between
the findings obtained using the student population as a sample and those obtained using
social workers with actual work experience [
47
]. Before the experiment, all participants
were informed of the instructions and procedures, and provided their informed consent.
All subjects had normal or corrected vision (no myopic astigmatism) and had sufficient
sleep for the previous 24 h. The lighting conditions were kept as consistent as possible
during all experiment sessions.
3.5. Laboratory Environment and Equipment
This study was approved by the Institution Review Board of the Research Center for
Human Factors and Management Ergonomics, Xi’an University of Science and Technology,
and it was performed following relevant ethical guidelines and regulations. The experimen-
tal task was designed using the E-prime 3.0 experimental platform, and the RED-m remote
video-based eye tracker made by SensoMotoric Instruments in Teltow, Germany was used
to collect ocular movements at a sampling rate of 60 Hz, with 0.5-degree calibration pre-
cision. The experimental materials were presented on a 15.6-inch laptop (Dell Precision
M4800) with 1920
×
1080-pixel resolution, on which software was installed, including SMI
iView RED-m and Begaze 3.7. The SMI iView RED-m was used to collect eye-movement
data, which would be analyzed offline by Begaze 3.7.
3.6. Data Collection and Analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted on the behavioral-response data recorded by the
E-prime 3.0 experimental platform, the SART scale data, and the eye-movement data
recorded by SMI iView RED-m. The number of valid data points collected by the E-prime
experimental platform for each UI level was 55, 55, and 56, and after excluding eight invalid
eye-movement data points (i.e., data from eight participants with tracking ratios less than
90% and calibration accuracy higher than 1 degree were removed), we found that the
number of valid eye-movement data points collected for each UI level was 51, 53, and
54. IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0 was used for statistical analysis, and the significance level
used was
α
= 0.05. A multi-way ANOVA was used to determine the effect of the UI level
and representation frame on the dependent variable. Post hoc tests were analyzed using
Scheffé’s method, as the number of subjects varied between the UI levels [48,49].
4. Results
4.1. SPAM Measurement Results
The descriptive statistics and multi-way-ANOVA analyses were conducted using SPSS
software on the three-stage and total scores of SA obtained by SPAM between UI levels of
VIS and the presentation frames, and the results are shown in Tables 3and 4.
Table 3. The stage and total-SA scores obtained by SPAM.
UI Level Presentation
Frame
SSA1 SSA2 SSA3 SPAM Number of
Subjects
Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Mean ±SD
UI1
PP 38.36 ±4.200 13.82 ±5.608 18.00 ±5.578 77.98 ±9.440 55
NN 30.55 ±7.798 12.36 ±6.929 15.27 ±7.163 64.65 ±12.667 55
Total 34.45 ±7.368 13.09 ±6.317 16.64 ±6.535 71.31 ±12.980 110
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 11 of 26
Table 3. Cont.
UI Level Presentation
Frame
SSA1 SSA2 SSA3 SPAM Number of
Subjects
Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Mean ±SD
UI2
PP 37.45 ±5.170 18.55 ±3.558 14.91 ±5.733 78.79 ±7.790 55
PN 35.64 ±5.362 18.91 ±3.146 14.73 ±5.039 76.97 ±8.241 55
NP 30.91 ±8.665 18.55 ±4.045 17.27 ±4.889 74.14 ±12.478 55
NN 30.00 ±7.454 17.45 ±5.517 14.36 ±6.013 68.69 ±11.334 55
Total 33.50 ±7.465 18.36 ±4.171 15.32 ±5.522 74.65 ±10.790 220
UI3
PP 37.32 ±5.219 18.21 ±3.865 26.07 ±5.284 90.68 ±9.422 56
PN 35.00 ±7.628 18.04 ±4.439 23.57 ±6.723 85.12 ±11.849 56
NP 30.54 ±6.444 18.93 ±3.121 23.21 ±8.551 80.76 ±13.314 56
NN 30.18 ±8.200 15.71 ±6.566 25.00 ±7.385 78.77 ±14.413 56
Total 33.26 ±7.553 17.72 ±4.800 24.46 ±7.130 83.83 ±13.132 224
Total
PP 37.71 ±4.880 16.87 ±4.906 19.70 ±7.255 82.53 ±10.616 166
PN 35.32 ±6.581 18.47 ±3.861 19.19 ±7.402 81.08 ±10.968 111
NP 30.72 ±7.593 18.74 ±3.597 20.27 ±7.563 77.48 ±13.270 111
NN 30.24 ±7.781 15.18 ±6.672 18.25 ±8.382 70.75 ±14.123 166
Note. SSA1: the stage score of SA1; SSA2: the stage score of SA2; SSA3: the stage score of SA3; SPAM: total score of
SA; PP: positive–positive frame; PN: positive–negative frame; NP: negative–positive frame; NN negative–negative
frame; SD: standard deviation.
Table 4. Multi-way-ANOVA-analysis results of each stage and the total score of SPAM.
Source Dependent
Variable
Type III Sum of
Squares df Mean
Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
UI
SSA1 44.263 2 22.131 0.482 0.618 0.002
SSA2 1461.981 2 730.991 30.949 <0.001 0.102
SSA3 10,180.866 2 5090.433 126.546 <0.001 0.318
SPAM 14,016.264 2 7008.132 54.820 <0.001 0.168
Frame
SSA1 5862.853 3 1954.284 42.580 <0.001 0.190
SSA2 378.501 3 126.167 5.342 0.001 0.029
SSA3 257.312 3 85.771 2.132 0.095 0.012
SPAM 12,233.303 3 4077.768 31.898 <0.001 0.150
UI ×Frame
SSA1 15.020 4 3.755 0.082 0.988 0.001
SSA2 69.011 4 17.253 0.730 0.571 0.005
SSA3 529.437 4 132.359 3.290 0.011 0.024
SPAM 537.428 4 134.357 1.051 0.380 0.008
Note: SSA1: the stage score of SA1; SSA2: the stage score of SA2; SSA3: the stage score of SA3; SPAM: total score
of SA.
4.1.1. Differences between UI Levels
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the three-stage SA theory, and the overall
scores of SA between UI levels were obtained. It was found that the stage score of perception
(SSA1) was not significantly different (F (2, 544) = 0.482, p= 0.618,
η2
p
= 0.002), the overall
mean of which showed a trend of UI1 > UI2 > UI3. The stage score of comprehension
(SSA2) was significantly different (F (2, 544) = 30.949, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.102), and a post
hoc test revealed that the SSA2 of UI2 and UI3 were significantly greater than that of UI1
(p< 0.001), with the overall mean showing a trend of UI2 > UI3 > UI1. The stage score of
projection (SSA3) was significantly different (F (2, 544) = 126.546, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.318),
and a post hoc test revealed that the SSA3 of UI1 was significantly greater than that of UI2
(p= 0.012 < 0.05) and that the SSA3 of UI3 was significantly greater than that of UI1 and
UI2 (p< 0.001), with the overall mean showing a trend of UI3 > UI1 > UI2. The total SPAM
score was significantly different (F (2, 544) = 54.820, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.168), and the post hoc
test revealed that the total score of UI3 was significantly greater than that of UI1 and UI2
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 12 of 26
(p< 0.001), that the difference between UI1 and UI2 was significant (p= 0.042 < 0.05), and
that the mean value of the total SPAM score was ranked UI3 > UI2 > UI1 (Figure 5).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 12 of 27
SPAM
3
4077.768
31.898
<0.001
0.150
UI × Frame
SSA1
4
3.755
0.082
0.988
0.001
SSA2
4
17.253
0.730
0.571
0.005
SSA3
4
132.359
3.290
0.011
0.024
SPAM
4
134.357
1.051
0.380
0.008
Note: SSA1: the stage score of SA1; SSA2: the stage score of SA2; SSA3: the stage score of SA3; SPAM:
total score of SA.
4.1.1. Differences between UI Levels
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the three-stage SA theory, and the overall
scores of SA between UI levels were obtained. It was found that the stage score of percep-
tion (SSA1) was not significantly different (F (2, 544) = 0.482, p = 0.618, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.002), the
overall mean of which showed a trend of UI1 > UI2 > UI3. The stage score of comprehen-
sion (SSA2) was significantly different (F (2, 544) = 30.949, p < 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.102), and a
post hoc test revealed that the SSA2 of UI2 and UI3 were significantly greater than that of
UI1 (p < 0.001), with the overall mean showing a trend of UI2 > UI3 > UI1. The stage score
of projection (SSA3) was significantly different (F (2, 544) = 126.546, p < 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.318),
and a post hoc test revealed that the SSA3 of UI1 was significantly greater than that of UI2
(p = 0.012 < 0.05) and that the SSA3 of UI3 was significantly greater than that of UI1 and
UI2 (p < 0.001), with the overall mean showing a trend of UI3 > UI1 > UI2. The total SPAM
score was significantly different (F (2, 544) = 54.820, p < 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.168), and the post
hoc test revealed that the total score of UI3 was significantly greater than that of UI1 and
UI2 (p < 0.001), that the difference between UI1 and UI2 was significant (p = 0.042 < 0.05),
and that the mean value of the total SPAM score was ranked UI3 > UI2 > UI1 (Figure 5).
Figure 5. The stage and total scores of SA obtained by SPAM for each UI level. Note. SSA: the stage
score of SA; SPAM: total score of SA; SA1: perception stage; SA2: comprehension stage; SA3: pro-
jection stage; Error bar: standard error.
4.1.2. Differences between Presentation Frames
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted for three-stage and total SA scores under each
presentation frame, and it was found that the differences in the perception stage score
(SSA1) were significant (F (3, 544) = 42.580, p < 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.190), with post hoc tests find-
ing significant differences between the frames except between the NP and NN frames,
with the overall mean presented as PP > PN > NP > NN. The difference in the comprehen-
sion stage score (SSA2) was significant (F (3, 544) = 5.342, p = 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.029); post hoc
tests found the SSA2 of NN frame significantly smaller than that of PP, PN, and NP frames
(p < 0.001, p = 0.019 < 0.05); and the overall mean was NP > PN > PP > NN. By contrast,
people scored higher SSA2 in the comprehension stage under a combination of positive
Figure 5.
The stage and total scores of SA obtained by SPAM for each UI level. Note. SSA: the
stage score of SA; SPAM: total score of SA; SA1: perception stage; SA2: comprehension stage; SA3:
projection stage; Error bar: standard error.
4.1.2. Differences between Presentation Frames
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted for three-stage and total SA scores under each
presentation frame, and it was found that the differences in the perception stage score
(SSA1) were significant (F (3, 544) = 42.580, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.190), with post hoc tests finding
significant differences between the frames except between the NP and NN frames, with
the overall mean presented as PP > PN > NP > NN. The difference in the comprehension
stage score (SSA2) was significant (F (3, 544) = 5.342, p= 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.029); post hoc tests
found the SSA2 of NN frame significantly smaller than that of PP, PN, and NP frames
(p< 0.001, p= 0.019 < 0.05); and the overall mean was NP > PN > PP > NN. By contrast,
people scored higher SSA2 in the comprehension stage under a combination of positive and
negative frames. For the projection stage score (SSA3), the differences were not significant
across presentation frames (F (3, 544) = 2.132, p= 0.095,
η2
p
= 0.012), with the overall mean
presented as NP > PP > PN > NN. For the total SPAM score, the differences were significant
(F (3, 544) = 31.898, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.150), and post hoc tests revealed that there were
significant differences between the frames except for the PP and PN frames, NP frame, and
PN frame, with the overall mean presented as PP > PN > NP > NN (Figure 6).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 13 of 27
and negative frames. For the projection stage score (SSA3), the differences were not sig-
nificant across presentation frames (F (3, 544) = 2.132, p = 0.095, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.012), with the over-
all mean presented as NP > PP > PN > NN. For the total SPAM score, the differences were
significant (F (3, 544) = 31.898, p < 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.150), and post hoc tests revealed that there
were significant differences between the frames except for the PP and PN frames, NP
frame, and PN frame, with the overall mean presented as PP > PN > NP > NN (Figure 6).
