ChapterPDF Available
M
Mate Poaching
Maryanne L. Fisher
1
and T. Joel Wade
2
1
Department of Psychology, Saint Marys
University, Halifax, NS, Canada
2
Department of Psychology, Bucknell University,
Lewisburg, PA, USA
Synonyms
Mate-Stealing;Non-Independent Mate Choice
Definition
Mate poaching is engaging in premeditated
behaviors to attract either short-term or long-
term mates by luring them away from their
already established relationship.
Introduction
Generally, mate poaching refers to behaviors
whereby one attempts to romantically attract an
individual away from an existing monogamous
relationship. Mate poaching has been dened in
two different ways in the literature, with the sec-
ond denition serving as a clarication of the rst
denition. Schmitt and Buss (2001) originally
dened mate poaching as the process of romanti-
cally attracting someone who is already in a
relationship. However, Davies et al. (2007)
pointed out that Schmitt and Buss(2001)deni-
tion allowed for an overestimation of the fre-
quency of mate poaching, because it did not
specify two important criteria that are necessary
for mate poaching to have occurred. Davies et al.
(2007) stipulated that the relationship must be
exclusive such that sex with an individual other
than ones partner invalidates the relationship, and
poachers must know that the relationship they are
poaching from is exclusive. Thus, Davies et al.
(2007) revised the denition of mate poaching
such that it is said to occur when a person has
attracted, or tried to attract, a mate whom they
know is already in an existing exclusive relation-
ship, in order to form a new sexual or romantic
relationship. Further issues related to denition
are discussed by Davies et al. (2019).
It is helpful to distinguish mate poaching from
other forms of romantic attraction. Mate poaching
is distinct, because it presents costs that are not
found during typical, early stages of relationship
initiation or romantic attraction. For example, in
addition to the potential rejection by the person
one approaches, the persons mate may learn of
the attempted poaching and seek retaliation.
These costs are further discussed in the following
sections. Mate poaching also entails a specic set
of personality characteristics, which are presented
in an upcoming section.
Evolutionary psychology offers a way to make
sense of mate poaching. Mate poaching is a form
of nonindependent mate choice that is an adaptive
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020
T. K. Shackelford, V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1744-1
solution to the problem of locating a mate of
suitable quality from the local pool of those
whom are currently available. For men, this prob-
lem is thought to have been frequently occurring
in ancestral environments where women, in par-
ticular, were mated early in life, pregnant much of
their lifetime, and/or in situations involving
polygyny where a few men dominated local mat-
ing contexts. For women, it would have been an
adaptive solution to issues around resource con-
trol, such that women could try to invade an
existing relationship to secure a mate with owner-
ship of limited resources. Those resources, in turn,
may positively have impacted on their ability to
successfully bear and raise children, thereby
improving their reproductive success and tness.
Putting all of these ideas together creates an
interesting and unique mating-related situation.
Mate poachers (i.e., those who poach someones
mate) are theorized to engage in premeditated
behaviors to attract either short-term or long-
term mates via luring them away from their
already established relationship (Moran and
Wade 2019; Schmitt and Buss 2001). Mate
poaching involves covert behaviors and indirect
tactics, unlike other forms of romantic attraction,
due to the potential risks involved in luring an
already involved mate. Furtive glances, moving
into ones social network, and encouraging the
target to realize the problems and dissatisfaction
with their existing relationship are examples of the
sorts of covert behaviors a poacher may perform.
The discrete nature of romantic attraction is key,
as direct irting may lead to violence by an
existing partner and be seen as in poor taste by
the general social community (e.g., Schmitt and
ISDP 2004). Of these various strategies of mate
poaching, becoming the intended poachees
friend is viewed as a very effective strategy
which mitigates risk, However, individuals who
use that strategy are perceived by others as less
warm, less nurturant, and less friendly than those
who merely make the acquaintance of the poachee
(Mogilski and Wade 2013).
Occurrence and Prevalence
Estimates regarding the prevalence of mate
poaching vary, partly due to whether it is exam-
ined with respect to whether one attempts to lure a
short- versus long-term mate, or whether it
includes any experience versus frequent experi-
ence. For example, in a sample of US college
students and older adults (average of 41 years),
Schmitt and Buss (2001; study 1) report over 70%
had experience in attracting an already involved
partner, with about equal experience in attracting
short- versus long-term mates. Almost 30%
reported frequently trying to attract long-term
mates, while 10% frequently attempted to attract
short-term mates. Mate poaching specically,
though, was lower, with about 50% having some
experience in short- or long-term relationships,
with few stating they had frequent attempts.
