ArticlePDF Available

“I want to fit in… but I don’t want to change myself fundamentally”: A qualitative exploration of the relationship between masking and mental health for autistic teenagers

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background Previous research has identified an association between masking and mental health for autistic people. However, the direction of causality and mechanisms involved in this relationship are not well understood. This qualitative study aimed to investigate autistic teenagers’ experiences of masking, mental health and how the two develop and interact. Methods Twenty autistic teenagers took part in a semi-structured interview. The interviews were analysed using Reflexive Thematic Analysis. Results From the analysis, one theme was identified to conceptualise masking as described by participants. Five more inter-related themes were identified, each involved both in the relationship between masking and mental health and conversely in the relationship between authenticity and mental health. Participants described how masking and mental health both influence each other, and both are influenced by social and environmental factors. Conclusions The findings are consistent with previous research indicating that masking is associated with mental health difficulties. Our analysis presents a broader conceptualisation of masking than previously defined in the literature, placing social oppression of autistic people at the heart of the relationship between masking and mental health. The findings have implications for diagnostic services, post-diagnostic support and therapeutic interventions, highlighting the need to challenge deficit-based narratives of autism.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
Available online 11 November 2022
1750-9467/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
I want to t in but I dont want to change myself
fundamentally: A qualitative exploration of the relationship
between masking and mental health for autistic teenagers
Louise Chapman
a
, Kieran Rose
b
,
c
, Laura Hull
d
,
e
,
*
, William Mandy
e
a
Central London Community Healthcare NHS Trust, London, UK
b
The Autistic Advocate, UK
c
Department for Psychology (Honorary Research Associate), University of Sunderland, Sunderland, UK
d
Centre for Academic Mental Health, Department of Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
e
Research Department for Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Autism
Masking
Authenticity
Mental health
Young people
ABSTRACT
Background: Previous research has identied an association between masking and mental health
for autistic people. However, the direction of causality and mechanisms involved in this rela-
tionship are not well understood. This qualitative study aimed to investigate autistic teenagers
experiences of masking, mental health and how the two develop and interact.
Methods: Twenty autistic teenagers took part in a semi-structured interview. The interviews were
analysed using Reexive Thematic Analysis.
Results: From the analysis, one theme was identied to conceptualise masking as described by
participants. Five more inter-related themes were identied, each involved both in the rela-
tionship between masking and mental health and conversely in the relationship between
authenticity and mental health. Participants described how masking and mental health both in-
uence each other, and both are inuenced by social and environmental factors.
Conclusions: The ndings are consistent with previous research indicating that masking is asso-
ciated with mental health difculties. Our analysis presents a broader conceptualisation of
masking than previously dened in the literature, placing social oppression of autistic people at
the heart of the relationship between masking and mental health. The ndings have implications
for diagnostic services, post-diagnostic support and therapeutic interventions, highlighting the
need to challenge decit-based narratives of autism.
* Correspondence to: Centre for Academic Mental Health, Oakeld House, Oakeld Grove, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK.
E-mail address: laura.hull@bristol.ac.uk (L. Hull).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/rasd
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2022.102069
Received 28 January 2022; Received in revised form 11 October 2022; Accepted 6 November 2022
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
2
1. Introduction
Autism represents one form of neurodivergence
1
and is characterised by differences in social communication, sensory, cognitive
and emotional processing, sociality and interests (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Using current diagnostic tools and con-
ventions, prevalence rates suggest that 1.1% of people living in the UK are autistic (Baird et al., 2006; Brugha et al., 2009, 2012).
However, it is well documented that current diagnostic tools are less sensitive to diagnosing women and girls, and people with higher
cognitive abilities, and so it is possible that these prevalence gures are underestimates of the true proportion of the population who
are autistic (Kirkovski, Enticott, & Fitzgerald, 2013; Ratto et al., 2018).
Across the life span, autistic people are more likely to experience co-occurring mental health conditions than non-autistic people
(Kim, Szatmari, Bryson, Streiner, & Wilson, 2000; Lai et al., 2019; Mazzone et al., 2013; Rai et al., 2018). Simonoff et al. (2008)
assessed the prevalence of mental health difculties in a population-derived sample of autistic adolescents, aged 1014, and found that
70% experienced at least one mental health condition. Varying explanations have been proposed to make sense of these ndings.