Figure 6. The stage and total scores of SA obtained by SPAM for each presentation frame. Note. PP:
positivepositive frame; PN: positivenegative frame; NP: negativepositive frame; NN: negative
negative frame; SSA: the stage score of SA; SPAM: total score of SA; Error bar: standard error.
4.2. SART-Measurement Results
The descriptive statistic and multi-way-ANOVA analysis were conducted by SPSS
software on the three-dimensional and total scores of SA obtained by SART between UI
levels of VIS and the presentation frames, and the results are shown in Tables 5 and 6.
Table 5. The dimension and the total scores obtained by SART.
UI Level
Presentation Frame
SAD
SAS
SSU
SART
Number of
Subjects
Mean ± SD
Mean ± SD
Mean ± SD
Mean ± SD
UI1
PP
7.62 ± 3.088
19.73 ± 3.274
14.53 ± 2.899
26.64 ± 6.873
55
NN
9.18 ± 3.074
18.91 ± 3.540
13.89 ± 3.077
23.62 ± 7.230
55
Total
8.40 ± 3.166
19.32 ± 3.419
14.21 ± 2.993
25.13 ± 7.183
110
UI2
PP
8.91 ± 3.032
20.18 ± 3.394
15.80 ± 2.288
27.07 ± 6.675
55
PN
9.15 ± 2.752
19.95 ± 3.223
15.69 ± 2.559
26.49 ± 6.330
55
NP
9.67 ± 3.163
19.05 ± 3.498
14.51 ± 2.538
23.89 ± 6.554
55
NN
9.89 ± 3.298
19.15 ± 4.093
15.00 ± 2.912
24.25 ± 8.168
55
Total
9.40 ± 3.072
19.58 ± 3.577
15.25 ± 2.620
25.43 ± 7.057
220
UI3
PP
9.68 ± 3.197
20.45 ± 3.291
16.04 ± 2.508
26.80 ± 6.274
56
PN
9.91 ± 3.492
19.86 ± 3.170
15.27 ± 2.408
25.21 ± 6.851
56
NP
10.43 ± 3.230
19.41 ± 3.085
15.02 ± 2.378
24.00 ± 6.090
56
NN
10.73 ± 3.539
19.95 ± 3.993
15.71 ± 2.455
24.93 ± 7.221
56
Total
10.19 ± 3.371
19.92 ± 3.401
15.51 ± 2.453
25.24 ± 6.657
224
Total
PP
8.74 ± 3.204
20.12 ± 3.314
15.46 ± 2.646
26.84 ± 6.572
166
PN
9.53 ± 3.156
19.90 ± 3.182
15.48 ± 2.482
25.85 ± 6.599
111
NP
10.05 ± 3.205
19.23 ± 3.286
14.77 ± 2.460
23.95 ± 6.296
111
NN
9.94 ± 3.352
19.34 ± 3.886
14.87 ± 2.907
24.27 ± 7.524
166
Note. SAD: the dimension score of attention demand; SAS: the dimension score of attention supply;
SSU: the dimension score of situational understanding; SART: total score of SA; PP: positiveposi-
tive frame; PN: positivenegative frame; NP: negativepositive frame; NN: negativenegative
frame; SD: standard deviation.
Figure 6.
The stage and total scores of SA obtained by SPAM for each presentation frame. Note. PP:
positive–positive frame; PN: positive–negative frame; NP: negative–positive frame; NN: negative–negative
frame; SSA: the stage score of SA; SPAM: total score of SA; Error bar: standard error.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 13 of 26
4.2. SART-Measurement Results
The descriptive statistic and multi-way-ANOVA analysis were conducted by SPSS
software on the three-dimensional and total scores of SA obtained by SART between UI
levels of VIS and the presentation frames, and the results are shown in Tables 5and 6.
Table 5. The dimension and the total scores obtained by SART.
UI Level Presentation
Frame
SAD SAS SSU SART Number of
Subjects
Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Mean ±SD
UI1
PP 7.62 ±3.088 19.73 ±3.274 14.53 ±2.899 26.64 ±6.873 55
NN 9.18 ±3.074 18.91 ±3.540 13.89 ±3.077 23.62 ±7.230 55
Total 8.40 ±3.166 19.32 ±3.419 14.21 ±2.993 25.13 ±7.183 110
UI2
PP 8.91 ±3.032 20.18 ±3.394 15.80 ±2.288 27.07 ±6.675 55
PN 9.15 ±2.752 19.95 ±3.223 15.69 ±2.559 26.49 ±6.330 55
NP 9.67 ±3.163 19.05 ±3.498 14.51 ±2.538 23.89 ±6.554 55
NN 9.89 ±3.298 19.15 ±4.093 15.00 ±2.912 24.25 ±8.168 55
Total 9.40 ±3.072 19.58 ±3.577 15.25 ±2.620 25.43 ±7.057 220
UI3
PP 9.68 ±3.197 20.45 ±3.291 16.04 ±2.508 26.80 ±6.274 56
PN 9.91 ±3.492 19.86 ±3.170 15.27 ±2.408 25.21 ±6.851 56
NP 10.43 ±3.230 19.41 ±3.085 15.02 ±2.378 24.00 ±6.090 56
NN 10.73 ±3.539 19.95 ±3.993 15.71 ±2.455 24.93 ±7.221 56
Total 10.19 ±3.371 19.92 ±3.401 15.51 ±2.453 25.24 ±6.657 224
Total
PP 8.74 ±3.204 20.12 ±3.314 15.46 ±2.646 26.84 ±6.572 166
PN 9.53 ±3.156 19.90 ±3.182 15.48 ±2.482 25.85 ±6.599 111
NP 10.05 ±3.205 19.23 ±3.286 14.77 ±2.460 23.95 ±6.296 111
NN 9.94 ±3.352 19.34 ±3.886 14.87 ±2.907 24.27 ±7.524 166
Note. SAD: the dimension score of attention demand; SAS: the dimension score of attention supply; SSU:
the dimension score of situational understanding; SART: total score of SA; PP: positive–positive frame; PN:
positive–negative frame; NP: negative–positive frame; NN: negative–negative frame; SD: standard deviation.
Table 6. Muti-way-ANOVA-analysis results of each dimension and the total score of SART.
Source Dependent
Variable
Type III Sum of
Squares df Mean
Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
UI
SAD 213.485 2 106.742 10.456 <0.001 0.037
SAS 40.005 2 20.003 1.660 0.191 0.006
SSU 157.761 2 78.880 11.552 <0.001 0.041
SART 33.855 2 16.927 0.361 0.697 0.001
Frame
SAD 134.621 3 44.874 4.396 0.005 0.024
SAS 90.567 3 30.189 2.506 0.058 0.014
SSU 86.496 3 28.832 4.222 0.006 0.023
SART 833.414 3 277.805 5.922 0.001 0.032
UI ×Frame
SAD 5.603 4 1.401 0.137 0.969 0.001
SAS 11.197 4 2.799 0.232 0.920 0.002
SSU 20.517 4 5.129 0.751 0.558 0.005
SART 64.533 4 16.133 0.344 0.848 0.003
Note. SAD: the dimension score of attention demand; SAS: the dimension score of attention supply; SSU: the
dimension score of situational understanding; SART: total score of SA.
4.2.1. Differences between UI Levels
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on each dimension, and total scores were ob-
tained by SART among UI levels. The results show that the dimension scores of AD
and SU (SAD and SSU) were significantly different among UI levels (F (2, 544) = 10.456,
p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.037; F (2, 544) = 11.552, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.041). Through a post hoc compara-
tive analysis, it was found that UI3 was significantly greater than UI2 and UI1 in the SAD
(p= 0.036 < 0.05, p< 0.001) and that there were significant differences between UI1 and UI2
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 14 of 26
(p= 0.027 < 0.05), with UI2 obtaining a higher score. It can be seen from the mean value that
the SAD increased with the UI level, that is, UI3 > UI2 > UI1; for the SU dimension, the SSU
of UI3 and UI2 were significantly greater than that of UI1 (p< 0.001, p= 0.003 < 0.05), and
subjects’ acquisition and understanding of the information showed a trend of increasing with
UI levels. For the dimension scores of AS (SAS), the differences were not significant between UI
levels (F (2, 544) = 1.660, p= 0.191,
η2
p
= 0.006), and the mean value was UI2 > UI3 > UI1. There
was no significant difference in the total score of SART (F (2, 544) = 0.361, p= 0.697,
η2
p
= 0.001),
but the overall mean showed UI2 > UI3 > UI1 (Figure 7).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 14 of 27
Table 6. Muti-way-ANOVA-analysis results of each dimension and the total score of SART.
Source
Dependent Variable
Type III Sum of Squares
df
Mean Square
F
Sig.
Partial Eta Squared
UI
SAD
213.485
2
106.742
10.456
<0.001
0.037
SAS
40.005
2
20.003
1.660
0.191
0.006
SSU
157.761
2
78.880
11.552
<0.001
0.041
SART
33.855
2
16.927
0.361
0.697
0.001
Frame
SAD
134.621
3
44.874
4.396
0.005
0.024
SAS
90.567
3
30.189
2.506
0.058
0.014
SSU
86.496
3
28.832
4.222
0.006
0.023
SART
833.414
3
277.805
5.922
0.001
0.032
UI × Frame
SAD
5.603
4
1.401
0.137
0.969
0.001
SAS
11.197
4
2.799
0.232
0.920
0.002
SSU
20.517
4
5.129
0.751
0.558
0.005
SART
64.533
4
16.133
0.344
0.848
0.003
Note. SAD: the dimension score of attention demand; SAS: the dimension score of attention supply;
SSU: the dimension score of situational understanding; SART: total score of SA.
4.2.1. Differences between UI Levels.
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on each dimension, and total scores were ob-
tained by SART among UI levels. The results show that the dimension scores of AD and
SU (SAD and SSU) were significantly different among UI levels (F (2, 544) = 10.456, p <
0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.037; F (2, 544) = 11.552, p < 0.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.041). Through a post hoc compara-
tive analysis, it was found that UI3 was significantly greater than UI2 and UI1 in the SAD
(p = 0.036 < 0.05, p < 0.001) and that there were significant differences between UI1 and UI2
(p = 0.027 < 0.05), with UI2 obtaining a higher score. It can be seen from the mean value
that the SAD increased with the UI level, that is, UI3 > UI2 > UI1; for the SU dimension,
the SSU of UI3 and UI2 were significantly greater than that of UI1 (p < 0.001, p = 0.003 <
0.05), and subjects acquisition and understanding of the information showed a trend of
increasing with UI levels. For the dimension scores of AS (SAS), the differences were not
significant between UI levels (F (2, 544) = 1.660, p = 0.191, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.006), and the mean value
was UI2 > UI3 > UI1. There was no significant difference in the total score of SART (F (2,
544) = 0.361, p = 0.697, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.001), but the overall mean showed UI2 > UI3 > UI1 (Figure
7).
