ArticlePublisher preview available

Appreciation of Form in Bach’s Well-Tempered Clavier

University of California Press
Music Perception: an interdisciplinary journal
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

While previous research has raised doubts about the ability of listeners to perceive large-scale musical form, we hypothesize that untrained and unfamiliar listeners can, indeed, recognize structure when cognitive form judgments (coherence and predictability) are differentiated from enjoyment ratings (pleasantness, interest, and desire to hear again). In a between-groups experiment, listeners (n = 125) were randomly assigned to hear one of four versions of Bach’s Prelude in C minor from Book I of The Well-Tempered Clavier: 1) the original; 2) a mildly scrambled one in which two larger sections were switched; 3) a highly scrambled one; and 4) a randomized one. Significant differences were observed between versions in ratings of coherence and predictability, but not in ratings of pleasantness, interest, or desire to hear again. Individuals who had played the piece before could also explicitly identify structural intervention. It was assumed that relative incoherence would result in higher complexity and, thus, be reflected in longer retrospective duration estimates; however, estimates did not differ between stimuli. These results suggest that untrained listeners can evaluate global form, independently of their level of familiarity with a musical piece, while also suggesting that awareness of incoherence does not always correspond with decreased enjoyment.
This content is subject to copyright.
APPRECIATION OF FORM IN BACHSWELL-TEMPERED CLAVIER:
EFFECTS OF STRUCTURAL INTERVENTIONS ON PERCEIVED COHERENCE,
PLEASANTNESS,AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION ESTIMATES
GEOFFREY MCDONALD &CLEMENS WO
¨LLNER
University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
W
HILE PREVIOUS RESEARCH HAS RAISED DOUBTS
about the ability of listeners to perceive large-scale
musical form, we hypothesize that untrained and
unfamiliar listeners can, indeed, recognize structure
when cognitive form judgments (coherence and pre-
dictability) are differentiated from enjoyment ratings
(pleasantness, interest, and desire to hear again). In
a between-groups experiment, listeners (n= 125) were
randomly assigned to hear one of four versions of
Bach’s Prelude in C minor from Book I of The Well-
Tempered Clavier: 1) the original; 2) a mildly scrambled
one in which two larger sections were switched; 3)
a highly scrambled one; and 4) a randomized one. Sig-
nificant differences were observed between versions in
ratings of coherence and predictability, but not in rat-
ings of pleasantness, interest, or desire to hear again.
Individuals who had played the piece before could also
explicitly identify structural intervention. It was
assumed that relative incoherence would result in
higher complexity and, thus, be reflected in longer ret-
rospective duration estimates; however, estimates did
not differ between stimuli. These results suggest that
untrained listeners can evaluate global form, indepen-
dently of their level of familiarity with a musical piece,
while also suggesting that awareness of incoherence
does not always correspond with decreased enjoyment.
Received: December 17, 2021, accepted August 2, 2022.
Key words: form perception, structure, coherence,
music theory, baroque music
W
HAT IS THE IMPORTANCE OF LARGE
-
SCALE
structure to musical perception? The answer
depends on whom is asked. While music
theorists and composers have traditionally assumed the
salience of global musical structure as an article of faith,
empirical research has proffered scant evidence that
listeners recognize such structures (Karno & Konec
ˇni,
1992; Gotlieb & Konec
ˇni, 1985; Konec
ˇni, 1984; Till-
mann & Bigand, 2004; Eitan & Granot, 2008), resulting
in questions about the relevance of form to musical
appreciation. Given the inconclusive nature of null
effects, along with divergent findings when using differ-
ent repertoires (Lalitte & Bigand, 2006) and experimen-
tal paradigms (Granot & Jacoby, 2012), we decided to
revisit and refine questions surrounding the recognition
of form (explicit and implicit) by a variety of listeners
(trained and untrained, familiar and unfamiliar).