Their sample reported that rate of poaching
toward oneself was higher, with an estimated
85% of people reporting someone had attempted
to poach them (Schmitt and Buss 2001).
There are some noteworthy sex differences.
Schmitt and Buss (2001) found men (6064%)
are more likely to have attempted to poach
a short-term mate than women (3849%,
depending on age). As an interesting aside, they
reported that mate poaching tactics are perceived
to be less effective when one attempts to inltrate
an existing highly committed relationship com-
pared to less committed relationships.
Davies et al. (2007) argued that these frequen-
cies from Schmitt and Buss (2001) may be over-
estimates due to differences in their denition.
They later reported their sampling procedure
may also be responsible; using a similar proce-
dure, Davies et al. (2019) found no signicant
difference in frequencies, as compared Schmitt
and Buss (2001).
In addition to the possible causes of variation
in prevalence already mentioned, geographic
location (as a proxy for culture) is an important
consideration. Across a large sample (i.e.,
53 nations, almost 17,000 participants), Schmitt
and members of the International Sexuality
Description Project (2004) found mate poaching
was most common in Southern Europe, South
2 Mate Poaching
America, Western Europe, and Eastern Europe
and was relatively infrequent in Africa, South/
Southeast Asia, and East Asia. Men were more
likely than women to report having made and
succumbed to short-term poaching across all
regions, but the differences between men and
women were often smaller in more gender-
egalitarian regions. That is, similar to the rates
reported using the US samples, Schmitt et al.
(2004) document around 62% of men and 40%
of women have attempted to attract someone who
was already in a relationship with someone else
for a short-term sexual relationship. However,
they argue that cultural differences exist: short-
term poaching is less among East Asian men
(around 29%), and under 30% for women from
Middle East, Africa, South/Southeast Asia, and
East Asia. Men seldom engage in long-term
mate poaching, with rates higher for women,
although there are cultural differences in effects
due to sex (i.e., Oceania shows minimal difference
whereas Africa shows large differences).
Those who attempt to mate poach tend to be
moderately successful at a rate of 65% or higher
(Schmitt et al. 2004, Table 4). Women are gener-
ally more successful at short-term mate poaching
than men (particularly in Western Europe; 90%
vs. 84%) but typically not signicantly so, with
the exception of Africa that shows the reverse
trend (65.9% vs. 78.6%). The occurrence and
prevalence of poaching for long-term relation-
ships is similar for men and women, with no
signicant sex differences except for South
America where more men report it ever happening
(86% vs. 72%), and happening more often. They
also found that about 70% of the sample world-
wide reported receiving someones mate-
poaching attempt.
Some scholars document minimal sex differ-
ences in prevalence rates of mate poaching, which
is in contrast to the abovementioned research
ndings. While the literature on indelity shows
men report higher rates than women, poaching is
reported nearly equally by the sexes. However,
women, but not men, may show a distinct,
increased interest in men who are involved in a
relationship. That is, Parker and Burkley (2009)
suggest mate copying may be a possible
explanation for womens interest in mate
poaching. Their experimental evidence shows
men are interested in women to the same degree
regardless of whether they are single or not, while
single women are more interested in pursuing men
who are attached than single. By engaging in mate
copying, women are ensuring that the mate has
been pre-screenedfor quality, resourcefulness,
and willingness to commit to a family life, by
another woman. This nding does not imply that
men are immune from mate copying; Moran and
Wade (2019) report that men are more likely to
engage in mate copying when a couple is com-
posed of a man who is more attractive than his
partner. This pattern occurs because men view the
woman in this couple as possessing very high
qualities since she was selected by this highly
attractive man. Finding and starting a relationship
with a woman with characteristics similar to the
woman in this couple would potentially confer
benets to the male copiers future offspring.
Costs Associated with Mate Poaching
Mate poaching is a form of mating competition,
and as such, it can lead to various forms of retal-
iation from the person who loses the mate. There
may be immediate repercussions for poaching via
violence or other forms of aggression. There may
be also a decrease in social standing, given one
has breached social norms around exclusivity and
relationships (see Sunderani et al. 2013, for
discussion).
The poachers who are themselves currently in
an existing, exclusive romantic relationship must
deceive their partner. They may lie or manipulate
their partner, keep themselves out of close prox-
imity to the partner (e.g., be away for work),
establish their independence (e.g., I need
space), and perhaps increase their affection and
resource allocation toward the current mate in an
effort to avoid drawing suspicions of indelity
(Schmitt and Shackelford 2003).
