Content uploaded by Özlem Yeter
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Özlem Yeter on Feb 02, 2023
Content may be subject to copyright.
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
144
Threat of War on Cognitive Development
of Refugee Children
Özlem Yeter a, *, Hugh Rabagliati b, Duygu Özge a
a Middle East Technical University, Turkey
b University of Edinburgh, UK
Received August 25, 2022; Revised September 7, 2022; Accepted December 18, 2022
Abstract. War trauma is often accompanied by poor living conditions in the new environment in
a manner preserving or even deteriorating the negative influences of war. Several researchers have
investigated the refugee experiences of displaced children. Often they have focused on the detrimental
effects of war on psychological well-being, mental health, educational settings, social adaptation, quality
of nutrition, financial difficulties, safety and language learning experiences. Each of these effects has
been proven to negatively affect cognitive abilities; however, the current study reviews the key studies
to reveal the cognitive and linguistic outcomes of holding refugee status in the early childhood period.
Doing this, we aim to reveal the adverse conditions that affect refugee children’s three core abilities of
executive functions, namely working memory, inhibitory control and shifting. In addition to cognitive
outcomes, we present the factors that have an impact on these children’s native language development
and their experiences with the language spoken in the host country in the context of schooling. This
study suggests that refugee children should be assessed for their cognitive and language abilities after
arriving in the country of resettlement so that their needs can be identified and addressed effectively.
Caretakers should also be given both psychological and financial support to enrich their children’s
language and cognitive input. Also, the outcomes of the research in this field should be effectively
shared with different stakeholders from the caregivers and teachers of the refugee children to the NGOs
and policymakers responsible to take solid actions to counter the adverse effects of displacement.
Keywords: refugee children, cognitive development, war trauma, executive function, language
development.
Єтер Озлем, Рабаґліаті Г’ю, Озґе Дуйґу. Війна як загроза когнітивному розвитку
дітей-біженців.
Анотація. Воєнну травму часто супроводжують погані умови життя в новому середовищі,
які зберігають або й погрішують негативні наслідки війни. Деякі дослідники вивчали досвід
дітей-біженців у статусі переміщених осіб. Часто автори зосереджувалися на згубних наслідках
війни для психологічного благополуччя дитини, її психічного здоров’я, освітніх умов, соціальної
адаптації, якості харчування, фінансових труднощів, безпеки та проблемах вивчення мови.
Доведено, що кожен із названих аспектів негативно впливає на когнітивні здібності. Ця праця
має за мету проаналізувати ключові дослідження, аби з'ясувати когнітивні та лінгвістичні
наслідки перебування в статусі біженця в період раннього дитинства. Автори прагнули виявити
несприятливі умови, які впливають на три основні екзекутивні функції мозку дітей-біженців, а
саме: оперативну пам’ять, гальмівний контроль і зсув. Окрім когнітивних наслідків,
* Corresponding author. Özlem Yeter, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2184-3192, E-mail: o.yeter@rug.nl
© Yeter, Özlem, Rabagliati, Hugh; Özge, Duygu 2022. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0).
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics, 9(2), 144–159. https://doi.org/10.29038/eejpl.2022.9.2.yet
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
145
обговорюємо чинники, які можуть вплинути на розвиток рідної мови у цих дітей та їхній досвід
спілкування мовою, якою розмовляють у приймаючій країні, у контексті шкільного навчання. Це
дослідження засвідчує про те, що у дітей-біженців слід оцінювати їхні когнітивні та мовні
здібності після прибуття в країну переселення, щоб можна було визначити їхні потреби та
ефективно задовольнити їх. Піклувальникам також слід надавати і психологічну, і фінансову
підтримку, щоб вони могли збагатити мову та когніцію їхніх дітей. Крім того, результати
дослідження в цій царині слід активно поширювати серед різних зацікавлених сторін, – від
піклувальників і вчителів дітей-біженців до громадських організацій та політиків,
відповідальних за прийняття рішучих заходів для протидії негативним наслідкам переміщення.
Ключові слова: діти-біженці, когнітивний розвиток, екзекутивна функція, розвиток мови,
воєнна травма.
Introduction
Even today, wars affect millions of people and force them to seek asylum
outside their countries: 6.8 million people from The Syrian Arab Republic,
4.6 million from Venezuela, 2.7 million people from Afghanistan, 1.2 people from
Myanmar, and very recently 6.3 million people from Ukraine (UNHCR, 2021b) were
forced to flee and resettle in countries that are new to them. More than 27.1 million
people worldwide are in refugee status, around half of which consist of children
under 18 (UNHCR, 2021b). This number reaches as high as 89.3 million when
internally and externally displaced people, stateless people and asylum seekers are
also included (UNHCR, 2021). These individuals go through difficult and sometimes
life-threatening experiences such as torture, physical assault, fear, malnutrition,
separation from family, loss of loved ones, loss of property, displacement, harsh
living conditions, lack of health care and lack of education (Klugman, 2022; UN,
2014; WHO, 2021). The aftermath of war remains to be stressful even after
resettlement in a new country as it is the beginning of another challenging journey.
Throughout this journey in the host country, refugees encounter several problems that
may cause them experience excessive stress: poverty, social integration difficulties,
language barrier, and discrimination (Hadfield et al., 2017; Şafak-Ayvazoğlu,
Kunuroglu, & Yağmur, 2021; Tummala-Narra & Claudius, 2013).
Children’s cognition is especially more vulnerable to adverse experiences as
they are still in a developmental phase (Brown et al., 2012; Woodburn et al., 2021).
