Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Citation: Bosacki, S.; Tardif-Williams,
C.Y.; Roma, R.P.S. Children’s and
Adolescents’ Pet Attachment,
Empathy, and Compassionate
Responding to Self and Others.
Adolescents 2022,2, 493–507. https://
doi.org/10.3390/adolescents2040039
Academic Editor: Laura L. Hayman
Received: 25 October 2022
Accepted: 23 November 2022
Published: 29 November 2022
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2022 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
Article
Children’s and Adolescents’ Pet Attachment, Empathy, and
Compassionate Responding to Self and Others
Sandra Bosacki *, Christine Yvette Tardif-Williams and Renata P. S. Roma
Department of Educational Studies, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON L2S 3A1, Canada
*Correspondence: sbosacki@brocku.ca
Abstract:
The research shows that children’s and adolescents’ attachments with pets play an im-
portant role in their social and emotional lives. However, little research exists on how children and
adolescents show compassion toward themselves and others and how this caring ability relates to
their attachment with pets. This study explored the relations among children’s and adolescents’
compassion, empathy, and attachment to their pets. Sixty-one Canadian youths aged 7–15.9 years
(
M= 11.35
) participated in this study (n= 38 g, 23 b) and completed self-report questionnaires
including the Lexington Attachment to Pets scale, subscale of Dispositional Positive Emotions, Self-
compassion scale for children, and Bryant’s Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents (IECA).
The results showed older adolescents were more likely than younger children to score high on cogni-
tive and emotional dimensions of empathy and be less likely to feel alone and isolated. High levels
of emotional pet attachment related to high levels of the ability to feel sad when witnessing others
in distress and high levels of mindful, compassionate, and self-responding feelings of compassion
for others. The results also showed that those participants who experienced intensified emotional
attachment toward their pets (either current or in the past), were less likely to judge themselves in
negative ways. Overall, our findings suggest that children’s emotional attachments with pets relate to
their ability to respond to themselves and others in kind and compassionate ways and not necessarily
their ability to understand another person’s emotions.
Keywords: children; pets; attachment; empathy; compassion
1. Introduction
Children learn to care for themselves, other humans, and animals from an early age
and this ability develops as they enter adolescence. However, little research exists on how
children’s and adolescents’ abilities to understand the emotions of others and respond
with compassion toward others and themselves relate to their ability to form emotional
attachments with their pets. To address this gap in the literature, this study explored the
relations among children’s and adolescents’ ability to respond in compassionate ways
toward themselves and others, empathy, and attachment to their pets.
In late childhood and early adolescence, relationships with others become increasingly
important—particularly with peers, as well as animals and pets [
1
,
2
] In tandem with
the re-organization of social relationships, children’s and adolescents’ sense of self and
identity becomes heightened as they become more concerned about evaluations from
others [
3
]. Although studies show that intimate relationships with peers play critical roles
in children’s and adolescents’ social-emotional and moral development [4–6], few studies
explore how young people’s relationships and self-cognitions link to their relationships
with animals, particularly with their pets [
1
,
7
]. Studies also show how children learn
from their relationships with animals including cognitively (e.g., making fine-grained
distinctions between self and animal “other”), socially (e.g., in terms of interactions and
play), and morally (e.g., nurturing and responsibility; [
8
]. However, few studies examine the
social-cognitive dimensions of children’s relationships with pets and how such attachments
influence one’s feelings and thoughts toward oneself [1,9].
Adolescents 2022,2, 493–507. https://doi.org/10.3390/adolescents2040039 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/adolescents
Adolescents 2022,2494
Past studies show children and adolescents with higher mindreading skills such as
emotional recognition and perspective-taking also experience high levels of perceived
quality of, and satisfaction with, relationships (e.g., lack of conflict, emotional warmth,
and supportive relationships) with their siblings and friends [
9
–
12
]). However, to the best
of our knowledge, researchers have yet to explore the relations among compassionate
self-responding, the multi-dimensional aspects of empathy, and children’s and adolescents’
perceptions of their emotional attachments with their pets. Furthermore, with the exception
of one study [
13
] that showed young adolescents with strong emotional attachments to
pets showed a higher sense of self-esteem and autonomy, to date, there remains a lack of
studies that specifically investigate the links between young people’s pet attachments and
their tendency to respond to others and themselves in compassionate and caring ways.
1.1. Relational-Developmental Approach to Self and Attachment Relations—Role of Pets
According to relational developmental approaches to attachment relations with others
and emotional and social development [
6
], pets (e.g., dogs, cats, fish) can be a source of
psychological support in a manner similar to close human others and attachments with
primary caregivers [
1
]. Studies show that most pet owners often anthropomorphize their
pets [
14
], and the anthropomorphization of pets has been found to be related to the percep-
tions of pets as a family member or friend [
15
]. In other words, children and adults often
ascribe human characteristics, including emotions (e.g., my dog loves me) and cognitions
(e.g., my dog understands me) to their pets. Viewing pets as human-like with distinct
personalities may foster the perception of pets as close others and thus facilitate pet-human
interactions that mirror human–human relations [
1
,
9
]. Thus, if children conceptualize their
pets as a close friend, pets may satisfy some of their owners’ psychological needs and
support well-being, just as another person would [15].
Similar to relational attachments with significant others ([
16
] as a dynamic emotional
process between animal and child, studies show that children’s and adolescents’ pet attach-
ments are also related to increased levels of socio-emotional abilities and well-being [
1
,
9
]
However, little is known about connections among children’s and adolescents’ compas-
sionate responding to self and others, their empathy skills such as understanding others’
emotions and their relationships with animals. Throughout later childhood and adoles-
cence, youths become increasingly aware of cultural conceptions and judgements about
the role of nature and animals in people’s lives [
17
]. Given that young adolescents become
increasingly sensitive to the judgements of others and grow in self-consciousness and
self-doubt, such thinking may influence how they feel about themselves and others, as well
as respond toward animals including their pets [18,19].
Researchers suggest that pets serve as a trusted source of emotional comfort and
support within the family circle [
1
,
20
], and may feature strongly in their emotional lives.
However, it remains unknown as to whether the emotional strength of this pet relationship
supports (or hinders) them in their ability to respond to themselves and others in kind and
compassionate ways. Although there is growing evidence of the potential psychosocial
importance of adolescents’ relationships with animals especially during older childhood
and adolescence [
7
,
9
], the nature of emotional attachments to pets and how these might
vary among different age groups during later childhood and adolescence, and between
girls and boys, has received relatively little attention.