It may also lead to the presence of future in-
delity concerns and feeling uncertain about the
future of the relationship. There are other costs,
too, such as feeling guilty, issues around
Mate Poaching 3
deception and lying, hiding the relationship, and
potential rejection by ones family (Schmitt and
Buss 2001). Individuals who then form a relation-
ship with the poacher (or person poached) may
experience feelings of jealousy, emotional pain,
anxiety, and sadness; they may face serious risks
due to these emotions such as intimate partner
violence and homicide (see Sunderani et al.
2013, for discussion).
For men, there are also costs related to poten-
tially depleting their resources while engaging in
poaching behavior, especially if those resources
could be used more productively to gain access to,
and retain mates. Men who lose their partner to a
mate poacher face further costs, not only do they
experience the loss of a mate (i.e., the poached
individual) but also they risk losing any invested
resources. Misallocated investments may also
stem from the possibility of raising a child who
is genetically unrelated, if their partner is poached,
becomes pregnant, and then returns to the
relationship.
Both sexes face the increased risk of illness; as
one accesses new sexual partners, risks of sexu-
ally transmitted infections and diseases increase.
There is also an increased probability of partners
future indelity, if they were successfully poached
in the past. Women face additional costs of poten-
tial unwanted pregnancy, as well as potential feel-
ings of self-degradation, and concerns about
future indelity (Schmitt and Buss 2001).
Reasons to Poach a Mate
The reasons to poach a mate vary. Schmitt and
Buss (2001) found several potential reasons, as
follows. First, within a long-term context, men
may be enticed by a womans physical attractive-
ness while women may be enticed by a man with
resources. Indeed, highly attractive women are
more likely to be the targets of mate-poaching
attempts and less likely to remain faithful in a
relationship. Women do not show the same benet
of gaining an attractive mate via poaching
although men who are targeted by poachers self-
report they are physically attractive (Sunderani
et al. 2013). Men also report a potential benet
of gaining access to a variety of sexual partners, as
well as easy sexual access within short-term mate
poaching contexts. Thus, sexual variety, attracting
a beautiful partner, and freedom from responsibil-
ity for men are considered short-term benets
(Schmitt and Buss 2001).
Men do not consider receiving resources as
benecial as women do, or consider gaining a
partner with the ability to accrue resources as
benecial. Interestingly, the benet of receiving
immediate resources was considered strongest in
women considering short-term mate poaching, as
opposed to long-term mate poaching. Women also
reported the additional reasons of taking revenge
on a rival and gaining an already proven mate.
Personality of Mate Poachers and Those
Who Are Poached
Schmitt and Buss (2001) examined various per-
sonality traits in relation to mate poaching. They
found that agreeable and conscientious (i.e., care
and think about others feelings) people are sig-
nicantly less likely to be poachers regardless of
sex. Further, those who self-describe as having
erotophilic dispositions more likely to have tried
to poach. Individuals who reported success at
poaching generally are more sexually attractive
and lack sexual exclusivity. Extraverts and those
open to experience receive more mate-poaching
attempts, as do those who describe themselves
as being emotionally investing in a relationship.
Disagreeable, unconscientious, and neurotic indi-
viduals tended to accept mate poachersadvances,
as did those describing themselves as mean,
unreliable, adulterous, masculine, and unloving.
A lower level of conscientiousness is associ-
ated with increased sexual risk-taking, as well as
perceived benet of such behaviors. The lack of
empathy linked to being unconscientious, higher
disagreeableness, and disregard for otherswell-
being may empower individuals to poach a mate
when faced with an opportunity and decrease their
perception of any associated costs (Mitchell et al.
2019). Indeed, Foster et al. (2014) report that
poachers tend to have more uninhibited sexual
attitudes and behaviors. Poachers of both sexes
4 Mate Poaching
self-report a higher number of lifetime sex part-
ners, more lifetime casual sex partners, and more
lifetime dating partners (Arnocky et al. 2013).
Sunderani et al. (2013) report that men who
poach are better looking, taller, and have higher
cortisol levels, which is a hormone that has been
linked with impulsivity and extroversion. They
also have higher self-esteem, which may lead
them to believe they will be more successful at
poaching and subsequently more likely to attempt
to poach. They have higher criminal tendencies
and more of a cold affect (i.e., little concern about
others). They suggest that these characteristics
enable the individual to behave without feeling
empathy or facing moral objections with respect
to poaching. They also surprisingly found that
men with lower levels of testosterone were more
successful at mate poaching, which requires fur-
ther study. In contrast, they found for women who
poach, the only important factor is that they are
attractive.