Formal analysis of Western tonal music typically
tracks ‘‘expressive/dramatic trajectories toward generi-
cally obligatory cadences’’ (Hepokoski & Darcy, 2006,
p. 13); and, despite a diversity of methodologies, there is
a shared conviction among music theorists that well-
composed musical form represents what Wallace Berry
called the composer’s ‘‘intellectual control over the
musical ideas which bring a composition into existence’’
(1986, p. xiii). Composers, too, seem to agree that the
assembly of musical components (‘‘building blocks,’ as
Mahler called them; see Bauer-Lechner, 1923, p. 120)
into a unified whole is both aesthetically important and
creatively demanding. Copland described composi-
tional ‘‘continuity and flow’ as the ‘‘be-all and end-all
of every composer’s existence’’ (1939/2002, p. 25). He
further held that form implicates not just the compo-
ser’s creative process but the listener’s cognitive one:
What to Listen for in Music is, after all, a didactic book
written for a lay audience. A similar view on the per-
ceptual relevance of large-scale structure was espoused
by Schoenberg: ‘‘The relaxation which a satisfied listener
experiences when he can follow an idea, its develop-
ment, and the reasons for such development is closely
related, psychologically, to a feeling of beauty’’ (1950/
1984, p. 215).
The psychological perspective on musical form
Schoenberg alluded to does not necessarily refer to
clearly defined forms like those found in music theory
texts, but rather deals with more general principles of
organization. It is this expanded conception of ‘‘form’
as overall musical Gestalt that we mostly concern our-
selves with below (see Ashley, 2017, for a review of
cognitive approaches to form). Here, a motivating
Music Perception,VOLUME 40, ISSUE 2, PP. 150–167, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312. ©2022 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA ALL
RIGHTS RESERVED.PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESSS
REPRINTS AND PERMISSIONS WEB PAGE,HTTPS://WWW.UCPRESS.EDU/JOURNALS/REPRINTS-PERMISSIONS. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2022.40.2.150
150 Geoffrey McDonald & Clemens Wo¨llner
... Nicholas Cook indica que "normalmente se espera que el análisis musical nos diga algo sobre cómo experimentamos la música" (1987, p. 219), y si bien se han dado notables avances en el estudio empírico de la percepción de la forma y estructura musical (Krumhansl, 1999;McDonald y Wöllner, 2022), el estudio de la partitura sigue siendo el recurso fundamental en el análisis musical. Dentro de esta aparente dicotomía entre análisis de la partitura y de la interpretación, la metodología tripartita de Nattiez (2021) ha buscado combinar el análisis de la partitura con el estudio de su percepción; según Nattiez, lo ideal es plantear experimentos empíricos que expliquen cómo percibe el oyente la música, pero en su defecto, el análisis de la interpretación grabada es un sustituto aceptable. ...
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACT: This paper presents a multidisciplinary exploration of Luis Humberto Salgado's Second piano sonata. Quantitative data were extracted through the computerised analysis of both the score and the recorded performance of this sonata; later, these data were analysed and visualised employing a number of statistical methods. Principal among the findings of this study is the correlation between tonal complexity and tempo in the first two movements, which suggest its use as an expressive device; while the third movement displays great tonal diversity, it is not associated with greater expressivity. The methods applied during this research may be generalised for the study of other pieces by Salgado and other composers.
... Musical-temporal structures exceeding the psychological present require long-term memory in order to be perceived as coherent and organized sounds. There is some argument as to whether larger musical structures are mainly of conceptual relevance for composers in the Western classical tradition, and hence whether violations of structural coherence can be detected by listeners at all (Karno & Konecni, 1992;McDonald & Wöllner, 2022). Composers may achieve a sense of coherence by relating different parts of a composition to each other, for instance, in terms of similarities between musical themes (Levinson, 1997). ...