Although several action plans addressing financial, health and safety problems of
refugee populations have been made (e.g., European Commission, 2016; UNICEF,
2019), there is no comprehensive action plan addressing the enhancement of
cognitive development of refugee children (Brown et al., 2012; Mehnert et al., 2013;
Woodburn et al., 2021). It is highly crucial that the cognitive needs of refugee
children are addressed because early cognitive skills predict later life achievements
(Blair & Razza, 2007; Sasser, Bierman, & Heinrichs, 2015), physical health (Batty,
Deary, & Gottfredson, 2007; Miller, Barnes, & Beaver, 2011) and social adaptability
(Fong & Iarocci, 2020; Gligorović & Buha Ðurović, 2014).
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
146
Method
This paper aims to bring together findings from various disciplines related to the
cognitive and linguistic outcomes of holding refugee status. Throughout this review,
we aim to reveal the adverse conditions that affect refugee children’s three core
executive functions, namely working memory, inhibitory control and shifting, ii)
present the factors that have an impact on their language development, and iii) to
present possible directions for future research.
Results and Discussion
Effect of Refugee Status on Executive Functioning
The term executive function (EF) is used to refer to cognitive processes
including working memory (WM), inhibitory control (IC) and shifting ability that is
responsible for purposeful, goal-oriented activity enabling physical, cognitive and
emotional self-control (Corbett et al., 2009; Diamond, 2013; Lezak, 1995). Being a
sub-component of short-term memory, WM deals with the manipulation of
information while processing a complex cognitive task (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974).
The second core EF reviewed in this paper is IC, which refers to the ability “to
control one’s attention, behaviour, thoughts, and/or emotions to override a strong
internal predisposition or external lure, and instead do what’s more appropriate or
needed” (Diamond, 2013, p. 137). As for shifting, it is the ability to shift between two
or more competing mind-sets, environments or situations selectively and
appropriately (Davidson et al., 2006; Scott, 1962).
Traumatic Experiences
EFs may be hindered by depression, stress and traumatic experiences (Ilonen et
al., 2000). Refugees are specifically prone to mental health problems due to life-
threatening events they go through both following and prior to their arrival in the host
country (Eruyar, Maltby, & Vostanis, 2018; Özer et al., 2016). Their EFs are likely to
be adversely affected too (Park et al., 2014). This effect become especially prominent
if adversities are experienced in the early years of life because the brain is still in the
process of maturation, which puts children’s cognitive functioning in a vulnerable
position (Bick & Nelson, 2016). For instance, in a recent study with children who
were displaced before the age of 5 because of the Syrian war, refugee children
performed poorer on their WM, IC and shifting abilities than their non-refugee peers
(Yeter, Rabagliati, & Özge; 2021). This is one of the first pieces of evidence showing
that war trauma experienced at early ages that are critical for brain maturation may
influence EFs negatively. In line with this finding, Gabrys, Dixon, & Anisman (2017)
could find no association between trauma and shifting ability for university students
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
147
who experienced trauma at the age of 6 and older whereas the ones reporting adverse
traumatic experiences before the age of 5 had more difficulty in shifting tasks. This
highlights that early childhood period is particularly sensitive to trauma exposure (see
also; Pang et al., 2014; Sack et al., 1996; Skowron et al., 2014; Tuncer, 2021).
Parents’ or caretakers’ psychological well-being is another significant factor in
children’s mental health and their cognitive development. Refugee caregivers might
be depressed, traumatised and stressed due to cumulative adverse experiences both
before and after migration to the country of asylum (Browne et al., 2017; Gredebäck
et al., 2021). Bryant and colleagues (2018) interviewed 411 refugee caretakers
regarding their trauma history and postmigration difficulties and they found that
individuals with greater trauma exposure had harsher parenting styles, which led to
higher levels of hyperactivity and emotional problems in children. Several other
studies found parallel findings such that harsh and inattentive parenting may cause
attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which crucially leads children to
lag behind their typically developing peers in their WM and IC (Joseph et al., 2021;
Nyman et al., 2010). Yet, considering the third component of EF, namely shifting
ability, the findings are inconsistent (Elosúa, Del Olmo, & Contreras, 2017; Oades &
Christiansen, 2008; cf., Irwin et al., 2019). Moreover, adverse experiences of
caretakers may also result in neglected and uninvolved parenting. This type of
parenting style may adversely impact children’s psychological and cognitive well-
being too (Garber, 2006; Locke et al., 1996; Hermansen et al., 2022; Sulik et al.,
2015).
Schooling
School is the first place children step their foot out of their houses, where they
socialize with their peers and get involved in intellectually demanding activities,
which in turn leads to enhanced linguistic and cognitive development (Albert et al.,
1995; Brod et al., 2017; Heckman, 2006; Kim, 2015; Parisi et al., 2012; Yeniad et al.,
2014). However, this educational process is usually interrupted in conflict-zones
(Ahmadzadeh et al., 2014). About half of the refugee children have no access to
schooling (UNHCR, 2018) and those who go to school are 5 times more likely to
drop out than their non-refugee peers in the country of resettlement (UNICEF, 2017).
Due to interrupted schooling, refugee children who are resettled in a host
country are likely to have less skills than expected for the grade level their age falls
into (Dryden-Peterson, 2015). As a result, they are assigned to grades lower than their
age, which triggers an increase in the dropout rate (Sunny et al., 2017; Wils, 2004).
The underlying reason behind this pattern could be that these students receive
education that targets improving skills that are below their cognitive capacity. Thus,
they cannot benefit from the challenging and enriched learning environments that
boost their EF (Diamond & Lee, 2011). A recent study by Kim and colleagues (2020)
provided supporting evidence for this such that Syrian refugee children who attended
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
148
a grade with peers younger than them in Lebanon showed poorer performance on
cognitive tasks.
