ArticlePDF Available

A school intervention promotes compassion, empathy and social relationships in children

Authors:
  • GRUPOAWE
  • Centro Científico Tecnológico Conicet Confluencia

Abstract and Figures

There is increasing evidence to show that compassion and altruism are highly beneficial when cultivated from an early age. In the present study we explore the benefits of an intervention programme that seeks to develop affective empathy, compassion and altruism in 9-year-old children from a school of Buenos Aires, Argentina. A pretest-posttest controlled study was carried out with 48 children, half of whom carried out the intervention. The remaining children, who constituted the waitlist group, participated in academic tutoring activities conducted by the same research team. The intervention was successful in promoting children's altruistic and compassionate attitudes, evidenced by the results obtained in the universal altruism test, social integration, and affective response. Effect sizes ranged from medium to large in the intervention group. These positive findings were not observed in the waitlist group (non-significant effect sizes). This study shows that children's self-awareness and awareness of others tend to foster compassion and affective empathy, contributing to individual and collective well-being.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A school intervention
promotes compassion,
empathy and social
relationships in children
Marcela Kappelmayer
Belgrano University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Andrea Czar
Belgrano University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Maria Tresca
Belgrano University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Paola DAdamo
CCT Patagonia Norte, Bariloche, Argenina
Mariana Lozada
CCT Patagonia Norte, Bariloche, Argenina
Abstract
There is increasing evidence to show that compassion and altruism are highly benecial
when cultivated from an early age. In the present study we explore the benets of an
intervention programme that seeks to develop affective empathy, compassion and altru-
ism in 9-year-old children from a school of Buenos Aires, Argentina. A pretest-posttest
controlled study was carried out with 48 children, half of whom carried out the inter-
vention. The remaining children, who constituted the waitlist group, participated in aca-
demic tutoring activities conducted by the same research team. The intervention was
successful in promoting childrens altruistic and compassionate attitudes, evidenced by
the results obtained in the universal altruism test, social integration, and affective
response. Effect sizes ranged from medium to large in the intervention group. These
Corresponding author:
Mariana Lozada, CCT Patagonia Norte, Pioneros 2350, Bariloche 8400, Argentina.
Emails: mariana.lozada@gmail.com, lozadam@comahue-conicet.gob.ar
Article
School Psychology International
2023, Vol. 44(5) 515532
© The Author(s) 2022
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/01430343221145668
journals.sagepub.com/home/spi
positive ndings were not observed in the waitlist group (non-signicant effect sizes).
This study shows that childrens self-awareness and awareness of others tend to foster
compassion and affective empathy, contributing to individual and collective well-being.
Keywords
Affective empathy, altruism, children, compassion, social integration
Introduction
Growing evidence suggests the need to foster caring behaviours like affection and com-
passion within educational contexts to alleviate psychological suffering (e.g., Potvin
et al., 2022). Present day schools are plagued by severe social conicts such as exclusion,
bullying, violence, depression, loneliness, anxiety and attentional disorders (e.g.,
Moyano et al., 2019). This context affects social relationships in educational settings,
compromising childrens health and wellbeing. For this reason, the cultivation of socio-
emotional skills like empathy, compassion and altruism during childhood is becoming
increasingly important, and promising results have been achieved by mindbody integra-
tion programmes (e.g., Carro et al., 2020, 2021; Estévez López et al., 2009; Flook et al.,
2015; Lozada et al., 2014a, 2017). The purpose of this research seeks to evaluate whether
a compassionate-based-intervention promotes social integration, affective empathy and
altruistic attitudes in primary school children from Argentina.
Review of terms
Empathy. Empathy has been dened as the capacity to feel and understand other peoples
emotions (Decety & Meyer, 2008; Decety et al., 2016). From an embodied-enactive per-
spective, empathy is the ability to resonate with another person by putting oneself in their
place, overriding self-centeredness (e.g., Decety, 2010; Depraz et al., 2003). Empathy
involves affective and cognitive dimensions (e.g., Singer & Klimecki, 2014).
Cognitive empathy refers to the capacity to understand othersthoughts and feelings
without getting emotionally involved, whereas affective empathy involves feeling the
emotion that another person is experiencing (e.g., Batson & Ahmad, 2001; Mikulincer
& Shaver, 2017). In recognition of the suffering of others, young children tend to
show the affective rather than the cognitive component of empathy (Hoffman, 1996;
McDonald & Messinger, 2011).
Compassion and altruism. It has been proposed that altruism is a human capacity that can
benet both the helper and the person being helped (Lozada et al., 2011). Compassion
and altruism, associated with affective empathy, require more than just feeling others
emotions. Altruism involves the intention to benet others and compassion can be
dened as the intention to relieve otherssuffering (e.g., Halifax, 2012; Singer &
516 School Psychology International 44(5)
Klimecki, 2014). Compassionate acts require emotional balance on the part of the person
feeling compassion in order to counteract possible psychological distress. The serenity
that emerges from emotional regulation tends to foster compassion and altruism (e.g.,
Porges, 2017).
Social integration. Peer social relationships are crucial to a childs healthy development
(Eccles, 1999; Vanaelst et al., 2012). Several studies have shown that negative social
interactions can signicantly affect their health and wellbeing (e.g., Carro et al., 2020,
2021; Lozada et al., 2014a, 2017; Ponzi et al., 2016; Vanaelst et al., 2012). We consider
social integration to be a parameter that indicates the diversity and richness of social
bonds and illustrates the degree to which a student feels integrated into their school
peer group (Moyano et al., 2019).
Review of interventions
Previous studies have revealed that empathy is a key factor for the development of com-
passion and healthy interpersonal relationships (e.g., Decety et al., 2016; Eisenberg,
2002; Ozawa-de Silva & Dodson-Lavelle, 2011). Altruistic behaviour is highly affected
by social contexts (e.g., Carro et al., 2020; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; Lozada et al.,
2014b, 2017; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2005, 2017), and environments that promote
perspective-taking can foster the development of this human ability (e.g., Batson et al.,
1988). Several studies have shown that empathy, compassion and altruism are strongly
interrelated, and can improve social integration, cognitive function and general wellness
in children (e.g., Carro et al., 2020, 2021, 2022; Flook et al., 2015; Lozada et al., 2014a,
2014b, 2017; Ozawa-de Silva & Dodson-Lavelle, 2011; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015;
Spinrad & Gal, 2018). In particular, the positive effect of altruistic acts on the immune
system and psycho-physiological wellbeing of the helper has been demonstrated (e.g.,
Brown et al., 2005; Cohen & Janicki-Deverts, 2009; Kok & Fredrickson, 2010;
Maratos & Shefeld, 2020). All this evidence highlights the behavioural plasticity of
these vital social skills, and therefore the value of promoting their development in
early childhood.
Interventions to foster altruism and compassion in children have been conducted
mainly in developed countries (Perkins et al., 2022). Previous research has shown that
empathic concern, compassion and altruism can be encouraged through the implementa-
tion of the Kindness curriculum, cognitively based compassion training (CBCT) pro-
grammes, and prosocial embodied activities (e.g., Carro et al., 2020; Flook et al.,
2015; Kirby et al., 2017; Lozada et al., 2014a; Ozawa-de Silva & Dodson-Lavelle,
2011). For example, interventions based on the Kindness Curriculum performed over
12 weeks favoured self-regulation and altruism in preschool children (e.g., Flook
et al., 2015). In other studies, prosocial-based interventions contributed to the enhance-
ment of altruistic attitudes, while reducing chronic stress (Carro et al., 2020, 2021;
Lozada et al., 2014b, 2017). Moreover, CBCT has been effective in fostering compassion
towards others (e.g., Ozawa-de Silva & Dodson-Lavelle, 2011; Reddy et al., 2013).