1.2. Empathy and Compassion in Children
Compassion and empathy are multi-dimensional, complex social-cognitive abilities
that develop from relationships with others [
21
,
22
]. Developmental biosocial-cognitive
theory highlights the importance of empathy and compassion to young people’s socio-
emotional adjustment [
4
,
23
–
26
]. For example, studies show that understanding other’s
thoughts and feelings (cognitive empathy) and feeling for another person in distress
(affective empathy), relates to positive social interactions and effective communication with
others [
27
]. Regarding compassion for self and others, studies show how the ability to
Adolescents 2022,2495
be mindful and kind to others and oneself relates to more positive social and emotional
adjustment in older children and adolescents [
22
,
28
]. Across time, research shows that
being more empathic in adolescence predicts higher empathy at the age of 35 years, as
well as better self-reported communication skills and social competence. Such studies
indicate that the identification of individual differences in empathy and compassion may
help expose the mechanisms that underlie differences in socio-emotional functioning and
abilities to form attachments with other living beings including animals.
1.3. Empathy and Compassion in Children—Links to Relationships with Pets
In particular, one study [
29
] shows that adults’ secure attachment to their pets relates
to their ability to have secure human relationships and to be able to show affective empathy
and understand the emotions of others. Building on this study, our research explores if
children’s and adolescent’s empathy and compassionate responding to self and others
is associated with pet attachment. Given middle childhood and early adolescence is an
important time for social-emotional development, identity, and relationship formation [
1
],
this phase in development provides an opportunity to study the role of pet attachment
in young people’s lives. In addition, due to increased gender-role stereotypes in older
childhood and early adolescence, studies show mixed findings about the role of gender
in empathy, compassion and pet attachment. For example, some studies show girls score
higher than boys on empathy and compassion for others [
9
], however it remains unclear if
gender differences occur in the relations between children’s attachment to pets and their
ability to show compassionate responding to self and other.
1.4. Compassion, Empathy, and Social Functioning
Higher levels of compassionate self-responding are shown to relate to high levels
of psychological functioning such as emotional well-being and personal happiness in
adolescents and adults [
22
], but little is known about the role compassionate self-responding
plays in children’s and adolescents’ lives and how they relate to other people and their
pets. Given that the most children and adults perceive pets as a part of their family and
attribute human characteristics to pets, attachments with pets should act in similar ways
to human attachment in terms of empathy and compassion [
9
,
30
]. Although past studies
show associations between compassion and concern for others and compassionate self-
responding in adults [
22
,
31
], studies with youth are limited. For example, some studies
show no relations between cognitive empathy or the ability to understand the emotions
of others and self-compassion in adolescents and suggest more research is needed on this
topic [32].
1.5. Unique Dimensions of Self-Compassion
Recent studies highlight the importance of the categorization of the items of self-
compassion as a bipolar continuum with compassionate self-responding on one end (i.e.,
self-kindness, common humanity, and mindfulness) and the opposite side of the con-
tinuum includes uncompassionate responding (i.e., self-judgment, isolation, and over-
identification) [
22
,
33
–
35
]. This bi-polar continuum represents compassionate and uncom-
passionate dimensions of responding to oneself and can be explained by the theory of social
mentalities [
1
]. Other researchers also promote that compassionate (e.g., self-kindness) and
uncompassionate self-responding (e.g., self-criticism) reflect different processes [
1
,
22
,
36
],
though evidence exists showing that compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding
may share the same neural circuit.
Studies show that higher levels of compassionate and mindful self-responding lead
to an increase in adaptive psychological functions (such as self-care behavior; [
37
], and
a reduction in maladaptive coping strategies, which can exert benefits for psychological
functioning [
22
,
31
,
38
,
39
]. Such studies suggest that compassionate self-responding may
play a protective role in helping youth to care for themselves in times of distress and may
help with psychological functioning [22].
Adolescents 2022,2496
1.6. The Present Study
This cross-sectional study explored levels of self- and other-compassion, empathy,
and pet attachment in typically developing children and adolescents. Following previous
findings [
9
], the first aim was to investigate developmental and individual differences
(e.g., as a function of age and gender) in the levels of empathy, compassion, and pet
attachment. The second aim was to examine the links among the three variables (hereby
distinguishing total self-compassion score and the different dimensions of self-compassion
in terms of uncompassionate and compassionate responses to the self, as well as the three
factors of empathy). Based on past studies that suggest positive relations among empathy,
compassion, and attachment relations with primary caregivers [
16
,
21
], peers [
40
], and
pets [
9
,
41
], we predicted that high levels of compassion for self and others would relate
to high levels of empathic responding and understanding, and strong pet attachment.
Based on past studies, we predicted that adolescents would score higher on empathy than
younger children [
21
]. Finally, given the past mixed findings in terms of gender and age
differences in compassion (self-other) [
22
] and pet attachment [
42
] our examination of age
or gender differences in these variables remained exploratory.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
This analysis involved a sample of 61 Canadian children and adolescents (38 girls;
23 boys; Mage = 11.35, SD = 2.57, range = 7–15.9 years) from a middle to high socio-
economic status. All participants used English as their first language. As reported by the
participants, the majority of the participants (80%) lived with a dog or cat at home, while
56% also had other pets including fish, rabbits, and reptiles, whereas 16.3% of participants
reported that they currently did not live with any pets.
2.2. Procedures
As part of a larger study that involved self-report questionnaires and interviews on
young people’s moral reasoning and attachment to pets [
43
], the present paper focuses
on the children’s and adolescents’ self-report questionnaires only. Upon receiving ethical
clearance from the participating university ethics review board and informed written
consents of parents and verbal assent from the youth participants, a team of graduate
student researchers specifically trained to work with youth and their families on moral
reasoning and pet attachment administered the questionnaires during individual sessions
with the participants. Overall, the questionnaires required participants about 30 min to
be completed. No child dropped out of the study. The participants were free to withdraw
from the study at any time. Confidentiality was ensured to all the participants.
2.3. Measures
Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (23 questions). We used the Lexington Attachment
to Pets Scale (LAPS; ref. [
44
] to measure the affectionate attachment relations between the
children and their pet (if they currently have one or, if they did not have one currently, they
completed this questionnaire in response to how they treated their pets in the past). This
23-item scale includes items such as “Quite often I confide in my pet” and “I love my pet
because it never judges me”. Each question is scored on a four-point scale from 0 = strongly
disagree, to 3 = strongly agree, with two items being reverse scored. The total scores can
vary from 23 to 115. The questions comprise one scale with a high coefficient (
α
= 0.93)
and suggest that this is a reliable tool to measure aspects of children’s and adolescents’
attachment relations with companion animals.