The medium of instruction is another crucial topic that should be considered.
School is a means of social integration, especially for refugee children (Osman et al.,
2020). However, if the child cannot understand the language, s/he might face social
exclusion, bullying, racism, which would lead to depression, stress and cognitive
impairments in turn (Birman, Trickett, & Buchanan, 2005; Coogan et al., 2020; Çelik
& İçduygu, 2019; Steinberger & Barch, 2021). Moreover, it could be overwhelming
for the displaced children to be immersed in a language other than their mother
tongue while trying to catch up with their peers in the school, and they may fail to
meet the objectives of the lessons due to the language barrier, which would
eventually delay the cognitive development (Frumkin, 2013; Ibragimova & Tarasova,
2018; Tunga, Engin, & Çağıltay, 2020; Tsimpli et al., 2020). Such disadvantageous
educational conditions may render refugee children behind their non-refugee peers in
cognitive functioning (Gagné et al., 2018; Wilkinson, 2002).
Socioeconomic Status and Home Environment
Socioeconomic status (SES), which is usually measured by the family income
and maternal education level, is documented to be strongly associated with children’s
cognitive development (Huang et al., 2021; Lambert et al., 2017; Lynn, 1990;
Sheridan et al., 2017). Unfortunately, due to various reasons (e.g., language barrier
and legal restrictions) many refugee families go through financial difficulties and live
in poor conditions in the country of resettlement (UNHCR, 2014; UN, 2014). Assari
(2020) proposed that SES might even have a healing effect on the after-effects of
trauma since richer and healthier nutrition promotes neurocognitive development (Liu
& Raine, 2017). Yet, children growing up in low-income households have poor
access to good quality nutrition they need to develop both physically and cognitively
(Lee & Jackson, 2017). For instance, a recent study by Chen and colleagues (2019)
tested 12-18-year-old Syrian refugees in Jordan for their WM and IC, and they found
that although those who had more traumatic experiences showed more PTSD
symptoms, their WM and IC scores were not associated with trauma exposure or
PTSD, but with poverty. Thus, Chen et al. (2019) concluded that poverty is a stronger
predictor of EF than trauma exposure. Mother education is another determinant of
healthy nutrition. Wachs & McCabe (2001) showed that mothers with higher
education were making healthier dietary choices, and thus, children with more
educated mothers had better nutrition intake. This was also the case for pregnant
women; more educated mothers had more nutritional knowledge (Abdul Manaf et al.,
2014; Cheng et al., 2009).
Parents are the first individuals the children interact with and the quality of the
parent-child relationship is highly associated with maternal education and household
income (Kong et al., 2015; Rouchun et al., 2021). Parents with higher levels of
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
149
education and income also provide higher quality and quantity of linguistic and
cognitive input to their children: they play educative games, interact more, provide
linguistically and cognitively stimulating materials such as books, and computers,
and afford good schools, which stimulate the cognitive networks (APA, 2017;
Weiland et al., 2017). Therefore, children with high socioeconomic background
develop better psychological and academic skills (Anders et al., 2013; Gottfried et al.,
2014) as well as WM, IC and shifting abilities compared to low-SES children
(Asadollahpour et al., 2015; Cascio et al., 2022; Clark et al., 2013; Micalizzi et al.,
2019; Suor et al., 2017).
Language Development in Refugee Children
The language input provided by the family plays a crucial role in vocabulary
development (Bohnacker, Lindgren, & Öztekin, 2016; Morton & Harper, 2007;
Ongun, 2018). Children’s L1 input resources expand as they grow up and build their
social circles (Sun et al., 2016). However, in the case of forced displacement, refugee
children’s L1 exposure does not exceed the house input because they usually start
getting input a language different than their L1 when they arrive in the host country
(e.g., television, school, society, etc.). So, unlike their non-refugee peers, their L1
development remains dependent on home input (Dixon et al., 2012; Duursma et al.,
2007; Scheffner Hammer et al., 2008). For children who arrive in the host country at
an early age, L2 exposure starts before they master their L1. As a result, they are
more likely to show regression in their L1 development (Jia & Aaronson, 2003;
McDonald, 2006; Portocarrero et al., 2007).
School is another major source of language input after home (Schwartz & Katzir,
2012). Usually, the language of instruction is different from the one spoken at home
for minorities, immigrants and refugees. When the formal language is different from
the one spoken at home, a shift of language dominancy from home language to
school language can be observed (Gagarina & Klassert, 2018; Kohnert & Bates,
2002). That is, children obtain higher vocabulary scores in the majority language, but
perform significantly lower in their home language after exposure to the majority
language (Gibson et al., 2012; Hammer et al., 2008; Kan & Kohnert, 2005; Oller et
al., 2007), and this difference between the languages become more evident as the
systematic L2 exposure at school increases (Kohnert & Bates, 2002). Yeter and
colleagues (2021) investigated language abilities of 9-year-old Syrian children who
arrived in Turkey around the age of 5 and compared their performance to non-refugee
Arabic-Turkish minority bilinguals. Arabic was the dominant language at refugee
homes while it was Turkish for the non-refugee bilinguals. Syrian children’s Arabic
performance was poorer than non-refugee bilinguals’ Turkish after 2-3 years of
schooling. Mori & Calder (2013) investigated the vocabulary abilities of bilingual
Japanese students who attend Japanese-medium supplementary high schools in the
U.S. and found high correlations between age of arrival and vocabulary size in the
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
150
language of the host country. While the L1 Japanese participants who arrived in the
U.S. before the age of 9 developed good L2 vocabulary at the cost of diminished L1,
those whose age of arrival was above 9 could maintain their L1 vocabulary with
grade-level equivalent vocabulary in L2 (Mori & Calder, 2013). This may suggest
that a later age of arrival may be beneficial for L1, but disadvantageous for L2.