These studies have shown that the promotion of empathy, altruism and compassion in
Kappelmayer et al. 517
children is most effectively achieved by the implementation of compassionate-based
interventions together with mindfulness practices, rather than with mindfulness interven-
tions alone (Cheang et al., 2019). These two factors were taken into consideration in this
study, which involved a compassion-based intervention suitable for children inhabiting a
large city in a developing country.
The polyvagal theory (e.g., Porges, 2017) proposes that interventions that encourage
secure, affective, kind and non-judgmental experiences promote empathetic and compas-
sionate attitudes towards peers. Feeling secure promotes social engagement, whereas
feeling threatened elicits defensive behaviour. Safe social contexts favour inner connect-
edness and connectedness with others, nurturing healthy interpersonal interactions and
strengthening compassion and altruism (Porges, 2017).
Description of the present study
We developed an intervention designed to cultivate warm feelings towards oneself and
others. Inspired by previous studies that began to analyse the effects of promoting
empathy, altruism and compassion in children (Perkins et al., 2022), in the current inter-
vention we included 4 types of activities: (a) mindful awareness, to strengthen the ability
to focus on the present moment, (b) affective empathy, to foster self-affection and feel-
ings of warmth and benevolence towards others, (c) cognitive empathy, to strengthen
the participantsability to recognize that their own perspective might be different from
that of others and (d) kindness and compassion, to develop social skills by proposing
tasks related to helping others. In the present study, we evaluated the impact of this inter-
vention on empathy, altruism and social integration in 9-year-old children from a school
in Argentina, a developing country. We hypothesized that after participating in the inter-
vention, children would show higher levels of the above-mentioned social skills, whereas
these changes would not be observed in children from the waitlist group.
Methods
Participants
The current study, which followed a quasi-experimental design with randomization of
group assignment, involved 48 children (910 years old) who attended a private
school in Buenos Aires, Argentina. Two fourth grade groups (from the same school
but with different teachers) were randomly assigned to either the intervention group
(N=26, 16 girls and 10 boys) or the waitlist group (control; N=22, 13 girls and 9
boys). That is, fourth grade A was the Intervention group and fourth grade B the waitlist
group. There were no children with learning disorders and/or psychiatric concerns in
either grade.
It is worth noting that since one of the aims of our study was to work on group dynam-
ics and social integration, a previously established school class constituted our interven-
tion group. This semi-randomized procedure has also been used in previous
investigations, where similar interventions achieved improvements in social relationships
518 School Psychology International 44(5)
within a group of peers (e.g., Carro et al., 2020, 2021; Fuentes et al., 2018a, 2013b;
Lozada et al., 2014a). Thus, the fact that we focused on improving social skills in a
group of classmates already interacting together precluded the use of a randomized
sample. The study was approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee (CEIC)
and was performed according to the World Medical Association Declaration of
Helsinki guidelines. The intervention activities were rst explained to parents and
school authorities, who all gave their informed consent. Furthermore, the children volun-
tarily agreed to participate, and were free to stop doing the activities if they so desired.
The data were kept under conditions of anonymity and condentiality.
Research procedure. The classroom teacher and two child psychologists from the research
team carried out the programme during 10 weeks in the intervention and the wait-list
groups. The classroom teachers played a key role, as they sustained the programme
from week to week through daily implementation of the learned tasks and activities.
Waitlist group (Control group). In this group the programme sought to develop learn-
ing strategies and study skills through academic tutoring, focusing primarily on fostering
autonomous learning, effective study habits and reading comprehension strategies.
Intervention group. In this group the programme sought to foster empathy and com-
passion, through the practice of two nodal exercises: Connecting Pairs and Loving
Kindness Meditation (LKM) (Figure 1) performed on a daily basis throughout the
entire programme.
Figure 1. Two core exercises carried out daily: loving kindness meditation and connecting pairs.
Kappelmayer et al. 519
Intervention. The intervention consisted of 10 sessions carried out in 50-min weekly
encounters that took place within the school day. The programme was inspired by the
Kindness curriculum and CBCT interventions (e.g., Flook et al., 2015; Ozawa-de Silva
& Dodson-Lavelle, 2011) but recreated by the researchers for the Argentinian population
being studied, using storytelling books and imagery exercises, which were supported by
visual, graphic and audio material as well as music, stories and role-play activities, as
described below.
The intervention consisted of four lines of work: (1) Mindful awareness and emotional
regulation, (2)1 affective empathy, (3) cognitive empathy, and (4) compassionate acts.
(1). Mindful awareness and emotional regulation.This aspect of the intervention,
carried out at the beginning of each session, aimed to promote the ability to focus
on the present moment, emotional regulation and interoceptive awareness (awareness
of bodily sensations). Participants were invited to concentrate on their own breathing,
to develop mindful awareness. They were guided to perceive their thoughts and their
bodily sensations (interoception). At this point we intended to control the monkey
mind(i.e., the wandering non-present mind), by trying to focus on a specic activity
and be aware of when the mind wandered and interfered with the task.
After the second encounter, children were invited to perform LKM, a practice that pro-
motes a state of unconditional kindness and compassion toward all beings (e.g.,
Fredrickson et al., 2008; Hofmann et al., 2011). This meditation cultivates feelings of
unconditional love, affection and empathy, focusing on the progressive expression of
friendly wishes, from close relationships to all humanity.
Other activities in this part of the intervention included storytelling and imagery exer-
cises, supported by visual, graphic and audio material as well as music, stories and role-
play activities, such as How full is your bucketby Tom Rath and Mary Reckmeyer,
The invisible boyby Trudy Ludwig, I wish you moreby Amy Krouse Rosenthal
and Tom Lichtenheld and The golden ruleby Ilene Cooper, in addition to developing
kindness awareness programmes at school, garlands of gratitude, etc.
(2). Affective empathy (Opening our hearts). The goal of this aspect was to foster self-
affection and feelings of warmth and benevolence toward others, by accepting difcult
emotions, cultivating gratitude and resonating with otherspain. We aimed to develop
the ability to readthe emotions of others by evaluating their facial microexpressions.
Furthermore, context reading was promoted by paying special attention to the kind of
information provided by contextual cues when faces were not seen. We used the meta-
phor of the detective to motivate the children to become good investigatorsand iden-
tify their own and othersemotions. Children were taught that every emotion has a
meaning and expresses a message that is important to understand if we are to
connect meaningfully with others.
520 School Psychology International 44(5)
Along the same lines, the value of emotions was explored by attempting to recognize the
underlying needs they expressed. Affective empathy was fostered through empathic lis-
tening in order to connect with the emotions of others, while overriding self-centeredness.
The Affective Connecting Pairs (ACP) component was the core exercise of this line of work:
the children were asked to work in pairs previously assigned by the research team, consider-
ing the positive and negative elections of the groups pre-test sociogram (described below).
At the start of this intervention the children were asked to play with their chosen peers (posi-
tive elections), and towards the end they were paired up with their non-chosen peers (negative
elections). During the ACP exercise children were invited to share white pearlsof gratitude:
the gestures of others that we feel grateful for, and black pearls, the gestures that hurt them
or made them suffer. Each child took turns to share a white and a black pearl, while the
partner listened empathically. ACP time took place for ve minutes every day, guided by
the teacher, as one of the core activities of the intervention.