One explanation highlights the overlap between autistic traits and experiences of mental health difculties, for instance social
anxiety (Cath, Ran, Smit, Van Balkom, & Comijs, 2008). This explanation is weakened by evidence that traits that overlap between
social anxiety and autism, such as social communication differences, are supercial and empirically distinguishable from one another
(for example; Pickard, Rijsdijk, Happ´
e, & Mandy, 2017). Another explanation of the high co-occurrence of autism and some mental
health difculties is that they share common underlying causes. In support of this idea, research has identied some overlapping
genetic and neural underpinnings of autism and some mental health difculties (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics
Consortium, 2013; Hollocks & et al., 2014). However, the total amount of shared variance explained by genetic overlap between
autism and mental health conditions is small (Rylaarsdam & Guemez Gamboa, 2019).
Another hypothesis is that the experience of being autistic, or having autistic traits, has a direct impact on ones mental health. For
instance, autistic traits such as sensory processing differences could make ones experience of the word innately more stressful and
anxiety-provoking (Ozsivadjian, Knott, & Magiati, 2012). Indeed, sensory sensitivities, alexithymia (difculties identifying and
describing feelings) and social communication differences are each associated with an increased chance of experiencing mental health
difculties (Milosavljevic et al., 2016; Rai et al., 2018; Tseng, Fu, Cermak, Lu, & Shieh, 2011).
Another avenue of explanation has considered autism as an indirect risk factor for mental health difculties. This posits that autistic
peoples increased chance of experiencing mental health difculties is mediated by social, environmental and contextual factors (e.g.,
Brede et al., 2020). Such an approach is in line with the social model of disability (Shakespeare, 2006), as it seeks to move away from
locating problems within individuals and instead seeks to describe how external, social factors are often the root of disabling expe-
riences. This can be applied to the mental health experiences of autistic people. Autistic people, across the lifespan, are more likely to
have experienced victimisation, bullying, trauma and a range of other life events known to be risk factors for the development of
mental health difculties (Berg, Shiu, Acharya, Stolbach, & Msall, 2016; Grifths et al., 2019). For example, experiences of bullying
mediate the association between social communication difculties and depression (Rai et al., 2018). It appears that non-autistic
peoples attitudes and behaviour towards autistic people also impacts mental health. Autistic people who experience depression
perceive lower autism acceptance from others and have lower personal autism acceptance (Cage, Di Monaco, & Newell, 2018). Milton
(2013c) describes the broad societal tendency to pathologise and ostracise people who do not conform to social normative standards
and the oppressive impact this has on autistic people.
Masking, or ‘social camouaging, may be another important factor in understanding why autistic people are more likely to
experience mental health difculties and particularly how this is mediated by the social and environmental context (see Cook, Hull,
Crane & Mandy, 2021 for review). We dene masking as the experience of changing oneself in order to t in with the expectation of
others, specically within the context of hiding or changing autistic characteristics, which may overlap with more general experiences
of impression management (Ai, Cunningham, & Lai, 2022; Lawson, 2020). Autistic people have described experiences of suppressing
aspects of their authentic, autistic selves in social situations, and of ‘putting on an actto present oneself as neurotypical (Hull et al.,
2017).
Masking can involve deliberately and consciously using specic strategies, for instance self-monitoring body language and facial
expressions, developing a ‘scriptfor social situations, or forcing oneself to make eye contact (Cook, Hull, Crane, & Mandy, 2021; Hull
et al., 2019). However, accounts also suggest that masking can also develop and be maintained subconsciously (Pearson & Rose, 2021).
For instance, autistic people diagnosed in late adolescence or adulthood have described masking unconsciously when undiagnosed
(Bargiela, Steward, & Mandy, 2016). Furthermore, some autistic people have emphasised how masking can become so automatic and
pervasive while navigating ‘the neurotypical worldthat aspects of ones identity can become confusing, and it can feel unclear even to
the individual where the mask begins and ends (Bargiela et al., 2016; Hull et al., 2017; Tierney, Burns, & Kilbey, 2016).
Masking is driven by multiple challenges faced by autistic people. It can develop as a protective mechanism to avoid re-
experiencing bullying, rejection, discrimination or ostracism (Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Hull et al., 2017). Research suggests
1
Neurodiversity is a non-pathologising term, originally coined by Judy Singer in 1998, used to describe the wide variety in neurological proles
found within the human species. Someone who is ‘neurodivergenthas a brain that functions in ways that diverge signicantly from the dominant
societal standards of ‘normal(Walker, 2014). Silberman (2016) argues for autism to be recognised as a form of neurodivergence; characterised by
difference not decit. The autistic rights movement supports the recognition of autism as a form of neurodivergence. There are controversies and
differing ideas about how autism should be conceptualised (Baron-Cohen, 2019). Milton (2019) argues that differing opinions must not prevent
‘interactional expertise being developed where autistic people can lead and co-produce on meaningful research.
