ArticlePDF Available

Elements of musical and dance sophistication predict musical groove perception

Frontiers
Frontiers in Psychology
Authors:
  • NIRx Medical Technologies LLC

Abstract and Figures

Listening to groovy music is an enjoyable experience and a common human behavior in some cultures. Specifically, many listeners agree that songs they find to be more familiar and pleasurable are more likely to induce the experience of musical groove. While the pleasurable and dance-inducing effects of musical groove are omnipresent, we know less about how subjective feelings toward music, individual musical or dance experiences, or more objective musical perception abilities are correlated with the way we experience groove. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate how musical and dance sophistication relates to musical groove perception. One-hundred 24 participants completed an online study during which they rated 20 songs, considered high- or low-groove, and completed the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index, the Goldsmiths Dance Sophistication Index, the Beat and Meter Sensitivity Task, and a modified short version of the Profile for Music Perception Skills. Our results reveal that measures of perceptual abilities, musical training, and social dancing predicted the difference in groove rating between high- and low-groove music. Overall, these findings support the notion that listeners’ individual experiences and predispositions may shape their perception of musical groove, although other causal directions are also possible. This research helps elucidate the correlates and possible causes of musical groove perception in a wide range of listeners.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Frontiers in Psychology 01 frontiersin.org
Elements of musical and dance
sophistication predict musical
groove perception
Samantha R. O’Connell
1
*, Jessica E. Nave-Blodgett
2, Grace E.
Wilson
2, Erin E. Hannon
2 and Joel S. Snyder
2*
1 Caruso Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Keck School of Medicine of USC,
University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States, 2 Department of Psychology,
University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, United States
Listening to groovy music is an enjoyable experience and a common human
behavior in some cultures. Specifically, many listeners agree that songs
they find to bemore familiar and pleasurable are more likely to induce the
experience of musical groove. While the pleasurable and dance-inducing
eects of musical groove are omnipresent, we know less about how
subjective feelings toward music, individual musical or dance experiences,
or more objective musical perception abilities are correlated with the way
weexperience groove. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate how
musical and dance sophistication relates to musical groove perception. One-
hundred 24 participants completed an online study during which they rated
20 songs, considered high- or low-groove, and completed the Goldsmiths
Musical Sophistication Index, the Goldsmiths Dance Sophistication Index, the
Beat and Meter Sensitivity Task, and a modified short version of the Profile for
Music Perception Skills. Our results reveal that measures of perceptual abilities,
musical training, and social dancing predicted the dierence in groove rating
between high- and low-groove music. Overall, these findings support the
notion that listeners’ individual experiences and predispositions may shape
their perception of musical groove, although other causal directions are also
possible. This research helps elucidate the correlates and possible causes of
musical groove perception in a wide range of listeners.
KEYWORDS
auditory perception, musical sophistication, dance sophistication, groove, online
studies
Introduction
Moving to music is a common and pleasurable human behavior. Certain songs groove
in that they encourage spontaneous movement and feelings of enjoyment (Madison, 2006;
Madison etal., 2011; Janata etal., 2012; Matthews etal., 2020). Musical groove is recognized
as a characteristic of songs encompassing genres such as jazz, pop, rock, hip hop, R&B, soul,
and funk, made popular by artists like Stevie Wonder, Michael Jackson, and James Brown
(Danielsen, 2006). e origins of groove are thought to berooted in West African rhythms
TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 17 November 2022
DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Evangelos Himonides,
University College London,
UnitedKingdom
REVIEWED BY
Olivier Senn,
Lucerne University of Applied Sciences and
Arts, Switzerland
Chen-Gia Tsai,
National Taiwan University, Taiwan
*CORRESPONDENCE
Samantha R. O’Connell
sroconne@usc.edu
Joel S. Snyder
joel.snyder@unlv.edu
SPECIALTY SECTION
This article was submitted to
Performance Science,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
RECEIVED 26 July 2022
ACCEPTED 21 October 2022
PUBLISHED 17 November 2022
CITATION
O’Connell SR, Nave-Blodgett JE,
Wilson GE, Hannon EE and
Snyder JS (2022) Elements of musical and
dance sophistication predict musical
groove perception.
Front. Psychol. 13:998321.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
COPYRIGHT
© 2022 O’Connell, Nave-Blodgett, Wilson,
Hannon and Snyder. This is an open-access
article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License (CC
BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in
other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) and the copyright
owner(s) are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in
accordance with accepted academic
practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does not
comply with these terms.
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 02 frontiersin.org
(Pressing, 2002). Early songs with groove are oen associated with
swing, a type of jazz music composed of “swinging” rhythms in
which the beat is unevenly subdivided to sound like a lilt
(Buttereld, 2010b). As music evolved, groove became an
umbrella term describing a phenomenon in which musical
rhythms invoke movement (Iyer, 2002). Songs with musical
groove have become popular as naturalistic stimuli to study
interactions between auditory and motor brain regions (Zatorre
et al., 2007; Patel and Iversen, 2014). Listening to songs with
groove can enhance performance on a range of physical tasks
(Karageorghis and Terry, 1997; Styns etal., 2007; Buhmann etal.,
2016) by eliciting longer strides and faster steps while walking
(Leow etal., 2014), running (Edworthy and Waring, 2006), and
rowing (Rendi et al., 2016). Even without accompanying
movement, just listening to music with groove may have the
power to excite neurons in the motor system (Wilson and Davey,
2002; Stupacher etal., 2013; Ross etal., 2016; Matthews etal.,
2020; Martín-Fernández etal., 2021). As a result, musical groove
listening is gaining traction as an enjoyable and therapeutic gait
treatment for movement-related disorders such as Parkinson’s
disease (Nombela etal., 2013; Leow etal., 2014).
To understand this musical phenomenon, researchers have
studied the specic auditory components that may contribute to
the sensation of groove (Stupacher et al., 2016a). Converging
empirical evidence indicates that timing-based auditory properties
such as a salient, low-pitched beat (Drake etal., 2000; Madison
etal., 2011; Burger etal., 2012; Janata etal., 2012; Stupacher etal.,
2016a; Hove et al., 2019), moderate rhythmic complexity
(Temperley, 1999; Danielsen etal., 2014; Madison and Sioros,
2014; Sioros et al., 2014; Witek etal., 2014; Wesolowski and
Hofmann, 2016; Witek, 2017; Matthews et al., 2019), and a
medium tempo of about 120 beats per minute (MacDougall and
Moore, 2005; Styns etal., 2007; Kornysheva etal., 2010; Janata
etal., 2012; Leow et al., 2014; Michaelis etal., 2014; Stupacher
etal., 2016a; Etani etal., 2018; Liu etal., 2018) have all been
described as dening characteristics of musical groove. Beat-based
musical elements may also activate neural motor networks.
Listening to beat-based rhythms related to groove, without
accompanying physical movement, engages auditory (Snyder and
Large, 2005; Fujioka etal., 2009), prefrontal (Fukuie etal., 2022),
and sensorimotor brain regions (Grahn and Brett, 2007; Grahn
and Rowe, 2009, 2013; Fujioka etal., 2012). Additionally, listening
to beats and rhythms can encourage kinesthetic movement by
providing a temporal anchor to synchronize our bodies to the
music (Iyer, 2002; Leman, 2012; Leow etal., 2021) and with one
another (Kokal etal., 2011; Cirelli etal., 2014; Stupacher etal.,
2017a,b). Performing synchronized movements can lead to
arousal (Bowling et al., 2019), activation of reward networks
(Menon and Levitin, 2005; Kokal etal., 2011; Zatorre, 2015;
Matthews et al., 2020), and the release of feel-good
neurotransmitters such as endorphins and oxytocin (Tarr etal.,
2014, 2015; Josef etal., 2019), likely contributing to the overall
enjoyable experience of being “in the groove” (Madison, 2006; De
Bruyn etal., 2009; Janata etal., 2012).
ere is a consensus that those with formal music training may
have enhanced auditory perception (Kraus and Chandrasekaran,
2010; Strait etal., 2012, 2014, 2015; Kraus etal., 2014; Slater etal.,
2015; Habibi etal., 2016) and emotional responses to music (Blood
and Zatorre, 2001; Liu etal., 2018); however, there is a lack of
consensus regarding how musical expertise may shape perception of
musical groove. On one hand, research indicates that musicians’
perception of groove may beenhanced compared to non-musicians
(Stupacher etal., 2013; Ross et al., 2016; Matthews etal., 2019).
Musicians’ responsiveness to musical groove may beattributed to
their ability to hear minute changes in acoustic elements better than
non-musicians (Stupacher etal., 2016b). Musicians, compared to
non-musicians, potentially have more awareness of musical elements
important to musical groove such as harmonic complexity
(Matthews etal., 2019), rhythmic complexity (Grahn and Rowe,
2009; Stupacher etal., 2017c; Matthews etal., 2019), tempo (Etani
etal., 2018), syncopation (Madison and Sioros, 2014; Witek etal.,
2014; Senn et al., 2018; Matthews et al., 2019), micro-timing
deviations (Davies etal., 2013; Kilchenmann and Senn, 2015; Senn
etal., 2016), and beat perception (Grahn and Rowe, 2009; Stupacher
et al., 2017c; Nguyen et al., 2022). Additionally, relative to
non-musicians, musicians’ motor systems may react more robustly
to music with groove (Stupacher etal., 2013), possibly allowing for
better balance control (Ross etal., 2016). is could arise from
extensive training involving the synchronization of movements to
the beat when producing musical sounds (Stupacher etal., 2013),
resulting in stronger integration between perceptual and motor
brain networks (Zatorre etal., 2007; Luo et al., 2012; Patel and
Iversen, 2014; Martín-Fernández etal., 2021).
On the other hand, movement to music with groove may bea
phenomenon experienced by a wide range of listeners (Madison,
2006; Madison etal., 2011; Janata etal., 2012), and thus formal
expertise may beunnecessary for musical groove perception. For
example, multiple studies have found no dierences between
musicians and non-musicians in their susceptibility to groove
(Buttereld, 2010a; Frühauf etal., 2013; Hofmann etal., 2017).
Most recently, Senn et al. (2019b) showed only marginal main
eects of musical expertise on groove ratings when comparing
musicians, amateur musicians, and non-musicians. In another
study, non-musicians perceived music as groovier than musicians
(Witek et al., 2014). Across these studies, musicians and
non-musicians tend to agree on which songs are more or less
“groovy”; however, their musical experiences may drive their
preference for groove genres with higher or lower levels of musical
complexity. For example, while musicians may rate more complex
music, like jazz and funk, to be groovier” (Pressing, 2002;
Matthews etal., 2019), non-musicians may beinclined to rate pop
and rock higher in groove because it is less complex and more
familiar (Senn etal., 2021a). Taken together, factors such as innate
biological traits, musical preferences, and musical exposure, rather
than musical skills gained from playing an instrument, may have
equal or greater eects on how weperceive the groove.
While previous research has revealed brain and behavior
enhancements due to music training (Kraus and Chandrasekaran,
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 03 frontiersin.org
2010; Skoe and Kraus, 2010; Strait and Kraus, 2011; Slater and
Kraus, 2016), musicality varies within populations of those with
and without musical expertise (Zatorre, 2013; Nave-Blodgett etal.,
2021a,b). is may be because biological and environmental
benets may contribute to heightened musicality in both
musicians and non-musicians. In some instances, musicality may
becultivated due to an availability of resources (Corrigall etal.,
2013). In other instances, one’s musicality may bea predisposed
trait (Peretz etal., 2007; Tan etal., 2014; Mankel and Bidelman,
2018) that remains somewhat hidden due to a lack of nancial or
familial support (Schellenberg, 2015) or a lack of interest in learning
to play music; however, some of these untrained individuals may
become avid music appreciators and develop similar skills to
musicians through hours of listening or other activities such as
playing music video games (Pasinski etal., 2016). Furthermore, in
both musicians and non-musicians, musical ability (Swaminathan
and Schellenberg, 2018) and appreciation for certain types of
music may bedictated by one’s personality (McCrae, 2007; Luck
etal., 2010; Nusbaum etal., 2014; Colver and El-Alayli, 2015;
Swaminathan and Schellenberg, 2018; Kuckelkorn etal., 2021)
and music preferences (Madison, 2006; Salimpoor etal., 2013;
Wesolowski and Hofmann, 2016; Madison and Schiölde, 2017;
Senn etal., 2019b, 2021a; Kowalewski et al., 2020). erefore,
there is a growing need to understand individual dierences in
music perception that are not based on formal music training.
Groove has oen been studied in the context of music
performance: playing the music of a particular genre (e.g., jazz and
funk), how the music is performed, or the enjoyable sensation of
being “in the pocket” when musicians synchronize with the music
and with one another (Berliner, 1994; Hosken, 2021). Historically,
however, much of music was written for the purposes of dancing to
music. For instance, songs from music genres known for groove
rhythms, such as jazz or Afro-Cuban music, were rst composed to
accompany dance forms such as tap dance, swing dance (Madison,
2006), and Latin dances (Hughes, 2001). Oentimes, it is hard to
explain the feeling of groove without mentioning “movement” or
“dancing.” While there is an undeniable connection between
musical groove and dance (Merker, 2014; Fitch, 2016), there is a
surprising dearth of empirical studies investigating the inuence of
dance experience on musical groove perception (Bernardi etal.,
2017), or even general music perception.
As is the case with musical listening skills, dance-related skills
may behard to predict. Some dancers, like ballerinas, may possess
years of professional training with a dance company while others
may have years of self-taught experience dancing socially in a club.