Figure 7. The dimension and total scores obtained by SART for each UI level. Note. Score: the di-
mension score of SA; SART: total score of SA; AD: attention demand dimension; AS: attention sup-
ply dimension; SU: situation understanding dimension; Error bar: standard error.
Figure 7.
The dimension and total scores obtained by SART for each UI level. Note. Score: the
dimension score of SA; SART: total score of SA; AD: attention demand dimension; AS: attention
supply dimension; SU: situation understanding dimension; Error bar: standard error.
4.2.2. Differences between Presentation Frames
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the dimension and total scores obtained by
SART among the representation frames, and the results show significant differences in the
AD dimension scores (F (3, 544) = 4.396, p= 0.005,
η2
p
= 0.024), with post hoc tests finding
that the SAD of the PP frame was significantly smaller than NP and NN (p= 0.011 < 0.05,
p= 0.009 < 0.05) and that the overall mean was presented as NP > NN > PN > PP; in the AS
dimension, the differences were only marginally significant (F (3, 544) = 2.506, p= 0.058,
η2
p
= 0.014), and the mean values showed PP > PN > NN > NP. The differences between the
SU dimension did not differ significantly across the presentation frames (F (3, 544) = 4.222,
p= 0.006,
η2
p
= 0.023), but the mean values were presented as PN > PP > NP > NN for the
SSU. Moreover, the total SART score differed significantly across the presentation frames
(F (3, 544) = 5.922, p= 0.001 < 0.05,
η2
p
= 0.032), and post hoc tests found that PP was
significantly greater than NP and NN (p= 0.008 < 0.05, p= 0.09 < 0.05); the overall mean
value was presented as PP > PN > NN > NP (Figure 8). The subjective SA scores of subjects
toward a certain situation differed under different combinations of the representation
frames of VIS. When the frames were guided by the positive frame, the highest subjective
SA score was obtained, and when the frames were guided by the negative frame, the lowest
subjective score was obtained.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 15 of 26
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 15 of 27
4.2.2. Differences between Presentation Frames
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the dimension and total scores obtained by
SART among the representation frames, and the results show significant differences in the
AD dimension scores (F (3, 544) = 4.396, p = 0.005, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.024), with post hoc tests finding
that the SAD of the PP frame was significantly smaller than NP and NN (p = 0.011 < 0.05,
p = 0.009 < 0.05) and that the overall mean was presented as NP > NN > PN > PP; in the AS
dimension, the differences were only marginally significant (F (3, 544) = 2.506, p = 0.058,
𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.014), and the mean values showed PP > PN > NN > NP. The differences between
the SU dimension did not differ significantly across the presentation frames (F (3, 544) =
4.222, p = 0.006, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.023), but the mean values were presented as PN > PP > NP > NN
for the SSU. Moreover, the total SART score differed significantly across the presentation
frames (F (3, 544) = 5.922, p = 0.001 < 0.05, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.032), and post hoc tests found that PP
was significantly greater than NP and NN (p = 0.008 < 0.05, p = 0.09 < 0.05); the overall
mean value was presented as PP > PN > NN > NP (Figure 8). The subjective SA scores of
subjects toward a certain situation differed under different combinations of the represen-
tation frames of VIS. When the frames were guided by the positive frame, the highest
subjective SA score was obtained, and when the frames were guided by the negative
frame, the lowest subjective score was obtained.
Figure 8. The dimension and total scores obtained by SART for each presentation frame. Note. Score:
the dimension score of SA; SART: total score of SA; AD: attention-demand dimension; AS: attention-
supply dimension; SU: situation-understanding dimension; PP: positivepositive frame; PN: posi-
tivenegative frame; NP: negativepositive frame; NN: negativenegative frame; Error bar: stand-
ard error.
4.3. Overall-Risk Perception
To determine whether there are differences in subjects perceptions of risk under dif-
ferent representation frames, we conducted further statistical analysis on subjects scores
on Q7 in each context (as shown in Table 7). A 7-point Likert scale was used to measure
subjects overall-risk perception, with higher scores representing perceptions that the ju-
risdiction is safer and lower scores representing perceptions that the jurisdiction is more
dangerous.
Figure 8.
The dimension and total scores obtained by SART for each presentation frame. Note.
Score: the dimension score of SA; SART: total score of SA; AD: attention-demand dimension; AS:
attention-supply dimension; SU: situation-understanding dimension; PP: positive–positive frame;
PN: positive–negative frame; NP: negative–positive frame; NN: negative–negative frame; Error bar:
standard error.
4.3. Overall-Risk Perception
To determine whether there are differences in subjects’ perceptions of risk under
different representation frames, we conducted further statistical analysis on subjects’ scores
on Q7 in each context (as shown in Table 7). A 7-point Likert scale was used to measure
subjects’ overall-risk perception, with higher scores representing perceptions that the
jurisdiction is safer and lower scores representing perceptions that the jurisdiction is
more dangerous.
Table 7. Tests of between-subjects effects. Dependent Variable: Q7.
Source Type III Sum
of Squares df Mean
Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
UI 0.422 2 0.211 0.297 0.743 0.001
Frame 32.340 3 10.780 15.201 <0.001 0.077
UI ×Frame 8.061 4 2.015 2.841 0.024 0.020
A multi-way ANOVA on the Q7 scores revealed that the overall perception of risk
did not differ significantly among UI levels (F (2, 544) = 0.297, p= 0.743,
η2
p
= 0.001), with
the mean values showing a trend of UI2 > UI1 > UI3. However, the difference between
presentation frames was significant (F (3, 544) = 15.201, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.077), and a post
hoc test revealed that the Q7 score of the PP frame was significantly greater than that of
PN and NN (p= 0.021 < 0.05, p< 0.001), that NP was significantly greater than that of NN
(p< 0.001), and that the mean score of Q7 was generally presented as PP > NP > PN > NN,
with a higher Q7 score representing less perceived risk. The interactions between UI and
frame were significant (F (4, 544) = 2.841, p= 0.024,
η2
p
= 0.020), and further simple-effects
analysis revealed that the difference in perceived risk across presentation frames within
UI1 was not significant (F (1, 544) = 2.166, p= 0.142 > 0.05,
η2
p
= 0.004), that the difference
in perceived risk within UI2 was significant (F (3, 544) = 13.374, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.069),
and that the Q7 score of the PP frame was significantly greater than that of PN and NN
(p= 0.003 < 0.05, p< 0.001). The Q7 score of the NP frame was significantly greater than that
of the NN frame (p< 0.001); the difference in perceived risk across representation frames
within UI3 was significant (F (3, 544) = 4.885, p= 0.002 < 0.05,
η2
p
= 0.026), and the Q7 score
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 16 of 26
of the PP and NP frame was significantly greater than that of the NN frame (p< 0.001,
p= 0.043 < 0.05) (Figure 9).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 16 of 27
Table 7. Tests of between-subjects effects. Dependent Variable: Q7
Source Type III Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
UI 0.422 2 0.211 0.297 0.743 0.001
Frame 32.340 3 10.780 15.201 <0.001 0.077
UI × Frame 8.061 4 2.015 2.841 0.024 0.020
A multi-way ANOVA on the Q7 scores revealed that the overall perception of risk
did not differ significantly among UI levels (F (2, 544) = 0.297, p = 0.743, 𝜂
= 0.001), with
the mean values showing a trend of UI2 > UI1 > UI3. However, the difference between
presentation frames was significant (F (3, 544) = 15.201, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.077), and a post
hoc test revealed that the Q7 score of the PP frame was significantly greater than that of
PN and NN (p = 0.021 < 0.05, p < 0.001), that NP was significantly greater than that of NN
(p < 0.001), and that the mean score of Q7 was generally presented as PP > NP > PN > NN,
with a higher Q7 score representing less perceived risk. The interactions between UI and
frame were significant (F (4, 544) = 2.841, p = 0.024, 𝜂
= 0.020), and further simple-effects
analysis revealed that the difference in perceived risk across presentation frames within
UI1 was not significant (F (1, 544) = 2.166, p = 0.142 > 0.05, 𝜂
= 0.004), that the difference
in perceived risk within UI2 was significant (F (3, 544) = 13.374, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.069), and
that the Q7 score of the PP frame was significantly greater than that of PN and NN (p =
0.003 < 0.05, p < 0.001). The Q7 score of the NP frame was significantly greater than that of
the NN frame (p < 0.001); the difference in perceived risk across representation frames
within UI3 was significant (F (3, 544) = 4.885, p = 0.002 < 0.05, 𝜂
= 0.026), and the Q7 score
of the PP and NP frame was significantly greater than that of the NN frame (p < 0.001, p =
0.043 < 0.05) (Figure 9).
Figure 9. The interactive effects of Q7 score between UI-levels and presentation frames. Note. Non-
estimable means are not plotted, so UI1 is represented by a dotted line: UI1 contains a small amount
of VIS, and only two presentation frames exist in this interface, a positive frame (PP) and a negative
frame (NN), with no combined frames (PN and NP); the colored bar represents the subject’s per-
ceived level of risk (green = larger Q7 score and less perceived risk, red = smaller Q7 scores and
more perceived risk).
4.4. Analysis of Eye-Movement Indicators
4.4.1. Overall Distribution of Attention
The valid eye-movement data of the subjects were imported into MATLAB R2022a,
and a simple tool for examining fixations (ASTEF) developed by the laboratory of Nocera
Figure 9.
The interactive effects of Q7 score between UI-levels and presentation frames. Note.
Non-estimable means are not plotted, so UI1 is represented by a dotted line: UI1 contains a small
amount of VIS, and only two presentation frames exist in this interface, a positive frame (PP) and a
negative frame (NN), with no combined frames (PN and NP); the colored bar represents the subject’s
perceived level of risk (green = larger Q7 score and less perceived risk, red = smaller Q7 scores and
more perceived risk).
4.4. Analysis of Eye-Movement Indicators
4.4.1. Overall Distribution of Attention
The valid eye-movement data of the subjects were imported into MATLAB R2022a, and
a simple tool for examining fixations (ASTEF) developed by the laboratory of Nocera et al.
was run [
50
]. Based on the spatial-statistical algorithm [
51
], the NNI was calculated to check
the average dispersion of subjects’ gaze points in each context; the closer the mean value of
NNI to 1, the more discrete the distribution of gaze points. A multi-way-ANOVA analysis
was conducted on the NNI, and the results are shown in Table 8.
Table 8. Tests of between-subjects effects. Dependent Variable: NNI.
