ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Background and objectives: The Best Possible Self (BPS) has been found to be an effective manipulation to temporarily improve optimism and affect. The BPS has been used in different formats. In some versions, participants just write about their best possible future, while in others this is combined with imagery. An imagery only version has not been tested yet. The aim of the current study was to examine the effectiveness of three different versions of the BPS and their equivalence in improving optimism and affect. Methods: In an online study format, participants (N = 141) were randomly assigned to one of four conditions: (1) writing and imagery BPS; (2) writing BPS; (3) imagery BPS; and (4) a typical day (TD) control condition. Results: Results showed that each BPS condition significantly improved optimism (i.e. increased positive future expectancies and decreased negative future expectancies) and affect (i.e. increased positive affect and decreased negative affect). Equivalence testing showed that all online BPS conditions were equivalent in increasing optimism and affect, thereby confirming that both the writing and imagery elements of the BPS can independently from each other increase optimism and positive affect in a healthy population. Limitations: Only the immediate effects of the BPS formats on increasing optimism and affect were measured. Conclusions: The BPS manipulation can be employed in different ways for potential future exploration, depending on the research question, design and context and/or E-mental health applications for the treatment of individuals suffering from psychological complaints.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 79 (2023) 101837
Available online 23 January 2023
0005-7916/© 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
The effectiveness and equivalence of different versions of a brief online Best
Possible Self (BPS) manipulation to temporary increase optimism and affect
Jantine J.L.M. Boselie
a
,
*
, Linda M.G. Vancleef
b
, Susan van Hooren
a
, Madelon L. Peters
b
a
Clinical Psychology, Open University, 6401 DL, Heerlen, the Netherlands
b
Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, the Netherlands
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Optimism
Positive affect
Best possible self
Writing
Imagery
Online intervention
ABSTRACT
Background and objectives: The Best Possible Self (BPS) has been found to be an effective manipulation to
temporarily improve optimism and affect. The BPS has been used in different formats. In some versions, par-
ticipants just write about their best possible future, while in others this is combined with imagery. An imagery
only version has not been tested yet. The aim of the current study was to examine the effectiveness of three
different versions of the BPS and their equivalence in improving optimism and affect.
Methods: In an online study format, participants (N =141) were randomly assigned to one of four conditions: (1)
writing and imagery BPS; (2) writing BPS; (3) imagery BPS; and (4) a typical day (TD) control condition.
Results: Results showed that each BPS condition signicantly improved optimism (i.e. increased positive future
expectancies and decreased negative future expectancies) and affect (i.e. increased positive affect and decreased
negative affect). Equivalence testing showed that all online BPS conditions were equivalent in increasing opti-
mism and affect, thereby conrming that both the writing and imagery elements of the BPS can independently
from each other increase optimism and positive affect in a healthy population.
Limitations: Only the immediate effects of the BPS formats on increasing optimism and affect were measured.
Conclusions: The BPS manipulation can be employed in different ways for potential future exploration, depending
on the research question, design and context and/or E-mental health applications for the treatment of individuals
suffering from psychological complaints.
1. Introduction
Optimism is the tendency to expect that good things will happen in
the future (Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010). This tendency to hold
global positive future expectancies has been associated with benecial
coping strategies (Rasmussen, Wrosch, Scheier, & Carver, 2006),
applying different coping strategies more exibly (Rasmussen et al.,
2006; Solberg Nes, Segerstrom, & Sephton, 2005), increased subjective
well-being and longevity and decreased illness (King, 2001; Lee et al.,
2019; Rozanski, Bavishi, Kubzansky, & Cohen, 2019). Importantly,
research has shown that optimism is modiable with a manipulation
called the Best Possible Self (BPS; Peters, Flink, Boersma, & Linton,
2010), making it possible to investigate the causal inuence of opti-
mism. The BPS is a positive future thinking technique based on work by
King (2001) in which participants are typically asked to write about and
imagine a life in the future where everything turned out for the best. In
the control condition, participants write about and imagine a typical day
(TD). A recent meta-analysis demonstrated that the BPS manipulation is
effective in increasing optimism and positive affect (Carrillo et al.,
2019).
The BPS has been applied successfully with different delivery modes
(i.e. online or face-to-face; individually or in a group) and with different
components, such as a writing component only or combining writing
and imagery components (Carrillo et al., 2019; Loveday, Lovell, &
Jones, 2018). Both writing and imagery have been shown to have
benecial effects. In the case of writing, results of a meta-analysis
demonstrated that disclosing information, thoughts, and feelings about
personal and meaningful topics through writing (also known as exper-
imental emotional disclosure) has psychological and health benets
(Frattaroli, 2006). Similar to the positive effects of writing, a brief
guided mental imagery exercise has been shown to have benecial ef-
fects on mental health (Bigham, McDannel, Luciano, & Salgado-Lopez,
* Corresponding author. Clinical Psychology, Open University, P.O. Box 2960, 6401 DL, Heerlen, the Netherlands.
E-mail addresses: jantine.boselie@ou.nl (J.J.L.M. Boselie), l.vancleef@maastrichtuniversity.nl (L.M.G. Vancleef), Susan.vanHooren@ou.nl (S. van Hooren),
madelon.peters@maastrichtuniversity.nl (M.L. Peters).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2023.101837
Received 17 January 2022; Received in revised form 23 December 2022; Accepted 21 January 2023
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 79 (2023) 101837
2
2014). In an attempt to maximise the effectiveness of the BPS exercise as
a single session manipulation, Peters et al. (2010) added a 5 min mental
imagery component to the original writing exercise. However, moder-
ator analysis of a recent meta-analysis showed that the BPS manipula-
tion with or without the imagery component yielded similar outcomes
(Carrillo et al., 2019). Nevertheless, the effectiveness of the BPS with
and without the imagery component have not been directly compared in
one study, neither whether imagery alone can be an effective
manipulation.