(3). Cognitive empathy (Understanding othersthoughts).The goal of this aspect was
to favour metacognition skills by considering other peoples points of view as well as
the participantsown, strengthening their ability to recognize that their own perspec-
tive might be different from that of others. They were invited to try to understand
othersthoughts, motives and feelings, even when these were very different from
their own. This was achieved through the Perspective Connecting Pairs activity, in
which each pair was given a set of cards with different characters and situations.
By taking turns, each child was invited to share what the different characters might
be thinking, while the partner listened empathically. The aim was to help children
appreciate alternative points of view, under the premise: My point of view is just
one of many. This activity, conducted for 5 minutes on a daily basis and guided by
the classroom teacher, was carried out after the ACP. Both empathic instances there-
fore constituted the connecting pair time, a core activity of the intervention (Figure 1).
(4). Acts of kindness and compassion (Compassion kid-t): The fourth aspect focused
on kindness and compassionate acts. The goal was to develop social skills by propos-
ing tasks related to helping others (i.e., how would you help someone in need or with
certain difculties). Each session included stories and poems which exemplied acts
of kindness that favoured the development of a loving disposition towards the suffer-
ing of others. This was the most thorough line of work and took place over 4 weeks.
We rst focused on acts of kindness and then shifted towards compassionate acts (i.e.,
moving away from: Who did something good to me this week?,toWho did some-
thing good to someone suffering?). Impartiality and equanimity skills were also
worked on at this point. As people tend to be kinder to their loved ones, we sought
to develop the aforementioned qualities towards others, which involved a true compas-
sionate attitude (mental disposition). To this end, we proposed tasks of increasing dif-
culty, progressing from kindness to a dear friend to kindness to someone in need.
It is noteworthy that throughout the programme LKM and Connecting Pairs (Pearls and
Cards) were carried out daily by the teacher (Figure 1).
Kappelmayer et al. 521
Furthermore, the level of difculty of the activities and tasks increased progressively
throughout the programme. For example, at rst children were grouped with the peers
they chose to play with, while later they were grouped with children they had less afnity
with. This was achieved by letting the children spontaneously group together at the begin-
ning and then pairing them up later with their non-chosen peers.
Measurement/assessment
Before and after the intervention, children from both the experimental and control groups
were individually invited by a psychologist from the research team to complete the ques-
tionnaires described below. These were administered at school in the Spanish language,
and lasted approximately 15 min. The questionnaires were designed to shed light on the
participants altruistic behaviour, affective empathy and social integration, which were
estimated through the following dependent variables: universal altruism test, affective
response, and social preference index, as detailed below.
Altruistic behaviour was assessed by means of the Universal Altruism test, an adaptation of
the Dictator Game (e.g., Avinun et al., 2011), in which students had to make a decision in order
to benet unknown children. The children received 10 pieces of candy and were given the fol-
lowing choice: they could place as many as they desired in a box for hospitalized children or
keep them all for themselves. This single-trail pre- and post-intervention task was conducted
under condential conditions, each child performing the task without being observed (by
their peers or the researcher). The total number of candies donated by each group was compared
before and after the intervention. This test, which consists of a single question, was previously
performed in Carro et al. (2020) and proved to be valid for Argentine children of this age group.
Social integration within each group was evaluated by means of a Sociogram (Moreno,
1972). In this Sociometric Questionnaire children were asked to list peers they would
choose to play with and those they would not. Playmate choice during childhood encom-
passes reciprocity and pro-sociality (Garaigordobil, 2005), and is a valid methodology for
assessing peer acceptance (e.g., Oberle, 2018; Wentzel et al., 2004). This questionnaire,
which has been found to have testretest reliability (e.g., Kalfus & Berler, 1985), was admi-
nistered pre and post-intervention in both groups. Variations in the number of chosen (i.e.,
positive elections) and non-chosen peers (i.e., negative elections) illustrate differences in
the social preference pattern for each participant. This pattern of social relationships was
compared before and after the intervention by means of the Social Preference index,
which subtracts the number of peers not chosen to play with from the ones who were
chosen. This methodology, widely used to assess social relationships in children, consists
of spontaneously answered questions that capture social relationship dynamics between
peers and can be used as an indicator of social integration within a group. Sociograms
are widely applied to assess social relationships within a group of children (Carro et al.,
2022; Fuentes et al., 2018a, 2018b; Lozada et al., 2014b). This sociometric instrument
depicts the intricacy of social links, reecting the structure and patterns of interpersonal
relationships in a group (Moreno, 1972). In educational contexts, social interrelationships
have been extensively explored and interpreted through this valuable tool (Carro et al.,
2022; Fuentes et al., 2018a, 2018b; Lozada et al., 2014b).
522 School Psychology International 44(5)
We evaluated the childrensAffective empathy by means of the Affective Response
dimension extracted from the Multidimensional Scale of Empathy, a scale adapted for
9- to 12-year-old Argentine children. This scale assesses the predisposition of an individ-
ual to resonate with othersemotions and was addressed by the following questions: (a)
When I see someone crying, I feel like crying myself; (b) When I see someone dancing,
I feel like moving my feet; (c) When I am with someone who is sad, I also feel sad.
Participants had to indicate their agreement with each item on a scale with four
options (i.e., always, often, sometimes and never). This scale showed good validity
and reliability: the McDonalds omega of this dimension was 0.714 for the experimental
group, and 0.78 for the control group.
Statistics
We conducted intraindividual paired comparisons, because we were interested in evalu-
ating pre- post-intervention changes per child. In this way, we intended to go beyond
interpersonal variability, which is particularly relevant for small samples. We thus pre-
cluded the effect of potential differences in initial values between groups.
Normal distribution was evaluated by means of the Shapiro Wilk test. The Wilcoxon
matched pair test was used for evaluating pre-post changes for non-normal data sets,
while the paired t test was used when data followed a normal distribution. The IBM
SPSS Statistics 23 programme was used for statistical analysis.
Cohensdwas calculated as a measure of effect size. The effect size provides add-
itional information about the magnitude of the differences observed while statistical sig-
nicance reports the existence of a signicant effect. However Pvalue does not show the
size of the effect (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012). Therefore, this quantitative measure estimates
the magnitude of the intervention effect (i.e, a larger effect size indicates a greater effect),
since it compares effects not biased by the sample size.
Results
Most of the data followed a non-normal distribution, except for the Social Preference
Index of the intervention group (Table 1).
Table 1. Normality test values for data set.
Shapiro Wilk Test
Statistic df p- value
Affective Response Intervention Group 0.91 25 .03
Affective Response Control Group 0.89 25 .01
Universal Altruism Intervention Group 0.72 25 .00
Universal Altruism Intervention Group 0.79 23 .00
Social Preference Intervention Group 0.94 25 .17
Social Preference Control Group 0.91 23 .05
Kappelmayer et al. 523
After the intervention, signicant changes in every parameter were detected in the
intervention group. The results obtained from the pre-post intervention comparison
of the Universal Altruism test showed a signicant increase (70%) in the children
who took part in the programme (p=.031). In contrast, no signicant changes were
found in the control group (p=.7) (Figure 2). Cohens d test was 0.73 for the experi-
mental group, showing a large effect, while d=0.1 for the control group, showing no
effect. Thus, participants shared signicantly more candies at the end of the interven-
tion, expressing greater compassion for hospitalized children who were completely
unknown to them. Moreover, children from the intervention group displayed a
greater Affective Response at the end of the intervention (p=.05) (Table 2), a
pattern not observed in the children from the control group (p=.26). This empathic
concern dimension suggests caring resonance with the emotions of others. Effect size
was medium for the experimental group (Cohensd=0.5) and small for the control
group (d=0.36).