With the rise of social media, dance access has also become more
widespread. Today, anyone with access to phone applications like
TikTok can create, share, and learn dance choreography without
having any prior experience. Dance experience or expertise may
also behard to assess because it can bedicult to disassociate from
musical experience. For instance, tap dance straddles the ne line
of being both music and dance because the art form equally values
the importance of rhythmic sounds and movement. For this
reason, many tap dancers identify as both musicians and dancers
(Hill, 2010). In fact, the division of music and dance seems bea
Western-focused mindset (Trehub etal., 2015). For example, in
Nigeria and in India the very same term (nkwa and sangeet,
respectively) is used for musical performance and dance (Balkwill
and ompson, 1999; Clayton, 2000). As the term groove itself is
at the intersection of dance and music, it is important to study the
inuence of dance experience on musical groove perception
regardless of one’s dance experience or how dance is identied.
Trained dancers, compared to non-dancers or non-trained
dancers, may possess heightened functioning of sensorineural
networks that may enhance their perception of musical groove.
For instance, those with dance training show increased cortical
thickness in superior temporal brain regions compared to
non-experts (Karpati etal., 2017): these regions are vital to the
auditory-motor integration network used during music listening
and production (Bangert etal., 2006; Zatorre etal., 2007; Gordon
etal., 2018). Additionally, trained dancers reveal enhancements in
sensorimotor integration (Karpati et al., 2016) and appear to
outperform non-trained dancers and non-musicians in
audiovisual beat perception and production tasks (Nguyen etal.,
2022). Furthermore, trained dancers, like trained musicians, show
cortical phase synchrony in beta and gamma frequency bands
during passive viewing of dance with music (Poikonen et al.,
2018). ese frequency bands have been implicated in musical
beat encoding and auditory-motor brain interactions (Fujioka
etal., 2009). Together, these studies suggest that dancers may
exhibit training-induced neuroplasticity in sensorimotor regions
that may engender heightened perception of the musical beat- a
crucial component of musical groove.
Although dance expertise may hone music perception, feeling
the groove may not bedependent on having superior perceptual
or motor skills. Instead, the pleasure wefeel from listening to
music with groove may depend on our physical movement with
music. For instance, those without formal dance training felt the
most pleasure and arousal when moving spontaneously to high-
groove music compared to low-groove music or when listening to
music without movement (Bernardi et al., 2017). is may
bebecause moving to music helps us understand the beat and
meter through embodiment (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2006,
2008; Leman, 2012; Chemin et al., 2014; Lee etal., 2015). e habit
of moving to music may also facilitate enjoyment of music with
groove. Head movements to the beat of the music produce
vestibular self-stimulated responses that may play an integral role
in the understanding of musical beat (Phillips-Silver and Trainor,
2007; Todd and Lee, 2015), and meter (Phillips-Silver and Trainor,
2008; Trainor etal., 2009), and may activate brain circuits involved
in reward (Todd and Lee, 2015; Reybrouck etal., 2019).
Additionally, high-groove music can strengthen the link
between beat and movement because it tends to besyncopated
(Janata etal., 2012; Witek, 2017; Witek etal., 2017), and the
experience of syncopation depends on a strong, internally
maintained beat (Pressing, 2002; Keller and Schubert, 2011; Sioros
etal., 2014; Witek and Clarke, 2014). Knowing the locations of
beats in time can help us synchronize our movements with the
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 04 frontiersin.org
music and with others (De Bruyn etal., 2009). Past experiences
moving to the music may also facilitate meter awareness. ose
without formal dance training, but with experience dancing
specic choreography, were better at tapping along to the music’s
beat than those who did not learn the choreography (Lee etal.,
2015). Furthermore, dance familiarity can beacquired through
observation. Frequent spectators of dance, compared to novice
dance spectators, showed increased corticospinal excitability as
they viewed the form of dance with which they were most familiar
(Jola etal., 2012). What is unclear, however, is whether these
increases in meter perception and motor activation due to
repeated dance observation translate to a heightened perception
of musical groove. erefore, there is a great need for investigations
that directly study dierences in music perception in those with
varying degrees of dance experience.
In the present study, weinvestigated how musical and dance
sophistication may inuence musical groove perception in adult
listeners with a wide range of artistic experiences. e rst aim of
this investigation was to understand how variations in musical
sophistication predict musical groove perception. Specically,
wemeasured how both objective and subjective (self-reported)
components of musical sophistication predict musical groove
ratings. Musical sophistication is the possession of heightened
music skills and engagement, and contains attributes such as
musical understanding, appreciation, evaluation, and
communication alongside skills such as playing an instrument,
improvisation, and possessing a sense of rhythm and pitch (Hallam
and Prince, 2003; Hallam, 2010; Müllensiefen et al., 2014).
Objective components were perceptual musical skills measured
using e Prole for Music Perception Skills (Law and Zentner,
2012; Zentner and Strauss, 2017) and the Beat and Meter Sensitivity
Task (Nave-Blodgett etal., 2021a,b). Subjective components were
measured using the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index
(Müllensiefen et al., 2014). Wepredicted that musical training, beat
sensitivity, and measure sensitivity would bethe most reliable
predictors of musical groove perception, though other possible
predictors could include active engagement, accent perception, or
rhythm perception. It is vital to understand these subtleties in
musicality across a wide range of listeners because musical groove’s
likeability and eects on movement seem omnipresent (Madison,
2006; Madison etal., 2011; Janata etal., 2012), and thus potentially
independent of skills that are only honed via formal music training
(Leow etal., 2014).
e second aim of this study was to investigate the impact of
dance sophistication on musical groove perception. Dance
sophistication is the possession of heightened dance enjoyment,
knowledge, or skills without necessarily undergoing formal dance
training (Rose et al., 2020). Weanalyzed responses from the
Goldsmiths Dance Sophistication Index (Rose etal., 2020), a new
dance self-report assessment that distinguishes experience in
dance participation from experience in dance observation to
measure ones overall dance comprehension. e present study
marks one of the rst investigations studying dance experience
and musical groove perception. While there is little published
work on how dance appreciation or experience may shape the way
weperceive music with groove, wehypothesized dance training to
bea strong predictor of musical groove perception in this model.
Because weinvestigated listeners with varying degrees of dance
experience, perception of musical groove in individuals with less
dance experience may bemore dependent on personal traits that
make them more open to dancing in social settings.
Materials and methods
Participants
One hundred seventy-one adults completed the study. A priori
power analyses using G*Power (Faul etal., 2009) determined that
for a multiple regression model with seven predictors, data from
153 participants was eective in achieving a power (1–β) of 0.95
to detect a medium eect size (f
2
= 0.15) at a statistical signicance
level of α = 0.05. Most participants were UNLV undergraduates
enrolled in a psychology course (n = 146). e remaining
participants were recruited by word of mouth, email, or by
announcements posted on social media platforms (e.g., Facebook,
Instagram, and Twitter). Twenty-three participants were excluded
due to poor performance on the initial headphone check (see
“Headphone check” for details); eight participants were excluded
due to incorrect answers on compliance checks (see “Compliance
check” for details); one participant was excluded due to an
excessively noisy environment while completing the study; and 15
participants were excluded due to issues loading the stimuli. e
nal 124 participants were between the ages of 18–44 years old
(M = 22.6 years, SD = 5.77 years, females = 80) and had no history
of learning, neurological, or motor disorders. Power analyses
using G*Power (Faul etal., 2009) determined this sample size was
eective in achieving a power (1–β) of 0.885 to detect a medium
eect size (f2 = 0.15) at a statistical signicance level of α = 0.05.
While musicians and dancers were not actively recruited for the
present study, participants reported varying degrees of music and
dance experience (see Table 1 for detailed music and
dance experience).
Procedure
All testing was implemented online using Qualtrics (Qualtrics,
Provo, UT, United States) and LimeSurvey (LimeSurvey,
Hamburg, Germany). Participants followed an internet link to
access the experiment (link can befound on the project’s Open
Science Foundation Repository).1 Participants were required to
sign a consent form before beginning the study. Participants were
asked to complete the experiment on a computer over headphones
in a quiet environment. Participants proceeded through the study
1 https://osf.io/g3y7c/
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 05 frontiersin.org
beginning with the most dicult and attentionally taxing
measures, described below in order of administration. Participants
were oered opportunities to take short breaks aer each test and
subtest. Total test time was 60–90 min.
Headphone check
To ensure that participants were using headphones as
requested, and could hear the auditory stimuli clearly, they
completed a short assessment prior to beginning the experiment.
In each trial, participants were presented with three tones and
asked to indicate which was the quietest: the correct answer could
only bediscerned if the individual was wearing headphones rather
than listening in free-eld (Woods etal., 2017). Weexcluded data
from participants who did not correctly answer at least ve out of
the six trials.
Profile for music perception skills
First, participants completed the short version of the Prole
for Music Perception Skills (PROMS; Zentner and Strauss, 2017).
is music aptitude battery objectively measures perceptual
musical skills across multiple modalities in both musically trained
and untrained individuals (Law and Zentner, 2012).
Weadministered the Rhythm, Embedded Rhythm (rhythm-to-
melody), Tempo, and Accent subtests because of their theorized
importance to the feeling of musical groove (Witek etal., 2014;
Etani etal., 2018; Matthews etal., 2019; Fukuie etal., 2022) and
their robustness against noisy testing environments (Zentner and
Strauss, 2017). We also chose to use the Melody subtest as an
exploratory measure as previous research has yet to report that
melody inuences musical groove. Each subtest consists of eight
to ten trials with a total testing time of 25 min. In each subtest, a
trial consisted of a standard auditory stimulus (played twice)
followed by one comparison auditory stimulus. Participants
indicated (1) if the comparison stimulus was the same as the
standard stimulus and (2) how condent they were in their answer.
e Melody subtest assessed the ability to recognize either
tonal (easy) or atonal (dicult) melodies. Two-bar, eighth-note
melodies were played by a MIDI harpsicord monophonically in
4/4 time. In dierent trials, one note of the comparison melody
would dier from the reference melody by one semitone. e
Rhythm subtest assessed the ability to recognize percussive
rhythmic motifs. Two-bar phrases played equally accented in 4/4
time were composed of quarter, eighth, and sixteenth notes. Trials
varied in diculty by where the rhythmic deviant was located
between the reference and comparison stimuli (i.e., easy trials had
rhythmic deviants presented on downbeats and hard trials had
rhythmic deviants presented on up-beats). e Embedded
Rhythm subtest assesses the ability to recognize a percussive
rhythmic motif when it is presented as part of a melody. Two-bar
monophonically played and equally accented phrases in 4/4 time
were composed of eighth and quarter notes. e reference
stimulus was presented as a simple rhythm while the comparison
stimulus was presented as a tonal melody. Participants were asked
to identify whether the rhythm of the melody in the comparison
stimulus matched the rhythm of the reference stimulus. e
Tempo subtest assessed the ability to discriminate the speed at
which music is played. Reference and comparison stimuli were
polyphonically played in 4/4 time. e comparison stimuli ranged
in diculty by being 1 BPM (dicult) to 7 BPM (easy) dierent
from the reference stimulus. e Accent subtest assessed the
ability to discriminate the relative emphasis given to certain notes
in a rhythmic pattern. Across two identical rhythmic motifs
presented in 4/4 time monophonically by a MIDI drum sound,
accented notes were presented as 3 dB louder than non-accented
notes. Easy trials had more accent variations between reference
and comparison stimuli compared to moderate and dicult trials.
More detailed information on these subtests can befound in Law
and Zentner (2012).
Beat and meter sensitivity task
In the next task, participants completed the Short BMS, a brief
version of the Nave-Blodgett et al. (2021a,b) Beat and Meter
Sensitivity Task (BMS), presented via Qualtrics. e BMS uses
naturalistic music stimuli to assess auditory beat and meter
sensitivity in individuals with varying levels of musical expertise,
and does not require familiarity with musical terms, theory, or
notation. In the Short BMS, participants listened to brief excerpts
of commercially-recorded ballroom dance music overlaid with a
custom click track that either matched or mismatched the music
at the beat and measure levels (four possible alignment
TABLE1 Participants’ musical and dance experience: self-reported category of expertise.
n (%)
Musical experience
NE OM RM SAM PM To tal
Dance experience NE 37 (29.8%) 31 (25%) 7 (5.6%) 3 (2.4%) 2 (1.6%) 80 (64.5%)
OD 12 (9.6%) 7 (5.6%) 3 (2.4%) 2 (1.6%) 0 (0%) 8 (6.4%)
RD 2 (1.6%) 5 (4%) 1 (0.8%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 8 (6.4%)
SAD 4 (3.2%) 3 (2.4%) 2 (1.6%) 1 (0.8%) 0 (0%) 10 (8.1%)
PD 0 (0%) 1 (0.8%) 0 (0%) 1 (0.8%) 0 (0%) 2 (1.6%)
Tot a l 55 (44%) 47 (37.9%) 13 (10.4%) 7 (5.6%) 2 (1.6%) 124 (100%)
No experience (NE) = have no experience playing/participating. Occasional musician (OM)/Occasional dancer (OD) = less than weekly practice/participation. Recreational musician (RM)/
Recreational dancer (RD) = weekly practice or recreational playing/performance. Serious Amateur Musician (SAM)/Serious Amateur Dancer (SAD) = extensive commitment to practice and/or
recreational music or dance activity. Professional musician (PM)/Professional dancer (PM) = paid to perform and/or teach music or dance. Va lues are expressed as n (% of reported sample).