Since both writing and imagery exercises in general have been shown
to be effective (Bigham et al., 2014; Frattaroli, 2006) but the BPS
manipulation with or without the imagery component yield similar
outcomes (Carrillo et al., 2019), we hypothesised that all three BPS
formats (writing and imagery BPS; writing BPS; imagery BPS) compared
to the control TD condition 1) are effective in improving optimism (i.e.
increasing positive future expectancies and decreasing negative future
expectancies) and affect (i.e. increasing positive affect and decreasing
negative affect), and (2) all three BPS formats are equivalent in effec-
tiveness. Insight into the independent effects of both BPS elements (i.e.
writing and imagery) in improving optimism and affect increases the
ability to apply the BPS more exibly, tailored to study goals and/or
E-mental health applications for the treatment of individuals suffering
from psychological complaints.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
A total of 141 (22 male; mean age =22.1 (SD =7.8) years, age range
1879 years) healthy participants were included in this online study.
Participants were recruited via posters shown at diverse locations at
Maastricht University and advertisements on social media platforms.
Furthermore, psychology students could sign up for the study via a
research participation platform for course credits. Participants were
randomly assigned within the online program (Qualtrics) to one of four
conditions: BPS writing and imagery condition (n =34; 5 male; mean
age =21.3 (SD =6.1) years); BPS writing condition (n =37; 6 male;
mean age =20.6 (SD =2.4) years); BPS imagery condition (n =37; 5
male; mean age =24.5 (SD =12.4) years) and the TD control condition
(n =32; 6 male; mean age =21.7 (SD =6.3) years). One participant did
not disclose her age (BPS imagery condition). Participants could choose
to either execute the study in Dutch (n =48) or in English (n =93),
depending on their native language/preference.
2.2. Optimism manipulation
Participants in the BPS condition were required to either 1) write and
imagine, 2) write or 3) imagine about a life in the future where every-
thing turned out for the best. Participants in the control condition had to
write about a typical day. The BPS instructions were as follows (Peters
et al., 2010; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006): Think about your best
possible self means that you imagine yourself in the future, after everything
has gone as well as it possibly could. You have worked hard and succeeded at
accomplishing all the goals of your life. Think of this as the realisation of your
dreams, and that you have reached your full potential. The TD instructions
were as follows: Think about your typical daymeans that you take notice of
ordinary details of your day that you usually dont think about. These might
include particular classes or meetings you attend to, people you meet, things
you do, typical thoughts you have during the day. Think of this as moving
through your typical day, hour after hour. All participants were requested
to think for 1 min about what to write and/or imagine. Participants were
than instructed to write uninterrupted for 15 min, or imagine for 5 min
or write and imagine their BPS for 20 min (i.e. 15 min writing and 5 min
imagery). Participants in the TD condition were requested to think for 1
min about what to write and imagine, then to write for 15 min, followed
by 5 min of imaging the story they had just nished writing.
2.3. Questionnaires
2.3.1. Optimism
The Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R) (Scheier, Carver, &
Bridges, 1994) measures dispositional optimism and was used in the
current study to examine baseline differences in optimism between the
conditions. It consists of 10 items: three positively phrased items (e.g.
‘Im always optimistic about my future), three negatively phrased items
(e.g. ‘I rarely count on good things happening to me) and four ller
items (e.g. ‘Its important for me to keep busy). The items are rated on a
ve-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
agree). The total LOT-R score is obtained by summation of the scores on
the positively phrased items and the reversed scores on the negatively
phrased items. The total score ranges from 10 to 30. Higher scores reect
higher levels of dispositional optimism. The LOT-R has been demon-
strated to be a reliable and valid measurement instrument (Scheier et al.,
1994). A Dutch and English version of the LOT-R was used. The internal
consistency in this study was satisfactory with a Cronbachs alphas of
0.76 (Dutch version) and 0.84 (English version).
2.3.2. Affect
Affect was measured with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS) (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS consists of 20
items, with 10 items that measure positive affect (PA; e.g., ‘excitedand
‘inspired) and 10 items that measure negative affect (NA; e.g. ‘nervous
and ‘afraid). Participants indicate the degree to which a certain feeling
is present at that moment on a ve-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not
at all) to 5 (extremely). Subscale scores can range from 10 to 50, with
higher scores on NA items reecting higher levels of emotional distress.
In contrast, high PA scores correspond to experiencing more feelings
that are pleasurable. The PANAS subscales have been demonstrated to
be valid and reliable (Crawford & Henry, 2004). Cronbachs alphas in
the present study for the baseline measurement on the positive subscale
were 0.84 (Dutch version) and 0.84 (English version). Cronbachs alphas
for the baseline measurement on the negative subscale were 0.90 (Dutch
version) and 0.83 (English version).
2.3.3. Future expectancies
The Future Expectancies (FEX) scale (Hanssen, Peters, Vlaeyen,
Meevissen, & Vancleef, 2013) is an adaptation of the Subjective Prob-
ability Task (SPT; MacLeod, Byrne, & Valentine, 1996) and measures
positive and negative future expectancies. It consists of 20 items that
make statements about positive (n =10; e.g. ‘people will admire you)
and negative (n =10; e.g. ‘things will not turn out as you had hoped)
future events. The statements cover ve different domains (work,
health, personal, social and general). The items are rated on a
seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all likely to occur) to 7
(extremely likely to occur). Higher scores reect a higher estimated
likelihood of positive (FEX-Pos) or negative (FEX-Neg) future events.
Total scores range from 10 to 70. The internal consistency of the FEX
subscales have been demonstrated to be satisfactory (Hanssen et al.,
2013). Cronbachs alphas in the present study for the baseline mea-
surement on the positive subscale were 0.91 (Dutch version) and 0.85
(English version). Cronbachs alphas for the baseline measurement on
the negative subscale were 0.87 (Dutch version) and 0.83 (English
version).
2.4. Procedure
Participants were informed about the online study procedure. After
they had given consent for their participation, they completed questions
on demographic information, the LOT-R, the FEX and the PANAS. Next,
participants completed one of the four manipulations: writing and im-
agery BPS; writing BPS; and imagery BPS, writing and imagery TD. Af-
terwards, the participants were asked to ll out the FEX and the PANAS
again, to measure the changes in optimism and affect, respectively. The
J.J.L.M. Boselie et al.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 79 (2023) 101837
3
data were collected online using the Qualtrics program. Participants
could indicate whether they wanted to receive 0.5 research points
(available for psychology students of University) or enter a lottery,
where ve participants were randomly selected to receive a
5-euro
voucher. Participants were debriefed via e-mail after data collection for
the study was completed. This research was approved by the
University.