Interestingly, after the intervention a signicant increase (43.3%) in the social prefer-
ence index (paired ttest 2.22; p=.022) was detected in the children who took part in the
activities. This indicates that fewer children were rejected while more were chosen as
playmates. Conversely, this trend was not observed in the control group, whose
Figure 2. Pre- and post-intervention comparisons of the Universal Altruism test results for the
intervention group (IG) and the control group (CG). Effect size for Universal Altruism was large
for the IG (d=0.73), while it was non-signicant for the CG (d=0.1).
524 School Psychology International 44(5)
pattern of social relationships did not change substantially (Wilcoxon p=.95) (Figure 3).
The effect size, indexed by Cohensdtest was medium for the experimental group (d=0.50)
while no effect was found for the control group (d=0.02).
Table 2. Mean values (±SE) of affective response, altruism test and social preference index pre-and
post-intervention for both the experimental and control groups. Wilcoxon p-value.
PRE POST p-value
Affective response
Intervention group 6.7 (0.48) 7.5 (0.37) .05
Control group 10.2 (0.32) 10.8 (0.28) .26
Altruism test
Intervention group 1.36 (0.34) 2.32 (0.55) .03
Control group 2.48 (0.48) 2.61 (0.55) .7
Social preference
Intervention group 4.24 (0.87) 6.08 (0.93) .02
Control group 3.43 (0.62) 3.39 (0.61) .95
Figure 3. Pre- and post-intervention comparisons of the social preference index (SPI) for the
intervention group (IG) and the control group (CG). Effect size was medium for the IG (d=0.50)
while no effect was found for the CG (d=0.02).
Kappelmayer et al. 525
Discussion
This study shows that a long-term school intervention that sought to cultivate mindful
awareness, emotional regulation, empathic concern, and acts of kindness and compassion
promoted universal altruism and affective responses. Moreover, social integration was
also enhanced within the group that took part in the programme. Thus, our intervention
fostered compassionate attitudes in the participants, since their intention to alleviate the
suffering of unknown hospitalized children was greatly increased, as were their positive
emotions and affective empathy. In contrast, these changes were not observed in the wait-
list group. Our ndings indicate that when children are given the opportunity to develop
self-awareness and awareness of others in a socio-affective and socio-cognitive context,
feelings of compassion and caring emerge. In other words, consciousness of shared
humanity could have contributed to the emergence of compassion. An overall positive
affective resonance is therefore nurtured, and healthy social relationships are
strengthened.
The mindfulness practices carried out at the beginning of each session have shown to
play a signicant role in the promotion of serenity and appeasement, as reported in other
studies (e.g., Dambrun et al., 2019; Nadler et al., 2017; Wisner, 2014). This is in line with
the Polyvagal Theory, which proposes that physiological states of calmness are crucial
for experiencing compassion and social engagement (Porges, 2017). Furthermore, this
theory posits that prosocial behaviour is inuenced by vagal pathways involved in the
stress response (Porges, 2017). In addition to encouraging calmness and serenity, the
intervention focused on developing equanimity and impartiality, thus compassionate atti-
tudes were enhanced, which in turn contributed to greater social integration. This was
also evidenced by the fact that after the intervention participants were more prone to
sharing their goods with children in need who were unknown to them. The condition
of anonymity under which the intervention was conducted precluded any possible exter-
nal pressure which might have inuenced childrens decision making, thereby highlight-
ing the emergence of their intrinsic altruistic motivation. Compassion and altruism entail
feelings of warmth and concern for others in need and willingness and action to help them
(Gu et al., 2017; Halifax, 2012; Lozada et al., 2011; Singer & Klimecki, 2014).
Interestingly, these human virtues display great behavioural plasticity during childhood,
as shown in several investigations (Carro et al., 2020, 2021; Flook et al., 2015; Lozada
et al., 2014a; Ozawa-de Silva & Dodson-Lavelle, 2011; Viglas & Perlman, 2018;
Warneken & Tomasello, 2009).
An increase in the affective empathic response, described as the capacity to perceive
and share the emotions of others, was observed. This nding suggests that socio-
emotional skills were enhanced through the intervention. It has been proposed that
young children tend to express affective empathy, as they intend to comfort those who
are suffering, rather than exhibit more detached ways of understanding othersemotions
and thoughts (Hoffman, 1996; McDonald & Messinger, 2011). Our results are in line
with this developmental tendency to empathize since affective empathy was promoted
by the intervention. This affective attunement with others has been associated with com-
passion (e.g., Halifax, 2012; Porges, 2017) and may have contributed to the increase
526 School Psychology International 44(5)
observed in the social integration of the group of classmates after the intervention.
Accordingly, previous studies have proposed that compassion and empathy result in
deeper and more positive social bonds (e.g., Decety et al., 2016; Post, 2005).
Moreover, the lack of pre-post variation recorded in the waitlist group supports the
possibility that the changes observed in the intervention group can be attributed to par-
ticipation in the programme. However, it is noteworthy that although social preference
rates were similar in both groups, the experimental group began the intervention with
lower Universal Altruism and Affective Response values than the waitlist group. This
might be due to group characteristics that are beyond our control, for example a certain
group might evidence healthier relationships than another one. Despite this limitation,
our aim was to compare pre-post changes in each individual, thus lessening the poten-
tial deviation introduced by between-person variability. The relative changes pro-
moted during the intervention might not be affected by baseline levels.
Notwithstanding, a low-baselineeffect might be possible; therefore, further research
is needed to assess the benets of this kind of intervention in a larger and more diverse
sample, for example, in groups with different initial altruism and empathy levels.
Since there is a lack of research involving compassion-based interventions in develop-
ing countries, this study aims to redress this situation, as it has been conducted with
children living in a large city of South America. A potential limitation of the
current survey might be the fact that universal altruism was measured through a single-
trail pre- and post-intervention task that evaluated altruistic attitudes through a specic
response which might not reect long lasting behavioural changes. It would be inter-
esting for future studies to include alternative measurement assessments involving
tasks that could improve the methods of analysing the behavioural plasticity of this
social skill.
In conclusion, the present intervention enhanced childrens compassionate attitudes,
evidenced by the increase found in their affective and altruistic responses, positive emo-
tions and social integration. The safe, affective environment generated by the intervention
promoted a greater sense of connectedness among participants, both with themselves and
with others, which encouraged acts of kindness and compassion. These positive out-
comes are in line with previous studies which reveal that when compassion and empathic
concern are cultivated, signicant benets to health and wellbeing can be accrued (e.g.,
Brown & Brown, 2015; Carro et al., 2020, 2021; Lozada et al., 2011, 2014a, 2014b;
Ozawa-de Silva & Dodson-Lavelle, 2011; Porges, 2017; Post, 2005). Within the
current global post-pandemic context, in which social ties have been particularly affected
(e.g., Larivière-Bastien et al., 2022), fostering social integration and empathic attitudes
might be of particular importance for school psychologists. The promotion of compassion
and altruism in the school environment could contribute to enhanced individual and col-
lective psychological wellbeing.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all the children who participated in the study, and their parents and
schoolteachers.
Kappelmayer et al. 527
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the study conception and design. The intervention was conducted by MK,
AC and MT. Data collection and analysis were performed by MK, AC, MT, PD and ML. The rst
draft of the manuscript was written by PD and ML, and all authors commented on previous versions
of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the nal manuscript.