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 06 frontiersin.org
conditions). e musical excerpts were taken from six ballroom
dance pieces, three of which were scored in 3/4 time (triple meter)
and three of which were scored in 4/4 time (duple meter). e
click track could fully match the beat and measure of the musical
excerpt (beat matching/measure matching; e.g., a click track in 4/4
paired with a musical excerpt in 4/4), match the beat but not the
measure (beat matching/measure mismatching; e.g., a click track
in 3/4 paired with a musical excerpt in 4/4 where the beat of the
click track and music aligns), match the measure of the music but
not the beat (beat mismatching/measure matching; e.g., a click
track in 3/4 paired with a musical excerpt in 4/4 where the
measure-level downbeat matches but the beat does not), or not
match either the beat or measure of the music (beat mismatching/
measure mismatching; e.g., a click-track in 3/4 with a beat-level
tempo 15% faster or slower than the musical excerpt in 4/4).
Please consult the Open Science Foundation Repository2 for
methods and stimulus information specic to the Short BMS, and
Nave-Blodgett etal. (2021a,b) for general methods.
Aer listening to each musical excerpt and click track pairing,
participants rated the t of the click track to the music using a
four-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (“Not Well at All”) to
4 (“Very Well”). Participants were given four practice trials to
experience the stimuli and the rating scale prior to starting the
experimental portion of the Short BMS. e Short BMS consists
of 30 pairs of musical excerpts/click tracks. e musical excerpt/
click track pairings were three musical measures long, which
translated to approximately 6–8 s per trial. e task took
approximately 10 min for participants to complete. e Short BMS
results in two scores per participant, a beat sensitivity score that
indicates participants’ ability to distinguish between beat-
matching and beat-mismatching metronomes, and a meter
sensitivity score that indicates participants’ ability to distinguish
between metronomes that fully match the beat and measure of the
2 https://osf.io/8nfvq/
music and those metronomes that match the beat of the music but
not the measure.
Musical groove judgment task
Following the Short BMS, participants completed the Musical
Groove Judgment Task (MGJT). Participants listened to 15-s clips
of 10 high-groove (HG) and 10 low-groove (LG) songs and made
judgments on what they heard. e ten songs rated highest in
groove and the ten songs rated lowest in groove were chosen for
this study from the Janata etal. (2012) music library (see Table2
for complete song list). In this task, groovy was dened as how
much a song makes youwant to dance. On a seven-point Likert
scale, they answered the following questions: (1) “Is this song
groovy? (i.e., does it make youwant to dance?),” (2) “Did youenjoy
this song?,” and (3) “Are youfamiliar with this song?.” Likert scale
choices ranged from Not groovy at all, I do not like it at all, and
is song is not familiar at all to Very groovy, I like it very much,
and is song is very familiar, respectively. Stimuli were truncated
to 15-s segments using Audacity 2.1.2 (Audacity Team, 2021) and
normalized to bethe same volume. As in Janata etal. (2012), song
stimuli were segmented starting at ~ 45 s into the song. is task
took about 5 min to complete (Table3).
Goldsmiths musical sophistication index
Upon completion of the MGJT, participants completed the
Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index Self-Report Inventory
(Gold-MSI), a 39-item psychometric instrument used to quantify
the amount of musical engagement, skill, and behavior of an
individual (Müllensiefen et al., 2013). e questions on this
assessment are grouped into ve subscales: Active Engagement,
Perceptual Abilities, Musical Training, Singing Abilities, and
Emotions (see Müllensiefen etal., 2013, 2014 for each subscale’s
detailed question information). e Active Engagement subscale
comprised questions that described a range of active musical
engagement behaviors (e.g., “I keep track of new music that
Icome across (e.g., new artists or recordings)” or “I do not spend
much of my disposable income on music”). e Perceptual
Abilities subscale comprised questions that each represented the
self-assessment of cognitive musical ability and music listening
skills (e.g., “I can tell when people sing or play out of tune”). e
Musical Training subscale combined questions involving the
extent of self-reported musical training and practice (e.g., “I
engaged in regular daily practice of a musical instrument
including voice for __ years”), and the degree of self-assessed
musicianship (“I would not consider myself a musician”). e
Singing Abilities subscale comprised questions that reected upon
dierent self-reported skills and activities related to singing (e.g.,
“I amnot able to sing in harmony when somebody is singing a
familiar tune”). e Emotions subscale comprised questions
describing self-reported behaviors that happen frequency in
response to an external music source. ese questions were not
assessing planned behaviors or those that could change based on
increased musical experience (e.g., “I hardly ever hum or sing
along to music”). All items, except those assessing Musical
TABLE2 Participants’ musical and dance experience: self-reported
characteristics.
Characteristic n M SD Range
Age started music lessons 37 9.7 3.7 4–16
Years of music lessons 37 6.5 4.2 1–20
Age started music ensemble 50 11.7 2.2 5–16
Years of music ensemble 50 5.8 4.7 1–30
Average hours of daily playing 37 2.7 2.5 0.5–11
Age started dance lessons 39 9.4 6.8 2–35
Years of dance lessons 39 8.6 7.6 0.5–27
Average hours of daily dancing 23 3.0 2.0 0.25–8
Hours of music listening per week 124 15.0 14.4 0–70
Years musical and dance training, age started musical and dance training, and hours
daily playing only include participants with relevant experience. Hours of music
listening per week include all participants. ose with both music and dance experience
are not included in the separate totals for musical and dance experience.
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 07 frontiersin.org
Training, are scored on a seven-point Likert scale with choices that
range from Completely disagree to Completely agree. e composite
score of these ve subscales makes up an individuals General
Musical Sophistication score (Müllensiefen et al., 2013). More
details about the Gold-MSI can be found in Müllensiefen
etal. (2013).
Goldsmiths dance sophistication index
Aer the Gold-MSI, participants completed the Goldsmiths
Dance Sophistication Index (Gold-DSI), a 26-item standardized
self-report instrument used to quantify individual dierences in
doing dance (i.e., participatory dance experience), watching dance
(i.e., observational dance experience), and ones knowledge about
dance (Rose etal., 2020). Like the Gold-MSI, the Gold-DSI is
designed to measure a wide range of dance skills, behaviors, and
engagement in a general population (Rose et al., 2020). e
Gold-DSI is comprised of two separate inventories: Participatory
Dance Experience and Observational Dance Experience. e
composite score of four subtests (Body Awareness, Social Dancing,
Urge To Dance, and Dance Training) contribute to the
Participatory Dance Experience score while the composite score
on six separate questions comprises the Observational Dance
Experience score (see Rose etal., 2020 for each subscale’s detailed
question information). e questions were randomized per
participant. e Body Awareness subscale consisted of items that
ask about the degree of self-assessed movement and coordination
(e.g., “I nd it easy to learn new movements”). e Social Dancing
subscale consisted of items describing self-reported behaviors
about one’s time spent dancing with others and the emotions felt
around dancing in public places (e.g., “If someone asks me to
dance, Iusually say yes”). e Urge To Dance subscale consisted
of items describing self-reported physical and emotional responses
to music related to dance and how much time spent dancing (e.g.,
“When I dance, I feel better”). e Dance Training subscale
consisted of questions describing the extent of one’s formal dance
experience and their self-assessed level of dance ability (e.g., “I
have taken regular dance classes at least once a week for __ years”).
e Observational Dance Experience subscale consisted of items
that ask the extent to which one self-reports watching dance
in-person or on TV/online and the emotions felt when watching
dance (e.g., “I like watching people dance”). All items, except those
assessing Dance Training, are scored on a seven-point Likert scale
with choices that range from Completely disagree to Completely
agree. More details about the Gold-DSI can befound in Rose
etal. (2020).
Demographics
e nal task participants completed was a demographics
questionnaire that asked questions about health history, music
experience, dance experience, exercise, and engagement with
music listening.
Compliance check
roughout the study, we utilized a set of previously
published questions to ensure participants were adequately
attending to the experimental task (Mehr etal., 2018). Within
each experimental block, participants were asked one time to
answer the following question: “What color is the sky? Please
answer this incorrectly, on purpose, by choosing red instead of
blue.” e possible response options were “Green,” “Blue,” “Red,
or “Yellow”. e correct response was only presented in each
answer slot once. is question was presented a total of ve
times. Participant who did not select “Red” to all ve questions
were excluded from analysis.
Aer the completion of the experiment, participants were
asked to answer compliance questions to ensure that the
experiment was completed with eort in an environment with
minimal distraction. e rst question stated, “People are working
on this task in many dierent places. Please tell us about the place
you were at when working on this task. Please answer honestly.
Response options were (1) “I worked on this study in a very noisy
place,” (2) “I worked on this study in a somewhat noisy place,” (3)
“I worked on this study in a somewhat quiet place,” or (4) “I
worked on this study in a very quiet place. ose who answered
the question with “I worked on this study in a very noisy place” or
“I worked on this study in a somewhat noisy place” were excluded
from analysis. e second question asked, “Please tell us if youhad
TABLE3 Songs used in the musical groove judgment task.
Song name Artist Groove Genre Groove
rating
Superstition Stevie Wonder High Soul 108.7
It’s a Wrap FHI (Funky Hobo #1) High Soul 105.9
Flash Light Parliament High Soul 105.1
Lady Marmalade Patti LaBelle High Soul 102.5
Up for the
Downstroke
e Clinton
Administration
High Soul 102.4
Mama Cita Funk Squad High Soul 101.6
Music Leela James High Soul 101.1
If IAin’t Got You Alicia Keys High Soul 98.7
Sing, Sing, Sing Benny Goodman High Jazz 97.4
In the Mood Glenn Miller High Jazz 96.9
Space Oddity David Bowie Low Rock 38.7
Ray Dawn Balloon Trey Anastasio Low Rock 38.5
Druid Fluid Yo-Yo Ma, Mark
O’Connor, and Edgar
Meyer
Low Folk 38.1
Flandyke Shore e Albion Band Low Folk 36.5
Citi Na GCumman William Coulter and
Friends
Low Folk 35.2
Dawn Star Dean Magraw Low Folk 34.8
Fortuna Kaki King Low Folk 32.6
Beauty of the Sea e Gabe Dixon Band Low Rock 32.1
Sweet ing Alison Brown Low Folk 30.9
Hymn for Jaco Adrian Legg Low Folk 29.3
Groove = groove category (i.e., low or high). Groove rating values are derived from
Janata etal. (2012).
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 08 frontiersin.org
diculty loading the sounds. Please answer honestly.” From “Yes”
or “No” response choices, any participant who responded with
“Yes” was excluded from analysis. e nal question asked, “How
carefully did you complete this experiment? Please answer
honestly.” From the response options of (1) “Not at all carefully,
(2) “Slightly carefully,” (3) “Moderately carefully,” (4) “Quite
carefully,” and (5) “Very carefully,” those who answered with “Not
at all carefully,” “Slightly carefully,” or “Moderately carefully” were
excluded from analysis.
Statistical design
All data for this study can befound here: https://osf.io/g3y7c/.
e main analysis was a stepwise multiple linear regression that
predicted musical groove sensitivity score, which is the dierence
between mean high-groove music ratings and mean low-groove
music ratings (Mhigh-groove music– Mlow-groove music). Musical groove
sensitivity scores can range from –6 to + 6. ose with higher
musical groove sensitivity scores (score = 4–6) perceive greater
dierences between high- and low-groove songs. ose with lower
musical groove sensitivity scores (score = 1–3) either perceive less
dierences between high-and low-groove songs or no dierences
(score = 0) between the two groove types. A score evaluating the
dierence between high- and low-groove music was employed
rather than separately regressing ratings on high- and low-groove
songs for two reasons. First, a dierence score uses all the groove
rating data in a single outcome measure. Second, a dierence score
controls for response bias or how individual participants use the
subjective rating scale for “grooviness.” For example, two
participants might provide dierent numbers for the same
subjective amount of groove for a low-groove song, but it is
assumed that the increased rating they would each provide for a
high-groove song would reect an accurate measure of their
sensitivity for the dierence between low-groove and high-groove
songs. Overall, this allows for a more nuanced measurement of
musical groove that captures how individuals dierently rate high-
and low-groove music.
A bidirectional stepwise linear regression analysis was
performed in the R statistical soware environment (R Core
Team, 2022) to assess how subtests of the Gold-MSI, the Gold-
DSI, the PROMS, and the Short BMS may predict musical groove
sensitivity. First, weused the stepAIC() function from the MASS
package to choose predictors for a best-t model based on the
Akaike Information Criterion (AIC): a measure of t that
estimates the quality of each model. is automatic function
evaluates models in parallel to avoid overtting of data and
cherry-picking of predictors (Venables and Ripley, 2002). en,
wechose the best-t model based on the lowest AIC value and ran
a multiple linear regression analysis using the lm() function on
the resulting automatically chosen predictors.
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) evaluating musical groove,
familiarity, and likeability ratings and Pearson’s r correlation
coecients between the predictor and criterion variables for the
resulting stepwise multiple regression analysis were calculated in
SPSS 28 (IBM, Chicago, IL, United States). e ANOVA for
musical groove, likeability, and familiarity ratings was replicated
from Janata etal. (2012) and was conducted with the musical
excerpt as a case (i.e., data averaged across participants for each
excerpt) rather than the participant. is was intentional to
validate this dataset against the original ndings of Janata
etal. (2012).
Results
Relation of musical groove, likeability,
and familiarity
Replicating prior work (Janata etal., 2012), a one-way analysis
of variance (ANOVA) with a two-tailed alpha level of 0.05 was
conducted with musical excerpt as a case (i.e., data averaged across
participants for each excerpt). e results conrmed that listeners
in the Musical Groove Judgment Task gave higher groove ratings
to high-groove (M = 5.42, CI = 5.05, 5.79) than to low-groove
excerpts (M = 2.14, CI = 1.81, 2.47), F1,18 = 226.02, p < 0.001,
η2 = 0.926 (see Figure1A). is conrms that the songs identied
in this study, borrowed from Janata etal. (2012), were categorized
correctly as high-groove or low-groove by the researchers and
conrmed by listener ratings. ere were also statistically
signicant positive correlations between mean musical groove
ratings and likeability ratings, r (18) = 0.79, p < 0.001; groove and
familiarity ratings, r (18) = 0.70, p < 0.001; and familiarity and
likeability ratings, r (18) = 0.88, p < 0.001 (see Figure1B).