Other Directed Compassion: Subset of the Dispositional Positive Emotion Scale (5 Ques-
tions.). This 5-point Likert-type 5-item scale (from 1 = strongly disagree to
5 =
strongly agree)
(e.g., item, “It is important to care for people who need help.”) is used to assess children’s
behaviors toward other humans in different contexts. The total scores can vary from 5 to
25, with an internal reliability coefficient of (
α
= 0.70). The version used in our study was
Adolescents 2022,2497
adapted to match the scale of [
45
] to ensure the items were child-friendly as the original
version contained a 7-point Likert scale. [
46
] also found links between high levels of com-
passion for others and self with psychological well-being. Such findings suggest that this
is a reliable tool to measure aspects related to behavior and attitudes of compassionate
responding toward human beings.
The Self-Compassion Scale modified for Children (Ages 8–12) [
45
]. This 5-point Likert-
type scale contained 12 items and asked children to rate how they feel toward themselves
(e.g., “I try to be kind toward those things about myself I do not like.”) using a five-point
scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always. The total scores can vary from 12 to 60 with an
internal reliability coefficient of (
α
= 0.78). This scale has been used in past studies with
children ([
22
], and has been referred to as a reliable and valid tool to measure compassionate
self-responding in children. Such findings suggest that this is a reliable tool to measure
aspects related to compassionate behavior and attitudes toward oneself in children.
Bryant’s Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents (IECA: 7). To reveal the
complexity underlying the ability to feel “with” another person or empathy, researchers
use three factors within the total empathy scale. For example, ref. [
47
] revealed a 19-item,
three-factor structure that includes a cognitive factor (understanding feelings), referring to
situations where the child understands others’ emotional states, and two affective factors.
The first affective factor refers to feelings of sadness which reflects empathic concerns
defined as a state in which the child feels “sorry for” the other as a result of perceiving
his/her distress. The second affective empathy factor is tearful reaction, which reflects
sympathy and emotional contagion or the emotional state as a direct result of perceiving
the other person’s emotion. The combined cognitive and affective factors of empathy of
IECA thus assesses the emotional responsiveness and cognitive insight.
Given that the three-factor structure appears to be appropriate for children and sup-
ports the validity of the cognitive and affective components of the scale and the construct
of empathy [
48
–
50
], we used the 22-item Bryant’s empathy index (English version). The
total scores can vary from 22 to 88. To achieve better differentiation, following [
50
]’s indica-
tions (i.e., to avoid yes/no designs as they reduce the variability in responses), the scoring
system was modified from the original scales’s yes/no responses to a 4-point Likert scale
(1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree). The scale had a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of
0.79, 95% Confidence Interval (CI) = 0.091–0.213, and a mean index of 65.14 ranging from 2
to item 19 to 3.79 to item 11.
Following past research [
51
], we used the 3 subscales to measure dimensions of
empathy. The first factor included 9 items referring to comprehension of other people’s
feelings (e.g., item 9: “Girls who cry because they are happy are silly”); the second factor
consisted of 6 items related to emotions of sadness (e.g., item 12: “It makes me sad to see a
boy who cannot find anyone to play with”); and finally, the third factor included 4 items
concerning sympathetic attitudes and the reaction to other people’s emotions (e.g., item 19:
“Seeing a girl who is crying makes me feel like crying”).
We followed the structure of the 3-factor model proposed by other researchers [
47
].
More specifically, we used understanding feelings (Factor 1, cognitive content in terms of
understanding other people’s emotions), feelings of sadness (Factor 2, affective content
in the form of feelings of sadness or concern for others), and tearful reaction (Factor 3,
sympathetic reactions). Additionally, as in [
47
], their results showed that the following
items did not load in any of the three identified factors: item 7 (“Even when I do not know
why someone is laughing, I laugh too”), item 15 (“Grown-ups sometimes cry even when
they have nothing to be sad about”), and item 22 (“I do not feel upset when I see a classmate
being punished by a teacher for not obeying school rules”); accordingly, these items were
not included from the analysis. To calculate the reliability of the subscales, we used data
from the total sample. The internal consistency for the factor understanding of feelings (9
items) was 0.76, for the factor feelings of sadness (6 items) it was 0.83, and for the factor
tearful reaction (4 items) it was 0.78. These values were higher than those obtained by past
researchers: 0.56, 0.72, and 0.71, respectively [47].
Adolescents 2022,2498
3. Results
3.1. Descriptives and Mean Differences
To explore our first research question, which was to investigate developmental and
individual differences among the main test variables, we first conducted preliminary
analysis of the score distribution in terms of means, standard deviations, and range of
variables. To explore for gender differences, we conducted t-tests among the main test
variables (see Table 1). No significant differences were found. That is, girls and boys were
more alike than different on measures of compassion for self and other, empathy, and
attachment to pets.
Table 1. Descriptives and gender differences for main test variables.
Gender N Mean SD t Sig
Self-Compassion Self-Kindness Subscale Male 20 3.35 0.81
−0.057 0.47
Female 26 3.37 0.96
Self-Compassion Self-Judgement Subscale Male 20 2.50 1.10
−1.34 0.07
Female 29 2.88 0.87
Total Positive Self-Compassion Score Subscale Male 19 21.47 4.33 0.35 0.50
Female 24 21.00 4.51
Total Negative Self-Compassion Score Subscale Male 19 19.00 4.61 0.48 0.14
Female 24 18.42 3.34
Self-Compassion Common Humanity Subscale Male 20 3.45 1.15 0.53 0.72
Female 28 3.27 1.18
Self-Compassion Isolation Subscale Male 20 2.80 0.89
−0.787 0.49
Female 28 3.00 0.85
Self-Compassion Mindfulness Subscale Male 19 3.74 0.89 0.56 0.84
Female 28 3.59 0.89
Self-Compassion Over-Identification Subscale Male 20 3.23 0.98 0.75 0.16
Female 28 3.04 0.77
Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents
Total Mean Score
Male 23 2.89 0.33
−0.97 0.29
Female 38 2.98 0.40
Understanding Feelings Male 18 28.44 4.11 0.51 0.84
Female 36 27.83 4.16
Feelings of Sadness Male 19 20.95 3.06 0.58 0.44
Female 37 20.41 3.62
Tearful Reactions Male 19 8.68 2.95
−0.52 0.80
Female 37 8.73 3.21
The Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale
Total Score
Male 23 74.00 5.93 1.24 0.52
Female 38 71.75 7.35
The Dispositional Positive Emotional Scale
Total Score
Male 23 22.09 1.55 1.03 0.15
Female 38 21.48 2.56
Years. Months (11.10) Male 23 1.57 0.51
−0.10 0.84
Female 38 1.58 0.50
Given that a minority of the sample (16.3%) reported that they currently did not live
with any pets, to compare groups of children with and without pets, t-tests were conducted
on all test variables. The results showed that groups with and without pets did not differ
on any of the test variables. Thus, we collapsed the two groups and included the total
sample in our analyses.