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
3
masking is one response to autism-related stigma (Cook, Hull, Cage & Mandy, 2021; Perry, Mandy, Hull, Cage, & Holloway, 2021).
Autistic people have explained masking as a search for acceptance from others (Cage et al., 2018), which may also be driven by shame
and self-criticism (Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019). Masking can be driven by a sense, often based on negative past experiences, that
friendships and connections with non-autistic others must be made ‘on neurotypical terms(Hull et al., 2017; Pearson, Rees et al.,
2021).
Existing research has begun investigating the mental health consequences of masking. Autistic people who mask more also report
higher levels of anxiety (Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Hull, Levy et al., 2021; Livingston, Shah, & Happ´
e, 2019), and some studies
have also found an association between masking and depression (Cage et al., 2018; Hull, Levy et al., 2021). Although these ndings
strongly suggest there is a correlation between masking and mental health difculties, the direction of causality cannot yet be inferred
(Cook, Hull, Crane & Mandy, 2021).
In qualitative studies, autistic people describe fears of being bullied, victimised, ostracised or rejected as driving the need to mask
(Bargiela et al., 2016; Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Hull et al., 2017; Livingstone, Shah, & Happ´
e, 2019), as well as anxieties about
appearing different, not tting in or making ‘social errors(Bargiela et al., 2016; Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Hull et al., 2017;
Tierney et al., 2016). Masking itself has been described as stressful and anxiety provoking, particularly in relation to fears relating to
being ‘found outor ‘getting it wrong (Bargiela et al., 2016; Hull et al., 2017). The association between masking and mental health
may therefore not be unidirectional, as mental health issues may drive masking and be present during masking experiences.
Masking is described as mentally, physically and emotionally draining, and burnout and exhaustion have been identied as both
short and long-term consequences (Bargiela et al., 2016; Hull et al., 2017; Tierney et al., 2016; Livingstone, Shah, & Happe, 2019; Cage
et al., 2018). Of serious concern is that masking has also been identied as a predictor of suicidality in autistic adults (Cassidy, Bradley,
Shaw, & Baron-Cohen, 2018). Autistic people have described masking having a negative impact on self-esteem due to perceived social
failings and ineffective masking (Livingston, Shah & Happe, 2019). Masking can also lead to a sense of identity confusion relating to a
loss of ‘authentic self(Bargiela et al., 2016; Hull et al., 2017; Tierney et al., 2016), and a disconnection from other people (Hull et al.,
2017; Livingston, Shah and Happe, 2019).
One limitation of the current masking literature is its focus on adults, with few studies conducted with autistic young people (Cook,
Hull, Crane & Mandy, 2021). One previous study has explored the experience of masking in autistic and non-autistic young people
qualitatively and quantitatively (Bernardin, Mason, Lewis, & Kanne, 2021). This study identied similar reasons for masking as re-
ported by adults, including avoiding negative perceptions by others, and to feel accepted by other people. Consequences of masking
included negative emotions and feeling tired, although mental health problems were not explicitly asked about or reported in results.
This study suggests that further exploration of the relationship between masking and mental health in autistic young people would be
benecial, as research in adults has suggested many (correlational) associations. No previous research has focused specically on the
relationship between masking and mental health for autistic children and young people. It is therefore important to gather qualitative
information to determine whether the associations reported in adult samples also apply in younger populations.
A challenge of investigating masking in teenagers, while they are still navigating the developmental stage of adolescence, will be
making sense of how autistic masking may occur within or alongside typical adolescent social identity processes. It is essential to hear
autistic teenagersnarratives and lived experiences of the process to understand what may be unique about masking during adoles-
cence and in adolescent social contexts. This is particularly important as existing qualitative research has indicated that adolescent
experiences and contexts, such as bullying, school transitions and increasing neurotypical social pressures and expectations, are
involved in the development of autistic masking which continues into adulthood (Hull et al., 2017; Tierney et al., 2016). Autistic adults
also report ‘habit(Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019) as a reason for camouaging, which related to being taught or conditioned to see
social differences as being ‘badas a child. It is important to understand this from the perspective of autistic young people.