Conclusions and Future Directions
In conclusion, war displacement results in a chain of disadvantages for the
healthy cognitive and linguistic development of a child. The trauma brought by the
war atrocities before dislocation is likely to have direct negative impacts on the
maturing brain and cognition. It is very likely to cause PTSD and decrease the
volume of brain areas crucial for higher-order cognitive abilities. Limitations in the
exposure to mother tongue also cause refugee children to fall behind their non-
refugee bilingual peers. The trauma of dislocation, parental distress and the adverse
living conditions in the relocated country sustains, and even intensifies all the
negative cognitive consequences that are typically caused by pre-migration
experiences. Poor nutrition, low socioeconomic status, insecurity experienced due to
lack of healthy and consistent home environment, poor parenting arising from poor
psychological well-being of the caregivers, discrimination, and disadvantages in
schooling (e.g., interruptions as well as postponed, limited or no access to schooling
in the relocated country, drop-out rates, losing the right for education in mother
tongue, etc.) are some factors that further cause serious limitations in children's
psychological and cognitive well-being.
Poor WM, IC and shifting abilities have often been associated with long-term
cognitive, psychological, social, and physical health problems. These abilities are
also correlated with future financial difficulties, substance dependence and criminal
behaviour (Moffitt et al., 201). Hence, being a war-torn refugee would have life-long
adverse consequences for children in general. Therefore, it is of utmost importance to
offer sustainable programs to prevent or to heal displacement trauma in refugee
children as well as programs fostering cognitive, linguistic and psychological
development. These actions would improve healthier adaptation of these children in
the society, which would indirectly enhance the welfare of the society.
United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees (UNHCR) provides financial
support, basic goods, shelter and food to displaced individuals in many countries
(UNHCR, 2021a). However, refugees have limited to no access to mental health
services in some countries (International Medical Corps, 2015) and a systematic
action plan for refugees’ cognitive well-being is non-existent. First, it is imperative
that an assessment is made to check the psychological well-being and cognitive
abilities of refugee children before they are registered to schools. Following this
investigation, needs of refugee children can be identified and intervention strategies
addressing their needs can be implemented so that they will be able to meet their
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
151
potential academically and build healthier connections with their peers (Diamond &
Lee, 2011). Psychological support should not be limited to refugee children only.
Caregivers should also be able to benefit from mental health services when necessary.
Through trainings and outreach activities, the caregivers should also be informed of
their children’s psychological and cognitive well-being.
Second, teachers in the host countries should be given special training for better
integration of refugee children in the classroom. Governments should offer seminars
to teachers with refugee children in their classes with the right techniques and
approaches for children with psychological difficulties (PTSD, depression, anxiety,
ADHD, etc.) to optimise learning outcomes.
Third, language classes both in refugees’ mother tongue and the language of the
host country should be provided for the children to facilitate their competence both in
their first and second language. Training in the majority language should be provided
for the caregivers to ease their adaptation process, which would also help them find a
job more easily. This in return would decrease the caregivers’ level of distress.
Finally, more research needs to be conducted to have a better understanding of
how being a refugee influences the cognitive and linguistic development of a child.
Also, the outcomes of the research in this field should be effectively shared with
different stakeholders from the caregivers and the teachers of the refugee children to
the NGOs and policy makers responsible to take solid actions to counter the adverse
effects of displacement. If wars cannot be prevented, raising awareness about these
issues becomes crucial to pave the way for diminishing these adverse effects.
References
Abdul Manaf, Z., Johari, N., Yee Mei, L., Sim Yee, N., Kai Yin, C., & Wai Teng, L. (2014).
Nutritional Status and Nutritional Knowledge of Malay Pregnant Women in Selected Private
Hospitals in Klang Valley. Jurnal Sains Kesihatan Malaysia, 12(2), 53–62.
https://doi.org/10.17576/JSKM-2014-1202-08
Ahmadzadeh, H., Hashem, L., Husseini, J. Al, Wahby, S., Alasil, M., Bali, Z., & Waziri, H.
(2014). Ensuring quality education for young refugees from Syria (12 – 25 years) - A
mapping exercise Research report supervised by Dawn Chatty. In Refugee Studies Centre.
Retrieved from http://s3.amazonaws.com/inee-assets/resources/rr-syria-youth-education-
2014.pdf
Albert, M. S., Jones, K., Savage, C. R., Berkman, L., Seeman, T., Blazer, D., & Rowe, J. W.
(1995). Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful
aging. Psychology and Aging, 10(4), 578–589. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.578
Anders, Y., Grosse, C., Rossbach, H. G., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2013). Preschool and primary
school influences on the development of children’s early numeracy skills between the ages of
3 and 7 years in Germany. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 24(2), 195–211.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09243453.2012.749794
APA. (2017). Education and socioeconomic status [Fact sheet]. Retrieved from
https://www.apa.org/pi/ses/resources/publications/factsheet-education.pdf
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
152
Asadollahpour, F., Baghban, K., Mirbalochzehi, P., Naderifar, E., & Tahmasebi, B. (2015). The
performance of bilingual and monolingual children on working memory tasks. Iranian
Rehabilitation Journal, 13(3), 53–57.