Source Type III Sum of
Squares df Mean
Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
UI 2.215 2 1.107 76.549 <0.001 0.227
Frame 1.136 3 0.379 26.188 <0.001 0.131
UI ×Frame 0.394 4 0.099 6.812 <0.001 0.050
The descriptive statistics showed that the mean values of subjects’ NNI fixation
indexes were all less than 1, meaning that the subjects’ gaze points had an overall clustered
distribution. A multi-factor ANOVA was conducted on the NNI for each UI level and
presentation frame (Table 8), and it was found that the main effects were significant
(F (2, 520) = 76.549, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.227; F (3, 520) = 26.188, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.131). Then,
further post hoc tests and simple-effects analyses were carried out to obtain the following
results: comparing the NNI of each UI level, the NNI of UI1 was significantly smaller than
that of UI2 and UI3 (p< 0.001); the NNI of UI2 was significantly smaller than that of UI3
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 17 of 26
(p< 0.001), with the mean value specifically reflected as UI3 > UI2 > UI1. The NNI of the
PP and PN frames was significantly larger than that of the NP and NN frames (p< 0.001),
with the mean value specifically reflected as PN > PP > NP > NN. The interaction effect
between each UI level and presentation frame was significant (F (4, 520) = 6.812, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.050), as evidenced by the decreased NNI index from the positive guided frame to
the negative frame within each UI level (Figure 10). The correlation between the NNI and
the total SA score was further tested, and the results are shown in Table 9. It is shown
that the Pearson correlation coefficient between NNI and SPAM score is r = 0.447 > 0.4
(p< 0.001), showing a significant moderate positive correlation; the Pearson correlation
coefficient between NNI and SART score is r = 0.225 > 0.2 (p< 0.001), showing a significant
low positive correlation (Figure 11).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 17 of 27
et al. was run [50]. Based on the spatial-statistical algorithm [51], the NNI was calculated
to check the average dispersion of subjects’ gaze points in each context; the closer the mean
value of NNI to 1, the more discrete the distribution of gaze points. A multi-way-ANOVA
analysis was conducted on the NNI, and the results are shown in Table 8.
Table 8. Tests of between-subjects effects. Dependent Variable: NNI
Source Type III Sum
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
UI 2.215 2 1.107 76.549 <0.001 0.227
Frame 1.136 3 0.379 26.188 <0.001 0.131
UI × Frame 0.394 4 0.099 6.812 <0.001 0.050
The descriptive statistics showed that the mean values of subjects NNI fixation in-
dexes were all less than 1, meaning that the subjects gaze points had an overall clustered
distribution. A multi-factor ANOVA was conducted on the NNI for each UI level and
presentation frame (Table 8), and it was found that the main effects were significant (F (2,
520) = 76.549, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.227; F (3, 520) = 26.188, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.131). Then, further
post hoc tests and simple-effects analyses were carried out to obtain the following results:
comparing the NNI of each UI level, the NNI of UI1 was significantly smaller than that of
UI2 and UI3 (p < 0.001); the NNI of UI2 was significantly smaller than that of UI3 (p <
0.001), with the mean value specifically reflected as UI3 > UI2 > UI1. The NNI of the PP
and PN frames was significantly larger than that of the NP and NN frames (p < 0.001),
with the mean value specifically reflected as PN > PP > NP > NN. The interaction effect
between each UI level and presentation frame was significant (F (4, 520) = 6.812, p < 0.001,
𝜂
= 0.050), as evidenced by the decreased NNI index from the positive guided frame to
the negative frame within each UI level (Figure 10). The correlation between the NNI and
the total SA score was further tested, and the results are shown in Table 9. It is shown that
the Pearson correlation coefficient between NNI and SPAM score is r = 0.447 > 0.4 (p <
0.001), showing a significant moderate positive correlation; the Pearson correlation coef-
ficient between NNI and SART score is r = 0.225 > 0.2 (p < 0.001), showing a significant low
positive correlation (Figure 11).
Figure 10. Interaction effects of NNI across UI levels and presentation frames. Note. Non-estimable
means are not plotted, so UI1 is represented by a dotted line: UI1 contains a small amount of VIS,
and only two presentation frames exist in this interface, a positive frame (PP) and a negative frame
(NN), with no combined frames (PN and NP).
Figure 10.
Interaction effects of NNI across UI levels and presentation frames. Note. Non-estimable
means are not plotted, so UI1 is represented by a dotted line: UI1 contains a small amount of VIS,
and only two presentation frames exist in this interface, a positive frame (PP) and a negative frame
(NN), with no combined frames (PN and NP).
Table 9. NNI and SPAM correlation table.
NNI SPAM SART
NNI
Pearson Correlation 1 0.447 ** 0.270 **
Sig. (2-tailed) <0.001 < 0.001
N 530 530 530
SPAM
Pearson Correlation 0.447 ** 1 0.225 **
Sig. (2-tailed) <0.001 < 0.001
N 530 530 530
SART
Pearson Correlation 0.270 ** 0.225 ** 1
Sig. (2-tailed) <0.001 <0.001
N 530 530 530
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 18 of 26
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 18 of 27
Table 9. NNI and SPAM correlation table.
NNI SPAM SART
NNI
Pearson Correlation 1 0.447 ** 0.270 **
Sig. (2-tailed) <0.001 < 0.001
N 530 530 530
SPAM
Pearson Correlation 0.447 ** 1 0.225 **
Sig. (2-tailed) <0.001 < 0.001
N 530 530 530
SART
Pearson Correlation 0.270 ** 0.225 ** 1
Sig. (2-tailed) <0.001 <0.001
N 530 530 530
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
(a) (b)
Figure 11. Correlation between NNI and SA scores obtained by SPAM and SART: (a) among UI
levels; (b) among presentation frames.
4.4.2. Partial Distribution of Attention
There were seven divided areas of interest (AOIs), where AOI1 is the main percep-
tion module, AOI2 is the comprehension module, and AOI3 is the projection module;
these are the main research modules of this paper. By contrast, AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6 are
the control-variable modules of this paper, and AOI7 is the module for the presentation
of SPAM probe questions, as shown in Figure 12.
Figure 11.
Correlation between NNI and SA scores obtained by SPAM and SART: (
a
) among UI levels;
(b) among presentation frames.
4.4.2. Partial Distribution of Attention
There were seven divided areas of interest (AOIs), where AOI1 is the main perception
module, AOI2 is the comprehension module, and AOI3 is the projection module; these
are the main research modules of this paper. By contrast, AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6 are the
control-variable modules of this paper, and AOI7 is the module for the presentation of
SPAM probe questions, as shown in Figure 12.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 19 of 27
Figure 12. Interface AOI division.
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the average regression count (RCAOI), the
percentage of fixation count (FCAOI), and the average fixation duration (FDAOI) in the
AOI as dependent variables, and on the AOI and the three-stage score of SA as independ-
ent variables. A post hoc test revealed that for the RCAOI, FCAOI was significantly
greater in AOI1, AOI2, AOI3, and AOI7 than in AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6, and to some ex-
tent, the use of AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6 as control variables in this paper was valid (as
shown in Figure 13). Consequently, no statistical analysis was subsequently conducted
for these three AOIs, and a multi-way ANOVA was only conducted for the indicators
related to the key AOIs. The results of the descriptive statistics and multi-way analysis are
shown in Table 10.
(a) (b)
Figure 13. The partial distribution of attention among all the AOIs: (a) average regression count
among AOIs; (b) average fixation count among AOIs.
Table 10. Results of the multivariate-ANOVA-main-effects test for each AOI eye-movement indica-
tor.
Independent
Variable
Dependent
Variable
Type III Sum
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
AOI
RCAOI 7.223 3 2.408 9.114 <0.001 0.196
FDAOI 263,442.561 3 87,814.187 34.109 <0.001 0.477
FCAOI 38.821 3 12.940 1.797 0.152 0.046
SA RCAOI 18.116 3 6.039 22.857 <0.001 0.380
FDAOI 143,035.259 3 47,678.420 18.520 <0.001 0.332
Figure 12. Interface AOI division.
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the average regression count (RCAOI), the
percentage of fixation count (FCAOI), and the average fixation duration (FDAOI) in the
AOI as dependent variables, and on the AOI and the three-stage score of SA as independent
variables. A post hoc test revealed that for the RCAOI, FCAOI was significantly greater in
AOI1, AOI2, AOI3, and AOI7 than in AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6, and to some extent, the use of
AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6 as control variables in this paper was valid (as shown in Figure 13).
Consequently, no statistical analysis was subsequently conducted for these three AOIs, and
a multi-way ANOVA was only conducted for the indicators related to the key AOIs. The
results of the descriptive statistics and multi-way analysis are shown in Table 10.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 19 of 26
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 19 of 27
Figure 12. Interface AOI division.
A multi-way ANOVA was conducted on the average regression count (RCAOI), the
percentage of fixation count (FCAOI), and the average fixation duration (FDAOI) in the
AOI as dependent variables, and on the AOI and the three-stage score of SA as independ-
ent variables. A post hoc test revealed that for the RCAOI, FCAOI was significantly
greater in AOI1, AOI2, AOI3, and AOI7 than in AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6, and to some ex-
tent, the use of AOI4, AOI5, and AOI6 as control variables in this paper was valid (as
shown in Figure 13). Consequently, no statistical analysis was subsequently conducted
for these three AOIs, and a multi-way ANOVA was only conducted for the indicators
related to the key AOIs. The results of the descriptive statistics and multi-way analysis are
shown in Table 10.
(a) (b)
Figure 13. The partial distribution of attention among all the AOIs: (a) average regression count
among AOIs; (b) average fixation count among AOIs.
Table 10. Results of the multivariate-ANOVA-main-effects test for each AOI eye-movement indica-
tor.
Independent
Variable
Dependent
Variable
Type III Sum
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
AOI
RCAOI 7.223 3 2.408 9.114 <0.001 0.196
FDAOI 263,442.561 3 87,814.187 34.109 <0.001 0.477
FCAOI 38.821 3 12.940 1.797 0.152 0.046
SA RCAOI 18.116 3 6.039 22.857 <0.001 0.380
Figure 13.
The partial distribution of attention among all the AOIs: (
a
) average regression count
among AOIs; (b) average fixation count among AOIs.
Table 10.
Results of the multivariate-ANOVA-main-effects test for each AOI eye-movement indicator.
Independent
Variable
Dependent
Variable
Type III Sum of
Squares df Mean
Square F Sig. Partial Eta
Squared
AOI
RCAOI 7.223 3 2.408 9.114 <0.001 0.196
FDAOI 263,442.561 3 87,814.187 34.109 <0.001 0.477
FCAOI 38.821 3 12.940 1.797 0.152 0.046
SA
RCAOI 18.116 3 6.039 22.857 <0.001 0.380
FDAOI 143,035.259 3 47,678.420 18.520 <0.001 0.332
FCAOI 209.763 3 69.921 9.709 <0.001 0.206
AOI ×SA
RCAOI 9.531 9 1.059 4.008 <0.001 0.244
FDAOI 133,411.300 9 14,823.478 5.758 <0.001 0.316
FCAOI 295.330 9 32.814 4.556 <0.001 0.268
Note. RCAOI: average number of regression counts in area of interest; FDAOI: average fixation duration in area
of interest; FCAOI: average number of fixation counts in area of interest.
From the results, it can be seen that for the RCAOI, there was a significant differ-
ence between the AOIs (F (3, 112) = 9.114, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.196), with post hoc tests
finding that the RCAOI of AOI2 was significantly greater than that of AOI1 and AOI7
(p= 0.024 < 0.05, p< 0.001), with the mean value presenting as AOI2 > AOI3 > AOI1 > AOI7.