The dark triad of personality traits (DT) also
matters. Kardum et al. (2015) reported that the DT
predicts mate poaching experiences better than the
Big Five traits, which are openness, conscien-
tiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neu-
roticism. These authors report that the DT trait of
psychopathy and the Big Five trait of extraversion
are the most consistent predictors of mate
poaching experiences. However, there are also
noteworthy sex differences. The DT better pre-
dicts being the target of a poaching attempt in
women and being successfully poached in men.
Also, the Big Five traits better predict success at
mate poaching for men.
The relationship between personality and mate
poaching is small to moderate, according to
Kardum et al. (2018). Poaching attempts,
poaching success, and being the target of
poaching appear to be more strongly related to
personality than being successfully poached or
being the victim of poaching. They further report
that the link between mate poaching and person-
ality are cross-cultural and similar in both short-
and long-term relationships.
Targets of Mate Poaching
There are particular qualities of individuals who
make them more susceptible targets for being
poached. For example, tactics related to mate
poaching are perceived to be more effective
when the relationship where someone is being
poached from is long-distance, not committed,
about to end, or that is a dating relationship
(Schmitt and Buss 2001). The specic role of the
type of relationship within a mate poaching con-
text depends on the tactic. Women are viewed as
most effective when they use tactics related to
emphasizing their physical appearance, while
men are seen as most effective when they empha-
size their resources and generosity. In short-term
contexts, women are perceived to be most effec-
tive when they advertise, provide, and arrange
easy sexual access. Men who manipulated the
emotional delity of the existing male partner
were seen as effective at poaching, while women
who manipulated the sexual delity of the existing
female partner were also seen as effective
(Schmitt and Buss 2001).
Individuals who are extraverted and high in
openness (sensation seekers), sexy, adulterous,
or high in neuroticism are typical targets also.
There may be sex differences also. Moran and
Wade (2019) report that men are most likely to
attempt to poach from a couple where there is a
discrepancy in the attractiveness of the members
of the couple. Specically, men are more likely to
attempt to poach from a couple where the woman
is more attractive than her partner, and men think
that a poaching attempt towards this type of cou-
ple will be most successful. The duration of the
relationship matters also. Moran et al. (2017) and
Moran and Wade (2019) report that for short-term
mating, men are more willing to poach women
who are more attractive than the mates those
women are involved with in a relationship.
Disguising Mate Poaching
Since mate poaching can have dire consequences
for mate poachers if their behavior is detected,
individuals may disguise their poaching attempts.
Mate Poaching 5
Tooke and Camire (1991) point out that men in
particular use a complex repertoire of deceptive
tactics to gain access to reproductively viable
women. Schmitt and Shackelford (2003) speci-
cally examined tactics within the context of mate
poaching, rst within the context of how people
signal an openness to being poached and then in
terms of disguising their poaching.
In the rst part, they asked students (11 men
and 18 women) to nominate behaviors that they
think people perform while already in a relation-
ship to let others know they are open to another
relationship. The participants were asked to con-
sider behaviors one might do to advertise an inter-
est in short- and long-term relationships. Then,
they asked students (26 men and 46 women) to
rate the effectiveness of the behaviors in signaling
interest in a new relationship. They found the
most effective tactics for men seeking a short-
term relationship were to enhance the potential
mate (e.g., boost her ego, compliment her, tell
her she deserves someone better), use humor,
and be generous (e.g., show they are caring,
polite, helps her with work or chores), while for
women, it was arranging and providing easy sex-
ual access, and enhancing the potential mate. For
men, seeking a long-term relationship effective
tactics were the same, while for women, it was
to develop emotional closeness (conde in him,
try to be a good friend, discuss mutual interests),
mention they are looking for a replacement mate,
and being generous.
In the second part, they again used an act
nomination approach and asked students
(20 men, 37 women) to list ways people disguise
mate-poaching attempts for short- and long-term
relationships. The poachers were stated to cur-
rently be in a relationship and who are attempting
to disguise their actions in order to maintain that
relationship. Then, another group of students
(36 men and 44 women) rated the effectiveness
of the tactics. The results indicated that the
most effective ways to disguise mate-poaching
attempts by men are as follows: talk with current
partner about their future together as a family, pay
closer attention to their current partner, keep con-
stant watch on their current partner to see if they
have suspicions, have deep emotional talks with
their current partner, spend more quality time with
the current partner, pretend one is happy with the
current partner, spend less time away from the
current partner, tell the current partner he is satis-
ed with their relationship, and do not discuss the
new partner with anyone.