Assari, S. (2020). Family Socioeconomic Status and Exposure to Childhood Trauma: Racial
Differences. Children, 7(6), 57. https://doi.org/10.3390/children7060057
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of
Learning and Motivation (pp. 47–89). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J., & Gottfredson, L. S. (2007). Premorbid (early life) IQ and Later
Mortality Risk: Systematic Review. Annals of Epidemiology, 17(4), 278–288.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.07.010
Bick, J., & Nelson, C. A. (2016). Early adverse experiences and the developing brain.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 177–196. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.252
Birman, D., Trickett, E., & Buchanan, R. M. (2005). A tale of two cities: Replication of a study on
the acculturation and adaptation of immigrant adolescents from the former Soviet Union in a
different community context. American Journal of Community Psychology, 35(1–2), 83–101.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-005-1891-y
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2016). Poverty, Stress, and Brain Development: New Directions for
Prevention and Intervention. Academic Pediatrics, 16(3), S30–S36.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2016.01.010
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating Effortful Control, Executive Function, and False Belief
Understand... Child Development, 78(2), 647–663. Retrieved from
http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy-
iup.klnpa.org/ehost/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?vid=1&sid=be96dddc-40b5-4e9a-916c-
4065f92670dc%40sessionmgr102
Bohnacker, U., Lindgren, J., & Öztekin, B. (2016). Turkish- and German-speaking bilingual 4-to-
6-year-olds living in Sweden: Effects of age, SES and home language input on vocabulary
production. Journal of Home Language Research, 1(0), 17. https://doi.org/10.16993/jhlr.26
Brod, G., Bunge, S. A., & Shing, Y. L. (2017). Does One Year of Schooling Improve Children’s
Cognitive Control and Alter Associated Brain Activation? Psychological Science, 28(7), 967–
978. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617699838
Brown, T. T., Kuperman, J. M., Chung, Y., Erhart, M., McCabe, C., Hagler, D. J., Venkatraman,
V. K., Akshoomoff N., Amaral, D. G., Bloss C. S., Casey, B. J., Chang, L., Ernst, T. M.,
Fraizer, J. A., Gruen, J. R., Kaufman, W. E., Kenet, T., Kennedy, D. N., Murray, S. S.,
Sowell, E. R., Jernigan, T. L., Dale, A. M. (2012). Neuroanatomical assessment of biological
maturity. Current Biology, 22(18), 1693–1698. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.002
Browne, D. T., Kumar, A., Puente-Duran, S., Georgiades, K., Leckie, G., & Jenkins, J. (2017).
Emotional problems among recent immigrants and parenting status: Findings from a national
longitudinal study of immigrants in Canada. PLOS ONE, 12(4), e0175023.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175023
Bryant, R. A., Edwards, B., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Forbes, D., Felmingham, K. L., …
Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (2018). The effect of post-traumatic stress disorder on refugees’ parenting
and their children’s mental health: a cohort study. The Lancet Public Health, 3(5), e249–e258.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(18)30051-3
Cascio, C. N., Lauharatanahirun, N., Lawson, G. M., Farah, M. J., & Falk, E. B. (2022). Parental
education is associated with differential engagement of neural pathways during inhibitory
control. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 260. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-04152-4
Çelik, Ç., & İçduygu, A. (2019). Schools and Refugee Children: The Case of Syrians in Turkey.
International Migration, 57(2), 253–267. https://doi.org/10.1111/imig.12488
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
153
Chen, A., Panter-Brick, C., Hadfield, K., Dajani, R., Sheridan, M., & Hamoudi, A. (2019). Minds
under siege: Cognitive signatures of poverty and trauma in refugee and non-refugee
adolescents. Child Development, 90(6), 1856–1865. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13320
Cheng, Y., Dibley, M. J., Zhang, X., Zeng, L., & Yan, H. (2009). Assessment of dietary intake
among pregnant women in a rural area of western China. BMC Public Health, 9, 1–9.
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-9-222
Clark, C. A. C., Sheffield, T. D., Chevalier, N., Nelson, J. M., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013).
Charting early trajectories of executive control with the shape school. Developmental
Psychology, 49(8), 1481–1493. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030578
Coogan, P., Schon, K., Li, S., Cozier, Y., Bethea, T., & Rosenberg, L. (2020). Experiences of
racism and subjective cognitive function in African American women. Alzheimer’s and
Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Disease Monitoring, 12(1), 1–8.
https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12067
Corbett, B. A., Constantine, L. J., Hendren, R., Rocke, D., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Examining
executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166(2–3), 210–222.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.005
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive
control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory,
inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 411–424. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070221
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in
Children 4 to 12 Years Old. Science, 333(August), 959–964.
Dixon, L. Q., Zhao, J., Quiroz, B. G., & Shin, J. Y. (2012). Home and community factors
influencing bilingual children’s ethnic language vocabulary development. International
Journal of Bilingualism, 16(4), 541–565. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911429527
Dryden-Peterson, S. (2015). The Educational Experiences of Refugee Children in Countries of
First Asylum. Washington, DC.
Duursma, E., Romero-Contreras, Si., Szuber, A., Proctor, P., Snow, C., August, D., & Calderón,
M. (2007). The role of home literacy and language environment on bilinguals’ English and
Spanish vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(1), 171–190.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716406070093
Elosúa, M. R., Del Olmo, S., & Contreras, M. J. (2017). Differences in Executive Functioning in
Children with Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Frontiers in
Psychology, 8(JUN), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00976
Eruyar, S., Maltby, J., & Vostanis, P. (2018). Mental health problems of Syrian refugee children:
the role of parental factors. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(4), 401–409.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1101-0
European Commission. (2016). State of Play: Measures to Address the Refugee Crisis.