We also observed a significant difference between SA stages (F (3, 112) = 22.857, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.380), with stage SA3 being significantly greater than stage SA2, with mean value of
SA3 > Q7 > SA1 > SA2. For the FDAOI, the difference between the AOIs was significant
(F (3, 112) = 34.109, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.477), and the post-hoc-test analysis revealed that the
gaze time in AOI7 was significantly greater than that of other AOIs (p< 0.001), that the
overall mean value showed that AOI7 > AOI3 > AOI2 > AOI1, and that the difference
between the SA stages was significant (F (3, 112) = 18.520, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.332). Post-
hoc-test analysis showed that the FDAOI of SA3 was significantly longer than that of SA2
(p< 0.001) and that the overall mean showed Q7 > SA3 > SA1 > SA2. For the FCAOI, it was
not significant across AOIs (F (3, 112) = 1.797, p= 0.152,
η2
p
= 0.046), but it was significant
among SA stages (F (3, 112) = 9.709, p< 0.001,
η2
p
= 0.206), with the overall mean showing
SA3 > Q7 > SA1 > SA2. All the indicators mentioned above had a significant interaction
effect (p< 0.001) between the two; that is, in the SA1, subjects paid more attention to
AOI1; in the SA2, subjects paid more attention to AOI2; and in the SA3, subjects paid more
attention to AOI3 (Figure 14).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 20 of 26
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 20 of 27
FCAOI 209.763 3 69.921 9.709 <0.001 0.206
AOI × SA
RCAOI 9.531 9 1.059 4.008 <0.001 0.244
FDAOI 133,411.300 9 14,823.478 5.758 <0.001 0.316
FCAOI 295.330 9 32.814 4.556 <0.001 0.268
Note. RCAOI: average number of regression counts in area of interest; FDAOI: average fixation
duration in area of interest; FCAOI: average number of fixation counts in area of interest.
From the results, it can be seen that for the RCAOI, there was a significant difference
between the AOIs (F (3, 112) = 9.114, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.196), with post hoc tests finding that
the RCAOI of AOI2 was significantly greater than that of AOI1 and AOI7 (p = 0.024 < 0.05,
p < 0.001), with the mean value presenting as AOI2 > AOI3 > AOI1 > AOI7. We also ob-
served a significant difference between SA stages (F (3, 112) = 22.857, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.380),
with stage SA3 being significantly greater than stage SA2, with mean value of SA3 > Q7 >
SA1 > SA2. For the FDAOI, the difference between the AOIs was significant (F (3, 112) =
34.109, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.477), and the post-hoc-test analysis revealed that the gaze time in
AOI7 was significantly greater than that of other AOIs (p < 0.001), that the overall mean
value showed that AOI7 > AOI3 > AOI2 > AOI1, and that the difference between the SA
stages was significant (F (3, 112) = 18.520, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.332). Post-hoc-test analysis
showed that the FDAOI of SA3 was significantly longer than that of SA2 (p < 0.001) and
that the overall mean showed Q7 > SA3 > SA1 > SA2. For the FCAOI, it was not significant
across AOIs (F (3, 112) = 1.797, p = 0.152, 𝜂
= 0.046), but it was significant among SA
stages (F (3, 112) = 9.709, p < 0.001, 𝜂
= 0.206), with the overall mean showing SA3 > Q7 >
SA1 > SA2. All the indicators mentioned above had a significant interaction effect (p <
0.001) between the two; that is, in the SA1, subjects paid more attention to AOI1; in the
SA2, subjects paid more attention to AOI2; and in the SA3, subjects paid more attention
to AOI3 (Figure 14).
(a) (b)
(c)
Figure 14.
Interaction effect of attention allocation between the key AOIs and three stages of SA:
(
a
) average regression count among key AOIs; (
b
) average fixation duration among key AOIs; and
(c) average fixation count among key AOIs. Error bar: standard error.
5. Discussion
5.1. Discussion of SA Scores at Different UI Levels
The statistical analysis of the three stages of SA and total scores obtained by SPAM
revealed that in SA1, with the improvement in the UI level, SSA1 decreased. The reason for
this is that attention is the key link of this stage, and as the UI level increases, subjects need
to perceive and process more information. Thus, they experience an overload of attention
resources caused by the increased amount of information at the interface. As a result, a
decrease in the level of perception [
52
], combined with the participants’ increased scores in
the AD dimension, as measured by SART, could also be demonstrated. In the stage SA2,
subjects scored the highest SSA2 on UI2, probably because UI2 provides the rectified rate
information of hidden hazards to help subjects understand, based on UI1, and the amount
of information displayed in UI2 is less than that of UI3, so the appropriate information
does not easily confuse subjects’ judgment. Previous studies have found that excessive
information display presents a greater cognitive load and interference to people regarding
filtering, classifying, reorganizing, and integrating information, making people prone to
perceptual and comprehension errors, which in turn leads to human-factor errors [
53
,
54
].
Therefore, if the threshold of information presentation can be found in the interface design,
it can help people better understand the information, to some extent. In the SA3, the
addition of the projection module to UI3 helped improve the subjects’ risk prediction.
For the total SA score, it was shown that the higher the UI level, the better subjects’ SA.
Scholar Zhi suggested that the decreasing effect of SA due to attention allocation can be
substantially improved by enhancing the quality of the interface [
55
]. Although the increase
in the amount of information displayed due to the increased UI level led to a decrease in
subjects’ SSA1, as the logic and correlation between the information increased, the interface
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 21 of 26
provided more objective and comprehensive information rather than less information,
which contributed to subjects’ global perception, comprehension, and prediction, thus
increasing subjects’ level of SA.
The statistical analysis of the three-dimensional and total scores obtained by SART
for each UI level showed that as the amount of VIS presented increased, the demand
for subjects’ attention resources increased, with the SAD showing an increasing trend.
However, to some certain extent, the VIS presentation interface, designed based on the
three-stage SA theory, helped subjects better perceive, understand, and predict information,
and thus the SSU showed an increasing trend with the increase in the UI level. In the AS
dimension, the degree of mental arousal, concentration of attention, and total spare capacity
of subjects in the corresponding situations showed a tendency for higher-level interfaces to
achieve a higher score, while in the attention-allocation dimension, subjects subjectively
perceived UI2 to be better (Figure 15). Relatively speaking, this may be because humans’
attention resources are limited [
56
], and with limited attentional resources, increased UI
levels lead to an increase in the amount of information that subjects need to perceive and
process, making it difficult for subjects to allocate their attention resources well. As the
higher levels of UI provide a combination of information to aid comprehension, the overall
supply of attention to the subject is increased. As for the overall SART scores, although
there was no significant difference, the means indicated that subjects tended to feel that
their perceptual comprehension and prediction ability were better and that their level of
SA was higher, which was generally consistent with the SPAM results. Although some
differences exist between UI2 and UI3, people subjectively may prefer the presentation of
appropriate information, while the actual situation showed that the subjects had the best
SA in UI3, which may be because of the amount of visual information displayed on the
interface and because of the logic and relevance of the information.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 22 of 27
Figure 15. Ten sub-dimension scores of SART of each UI level.
5.2. Discussion of SA Scores for Different Presentation Frames
A statistical analysis of the three-dimensional and total scores obtained by SPAM
showed that the total SA score, SSA1, and SSA2 decreased with the presentation frame of
VIS from positive to negative. Combined with the Q7 scores, the level of overall perceived
risk for the jurisdiction varied across the presentation frames. In the positive frame, sub-
jects perceived less risk and had a higher level of SA, while in the negative frame, subjects
perceived more risk and had a lower level of SA. The reason for the higher level of SA
under the perception of less risk may be the fact that different presentation frames of VIS
evoke different emotional attitudes of subjects, and thus lead to changes in their cognitive
strategies and levels [57,58]. In a positive frame, people tend to perceive and judge posi-
tively and optimistically, with positive emotions facilitating the processing of information
[59]. By contrast, a negative frame tends to be perceived in a negative and pessimistic
manner, with negative emotions inhibiting cognitive processes by inhibiting automatic
processing [60]. The framing effect was thus evident between positive and negative
presentation frames. The reason for the higher SSA2 under the PN and NP frames com-
pared with the PP and NN frames may be that subjects emotional attitudes were neutral-
ized under the combined positive and negative presentation frames, thus enabling them
to integrate relevant information more objectively. In SA3, the SSA3 was not significant
across the presentation frames, further revealing that the effect of the framing effect was
mainly concentrated in the perception and understanding stages of SA. Overall, the sub-
jects risk-perception magnitude and level of SA showed a significant framing effect.
A statistical analysis of the three-dimensional and total scores obtained by SART
among representation frames showed that subjects subjective SAD was highest under the
negative frame because people subjectively perceive this situation to be more complex,
variable, and unstable, requiring more attention resources to make judgments and per-
ceptions. Regarding the AS dimension, subjects degree of mental arousal, concentration
of attention, and spare capacity in different presentation frames tended to be better when
guided by the positive frame, while the distribution of attention tended to be worse in a
positive and negative combination frame (Figure 16). This is probably because when in-
formation was presented in the positivenegative or negativepositive combination
frames, it somewhat distracted subjects attention, and although subjects subjective intu-
itive level of AS was lower, subjects ability to comprehend information improved, owing
to the comprehension and integration of the information presented, so the SPAM score
reflected a trend of improvement. In sum, on the SU dimension and the overall SART
score, there was a tendency for people to subjectively view the positive frame as superior
to the negative frame, which is consistent with subjects’ performance of SA as reflected in
the SPAM score.
Figure 15. Ten sub-dimension scores of SART of each UI level.
5.2. Discussion of SA Scores for Different Presentation Frames
A statistical analysis of the three-dimensional and total scores obtained by SPAM
showed that the total SA score, SSA1, and SSA2 decreased with the presentation frame of
VIS from positive to negative. Combined with the Q7 scores, the level of overall perceived
risk for the jurisdiction varied across the presentation frames. In the positive frame,
subjects perceived less risk and had a higher level of SA, while in the negative frame,
subjects perceived more risk and had a lower level of SA. The reason for the higher level
of SA under the perception of less risk may be the fact that different presentation frames
of VIS evoke different emotional attitudes of subjects, and thus lead to changes in their
cognitive strategies and levels [
57
,
58
]. In a positive frame, people tend to perceive and
judge positively and optimistically, with positive emotions facilitating the processing of
information [
59
]. By contrast, a negative frame tends to be perceived in a negative and
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 22 of 26
pessimistic manner, with negative emotions inhibiting cognitive processes by inhibiting
automatic processing [
60
]. The framing effect was thus evident between positive and
negative presentation frames. The reason for the higher SSA2 under the PN and NP
frames compared with the PP and NN frames may be that subjects’ emotional attitudes
were neutralized under the combined positive and negative presentation frames, thus
enabling them to integrate relevant information more objectively. In SA3, the SSA3 was not
significant across the presentation frames, further revealing that the effect of the framing
effect was mainly concentrated in the perception and understanding stages of SA. Overall,
the subjects’ risk-perception magnitude and level of SA showed a significant framing effect.
A statistical analysis of the three-dimensional and total scores obtained by SART
among representation frames showed that subjects’ subjective SAD was highest under the
negative frame because people subjectively perceive this situation to be more complex, vari-
able, and unstable, requiring more attention resources to make judgments and perceptions.