The researchers also examined what the most
effective ways for women to disguise their
poaching attempts are. Like men, many of these
tactics are oriented toward their current romantic
partner, such that they are attempting to maintain
that relationship. The tactics are: maintain her
daily routine to avoid causing suspicion, do not
change her physical look, always return home at
same time each day, wear her relationship (e.g.,
wedding, engagement, promise) ring all the time,
do not discuss the new partner with anyone,
become more romantic with the current partner,
attend fewer parties so as to be seen less, have sex
more often with the current partner, rekindle the
romance of the current relationship, and do not act
like anything is different.
The authors also examined how many of these
actions are also effective for men and women to
keep their social community from knowing they
are engaged in mate poaching (Schmitt and
Shackelford 2003). However, while there is
overlap in some tactics, there are also tactics
that seem specically oriented toward hiding
information about the mate poaching from
ones social community. For men, the most effec-
tive ways are as follows: do not discuss the new
partner with anyone, keep a constant watch on
the current partner, think before he speaks in
public, have deep emotional talks with the cur-
rent partner, spends less time away from the
current partner, behave affectionately toward
the current partner, talk with the current partner
about their future together as a family, pretend he
is happy with the current partner, and get the
current partner pregnant.
A similar list was obtained for women; the
most effective ways for women to disguise their
poaching attempts are as follows: maintain her
daily routine, do not change her physical look,
always return home at same time each day, do
not act like anything is different, keep her conver-
sation routine and not talk about what is really
6 Mate Poaching
going on in her life, never tell anyone about her
new partner, keep the current partner sexually
satised, wear her relationship ring all the time,
behave affectionately toward the current partner,
and rekindle the romance of the current
relationship.
Schmitt and Shackelford (2003) concluded
that overall, women are most effective at mate
poaching when they advertised or provided sexual
intimacy and discounted improvements in physi-
cal appearance. Women were seen to be most
effective at disguising their mate poaching when
maintaining daily routines. For men, the most
effective tactics are when they advertised their
resources and used tactics related to establishing
an emotional connection. The best disguise for
men was when they talked to their current partner
about their future together as a family.
Some of these ndings have been recently
replicated. For example, Moran and Wade
(2017) performed a two-part study. In study 1,
they asked heterosexual men to nominate acts
they would use to mate poach someone speci-
cally for a short-term relationship. In study 2, men
and women reported which of those acts would be
most effective. The most effective acts were
spending time together, being attentive, being
compassionate, helping her with her problems,
and complimenting her. The authors propose
these acts, when performed by men, signal altru-
ism and emotional commitment, which women
prefer in mates.
Relationships Outcomes
Despite the volume of research on mate poaching,
there has been a dearth of investigation into the
quality of relationships involving poached indi-
viduals. Two studies are worth noting, though,
that address this issue.
Foster et al. (2014) examined the relationship
quality of 138 heterosexual young adults over a
10-week period. They reported that poached indi-
viduals perceive potential relationship alternatives
to be higher in quality and have more indelity in
their current relationship than non-poached indi-
viduals. Poached individuals are less committed
to their current relationship and less satised over-
all, as compared to those who were not poached.
Belu and OSullivan (2018) extended this prior
work by examining a variety of issues related to
relationship quality in 675 heterosexual adults,
which included a specically solicited sample of
those with mate poaching experience. They report
individuals in romantic relationships formed by
poaching rated their relationships as having lower
satisfaction, commitment, and trust, higher jeal-
ousy, and higher rates of emotional and sexual
indelity compared to those in non-poached rela-
tionships (Belu and OSullivan 2018; largely rep-
licating Foster et al. 2014). Further, those who
were poached compared to those who did the
poaching also rated their current relationship as
lower in commitment (Belu and OSullivan
2018). The link between poaching and relation-
ship quality is inuenced by sociosexuality.
Poachers tend to have more permissive and less
restrictive views of sexual behavior, and require
less commitment which is often the cornerstone of
exclusive relationships. Thus, is it not surprising
that mate poachers have an increased probability
of engaging in an indelity while in a relationship
(Belu and OSullivan 2019).