Stepwise multiple linear regression
analysis
We rst entered a total of 17 predictors into the stepwise linear
regression model. e predictors were the total scores of each of
the Gold-MSI subscales (i.e., Active Engagement, Perceptual
Abilities, Emotions, Singing Abilities, and Musical Training), the
total scores of each of the Gold-DSI subscales (i.e., Body
Awareness, Social Dancing, Urge To Dance, and Dance Training,
and Observational Dance Experience), the total scores of each
PROMS subtest (i.e., Melody, Tempo, Accent, Rhythm, and
Embedded Rhythm), and the total scores on each of the Short
BMS measures (i.e., beat sensitivity and measure sensitivity). Aer
running the stepAIC() function, wechose the best tting model
based on the lowest AIC value (AIC = 22.612). e selected
model for analysis contained seven predictors: three predictors
from the Gold-MSI (Perceptual Abilities, Musical Training, and
Emotions), two predictors from the Gold-DSI (Social Dancing
and Dance Experience), one predictor from the PROMS (Accent),
and one predictor from the Short BMS (beat sensitivity).
Weregressed these predictors using the lm() function to nd that
Perceptual Abilities, Musical Training, and Social Dancing scores
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 09 frontiersin.org
signicantly predicted musical groove sensitivity, F (7,
116) = 5.091, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.24, adj. R2 = 0.19 (see Table 4).
Emotions, Dance Training, Accent and beat sensitivity scores were
not statistically signicant predictors of groove sensitivity,
ps > 0.05. Multicollinearity was assessed: VIF were below 2.50
suggesting no presence of multicollinearity (Kutner et al., 2005).
e accompanying correlations between predictor and criterion
variables can befound in Table5.
Discussion
e present study investigated individual dierences in music
and dance characteristics that may contribute to musical groove
perception. Specically, this online experiment examined 17
potential predictors and assessed how facets of musical
sophistication, dance sophistication, and performance on
music-based perceptual tasks inuenced individuals’ sensitivity to
musical groove. Although previous studies focused on the acoustic
components of music (Witek etal., 2014; Stupacher etal., 2016a;
Senn et al., 2017, 2018) and the way music is performed that
makes the music itself “groovy” (Hurley etal., 2014; Witek and
Clarke, 2014; Kilchenmann and Senn, 2015; Senn etal., 2016),
here we chose to ask how individual dierences in listeners
experiences, training, and perceptual skills might shape the way
they experience musical groove. Our study is novel in that weuse
a new measure, the musical groove sensitivity score, which can
beused in a regression framework to examine the relationship
between an individual’s groove perception and other individual
dierence measures.
In general, our participants agreed on ratings of musical
groove, familiarity, and likeability. Songs previously rated as high
and low in musical groove by listeners in Janata etal. (2012) were
rated similarly by the listeners in the present study. Specically,
AB
FIGURE1
Mean musical groove ratings and correlations for musical groove, likeability, and familiarity. (A) Bar graphs of mean musical groove ratings (N = 20;
high-groove = 10, low-groove = 10) based on musical excerpt as a case (i.e., data averaged across participants for each excerpt). Error bars indicate
95% confidence intervals. Results reveal statistically significant dierences between high-groove (black) and low-groove (grey) mean song ratings
F1,18 = 226.02, p < 0.001. (B) Relationships between mean musical groove ratings, mean likeability ratings, and mean familiarity ratings (N = 20; high-
groove = 10, low-groove = 10). Results show statistically significant positive correlations between musical groove and likeability ratings, musical
groove and familiarity ratings, and likeability and familiarity ratings.
TABLE4 Stepwise multiple linear regression results.
Vari a b le B97.5% CI for βSEBβt p
LL UL
Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities 0.03 0.24 0.29 0.01 0.27 2.46 0.015*
Gold-MSI Musical Training 0.02 0.24 0.20 0.01 0.22 2.15 0.034*
Gold-MSI Emotions 0.03 0.14 0.21 0.02 0.17 1.78 0.077
Gold-DSI Social Dancing 0.02 0.18 0.23 0.01 0.21 2.17 0.032*
Gold-DSI Dance Training 0.03 0.21 0.15 0.02 0.18 1.73 0.087
BMS Beat Sensitivity 0.20 0.15 0.42 0.15 0.13 1.38 0.171
PROMS Accent 0.09 0.03 0.26 0.06 0.14 1.51 0.133
Criterion variable, musical groove sensitivity score. AIC = 22.612, F(7, 116) = 5.091, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.24, adj. R2 = 0.19. B, unstandardized regression coecients. CI, condence interval. LL,
lower limit. UL, upper limit. β, standardized regression coecients. *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01. Bolded values emphasize the signicant predictors found in the stepwise regression model.
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 10 frontiersin.org
our listeners rated high-groove music as being signicantly more
“groovy” than low-groove music. As in Janata etal. (2012), our
participants also rated high-groove songs as more familiar and
more likeable than low-groove songs. Musical groove ratings,
familiarity ratings, and likeability ratings all had strong, positive
relationships with one another.
Using an AIC-based stepwise model selection, seven out of 17
possible predictors from subtest scores of the Gold-MSI, Gold-
DSI, PROMS, and Short BMS were chosen to predict the musical
groove sensitivity score. e seven selected predictors (Gold-MSI
Perceptual Abilities, Gold-MSI Musical Training, Gold-MSI
Emotions, Gold-DSI Dance Training, Gold-DSI Social Dancing,
PROMS Accent, and Short BMS beat sensitivity) together
accounted for 24% of the variance in musical groove sensitivity
score. Of these predictors, self-reported Perceptual Abilities,
Musical Training, and Social Dancing scores separately predicted
musical groove dierence ratings compared to the other predictors
in the model. Emotions, Dance Training, Accent, and beat
sensitivity scores did not signicantly predict the dierence
between high-and low-groove music ratings.
Perceptual abilities and musical training
e Perceptual Abilities subtest of the Gold-MSI is comprised
of self-reported views of song recognition, tonal perception, genre
identication, and how well one can judge others’ musical abilities
(Müllensiefen et al., 2014). Pearson r correlations reected a
positive relationship between Perceptual Abilities and musical
groove sensitivity scores indicating that those who think they are
good at judging others’ musical abilities, identifying musical
genres, recognizing familiar music, and spotting mistakes in
performances tend to rate high-and low-groove music more
distinctly. Perceptual Abilities also had signicant positive
correlations with Gold-MSI Musical Training, Gold-MSI
Emotions, Gold-DSI Dance Training, Gold-DSI Social Dancing,
PROMS Accent, and Short BMS beat sensitivity.
e Musical Training subtest of the Gold-MSI is comprised of
self-reported views of musicianship as well as quantitative
measurements of practice time, formal training, and instrument
type (Müllensiefen etal., 2014). Pearson r correlations reected a
barely positive relationship between Musical Training and musical
groove sensitivity scores indicating those that consider themselves
to be musicians, those that are complimented more oen on
performance quality, and those who report more hours of daily
practice, greater years of formal music training, and increased
numbers of instruments played rated high-and low-groove music
more distinctly. Musical Training also had signicant positive
correlations with Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities, Gold-MSI
Emotions, Gold-DSI Dance Training, PROMS Accent, and Short
BMS beat sensitivity, but was not signicantly correlated with
Gold-DSI Social Dancing.
In the stepwise regression model, the Perceptual Abilities
score was a signicant positive predictor of musical groove
sensitivity score. Interestingly, Musical Training score was a
signicant negative predictor of musical groove sensitivity score.
is regression result was surprising considering that when
correlated on its own, Music Training score has a barely positive
association with musical groove sensitivity score. It is only when
other predictors are considered in the regression model, however,
that Musical Training has a negative regression coecient.
ese unpredicted results may beconnected to the positive
association found between Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities and
Musical Training subtest scores. Individuals who possess more
honed perceptual abilities may have more musical training. Five
out of seven questions on the Gold-MSI Musical Training subtest
ask about quantitative hours of practice and years of training.
erefore, it seems that music training quantity is highly weighted
in the nal subscale score and is designed to identify individuals
with formal music training. Questions that comprise the
Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities subtest, such as judging musical
abilities and spotting mistakes during performances, are also some
of the many skills that are taught and honed when formally
learning to sing or play an instrument at a high level.
Additionally, those with formal music training may rate songs
with groove dierently from those without formal training.
Previous research has shown that musicians have rated more
complex music, like jazz and funk, to be“groovier” (Pressing,
2002; Matthews etal., 2019) while non-musicians have rated less
complex music, such as pop and rock, higher in groove (Senn
etal., 2021a). is may bedue to musicians understanding and
appreciating more complex music and how familiar musicians are
TABLE5 Correlations between variables in stepwise multiple linear regression analysis.
Vari a b le 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Musical Groove Sensitivity Score
2. Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities 0.31** --
3. Gold-MSI Musical Training 0.02 0.55***
4. Gold-MSI Emotions 0.31** 0.53*** 0.33***
5. Gold-DSI Social Dancing 0.21*0.19*0.05 0.15
6. Gold-DSI Dance Training 0.05 0.37*** 0.35*** 0.27** 0.52***
7. BMS Beat Sensitivity 0.29** 0.36*** 0.28** 0.37*** 0.16 0.22*
8. PROMS Accent 0.20*0.28** 0.37*** 0.19* 0.003 0.17 0.43***
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 11 frontiersin.org
with their genre of expertise. For instance, a jazz musician may
bebetter at dierentiating between high- and low-groove music if
they were making ratings across only jazz music as opposed to
rating only pop music. What is missing from the Gold-MSI,
however, is an assessment about the type of music these musicians
play. While this specic examination did not collect sucient
demographic data about the type of genre musicians claimed
expertise in, future research should consider comparing musical
groove sensitivity scores across a variety of musicians with
dierent types of expertise to see if familiarity with a certain genre
can drive musical groove ratings.
Conversely, possessing greater perceptual abilities may belinked
to more experience with music (e.g., avid listening or attending
concerts), or associated with self-taught, informal training (e.g.,
funk players with 20+ years of band experience), but may not
beindicative of formal music training (e.g., conservatory-trained
classical musicians). Müllensiefen etal. (2014) found that both the
Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities and Music Training subscale scores
had strong positive associations with perceptual musical skills tasks
such as the Gold-MSI Beat Perception and Melody Memory tests.
Our data also supports this notion with positive, signicant
relationships between the Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities, Gold-MSI
Musical Training, Short BMS beat sensitivity, and PROMS Accent—
two perceptual tasks that measure performance on music-related
skills designed to potentially identify musical ability not necessarily
honed through training.
Considering the positive and negative associations between
Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities and Musical Training in both the
correlation and regression analyses, respectively, the regression
result seems to imply that among those who score higher on
Perceptual Abilities, avid music appreciators and those with
informal music training may make greater distinctions between
high- and low-groove music compared those with formal music
training. For instance, the questions that make up the Gold-MSI
Perceptual Abilities subtest ask about genre identication and
recognition of familiar and novel songs. While these abilities can
belearned through formal music training, they can also berened
through frequent music listening or informal music training. e
high-and low-groove songs that were selected for this study
belonged to diering genres: high-groove songs were previously
categorized as belonging to soul and jazz genres while low-groove
songs were previously identied as belonging to rock and folk
genres (Janata etal., 2012). Avid music appreciators who have
experience listening to a wide variety of music, or informally
trained musicians with ample experience playing in funk and soul
bands, may beable to easily identify high-and low-groove music
genres as “dance” or “non-dance” songs, respectively, based on
genre, but not necessarily on how much they make them want to
dance. Frequent music listeners are also potentially better than
music novices at recognizing familiar and unfamiliar music.
Because songs higher in groove in this study and others (Janata
etal., 2012; Senn etal., 2021a) were also rated as more familiar,
these individuals may rate high- and low-groove music more
distinctly based on familiarity rather than how much it makes
them want to dance. Future research should consider matching
high- and low-groove songs on genre and familiarity to further
disentangle groove from familiarity and its associations with
pleasurable movement.
It is also possible that the discrepancy found between the
Gold-MSI Musical Training score and the musical groove sensitivity
score in the correlation and regression analyses may beaected by
suppressor variables in the stepwise regression. Suppressed variables
are sometimes identied by being highly positively correlated with
other signicant predictors within the regression model but are not
signicantly positively correlated with the criterion variable. ese
types of predictors may therefore have dierent relationships with a
criterion variable when doing simple correlation versus multiple
regression (Pandey and Elliott, 2010). e Gold-MSI Musical
Training predictor seemed to t this mold: it is signicantly positively
correlated with Gold-MSI Perceptual Abilities but not with musical
groove sensitivity and has a signicant negative regression coecient
in the regression model. While this study was more exploratory in
identifying potential variables that may predict musical groove
sensitivity score, it is possible that removing the Musical Training
score from the model may increase the magnitude in the relationship
between other signicant predictors and the criterion variable
(Mackinnon etal., 2000).