Declaration of conicting interests
The author(s) declared no potential conicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/
or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following nancial support for the research, authorship, and/
or publication of this article: This work was supported by the National University of Comahue and
National Council of Scientic and Technological Research of Argentina; Fondo para la
Investigación Cientíca y Tecnológica, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Cientícas y
Técnicas (grant number PICT-2020-SERIEA-03363, PIP # 11220170100498C).
Ethical approval
All research procedures were performed according to the World Medical Association Declaration of
Helsinki.
Informed consent
This includes obtaining fully informed consent from the parents of children who participated in the
study.
ORCID iD
Mariana Lozada https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0391-5479
References
Avinun, R., Israel, S., Shalev, I., Gritsenko, I., Bornstein, G., Ebstein, R. P., & Knafo, A. (2011).
AVPR1A variant associated with preschoolerslower altruistic behavior. PloS ONE,6(9),
e25274. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025274
Batson, C. D., & Ahmad, N. (2001). Empathyinduced altruism in a prisoners dilemma II: What if
the target of empathy has defected? European Journal of Social Psychology,31(1), 2536.
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1002/ejsp.26
Batson, C. D., Dyck, J. L., Brandt, J. R., Batson, J. G., Powell, A. L., McMaster, M. R., & Griftt,
C. (1988). Five studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy-altruism hypothesis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,55,5277. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/
0022-3514.55.1.52
Brown, S. L., & Brown, R. M. (2015). Connecting prosocial behavior to improved physical health:
Contributions from the neurobiology of parenting. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,55,
117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.04.004
528 School Psychology International 44(5)
Brown, W. M., Consedine, N. S., & Magai, C. (2005). Altruism relates to health in an ethnically
diverse sample of older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological
Sciences and Social Sciences,60(3), 143152. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/60.3.P143
Carro, N., DAdamo, P., & Lozada, M. (2020). An effective intervention can contribute to reducing
perceived stress while promoting social integration in children. EJREP,18(1), 183201. https://
doi.org/10.25115/ejrep.v18i50.2600
Carro, N., DAdamo, P., & Lozada, M. (2021). A school intervention helps decrease daily stress
while enhancing social integration in children. Behavioral Medicine,18. https://doi.org/10.
1080/08964289.2020.1738319
Carro, N., Kuperman, M., Dadamo, P., & Lozada, M. (2022). Social relationships in children:
favourable inuence of activities promoting self-awareness and empathic interaction. Journal
of Complex Networks,10(1). https://doi.org/10.1093/comnet/cnab049
Cheang, R., Gillions, A., & Sparkes, E. (2019). Do mindfulness based interventions increase
empathy and compassion in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Journal of Child
and Family Studies,28(7), 17651779. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01413-9
Cohen, S., & Janicki-Deverts, D. (2009). Can we improve our physical health by altering our social
networks? Perspectives on Psychological Science,4(4), 375378. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.
1745-6924.2009.01141.x
Dambrun, M., Berniard, A., Didelot, T., Chaulet, M., Droit-Volet, S., Corman, M., Juneau,
C., & Martinon, L. M. (2019). Unied consciousness and the effect of body scan medita-
tion on happiness: Alteration of inner-body experience and feeling of harmony as central
processes. Mindfulness,10(8), 15301544. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1007/s12671-
019-01104-y
Decety, J. (2010). The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Developmental Neuroscience,
32(4), 257267. https://doi.org/10.1159%2F000317771
Decety, J., Bartal, I. B. A., Uzefovsky, F., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2016). Empathy as a driver of pro-
social behaviour: Highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,371(1686), 20150077. https://doi.org/
10.1098%2Frstb.2015.0077
Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social
developmental neuroscience account. Development and Psychopathology,20(4), 10531080.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579408000503
Depraz, N., Varela, F. J., & Vermersch, P. (2003). On becoming aware: A pragmatics of experien-
cing (Vol. 43). John Benjamins Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1075/aicr.43
Eccles, J. S. (1999). The development of children ages 6 to 14. The Future of Children,9(2), 3044.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1602703
Eisenberg, N. (2002). Empathy-related emotional responses, altruism, and their socialization. In
R. J. Davidson, & A. Harrington (Eds.), Visions of compassion: Western scientists and
Tibetan Buddhists examine human nature (pp. 131164). Oxford University Press. https://doi.
org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130430.003.0007
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon, & R. A. Fabes (Eds.),
Handbook of child psychology (pp. 701777). New York: Wiley.
Estévez López, E., Martínez Ferrer, B., & Jiménez Gutiérrez, T. I. (2009). Las relaciones sociales en
la escuela: El problema del rechazo escolar. Educational Psychology,15(1), 4560. https://
www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=613765489007
Flook, L., Goldberg, S. B., Pinger, L., & Davidson, R. J. (2015). Promoting prosocial behavior and
self-regulatory skills in preschool children through a mindfulness-based kindness curriculum.
Developmental Psychology,51(1), 4451. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038256
Kappelmayer et al. 529
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build
lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential per-
sonal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,95(5), 10451062. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0038256
Fuentes, M. A., Cárdenas, J. P., Carro, N., & Lozada, M. (2018a). Development and complex
dynamics at school environment. Complexity,110. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/3963061
Fuentes, M. A., Cárdenas, J. P., Carro, N., & Lozada, M. (2018b). Social network plasticity in children.
Transylvanian Review,1(11). https://transylvanianreviewjournal.org/index.php/TR/article/view/2211
Garaigordobil, M. (2005). Programa Juego 6-8 años. Juegos cooperativos y creativos para grupos
de niños de 6 a 8 años. Madrid: Pirámide. http://hdl.handle.net/11162/61134
Gu, J., Cavanagh, K., Baer, R., & Strauss, C. (2017). An empirical examination of the factor struc-
ture of compassion. PLoS ONE,12(2), e0172471. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172471
Halifax, J. (2012). A heuristic model of enactive compassion. Current Opinion in Supportive and
Palliative Care,6(2), 228235. https://doi.org/10.1097/spc.0b013e3283530fbe
Hoffman, M. L. (1996). Empathy and moral development. The Annual Report of Educational
Psychology in Japan,35, 157162. https://doi.org/10.5926/arepj1962.35.0_157
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., & Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness and compassion medita-
tion: Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical Psychology Review,31(7), 11261132.
https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cpr.2011.07.003
Kalfus, G. R., & Berler, E. S. (1985). Test-Retest reliability of sociometric questionnaire across four
grade levels. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,14(4), 345347. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15374424jccp1404_13
Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Steindl, S. R. (2017). A meta-analysis of compassion-based inter-
ventions: Current state of knowledge and future directions. Behavior Therapy,48(6), 778792.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.06.003
Kok, B. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Upward spirals of the heart: Autonomic exibility, as indexed
by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness.
Biological Psychology,85(3), 432436. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.biopsycho.2010.09.005
Larivière-Bastien, D., Aubuchon, O., Blondin, A., Dupont, D., Libenstein, J., Séguin, F., Tremblay,
A., Zarglayoun, H., Herba, C. H., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2022). Childrens perspectives on
friendships and socialization during the COVID19 pandemic: A qualitative approach. Child:
Care, Health and Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12998
Lozada, M., Carro, N., DAdamo, P., & Barclay, C. (2014a). Stress management in children: A
pilot study for 7-9 year olds. Journal of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics,35, 144147.
http://hdl.handle.net/11336/26677. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000026
Lozada, M., Carro, N., Kapelmayer, M., Kelmanowicz, V., Czar, A., & DAdamo, P. (2017).