2.5. Data analyses
In total, 243 participants were included in the study. Participants
who provided incomplete data were removed from the dataset (n =96).
Data from six participants were removed (remaining N =141) as the
total duration of the study completion exceeded more than 2 h, while the
expected study duration was estimated to take at most around 26 min
(BPS condition: 1 min contemplating about the story to write, 15 min
writing, 5 min imagery and 5 min to complete the questionnaires).
Durations above 2 h are not considered to represent a conscientious
attempt. Data were checked for normal distribution and reliability an-
alyses were performed on the LOT-R questionnaire, positive and nega-
tive PANAS questionnaires, and the positive and negative FEX
questionnaires. Analysis of variance analyses (ANOVAs) were used to
check for baseline differences between the conditions for optimism
(LOT-R) and age. Sensitivity analysis (Lakens, 2022; Perugini, Gallucci,
& Costantini, 2018) with G*Power (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner,
2007) was performed to determine which effect sizes this study is sen-
sitive to detect, as a priori sample size calculation was not performed.
The effectiveness of the optimism manipulation on increasing positive
and negative future expectancies and positive and negative affect was
tested with repeated measures ANOVAs with condition as a between
subjectsfactor, and time (pre-and post-measurements of the FEX and
PANAS) as a repeated factor. A signicant condition ×time interaction
effect was followed up with an ANOVA with difference scores and
posthoc pairwise comparisons to test the differences between the four
conditions. To examine the changes in the FEX and PANAS scores within
each condition, follow-up analyses were performed using
paired-samples t-tests. Cohens d was calculated (based on the formula d
=t (n1 +n2)/n1n2 df) derived from Nakagawa and Cuthill (2007).
In case of non-signicant differences between the three BPS formats,
equivalence tests were conducted. Additional equivalence testing was
necessary, as failing to nd a signicant difference between groups is not
equal to showing that the groups were comparable in their effect (i.e.
equivalently effective; Lakens, 2017; Lakens, Scheel, & Isager, 2018). To
test whether the BPS groups were equivalent in their effects on
improving optimism and affect, equivalence testing was conducted with
a two one-sided test (TOST) procedure using a predesigned spreadsheet
(Lakens, 2017). In the TOST procedure, an upper and lower equivalence
bound is specied based on the smallest effect size of interest (SESOI).
Given our sample size,
α
(0.05) and desired power (0.9), our SESOI was
determined as Cohens d =0.8, with the resulting equivalence bounds
being d =- 0.8 and d =0.8.
3. Results
3.1. Baseline descriptives
Results of the ANOVAs revealed no signicant differences between
the three BPS formats (BPS writing and imagery (optimism: m =13.9;
SD =4.50; age: m =21.3; SD =6.13), BPS writing (optimism: m =13.9;
SD =4.50; age: m =20.3; SD =6.13), BPS imagery (optimism: m =14.0;
SD =4.42; age: m =24.5; SD =12.42) and TD condition (optimism: m =
15.3; SD =3.75; age: m =21.7; SD =6.29) in optimism level (i.e., Lot-R
questionnaire; F (3, 137) =0.85, p =.47) and age (F (3, 137) =1.77, p =
.16) at baseline. Sensitivity analysis (Lakens, 2022; Perugini et al., 2018)
with G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) indicated that repeated measures
ANOVA (4 groups; 4 measurements) with N =141 would be sensitive to
detect small effects (i.e.,
η
2 =0.014; Cohens d =0.24) with 80% power
(alpha =.05).
3.2. Manipulation effects
The repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a signicant interaction
effect for FEX positive future expectancies (F (3, 137) =5.18, p =.01,
η
p
2
=0.10), FEX negative future expectancies (F (3, 137) =3.69, p =
.01,
η
p
2
=0.08), PANAS PA (F (3, 137) =3.74, p =.01,
η
p
2
=0.08) and
PANAS NA (F (3, 137 =2.80, p =.04,
η
p
2
=0.06). See Table 1 for the
mean (SD) of future expectancies and affect pre- and post-manipulation,
displayed per condition. Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that
all BPS conditions (writing and imagery; writing; imagery) signicantly
differed from the TD condition on the difference scores for FEX positive
future expectancies (p =.03, p <.001, p =.02,respectively), PANAS PA
(p =.03, p =.05, p =.04, respectively) and FEX negative future ex-
pectancies (p =.03, p =.02, respectively), with the exception of the BPS
writing and imagery format compared to the TD condition on FEX
negative future expectancies (p =.28). None of the BPS format condi-
tions differed signicantly from the TD condition on decreasing PANAS
NA (p =.11, p =.06, p =.23, respectively). Follow-up paired t-test
analyses (Table 2) showed that participants in the BPS writing and im-
agery condition, BPS writing and BPS imagery all scored signicantly
higher on positive future expectancies and PA, and lower on negative
future expectancies and NA after the manipulation. Participants in the
TD condition did not score signicantly different on any of the variables
following the TD instruction. To test whether the BPS groups were
equivalent in their effectiveness in increasing FEX future expectancies
and PANAS affect scores, equivalence testing was conducted. Results of
the TOST procedure showed that the BPS conditions were equivalent in
their effectiveness, with the observed effect sizes falling signicantly
within the equivalent bounds of d =- 0.8 and d =0.8. See Table 3 for the
results of the TOST procedure.