Fong, V. C., & Iarocci, G. (2020). The Role of Executive Functioning in Predicting Social
Competence in Children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Research,
13(11), 1856–1866. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2350
Gabrys, R. L., Dixon, K., & Anisman, H. (2017). Traumatic Life Events in Relation to Cognitive
Flexibility: Moderating Role of the BDNF Val66Met Gene Polymorphism. Frontiers in
Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00241
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
154
Gagarina, N., & Klassert, A. (2018). Input Dominance and Development of Home Language in
Russian-German Bilinguals. Frontiers in Communication, 3.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00040
Gagné, M., Janus, M., Milbrath, C., Gadermann, A., & Guhn, M. (2018). Early emotional and
communication functioning predicting the academic trajectories of refugee children in
Canada. Educational Psychology, 38(8), 1050–1067.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2018.1475627
Garber, J. (2006). Depression in Children and Adolescents. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 31(6), 104–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2006.07.007
Gibson, T. A., Oller, D. K., Jarmulowicz, L., & Ethington, C. A. (2012). The receptive-expressive
gap in the vocabulary of young second-language learners: Robustness and possible
mechanisms. Bilingualism, 15(1), 102–116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000490
Gligorović, M., & Buha Ðurović, N. (2014). Inhibitory control and adaptive behaviour in children
with mild intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 58(3), 233–242.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12000
Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Bathurst, K., Guerin, D. W., & Parramore, M. M. (2014).
Socioeconomic status in children’s development and family environment: Infancy through
adolescence. Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development, (2), 189–207.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410607027-17
Gredebäck, G., Haas, S., Hall, J., Pollak, S., Karakus, D. C., & Lindskog, M. (2021). Social
cognition in refugee children: an experimental cross-sectional study of emotional processing
with Syrian families in Turkish communities. Royal Society Open Science, 8(8).
https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.210362
Hadfield, K., Ostrowski, A., & Ungar, M. (2017). What can we expect of the mental health and
well-being of Syrian refugee children and adolescents in Canada? Canadian Psychology,
58(2), 194–201. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000102
Heckman, J. J. (2006). Investing in Disadvantaged Children. Social Sciences, 312(June), 2005–
2007.
Hermansen, T. K., Syrstad, K. E., Røysamb, E., & Melinder, A. M. D. (2022). Child internalizing
and externalizing behaviors: Interplay between maternal depressive symptoms and child
inhibitory control. JCPP Advances, (February). https://doi.org/10.1002/jcv2.12107
Huang, X., Hua, L., Zhou, X., Zhang, H., Zhang, M., Wang, S., … Wang, X. (2021). The
Association between Home Environment and Quality of Life in Children and Adolescents in
Hangzhou City, China. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 30(6), 1416–1427.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-01951-1
Ibragimova, E. R., & Tarasova, A. N. (2018). Language-related problems of international students
of Elabuga Institute of Kazan Federal University. Espacios, 39(2).
Ilonen, T., Taiminen, T., Karlsson, H., Luerma, H., Tuimala, P., Leinonen, K.-M., … Salokangas,
R. K. R. (2000). Impaired Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in first-episode severe
depression. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 54(4), 275–280.
https://doi.org/10.1080/080394800448156
Irwin, L. N., Kofler, M. J., Soto, E. F., & Groves, N. B. (2019). Do children with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have set shifting deficits? Neuropsychology, 33(4),
470–481. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000546
Jia, G., & Aaronson, D. (2003). A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning
English in the United States. In Applied Psycholinguistics (Vol. 24).
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716403000079
Joseph, H. M., Mckone, K. M. P., Molina, B. S. G., & Shaw, D. S. (2021). Maternal Parenting and
Toddler Temperament : Predictors of Early School Age Attention ‑ Deficit / Hyperactivity
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
155
Disorder ‑ Related Behaviors. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 763–773.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00778-0
Kan, P. F., & Kohnert, K. (2005). Preschoolers learning Hmong and English: Lexical-semantic
skills in L1 and L2. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 372–383.
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/026)
Kim, H. Y., Brown, L., Tubbs Dolan, C., Sheridan, M., & Aber, J. L. (2020). Post-migration risks,
developmental processes, and learning among Syrian refugee children in Lebanon. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 69(May), 101142.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101142
Kim, Y. (2015). The roles of schooling types in first language development of Korean overseas
children. KEDI Journal of Educational Policy, 12(1), 61–81.
Kluczniok, K., Lehrl, S., Kuger, S., & Rossbach, H.-G. (2013). Quality of the home learning
environment during preschool age – Domains and contextual conditions. European Early
Childhood Education Research Journal, 21(3), 420–438.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2013.814356
Klugman, J. (2022). The Gender Dimensions of Forced Displacement: A synthesis of New
Research.
Kohnert, K. J., & Bates, E. (2002). Balancing Bilinguals II: Lexical Comprehension and Cognitive
Processing in Children Learning Spanish and English. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing Research, 45(2), 347–359. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/027)
Kong, F., Chen, Z., Xue, S., Wang, X., & Liu, J. (2015). Mother’s but not father’s education
predicts general fluid intelligence in emerging adulthood: Behavioral and neuroanatomical
evidence. Human Brain Mapping, 36(11), 4582–4591. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22934
Lambert, H. K., King, K. M., Monahan, K. C., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Differential
associations of threat and deprivation with emotion regulation and cognitive control in
adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 29(3), 929–940.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000584
Lee, D., & Jackson, M. (2017). The Simultaneous Effects of Socioeconomic Disadvantage and
Child Health on Children’s Cognitive Development. Demography, 54(5), 1845–1871.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-017-0605-z
Liu, J., & Raine, A. (2017). Nutritional status and social behavior in preschool children: the
mediating effects of neurocognitive functioning. Maternal & Child Nutrition, 13(2), e12321.
https://doi.org/10.1111/mcn.12321
Locke, C. J., Southwick, K., McCloskey, L. A., & Fernández-Esquer, M. E. (1996). The
Psychological and Medical Sequelae of War in Central American Refugee Mothers and
Children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 150(8), 822.
https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.1996.02170330048008
Lynn, R. (1990). The role of nutrition in secular increases in intelligence. Personality and
Individual Differences, 11(3), 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(90)90241-I
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor
grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory
and Language, 55(3), 381–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.006
Mehnert, J., Akhrif, A., Telkemeyer, S., Rossi, S., Schmitz, C. H., Steinbrink, J., … Neufang, S.