Regarding the AS dimension, subjects’ degree of mental arousal, concentration of attention,
and spare capacity in different presentation frames tended to be better when guided by
the positive frame, while the distribution of attention tended to be worse in a positive and
negative combination frame (Figure 16). This is probably because when information was
presented in the positive–negative or negative–positive combination frames, it somewhat
distracted subjects’ attention, and although subjects’ subjective intuitive level of AS was
lower, subjects’ ability to comprehend information improved, owing to the comprehension
and integration of the information presented, so the SPAM score reflected a trend of im-
provement. In sum, on the SU dimension and the overall SART score, there was a tendency
for people to subjectively view the positive frame as superior to the negative frame, which
is consistent with subjects’ performance of SA as reflected in the SPAM score.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, x FOR PEER REVIEW 23 of 27
Figure 16. Ten sub-dimension scores of SART of each presentation frame.
5.3. Discussion of Attention Distribution
Based on the data of eye-movement indexes, the attention distribution of the subjects
in each UI level and presentation frame was statistically analyzed. It was found that the
overall spatial distribution of the subjects fixation points was aggregated. This is probably
because the SPAM requires subjects to locate certain modules of VIS in the interface, to
answer the corresponding probe questions. When individuals are driven by the search
task, owing to limited cognitive resources, attention-selection mechanisms encourage in-
dividuals to focus on areas where they expect to receive relevant information, and create
stimulus filters to prevent individuals from being distracted by irrelevant information
[40], resulting in a more aggregate gaze. The NNI values of the subjects were larger at the
higher UI levels and when the UI was guided by the positive frame, with a more discrete
distribution of gaze points and a relatively higher level of SA. This may be because as the
UI level increases, subjects need to allocate their limited attention to the additional VIS
modules, thus distributing their attention more discretely, and the three-level UI design
helps to increase their level of SA. The highest aggregation of gaze distribution in the neg-
ative frame may be due to negative information motivating decision makers to process
information and to thus devote more attention to it, but it may also be due to people pay-
ing more attention to the red than the green stimuli. The reason the level of SA was higher
when the distribution of gaze points was more discrete is that people tend to perceive and
understand judgments from a global perspective, which is also in line with the research
findings, to some extent [40,61,62].
Statistical analysis of the average number of regression counts, average fixation
count, and average fixation duration of the AOIs showed that the subjects local-attention
distribution in each situation was that they paid more attention to AOI1 in SA1, AOI2 in
SA2, and AOI3 in SA3, and that there was no significant difference in their attention to the
SPAM probe questions. It can be indicated that the subjects searched for and located the
corresponding information in the interface for decision making based on the SPAM ques-
tions. However, it does not mean that the higher the level of decision making, the more
likely it is that one can accurately locate the corresponding information module. The SART
and SPAM scores showed that SA was influenced by different UI levels and presentation
frames, and the phenomenon of subjects failing to make accurate decisions despite being
able to accurately locate information proved the validity of this experiment, to a certain
extent.
Figure 16. Ten sub-dimension scores of SART of each presentation frame.
5.3. Discussion of Attention Distribution
Based on the data of eye-movement indexes, the attention distribution of the subjects
in each UI level and presentation frame was statistically analyzed. It was found that the
overall spatial distribution of the subjects’ fixation points was aggregated. This is probably
because the SPAM requires subjects to locate certain modules of VIS in the interface, to
answer the corresponding probe questions. When individuals are driven by the search
task, owing to limited cognitive resources, attention-selection mechanisms encourage
individuals to focus on areas where they expect to receive relevant information, and create
stimulus filters to prevent individuals from being distracted by irrelevant information [
40
],
resulting in a more aggregate gaze. The NNI values of the subjects were larger at the
higher UI levels and when the UI was guided by the positive frame, with a more discrete
distribution of gaze points and a relatively higher level of SA. This may be because as the
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 23 of 26
UI level increases, subjects need to allocate their limited attention to the additional VIS
modules, thus distributing their attention more discretely, and the three-level UI design
helps to increase their level of SA. The highest aggregation of gaze distribution in the
negative frame may be due to negative information motivating decision makers to process
information and to thus devote more attention to it, but it may also be due to people paying
more attention to the red than the green stimuli. The reason the level of SA was higher
when the distribution of gaze points was more discrete is that people tend to perceive and
understand judgments from a global perspective, which is also in line with the research
findings, to some extent [40,61,62].
Statistical analysis of the average number of regression counts, average fixation count,
and average fixation duration of the AOIs showed that the subjects’ local-attention dis-
tribution in each situation was that they paid more attention to AOI1 in SA1, AOI2 in
SA2, and AOI3 in SA3, and that there was no significant difference in their attention to
the SPAM probe questions. It can be indicated that the subjects searched for and located
the corresponding information in the interface for decision making based on the SPAM
questions. However, it does not mean that the higher the level of decision making, the
more likely it is that one can accurately locate the corresponding information module. The
SART and SPAM scores showed that SA was influenced by different UI levels and presenta-
tion frames, and the phenomenon of subjects failing to make accurate decisions despite
being able to accurately locate information proved the validity of this experiment, to a
certain extent.
6. Limitations and Future Directions
The limitations and future research prospects for this study are as follows.
(1)
This study investigated the framing effect of VIS. To make this study more practical,
the safety and danger VIS elements were presented in green and red, respectively,
based on the realistic situation and Chinese culture while manipulating the attributing
frame of the VIS. Considering that presenting safety and danger information in the
same color would have been out of context, different visual saliencies of the stimuli
may also have had an impact on an individual’s visual processing, to a certain extent,
which can be further investigated in the future by controlling the visual characteristics.
Other features of VIS, such as color and shape, can also be further explored in the
future, to investigate effective ways of VIS presentation and thus improve the stage
level of SA.
(2)
This study measured the cognitive processing of the subjects in different presentation
frames using subjective scales (focusing on subjective behavioral performance) and
eye-movement data (focusing on information-perception processes). In the future,
the internal cognitive processing of individuals under different construction and
presentation frames of VIS can be further explored in conjunction with EEG, in the
three stages of information perceiving, understanding, and predicting, to further
investigate the cognitive-processing differences of individuals.
7. Conclusions
This study took VIS as the research object, combined the theories related to SA and
the framing effect, and designed a three-level VIS presentation interface. On this basis,
we explored the framing effect of different VIS presentation frames, and at the same time,
we used eye-movement indicators to measure the subjects’ attention allocation, which to
some extent enriches the theoretical study of SA and the framing effect in the safety field.
Specifically, the following conclusions were drawn from this study.
(1)
For the construction of information, increasing the UI level effectively enhances the
level of SA for the subjects. Although the increase in the amount of information
displayed due to the increment in the UI level may lead to a decrease in subjects’
perceived level of SA and an increase in attentional demands, the higher-level interface
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 24 of 26
helps to increase subjects’, as it fully considers the three stages of SA: perception,
understanding, and prediction.
(2)
There is a framing effect on the presentation of VIS, and the representation frame
affects the individual’s SA. People perceive risk differently in different presentation
frames, with people perceiving less risk in positive frames and more risk in negative
frames. Subjects have better levels of perception and prediction of information, and
the highest levels of SA, in the fully positive frame, better levels of comprehension in
the combined frame, and the lowest levels of SA in the fully negative frame.
(3)
To a certain extent, the NNI provides a process index for this study, quantifying
the subjects’ eye-movement gaze strategies. The more discrete the distribution, the
better the subjects can integrate information from a global perspective, and the higher
the level of SA. The validity of this experiment is further supported by other eye-
movement metrics from the perspective of the partial distribution of attention.
This study can provide some reference for the design and optimization of the VIS
presentation interface. When designing the VIS presentation interface, researchers should
ensure that the amount of information displayed on the interface is moderate; at the same
time, the logic and correlation between the information on the interface should be improved;
that is, the layout of the VIS presentation interface should fully consider the three stages
of SA, so that the interface level can conform to the three stages of cognitive processing of
people’s information and help people improve their SA. For the selection of the presentation
frame of VIS, the positive and negative frames should be combined as much as possible,
so that people can better integrate relevant information, make judgments on the current
safety-risk situation, and improve SA.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, X.Y., L.S. and J.Y.; methodology, J.Y.; investigation, F.C.;
data curation, Z.G. and H.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, J.Y.; writing—review and editing,
X.Y.; visualization, J.Y.; project administration, X.Y. and L.S.; funding acquisition, X.Y. and L.S. All
authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding:
This research was funded by the Youth Project of Science and Technology Department
of Shaanxi Province—“Research on human–machine-interaction performance for dispatching and
monitoring operations in digital mine”, grant number 2020JQ760, and the Humanities and Social
Science Research project of the Ministry of Education Fund, grant number 22YJAZH104.
Institutional Review Board Statement: Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement: All the data in this study are presented.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Tan, Z.; Wu, Q. Research on visual evaluation of coal safety management. Saf. Coal Mines 2018,49, 230–233.
2.
Zhang, S.; Cai, Y.; Li, J. Visualization of COVID-19 spread based on spread and extinction indexes. Sci. China Inf. Sci.
2020
,63, 164102.
[CrossRef]
3.
Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory. In The psychology of Learning and Motivation: Cognition in Education; Mestre, J.P., Ross, B.H., Eds.;
Elsevier Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2011; Volume 55, pp. 37–76.
4.
Sweller, J. Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educ. Psychol. Rev.
2010
,22, 123–138.
[CrossRef]
5.
Sun, L.; Guo, Z.; Yuan, X.; Wang, X.; Su, C.; Jiang, J.; Li, X. An investigation of the effects of brain fatigue on the sustained attention
of intelligent coal mine VDT operators. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022,19, 11034. [CrossRef]
6. Weiser, M.; Brown, J.S. Designing calm technology. PowerGrid J. 1996,1, 75–85.
7.
Blackwell, A.; Green, T. Notational systems—The cognitive dimensions of notations framework. In HCI Models, Theories, and
Frameworks: Toward a Multidisciplinary Science; Carroll, J.M., Ed.; Morgan Kaufmann Publishers: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2003;
pp. 103–133.
8.
Blackwell, A.F.; Britton, C.; Cox, A.; Green, T.R.G.; Gurr, C.; Kadoda, G.; Kutar, M.S.; Loomes, M.; Nehaniv, C.L.; Petre, M.; et al.
Cognitive dimensions of notations: Design tools for cognitive technology. In Cognitive Technology: Instruments of Mind; Beynon,
M., Nehaniv, C.L., Dautenhahn, K., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2001; pp. 325–341.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 25 of 26
9.
Bowden, J.R.; Rusnock, C.F. Evaluation of human machine interface design factors on situation awareness and task performance.
Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2015,59, 1361–1365. [CrossRef]
10.
Chen, Y.V.; Qian, Z.C.; Lei, W.T. Designing a situational awareness information display: Adopting an affordance-based framework
to amplify user experience in environmental interaction design. Informatics 2016,3, 6. [CrossRef]
11.
Li, W.; Zakarija, M.; Yu, C.; McCarty, P. Interface design on cabin pressurization system affecting pilot’s situation awareness: The
comparison between digital displays and pointed displays. Hum. Factors Ergon. Manuf. 2020,30, 103–113. [CrossRef]
12.
Jiang, T.; Fang, H. The influence of user interface design on task performance and situation awareness in a 3-player diner’s
dilemma game. PLoS ONE 2020,15, e0230387. [CrossRef]
13. Endsley, M.R. Toward a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Hum. Factors 1995,37, 32–64. [CrossRef]
14.
Endsley, M. A survey of situation awareness requirements in air-to-air combat fighters. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol.