4. Discussion
The current studys results show that both elements of the BPS
manipulation, namely writing and imagery, are independently effective
in improving optimism (i.e. increasing positive future expectancies and
decreasing negative future expectancies) and affect (i.e. increasing
positive affect and decreasing negative affect), as every online Best
Possible Self (BPS) manipulation format (i.e., writing and imagery BPS;
writing BPS; imagery BPS) was found to be equivalently effective in
improving optimism and affect. This nding is in line with previous
studies, which have shown that either writing or imagery have bene-
cial effects on emotional and cognitive variables (Bigham et al., 2014;
Conroy & Hagger, 2018; Frattaroli, 2006; King, 2001). In the current
study, we showed that an imagery exercise as short as 5 min may already
have positive effects on well-being, albeit possibly only immediate ef-
fects. Notably, recent meta-analyses showed that there was no evidence
that adding an imagery task increased the benecial effects of a writing
task (Carrillo et al., 2019; Heekerens & Eid, 2021). Similarly, the current
studys effect sizes and equivalence testing do not support the idea that
combining both elements leads to a greater manipulation effect. One
could argue that this nding may not be surprising, as evidence of a
meta-analysis suggests that the efcacy of all psychological bona de
treatments are roughly equivalent (i.e. Dodo bird verdict; Wampold
et al., 1997). However, later meta-analyses and comparison studies have
identied differences between different treatments or found only sup-
port for this premise when looking at secondary outcome variables
(Marcus, OConnell, Norris, & Sawaqdeh, 2014; Tolin, 2010). Further-
more, previous research showed that mental imagery had stronger ef-
fects on emotions and cognitions than verbal processing of the same
material (Holmes, Lang, & Shah, 2009). Hence, current study results add
to previous ndings and shed light on the efcacy of the different BPS
formats.
J.J.L.M. Boselie et al.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 79 (2023) 101837
4
Next to the benets of being able to investigate the causal inuence
of optimism and affect and being able to conduct research online, the
current studys results specically show that the BPS manipulation can
be employed in different and exible ways for potential future explo-
ration, depending on the research question, design and context. The
benet of writing compared to imagery is for example the enhanced
controllability for research leader and/or therapist. A possible benet of
mental imagery is that it can be integrated, employed more easily and is
exible in daily life. Moreover, as both writing and imagery are inde-
pendently effective in improving optimism and affect, one could
consider taking into account the personal preference of the participant.
People differ for example in their ability to imagine (i.e. vividness;
Cumming & Eaves, 2018) or writing ability because of for example
dyslexia and functional illiteracy, making them more inclined to prefer
one method over the other. It should be noted that the current studys
results only refer to the immediate effects after conducting the BPS once.
Future research should examine whether a combined writing and im-
agery BPS intervention, which is applied multiple times and for a longer
duration may have a more benecial effect on the long term. It is
conceivable for example, that for longer durations one may want to
guide participants in their future orientation with a writing component,
but give them the exibility to use the imagery exercise in daily life or
with the purpose of having a booster effect.
Although participants reported signicantly less negative affect
within all the BPS format conditions and not in the TD condition, this
effect failed to reach signicance when the BPS conditions were directly
compared with the TD condition. This is in line with results from a meta-
analysis examining the effects of the BPS manipulation (Carrillo et al.,
2019). The obtained effect size for negative affect was considerably
small (d =0.192), which was even reduced to nil (d =- 0.047) when the
only study with a non-active control group (i.e. waiting list) was
excluded from the analysis. In contrast, the meta-analysis showed a
moderate effect size (d =.511) of the BPS manipulation on positive
affect. The authors suggested that these results imply that the BPS
manipulation might be more effective in increasing positive affect than
in decreasing negative affect, which is in line with the aim of positive
psychology exercises to promote positive emotions (protective factors)
rather than purely focussing on decreasing negative emotions (Layous &
Lyubomirsky, 2014). Surprisingly, all the BPS formats signicantly
decreased negative future expectancies compared to the TD condition,
with the exception of the BPS writing and imagery format. Thus,
although participants in the BPS writing and imagery format reported
signicantly less negative future expectancies, this effect was not
signicantly different from participants in the TD condition. It is unclear
why this effect was found solely for the BPS writing and imagery format.
Previous studies have repeatedly found that participants in the writing
Table 1
Mean (SD) of future expectancies and affect pre- and post-manipulation, displayed per condition.
BPS writing and imagery (n =34) BPS writing (n =37) BPS imagery (n =38) Typical Day (TD) (n =32)
Pre-
measurement
Post-
measurement
Pre-
measurement
Post-
measurement
Pre-
measurement
Post-
measurement
Pre-
measurement
Post-
measurement
m (SD) m (SD) m (SD) m (SD) m (SD) m (SD) m (SD) m (SD)
Positive future
expectancies
51.32 (9.15) 55.68 (6.64) 49.08 (8.89) 54.49 (7.26) 51.13 (7.84) 55.55 (7.54) 53.22 (6.52) 53.59 (8.56)
Negative future
expectancies
35.09 (11.28) 31.00 (10.07) 36.19 (8.23) 30.78 (8.23) 34.82 (9.16) 29.32 (8.98) 32.75 (8.18) 31.91 (10.09)
Positive affect 32.79 (7.28) 35.26 (6.71) 30.65 (6.84) 32.81 (7.88) 33.76 (5.92) 36.05 (5.67) 34.03 (6.86) 33.28 (7.13)
Negative affect 20.06 (7.88) 16.79 (6.39) 20.49 (6.64) 17.03 (5.81) 19.63 (8.02) 16.76 (7.99) 19.72 (6.92) 19.13 (8.22)
BPS =Best Possible Self; TD =Typical Day (control condition).
Table 2
Results of the paired-samples t-test follow-up analyses on future expectancies and affect, displayed per condition.
BPS writing and imagery BPS writing BPS imagery TD
m (SD) t Cohens
d (CI)
m (SD) t Cohens
d (CI)
m (SD) t Cohens
d (CI)
m (SD) t Cohens
d (CI)
Positive future
expectancies
4.35
(5.66)
4.49** 1.56
(1.012.11)
5.41
(5.75)
5.72** 1.91
(1.352.46)
4.42
(5.42)
5.03** 1.68
(1.142.21)
.38
(5.77)
.37 0.13
(.37.63)
Negative future
expectancies
4.09
(7.99)
2.98* 1.04
(.521.55)
5.41
(5.62)
5.85** 1.95
(1.392.51)
5.50
(5.24)
6.47** 2.16
(1.582.74)
.84
(7.28)
.66 0.24
(.26.74)
Positive affect 2.47
(5.41)
2.67* 0.93
(.421.44)
2.16
(4.89)
2.69* 0.90
(.411.38)
2.29
(4.21)
3.35** 1.12
(.621.61)
.75
(3.39)
1.25 0.45
(.06.95)
Negative affect 3.27
(5.11)
3.72** 1.30
(.761.83)
3.46
(4.99)
4.22** 1.41 (.89-
1,92)
2.87
(3.86)
4.58** 1.53
(1.002.05)
.59
(4.10)
.82 0.29
(.21.80)
BPS =Best Possible Self; TD =Typical Day (control condition). CI =95% condence interval of Cohens d. * =p <.05; ** =p <.001.