Fostering positive changes in health and social relationships in children. In C. Proctor (Ed.),
Positive psychology interventions in practice (pp. 143161). Springer International
Publishing AG. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-51787-2_9
Lozada, M., DAdamo, P., & Carro, N. (2014b). Plasticity of altruistic behavior in children. Journal
of Moral Education,43(1), 7588. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2013.878244
Lozada, M., DAdamo, P., & Fuentes, M. (2011). Intrinsic benets of human altruism. Journal of
Theoretical Biology,289,1216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.08.016
Maratos, F. A., & Shefeld, D. (2020). Brief compassion-focused imagery dampens physiological
pain responses. Mindfulness,11(12), 27302740. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01485-5
McDonald, N. M., & Messinger, D. S. (2011). The development of empathy: How, when, and why.
In J. J. Sanguineti, A. Acerbi, & J. A. Lombo (Eds.), Moral behavior and free will: A neurobio-
logical and philosophical approach (pp. 333359). IF-Press.
530 School Psychology International 44(5)
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2005). Attachment security, compassion, and altruism. Current
Directions in Psychological Science,14(1), 3438. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.
00330.x
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2017). An attachment perspective on compassion and altruism. In
P. Gilbert (Ed.), Compassion: Concepts, research and applications (pp. 187202). Routledge/
Taylor & Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315564296-11
Moreno, J. L. (1972). Fundamentos de la sociometría. Paidós.
Moyano, N., Ayllón, E., Antoñanzas, J. L., & Cano, J. (2019). Childrens social integration and low
perception of negative relationships as protectors against bullying and cyberbullying. Frontiers
in Psychology,10, 643. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00643
Nadler, R., Cordy, M., Stengel, J., Segal, Z. V., & Hayden, E. P. (2017). A brief mindfulness practice
increases self-reported calmness in young children: A pilot study. Mindfulness,8(4), 10881095.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0685-2
Oberle, E. (2018). Early adolescentsemotional wellbeing in the classroom: The role of personal
and contextual assets. Journal of School Health,88(2), 101111. https://doi.org/10.1111/josh.
12585
Ozawa-de Silva, B., & Dodson-Lavelle, B. (2011). An education of heart and mind: Practical and
theoretical issues in teaching cognitive-based compassion training to children. Practical
Matters,4(4), 128.
Perkins, N., Sehmbi, T., & Smith, P. (2022). Effects of kindness-and compassion-based meditation
on wellbeing, prosociality, and cognitive functioning in children and adolescents: A systematic
review. Mindfulness,125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-022-01925-4
Ponzi, D., Muehlenbein, M. P., Geary, D. C., & Flinn, M. V. (2016). Cortisol, salivary
alpha-amylase and childrens perceptions of their social networks. Social Neuroscience,
11(2), 164174. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1045988
Porges, S. W. (2017). Vagal pathways: Portals to compassion. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas,
S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of com-
passion science (pp. 189202). Oxford University Press.
Post, S. G. (2005). Altruism, happiness, and health: Its good to be good. International Journal of
Behavioral Medicine,12,6677. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_4
Potvin, A. S., Penuel, W. R., Dimidjian, S., & Jinpa, T. (2022). Cultivating skillful means of care in
schools through compassion practice and individual and joint inquiry. Mindfulness,15,117.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-022-01867-x
Reddy, S. D., Negi, L. T., Dodson-Lavelle, B., Ozawa-de Silva, B., Pace, T. W. W., Cole, S. P.,
Raison, C. L., & Craighead, L. W. (2013). Cognitive-based compassion training: A promising
prevention strategy for at-risk adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies,22(2), 219
230. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-012-9571-7
Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott, D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., &
Diamond, A. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and socialemotional development through a
simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: A rando-
mized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology,51(1), 52. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038454
Singer, T., & Klimecki, O. M. (2014). Empathy and compassion. Current Biology,24(18), R875R878.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.054
Spinrad, T. L., & Gal, D. E. (2018). Fostering prosocial behavior and empathy in young children.
Current Opinion in Psychology,20,4044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.004
Sullivan, G. M., & Feinn, R. (2012). Using effect sizeor why the P value is not enough.
Journal of Graduate Medical Education,4(3), 279282. https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-
D-12-00156.1
Kappelmayer et al. 531
Vanaelst, B., De Vriendt, T., Huybrechts, I., Rinaldi, S., & De Henauw, S. (2012). Epidemiological
approaches to measure childhood stress. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology,26(3), 280297.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-3016.2012.01258.x
Viglas, M., & Perlman, M. (2018). Effects of a mindfulness-based program on young childrens
self-regulation, prosocial behavior and hyperactivity. Journal of Child and Family Studies,
27(4), 11501161. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0971-6
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The roots of human altruism. British Journal of
Psychology,100(3), 455471. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X379061
Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004). Friendships in middle school: Inuences
on motivation and school adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology,96(2), 195. https://
doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.2.195
Wisner, B. L. (2014). An exploratory study of mindfulness meditation for alternative school stu-
dents: Perceived benets for improving school climate and student functioning. Mindfulness,
5(6), 626638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0215-9
Author biographies
Andrea Czar is a clinical psychologist. She obtained a degree in psychology and psycho-
pedagogy. She dedicates her professional clinical practice to work with children and their
families. She specializes in the design of positive interventions for children focusing on
the promotion of compassion and prosocial behavior.
Paola DAdamo is a researcher at the National Council for Scientic and Technological
Research and at the National University of Comahue, in Argentina. She has a PhD in
Biological Sciences. Her research topics focus on cognitive and socio-affective processes
related to health and well-being.
Marcela Kappelmayer is a clinical psychologist specialized in working with children
and families. Her research interest is the Design of Positive Interventions for the
Cultivation of Compassionate Disposition and Prosocial Behavior in Children.
Mariana Lozada is a researcher in the National Council of Science and Technology of
Argentina and the National University of Comahue. She got her PhD in neurobiology.
Her research topics are linked to embodied and situated approaches of social cognition
in children. She is particularly interested in enactive perspectives of cognitive science,
health and wellbeing.
Maria Tresca is an educational psychologist (psychopedagogue). She completed her
training at the Ramos Mejía Hospital in Buenos Aires. She is an assistant professor at
Favaloro University and a postgraduate professor at San Isidro University. She leads a
team that treats children with learning disabilities. Her research interests include Study
Skills and Learning strategies.
532 School Psychology International 44(5)
... self-compassion based) curricula is required. Recently, Kappelmayer et al. (2023) have demonstrated that an elementary-aged intervention more focused on promoting empathy, compassion and loving-kindness improved both individual and collective wellbeing of those (Argentinean) children that took part. That is, their curricula resulted in not only improved affective responding, but also altruism and social integration. ...
... Thus, recruitment of 60-70 pupils was considered a sensible medium when considering standard class sizes and allowing for attrition. As with Kappelmayer et al. (2023), as an element of our design was to investigate the effects of CMTpupils on prosocial behaviours in existing situations, this precluded random assignment of participants to each group. Six pupils (2 females, 4 males) from across the sample of 37 further took part in a focus group to discuss their experiences of CMT-Pupils in more detail. ...