1993
,3, 157–168.
[CrossRef]
15.
Durso, F.T.; Nickerson, R.S.; Dumais, S.T.; Lewandowsky, S.; Perfect, T.J. Handbook of Applied Cognition; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.:
Chinchester, UK, 2007.
16.
Endsley, M.; Jones, D. Designing for Situation Awareness: An Approach to User-Centered Design; CRC Press: London, UK, 2003;
pp. 50–54.
17.
O’Sullivan, M. Designing with the mind in mind: Simple guide to understanding user interface design rules by Jeff Johnson.
ACM SIGSOFT Softw. Eng. Notes 2011,36, 52. [CrossRef]
18.
Jones, D.G.; Endsley, M.R. Sources of situation awareness errors in aviation. Aviat. Space Environ. Med.
1996
,67, 507–512.
[PubMed]
19.
Hall, C.C.; Ariss, L.; Todorov, A. The illusion of knowledge: When more information reduces accuracy and increases confidence.
Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2007,103, 277–290. [CrossRef]
20. Todd, P.M. How much information do we need. Eur. J. Oper. Res. 2005,177, 1317–1332. [CrossRef]
21.
Rakhra, A.K.; Mann, D.D. Design and evaluation of individual elements of the interface for an agricultural machine. J. Agric. Saf.
Health 2018,24, 27–42. [CrossRef]
22.
Fu, Y.; Xu, B. Effects of display format of air traffic information on situation awareness and cognitive performance in cockpit.
Chin. J. Ergon. 2012,18, 13–17.
23.
Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science
1981
,211, 453–458. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
24.
Fagley, N.S.; Miller, P.M. Framing effects and arenas of choice: Your money or your life. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process.
1997
,
71, 355–373. [CrossRef]
25. McElroy, T.; Seta, J.J. Framing effects: An analytic–holistic perspective. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2003,39, 610–617. [CrossRef]
26.
Levin, I.P.; Schneider, S.L.; Gaeth, G.J. All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ.
Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1998,76, 149–188. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
27.
Calvin, K.L.; Wang, H.L. Color-concept associations: A cross-occupational and -cultural study and comparison. Color Res. Appl.
2014,39, 630–635.
28.
Endsley, M.R. A systematic review and meta-analysis of direct objective measures of situation awareness: A comparison of
SAGAT and SPAM. Hum. Factors 2019,63, 124–150. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
29.
Durso, F.T.; Hackworth, C.A.; Truitt, T.R.; Crutchfield, J.; Nikolic, D. Situation awareness as a predictor of performance in en route
air traffic controllers. Air Traffic Control Q. 1998,6, 1–20. [CrossRef]
30. Endsley, M.R. Measurement of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Hum. Factors 1995,37, 65–84. [CrossRef]
31.
Taylor, R.M. Situational awareness rating technique (SART): The development of a tool for aircrew systems design. In Situational
Awareness; Salas, E., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1990; pp. 111–128.
32.
Feng, C.; WanYan, X.; Liu, S.; Chen, H.; Zhuang, D.; Wang, X. Influence of different attention allocation strategies under workloads
on situation awareness. Acta Aeronaut. Astronaut. Sin. 2020,41, 123307.
33.
Wang, Y.; Zhou, S.; Liu, C. The influence of fixation pattern of student pilots on team situational awareness. Sci. Technol. Eng.
2021,21, 7784–7789.
34.
van de Merwe, K.; van Dijk, H.; Zon, R. Eye movements as an indicator of situation awareness in a flight simulator experiment.
Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 2012,22, 78–95. [CrossRef]
35.
Tsank, Y.; Eckstein, M.P. Domain specificity of oculomotor learning after changes in sensory processing. J. Neurosci.
2017
,37,
11469–11484. [CrossRef]
36.
Jones, D.G.; Endsley, M.R. Use of real-time probes for measuring situation awareness. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol.
2004
,14, 343–367.
[CrossRef]
37.
Su, Y.; Liu, Z.; Chen, C.; Su, H. The Chinese version of situation awareness rating technique: Research and application. Space Med.
Med. Eng. 2018,31, 513–519.
38. Schneider, W.; Chein, J.M. Controlled & automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cogn. Sci. 2003,27,
525–559.
39.
Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behav. Brain Sci.
2000
,23,
645–665. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023,20, 3325 26 of 26
40.
Moore, K.; Gugerty, L. Development of a novel measure of situation awareness: The case for eye movement analysis. Proc. Hum.
Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2010,54, 1650–1654. [CrossRef]
41.
Rayner, K. Eye movements and cognitive processes in reading, visual search, and scene perception. In Eye Movement Research:
Mechanisms, Processes and Applications; Findlay, J.M., Walker, R., Kentridge, R.W., Eds.; Elsevier Science: Amsterdam, The
Netherlands, 1995; Volume 6, pp. 3–22.
42.
Liu, T.; Yuizono, T. Mind mapping training’s effects on reading ability: Detection based on eye tracking sensors. Sensors
2020
,20, 4422.
[CrossRef]
43.
Jin, H.; Wang, D.; Wang, S.; Zhang, Y. A study on the relationship between the visual search features of traffic controllers and the
number of aircrafts. China Sci. 2015,10, 2291–2294.
44.
Marquart, G.; Cabrall, C.; de Winter, J. Review of eye-related measures of drivers’ mental workload. Procedia Manuf.
2015
,3,
2854–2861. [CrossRef]
45.
Nakatani, M.; Ohno, T.; Katagiri, Y.; Ai, N.; Hashimoto, S. The layout for the user-friendly manual: Case study on an internet set-up
manual. In Design, User Experience, and Usability. Theory, Methods, Tools and Practice; Marcus, A., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg,
Germany, 2011; Volume 6770, pp. 40–45.
46. Christ, R.E. Review and analysis of color coding research for visual displays. Hum. Factors 1975,17, 542–570. [CrossRef]
47.
Kühberger, A. The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process.
1998
,75, 23–55.
[CrossRef]
48. Montelpare, W.J.; Read, E.; McComber, T.; Mahar, A.; Ritchie, K. The one way analysis of variance and post hoc tests. In Applied
Statistics in Healthcare Research: Theoretical Concepts and Computational Methods Using SAS Applications and the Webulators; University
of Prince Edwar Island: Charlottetown, PE, Canada.
49.
Homack, S.R. Understanding what ANOVA post hoc tests are, really. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Southwest
Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA, USA, 1–3 February 2001.
50.
Marco, C.; Roberto, N.; Michela, T.; Francesco, D.N. ASTEF: A simple tool for examining fixations. Behav. Res. Methods
2008
,40,
373–382.
51.
Di Nocera, F.; Camilli, M.; Terenzi, M. A random glance at the flight deck: Pilots’ scanning strategies and the real-time assessment
of mental workload. J. Cogn. Eng. Decis. Mak. 2007,1, 271–285. [CrossRef]
52. Zhang, C.; Zhao, J.H. Vehicle navigation interface design based on situational awareness. Packag. Eng. 2016,37, 48–51.
53.
Li, P.; Zhang, L.; Dai, L.; Huang, W. Effects of digital human-machine interface characteristics on human error in nuclear power
plants. Nuclear Power Eng. 2011,32, 48–52.
54.
Zhang, L.; Yang, D.; Wang, Y. The effect of information display on human reliability in a digital control room. China Saf. Sci. J.
2010,20, 81–85.
55.
Zhi, J.Y.; Xiang, Z.R.; Li, R. Status and prospect of situation awareness theory in intelligent driving interface design. Packag. Eng.
2019,40, 35–39.
56. Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort; Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1973; pp. 218–226.
57.
Valentina, C.; Massimiliano, P.; Patrizia, C. A cognitive-emotional model to explain message framing effects: Reducing meat
consumption. Front. Psychol. 2021,12, 583209.
58.
Borah, P. Competitive framing, emotion and heterogeneity: Framing effects on Twitter in the case of an alt-right rally. Online Inf.
Rev. 2021,45, 1131–1147. [CrossRef]
59.
Li, E.; Xiao, F.; Zou, T.; Guo, J. Positive emotion of self-referential contexts could facilitate adult’s novel word learning: An fNIRS
study. Brain Lang. 2021,221, 104994. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
60.
Ma, Q.; Hu, Y.; Jiang, S.; Meng, L. The undermining effect of facial attractiveness on brain responses to fairness in the Ultimatum
Game: An ERP study. Front. Neurosci. 2015,9, 77. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
61.
Jin, H.; Liu, Y.; Zhu, G. Measuring situation awareness of tower controllers based on eye movement and performance analysis.
China Saf. Sci. J. 2017,27, 65–70.
62.
Jin, H.B.; Liu, Y.W.; Liu, W.H.; Zhang, C.W. Study of tower controllers’ situation awareness based on NNI fixation index. Sci.
Technol. Eng. 2016,16, 101–105.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note:
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual
author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to
people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
... Such interfaces in the case of complex systems, with numerous alarms or multiple data, present the disadvantage of difficult manipulation, often unintuitive leading to perception problems or even cognitive efficiency issues [15,22], considered even lacking a human-centric approach [19] New immersive technologies can address these problems and possibly even more, can provide a plus towards ensuring cognitive ergonomics. This can be achieved by multiple factors such as those though the hardware used, new software frameworks able to offer enriched experience, and more than that the diversity of of interfacing ways in the existing SCADA structures. ...
Article
Full-text available
Due to crucial importance of the electricity in almost every aspect of our life, power systems and their components continue to receive considerable attention, and important efforts are invested for development or improvement in the direction of smooth transition to full smart grid solutions. Even if operation and control of power system are well-known, new control solutions require careful and detailed investigations due to challenges emerged from high complexity, security or even the current operating conditions in high penetration of renewable energy sources or consummers with significant loads. In this context, the paper introduces a new concept and solution of augmented cyberphysical model to allow testing and simulation of the supervision, monitoring and control solutions in a mixed physical and virtual environment, facilitating complex investigation starting from common process level to complex interdependencies arise from communication infrastructure inherent failures to contingent issues such as software related or security attacks. The concept, architecture and an implementation on a real-time hardware-in-the-loop based platform are revealed and shown as an open and affordable research and development solution.
... RT is defined as the interval between perceiving the takeover information and executing the takeover operation. The Three-Dimensional SA Rating Technique (3D-SART) is a standardized tool for assessing SA in complex operational tasks [31]. Based on Endsley's theory of SA, this scale measures three core dimensions: perceptual resource demands, understanding, and projection. ...
Article
Full-text available
With the advancement of autonomous driving technology, especially the growing adoption of SAE Level 3 and above systems, drivers are transitioning from active controllers to supervisors who must take over in emergencies. For hearing-impaired drivers in a fatigued state, conventional voice alert systems often fail to provide timely and effective warnings, increasing safety risks. This study proposes an adaptive in-vehicle interface that combines visual and tactile feedback to address these challenges. Experiments were conducted to evaluate response accuracy, reaction time, and cognitive load under varying levels of driver fatigue. The findings show that the integration of visual and tactile cues significantly improves takeover efficiency and reduces mental strain in fatigued drivers. These results highlight the potential of multimodal designs in enhancing the safety and driving experience for hearing-impaired individuals. By providing practical strategies and evidence-based insights, this research contributes to the development of more inclusive and effective interaction designs for future autonomous driving systems.