Social dancing
Social Dancing is a Gold-DSI subtest comprised of self-
reported views of social dance enjoyment and engagement
(Müllensiefen etal., 2014). Pearson r correlations reect a positive
relationship between Social Dancing and musical groove
sensitivity scores, indicating those who have more engagement in
social dancing, greater experience dancing with others, and
heightened enjoyment participating in social dance rated high-
and low-groove music more distinctly. Social Dancing also had
signicant positive correlations with Gold-MSI Perceptual
Abilities and Gold-DSI Dance Training but was not signicantly
correlated with Gold-MSI Musical Training, Gold-MSI Emotions,
PROMS Accent, and Short BMS beat sensitivity.
e stepwise regression model revealed the Gold-DSI Social
Dancing score as a signicant positive predictor of musical groove
sensitivity score. Unexpectedly, Dance Training score was not a
signicant predictor of musical groove sensitivity score and when
correlated on its own, Dance Training did not have a signicant
association with musical groove sensitivity score.
e Gold-DSI Social Dancing score may be a signicant
predictor of musical groove sensitivity scores because it assesses
dance in the context of socialization and enjoyment: all previously
reported descriptors of how people feel and act when hearing songs
with groove (Janata etal., 2012). Fitch (2016) argues that “…core
aspects of musical rhythm, especially ‘groove’ and syncopation, can
only be fully understood in the context of their origins in the
participatory social experience of dance” (p.1). Considering the
positive association between Gold-DSI Social Dancing and Dance
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 12 frontiersin.org
Training scores, those who scored higher on Social Dancing may
beindividuals with extensive dance training. Oentimes, classical
dance forms such as ballet, modern, or lyrical are choreographed
and performed to low-groove songs while social, contemporary, and
percussive dance forms like jazz, tap, and hip-hop are performed to
high-groove songs. erefore, these individuals would be well-
trained in evaluating what is considered “groovy” music based on
the dance form with which the music is associated. It is also possible
that those with more formal dance training also are more likely to go
social dancing compared to those with less formal training. e
Gold-DSI Dance Training subtest does gather quantitative
information about formal dance training (e.g., years of involvement
in formal dance classes); however, the Social Dancing subtest does
not assess quantitative social dance experience (e.g., how many
hours per week spent social dancing at a party or club), but rather
the qualitative experience of dancing (e.g., “Dancing with other
people is a great night out as far as I’m concerned”). While the
Gold-DSI does not gather this information, future studies should
investigate whether those with more formal dance training also
spend more time social dancing.
Taking together the correlation and regression analyses between
Dance Training, Social Dancing, and musical groove sensitivity,
however, this regression analysis seems to indicate that among
frequent social dancers, those with less formal dance training (e.g.,
those who attend clubs and parties to dance with friends) tend to
hear greater dierences between high- and low-groove music than
those with more formal dance training (e.g., professional classical
ballerinas). is seems to contradict our original prediction that
Dance Training would bea signicant predictor of musical groove
sensitivity score. ose who scored higher on Social Dancing may
not be formal dancers, but experienced non-trained dancers or
dance appreciators who enjoy dancing with others as a form of
bonding and socialization. Because songs with groove are oen
danced to in social settings, those who feel more comfortable
dancing socially may have more familiarity with musical groove and
as a result, are better at identifying dierences between high- and
low-groove music. ose who enjoy social dancing may also
bepeople who have greater openness to experience or are more
extraverted. Previous research has found that those who report more
openness to experience also have more episodes of pleasurable
esthetic chills to music (Colver and El-Alayli, 2015), which may
suggest greater emotional connection to music. ose who self-
report as being more extraverted also have greater local and global
body movements, faster head speeds, and greater hand ux and
hand distance when moving to music belonging to high-groove
genres such as rock, jazz, Latin, techno, funk, and pop (Luck etal.,
2010). is may indicate that those who enjoy dancing to music
from high-groove genres may also engage in more movement while
dancing, and as a result, have a more embodied representation of the
music itself. rough movement, these individuals may develop a
better sense of the beat and facilitate more enjoyment of groove
through head movements that stimulate the vestibular system and
reward networks (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2007, 2008; Reybrouck
etal., 2019).
Limitations and future directions
A limitation to the current study was the online format, which
was chosen due to social-distancing restrictions during the
COVID-19 pandemic which made in-person testing not feasible.
For this reason weused subjective groove ratings, which may
depend on individual participants’ interpretation of the word
“groovy.” Although wedened groove for participants as “does it
make youwant to dance?,” it is nevertheless possible that to some
extent their ratings reect their associations between certain
musical genres and the word “groovy.” Similarly, our measures of
sensitivity to musical beat were based on subjective ratings of t
between a metronome and music. Collecting accurate temporal
information or nger tapping data in online tasks is unreliable due
to potential timing lags and lack of necessary equipment in
everyday households. A future extension of this work could
incorporate production tasks, such as a beat synchronization test
in which participants tap along to music. It is possible that the
ability to produce a beat accurately in time to music may bea
more reliable predictor of hearing dierences between high-and
low-groove music than purely perceptual beat sensitivity.
is study explored sensitivity ratings of 10 high- and 10
low-groove songs. We selected a subset of songs that were
exemplars of high and low-groove music based on previous work
(Janata etal., 2012; Stupacher etal., 2013) while also considering
the time needed to obtain good data in an online study without
participant fatigue. is design did not allow us for time to include
songs that have been previously rated as “mid-groove.” Future
research should consider using a wider range of songs that capture
high-, mid-, and low-groove music to obtain a more inclusive
landscape of dierent musical genres and preferences to see how
personal experiences and predilections can inuence perceptions
of songs with moderate groove.
Conclusion
e present study investigated the inuence of musical
sophistication, dance sophistication, and musical perceptual
abilities on musical groove perception. Wefound that perceptual
abilities, musical training, and social dancing are signicant
predictors of rating dierences between high-and low-groove
music. Overall, our results indicate that the experience of groove
may not besolely dependent on the way the music is written or
performed but also shaped by listeners’ individual experiences and
predispositions. Results from this investigation may help develop
more objective assessments of dance skills that can measure dance
ability in a wide array of individuals. Clinical implications of this
research may help with the development of musical therapeutic
tools for those diagnosed with movement impairments (e.g.,
Parkinson’s disease; Nombela etal., 2013; Krotinger and Loui,
2021) or developmental disorders (e.g., ADHD; Puyjarinet etal.,
2017), who have a harder time moving to the beat compared to
healthy and typically developing individuals, respectively.
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 13 frontiersin.org
Data availability statement
e datasets presented in this study can befound in online
repositories. e names of the repository/repositories and
accession number(s) can befound at: https://osf.io/g3y7c/.
Ethics statement
e studies involving human participants were reviewed and
approved by the University of Nevada, Las Vegas Institutional
Review Board. e participants provided their written informed
consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
SO’C, EH, and JS: conceptualization. SO’C, JN-B, and JS:
methodology. SO’C: formal analysis and data collection. SO’C and
GW: investigation setup. SO’C and JN-B: writing—original dra
preparation. SO’C, JN-B, EH, and JS: writing—review and editing. All
authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
is research was supported in part by the University of
Nevada, Las Vegas Foundation Board of Trustees Fellowship
awarded to SO’C. e publication fees for this article were
supported by the UNLV MSI Open Article Fund.
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank Jennifer Rennels and
Diego Vega for their expertise and feedback on writing
and methods. They also wish to thank Kris Gunawan,
William Blake Ridgway, and Kindy Insouvanh for their
expertise and guidance in the data analytic plan. Additionally,
the authors wish to thank Hannah Strauss for her assistance
in setting up the online version of the Profile for
Music Perception Skills. Some of the content of this manuscript
has previously appeared online in SO’C dissertation
(O’Connell, 2021).
Conflict of interest
e authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could
beconstrued as a potential conict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those
of the authors and do not necessarily represent those
of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher,
the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be
evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by
its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by
the publisher.
References
Audacity Team (2021). Audacity(R): Free Audio Editor and Recorder [Computer
application]. Version 3.0.0 retrieved October 16th, 2020 from https://audacityteam.org/
Balkwill, L.-L., and ompson, W. F. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of the
perception of emotion in music: Psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Percept.
17, 43–64. doi: 10.2307/40285811
Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., et al.
(2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists:
evidence from fMRI conjunction. NeuroImage 30, 917–926. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2005.10.044
Berliner, P. F. (1994). Berliner, P. F. (1994). inking in Jazz: e Innite Art of
Improvisation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bernardi, N. F., Bellemare-Pepin, A., and Peretz, I. (2017). Enhancement of
pleasure during spontaneous dance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/
fnhum.2017.00572
Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music
correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898
Bowling, D. L., Ancochea, P. G., Hove, M. J., and Tecumseh Fitch, W. (2019).
Pupillometry of groove: evidence for noradrenergic arousal in the link between
music and movement. Front. Neurosci. 12, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.01039
Buhmann, J., Desmet, F., Moens, B., Van Dyck, E., and Leman, M. (2016).
Spontaneous velocity eect of musical expression on self-paced walking. PLoS One
11, 1–19. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154414
Burger, B., ompson, M. R., Luck, G., Saarikallio, S., and Toiviainen, P. (2012).
Music moves us: beat-related musical features inuence regularity of music-induced
movement. in Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Music Perception
and Cognition. eds. E. Cambouropolos, C. Tsougras, P. Mavromatic, and K. Pastiadis
(essaloniki, Greece), 183–187.
Buttereld, M. (2010a). Participatory discrepancies and the perception of beats in
jazz. Music. Percept. 27, 157–176. doi: 10.1525/MP.2010.27.3.157
Buttereld, M. W. (2010b). Race and rhythm: the social component of the swing
groove. Jazz Perspect. 4, 301–335. doi: 10.1080/17494060.2010.561089
Chemin, B., Mouraux, A., and Nozaradan, S. (2014). Body movement selectively
shapes the neural representation of musical rhythms. Psychol. Sci. 25, 2147–2159.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614551161
Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., and Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony
increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193
Clayton, M. (2000). Time in Indian Music: Rhythm, Metre, and Form in North
Indian rāg Performance. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Colver, M. C., and El-Alayli, A. (2015). Getting aesthetic chills from music: the
connection between openness to experience and frisson. Psychol. Music 44, 413–427.
doi: 10.1177/0305735615572358
Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., and Misura, N. M. (2013). Music training,
cognition, and personality. Front. Psychol. 4, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222
Danielsen, A. (2006). Presence and Pleasure: e Funk Grooves of James Brown and
Parliament. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.
Danielsen, A., Otnæss, M. K., Jensen, J., Williams, S. C. R., and Østberg, B. C.
(2014). Investigating repetition and change in musical rhythm by functional MRI.
Neuroscience 275, 469–476. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.029
Davies, M., Madison, G., Silva, P., and Gouyon, F. (2013). e eect of microtiming
deviations on the perception of groove in short rhythms. Music Percept. 30, 497–510.
doi: 10.1525/mp.2013.30.5.497
De Bruyn, L., Leman, M., Moelants, D., and Demey, M. (2009). Does social
interaction activate music listeners? Lecture notes in computer science (including
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 14 frontiersin.org
subseries lecture notes in articial intelligence and lecture notes in bioinformatics)
5493 LNCS, 93–106.
Drake, C., Penel, A., and Bigand, E. (2000). Tapping in time with mechanically
and expressively performed music. Music Percept. 18, 1–23. doi: 10.2307/
40285899
Edworthy, J., and Waring, H. (2006). e eects of music tempo and loudness level
on treadmill exercise. Ergonomics 49, 1597–1610. doi: 10.1080/00140130600899104
Etani, T., Marui, A., Kawase, S., and Keller, P. E. (2018). Optimal tempo for groove:
its relation to directions of body movement and Japanese nori. Front. Psychol. 9,
1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00462
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power
analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res.
Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Fitch, W. T. (2016). Dance, music, meter and groove: a forgotten partnership.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00064
Frühauf, J., Kopiez, R., and Platz, F. (2013). Music on the timing grid: the inuence
of microtiming on the perceived groove quality of a simple drum pattern
performance. Music. Sci. 17, 246–260. doi: 10.1177/1029864913486793
Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Large, E. W., and Ross, B. (2009). Beta and gamma
rhythms in human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Ann. N. Y. Acad.
Sci. 1169, 89–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04779.x
Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., L arge, E. W., and Ross, B. (2012). Internalized timing of
isochronous sounds is represented in neuromagnetic beta oscillations. J. Neurosci.
32, 1791–1802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4107-11.2012
Fukuie, T., Suwabe, K., Kawase, S., Shimizu, T., Ochi, G., Kuwamizu, R., et al.
(2022). Groove rhythm stimulates prefrontal cortex function in groove enjoyers. Sci.
Rep. 12:7377. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-11324-3
Gordon, C. L., Cobb, P. R., and Balasubramaniam, R. (2018). Recruitment of the
motor system during music listening: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI data. PLoS One
13:e0207213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207213
Grahn, J. A., and Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of
the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 893–906. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893
Grahn, J. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal
interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. J. Neurosci. 29,
7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009
Grahn, J. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2013). Finding and feeling the musical beat: striatal
dissociations between detection and prediction of regularity. Cereb. Cortex 23,
913–921. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs083
Habibi, A., Cahn, B. R., Damasio, A., and Damasio, H. (2016). Neural correlates
of accelerated auditory processing in children engaged in music training. Dev. Cogn.
Neurosci. 21, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.04.003
Hallam, S. (2010). 21St century conceptions of musical ability. Psychol. Music 38,
308–330. doi: 10.1177/0305735609351922
Hallam, S., and Prince, V. (2003). Conceptions of musical ability. Res. Stud. Music
Educ. 20, 2–22. doi: 10.1177/1321103X030200010101
Hill, C. V. (2010). Tap Dancing America: A Cultural History. 1st Edn. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Hofmann, A., Wesolowski, B. C., and Goebl, W. (2017). e tight-interlocked
rhythm section: production and perception of synchronisation in jazz trio
performance. J. New Music Res. 46, 329–341. doi: 10.1080/09298215.2017.1355394
Hosken, F. (2021). e pocket: a theory of beats as domains. Available at https://
www.proquest.com/docview/2572567017?pq-origsite=gscholar&fromopenview=tr
ue. Accessed September 19, 2022].