Table 3
Results of the equivalence testing.
Positive future expectancies Negative future expectancies Positive affect Negative affect
d t df p d t df p d t df p d t df p
BPS writing and imagery &
BPS writing
0.19 2.59 68,66 0.006 0.19 2.54 58,70 0.007 0.06 3.11 66,69 0.001 0.04 3.21 68,18 0.001
BPS writing and imagery &
BPS imagery
0.01 3.33 68,34 0.001 0.21 2.48 55,90 0.008 0.04 3.21 62,17 0.001 0.09 2.99 61,09 0.002
BPS writing & BPS imagery 0.18 2.63 68 0.005 0.02 3.31 67,75 0.001 0.03 3,25 65,57 0.001 0.13 2.81 61,96 0.003
BPS =Best Possible Self, d =observed Cohens d. Note: the BPS conditions are equivalent in their effects on all the variables, as the scores fall within the equivalence
bounds d =- 0.8 and d =0.8. Based on the output of a TOST spreadsheet (Lakens, 2017).
J.J.L.M. Boselie et al.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 79 (2023) 101837
5
and imagery BPS compared to participants in the TD condition report
less negative future expectancies after the BPS exercise (e.g. Boselie,
Vancleef, & Peters, 2016, 2017; Boselie, Vancleef, Smeets, & Peters,
2014; Hanssen et al., 2013).
Having both BPS elements being independently effective increases
the ability to apply the BPS manipulation more exibly, tailored to a
study design, but also increases the exibility for the integration of the
BPS in online interventions or E-mental health applications for the
treatment of individuals suffering from psychological complaints. Pre-
vious research has shown that positive psychology exercises in general
can be applied in the clinical setting as a stand-alone intervention. The
BPS manipulation has been successfully incorporated in larger positive
psychology interventions (Draven, Moliver, & Thompson, 2015; Peters
et al., 2017; Pietrowsky & Mikutta, 2012) in patients with depression or
combating chronic pain conditions and has been implemented as an
adjuvant to contemporary psychological treatment approaches such as
cognitive behavioural therapy (Bannink, 2018; Geschwind, Arntz,
Bannink, & Peeters, 2019). The Best Possible Self (social relationships: i.
e. imagine and write about your best possible future interpersonal re-
lationships) exercise signicantly reduced hopelessness and increased
optimism in a population of suicidal patients that were admitted to an
inpatient psychiatric unit (Huffman et al., 2014). Having the option to
exibly adjust the BPS exercise to the patient and/or therapist prefer-
ence increases the possible clinical application possibilities. It should be
noted that the BPS exercise is often adjusted in clinical populations in
order to account for the potentially harmful impact of eliciting aware-
ness between a self-perceived greater discrepancy (Self Discrepancy
Theory; Higgins, 1989) between the actual self (i.e. who you are now)
and the ideal self (i.e. who you want to be). For instance, the BPS in-
structions for chronic pain patients were more directive and concrete
and they were asked to formulate what their best possible future will
look like, despite pain (Peters et al., 2017). Future research is necessary to
examine the BPS as a stand-alone intervention, and whether modied
instructions are necessary in a clinical setting and what the long-term
clinical effects are.
The ndings of this study have to be seen in the light of some limi-
tations. First, the generalisability of present ndings are limited, as the
sample consisted of predominantly younger adults with the same cul-
tural background (Western Europe). Second, a priori sample size
calculation was not performed. Sensitivity analysis indicated that this
study would not be able to reliably detect effects smaller than
η
2 =
0.014/Cohens d =0.24. A third limitation concerns the lack of follow-up
measurements. The current study assessed only the immediate effects of
the BPS intervention. Additionally, it is unclear whether there is a dose-
response relationship between the number of times the BPS exercise is
repeated and the effectiveness of the BPS exercise or whether there are
additional benets of combining writing and imagery components when
applying the BPS multiple times. Adding short and long-term measure-
ments provides the opportunity to exclude that the benecial effects of
the BPS are merely due to a priming effect and/or mood induction
(Heekerens & Eid, 2021). Additionally, several pairwise comparisons
were made, creating a risk of alpha ination for individual comparisons.
A common solution is to adjust the signicance threshold (alpha level)
during the null hypothesis, although some have argued this is only
necessary when conducting exploratory research (Cramer et al., 2016)
or if researchers make a decision about a joint null hypothesis after
rejecting at least one (and not all) constituent null hypotheses (i.e.
disjunction testing; Rubin, 2021). Finally, online assessment enables less
control over the task and procedure. However, the current study results
demonstrated that the BPS manipulation can be successfully deployed in
an online setting, which is in line with previous research (Carrillo et al.,
2019; Loveday et al., 2018). In a meta-analysis of experimental disclo-
sure, psychological health effect sizes actually tended to be larger when
studies had participants disclose at home compared to a controlled
setting (Frattaroli, 2006). The author suggested that participants feel
more comfortable at home, which as a result, may increase engagement
in the disclosure process.
In conclusion, both BPS elements (i.e. writing and imagery) were
successful both in combination and independently from each other in
increasing optimism and positive affect in a healthy population, making
it possible to apply the BPS exibly, considering the research question
and design. Future research is necessary to examine the effects of
employing an online BPS to treat psychological complaints and identify
the possible underlying working mechanisms of the BPS manipulation.