3.2. Correlational Analysis
3.2.1. Age and Compassion (Self- and Other), Empathy, and Pet Attachment
For our second research question, we conducted Pearson correlations to explore if
there were any associations among the test variables (see Table 2). The findings showed
that with age, cognitive, and affective empathy increased such as understanding another
person’s feelings (cognitive empathy) (r(59) = 0.31, p= 0.02), sympathetic empathy or the
ability to have tearful reactions (r(59) = 0.35, p= 0.02) to another person’s distress, and
Adolescents 2022,2499
the ability to show feelings of sadness (r(59) = 0.24, p= 0.07)—although this finding was
marginally significant and suggests a trend. For dimensions of self-compassion, age related
marginally to less feelings of isolation (r(59) =
−
0.26, p= 0.07). In contrast, no relations were
found between age and positive dimensions of self-compassion (r(59) =
−
0.13,
p= 0.41
) or
compassion for others. That is, the older children become and as they enter into adolescence,
they were more likely to feel empathy and understand others’ feelings, as well as be less
likely to feel alone and isolated.
3.2.2. Compassion for Self and Other, Empathy, and Pet Attachment
To continue to explore our second research question, which was to investigate the
associations among the main test variables, correlational analysis also showed that high
levels of compassionate self-responding (self-kindness) was marginally linked to high
levels of emotional empathy (feelings of sadness) (r(59) = 0.29, p= 0.06). High levels of
compassion and concern for others related to marginally heightened feelings of sadness
(r(59) = 0.22, p= 0.05), self-responding, or acting in kind and mindful (r(59) = 0.40, p= 0.01)
ways toward oneself, as well as to high levels of pet attachment (r(59) = 0.39, p= 0.00). In
contrast, high levels of attachment to animals related to low levels of uncompassionate
self-responding such as self-judgment (r(59) =
−
0.41, p= 0.01). That is, the more attached a
person was to an animal, the less likely they were to judge themselves in negative ways.
Regarding associations between compassion and the dimensions of empathy, the
correlations in Table 2show high levels of affective empathy (feelings of sadness) marginally
related to high levels of self-kindness (r(59) = 0.29, p= 0.06), common humanity (r(59) =
0.44, p= 0.00), and mindfulness, (r(59) = 0.47, p= 0.00). For attachment to pets, high levels
of attachment to pets related to high levels of compassionate self-responding including the
dimension of mindfulness (r(59) = 0.34, p= 0.02), as well as the ability to feel compassion
for others (r(59), = 0.39, p= 0.00). In terms of empathy, heightened levels of attachment
with pets related to high levels of dimensions of affective empathy such as feelings of
sadness (r(59) = 0.44, p= 0.00) and tearful reactions (r(59) = 0.39, p= 0.00), but not cognitive
empathy or the ability to understand feelings (r(59) =
−
0.03, p= 0.83). That is, the more
emotionally attached a child was to their pet, the more likely they were to be mindful and
compassionate in responding to themselves, feel more compassion for others, and feel sad
or tearful when they witnessed distress or sadness in others. No significant associations
were found between levels of pet attachment and cognitive empathy, or the ability to
understand and reason about another person’s feelings.
Adolescents 2022,2500
Table 2. Correlations among main test variables.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
1. Self-Kindness -
2. Self-Judgement −0.51 *** -
3. Self-Compassion Scale 0.60 *** −0.39 *** -
4. Positive Self Compassion 0.73 *** −0.13 0.87 *** -
5. Negative Self Compassion 0.42 *** −0.65 *** 0.80 *** 0.38 ** -
6. Common Humanity 0.47 *** −0.11 0.47 ** 0.78 *** 0.14 -
7. Isolation −0.28 * 0.04 −0.61 *** −0.42 *** −0.65 *** −0.23 -
8. Mindfulness 0.48 *** −0.21 0.71 *** 0.82 *** 0.38 ** 0.46 *** −0.33 ** -
9. Over-Identification −0.14 0.25 * −0.61 *** −0.32 ** −0.77 *** −0.24 0.32 ** −0.36 ** -
10. Index of Empathy 0.09 0.05 0.16 0.16 0.16 0.10 −0.37 ** 0.29 * −0.08 -
11. Understanding Feelings 0.14 −0.20 0.25 * 0.16 0.34 ** 0.14 −0.41 *** 0.25 −0.19 0.31 ** -
12. Feelings of Sadness 0.29 * −0.10 0.13 0.35 ** −0.11 0.44 *** −0.11 0.47 *** 0.11 0.37 ** 0.36 ** -
13. Tearful Reactions 0.01 −0.17 −0.06 −0.07 −0.03 0.25 0.00 0.17 0.01 0.40 *** 0.30 ** 0.47 *** -
14. Attachment to Pets 0.25 −0.41 *** 0.21 0.26 0.10 0.22 0.12 0.343 ** 0.02 0.15 −0.03 0.44 *** 0.39 *** -
15. Disp. Positive Emotional 0.20 −0.02 0.23 * 0.45 *** −0.03 0.31 ** −0.02 0.40 *** 0.01 0.11 0.22 * 0.56 *** 0.18 0.39 *** -
16. Age 0.18 −0.06 −0.09 * −0.13 −0.03 0.00 −0.26 * −0.11 0.24 0.41 *** 0.31 ** 0.24 * 0.35 ** 0.12 0.11 -
*** p< 0.01; ** p< 0.05; * p< 0.10; 1. Self-Compassion Self Kindness Subscale, 2. Self-Compassion Self Judgement Subscale, 3. Self-Compassion Scale, 4. Total Positive Self Compassion
Score Subscale, 5. Total Negative Self Compassion Score Subscale, 6. Self-Compassion Common Humanity Subscale, 7. Self-Compassion Isolation Subscale, 8. Self-Compassion
Mindfulness Subscale, 9. Self-Compassion Over-Identification Subscale, 10. Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents Total Mean Score, 11. F1- Understanding Feelings (Index of
Empathy for Children and Adolescents), 12. F2- Feelings of Sadness (Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents), 13. F3- Tearful Reactions (Index of Empathy for Children and
Adolescents), 14. The Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale Total Score, 15. The Dispositional Positive Emotional Scale Total Score, 16. Age in years.months.