The present study takes a qualitative approach to investigating the relationship between autistic teenagersexperiences of masking
and their mental health, to generate a richer understanding of the mechanisms that may underlie a relationship between two phe-
nomena. Autistic teenagerssocial experiences will be explored to elicit descriptions of how masking and mental health inuence one
another.
The key research questions were:
1. How do autistic teenagers describe their experiences of masking?
2. How do autistic teenagers describe the relationship between their experiences of masking and mental health?
2. Materials and methods
Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the NHS Health Research Authority and the Bloomsbury Research Ethics
Committee (Reference 17/LO/2055).
3. Participants
Eligibility criteria: Autistic teenagers (age =1319) who had received a diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder (including Autism,
Aspergers Syndrome, High Functioning Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorder) from an appropriate professional, and had IQ of
70 or above, were eligible to take part in the study. Autism diagnosis was conrmed via manual checking of health records, and IQ was
measured directly as part of the broader research project from which these participants were recruited.
Participants were initially recruited for another study (reported on in Hull et al., 2021b). Families were contacted for the original
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
4
study via healthcare services, voluntary sector organisations, and social media. Participants from the original study, who had indicated
they were happy to be re-contacted, were emailed with information about the current study. Thirty-ve families were emailed an
invitation to take part, and 27 responded. Out of these 27 families who initially responded, ve did not respond to follow up emails,
one dropped out prior to the interview due to mental health reasons and one was turned down when the sample size needed for males
was reached. Our target sample was 10 males and 10 females as we wished to represent a balance of perspectives, rather than seek a
representative sample of the autistic population.
Twenty autistic teenagers (age range =1319, mean =15 years old) living in multiple regions across England participated in this
study. Ten participants identied as male and ten identied as female. Twelve attended a specialist school or college for autistic young
people, seven attended a mainstream school or college and one was home-educated.
4. Procedure
Seventeen out of twenty of the interviews took place face to face in the participants home, one took place face to face at a university
research lab and two took place over a video call. All interviews were digitally recorded and lasted an average of 45 min (range: 1773
min).
Teenagers and their parents were sent separate information sheets in advance, were given opportunities to ask questions and signed
individual consent forms. Teenagers were reminded of their right to withdraw at any time. A visual stop and pause prompt was placed
on the table, giving teenagers the option to indicate visually, rather than verbally, that they would like to stop the interview if
necessary. Gender pronouns (e.g. he / she / they) and preferred language regarding autism (e.g. autistic person / person with autism)
were also conrmed and used accordingly throughout the interview.
Following each interview, a short debrief took place with the teenager, and where appropriate their parent, to discuss the research
topic, the teenagers experience of the interview and signposting for further support where necessary.
The semi-structured interview schedule (see Supplementary Material) was developed in consultation with all authors, and drawing
on best practice guidance (Barker, Pistrang, & Elliott, 2016). It was guided by existing research into the conceptualisation of masking
and its relationship with mental health (Hull et al., 2019), although care was taken not to assume that teenagers experiences of
masking would mirror adultsexperiences.
The interview schedule included questions about social experiences and presented an opportunity for participants to spontaneously
speak about masking without being directly asked about the pre-dened concept. Later in the interview, masking was asked about
directly, using a range of descriptions from the existing literature. Participants were asked about the cognitive, physical, emotional and
behavioural experience of masking, their mental health experiences and about the relationship between masking and their mental
health experiences.
The interview schedule was piloted in the early interviews, rened accordingly and used in all subsequent interviews. From this
process, questions were added about how experiences of diagnosis impacted experiences of masking and mental health. Clarifying
questions were also added about the differences between social experiences with autistic and non-autistic others. Finally, follow up
questions were added to ask about social messages participants had been inuenced by from wider society and their social
environment.
5. Qualitative data analysis
Interviews were transcribed verbatim and anonymised. Transcripts were analysed using a reexive Thematic Analysis approach, to
identify, analyse and report themes within the data (Braun & Clarke, 2006, 2019). Researchers were keen to recruit, in qualitative
research terms, a relatively large sample in order to hear from both males and female participants across the span of adolescence.
Thematic analysis was identied as a systematic and exible method which would allow researchers to analyse a large amount of data.