Declaration of interest, and role of funding organizations
This research was funded by grant 022.003.038 from NWO, The
Netherlands Organisation for Scientic Research, awarded to the Dutch-
Flemish Research School of Experimental Psychopathology. There are
no nancial or other relationships that might lead to a conict of
interest.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Jantine J.L.M. Boselie: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal
analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data curation, Writing original
draft. Linda M.G. Vancleef: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing
review & editing. Susan van Hooren: Writing review & editing.
Madelon L. Peters: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing review
& editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition.
Declaration of competing interest
There are no nancial or other relationships that might lead to a
conict of interest in submitting this manuscript. All authors have
contributed read and approved the paper. No other manuscripts from
the same dataset have been submitted or published elsewhere.
Data availability
Data will be made available on request.
References
Bannink, F. (2018). Positieve cognitieve gedrags-therapie. Tijdschrift Voor Psychotherapie,
44(1), 17.
Bigham, E., McDannel, L., Luciano, I., & Salgado-Lopez, G. (2014). Effect of a brief
guided imagery on stress. Biofeedback, 42(1), 2835.
Boselie, J. J., Vancleef, L. M., & Peters, M. L. (2016). The effects of experimental pain and
induced optimism on working memory task performance. Scandinavian Journal of
Pain, 12, 2532.
Boselie, J. J., Vancleef, L. M., & Peters, M. L. (2017). Increasing optimism protects
against pain-induced impairment in task-shifting performance. The Journal of Pain,
18(4), 446455.
Boselie, J. J., Vancleef, L. M., Smeets, T., & Peters, M. L. (2014). Increasing optimism
abolishes pain-induced impairments in executive task performance. Pain, 155(2),
334340.
Carrillo, A., Rubio-Aparicio, M., Molinari, G., Enrique, ´
A., S´
anchez-Meca, J., &
Ba˜
nos, R. M. (2019). Effects of the best possible self intervention: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. PLoS One, 14(9), Article e0222386.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2010). Optimism. Clinical Psychology
Review, 30(7), 879889. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.006
Conroy, D., & Hagger, M. S. (2018). Imagery interventions in health behavior: A meta-
analysis. Health Psychology, 37(7), 668679. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000625
Cramer, A. O. J., van Ravenzwaaij, D., Matzke, D., Steingroever, H., Wetzels, R.,
Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2016). Hidden multiplicity in
exploratory multiway ANOVA: Prevalence and remedies. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 23(2), 640647. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0913-5
Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2004). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large
non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(3), 245265. https://
doi.org/10.1348/0144665031752934
Cumming, J., & Eaves, D. L. (2018). The nature, measurement, and development of
imagery ability. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 37(4), 375393.
Draven, L. T. L., Moliver, N., & Thompson, D. (2015). Happiness intervention decreases
pain and depression, boosts happiness among primary care patients. Primary Health
Care Research & Development, 16(2), 114126.
J.J.L.M. Boselie et al.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 79 (2023) 101837
6
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: A exible
statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191.
Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 823.
Geschwind, N., Arntz, A., Bannink, F., & Peeters, F. (2019). Positive cognitive behavior
therapy in the treatment of depression: A randomized order within-subject
comparison with traditional cognitive behavior therapy. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 116, 119130.
Hanssen, M. M., Peters, M. L., Vlaeyen, J. W., Meevissen, Y. M., & Vancleef, L. M. (2013).
Optimism lowers pain: Evidence of the causal status and underlying mechanisms.
Pain, 154, 5358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.08.006, 0.
Heekerens, J. B., & Eid, M. (2021). Inducing positive affect and positive future
expectations using the best-possible-self intervention: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 16(3), 322347.
Higgins, E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs cause people
to suffer?. In B. Leonard (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp.
93136) Academic Press.
Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Shah, D. M. (2009). Developing interpretation bias
modication as a cognitive vaccinefor depressed mood: Imagining positive events
makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 118(1), 7688. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012590
Huffman, J. C., DuBois, C. M., Healy, B. C., Boehm, J. K., Kashdan, T. B., Celano, C. M.,
Lyubomirsky, S. (2014). Feasibility and utility of positive psychology exercises for
suicidal inpatients. General Hospital Psychiatry, 36(1), 8894.
King, L. A. (2001). The health benets of writing about life goals. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 27(7), 798807. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201277003
Lakens, D. (2017). Equivalence tests: A practical primer for t tests, correlations, and
meta-analyses. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(4), 355362.
Lakens, D. (2022). Sample size justication. Collabra: Psychology, 8(1), Article 33267.
Lakens, D., Scheel, A. M., & Isager, P. M. (2018). Equivalence testing for psychological
research: A tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2),
259269.
Layous, K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2014). The how, why, what, when, and who of happiness.
Positive emotion: Integrating the light sides and dark sides, 473495.
Lee, L. O., James, P., Zevon, E. S., Kim, E. S., Trudel-Fitzgerald, C., Spiro, A.,
Kubzansky, L. D. (2019). Optimism is associated with exceptional longevity in 2
epidemiologic cohorts of men and women. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 116(37), 1835718362.
Loveday, P. M., Lovell, G. P., & Jones, C. M. (2018). The best possible selves intervention:
A review of the literature to evaluate efcacy and guide future research. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 19(2), 607628.
MacLeod, A. K., Byrne, A., & Valentine, J. D. (1996). Affect, emotional disorder, and
future-directed thinking. Cognition & Emotion, 10, 6986.
Marcus, D. K., OConnell, D., Norris, A. L., & Sawaqdeh, A. (2014). Is the dodo bird
endangered in the 21st century? A meta-analysis of treatment comparison studies.
Clinical Psychology Review, 34(7), 519530.
Nakagawa, S., & Cuthill, I. C. (2007). Effect size, condence interval and statistical
signicance: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews, 82(4), 591605.
Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., & Costantini, G. (2018). A practical primer to power analysis
for simple experimental designs. International Review of Social Psychology, 31(1).
Peters, M. L., Flink, I. K., Boersma, K., & Linton, S. J. (2010). Manipulating optimism: Can
imagining a best possible self be used to increase positive future expectancies? The
Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(3), 204211. https://doi.org/10.1080/
17439761003790963
Peters, M. L., Smeets, E., Feijge, M., van Breukelen, G., Andersson, G., Buhrman, M., et al.