Adolescents 2022,2501
4. Discussion
This study explored the associations between children’s and adolescents’ ability to
show compassion toward themselves and others and their emotional attachments with
their pets. Overall, the present findings partially supported our predictions in that they
showed that young people’s attachments with their pets related to kind, mindful, and com-
passionate ways of responding to oneself and others, as well as being less self-judgmental.
Strong pet attachments also related to one’s ability to feel sadness for others in distress,
or to show affective empathy. In contrast to our predictions, no relations were found
between pet-attachment and the ability to understand another person’s feelings or show
cognitive empathy. Regarding age differences, findings supported our predictions that
adolescents were more likely than younger children to feel empathy in terms of feeling for
another person in distress and understanding others’ emotions. In addition, although we
did not make specific predictions in terms of age or gender differences in compassion, or
pet attachment, we found that compared to younger children, adolescents were also less
likely to feel alone and isolated [
5
]. Regarding gender differences, the findings showed
that girls and boys reported to be more alike than different in terms of their compassion
and empathy skills and attachments with their pets. Outlined below, we discuss our main
findings and contributions of our paper within the context of previous research findings.
We end with a final note on limitations and implications for future research and education.
4.1. Compassionate Responding to Self and Others, Empathy, and Pet Attachment
The main findings of our study suggest that compassionate responding to self and
others and feeling the emotions of others relates to higher levels of pet attachment. Further-
more, compassionate responding to self and others (feeling “with” another person, but also
wanting to help that person feel better—show care and comfort to self/other) showed no
relations to the cognitive dimensions of empathy or understanding the feelings of others.
The findings from this study showed that children’s and adolescent’s pet attachment was
related to compassionate self-responding such as being mindful and caring toward oneself.
Such self-caring skills and social awareness may help young people to connect themselves
emotionally with others and their pets that might, in turn, help to promote their well-being.
Alternatively, young people’s attachment with their pets may influence their ability to
respond to themselves and others in kind and compassionate ways.
Such findings support past research that suggests compassion is a moral emotion that
also incorporates intention to act in that the individual feels with and for another person
and also tries to help the other person feel better if experiencing distress. Past studies on
altruism or helping others to the detriment of your own needs is related to one’s ability to
feel compassion for another person [
52
] That is, the ability to show compassion for others
includes kindness and being mindful of another person [
53
]. Compassion for others also
includes feelings of altruism or the need to help another over yourself. In terms of decision
making, compassion for others can be determined by a cost–benefit analysis [
52
], in that
the person decides based on moral reasoning skills if there is more personal cost or benefit
to helping and being kind to others in distress.
Thus, our study found relations between high levels of compassionate self-responding
and high levels of other-responding and supports studies that show links between compas-
sionate self-responding and feeling concern and compassion for others [
3
,
53
]. Regarding
the links between dimensions of empathy and compassion for self and others, our study
showed that cognitive empathy was not related to dimensions of self-compassion, compas-
sion for others, nor to pet attachment. Such findings support past research [
32
] that shows
cognitive empathy and dimensions of self-compassion were unrelated. Our findings also
support past studies that show compassion and empathy are interrelated concepts that may
be influenced by other factors such as attachment levels with primary caregivers, friends as
well as pets [
9
]. Moving beyond past studies on pet attachment and social-emotional skills
in youth [
9
,
23
,
54
], our study shows novel findings in that it suggests that irrespective of
Adolescents 2022,2502
one’s cognitive empathizing skills, links between self and other compassion may extend to
one’s emotional attachments with their pets.
In addition, we examined both the groups of children: those who currently lived with
a pet and those that had lived with pets in the past, but at the current time of completing
the questionnaire did not have a pet. Our comparative analyses found that the two groups
did not differ in terms of compassion, empathy, or attachment to their pet, which suggests
that having a pet may influence one’s ability to have compassion for themselves even
when they do not currently live with a pet. As many children experience the loss of many
pets throughout their childhood and adolescence, this finding is important as it suggests
perhaps the experience of living with a pet may continue to serve as an emotional benefit
for the child, even when they do not live with a pet currently, as they may be in transition
between pets. Furthermore, how children and adolescents think and feel about pet loss,
their decision to adopt a new pet (or not), and their re-attachment with a new pet remain
unstudied. Such studies may be useful as many individuals live with multiple pets over
their life [55,56], and experience the life and death of at least one pet.
Our findings are correlational and, although they do not predict directionality, they
highlight the links among compassion, affective empathy, and pet attachment. Our find-
ings also support studies that suggest that compassion and empathy may influence pet
attachments [
16
] or, vice versa, that pet attachments may influence empathic skills and the
ability to show compassion to self and others. Thus, future longitudinal research needs to
continue to explore the developmental pathways of complex interrelations among empa-
thy, compassionate responding to self and others, and attachments with pets, particularly
during the transition between children and adolescence.
Regarding developmental differences, in support of past findings that show cognitive
and moral skills increase over time [
40
]. the present findings showed that empathic re-
sponding to others increased, especially understanding and responding to another person’s
feelings (cognitive and emotional empathy). The only age difference in compassion was
that of self-compassion in terms of feeling less isolated and alone as one gets older. Such
findings suggest that as children transition to adolescence, peer relationships may increase
in frequency and quality and thus may lead to less feelings of isolation and loneliness [
5
,
9
].
In this study, we did not find relations between age and young people’s pet attachment
and this result suggests that developmental changes in children’s pet attachment remains
to be explored. For example, studies that show as children become older they may be less
likely to care for or interact with animals [2,57].
In terms of gender differences, in this study, the girls and boys were more alike than
different on compassion for self and others, empathy, and attachment to pets. Such findings
support the gender X generation theory that suggests that, as children age, roles within
the family such as caring for pets are influenced by gender-role stereotypes [
49
,
54
]. Thus,
perhaps in our study, the older girls were more likely than older boys to care for pets or
vice versa, but given the relatively small sample, we were unable to explore this possibility.
Our findings demonstrate support for the notion that the act of caring for pets should
be viewed as “gender-neutral” [
20
]. However, findings from the present study contradict
past studies that show girls are more likely to show strong attachments to pets, as well
as care more for pets than boys [
7
,
9
,
58
]. Future research should continue to explore
the interactions among age and gender and how they influence the relations between
compassion and animal attachment, as gender-sensitive and developmentally appropriate
educational programs are necessary to help promote compassion and pet attachment across
all gender orientations [59].