Book
Full-text available
Music and dance can change our sense of time. Both rely on synchronizing our attention and actions with sounds and with other people, both involve memory and expectation, and both can give rise to experiences of flow and pleasure. Performing Time explores our experience of time in dance and music, from the perspectives of performers and audiences, and informed by the most recent research in dance science, musicology, neuroscience, and psychology. It includes discussions of tempo and pacing, coordination and synchrony, the performer's experience of time, audiences' temporal expectations, the effect of extreme slowness, and our individual versus collective senses of time. At its core, the book addresses how time and temporality in music and dance relate to current psychological and neuroscientific theories as well as to the aesthetic aims of composers, choreographers and performers. Bringing together new research on rhythm, time and temporality in both music and dance in one volume, the book contains overview chapters on the state of the art from leading researchers on topics ranging from the psychology, neuroscience, and philosophy of musical time to embodied timing in dance. In addition, numerous case studies regarding our temporal experience of music and dance are provided in shorter focus chapters, with their implications for further scientific study and artistic enquiry. Performing Time is an invaluable and comprehensive resource for students, researchers, educators, and artists alike, and for any reader interested in how the performing arts construct and play with time in our minds and bodies. Some chapters in this title are open access and available under the terms of a [CC BY-NC-ND 4.0 International] licence.
Article
Full-text available
The psychological relevance of large-scale musical structures has been a matter of debate in the music community. This issue was investigated with a method that allows assessing listeners' detection of musical incoherencies in normal and scrambled versions of popular and contemporary music pieces. Musical excerpts were segmented into 28 or 29 chunks. In the scrambled version, the temporal order of these chunks was altered with the constraint that the transitions between two chunks never created local acoustical and musical disruptions. Participants were required (1) to detect on-line incoherent linking of chunks, (2) to rate aesthetic quality of pieces, and (3) to evaluate their overall coherence. The findings indicate a moderate sensitivity to large-scale musical structures for popular and contemporary music in both musically trained and untrained listeners. These data are discussed in light of current models of music cognition.
Article
Full-text available
The presence of the phenomenological body is central to music in all of its varieties and contradictions. With the explosion of scholarly works on the body in virtually every field in the humanities, the social as well as the biomedical sciences, the question of how such a complex understanding of the body is related to music, with its own complexity, has been investigated within specific disciplinary perspectives. The Oxford Handbook of Music and the Body brings together these particular aspects of such relationships in a broad context and provides a platform for the discussion of the multidimensional interfaces of music and the body. It is organized into six sections, each discussing the topics that define the field: the moving and performing body; the musical brain and psyche; embodied mind, embodied rhythm; the disabled and sexual body; music as medicine; and the multimodal body. Connecting a wide array of diverse perspectives and presenting a survey of research and practice highlighting different facets, the Handbook provides an introduction into the rich world of music and the body.
Article
Full-text available
The coherent organisation of thematic material into large-scale structures within a composition is an important concept in both traditional and cognitive theories of music. However, empirical evidence supporting their perception is scarce. Providing a more nuanced approach, this paper introduces a computational model of hypothesised cognitive mechanisms underlying perception of large-scale thematic structure. Repetition detection based on statistical learning forms the model's foundation, hypothesising that predictability arising from repetition creates perceived thematic coherence. Measures are produced that characterise structural properties of a corpus of 623 monophonic compositions. Exploratory analysis reveals the extent to which these measures vary systematically and independently.
Article
Full-text available
The current review addresses two internal clock models that have dominated discussions in timing research for the last decades. More specifically, it discusses whether the central or the intrinsic clock model better describes the fluctuations in subjective time. Identifying the timing mechanism is critical to explain and predict timing behaviours in various audiovisual contexts. Music stands out for its prominence in real life scenarios along with its great potential to alter subjective time. An emphasis on how music as a complex dynamic auditory signal affects timing accuracy led us to examine the behavioural and neuropsychological evidence that supports either clock model. In addition to the timing mechanisms, an overview of internal and external variables, such as attention and emotions as well as the classic experimental paradigms is provided, in order to examine how the mechanisms function in response to changes occurring particularly during music experiences. Neither model can explain the effects of music on subjective timing entirely: The intrinsic model applies primarily to subsecond timing, whereas the central model applies to the suprasecond range. In order to explain time experiences in music, one has to consider the target intervals as well as the contextual factors mentioned above. Further research is needed to reconcile the gap between theories, and suggestions for future empirical studies are outlined.