(2017). Happy despite pain: A randomized controlled trial of an 8-week internet-
delivered positive psychology intervention for enhancing well-being in patients with
chronic pain. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 33(11), 962.
Pietrowsky, R., & Mikutta, J. (2012). Effects of positive psychology interventions in
depressive patientsa randomized control study. Psychology, 3(12), 1067.
Rasmussen, H. N., Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2006). Self-regulation
processes and health: The importance of optimism and goal adjustment. Journal of
Personality, 74(6), 17211747. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00426.x
Rozanski, A., Bavishi, C., Kubzansky, L. D., & Cohen, R. (2019). Association of optimism
with cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. JAMA Network Open, 2(9) (]).
Rubin, M. (2021). When to adjust alpha during multiple testing: A consideration of
disjunction, conjunction, and individual testing. Synthese, 199(3), 1096911000.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-021-03276-4
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from
neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the
life orientation test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 10631078.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063
Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to increase and sustain positive emotion:
The effects of expressing gratitude and visualizing best possible selves. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 1(2), 7382. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760500510676
Solberg Nes, L., Segerstrom, S. C., & Sephton, S. E. (2005). Engagement and arousal:
Optimisms effects during a brief stressor. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
31(1), 111120. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271319
Tolin, D. F. (2010). Is cognitivebehavioral therapy more effective than other therapies?:
A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 710720.
Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., Stich, F., Benson, K., & Ahn, H.-n. (1997).
A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona de psychotherapies:
Empirically," all must have prizes. Psychological Bulletin, 122(3), 203.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54(6), 10631070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.54.6.1063
J.J.L.M. Boselie et al.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
An important step when designing an empirical study is to justify the sample size that will be collected. The key aim of a sample size justification for such studies is to explain how the collected data is expected to provide valuable information given the inferential goals of the researcher. In this overview article six approaches are discussed to justify the sample size in a quantitative empirical study: 1) collecting data from (almost) the entire population, 2) choosing a sample size based on resource constraints, 3) performing an a-priori power analysis, 4) planning for a desired accuracy, 5) using heuristics, or 6) explicitly acknowledging the absence of a justification. An important question to consider when justifying sample sizes is which effect sizes are deemed interesting, and the extent to which the data that is collected informs inferences about these effect sizes. Depending on the sample size justification chosen, researchers could consider 1) what the smallest effect size of interest is, 2) which minimal effect size will be statistically significant, 3) which effect sizes they expect (and what they base these expectations on), 4) which effect sizes would be rejected based on a confidence interval around the effect size, 5) which ranges of effects a study has sufficient power to detect based on a sensitivity power analysis, and 6) which effect sizes are expected in a specific research area. Researchers can use the guidelines presented in this article, for example by using the interactive form in the accompanying online Shiny app, to improve their sample size justification, and hopefully, align the informational value of a study with their inferential goals.
Article
Full-text available
Psychologists must be able to test both for the presence of an effect and for the absence of an effect. In addition to testing against zero, researchers can use the two one-sided tests (TOST) procedure to test for equivalence and reject the presence of a smallest effect size of interest (SESOI). The TOST procedure can be used to determine if an observed effect is surprisingly small, given that a true effect at least as extreme as the SESOI exists. We explain a range of approaches to determine the SESOI in psychological science and provide detailed examples of how equivalence tests should be performed and reported. Equivalence tests are an important extension of the statistical tools psychologists currently use and enable researchers to falsify predictions about the presence, and declare the absence, of meaningful effects.
Article
Full-text available
Scientists often adjust their significance threshold (alpha level) during null hypothesis significance testing in order to take into account multiple testing and multiple comparisons. This alpha adjustment has become particularly relevant in the context of the replication crisis in science. The present article considers the conditions in which this alpha adjustment is appropriate and the conditions in which it is inappropriate. A distinction is drawn between three types of multiple testing: disjunction testing, conjunction testing, and individual testing. It is argued that alpha adjustment is only appropriate in the case of disjunction testing, in which at least one test result must be significant in order to reject the associated joint null hypothesis. Alpha adjustment is inappropriate in the case of conjunction testing, in which all relevant results must be significant in order to reject the joint null hypothesis. Alpha adjustment is also inappropriate in the case of individual testing, in which each individual result must be significant in order to reject each associated individual null hypothesis. The conditions under which each of these three types of multiple testing is warranted are examined. It is concluded that researchers should not automatically (mindlessly) assume that alpha adjustment is necessary during multiple testing. Illustrations are provided in relation to joint studywise hypotheses and joint multiway ANOVAwise hypotheses.
Article
Full-text available
Importance Optimism and pessimism can be easily measured and are potentially modifiable mindsets that may be associated with cardiovascular risk and all-cause mortality. Objective To conduct a meta-analysis and systematic review of the association between optimism and risk for future cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality. Data Sources and Study Selection PubMed, Scopus, and PsycINFO electronic databases were systematically searched from inception through July 2, 2019, to identify all cohort studies investigating the association between optimism and pessimism and cardiovascular events and/or all-cause mortality by using the following Medical Subject Heading terms: optimism, optimistic explanatory style, pessimism, outcomes, endpoint, mortality, death, cardiovascular events, stroke, coronary artery disease, coronary heart disease, ischemic heart disease, and cardiovascular disease. Data Extraction and Synthesis Data were screened and extracted independently by 2 investigators (A.R. and C.B.). Adjusted effect estimates were used, and pooled analysis was performed using the Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman random-effects model. Sensitivity and subgroup analyses were performed to assess the robustness of the findings. The Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) reporting guideline was followed. Main Outcomes and Measures Cardiovascular events included a composite of fatal cardiovascular mortality, nonfatal myocardial infarction, stroke, and/or new-onset angina. All-cause mortality was assessed as a separate outcome. Results The search yielded 15 studies comprising 229 391 participants of which 10 studies reported data on cardiovascular events and 9 studies reported data on all-cause mortality. The mean follow-up period was 13.8 years (range, 2-40 years). On pooled analysis, optimism was significantly associated with a decreased risk of cardiovascular events (relative risk, 0.65; 95% CI, 0.51-0.78; P < .001), with high heterogeneity in the analysis (I² = 87.4%). Similarly, optimism was significantly associated with a lower risk of all-cause mortality (relative risk, 0.86; 95% CI, 0.80-0.92; P < .001), with moderate heterogeneity (I² = 73.2%). Subgroup analyses by methods for assessment, follow-up duration, sex, and adjustment for depression and other potential confounders yielded similar results. Conclusions and Relevance The findings suggest that optimism is associated with a lower risk of cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality. Future studies should seek to better define the biobehavioral mechanisms underlying this association and evaluate the potential benefit of interventions designed to promote optimism or reduce pessimism.