As the research topic is in its infancy, an inductive, data-driven approach was chosen, in order to capture participants experiences and
how they have made sense of them, and to generate hypotheses about causality which could be tested in the future. During the process
of analysis, the researchers took a critical realist approach (Willig, 1999), to allow for consideration of how the broader social context
was impacting upon the participants experiences and the meanings they were making.
The twenty transcripts were read through in full by the rst author prior to starting coding and initial ideas for coding were noted.
Initial codes were then generated for the entire dataset, and these codes were then sorted into potential themes, each representing
unique patterns of shared meaning (Braun & Clarke, 2019). These initial themes were discussed and rened with the other authors,
particularly considering internal homogeneity and external heterogeneity (Patton, 1990). The rst author then returned to the data,
rstly the coded extracts and then the entire data set, to review and rework the themes, before the nal themes and subthemes were
dened and named. Finally, the analysis was written up, with contributions from all four authors, using extracts from the data to
support each theme.
A consensus approach was used throughout to strengthen the credibility of the ndings. The rst author met with the other re-
searchers multiple times during the analysis process, to share early iterations of the thematic map and to discuss different ways of
grouping and presenting the themes. The third author also read and coded excerpts of interviews. Differences in coding, interpretation
of themes, and selection of quotes were discussed until a consensus was reached across all four authors. The research team includes
neurodivergent and neurotypical members, who have a range of experience including academic and non-academic researchers, and
clinical psychologists.
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
5
6. Results
The analysis generated seven themes. All themes and subthemes are listed in Table 1. The theme ‘Keeping it Insidedescribes how
masking was conceptualised by participants and ‘Being Myselfdescribes participantsexperiences of not masking and, instead, feeling
more authentic. We note here that we use the term ‘autistic authenticityto mean situations in which people are not masking; however
we do not wish to imply that masking necessarily means inauthenticity. Instead, we use ‘authenticity to mean feeling accepted and
able to be ones (autistic) self, in contrast to needing to mask. We hope that a more appropriate term can be identied in future research
in collaboration with the autistic community.
All subsequent themes represent other factors involved in the relationships between masking, authenticity, and mental health.
‘How others treat me describes the participants past experiences, both positive and negative, of how other people have behaved
towards them. Participants gave examples of these experiences directly driving, or preventing, the need to mask. They also gave
examples of how these experiences indirectly drove masking through their impact on their ‘self-imageand ‘internal experiencessuch
as anxiety. Participants also described how the ‘Immediate environmental context contained triggers for masking, often through
anxiety or sensory overload. Finally, participants described the ‘self-fullling prophecyof masking and authenticity leading to further
mental health issues.
6.1. Theme 1: Keeping it inside
Consistent across participants was the experience of keeping things hidden from other people. Descriptions of masking differed
both between participants and within each participants experiences.
Some participants identied just a few examples of conscious self-suppression, for instance holding in ‘unrelatedor ‘inappropriate
thoughts, stopping habits that annoy others or times I have to mildly rein it in(Participant 3). Others gave detailed accounts of
masking in various ways and across various contexts. Some participants gave examples of hiding or suppressing parts of themselves or
their experience, particularly aspects seen as ‘differentor related to being autistic. For instance, participants described suppressing
and hiding emotions, sensations, stims, reactions, opinions, or interests. Participants described ‘putting on an actin order to appear
more ‘normal, for instance forcing themselves to make eye contact, having a character or persona, monitoring and adjusting voice and
mannerisms, and rehearsing what to say.
However, for many of these interviewees it appeared that strategies were the surface level presentation of a much deeper, and at
least partially subconscious, experience. Participants described how masking was often ‘not particularly tangible(Participant 10) as it
had been part of their experience of and interactions with the world for as long as they could remember. Masking was not limited to
suppressing ones autistic self; it involved the suppression of a whole range of sensory and emotional reactions:
I mean, every single time something distressing is told to me, Im like, I do masking. Im like, I need to try to hold this reaction
back. but this is easier said than done. (Participant 18)
Several of the young people who described experiences of bullying, disparagement and ostracism found it difcult and anxiety
provoking to discuss masking. Although it was not discussed directly in the interviews, there was a strong sense that past experiences
had been so distressing, and potentially traumatic, that masking was a conditioned response driven by need and safety. It was therefore
difcult to reect upon masking or unmasking, due to high levels of shame relating to unmasking or ‘being myself:
Table 1
List of themes and subthemes.