... This research employs spatial autocorrelation analysis to measure the similarity or correlation among adjacent regions, heiping identify data clustering and dispersion, as well as revealing the spatial patterns and dependencies of geographic phenomena. Additionally, unlike geographic statistical analysis, which only presents numeric results, visualization analysis uses graphical representations to enhance understanding of data structure, patterns, and relationships [12,13]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background In China, economic, urbanization, and policy differences between the eastern and western regions lead to uneven healthcare resources. This disparity is more pronounced in the west due to fewer healthcare personnel per thousand individuals and imbalanced doctor-to-nurse ratios, which exacerbates healthcare challenges. This study examines the spatial distribution of human resources in maternal and child healthcare from 2016 to 2021, highlighting regional disparities and offering insights for future policy development. Methods The data were sourced from the “China Health and Family Planning Statistical Yearbook” (2017) and the “China Health and Health Statistics Yearbook” (2018–2022). This study utilized GeoDa 1.8.6 software to conduct both global and local spatial autocorrelation analyses, using China’s administrative map as the base dataset. Results From 2016 to 2021, there was an upward trend in the number of health personnel and various types of health technical personnel in Chinese maternal and child healthcare institutions. The spatial distribution of these personnel from 2016 to 2021 revealed clusters characterized as high-high, low-low, high-low and low-high. Specifically, high-high clusters were identified in Guangxi, Hunan, Jiangxi, and Guangdong provinces; low-low in Xinjiang Uygur Autonomous Region and Inner Mongolia Autonomous Region; high-low in Sichuan province; and low-high in Fujian and Anhui provinces. Conclusions From 2016 to 2021, there was evident spatial clustering of health personnel and various health technical personnel in Chinese maternal and child healthcare institutions, indicating regional imbalances.
Article
Full-text available
Intelligent mines require much more mental effort from visual display terminal (VDT) operators. Long periods of mental effort can easily result in operator fatigue, which further increases the possibility of operation error. Therefore, research into how brain fatigue affects the sustained attention of VDT operators in intelligent mines is important. The research methods were as follows: (1) Recruit 17 intelligent mine VDT operators as subjects. Select objective physiological indicators, such as reaction time, error rate, task duration, flicker fusion frequency, heart rate, electrodermal activity, and blink frequency, and combine these with the subjective Karolinska Sleepiness Scale to build a comprehensive brain fatigue evaluation system. (2) According to the fatigue-inducing experiment requirements, subjects are required to carry out mathematical operations in accordance with the rules during the presentation time, determine whether the results of the operations fall within the [7, 13] interval, and continue for 120 min to induce brain fatigue. (3) Perform the standard stimulus button response experiment of the sustained attention to response task, before and after brain fatigue, and compare each result. The results show that: (1) When the standard stimulus appeared in the EEG experiment, the amplitude of the early N100 component before and after brain fatigue was significantly different. When the bias stimulus appeared, the average amplitudes of the P200 component and the late positive component, before and after brain fatigue, were significantly different, suggesting that the brain fatigue of VDT workers in coal mines would reduce sustained attention; (2) After the 120 min of the continuous operation task, the subjects showed obvious brain fatigue. The objective brain fatigue was followed by an increase in reaction time, an increase in error rate, a decrease in flicker fusion frequency, an increase in heart rate, an increase in electrodermal current, an increase in the number of blinks, and a larger pupil diameter, and both the subjective and objective data indicated more significant changes in the subjects’ brain fatigue at the 45th and 90th min. The results of the study could provide insight into the reduction in operational efficiency and safety of VDT operators in intelligent mines due to brain fatigue and further enrich the research in the area of brain fatigue in VDT operations.
Article
Full-text available
We tested the plausibility of a cognitive-emotional model to understand the effects of messages framed in terms of gain, non-loss, non-gain, and loss, and related to the health consequences of red/processed meat consumption. A total of 544 Italian participants reported their attitude toward reduced red/processed meat consumption and intention to eat red/processed meat (time 1 questionnaire). One week later, participants were randomly assigned to four different message conditions: (a) gain messages focused on the positive health outcomes associated with low meat consumption; (b) non-loss messages focused on the avoided negative health outcomes associated with low meat consumption; (c) non-gain messages focused on the missed positive health outcomes associated with high meat consumption; (d) loss messages focused on the negative health outcomes associated with high meat consumption (message sending). After reading the messages, participants answered a series of questions regarding their emotional and cognitive reactions to the messages, their evaluation of the messages, and again their attitude and intention toward red/processed meat consumption (time 2 questionnaire). Comparing different multivariate linear models under the Bayesian approach, we selected the model with the highest plausibility conditioned to observed data. In this model, message-induced fear influenced systematic processing, which in turn positively influenced message evaluation and attitude, leading to reduced intention to consume red/processed meat. Vice versa, message-induced anger reduced systematic processing, which in turn negatively influenced message evaluation, and led to no effect on attitude and intention. The comparison among message conditions showed that gain and non-loss messages activated integrated emotional and cognitive processing of the health recommendation, while loss and non-gain messages mainly activated emotional shortcuts toward attitude and intention. Overall, these results advance our comprehension of the effects of message framing on receivers' attitudes and intentions.
Article
Full-text available
As a reading comprehension strategy, mind mapping has a positive influence on the development of students’ reading ability. However, how mind mapping affects reading ability has not been well documented. In this study, we used eye tracking sensors to explore mind mapping’s effects on reading ability. The participants were foreign language learning university students in Dalian city, China. One group received foreign language reading teaching integrated with mind mapping training (experimental group), and the other group received regular foreign language reading teaching (control group). We analyzed eye movement indicators, including fixation-related indicators (number of fixations, fixation frequency, and mean fixation duration), regression count, saccade amplitude, and pupil diameter. In addition, the analysis of heat maps and fixation trajectory maps, which are specific tool for visualization of eye movement data and intuitive analysis of reading process, were explained. The results show that the number of fixations, fixation frequency, mean fixation duration, and regression count in the experimental group were all lower than in the control group, and the pupil diameter was larger than in the control group. The heat map and fixation trajectory map show convergence, mostly focusing on the position of keywords and key sentences, with relatively large saccade amplitude and more information obtained by one gaze. Moreover, they had a higher skipping reading rate, which enhanced reading speed to obtain information accurately and quickly. These empirical results indicate that mind mapping training was an effective method for improving students’ reading ability.
Article
Full-text available
To understand the influence of user interface on task performance and situation awareness, three levels of user interface were designed based on the three-level situation awareness model for the 3-player diner’s dilemma game. The 3-player diner's dilemma is a multiplayer version of the prisoner's dilemma, in which participants play games with two computer players and try to achieve high scores. A total of 117 participants were divided into 3 groups to participate in the experiment. Their task performance (the dining points) was recorded and their situation awareness scores were measured with the Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique. The results showed that (1) the level-3 interface effectively improved the task performance and situation awareness scores, while the level-2 interface failed to improve them; (2) the practice effect did exist in all three conditions; and (3) the levels of user interface had no effect on the task learning process, implying that the learning rules remained consistent across different conditions.
Article
Full-text available
The fundamental approach to improve pilots’ situation awareness (SA) would be to reorganize and restructure the presentation of information to fit pilot's cognitive model on the flight deck. This would facilitate pilots’ perception, understanding, and projection, hence making it easier to find the relevant targets. Sixty pilots (30 B‐737 pilots and 30 B‐777 pilots) participated in this study to investigate pilots’ SA while interacting with digital displays and moving pointed needle displays on cabin pressurization system. The results have shown significant differences on pilots’ perception, understanding, and overall SA between digital display and pointed display on the flight deck. Pilots significantly preferred the digital design cabin pressurization system, which is consistent with the proximity compatibility principle, and the position of the display on the center instrument panel is easily accessible to both pilots and does not require large head movements. There are some recommendations on the cabin pressurization design including the size of outflow valve position indicator, which should be significantly increased to provided saliency of information; color coding should be used on cabin altitude and differential pressure indicator to mark critical cabin altitude; and standard operating procedures shall include cabin altitude and differential pressure reading by pilot monitoring. The final and completed solution to the issues on the cabin pressurization system is to redesign the scattered pointed displays as integrated digital displays to fit the human‐centered principle.
Article
Learning words through contextual inference is a key way to enlarge one’s vocabulary especially for adults. However, few studies focused on the effects of different information contained in contexts on novel word learning. The present study used behavioral and fNIRS techniques to investigate the influences of positive, neutral and negative emotions inherent in self-related or other-related referential contexts. Participants were asked to perform a semantic consistency and a source judgment task after learning the relations between novel words and concepts in different contexts. The results showed that self-reference during lexical encoding could promote word learning generally. More importantly, there existed a self-positivity bias which is manifested in the significant interactions between contextual emotions and referential value. These interactions are related to the neural activities of the DLPFC and IFG. These results revealed the contextual information’s integrative contributions to semantic meaning acquisition and episodic source memory related with novel word learning.
Article
Purpose The current study has three main purposes: (1) replicate results from prior framing effects studies on social media. To do so, the study examines the influence of news frames (free speech vs. public order) on participants' attitudes toward an alt-right rally (2) expand prior research by examining the emotional reaction of participants to these frames and (3) probe the moderating effects of face-to-face heterogenous talk and heterogenous social media feeds. Design/methodology/approach Drawing from theoretical concepts such as competitive framing, emotions and heterogeneity, the study uses a randomized online experiment. The study examines a conversation in a Twitter thread that includes both free speech and public order frames in the comments to the thread. The total number of participants was 275. Findings The results show that free speech versus public order frame did not impact attitudes of the participants toward the alt-right rally. Findings also show the significant main effects of free speech and public order frames and the interaction of exposure to heterogeneity on emotional reactions of outrage and anger toward the alt-right rally. These findings suggest that framing research needs to take social media features into consideration for a complete picture of framing effects on social media. Originality/value Using a classic framing effects experiment, the study includes variables relevant to social media discussions on Twitter and examined the moderating effects of heterogeneity on emotional reactions. In addition, one of the important methodological contributions of the current study are the framing manipulations for an externally valid experimental design.
Article
Objective To examine evidence of sensitivity, predictiveness, and methodological concerns regarding direct, objective measures of situation awareness (SA). Background The ability to objectively measure SA is important to the evaluation of user interfaces and displays, training programs, and automation initiatives, as well as for studies that seek to better understand SA in both individuals and teams. A number of methodological criticisms have been raised creating significant confusion in the research field. Method A meta-analysis of 243 studies was conducted to examine evidence of sensitivity and predictiveness, and to address methodological questions regarding Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT), Situation Present Assessment Technique (SPAM), and their variants. Results SAGAT and SPAM were found to be equally predictive of performance. SPAM (64%) and real-time probes (73%) were found to have significantly lower sensitivity in comparison to SAGAT (94%). While SAGAT was found not to be overly memory reliant nor intrusive into operator performance, SPAM resulted in problems with intrusiveness in 40% of the studies examined, as well as problems with speed-accuracy tradeoffs, sampling bias, and confounds with workload. Concerns about memory reliance, the utility of these measures for assessing Team SA, and other issues are also addressed. Conclusion SAGAT was found to be a highly sensitive, reliable, and predictive measure of SA that is useful across a wide variety of domains and experimental settings. Application Direct, objective SA measurement provides useful and diagnostic insights for research and design in a wide variety of domains and study objectives.