Sexual Versus Emotional Mate Poaching
One area for future research may be to further
investigate how mate poaching may occur in sex-
ual versus emotional contexts. Research into in-
delity has typically examined it in terms of
whether the behaviors relate to sexual indelity
or emotional indelity. Mitchell et al. (2019)
extended this work by examining sexual versus
emotional mate poaching. They report that mate
poachers who engaged in sexual intimacy with
their poached partner differed the most from
those who did not poach their current partner on
several dimensions, including higher antagonism,
disinhibition, risk-taking, and lower humility and
honesty. They further documented that men who
engaged in sexual intimacy with their poached
partner reported higher levels of detachment,
antagonism, disinhibition, psychopathy, and
lower levels of conscientiousness, humility, and
Mate Poaching 7
honesty. The authors suggest that this combina-
tion of traits may allow a mate poacher to maintain
a physical relationship with an already partnered
individual because it includes a willingness to
deceive and manipulate others for personal gain,
and the discounting of potential costs.
For comparison, men who engaged in emo-
tional mate poaching still reported high levels of
antagonism, disinhibition, and ethical risk-taking,
and lower scores for humility and honesty than
non-poachers. However, their scores were lower
for ethical risk-taking than men who engaged in
physical mate poaching. The researchers propose
that the differences between the two poaching
groups may be due to seeking alternative paths
to poaching. That is, men who engage in emo-
tional mate poaching may be attempting to estab-
lish a romantic bond with trust rather than sexual
access. Women, for comparison, did not demon-
strate as many differences between the two forms
of poaching as men, and most of the traits were not
exhibited as strongly as men.
Conclusions
In this entry, we briey reviewed the extensive
literature on mate poaching. Our review started
with issues surrounding differences in deni-
tions of the behavior, and how it is distinct
from other forms of romantic attraction. We
then discuss prevalence, showing that there is
cross-cultural variation in mate poaching, but
also differences according to an individuals
sex and according to whether one examines any
poaching experiences or frequent experiences.
The considerable costs associated with mate
poaching were reviewed, followed by a discus-
sion about the potential benets one may
receive. We then shifted our focus to present an
overview of some of the ndings about person-
ality of those involved in mate poaching relation-
ships, as well as characteristics of targeted
individuals. Research on how mate poaching
may be disguised was reviewed, along with
details about some of the tactics men and
women are thought to use. We presented the
small body of research on the outcomes of
relationships that started with one individual
being poached. We then ended our review with
an example of an area for future research, which
is the examination of mate poaching along sex-
ual versus emotional lines, akin to the large cor-
pus of work on indelity. Collectively, our
review demonstrates the variety of ways mate
poaching has been explored using the lens of
evolutionary psychology and how the topic has
changed in scope over time.
Cross-References
Personality and Mate Poaching
Costs and Benets of Mate Poaching
Use of Mate Retention Strategies
References
Arnocky, S., Sunderani, S., & Vaillancourt, T. (2013).
Mate-poaching and mating success in humans. Journal
of Evolutionary Psychology, 11,6583.
Belu, C. F., & OSullivan, L. F. (2018). Why nd my own
when I can take yours?: The quality of relationships
that arise from successful mate poaching. Journal of
Relationships Research, 9(e6), 110.
Belu, C. F., & OSullivan, L. F. (2019). Once a poacher
always a poacher? Mate poaching history and its asso-
ciation with relationship quality. The Journal of Sex
Research.https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2019.16
10150. pre-print.
Davies, A. P., Shackelford, T. K., & Hass, R. G. (2007).
When a poachis not a poach: Re-dening human
mate poaching and re-estimating its frequency.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 702716.
Davies, A. P. C., Tranter, A. E., & Shackelford, T. K.
(2019). Not clearly dened, not reliably measured,
and not replicable: Revisiting the denition and mea-
surement of human mate poaching. Personality and
Individual Differences, 145, 103105.
Foster, J. D., Jonason, P. K., Shrira, I., Campbell, W. K.,
Shiverdecker, L. K., & Varner, S. C. (2014). What do
you get when you make someone elses partner your
own? An analysis of relationships formed via mate
poaching. Journal of Research in Personality, 52,
7890.
Kardum, I., Hudek-Knezevic, J., Schmitt, D. P., &
Grundler, P. (2015). Personality and mate poaching
experiences. Personality and Individual Differences,
75,712.
Kardum, I., Hudek-Knezevic, J., & Mehic, N. (2018).
Entry: Personality and mate poaching. In
8 Mate Poaching
T. K. Shackelford & V. A. Shackelford (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science.
Cham: Springer.