Theme Subtheme
1. Keeping it inside
2. Being myself
3. How others treat me Responses to social differences
Bullying, ostracism and victimisation
4. Self-Image
5. Immediate environmental context Familiarity of context
Characteristics of people
Specic and physical environments
Intersecting identities
6. Internal experiences drive masking Anxiety and worry
Depression and low mood
Sensory and cognitive overload
7. A self-fullling prophecy Anxiety
Mood
Self-Image
Connection
Exhaustion
Sensory and cognitive overload
Suicidality
Protection from bullying and ostracism
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
6
They seem trusting but then when you talk to them theyre like ‘you should have kept that inside your head. They dont
understand you in general. They have opinions about you going to college. I just want to live my life as a normal girl. I want to go
to college. I fucking hate talking about my autism. Everyday just feels like a battle and I just wish autism could be taken off my
records alreadyI thought if it got taken off my record then I wouldnt have to talk about it again. Thats whats hardthat I
just want to live my life as a normal person I wish there was a cure.(Participant 11)
6.2. Theme 2: being myself
All participants described experiences of feeling or knowing that they were not masking, and instead feeling able to be themselves.
One participant did not describe any experiences of masking and several other participants felt that overall, they did not relate to a
sense of ‘, ‘maskingor ‘putting on an act. One participant explained:
I dont need to. I just act how I actReally, I dont really hide it. I mean, I dont even know what the symptoms are, I cant
remember the symptoms of autism but I dont really care now. (Participant 2)
Participants who related more strongly to the idea of masking were also all able to identify at least one context when I dont have
to act(Participant 7).
‘Authenticity as a term was not used by participants, however it was conceptualised post-hoc to encompass the range of de-
scriptions participants gave of times they were not masking. Participants conceptualised these experiences in various ways including:
Im totally myself(Participant 8), Im putting on less of an act(Participant 10), I can just have a good time and be myselfI dont
have to pretend to be someone else (Participant 12), and I kind of become more of myself (Participant 15). Participants gave
specic examples of not needing to mask, and feeling authentic or most ‘myself:
Have actually interesting conversations with people as opposed to just sticking to football [or] something that I think the
other person might enjoy.(Participant 1)
With text, with writing, I dont have to spend all that energy on my facial expressions, so I can just put it into writing. And that
can actually make it easier to talk about some more difcult conversations like mental health (Participant 10)
I kind of become more of myselfI can make a joke [or] start talking about something I like.(Participant 15)
One participant also expressed some difculty in knowing what authenticity or ‘being myself would feel like.
6.3. Theme 3: how others treat me
Many participants described how masking developed as a response to other peoples behaviour towards them, including how others
have responded to their social differences. Participants described how experiences of other people valuing, supporting and accepting
their social differences reduced or prevented the need to mask in certain contexts.
In some instances, participants made direct links between the inuence of past social experience on the development of masking or
authenticity:
Because Id spent so much of my life not tting in and being ostracised. I thought that [masking] was the only way to be liked.
(Participant 17)
They werent like the popular group. So they kind of, they were more accepting so I could be myself.(Participant 20)
In other examples, social experiences were described as indirectly inuencing the development of masking or authenticity via their
emotional impact, for instance on mood, anxiety or self-image:
I remember when I was in like year seven, I used to get like bullied a lot. I was pretty nervous when I rst joined [Saturday club]
I was worried in case like people would make fun of my autism. I didnt even tell anyone I had autismI never told anyone
about my autism there. (Participant 15)
I couldnt quite be my, fully myself around everyone [but now] Im totally myself. People say at my new school how positive I
am and its nice to be around. Whereas at my old school its always oh Im so moody and all miserable. So its nice to get that
recognition that Im being positive cos I dont realise it. Its just because people are lovely, Im just at home. I think its just
theyre more caring. (Participant 8)
6.3.1. Theme 3.1: responses to social differences
This subtheme details how teenagers described developing masking as a gradual response to multiple criticisms and discourage-
ments from others over time. Specic experiences were shared of times when teenagers social differences were criticised by important
people in their lives:
Ive realised like why kids never used to like me, because my social skills werent good enough. But now that theyve got better
so hopefully no one will criticise me on them because Ive had teachers at school criticise me on them. And I go to a special needs
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
7
school, where they should really be encouraging meI used to be told a lot to keep things inside my head. Its been very hard
for me.(Participant 11)
People have this expectation that we have to look each other in the eyes. And if you dont, youre shady or youre lying or
youre a bit dodge. So I trained myself to look hereso many times Ive had teachers [say] look me in the eye when Im talking
to you, look me in the eye when Im talking to you. (Participant 17)