Hove, M. J., Vuust, P., and Stupacher, J. (2019). Increased levels of bass in popular
music recordings 1955–2016 and their relation to loudness. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 145,
2247–2253. doi: 10.1121/1.5097587
Hughes, G. (2001). e Oxford companion to jazz. Ref. Rev. 15:39. doi: 10.1108/
rr.2001.15.4.39.236
Hurley, B. K., Martens, P. A., and Janata, P. (2014). Spontaneous sensorimotor
coupling with multipart music. J. E xp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40,
1679–1696. doi: 10.1037/a0037154
Iyer, V. (2002). Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming
in African-American music. Music Percept. 19, 387–414. doi: 10.1525/
mp.2002.19.3.387
Janata, P., Tomic, S. T., and Haberman, J. M. (2012). Sensorimotor coupling in
music and the psychology of the groove. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 54–75. doi:
10.1037/a0024208
Jola, C., Abedian-Amiri, A., Kuppuswamy, A., Pollick, F. E., and Grosbras, M. H.
(2012). Motor simulation without motor expertise: enhanced corticospinal
excitability in visually experienced dance spectators. PLoS One 7:e33343. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0033343
Josef, L., Goldstein, P., Mayseless, N., Ayalon, L., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2019).
e oxytocinergic system mediates synchronized interpersonal movement during
dance. Sci. Rep. 9, 1894–1898. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37141-1
Karageorghis, C. I., and Terry, P. C. (1997). e psychophysical eects of music in
sport and exercise: a review. J. Sport Behav. 20, 54–68.
Karpati, F. J., Giacosa, C., Foster, N. E. V., Penhune, V. B., and Hyde, K. L. (2016).
Sensorimotor integration is enhanced in dancers and musicians. Exp. Brain Res. 234,
893–903. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4524-1
Karpati, F. J., Giacosa, C., Foster, N. E. V., Penhune, V. B., and Hyde, K. L. (2017).
Dance and music share gray matter structural correlates. Brain Res. 1657, 62–73.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.11.029
Keller, P., and Schubert, E. (2011). Cognitive and aective judgements of
syncopated musical themes. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 142–156. doi: 10.2478/
v10053-008-0094-0
Kilchenmann, L., and Senn, O. (2015). Microtiming in swing and funk aects the
body movement behavior of music expert listeners. Front. Psychol. 6, 1–14. doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01232
Kokal, I., Engel, A., Kirschner, S., and Keysers, C. (2011). Synchronized drumming
enhances activity in the caudate and facilitates prosocial commitment–if the rhythm
comes easily. PLoS One 6, 1–12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027272
Kornysheva, K., Von Cramon, D. Y., Jacobsen, T., and Schubotz, R. I. (2010).
Tuning-in to the beat: aesthetic appreciation of musical rhythms correlates with a
premotor activity boost. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31:64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20844
Kowalewski, D. A., Kratzer, T. M., and Freidman, R. S. (2020). Social music:
investigating the link between personal liking and perceived groove. Music. Percept.
37, 339–346. doi: 10.1525/mp.2020.37.4.339
Kraus, N., and Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music t raining for the development of
auditory skills. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 599–605. doi: 10.1038/nrn2882
Kraus, N., Slater, J., ompson, E. C., Hornickel, J., Strait, D. L., Nicol, T., et al.
(2014). Music enrichment programs improve the neural encoding of speech in at-
risk children. J. Neurosci. 34, 11913–11918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1881-
14.2014
Krotinger, A., and Loui, P. (2021). Rhythm and groove as cognitive mechanisms
of dance intervention in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One 16:e0249933. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0249933
Kuckelkorn, K. L., de Manzano, Ö., and Ullén, F. (2021). Musical expertise and
personality–dierences related to occupational choice and instrument categories.
Pers. Individ. Dif. 173:110573. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110573
Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C., Neter, J., and Li, W. (2005). Applied linear statistical
models (5th ed.) 408.
Law, L. N. C., and Zentner, M. (2012). Assessing musical abilities objectively:
construction and validation of the prole of music perception skills. PLoS One
7:e52508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052508
Lee, K. M., Barrett, K. C., Kim, Y., Lim, Y., and Lee, K. (2015). Dance and music
in “gangnam style”: how dance observation aects meter perception. PLoS One 10,
1–19. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134725
Leman, M. (2012). “Musical gestures and embodied cognition,” in Journèes
d’Informatique Musicale. eds. T. Dutoit, T. Todoro and N. d’Alessandro (Mons:
University of Mons), 5–7.
Leow, L.-A., Parrott, T., and Grahn, J. A. (2014). Individual dierences in beat
perception aect gait responses to low-and high-groove music. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 8, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00811
Leow, L. A., Watson, S., Prete, D., Waclawik, K., and Grahn, J. A. (2021). How
groove in music aects gait. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 2419–2433. doi: 10.1007/
s00221-021-06083-y
Liu, Y., Liu, G., Wei, D., Li, Q., Yuan, G., Wu, S., et al. (2018). Eects of musical
tempo on musicians’ and non-musicians’ emotional experience when listening to
music. Front. Psychol. 9, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02118
Luck, G., Saarikallio, S., Burger, B., ompson, M. R., and Toiviainen, P. (2010).
Eects of the big ve and musical genre on music-induced movement. J. Res. Pers.
44, 714–720. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.10.001
Luo, C., Guo, Z., Lai, Y., Liao, W., Liu, Q., Kendrick, K. M., et al. (2012). Musical
training induces functional plasticity in perceptual and motor networks. PLoS One
7, 1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036568
MacDougall, H. G., and Moore, S. T. (2005). Marching to the beat of the same
drummer: the spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. J. Appl. Physiol. 99,
1164–1173. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00138.2005
Mackinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., and Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the
mediation, confounding and suppression eect. Prev. Sci. 1, 173–181. doi:
10.1023/a:1026595011371
Madison, G. (2006). Experiencing groove induced by music: consistency and
phenomenology. Music. Percept. 24, 201–208. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.201
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 15 frontiersin.org
Madison, G., Gouyon, F., Ullén, F., Hörnström, K., and Umeå, P. (2011). Modeling
the tendency for music to induce movement in humans: rst correlations with low-
level audio descriptors across music genres. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
37, 1578–1594. doi: 10.1037/a0024323
Madison, G., and Schiölde, G. (2017). Repeated listening increases the liking for
music regardless of its complexity: implications for the appreciation and aesthetics
of music. Front. Neurosci. 11, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00147
Madison, G., and Sioros, G. (2014). What musicians do to induce the sensation of
groove in simple and complex melodies, and how listeners perceive it. Front. Psychol.
5, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00894
Mankel, K., and Bidelman, G. M. (2018). Inherent auditory skills rather than
formal music training shape the neural encoding of speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.
S. A. 115, 13129–13134. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811793115
Martín-Fernández, J., Burunat, I., Modroño, C., González-Mora, J. L., and
Plata-Bello, J. (2021). Music style not only modulates the auditory cortex, but also motor
related areas. Neuroscience 457, 88–102. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.01.012
Matthews, T. E., Witek, M. A. G., Heggli, O. A., Penhune, V. B., and Vuust, P.
(2019). e sensation of groove is aected by the interaction of rhythmic and
harmonic complexity. PLoS One 14, e0204539–e0204517. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pone.0204539
Matthews, T. E., Witek, M. A. G., Lund, T., Vuust, P., and Penhune, V. B. (2020).
e sensation of groove engages motor and reward networks. NeuroImage
214:116768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116768
McCrae, R. R. (2007). Aesthetic chills as a universal marker of openness to
experience. Motiv. Emot. 31, 5–11. doi: 10.1007/s11031-007-9053-1
Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., York, H., Glowacki, L., and Krasnow, M. M. (2018). Form
and function in human song. Curr. Biol. 28, 356–368.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.
cub.2017.12.042
Menon, V., and Levitin, D. J. (2005). e rewards of music listening: response and
physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. NeuroImage 28, 175–184. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053
Merker, B. (2014). Groove or swing as distributed rhythmic consonance:
introducing the groove matrix. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–4. doi: 10.3389/
fnhum.2014.00454
Michaelis, K., Wiener, M., and ompson, J. C. (2014). Passive listening to
preferred motor tempo modulates corticospinal excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci.
8:252. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00252
Müllensiefen, D., Gingras, B., Musil, J., and Stewart, L. (2014). e musicality of
non-musicians: an index for assessing musical sophistication in the general
population. PLoS One 9:e89642. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089642
Müllensiefen, D., Gingras, B., Stewart, L., and Musil, J. J. (2013). Goldsmiths
musical sophistication index (gold-MSI) v1.0 technical report and documentation
revision 0.3. Tech. Rep., 1–69.
Nave-Blodgett, J. E., Snyder, J. S., and Hannon, E. E. (2021a). Auditory superiority
for perceiving the beat level but not measure level in music. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Percept. Perform. 47, 1516–1542. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000954
Nave-Blodgett, J. E., Snyder, J. S., and Hannon, E. E. (2021b). Hierarchical beat
perception develops throughout childhood and adolescence and is enhanced in
ose with musical training. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 150, 314–339. doi: 10.1037/
xge0000903
Nguyen, T., Sidhu, R. K., Everling, J. C., Wickett, M. C., Gibbings, A., and
Grahn, J. A. (2022). Beat perception and production in musicians and dancers.
Music. Percept. 39, 229–248. doi: 10.1525/MP.2022.39.3.229
Nombela, C., Hughes, L. E., Owen, A. M., and Grahn, J. A. (2013). Into the groove:
can rhythm inuence Parkinson’s disease? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2564–2570.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.003
Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Hodges, D. A., and
Kwapil, T. R. (2014). Listening between the notes: aesthetic chills in everyday music
listening. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 8, 104–109. doi: 10.1037/a0034867
O’Connell, S. R. (2021). Exploring the relation between musical and dance
sophistication and musical groove perception. Available at https://digitalscholarship.
unlv.edu/thesesdissertations/4257. Accessed February 23, 2022.
Pandey, S., and Elliott, W. (2010). Suppressor variables in social work research:
ways to identify in multiple regression models. J. Soc. Social Work Res. 1, 28–40. doi:
10.5243/jsswr.2010.2
Pasinski, A. C., Hannon, E. E., and Snyder, J. S. (2016). How musical are music video
game players? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1553–1558. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0998-x
Patel, A. D., and Iversen, J. R. (2014). e evolutionary neuroscience of musical
beat perception: the action simulation for auditory prediction (ASAP) hypothesis.
Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057
Peretz, I., Cummings, S., and Dubé, M. P. (2007). e genetics of congenital
amusia (tone deafness): a family-aggregation study. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 81, 582–588.
doi: 10.1086/521337
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2006). Hearing what the body feels: auditory
encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105, 533–546. doi: 10.1016/j.
cognition.2006.11.006
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: auditory
encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105, 533–546. doi: 10.1016/j.
cognition.2006.11.006
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2008). Vestibular influence on auditory
metrical interpretation. Brain Cogn. 67, 94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.
2007.11.007
Poikonen, H., Toiviainen, P., and Tervaniemi, M. (2018). Naturalistic music and
dance: cortical phase synchrony in musicians and dancers. PLoS One 13, e0196065–
e0196018. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0196065
Pressing, J. (2002). Black Atlantic rhythm: its computational and transcultural
foundations. Music Percept. 19, 285–310. doi: 10.1525/gfc.2008.8.4.24.is
Puyjarinet, F., Bégel, V., Lopez, R., Dellacherie, D., and Dalla Bella, S. (2017).
Children and adults with attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder cannot move to
the beat. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11295-w
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R
Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/
Rendi, M., Szabo, A., and Szabó, T. (2016). Performance enhancement with music
in rowing sprint. Sport Psychol. 22, 175–182. doi: 10.1123/tsp.22.2.175
Reybrouck, M., Podlipniak, P., and Welch, D. (2019). Music and noise: same or
dierent? What our body tells us. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/
fpsyg.2019.01153
Rose, D., Müllensiefen, D., Lovatt, P., and Orgs, G. (2020). e goldsmiths dance
sophistication index (gold-DSI): a psychometric tool to assess individual dierences
in dance experience. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 1–13. doi: 10.1037/aca0000340
Ross, J. M., Warlaumont, A. S., Abney, D. H., Rigoli, L. M., and
Balasubramaniam, R. (2016). Inuence of musical groove on postural sway. J. E xp.
Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 308–319. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000198
Salimpoor, V. N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., Dagher, A.,
and Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory
cortices predict music reward value. Science 340, 216–219. doi: 10.1126/
science.1231059
Schellenberg, E. G. (2015). Music training and speech perception: a gene-environment
interaction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 170–177. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12627
Senn, O., Bechtold, T. A., Hoesl, F., and Kilchenmann, L. (2021a). Taste and
familiarity aect the experience of groove in popular music. Music. Sci. 25, 45–66.
doi: 10.1177/1029864919839172
Senn, O., Bullerjahn, C., Kilchenmann, L., and von Georgi, R. (2017). Rhythmic
density aects listeners’ emotional response to microtiming. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–21.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01709
Senn, O., Kilchenmann, L., Bechtold, T., and Hoesl, F. (2018). Groove in drum
patterns as a function of both rhythmic properties and listeners’ attitudes. PLoS One
13, e0199604–e0199633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199604
Senn, O., Kilchenmann, L., von Georgi, R., and Bullerjahn, C. (2016). e eect
of expert performance microtiming on listeners’ experience of groove in swing or
funk music. Front. Psychol. 7, 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01487
Senn, O., Rose, D., Bechtold, T., Kilchenmann, L., Hoesl, F., Jerjen, R., et al.