4.2. Compassion, Empathy, and Pet-Attachment
Our findings showed that high levels of compassionate responding to oneself related
to higher levels of attachment with pets. Although no significant associations were found
between the total empathy score and attachment with pets, when multiple dimensions
of empathy were explored, high levels of attachment with pets related to high levels of
Adolescents 2022,2503
emotional empathy (feeling sad) and tearful reactions (more attached to pet, more likely to
understand emotions and be tearful when see others feeling sad). In contrast, there were no
significant associations found with cognitive empathy or the ability to understand another
person’s feelings. The findings showed that high levels of compassionate self-responding
linked to high levels of emotional empathy (feelings of sadness). In contrast, high levels of
uncompassionate self-responding linked to high levels of cognitive empathy. High levels
of feeling compassion and concern for others related to high levels of feelings of sadness
and tearful reaction.
High levels of affective empathy (feelings of sadness) related to high levels of self-
kindness and common humanity, mindfulness, and attachment with animals. High levels of
attachment to pets related to high levels of compassion for self and others, but no relations
with empathy. Regarding dimensions of uncompassionate self-responding, such as feelings
of isolation, over-identification, and self-judgement, it was negatively related to high levels
of pet attachment and high levels of compassionate self-compassion (particularly the ability
to be mindful when responding to yourself) related to high levels of feeling compassion for
others and pet-attachment. Pet attachment and compassion for self and others related to
only the affective dimensions of empathy, not the cognitive dimension of empathy, or the
ability to understand emotions in others.
The current results also show that compassion for self and others (feeling “with”
another person, but also wanting to help that person feel better—show care and comfort
to self/other) is not the same as cognitive empathy or understanding another person’s
emotions. Such findings support research that suggests self-compassion and empathy are
complex multidimensional constructs that need to be more deeply explored in terms of
social context and social partners (including pets) [
52
]. The present study’s findings are
unique and contribute to the literature on moral emotions and cognition such as compassion
and attachment [
21
]. That is, the present study shows how motivations to care for self and
others are independent skills that relate differently to children’s attachments to pets. Our
findings also support research on the complexity of morality in children and adolescents
that shows that one’s moral thoughts and feelings are not necessarily connected to one’s
moral behaviors [
40
]. That is, the ability to understand another person’s emotions may not
necessarily indicate how a person will treat her/himself or another person or animal with
kindness and compassion.
4.3. Implications
This study reinforces the importance of exploring children’s perceptions of their
relationships with their pets and how they feel and care about themselves and others. In
addition, the present findings suggest that children and adolescents may benefit from
humane education programs that target particular dimensions of compassionate self-
responding and empathy rather than emotion recognition and other broad social skills. For
example, the present findings suggest the need for educators to combine Social-Emotional
Learning (SEL) [
60
,
61
], with mindful compassionate programs to further extend curricula
beyond basic social skills and emphasize how caring for animals helps children to learn
how to be kind and care for themselves [
37
,
62
,
63
]. As such, a balanced approach is needed—
one that involves developmentally appropriate educational programs that focus on the
promotion of mindfulness and compassion toward the self and others (humans, animals,
and nature). Future researchers and educators need to collaborate to co-create this balanced
approach as the overall goal of any educational or intervention program is to promote a
child’s sense of emotional well-being and social awareness [63,64].
Our findings highlight the need for SEL programs to further add to the importance of
social (interpersonal) and self-skills (intrapersonal) and emphasize that caring for animals
as emotional beings helps children to learn how to be kind and care for themselves, which
in turn helps to promote overall emotional well-being. More focus is needed on the
importance of self-processes, especially in terms of compassionate responding to oneself
Adolescents 2022,2504
and self-care. Such skills may serve as coping strategies to help youth deal with stressful
situations within learning contexts with one’s peer relations [11,65,66].
4.4. Limitations
In sum, our study suggests that the associations among children’s empathic skills,
feelings of compassion for self and others, and their pet attachments. However, despite
the novel contributions of our study, the findings need to be interpreted with caution
due to the following limitations. First, the majority of our measures were self-reported,
which may have increased the likelihood of participant bias due to social desirability.
Second, within the construct of psychosocial adjustment, other factors besides compassion
and empathy may have influenced children’s attachment to pets such as their ability to
regulate their emotions. Third, the majority of the sample was derived from a mainly
Euro-Canadian, English-speaking population within a middle-class SES. Fourth, this study
was a cross-sectional, correlational study and thus we could not investigate causality or the
directionality of relations among the key variables. Future research should include more
longitudinal studies that explore children within a more diverse sample. Finally, the sample
size precluded finer-grained analyses such as the exploration of two-way interactions
between child gender and pet types.
Overall, future studies need to unpack the tangled and knotted connections among the
dimensions of compassion, empathy, and attachment to pets in children. Moreover, more
research is needed to further explore these complex connection changes during adolescence
and emerging adulthood. In sum, our findings show that a child’s relationships with
animal-companions may be more tightly connected to their feelings of kindness and caring
toward oneself, which is crucial for well-being and overall psychosocial adjustment. Such
connections may help children connect with themselves emotionally rather than focus on
social relationships and cater to the needs of others to the detriment of oneself.
5. Conclusions
Despite these limitations, this study highlights that, irrespective of gender, children’s
and adolescents’ perceptions of their emotional relationships with their pets relates to how
they feel and care about themselves and others. Overall, the key finding of the present study
is that young people’s attachment with pets is associated with compassionate self- and other-
responding, but not necessarily with understanding the emotions of others. Theoretically,
our findings emphasize the utility of applying a relational developmental perspective
to research on well-being and relationship quality in studies of human-pet relations. In
practical terms, our findings hint at the importance of young people’s pet attachments
during the planning and implementation of humane education that promotes mindfulness
and compassion directed to self and other. Such findings may provide inspiration to
educators who wish to develop curriculum programs that aim to help children to develop
kind and compassionate relationships with themselves, as well as with significant humans
and animals in their lives.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, S.B., C.Y.T.-W., R.P.S.R.; methodology, S.B., C.Y.T.-W.,
R.P.S.R.; formal analysis, S.B., R.P.S.R.; resources, S.B., C.Y.T.-W., R.P.S.R.; writing—original draft
preparation, S.B.; writing—review and editing, C.Y.T.-W., R.P.S.R.; supervision, C.Y.T.-W., S.B.; project
administration, C.Y.T.-W., S.B. All the authors have read and agreed to the published version of the
manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement: Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement: Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement:
The data described in this manuscript are available upon request of
the authors.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors of this manuscript declare that there are no conflict of interest.
Adolescents 2022,2505
References
1.