Article
Full-text available
Background This study concerns the perception of musical segmentation during listening to live contemporary classical music. Little is known about how listeners form judgments of musical segments, particularly when typical section markers, such as cadences and fermatas, are absent [e.g., Sears et al. (2014)] or when the music is non-tonal (e.g., in much contemporary classical music). Aims The current study aimed to examine the listeners’ segmentation decisions in a piece of contemporary music, Ligeti’s “Fanfares”? Methods Data were gathered using a smartphone application [Practice & Research in Science & Music (PRiSM) Perception App] designed for this study by the Royal Northern College of Music (RNCM) Centre for PRiSM and the Oxford e-Research Centre. A total of 259 audience participants were asked to “tap” when they felt that a section had ended. Subjective responses were captured, as well as contextual data about the participants. Results The audience members demonstrated high levels of agreement regarding segmentation, mostly at places in the music involving breaks in the musical texture (one piano hand resting), changes in dynamic (volume), and changes in register/pitch. A sense of familiarity with contemporary repertoire did seem to influence the responses—the participants who self-reported being familiar with contemporary music used a wider range of cues to make their segmentation decisions. The self-report data analysis suggested that the listeners were not always aware of how they made decisions regarding segmentation. The factors which may influence their judgment of musical segmentation are, to some extent, similar to those identified by music analysis (Steinitz, 2011) but different in other ways. The effect of musical training was found to be quite small. Conclusion Whether musically trained and/or familiar with contemporary classical music or not, the listeners demonstrate commonalities in segmentation, which they are not always aware of. This study has implications for contemporary composers, performers, and audiences and how they engage with new music in particular.
Article
Full-text available
This work attempts to study the way higher music graduate students segment a contemporary music work, Itinerant, and to understand the influence of musical feature on segmentation. It attempts to test the theory stating that saliences contribute to organizing the music surface. The 42 students listened to the work several times and, in real time, they were requested to indicate the places on the score where they perceived structural boundaries. This work is characterised by its linearity, which could hinder identification of saliences and thereby, the establishment of structural boundaries. The participants show stability in the points of segmentation chosen. The results show significant coincidences among the participants in strategic places of the work, which leads us to conclude, in line with other researches, although in a work with different characteristics, that listeners can find a structural organisation in contemporary music that could allow them to understand it.
Book
Building on ideas first advanced by Arnold Schoenberg and later developed by Erwin Ratz, this book introduces a new theory of form for instrumental music in the classical style. The theory provides a broad set of principles and a comprehensive methodology for the analysis of classical form, from individual ideas, phrases, and themes to the large-scale organization of complete movements. It emphasizes the notion of formal function, that is, the specific role a given formal unit plays in the structural organization of a classical work.
Book
Edmund Gurney (1847–88) is today best known for his work on psychical research, but from a young age he harboured the ambition to be a composer and performer. Frustrated in this aim, he began writing on the philosophy and psychology of music. This work of 1880 was an attempt to apply a strictly scientific method of enquiry to music, and it is regarded as one of the most important and original treatises from the nineteenth century on musical aesthetics. Gurney discusses the sensations of pleasure and pain in relation to the senses, and goes on to examine how the listener differentiates between 'noises' and 'tones'. He explores whether there is an elemental difference between a 'good' and a 'bad' melody, the ultimate futility of the critic trying to describe music, and the 'moral' conclusion to be drawn from a preference for the music of Rossini over that of Beethoven.