(2019b). Preliminaries to a psychological model of musical groove. Front. Psychol.
10, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01228
Sioros, G., Miron, M., Davies, M., Gouyon, F., and Madison, G. (2014).
Syncopation creates the sensation of groove in synthesized music examples. Front.
Psychol. 5, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01036
Skoe, E., and Kraus, N. (2010). Auditory brain stem response to complex sounds:
a tutorial. Ear Hear. 31, 302–324. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181cdb272
Slater, J., and Kraus, N. (2016). e role of rhythm in perceiving speech in noise:
a comparison of percussionists, vocalists and non-musicians. Cogn. Process. 17,
79–87. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0740-7
Slater, J., Skoe, E., Strait, D. L., O’Connell, S., omps on, E., and Kraus, N. (2015).
Music training improves speech-in-noise perception: longitudinal evidence from a
community-based music program. Behav. Brain Res. 291, 244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.
bbr.2015.05.026
Snyder, J. S., and Large, E. W. (2005). Gamma-band activity reects the metric
structure of rhythmic tone sequences. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 117–126. doi:
10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.014
Strait, D. L., Chan, K., Ashley, R., and Kraus, N. (2012). Specialization among the
specialized: auditory brainstem function is tuned in to timbre. Cortex 48, 360–362.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.03.015
Strait, D. L., and Kraus, N. (2011). Playing music for a smarter ear: cognitive,
perceptual and neurobiological evidence. Music. Percept. 29, 133–146. doi: 10.1525/
MP.2011.29.2.133.Playing
O’Connell et al. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321
Frontiers in Psychology 16 frontiersin.org
Strait, D. L., O’Connell, S. R., Parbery-Clark, A., and Kraus, N. (2014). Musicians’
enhanced neural dierentiation of speech sounds arises early in life: developmental
evidence from ages 3 to 30. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2512–2521. doi: 10.1093/cercor/
bht103
Strait, D. L., Slater, J., O’Connell, S., and Kraus, N. (2015). Music training relates
to the development of neural mechanisms of selective auditory attention. Dev. Cogn.
Neurosci. 12, 94–104. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.01.001
Stupacher, J., Hove, M. J., and Janata, P. (2016a). Audio features underlying
perceived groove and sensorimotor synchronication in music. Music. Percept. 33,
571–589. doi: 10.1525/mp.2016.33.5.571
Stupacher, J., Hove, M. J., Novembre, G., Schütz-Bosbach, S., and Keller, P. E.
(2013). Musical groove modulates motor cortex excitability: a TMS investigation.
Brain Cogn. 82, 127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.03.003
Stupacher, J., Maes, P. J., Witte, M., and Wood, G. (2017a). Music strengthens
prosocial eects of interpersonal synchronization – if youmove in time with the
beat. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.007
Stupacher, J., Witte, M., and Wood, G. (2016b). Social eects of interpersonal
synchronization during listening to music compared to a metronome: What can
welearn from implicit measures? in Proceedings of the 9th International Conference
of Students of Systematic Musicology (SysMus16).
Stupacher, J., Witte, M., and Wood, G. (2017b). Go with the ow: subjective
uency of performance is associated with sensorimotor synchronization accuracy
and stability. in Proceedings of the 25th Anniversary Conference of the European
Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 163–166.
Stupacher, J., Wood, G., and Witte, M. (2017c). Neural entrainment to
polyrhythms: a comparison of musicians and non-musicians. Front. Neurosci. 11,
1–17. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00208
Styns, F., van Noorden, L., Moelants, D., and Leman, M. (2007). Walking on
music. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 769–785. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.007
Swaminathan, S., and Schellenberg, E. G. (2018). Musical competence is predicted
by music training, cognitive abilities, and personality. Sci. Rep. 8:9223. doi: 10.1038/
s41598-018-27571-2
Tan, Y. T., McPherson, G. E., Peretz, I., Berkovic, S. F., and Wilson, S. J. (2014). e
genetic basis of music ability. Front. Psychol. 5, 1–19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00658
Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., and Dunbar, R. (2015). Synchrony and exertion
during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding.
Biol. Lett. 11, 20150767–20150764. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767
Tarr, B., Launay, J., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Music and social bonding: self-
other merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 5, 1–10. doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096
Temperley, D. (1999). Syncopation in rock: a perceptual perspective. Pop. Musi c
18, 19–40. doi: 10.1017/S0261143000008710
Todd, N. P. M., and Lee, C. S. (2015). e sensory-motor theory of rhythm and
beat induction 20 years on: a new synthesis and future perspectives. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 9, 1–25. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00444
Trainor, L. J., Gao, X., L ei, J.-J., Lehtovaara, K., and Harris, L. R. (2009). e primal
role of the vestibular system in determining musical rhythm. Cortex 45, 35–43. doi:
10.1016/j.cortex.2007.10.014
Trehub, S. E., Becker, J., and Morley, I. (2015). Cross-cultural perspectives on
music and musicality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140096. doi: 10.1098/
rstb.2014.0096
Venables, W. N., and Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern Applied Statistics With S.
4th Edn. New York, NY: Springer.
Wesolowski, B. C., and Hofmann, A. (2016). ere’s more to groove than bass in
electronic dance music: why some people won’t dance to techno. PLoS One 11, 1–23.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163938
Wilson, E. M. F., and Davey, N. J. (2002). Musical beat inuences corticospinal
drive to ankle exor and extensor muscles in man. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 177–184.
doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00203-3
Witek, M. A. G. (2017). Filling in: syncopation, pleasure and distributed embodiment
in groove. Music. Anal. 36, 138–160. doi: 10.1111/musa.12082
Witek, M. A. G., and Clarke, E. F. (2014). Eects of polyphonic context,
instrumentation, and metrical location on syncopation in music. Music. Percept. 32,
201–217. doi: 10.1525/mp.2014.32.2.201
Witek, M. A. G., Clarke, E. F., Wallentin, M., Kringelbach, M. L., and Vuust, P.
(2014). Syncopation, body-movement and pleasure in groove music. PLoS One
9:e94446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094446
Witek, M. A. G., Popescu, T., Clarke, E. F., Hansen, M., Konvalinka, I.,
Kringelbach, M. L., et al. (2017). Syncopation aects free body-movement in musical
groove. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 995–1005. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4855-6
Woods, K. J. P., Siegel, M. H., Traer, J., and McDermott, J. H. (2017). Headphone
screening to facilitate web-based auditory experiments. Atten. Percept. Psychophys.
79, 2064–2072. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1361-2
Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning:
neural correlates and implications. Science 342, 585–589. doi: 10.1126/
science.1238414
Zatorre, R. J. (2015). Musical pleasure and reward: mechanisms and dysfunction.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 202–211. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12677
Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music:
auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
8, 547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152
Zentner, M., and Strauss, H. (2017). Assessing musical ability quickly and
objectively: development and validation of the short-PROMS and the mini-PROMS.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1400, 33–45. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13410
... They also noted that inconsistencies in existing groove research could stem from varying methodologies, musical repertoires, and participants' cultural backgrounds. For example, groove perception may be shaped by individual differences in dance experience (O'Connell et al., 2022). ...
... Dance experience, in particular, is rarely assessed in research on music perception. Yet, in keeping with more recent work, our study suggests that the urge to dance, social dancing, and body awareness may be important predictors of groove experience (Foster Vander Elst et al., 2021;Nave-Blodgett et al., 2021;O'Connell et al., 2022). However, given that we did not include covariates in our power analysis, our findings are exploratory. ...
Article
Full-text available
Studies demonstrate that low frequencies and syncopation can enhance groove—the pleasurable urge to move to music. This study examined the simultaneous effect of low-frequency amplitude and syncopation on groove by manipulating basslines in house music, a subgenre of electronic dance music (EDM). One hundred and seventy-nine participants listened to 20 novel house music clips in which basslines were manipulated across two levels of low-frequency amplitude and syncopation. Music and dance-related experience, as well as genre preferences, were also assessed. Groove perception was most pronounced for house tracks combining high low-frequency amplitude (LFA) and high syncopation, and least pronounced for tracks with low LFA, irrespective of syncopation. Exploratory correlation analysis revealed that groove perception is influenced by listeners’ preferences for energetic and rhythmic music styles, their urge to dance, and their propensity to experience an emotional connection to music. Our findings reveal that the urge to move when listening to music is shaped by the interplay of rhythmic complexity and sonic texture, and is influenced by dance and music experiences and preferences.
... • Musical training: Musical training subscale of the English-language Goldsmiths Music Sophistication Index [40] with seven items and a range of [7,49]. ...
... Similarly, people who generally feel compelled to dance (DSI, urge to dance scale) also felt more interest, pleasure, energy and urge to move in response to this study's stimuli, compared to people who in general are not eager to dance. This result is in agreement with Witek et al. [4], but it contrasts with O'Connell et al. [49], where no effect of the urge to dance scale on listeners' sensitivity to groove was found. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is a broad consensus in groove research that the experience of groove, understood as a pleasurable urge to move in response to music, is to some extent related to the complexity of the rhythm. Specifically, music with medium rhythmic complexity has been found to motivate greater urge to move compared to low or high complexity music (inverted-U hypothesis). Studies that confirmed the inverted-U hypothesis usually based their measure of complexity on the rhythmic phenomenon of syncopation, where rhythms with more and/or stronger syncopation are considered to be more complex than less syncopated rhythms. However, syncopation is not the same as complexity and represents only one rhythmic device that makes music complex. This study attempts the verification of the inverted-U hypothesis independently from syncopation. It uses a new stimulus set of forty idiomatic popular music drum patterns whose perceptual complexity was measured experimentally in a previous study. The current study reports the results of a listening experiment with n = 179 participants, in which the inverted-U hypothesis was not confirmed. Complexity did not have any significant effect on listeners’ urge to move (p = 834). Results are discussed in the context of the psychological model of musical groove, which offers a nuance to this null result: simple drum patterns motivate listeners to dance because they convey metric clarity; complex patterns invite dancing because they are interesting. Yet, overall, the urge to move does not seem to depend on complexity, at least in the case of idiomatic drum patterns that are typically encountered in the Western popular music repertoire.
Thesis
Full-text available
Bu araştırmanın temel amacı, Türkiye'de genel nüfusun müzik yeteneği kavramına ilişkin görüşleri ile müzikal sofistikasyon seviyeleri arasındaki ilişkiyi belirlemektir. Bu temel amaç doğrultusunda ilişkisel tarama modeli kullanılan araştırmada, 1278 katılımcıdan veri toplanmıştır. Veri toplama aracı olarak, katılımcıların demografik özelliklerini öğrenmek üzere Kişisel Bilgi Formu, Müzik Yeteneği Algısı Anketi ve bu çalışma kapsamında Türkçeye uyarlanarak geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışmaları yapılan Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI) kullanılmıştır. Türkçeye uyarlaması yapılan Gold-MSI'ın geçerlik ve güvenirlik analizleri sonucunda (n=118); Gold-MSI-TR envanterinin alfa katsayıları, Gutmann λ6 güvenirlik katsayıları, test-tekrar test korelasyon katsayıları oldukça yüksek iç tutarlılık göstermiştir. Envanterin tüm alt ölçekleri arasında ve genel müzikal sofistikasyon ile pozitif yönde anlamlı ve orta ile çok güçlü düzeyde ilişki olduğu bulunmuştur. Doğrulayıcı faktör analizi 2 model üzerinde incelenmiş ve sonuçlara göre en iyi uyum gösteren (5 ölçek + Genel Müzikal Sofistikasyon) model tercih edilmiştir. Araştırmada, katılımcıların müzik yeteneği algısı ve müzikal sofistikasyon seviyeleri; cinsiyet, yaş, meslek, eğitim durumu, sosyo-ekonomik statü (SES), doğdukları bölge, yaşadıkları bölge, müzik aleti çalma ve müzikle olan ilişkileri gibi demografik değişkenler açısından da incelenmiştir. Türkiye'de genel nüfusun müzik yeteneği kavramına ilişkin görüşleri (n=1152) altı ana tema altında toplanmıştır. Ayrıca, katılımcıların müzik yeteneği kavramına ilişkin görüşleri cinsiyet, yaşadıkları bölge, müzik aleti çalma ve müzikle ilişkilerine göre farklılık göstermiştir. Gold-MSI-TR envanterine ilişkin puanlar incelendiğinde ise (n=1278), katılımcıların genel müzikal sofistikasyon seviyeleri 'orta üstü' olarak belirlenmiştir. Bununla birlikte; cinsiyet, yaş, meslek, eğitim durumu, SES, doğdukları bölge, müzikle ilişki ve müzik aleti çalma gibi demografik değişkenlere göre anlamlı farklılıklar olduğu sonucuna ulaşılmıştır. Duygular alt ölçeği dışındaki tüm alt ölçekler ve genel müzikal sofistikasyon seviyeleri ile müzik yeteneği algıları arasında anlamlı ilişkiler bulunmuştur. Müzik yeteneğine ilişkin görüşlerin, genel müzikal sofistikasyon seviyesi üzerinde anlamlı ve büyük etkilere sahip olduğu saptanmıştır.