Kerns, K.A.; Dulmen, M.H.M.; Kochendorfer, L.B.; Obeldobel, C.A.; Gastelle, M.; Horowitz, A. Assessing children’s relationships
with pet dogs: A multi-method approach. Soc. Dev. 2022,5, 1–19. [CrossRef]
2.
Pike, A.; Coldwell, J.; Dunn, J. Sibling relationships in early/middle childhood: Links with individual adjustment. J. Fam. Psychol.
2005,19, 523–532. [CrossRef]
3.
Denham, S. Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Dev.
1986
,57, 194.
[CrossRef]
4.
Sutton, E.; Schonert-Reichl, K.A.; Wu, A.D.; Lawlor, M.S. Evaluating the reliability and validity of the self-compassion scale short
form adapted for children ages 8–12. Child Indic. Res. 2017,11, 1217–1236. [CrossRef]
5.
Tipper, B. ‘A dog who I know quite well’: Everyday relationships between children and animals. Child. Geogr.
2011
,9, 145–165.
[CrossRef]
6.
Muldoon, J.C.; Williams, J.M.; Currie, C. Differences in boys’ and girls’ attachment to pets in early-mid adolescence. J. Appl. Dev.
Psychol. 2019,62, 50–58. [CrossRef]
7. Kahn, P.H., Jr. Mind and morality. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2004,2004, 73–83. [CrossRef]
8.
Cassels, M.T.; White, N.; Gee, N.; Hughes, C. One of the Family? Measuring Young adolescents’ relationships with pets and
siblings. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2017,49, 12–20. [CrossRef]
9.
Bascoe, S.M.; Davies, P.T.; Cummings, E.M. Beyond warmth and conflict: The developmental utility of a boundary conceptualiza-
tion of sibling relationship processes. Child Dev. 2012,83, 2121–2138. [CrossRef]
10.
Howe, N.; Recchia, H. Sibling relationships as a context for learning and development. Early Educ. Dev.
2014
,25, 155–159.
[CrossRef]
11.
Jones, E.A.; Fiani, T.; Stewart, J.L.; Sheikh, R.; Neil, N.; Fienup, D.M. When one sibling has autism: Adjustment and sibling
relationship. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019,28, 1272–1282. [CrossRef]
12. Wellman, H. The Child’s Theory of Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990.
13. Amiot, C.E.; Bastian, B. Toward a psychology of human-animal relations. Psychol. Bull. 2015,141, 6–47. [CrossRef]
14.
McConnell, A.R.; Brown, C.M.; Shoda, T.M.; Stayton, L.E.; Martin, C.E. Friends with benefits: On the positive consequences of pet
ownership. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011,101, 1239–1252. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
15.
Kobak, R.; Bosmans, G. Attachment and psychopathology: A dynamic model of the insecure cycle. Curr. Opin. Psychol.
2019
,25,
76–80. [CrossRef]
16. Bonnett, M. Environmental concern, moral education and our place in nature. J. Moral Educ. 2012,41, 285–300. [CrossRef]
17.
Levy, S.; Ramírez, L.; Rosenthal, L.; Karafantis, D. The study of lay theories: A piece of the puzzle for understanding prejudice.
In Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us; Banaji, M.R., Gelman, S.A., Eds.; Oxford
University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; pp. 318–322. [CrossRef]
18. Marx, R. Kindness and compassion practice. Mindfulness 2022. [CrossRef]
19. Melson, G. Why the Wild Things Are. Animals in the Lives of Children; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001.
20.
Malti, T.; Eisenberg, N.; Kim, H.; Buchmann, M. Developmental trajectories of sympathy, moral emotion attributions, and moral
reasoning: The role of parental support. Soc. Dev. 2013,22, 773–793. [CrossRef]
21.
Neff, K. Commentary: The differential effects fallacy in the study of self-compassion: Misunderstanding the nature of bipolar
continuums. Mindfulness 2022,13, 572–576. [CrossRef]
22.
Daly, B.; Morton, L.L. Children with pets do not show higher empathy: A challenge to current views. Anthrozoös
2003
,16, 298–314.
[CrossRef]
23.
Domes, G.; Heinrichs, M.; Michel, A.; Berger, C.; Herpertz, S.C. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol. Psychiatry
2007,61, 731–733. [CrossRef]
24.
Gilbert, P. Social mentalities: A biopsychosocial and evolutionary approach to social relationships. In Interpersonal Cognition;
Baldwin, M.W., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 299–333.
25.
Westenberg, P.M.; Drewes, M.J.; Goedhart, A.W.; Siebelink, B.M.; Treffers, P.D. A developmental analysis of self-reported fears in
late childhood through mid-adolescence: Social-evaluative fears on the rise? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip.
2004
,45,
481–495. [CrossRef]
26.
Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Morris, A.S. Prosocial development. In The Oxford Handbook of Ddevelopmental Psychology Self and
Other; Zelazo, P.D., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; Volume 2, pp. 300–325.
27.
Klimecki, O.M.; Singer, T. The compassionate brain. In Oxford Handbook on Compassion Science; Doty, J.R., Worline, M., Simon-
Thomas, E., Cameron, D., Brown, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017.
28.
Shivers, C.M.; Jackson, J.B.; McGregor, C.M. Functioning among typically developing siblings of individuals with autism spectrum
disorder: A meta-analysis. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2019,22, 172–196. [CrossRef]
29.
Lyons-Ruth, K.; Jacobvitz, D. Attachment disorganization: Genetic factors, parenting contexts, and developmental transformation
from infancy to adulthood. In Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd ed.; Cassidy, J., Shaver, P.R.,
Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 666–697.
30.
Neff, K.D.; Hsieh, Y.-P.; Dejitterat, K. Self-compassion, achievement goals, and coping with academic failure. Self Identity
2005
,4,
263–287. [CrossRef]
Adolescents 2022,2506
31.
Marshall, S.L.; Ciarrochi, J.; Parker, P.D.; Sahdra, B.K. Is Self-compassion selfish? The development of self-compassion, empathy,
and prosocial behavior in adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 2019,30, 472–484. [CrossRef]
32.
Lathren, C.; Rao, S.; Park, J.; Bluth, K. Self-compassion and current close interpersonal relationships: A scoping literature review.
Mindfulness 2021,12, 1078–1093. [CrossRef]
33.
Muris, P.; Otgaar, H. The process of science: A critical evaluation of more than 15 years of research on self-compassion with the
Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness 2020,11, 1469–1482. [CrossRef]
34.