... In all cases, all T1 data were carried over, as missing data reflected the 4 children being absent from school on the date T2 data were collected. To preclude effects of potential differences in initial values between groups, as per Kappelmayer et al. (2023), between-groups t-tests were used to establish if the groups significantly differed at T1 (i.e., baseline). If differences at T1 were not significant, a mixed-measures ANOVA, with time (T1, T2) as the within-subject factor and group (CMT-Pupils, PSHE as Usual) as the between-subject factors, was used to explore each quantitative wellbeing measure. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Mental health difficulties in children are increasing, especially following transition from elementary to high-school education. In attempts to mitigate risk of these difficulties, proactive wellbeing interventions are becoming increasingly explored in school settings. Recently, Compassionate Mind Training (CMT) has been shown to be well-accepted and efficacious in promoting school staff wellbeing and prosocial behaviours. This paper outlines the impact of CMT as a pupil wellbeing intervention. Method Sixty-seven pupils aged 11–12 took part in either Personal, Social, Health and Economic (PSHE) lessons as usual (n=30), or CMT-Pupils (n=37) as their PSHE lessons, over a 5-week period. A mixed-methods quantitative and qualitative design was utilised to explore implementation and curricula effectiveness across several wellbeing parameters (e.g., anxiety, self-compassion, perfectionism, moods and feelings, self-esteem). Results Pupils reported positively on their experiences of the CMT-Pupils lessons, content and practices. Quantitative analyses revealed a significant time-by-group interaction effect for anxiety, reflecting differences in anxiety post CMT-Pupils vs. PSHE as usual. No further interactions reached significance. Qualitative analyses revealed benefits of CMT-Pupils for pupil and classroom behaviour, including emotion regulation, kindness to others and feelings of inclusion. Benefits were also found to extend to the class teachers. Conclusions CMT-pupils could be a promising school-based wellbeing intervention for improving prosocial behaviours, the classroom environment and protecting against deteriorations in child mental health. Larger scale explorations of CMT-Pupils across wider demographics, including investigation of who can deliver the curriculum efficaciously (e.g., teachers vs. external facilitators), are suggested as next steps for investigation. Preregistration This study was not preregistered.
... Health and Well-being of Primary Schoolchildren Promoting the health and well-being of primary schoolchildren is of utmost importance for their overall development. Extensive research has been conducted to investigate various factors and interventions associated with child well-being [1,2,8]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Study Aim. Physical activity is the key to fostering healthy growth and development in children. Encouraging children to engage in activities that enhance their speed abilities not only boosts their physical fitness but also contributes to their mental and emotional well-being. The purpose of the study - exploring speed abilities: a comparative study of 9-year-old primary school children in Ukraine and beyond. Materials and Methods. The study was conducted at Kharkiv Central Educational School № 105 in Ukraine. A total of 29 children from the 4th grade (17 boys and 12 girls) participated in the study. These children were previously screened by a medical practitioner and were found to be in good health. Informed consent was obtained from the parents or guardians of the children, emphasizing their voluntary participation and the potential benefits and risks associated with the study. The study focused on assessing the speed abilities of the participating children. The tests included: 1) Running 30 meters (The time taken by each participant to run a distance of 30 meters was recorded in seconds); 2) Jumping rope (The number of times each participant successfully completed jumping rope within one minute was measured). Results. The study revealed that the general level of development of speed abilities among the participating children corresponded to the average level observed for this age group. Jumping Rope Results: 1) Boys - the average number of successful jumps per minute was 74.94 ± 13.85; 2) Girls - the average number of successful jumps per minute was 76.00 ± 28.16. 30-Meter Run Results: 1) Boys - the average time taken to complete the run was 6.64 ± 0.10 seconds; 2) Girls - the average time taken to complete the run was 6.63 ± 0.11 seconds. Conclusions. The findings of this study contribute to the existing body of knowledge on the speed abilities of 9-year-old primary schoolchildren, offering valuable insights for educators, researchers, and policymakers. Understanding the specific performance levels of boys and girls in jumping rope and running can inform the development of targeted interventions and programs aimed at enhancing speed-related skills among this age group.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter provides an overview of several initiatives that use compassion-based psychoeducation and practices to enhance well-being, emotional regulation and prosocial behaviours in educational settings, and discusses theory underlying curricula contents. The term curriculum/s is used when talking about initiatives that have only one version; those that have two distinct components are referred to as curricula. Presented first is an overview of the initiatives we deliver in primary and secondary schools as well as in the higher education sector, including why these are needed. These initiatives range from those for pupils and higher/further education students, to those for the individuals who teach these populations. Key theory is then introduced that supports the curricula and initiatives. This includes compassion as an evolutionary-informed, biopsychosocial approach, how emotions can affect behaviour (with a focus on drive, threat and soothing systems), and how compassion-based practices can support regulation of these emotion systems and prosocial behaviours (including kindness towards self and others). In a final section, we overview research evidence as to compassion in education curricula and consider issues of implementation quality. In sum, drawing on evidence-based research, this chapter demonstrates the impact that short compassion-based interventions can have for educator, student and pupil well-being, the main theories and research supporting the curriculums, and factors that affect intervention efficacy.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Interventions involving kindness- and compassion-based meditation (KCBM) have been shown to have various benefits for adults, and there is growing interest in using KCBMs with children. This systematic review explores the effects of KCBM on wellbeing, prosociality, and cognitive functioning in children and adolescents. Methods Studies were eligible if they examined interventions that contained a proportion of KCBM above a set threshold, included child participants only, used any or no control group, and included at least one outcome measure related to wellbeing, prosociality, or cognitive functioning. Studies were assessed for quality using the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies, and findings were synthesised narratively. Results A systematic literature search of 11 databases up to February 2020 identified 3,073 papers. Ten studies were eligible for inclusion in the review, including 807 children. There was evidence of improvements in wellbeing in 47% of wellbeing outcome measures (including stress, anxiety, depression, negative affect, markers of inflammation, mindfulness, and self-compassion). Prosociality and cognitive functioning (visual perception and motor accuracy) were examined in 1 study each, and there was evidence of improvements in both outcomes. Effect sizes ranged from small to large. There was some evidence that interventions were more effective with younger, non-clinical populations and where intervention teachers were experienced. Study quality was generally weak. Conclusions There was no strong evidence base for positive effects of KCBM with children. However, the findings of the review are encouraging given the early stage of development of the field, and further research is warranted. Recommendations for future research include more robust methodological design, improved reporting, and a focus on developmental mechanisms of change. Systematic Review Registration PROSPERO CRD42014013065.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives We examined the extent to which compassion practices helped guide skillful means of care among educators. We engaged educators in a collaborative design (co-design) process that foregrounded two components: (1) contemplative practice and (2) developing skill in how social interactions are embedded within wider systems through individual and joint inquiry. We analyzed the ways educators developed awareness of social suffering and set intentions to alleviate suffering. We examined how co-design fostered an understanding of compassion and new ideas about how to respond skillfully to suffering in schools. Methods Using qualitative methods, we analyzed data from educators who participated in co-design, including their written reflections, field notes, semi-structured interviews, and surveys. Results Educators identified multiple opportunities for acting with compassion, including approaching school-based interactions with compassion, cultivating compassion for themselves, and envisioning school change through a lens of compassion. Educators’ experiences in co-design directly informed how they imagined compassionate action in their schools. Specific elements highlighted were contemplative practice, reflection, and individual and joint inquiry. Conclusions The adaptation of a general program on compassion training can benefit from attending to how to show compassion in the context of concrete interactions in schools, and this can support educators in developing skillful means of care. Our analyses provide insight into the components that supported educators to offer compassion and suggest that educators’ skillful means of care can be cultivated through both contemplative practice and inquiry into social suffering. We offer a conceptual model for developing skillful means of care educational settings.