Preprint
The groove experience has been defined as a pleasant sense of wanting to move along with the music. Tempo, the pacing of musical events affects our perception of melody, rhythm, motion, and emotion and as such it is considered a fundamental musical dimension. This study investigates how varying the tempo of popular music drum & bass patterns influences the groove experience in listeners. The experimental stimuli were based on five, drum & bass patterns sourced from western popular music (original tempi between 89-130 bpm). The sample-based reconstructions allowed for tempo manipulation without side-effects in the audio. Each pattern was presented to participants in three tempo variations (84%, 100%, and 116% tempo) the resulting stimuli had a tempo range between 75 and 151 bpm. Participants (n=81) rated the intensity of their groove experience for each of the 15 stimuli using the Experience of Groove Questionnaire (EGQ). Tempo was found to be positively correlated with both scales of the EGQ. Listeners found faster stimuli to be more movement-inducing and pleasurable than slow ones. Additionally, higher event density induced a greater urge to move. This casts a spotlight on the relationship between tempo and event density and suggests an important area of interest for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Interacting with music is a uniquely pleasurable activity that is ubiquitous across human cultures. Current theories suggest that a prominent driver of musical pleasure responses is the violation and confirmation of temporal predictions. For example, the pleasurable urge to move to music (PLUMM), which is associated with the broader concept of groove, is higher for moderately complex rhythms compared to simple and complex rhythms. This inverted U-shaped relation between PLUMM and rhythmic complexity is thought to result from a balance between predictability and uncertainty. That is, moderately complex rhythms lead to strongly weighted prediction errors which elicit an urge to move to reinforce the predictive model (i.e., the meter). However, the details of these processes and how they bring about positive affective responses are currently underspecified. We propose that the intrinsic motivation for learning progress drives PLUMM and informs the music humans choose to listen to, dance to, and create. Here, learning progress reflects the rate of prediction error minimization over time. Accordingly, reducible prediction errors signal the potential for learning progress, producing a pleasurable, curious state characterized by the mobilization of attentional and memory resources. We discuss this hypothesis in the context of current psychological and neuroscientific research on musical pleasure and PLUMM. We propose a theoretical neuroscientific model focusing on the roles of dopamine and norepinephrine within a feedback loop linking prediction-based learning, curiosity, and memory. This perspective provides testable predictions that will motivate future research to further illuminate the fundamental relation between predictions, movement, and reward.
Article
Full-text available
Hearing a groove rhythm (GR), which creates the sensation of wanting to move to the music, can also create feelings of pleasure and arousal in people, and it may enhance cognitive performance, as does exercise, by stimulating the prefrontal cortex. Here, we examined the hypothesis that GR enhances executive function (EF) by acting on the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (l-DLPFC) while also considering individual differences in psychological responses. Fifty-one participants underwent two conditions: 3 min of listening to GR or a white-noise metronome. Before and after listening, participants performed the Stroop task and were monitored for l-DLPFC activity with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Our results show that GR enhanced EF and l-DLPFC activity in participants who felt a greater groove sensation and a more feeling clear-headed after listening to GR. Further, these psychological responses predict the impact of GR on l-DLPFC activity and EF, suggesting that GR enhances EF via l-DLPFC activity when the psychological response to GR is enhanced.
Article
Full-text available
The ability to perceive and produce a beat is believed to be universal in humans, but individual ability varies. The current study examined four factors that may influence beat perception and production capacity: 1) expertise: music or dance, 2) training style: percussive or nonpercussive, 3) stimulus modality: auditory or visual, and 4) movement type: finger-tap or whole-body bounce. Experiment 1 examined how expertise and training style influenced beat perception and production performance using an auditory beat perception task and a finger-tapping beat production task. Experiment 2 used a similar sample with an audiovisual variant of the beat perception task, and a standing knee-bend (bounce) beat production task to assess whole-body movement. The data showed that: 1) musicians were more accurate in a finger-tapping beat synchronization task compared to dancers and controls, 2) training style did not significantly influence beat perception and production, 3) visual beat information did not benefit any group, and 4) beat synchronization in a full-body movement task was comparable for musicians and dancers; both groups outperformed controls. The current study suggests that the type of task and measured response interacts with expertise, and that expertise effects may be masked by selection of nonoptimal response types.
Article
Full-text available
Auditory perception of time is superior to visual perception, both for simple intervals and beat-based musical rhythms. To what extent does this auditory advantage characterize perception of different hierarchical levels of musical meter, and how is it related to lifelong experience with music? We paired musical excerpts with auditory and visual metronomes that matched or mismatched the musical meter at the beat level (faster) and measure level (slower) and obtained fit ratings from adults and children (5-10 years). Adults exhibited an auditory advantage in this task for the beat level, but not for the measure level. Children also displayed an auditory advantage that increased with age for the beat level. In both modalities, their overall sensitivity to beat increased with age, but they were not sensitive to measure-level matching at any age. More musical training was related to enhanced sensitivity in both auditory and visual modalities for measure-level matching in adults and beat-level matching in children. These findings provide evidence for auditory superiority of beat perception across development, and they suggest that beat and meter perception develop quite gradually and rely on lifelong acquisition of musical knowledge. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) is a gait intervention in which gait-disordered patients synchronise footsteps to music or metronome cues. Musical ‘groove’, the tendency of music to induce movement, has previously been shown to be associated with faster gait, however, why groove affects gait remains unclear. One mechanism by which groove may affect gait is that of beat salience: music that is higher in groove has more salient musical beats, and higher beat salience might reduce the cognitive demands of perceiving the beat and synchronizing footsteps to it. If groove’s effects on gait are driven primarily by the impact of beat salience on cognitive demands, then groove’s effects might only be present in contexts in which it is relevant to reduce cognitive demands. Such contexts could include task parameters that increase cognitive demands (such as the requirement to synchronise to the beat), or individual differences that may make synchronisation more cognitively demanding. Here, we examined whether high beat salience can account for the effects of high-groove music on gait. First, we increased the beat salience of low-groove music to be similar to that of high-groove music by embedding metronome beats in low and high-groove music. We examined whether low-groove music with high beat salience elicited similar effects on gait as high-groove music. Second, we examined the effect of removing the requirement to synchronise footsteps to the beat (i.e., allowing participants to walk freely with the music), which is thought to remove the cognitive demand of synchronizing movements to the beat. We tested two populations thought to be sensitive to the cognitive demands of synchronisation, weak beat-perceivers and older adults. We found that increasing the beat salience of low-groove music increased stride velocity, but strides were still slower than with high-groove music. Similarly, removing the requirement to synchronise elicited faster, less variable gait, and reduced bias for stability, but high-groove music still elicited faster strides than low-groove music. These findings suggest that beat salience contributes to groove’s effect on gait, but it does not fully account for it. Despite reducing task difficulty by equalizing beat salience and removing the requirement to synchronise, high-groove music still elicited faster, less variable gait. Therefore, other properties of groove also appear to play a role in groove’s effect on gait.
Article
Full-text available
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is associated with a loss of internal cueing systems, affecting rhythmic motor tasks such as walking and speech production. Music and dance encourage spontaneous rhythmic coupling between sensory and motor systems; this has inspired the development of dance programs for PD. Here we assessed the therapeutic outcome and some underlying cognitive mechanisms of dance classes for PD, as measured by neuropsychological assessments of disease severity as well as quantitative assessments of rhythmic ability and sensorimotor experience. We assessed prior music and dance experience, beat perception (Beat Alignment Test), sensorimotor coupling (tapping to high- and low-groove songs), and disease severity (Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale in PD individuals) before and after four months of weekly dance classes. PD individuals performed better on UPDRS after four months of weekly dance classes, suggesting efficacy of dance intervention. Greater post-intervention improvements in UPDRS were associated with the presence of prior dance experience and with more accurate sensorimotor coupling. Prior dance experience was additionally associated with enhanced sensorimotor coupling during tapping to both high-groove and low-groove songs. These results show that dance classes for PD improve both qualitative and quantitative assessments of disease symptoms. The association between these improvements and dance experience suggests that rhythmic motor training, a mechanism underlying dance training, impacts improvements in parkinsonian symptoms following a dance intervention.
Article
Full-text available
The neuroscience of music has recently attracted significant attention, but the effect of music style on the activation of auditory-motor regions has not been explored. The aim of the present study is to analyze the differences in brain activity during passive listening to non-vocal excerpts of four different music genres (classical, reggaeton, electronic and folk). A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment was performed. Twenty-eight participants with no musical training were included in the study. They had to passively listen to music excerpts of the above genres during fMRI acquisition. Imaging analysis was performed at the whole-brain-level and in auditory-motor regions of interest (ROIs). Furthermore, the musical competence of each participant was measured and its relationship with brain activity in the studied ROIs was analyzed. The whole brain analysis showed higher brain activity during reggaeton listening than the other music genres in auditory-related areas. The ROI-analysis showed that reggaeton led to higher activity not only in auditory related areas, but also in some motor related areas, mainly when it was compared with classical music. A positive relationship between the melodic-MET score and brain activity during reggaeton listening was identified in some auditory and motor related areas. The findings revealed that listening to different music styles in musically inexperienced subjects elicits different brain activity in auditory and motor related areas. Reggaeton was, among the studied music genres, the one that evoked the highest activity in the auditory-motor network. These findings are discussed in connection with acoustic analyses of the musical stimuli.
Article
Full-text available
Dance has become an important topic for research in empirical aesthetics, social and motor cognition, and as an intervention for neurodegenerative and neurodevelopmental disorders. Despite the growing scientific interest in dance, no standardized psychometric instrument exists to assess people’s dance experience. Here, we introduce the Goldsmiths Dance Sophistication Index (Gold-DSI), a 26-item questionnaire to measure individual differences in participatory and observational dance experience on a continuous scale. The Gold-DSI was developed in 3 stages: In the first stage, a set of 76 items was generated by adapting questions from the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Müllensiefen, Gingras, Musil, & Stewart, 2014) and as part of a stakeholder workshop using a grounded theory approach. The second stage focused on item reduction. Using a large-scale online survey (N = 424), hierarchical factor analysis was used to fit a model comprising of one general and six secondary factors (28 items in total). In Stage 3, six new items were added to specifically capture individual differences in dance observation. We then collected data from two samples for final model estimation (n = 127) and evaluation (n = 190). The final version of the Gold-DSI comprises 26 items; 20 items relate to 1 general factor that captures experience in dance participation. This includes 4 secondary factors: body awareness, social dancing, urge to dance, and dance training. A further 6 items separately measure experience in dance observation. In sum, the Gold-DSI provides a brief, standardized, and continuous assessment of doing, watching, and knowing about dance.
Article
Full-text available
Most music is temporally organized within a metrical hierarchy, having nested periodic patterns that give rise to the experience of stronger (downbeat) and weaker (upbeat) events. Musical meter presumably makes it possible to dance, sing, and play instruments in synchrony with others. It is nevertheless unclear whether or not listeners perceive multiple levels of periodicity simultaneously, and if they do, when and how they learn to do this. We tested children, adolescents, and musically trained and untrained adults with a new meter perception task. We presented excerpts of human-performed music paired with metronomes that matched or mismatched the metrical structure of the music at 2 hierarchical levels (beat and measure), and asked listeners to provide a rating of fit of metronome and music. Fit ratings suggested that adults with and without musical training were sensitive to both levels of meter simultaneously, but ratings were more strongly influenced by beat-level than by measure-level synchrony. Sensitivity to two simultaneous levels of meter was not evident in children or adolescents. Sensitivity to the beat alone was apparent in the youngest children and increased with age, whereas sensitivity to the measure alone was not present in younger children (5- to 8-year-olds). These findings suggest a prolonged period of development and refinement of hierarchical beat perception and surprisingly weak overall ability to attend to 2 beat levels at the same time across all ages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
The sensation of groove has been defined as the pleasurable desire to move to music, suggesting that both motor timing and reward processes are involved in this experience. Although many studies have investigated rhythmic timing and musical reward separately, none have examined whether the associated cortical and subcortical networks are engaged while participants listen to groove-based music. In the current study, musicians and non-musicians listened to and rated experimentally controlled groove-based stimuli while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging. Medium complexity rhythms elicited higher ratings of pleasure and wanting to move and were associated with activity in regions linked to beat perception and reward, as well as prefrontal and parietal regions implicated in generating and updating stimuli-based expectations. Activity in basal ganglia regions of interest, including the nucleus accumbens, caudate and putamen, was associated with ratings of pleasure and wanting to move, supporting their important role in the sensation of groove. We propose a model in which different cortico-striatal circuits interact to support the mechanisms underlying groove, including internal generation of the beat, beat-based expectations, and expectation-based affect. These results show that the sensation of groove is supported by motor and reward networks in the brain and, along with our proposed model, suggest that the basal ganglia are crucial nodes in networks that interact to generate this powerful response to music.
Article
Several studies have documented personality differences between musicians and non-musicians, and there have also been reports of personality differences between musicians playing different instruments. However, the samples have been small and findings are often inconsistent between studies. Here, we investigated Big Five personality differences between professional musicians, amateur musicians, and non-musicians in more than 7000 participants, as well as differences related to instrument categories. We had two specific hypotheses, i.e. that musicians would have higher openness than non-musicians, and that singers would be more extraverted than instrumentalists. Indeed, we found large group differences in openness, with professionals scoring higher than amateurs, who scored higher than non-musicians. Furthermore, singers were, as predicted, higher on extraver-sion than instrumentalists among both professionals and amateurs. We also found that professionals had higher neuroticism, lower agreeableness, and lower conscientiousness than amateurs. For professionals and amateurs, there were personality differences between instruments, but the patterns were inconsistent. This suggests that the differences were not primarily related to instrument choice per se, but instead possibly moderated by other factors such as musical genre and the social context of music making in each group.