Muris, P.; Petrocchi, N. Protection or vulnerability? A meta-analysis of the relations between the positive and negative components
of self-compassion and psychopathology. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2017,24, 373–383. [CrossRef]
35. Khoury, B. Compassion: Embodied and Embedded. Mindfulness 2019,10, 2363–2374. [CrossRef]
36.
Carsley, D.; Khoury, B.; Heath, N.L. Effectiveness of mindfulness interventions for mental health in schools: A comprehensive
meta-analysis. Mindfulness 2018,9, 693–707. [CrossRef]
37.
Ewert, C.; Vater, A.; Schröder-Abé, M. Self-compassion and coping: A meta-analysis. Mindfulness
2021
,12, 1063–1077. [CrossRef]
38.
MacBeth, A.; Gumley, A. Exploring compassion: A meta-analysis of the association between self-compassion and psychopathology.
Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012,32, 545–552. [CrossRef]
39.
Bajovic, M.; Rizzo, K. Meta-moral cognition: Bridging the gap among adolescents’ moral thinking, moral emotions and moral
actions. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 2021,2, 1–11. [CrossRef]
40.
Melson, G.F. Studying children’s attachment to their pets: A conceptual and methodological review. Anthrozoös
1990
,4, 91–99.
[CrossRef]
41.
Hirschenhauser, K.; Meichel, Y.; Schmalzer, S.; Beetz, A.M. Children love their pets: Do relationships between children and pets
co-vary with taxonomic order, gender, and age? Anthrozoös 2017,30, 1–456. [CrossRef]
42.
Rusu, A.S.; Costea-Barlutiu, C.; Turner, D.C. Interpersonal and pet attachment, empathy toward animals, and anthropomorphism:
An investigation of pet owners in Romania. People Anim. Int. J. Res. Pract.
2019
,2, 6. Available online: https://docs.lib.purdue.
edu/paij/vol2/iss1/6 (accessed on 9 March 2020).
43.
Johnson, T.P.; Garrity, T.F.; Stallones, L. Psychometric evaluation of the Lexington attachment to pets scale (LAPS). Anthrozoös
1992,5, 160–175. [CrossRef]
44.
Tardif-Williams, C.; Bosacki, S. Evaluating the impact of a humane education summer-camp program on school-aged children’s
relationships with companion animals. Anthrozoös 2015,28, 587–600. [CrossRef]
45.
López, A.; Sanderman, R.; Ranchor, A.V.; Schroevers, M.J. Compassion for others and self-compassion: Levels, correlates, and
relationship with psychological well-being. Mindfulness 2017,9, 325–331. [CrossRef]
46.
Lasa, A.; Holgaso, T.; Carrosco, M.; Del Barrio, G. The structure of Bryant’s empathy index for children: A cross-validation study.
Span. J. Psychol. 2008,11, 670–677.
47. Bryant, B.K. An index of empathy for children and adolescents. Child Dev. 1982,53, 413–425. [CrossRef]
48. Davis, M.H. Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach; Brown & Benchmark: Madison, WI, USA, 1996.
49.
Jolliffe, D.; Farrington, D.P. Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav.
2004
,9,
441–476. [CrossRef]
50.
Lucas-Molina, B.; Pérez-Albéniz, A.; Giménez-Dasí, M. The assessment of cyberbullying: The present situation and future
challenge. Pap. Del. Psicólogo. 2016,37, 27–35.
51. Bloom, P. Rational Compassion: Against Empathy: The Case for Rational Compassion; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 2016.
52.
Pommier, E.; Nef, K.D.; Tóth-Király, I. The development and validation of the Compassion Scale. Assessment
2020
,27, 21–39.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
53.
Gadomski, A.M.; Scribani, M.B.; Krupa, N.; Jenkins, P. Pet dogs and child physical activity: The role of child–dog attachment.
Pediatr. Obes. 2017,12, e37–e40. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
54.
Melson, G.F.; Peet, S.; Sparks, C. Children’s attachment to their pets: Links to socio-emotional development. Child. Environ. Q.
1991,8, 55–65.
55.
Mueller, M.K.; Richer, A.M.; Callina, K.S.; Charmaraman, L. Companion animal relationships and adolescent loneliness during
COVID-19. Animals 2021,11, 885. [CrossRef]
56.
Van Houtte, B.A.; Jarvis, P.A. The role of pets in preadolescent psychosocial development. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol.
1995
,16, 463–479.
[CrossRef]
57.
Westgarth, C.; Boddy, L.M.; Stratton, G.; German, A.J.; Gaskell, R.M.; Coyne, K.P.; Bundred, P.; Dawson, S. Pet ownership, dog
types and attachment to pets in 9–10 year old children in Liverpool, UK. BMC Vet. Res. 2013,9, 102. [CrossRef]
58. Kurdek, L.A. Pet dogs as attachment figures for adult owners. J. Fam. Psychol. 2009,23, 439–446. [CrossRef]
59.
Angantyr, M.; Hansen, E.M.; Eklund, J.H.; Malm, K. Reducing sex differences in children’s empathy for animals through a
training intervention. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2016,30, 273–281. [CrossRef]
60.
Dowling, K.; Barry, M.M. The effects of implementation quality of a school-based social and emotional well-being program on
students’ outcomes. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2020,10, 595–614. [CrossRef]
61.
Greenberg, M.T.; Domitrovich, C.E.; Weissberg, R.P.; Durlak, J.A. Social and emotional learning as a public health approach to
education. Future Child. 2017,27, 13–32. [CrossRef]
62. Cullen, M. Mindfulness-based interventions: An emerging phenomenon. Mindfulness 2011,2, 186–193. [CrossRef]
Adolescents 2022,2507
63.
Cullen, M.; Brito-Pons, G.B.; Roeser, R.W. Mindfulness-based emotional balance: History of development, curriculum and
research. In Handbook of Mindfulness-Based Programmes: Mindfulness Interventions from Education to Health and Therapy; Ivtzan, I.,
Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2019; pp. 64–76.
64.
Esposito, L.; McCune, S.; Griffin, J.A.; Maholmes, V. Directions in human–animal interaction research: Child development, health,
and therapeutic interventions. Child Dev. Perspect. 2011,5, 205–211. [CrossRef]
65.
Slomkowski, C.; Rende, R.; Conger, K.J.; Simons, R.L.; Conger, R.D. Sisters, brothers, and delinquency: Evaluating social influence
during early and middle adolescence. Child Dev. 2001,72, 271–283. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
66.
Thompson, R.; Simpson, J.; Berlin, L. Taking perspective on attachment theory and research: Nine fundamental questions. Attach.
Hum. Dev. 2022,24, 543–560. [CrossRef]