Article
Full-text available
The Best Possible Self (BPS) exercise promotes a positive view of oneself in the best possible future, after working hard towards it. Since the first work that attempted to examine the benefits of this intervention in 2001, studies on the BPS have grown exponentially and, currently , this is one of the most widely used Positive Psychology Interventions. However, little is yet known about its overall effectiveness in increasing wellbeing outcomes. Thus, the aim of this meta-analysis is to shed light on this question. A systematic literature search was conducted, and 29 studies (in 26 articles) met the inclusion criteria of empirically testing the intervention and comparing it to a control condition. In addition, BPS was compared to gratitude interventions in some of the included studies. A total of 2,909 participants were involved in the analyses. The outcome measures were wellbeing, optimism, depressive symptoms, and positive and negative affect. Results showed that the BPS is an effective intervention to improve wellbeing (d + = .325), optimism (d + = .334) and positive affect (d + = .511) comparing to controls. Small effect sizes were obtained for negative affect and depressive symptoms. Moderator analyses did not show statistically significant results for wellbeing, except for a trend towards significance in the age of the participants (years) and the magnitude of the intervention (total minutes of practice). In addition, the BPS was found to be more beneficial for positive and negative affect than gratitude interventions (d + = .326 and d + = .485, respectively). These results indicate that the BPS can be considered a valuable Positive Psychology Intervention to improve clients' wellbeing, and it seems that it might be more effective for older participants and with shorter practices (measured as total minutes of practice).
Article
Full-text available
Previous research suggests that a stronger focus on positive emotions and positive mental health may improve efficacy of Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT). Objectives were to compare differential improvement of depressive symptoms (primary outcome), positive affect, and positive mental health indices during positive CBT (P-CBT; CBT in a solution-focused framework, amplified with optional positive psychology exercises) versus traditional, problem-focused CBT (T-CBT). Forty-nine patients with major depressive disorder (recruited in an outpatient mental health care facility specialized in mood disorders) received two treatment blocks of eight sessions each (cross-over design, order randomized). Intention-To-Treat mixed regression modelling indicated that depressive symptoms improved similarly during the first, but significantly more in P-CBT compared to T-CBT during the second treatment block. Rate of improvement on the less-frequently measured secondary outcomes was not significantly different. However, P-CBT was associated with significantly higher rates of clinically significant or reliable change for depression, negative affect, and happiness. Effect sizes for the combined treatment were large (pre-post Cohen's d = 2.71 for participants ending with P-CBT, and 1.85 for participants ending with T-CBT). Positive affect, optimism, subjective happiness and mental health reached normative population averages after treatment. Overall, findings suggest that explicitly focusing on positive emotions efficiently counters depressive symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
Power analysis is an important tool to use when planning studies. This contribution aims to remind readers what power analysis is, emphasize why it matters, and articulate when and how it should be used. The focus is on applications of power analysis for experimental designs often encountered in psychology, starting from simple two-group independent and paired groups and moving to one-way analysis of variance, factorial designs, contrast analysis, trend analysis, regression analysis, analysis of covariance, and mediation analysis. Special attention is given to the application of power analysis to moderation designs, considering both dichotomous and continuous predictors and moderators. Illustrative practical examples based on G*Power and R packages are provided throughout the article. Annotated code for the examples with R and dedicated computational tools are made freely available at a dedicated web page (https://github.com/mcfanda/primerPowerIRSP). Applications of power analysis for more complex designs are briefly mentioned, and some important general issues related to power analysis are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Psychologists must be able to test both for the presence of an effect and for the absence of an effect. In addition to testing against zero, researchers can use the two one-sided tests (TOST) procedure to test for equivalence and reject the presence of a smallest effect size of interest (SESOI). The TOST procedure can be used to determine if an observed effect is surprisingly small, given that a true effect at least as extreme as the SESOI exists. We explain a range of approaches to determine the SESOI in psychological science and provide detailed examples of how equivalence tests should be performed and reported. Equivalence tests are an important extension of the statistical tools psychologists currently use and enable researchers to falsify predictions about the presence, and declare the absence, of meaningful effects.
Article
The best-possible-self intervention has been shown to effectively increase positive affect and optimism. Differences in timing and conceptualization of outcome assessments, however, complicate interpretations regarding the practical significance of these effects. To address this issue, we conducted a systematic literature search and included 34 randomized controlled trials into several meta-analyses. We coded the exact time of measurement and how outcomes were assessed. Results reveal small overall effects on positive affect (Hedge’s g = 0.28, 95% CI [0.16, 0.41]) and optimism (g = 0.21, 95% CI [0.04, 0.38]). Effects on positive affect were strongest among studies that assessed momentary affect immediately after the intervention, whereas effects on optimism were only significant if conceptualized as positive future expectations rather than a general orientation in life. Descriptive results indicate no substantial follow-up effects. Thus, the best-possible-self intervention might be thought of as a mood/expectation induction procedure. Further development may lead to sustained effects.
Article
Significance Optimism is a psychological attribute characterized as the general expectation that good things will happen, or the belief that the future will be favorable because one can control important outcomes. Previous studies reported that more optimistic individuals are less likely to suffer from chronic diseases and die prematurely. Our results further suggest that optimism is specifically related to 11 to 15% longer life span, on average, and to greater odds of achieving “exceptional longevity,” that is, living to the age of 85 or beyond. These relations were independent of socioeconomic status, health conditions, depression, social integration, and health behaviors (e.g., smoking, diet, and alcohol use). Overall, findings suggest optimism may be an important psychosocial resource for extending life span in older adults.