(2013). Developmental changes in brain activation and functional connectivity during
response inhibition in the early childhood brain. Brain and Development, 35(10), 894–904.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2012.11.006
Micalizzi, L., Brick, L. A., Flom, M., Ganiban, J. M., & Saudino, K. J. (2019). Effects of
socioeconomic status and executive function on school readiness across levels of household
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
156
chaos. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 331–340.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.01.007
Miller, H. V., Barnes, J. C., & Beaver, K. M. (2011). Self-control and health outcomes in a
nationally representative sample. American Journal of Health Behavior, 35(1), 15–27.
https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.35.1.2
Mori, Y., & Calder, T. M. (2013). Bilingual vocabulary knowledge and arrival age among
japanese heritage language students at Hoshuukoo. Foreign Language Annals, 46(2), 290–
310. https://doi.org/10.1111/flan.12027
Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: A
review of NIRS studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(DEC), 1–9.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00867
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage.
Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x
Nyman, A., Taskinen, T., Grönroos, M., Haataja, L., Lähdetie, J., & Korhonen, T. (2010).
Elements of Working Memory as Predictors of Goal-Setting Skills in Children With
Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(6), 553–562.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219410375001
Oades, R. D., & Christiansen, H. (2008). Cognitive switching processes in young people with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(1), 21–32.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.09.002
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual
language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(2), 191–230.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716407070117
Ongun, Z. (2018). Bilingualism, vocabulary knowledge and nonverbal intelligence: Turkish-
English bilingual children in the UK.
Osman, F., Mohamed, A., Warner, G., & Sarkadi, A. (2020). Longing for a sense of belonging—
Somali immigrant adolescents’ experiences of their acculturation efforts in Sweden.
International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 15(sup2), 1784532.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17482631.2020.1784532
Özer, S., Şirin, S., & Oppedal, B. (2016). Bahçeşehir Study of Syrian Refugee Children in Turkey.
Pang, E. W., Sedge, P., Grodecki, R., Robertson, A., MacDonald, M. J., Jetly, R., … Taylor, M. J.
(2014). Colour or shape: examination of neural processes underlying mental flexibility in
posttraumatic stress disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 4(8), e421–e421.
https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2014.63
Parisi, J. M., Rebok, G. W., Xue, Q. L., Fried, L. P., Seeman, T. E., Tanner, E. K., … Carlson, M.
C. (2012). The role of education and intellectual activity on cognition. Journal of Aging
Research, 2012, 20–24. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/416132
Park, S., Kim, B., Choi, N., Ryu, J., Cobham, V., Song, S., … Cho, S.-C. (2014). The effect of
persistent posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms on executive functions in preadolescent
children witnessing a single incident of death. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping: An International
Journal, 27(3), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2013.853049
Portocarrero, J. S., Burright, R. G., & Donovick, P. J. (2007). Vocabulary and verbal fluency of
bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(3),
415–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.015
Rizkalla, N., Mallat, N. K., Arafa, R., Adi, S., Soudi, L., & Segal, S. P. (2020). “Children Are Not
Children Anymore; They Are a Lost Generation”: Adverse Physical and Mental Health
Consequences on Syrian Refugee Children. International Journal of Environmental Research
and Public Health, 17(22), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17228378
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
157
Rouchun, D., Zongkui, Z., Shuailei, L., Qingqi, L., & Chen, G. (2021). Family socioeconomic
status and the parent-child relationship: Children’s Internet use as a moderated mediator.
Current Psychology, 40(9), 4384–4393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00356-0
Sack, W. H., Clarke, G. N., & Seeley, J. (1996). Multiple Forms of Stress in Cambodian
Adolescent Refugees. Child Development, 67(1), 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.1996.tb01722.x
Şafak-Ayvazoğlu, A., Kunuroglu, F., & Yağmur, K. (2021). Psychological and socio-cultural
adaptation of Syrian refugees in Turkey. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,
80(February 2020), 99–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2020.11.003
Sasser, T. R., Bierman, K. L., & Heinrichs, B. (2015). Executive functioning and school
adjustment: The mediational role of pre-kindergarten learning-related behaviors. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 30(PA), 70–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.09.001
Scheffner Hammer, C., Lawrence, F. R., & Miccio, A. W. (2008). Exposure to English Before and
After Entry into Head Start1: Bilingual Children’s Receptive Language Growth in Spanish
and English. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(1), 30–56.
https://doi.org/10.2167/beb376.0
Schwartz, M., & Katzir, T. (2012). Depth of lexical knowledge among bilingual children: The
impact of schooling. Reading and Writing, 25(8), 1947–1971. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-
011-9308-9
Scott, W. A. (1962). Cognitive Complexity and Cognitive Flexibility. Sociometry, 25(4), 405–414.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2307/2785779
Sheridan, M. A., Peverill, M., Finn, A. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Dimensions of childhood
adversity have distinct associations with neural systems underlying executive functioning.