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Good quality friendships and relationships are critical to the development of social competence and are associated with quality of life and mental health in childhood and adolescence. Through social distancing and isolation restrictions, the COVID-19 pandemic has had an impact on the way in which youth socialize and communicate with friends, peers, teachers and family on a daily basis. In order to understand children's social functioning during the pandemic, it is essential to gather information on their experiences and perceptions concerning the social changes unique to this period. The objective of this study was to document children and adolescents' perspectives regarding their social life and friendships during the COVID-19 pandemic, through qualitative interviews METHODS: Participants (N=67, 5-14 years) were recruited in May and June 2020. Semi-structured interviews were conducted via a videoconferencing platform. A thematic qualitative analysis was conducted based on the transcribed and coded interviews (NVivo) RESULTS: The upheavals related to the pandemic provoked reflection among the participants according to three main themes, each of which included sub-themes: 1) the irreplaceable nature of friendship, 2) the unsuspected benefits of school for socialization, and 3) the limits and possibilities of virtual socialization CONCLUSIONS: The collection of rich, qualitative information on the perspectives of children and adolescents provides a deeper understanding of the consequences of the pandemic on their socialization and psychological health and contributes to our fundamental understanding of social competence in childhood.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Affiliative processes are postulated to improve pain coping. Comparatively, compassion-focused imagery (CFI) also stimulates affiliate affect systems with a burgeoning behavioural, cognitive and physiological evidence base. Thus, the purpose of the present research was to investigate if engaging in brief CFI could improve pain coping. Methods Utilising a randomised repeated measures crossover design, 37 participants were subjected to experimental pain (cold pressor) following counter-balanced engagement with CFI or control imagery, 1 week apart. Salivary alpha-amylase (sAA) and questionnaire measures of emotional responding were taken: at baseline, following introduction to the imagery condition (anticipation), and immediately after the cold pressor pain task (actual). Results Participants exhibited increases in sAA levels in response to pain following control imagery but, no such changes were observed following CFI (i.e. there was a significant time-by-condition interaction). Pain tolerance (the length of time participants immersed their hands in the cold pressor) did not differ by imagery condition. However, sAA responses to actual pain predicted decreased pain tolerance in the CFI condition. Additionally, anticipatory sAA response predicted increased pain tolerance across both conditions. None of the emotional measures of well-being differed by imagery condition, nor by condition over time. Conclusions These data demonstrate that using CFI can curtail a physiological stress response to pain, as indicated by increases in sAA in the control imagery condition only, following pain; pain tolerance was not influenced by CFI. Compassion-based approaches may therefore help people cope with the stress associated with pain.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Empathy and compassion are important components of prosocial behaviour which can lead to greater peer acceptance and positive relationships in children and adolescents. As mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) have been shown to be effective in increasing empathy and compassion among adults, we aimed to systematically review the current literature to see if mindfulness-based interventions increased empathy and compassion in children and adolescents. Methods We systematically searched six databases, yielding 540 potentially relevant papers. Eight additional papers were identified through hand searching. After removing duplicates and screening titles and abstracts, the first two authors independently applied the inclusion/exclusion criteria to 34 full-text papers, leaving 16 eligible for inclusion. Studies that contained children and adolescents between 5 and 18 years old, measured empathy or compassion in some form and contained a mindfulness-based intervention were included. The first two authors independently checked the studies for methodological quality and data were extracted and synthesised narratively. Results We found convincing support in favour of MBIs increasing empathy in children and adolescents. Further, there was some evidence to suggest that MBIs increase self-compassion amongst this population and that this was correlated with an increase in mindfulness. Due to poor methodological quality in many of the included studies, these results should be interpreted with caution. Conclusions MBIs may be effective in increasing empathy and compassion in children and adolescents. Future research should concentrate on examining the mechanisms of change and the long-term effects of these interventions among a variety of different age groups and neurodiversity.
Article
Embodied and intersubjective experience is crucial to the social cognition processes on which interpersonal relationships are based. We assessed whether promoting these processes through participation in self-awareness and empathic interaction activities can influence social integration among peers in 7- to 8-year-old children. The intervention, conducted in a school context, included mindfulness-based practices (favouring self-awareness), empathic collaboration activities and perspective-taking instances (promoting other-oriented awareness). To evaluate social integration levels, children were asked to complete a sociometric questionnaire, each listing the peers they would choose to play with (positive ties) and those they would prefer not to play with (negative ties). Based on this relational data, two types of directed networks were constructed: positive networks (PN) and negative networks (NN), in two temporal instances (pre- and post-intervention). In both the experimental and waitlist groups, pre–post intervention changes in the topology of peer social networks were evaluated by analysing global network properties such as connectivity, degree distribution, density, reciprocity, transitivity and modular structure. Our findings showed that after participating in the intervention social integration was improved, reflected in increased general interconnection of positive ties: greater average connectivity and density in PN, more equitably distributed choices (no evident leadership), greater cohesion and an increase in the number of reciprocal interactions. Additionally, we found a lower level of social rejection; that is, lower average connectivity and density in NN, the persistence of only mutual negative choices and negative ties confined to small groups. These findings were not observed in the waitlist group. This study demonstrates how participation in an intervention that promotes social cognition processes can influence the structure of peer social networks, favouring social integration. The social network analysis provides quantitative evidence of the plasticity of social relationships in children, constituting a valuable tool for the assessment of this kind of intervention at a group level.
Book
This book searches for the sources and means for a disciplined practical approach to exploring human experience. The spirit of this book is pragmatic and relies on a Husserlian phenomenology primarily understood as a method of exploring our experience. The authors do not aim at a neo-Kantian a priori ‘new theory’ of experience but instead they describe a concrete activity: how we examine what we live through, how we become aware of our own mental life. The range of experiences of which we can become aware is vast: all the normal dimensions of human life (perception, motion, memory, imagination, speech, everyday social interactions), cognitive events that can be precisely defined as tasks in laboratory experiments (e.g., a protocol for visual attention), but also manifestations of mental life more fraught with meaning (dreaming, intense emotions, social tensions, altered states of consciousness). The central assertion in this work is that this immanent ability is habitually ignored or at best practiced unsystematically, that is to say, blindly. Exploring human experience amounts to developing and cultivating this basic ability through specific training. Only a hands-on, non-dogmatic approach can lead to progress, and that is what animates this book. (Series B)
Article
Introduction. This investigation evaluates whether an intervention involving mindfulness-based practices, socio-affective activities, and socio-cognitive instances can foster prosocial attitudes and positive social relationships, and reduce perceived stress in children. Method. The study was conducted with 44 children (20 girls and 24 boys) aged between 6 and 8 (M= 7.25 years, SD= 0.43), and followed a quasi experimental wait list design with two measurement time points (pre and post intervention). We analyzed relative changes in social integration, universal altruism and perceived stress levels. Results. Our results revealed that children who participated in the intervention showed an enhancement in social integration and universal altruism tests, and a decrease in the perceived stress indices. That is, children chose more peers as playmates and rejected fewer of them, while evidencing more prosocial attitudes. Discussion and Conclusion. Our findings suggest that this kind of intervention can contribute to fostering social integration and prosociality while promoting children’s health and wellbeing from an early age.
Article
Stress coping is highly relevant during childhood. This study analyses how the participation in a behavioral intervention involving mindfulness-based practices and empathic collaboration activities impact on diurnal cortisol rhythm and social integration in children. In both experimental and waitlist groups, we evaluated before and after the intervention: daily stress, by sampling salivary cortisol at three measurement time-points, and social integration, assessed by a social preference index. Daily average cortisol (DAC) and the area under the curve (AUC) differed when comparing pre-post intervention values in both groups: in the experimental group these measures decreased while in the waitlist group DAC and AUC increased. At the end of the intervention, the experimental group showed an enhancement in the social preference index whereas this parameter diminished in the waitlist group. This kind of behavioral intervention seems to be effective at reducing daily stress and improving social integration in Primary School children.