Mitchell, V. E., Mogilski, J. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., &
Welling, L. L. M. (2019). Mate poaching strategies
are differentially associated with pathological
personality traits and risk-taking in men and women.
Personality and Individual Differences, 142,
110115 .
Mogilski, J. K., & Wade, T. J. (2013). Friendship as a
relationship inltration tactic during human mate
poaching. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(4),
147470491301100415.
Moran, J., & Wade, T. J. (2017). Sex and the perceived
effectiveness of short-term mate poaching acts in
college students. Human Ethology Bulletin, 32(3),
109128.
Moran, J. B., & Wade, T. J. (2019). Perceptions of a
mismatched couple: The role of attractiveness on
mate poaching and copying. Evolutionary Behavioral
Sciences, online.
Moran, J. B., Kuhle, B. X., Wade, T. J., & Seid, M. A.
(2017). To poach or not to poach? Men are more willing
to short-term poach mated women who are more attrac-
tive than their mates. EvoS Journal: The Journal of
Evolutionary Studies Consortium, 8,5869.
Parker, J., & Burkley, M. (2009). Whos chasing whom:
The impact of gender and relationship status on mate
poaching. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
45, 10161019.
Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2001). Human mate
poaching: Tactics and temptations for inltrating
existing mateships. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80(6), 894917.
Schmitt, D. P., & International Sexuality Description Pro-
ject. (2004). Patterns and universals of mate poaching
across 53 nations: The effects of sex, culture, and
personality on romantically attracting another persons
partner. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
86(4), 560584.
Schmitt, D. P., & Shackelford, T. K. (2003). Nifty ways to
leave your lover: The tactics people use to entice and
disguise the process of human mate poaching. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(8),
10181035.
Sunderani, S., Arnocky, S., & Vaillancourt, T. (2013).
Individual differences in mate poaching: An exam-
ination of hormonal, dispositional, and behavioral
mate-value traits. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
42, 533545.
Tooke, W., & Camire, L. (1991). Patterns of deception in
intersexual and intrasexual mating strategies. Ethology
and Sociobiology, 12(5), 345364.
Mate Poaching 9
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
This research investigated how a couple’s discrepancy in attractiveness influences men’s decision to mate poach or mate copy. The participants (N = 97 heterosexual men) were presented with 3 photos of a quasi couple in which the woman was consistent, and the 3 men were unattractive, equally, or more attractive than her. This study used ranking questions to assess heterosexual men’s perception of a couple. Participants were asked to drag and drop the 3 randomized photos in order of preference for 8 randomized questions regarding mate poaching and mate copying. Eight Friedman tests were conducted and revealed a significant difference between the rankings of the photos for each situation. These findings suggest that there are clear differences between the conditional mating strategies men use. Results revealed that when a woman is more attractive than her mate, men desire to mate poach, and if a woman is less attractive than her partner, men desire to mate copy.
Article
Full-text available
Mate poaching occurs when a person attracts another, whom he or she knows is already in an exclusive relationship, into a sexual or romantic relationship. Mate poaching is involved in the evolution of many relationships. Yet, little is known about the quality of these relationships. We examined relationship quality between individuals whose relationships were formed via mate poaching versus not (i.e., a relationship formed serially without overlap with another relationship). We compared ratings of quality from the perspectives of poachers, poached, and co-poached individuals. Adult participants ( n = 660) in a romantic relationship responded to questions assessing relationship quality. Those in relationships formed from poaching rated their relationships as lower in relationship satisfaction, commitment and trust, and higher in jealousy, and had higher rates of romantic and sexuality infidelity in their current relationship compared to individuals in non-poached relationships. Those who were poached from an existing relationship rated their current relationship as lower in commitment than did those who poached their current partner into a relationship. The study also provides first insights regarding relationship quality for those who identify as co-poached. We discuss these findings in terms of implications for understanding how relationships are formed and the qualities of those that endure.
Article
Full-text available
Short-term poaching allows men to gain reproductive benefits with a mated woman without the costs of commitment (Schmitt & Buss, 2001). The current research explores whether men (N = 38) target mated women for short-term poaches as a function of their and their partners' relative physical attractiveness. By manipulating a hypothetical couple's attractiveness discrepancy, we found that men were more willing to strategically short-term poach mated women who were significantly more attractive than their mates compared to mated women who were equally or less attractive than their mates.