Development and Psychopathology, 29(5), 1777–1794.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001390
Skowron, E. A., Cipriano-Essel, E., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Teti, D. M., & Ammerman, R. T.
(2014). Early adversity, RSA, and inhibitory control: Evidence of children’s neurobiological
sensitivity to social context. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 964–978.
https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21175
Steinberger, D., & Barch, D. (2021). The Effect of Perceived Discrimination on Mental Health
and Cognitive Functioning. Modern Psychological Studies, 27(1).
https://doi.org/10.15154/1519007
Sulik, M. J., Blair, C., Mills-Koonce, R., Berry, D., & Greenberg, M. (2015). Early Parenting and
the Development of Externalizing Behavior Problems: Longitudinal Mediation Through
Children’s Executive Function. Child Development, 86(5), 1588–1603.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12386
Sun, H., Steinkrauss, R., Tendeiro, J., & De Bot, K. (2016). Individual differences in very young
children’s English acquisition in China: Internal and external factors. Bilingualism, 19(3),
550–566. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000243
Sunny, B. S., Elze, M., Chihana, M., Gondwe, L., Crampin, A. C., Munkhondya, M., … Glynn, J.
R. (2017). Failing to progress or progressing to fail? Age-for-grade heterogeneity and grade
repetition in primary schools in Karonga district, northern Malawi. International Journal of
Educational Development, 52, 68–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2016.10.004
Suor, J. H., Sturge-Apple, M. L., & Skibo, M. A. (2017). Breaking cycles of risk: The mitigating
role of maternal working memory in associations among socioeconomic status, early
caregiving, and children’s working memory. Development and Psychopathology, 29(4),
1133–1147. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941600119X
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
158
Tsimpli, I. M., Vogelzang, M., Balasubramanian, A., Marinis, T., Alladi, S., Reddy, A., & Panda,
M. (2020). Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of
Disadvantaged Children in India. Languages, 5(1), 10.
https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5010010
Tummala-Narra, P., & Claudius, M. (2013). The effect of surface sealant on micro-leakage and
solubility of nano-glassionomer restoration. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
Psychology, 19(3), 257–268. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032960
Tuncer, N. (2021). Comparing the Executive Function Skills of Turkish and Refugee Preschool
Children: Flexible Item Selection Task (Fist). European Journal of Education Studies, 3(1),
235–265. https://doi.org/10.46827/ejrs.v8i1.3535
Tunga, Y., Engin, G., & Çağıltay, K. (2020). A Literature Review on the Issues Encountered in
Educating Syrian Children in Turkey. Inonu University Journal of the Faculty of Education,
21(2020), 317–333. https://doi.org/10.17679/inuefd.535845
Umbel, V. M., Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, M. C., & Oller, D. K. (1992). Measuring Bilingual
Children’s Receptive Vocabularies. Child Development, 63(4), 1012.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1131250
Wachs, T. D., & McCabe, G. (2001). Relation of maternal intelligence and schooling to offspring
nutritional intake. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25(5), 444–449.
https://doi.org/10.1080/016502501316934879
Weiland, C., McCoy, D. C., Grace, E., & Park, S. O. (2017). Natural Window of Opportunity?
Low-Income Parents’ Responses to Their Children’s Impending Kindergarten Entry. AERA
Open, 3(1), 233285841668150. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858416681509
Wilkinson, L. (2002). Factors Influencing the Academic Success of Refugee Youth in Canada.
Journal of Youth Studies, 5(2), 173–193. https://doi.org/10.1080/1367626022013443
Wils, A. (2004). LATE ENTRANTS LEAVE SCHOOL EARLIER: EVIDENCE FROM
MOZAMBIQUE. 50(1), 17–37.
Woodburn, M., Bricken, C. L., Wu, Z., Li, G., Wang, L., Lin, W., … Cohen, J. R. (2021). The
maturation and cognitive relevance of structural brain network organization from early
infancy to childhood. NeuroImage, 238(April), 118232.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118232
Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Emmen, R. A. G., & Prevoo, M. J. L.
(2014). Cognitive flexibility children across the transition to school: A longitudinal study.
Cognitive Development, 31 (May 2019), 35–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.02.004
Yeter, Ö., Rabagliati, H., & Özge, D. (2021). Are Refugee Bilingual Children Disadvantaged in
Their Cognitive and Linguistic Abilities? Proceedings of the 45th Annual Boston University
Conference on Language Development. (790–804).
Sources
UN. (2014). Regional refugee & resilience plan 2016-2017. Retrieved from
http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/1944-2866.POI360%5Cnhttp://www.edelman.com/post/millennialsand-
brands/%5Cnhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1108/QMR0620130041%5Cnhttp://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.
1111/j.1468-2958.1977.tb00519.x/abstract%5Cnhttp://dx.doi.org/10.10
Özlem Yeter, Hugh Rabagliati, Duygu Özge
East European Journal of Psycholinguistics. Volume 9, Number 2, 2022
159
UNHCR. (2014). Living in the Shadows – Jordan Home Visits Report 2014. Retrieved from
http://www.unhcr.org/54b685079.pdf
UNHCR. (2018). Stepping up: Refugee Education in Crisis.
UNHCR. (2021a). Global Report. Retrieved from
https://www.unaids.org/sites/default/files/media_asset/JC3032_AIDS_Data_book_2021_En.p
df
UNHCR. (2021b). Global Trends: Forced Displacement in 2021. Retrieved from
https://www.unhcr.org/refugee-statistics
UNICEF. (2019). Action for Refugee Children. 32.
Threat of War on Cognitive Development of Refugee Children