When asked about past experiences that had inuenced the development of masking, teenagers identied experiences of negative
reactions or criticism from strangers:
So we were sitting waiting for the [exhibition] and Im, this was when I was very young, so I didnt know how to mask yet. So I
got up and started apping. And then a woman behind me was trying to see and said sit down little girl’… and then when we
were walking out, we saw on the guest book there was a guest in the guest book there was a message from the woman saying,
good, but that little girl should have sat down. (Participant 18)
Teenagers also identied how unhelpful narratives about autism, from individuals and from wider society, also played a role in the
development of masking. Participants felt that others misconceptions or prejudices impact the parts of themselves they are able to
share with others in social situations. These misconceptions included compliments for appearing non-autistic, pity when disclosing
autism, and holding prejudiced views:
Theres one person who knows, because I told her when I rst found out. And she was like, the rst thing she said she was like,
Really, you dont look autistic? And I was like, okay! Sure. So then since then I just havent bothered explaining.(Participant
20)
When asked about specic people they were more likely to mask around, participants gave examples of times people had shown
ignorance or prejudices about autism, and how these incidents increased their frustration, discomfort and anxiety in social situations
more generally, and reduced ease in being themselves, particularly with strangers:
[Its harder to be myself around] people who dont agree with the obvious facts, like the earth being round. and vaccines not
causing autism. And if they did, wed be in a far better world because it would mean people wouldnt be as dumb as them. I just
dont get it. Like I swear, people who are anti-vax. Why would we fake it? Why would we make it cause autism? Whats the
purpose?(Participant 6)
For one participant, their experience of being made to feel ‘alien during the autism assessment process played a role in the
development of masking:
I had spent the last 2, 3 years of my life being stared at, being assessed, being looked like a guinea pig, not poked and prodded
at, but like. Everyone writing on clipboards, asking me questions, asking my mum questions as if I was this alien from outer
spaceAnd as I said, although it was a relief [to receive the diagnosis], I was just like, OK, so now I denitely know that Im not
normal. Lets try and hide that.(Participant 17)
Many participants also described times when their differences were valued, accepted and supported by other people, and felt these
experiences had either prevented or limited the development of masking, or created contexts where masking was not necessary.
Participants gave examples of the kinds of overtly positive messages they had received from friends, family and school who valued, for
instance, their honesty, non-conformity, skills, kindness, and abilities:
A lot of people in my life do specify that its okay to be me. [brother] is very supportive of me and he doesnt really falter in his
faith in me. So like hes very condent in my own abilities and sometimes more so than I am of my own. (Participant 3)
[School] would say, dont care, just dont change yourself. Like you dont have to act. (Participant 12)
Participants also described more subtle things other people do which communicate that their differences are accepted:
[My friends arent] trying to like make me keep eye contact. I really hate thatpeople in my friendship group know that I that
I dont like keeping eye contact. And that if theres a lull in the conversation, then I might just like blurt out something.
(Participant 20)
Participants also valued times when they had explicitly been included, supported in a social situation, or when someone stood up
for them to someone else. These experiences helped teenagers feel more condent to honestly express what they were feeling.
6.3.2. Theme 3.2: bullying, ostracism and victimisation
Some participants described masking as a response to experiences of bullying, ostracism and victimization, as identied in this
subtheme. Participants spoke about experiences of bullying in mainstream school, before having to move on to another school. These
experiences were described as frightening, overwhelming and having a big impact on their mental health. Participants spoke about
developing a mask to protect themselves from further victimisation:
Trying to shield myself from more stuff. Just becoming a human cactus at this point, protecting their heart inside.(Participant
7)
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
8
[I mask because] Ive always been picked on because Im autistic. Whether people knew that or they didnt it doesnt matter.
Its the fact that I am different.(Participant 17)
Participants reported that through repeated bullying and ostracism, masking developed as a way to nd acceptance from their
peers. However, when the bullying did not stop, some participants instead took on the responsibility of educating their peers about
autism:
I noticed when I was there, a lot of the people made fun of the fact that I came from [specialist autism school] because they
recognised the tie colour or the jumper. But when I was, when I walked past some girls and I was like ohdont worry were not
as bad as you think we are. And then erm, soon we got up a conversation, and I talked about like how good autism can be and
that, how its, how its all right and not too bad and that. (Participant 15)
One participant shared how masking is also impacted by her fears relating to her knowledge of systemic injustices towards autistic
people, for instance, the treatment of autistic people sectioned under the Mental Health Act. Suppressing emotional or sensory ex-
pressions were therefore also a way to avoid potential harmful mental health interventions.