Article
Full-text available
It is well documented that many relationships form via mate poaching (i.e., stealing someone’s partner), but almost nothing is known about how these relationships function. Across three studies, we observed reliable evidence that individuals who were poached by their current romantic partners were less committed, less satisfied, and less invested in their relationships. They also paid more attention to romantic alternatives, perceived their alternatives to be of higher quality, and engaged in higher rates of infidelity compared to non-poached participants. Two longitudinal studies offered conflicting evidence regarding whether relationship dysfunction associated with mate poaching develops over time or is a stable quality. Evidence from a cross-sectional study suggests that individual differences in sociosexual-orientation help to explain link between mate poaching and relationship dysfunction. (word count: 120 words)
Article
Successful mate poaching is a form of infidelity that occurs when one partner knowingly attracts the mate of another with the intention of starting a sexual and/or romantic relationship with this individual. Relationships formed from poaching tend to be of lower quality than their non-poached counterparts. A history of poaching might reflect a sociosexuality that propels seeking new partnerships without regard for exclusivity. It is unknown whether serial poaching for relationship formation is linked to more permissive sociosexual orientation. Adults (N = 653; aged 25–40; 57% women) in a romantic relationship completed online surveys assessing mate poaching, poaching history, sociosexuality, and relationship quality (commitment, satisfaction, trust, jealousy). Those in a poached relationship at the time of the study had a more extensive history of poached relationships and a more permissive sociosexuality. Participants who reported a more extensive history of mate poaching reported poorer quality relationships. The link between poaching history and relationship quality was partially accounted for by sociosexuality. This research adds to our understanding of difficulties that may be associated with the relationships of individuals who use poaching as a relationship initiation strategy, and the challenges that permissive sociosexuality may present for maintaining long-term relationships.
Article
Schmitt and Buss' (2001) seminal definition of poaching labelled it as “behavior intended to attract someone who is already in a romantic relationship.” Davies, Shackelford, and Hass' (2007) definition stated that to be considered a poach, the targeted individual's relationship must be exclusive and the pursuer must be aware of this at the time. Davies et al. found percentages reporting experience with poaching among their participants were lower than percentages found by Schmitt and Buss among their participants. To investigate whether these differences in percentages were due to differences between the two samples, the current study was the first to compare percentages reporting experience with poaching secured through the different definitions in a single sample. Findings indicated no differences in percentages secured through the different definitions. Discussion argues, however, that as Schmitt and Buss' definition may be perceived as denoting the form of attraction denoted by Davies et al.'s definition as well as two other forms of attraction, findings may be reliable only if researchers secure individual findings for each form of attraction denoted by Schmitt and Buss' definition. In addition, researchers should reach a consensus regarding which of the forms of attraction denoted by Schmitt and Buss' definition constitutes poaching.
Article
Prior research has evaluated which personality traits predict mate poaching behavior (i.e., attempts to attract an individual known to be in a relationship with another person) by soliciting retrospective reports of mate poaching success. Here we investigate differences in personality among those who are presently within poached versus non-poached relationships. Furthermore, we distinguish relationships wherein the poacher was physically intimate with versus romantically interested in their partner when they were in a relationship with another person. Men within poached relationships scored higher on detachment, negative affectivity, disinhibition, antagonism, and psychoticism compared to non-poacher men. Furthermore, men who had been physically intimate with their partner while their partner was in a relationship with someone else scored higher on ethical risk-taking compared to non-poacher men. Women within poached relationships had higher scores on antagonism, detachment, and ethical risk-taking compared to non-poachers, although female poachers scored similarly to non-poacher men on all measures. These results suggest that individuals may use distinct mate poaching strategies associated with varying degrees of risk, and that men and women may employ these strategies differently.
Article
Although previous research suggests Big Five (BF) and Dark Triad (DT) traits (Narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy) are related to mate poaching, no studies have simultaneously examined the effects of both groups of traits on poaching. This study examined BF and DT personality traits, as well as their higher order factors as predictors of mate poaching behaviors in short-term relationships among a sample of 819 university students. Results showed DT better predicted mate poaching experiences than BF traits, though they better predicted poaching success in men. Regarding specific traits, psychopathy from DT, and extraversion from BF, were the most consistent predictors of mate poaching experiences. Considering sex differences, DT better predicted being the target of poaching in women, and being successfully poached in men. There were no consistent sex differences in the effects of BF traits on mate poaching. Generally, personality traits better predicted poaching attempts, poaching success, and being the target of poaching than other poaching experiences. Possible different roles of BF and DT traits for mate poaching experiences in men and women were discussed.