6.4. Theme 4: self image
Participants described feelings of shame, inferiority and low self-condence, often stemming from the experiences described in
theme one, and how these feelings and images inuenced the development of masking.
Participants spoke about feeling ashamed of their past social skills, behaviour or expression of distress. For instance, participants
described times when their sensory reactions to an environment, a period of high energy or hyperactivity, or strong emotional
expression had caused them to feel guilty for the impact on someone close to them, or ashamed of their behaviour. A negative self-
image drove and reinforced masking:
So it, maybe the anger would come to sort of at home with family rather than at the people at schoolI think it would bring
everyone else down. I know its about me, but I like. And then when I see them upset, it makes me upset. Cos Ive caused it, and
then Im feeling even more low.(Participant 8)
I dont really like talking about what they [my social skills] were like at the time because its really embarrassing because.
Because I used to board and partly why I left boarding is because the care team kept talking to me about what I used to be like.
And, I kept saying, please could you stop talking about what I used to be like? Im really ashamed of the person I used to be
Like because Im not the idiot I was then. Like I never thought about anything(Participant 11).
Some participants directly compared themselves to neurotypical peers, presenting themselves as inferior. Autism acceptance was
also discussed; low personal autism acceptance and low perceived autism acceptance from society impacted on self-image and the
development of masking. Participants indicated that their mask(s) developed as a response to the narrative that a neurotypical style of
communication was superior and most desirable. Some descriptions illustrated how the negative self-image they held, from these
narratives, directly drove masking:
Im quite awkward I suppose now in comparison to other people like sort of neurotypicals especially. They can just
communicate better I suppose. They just sort of have the most natural vibe, everyone listens to them and dont sort of mess up
when speaking or anythingI just really want to try hard. And since the awkwardness radiating off me so it isnt quite working
as wellI gesture a bit more. And ramble on because Im trying to keep the attention on me. (Participant 7)
On the other hand, some participants described feelings of pride and self-condence, which were rooted in or reinforced by positive
responses and messages from others. Some teenagers reported placing very little importance on other peoples opinions of them and,
for instance, did not feel impacted by peer pressure or a need to t in. Some shared strong convictions about acceptance between
autistic and non-autistic people needing to work both ways and illustrated in their descriptions how a positive self-image and a positive
narrative of being autistic directly reduced or prevented the need for masking:
I feel like if I got myself into a scenario in life where I have to completely redene myself, just to t the scene, Im questioning if
I want to be a part of that scene, because I feel like if I need to accept them a bit, they need to accept me a bitits just come
from this like sense that if I have to accept neurotypicalsway of thinking, then why cant they accept mine? So like, if you want
me to rein it in mostly, sure, thats perfectly ne. But if you want me to be a different person for like seven to eight hours of the
day, then youre asking for something that Im not in the mood to give you. In some ways autism makes me more capable than
normal people(Participant 3)
Some participants also spoke about valuing their personal strengths, which related to being autistic; teenagers expressed pride in
their intelligence, specic skills or abilities, self-awareness, creativity and ability to be a good friend.
Because I feel like autism in my case is better than being normalI wouldnt want to be normal.I wouldnt be as smart and want to
do as much learning as I am. I wouldnt have my amazing memory (Participant 6).
Some participants experienced a conict between valuing and wanting to be themselves, and wanting to feel comfortable, but
equally wanting to t in, connect, and be respected or admired. Teenagers reported that when they were able to unmask, or feel
accepted for their authentic self, both desires felt simultaneously possible:
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
9
Im just a free person in the civilized world and I can lie down whenever I want. In my mind, anywayI feel different but at the
same time I feel like I want to t in I just want to be with others. But I dont want to change myself fundamentally
(Participant 18)
Teenagers also spoke about how receiving their autism diagnosis shifted their self-image, which impacted on their masking. By
becoming aware of their social differences, they developed more self-compassion and respect and reduced masking; for instance, no
longer pushing themselves to do the things they previously felt they should want to do:
If Im nervous about like going to school or something and Ive learnt not to force myself to try and be in situations which I
know I wont like. I used to want to be invited to parties and things but now I know that I just, its really not for me. So thats
denitely helped(Participant 20)
6.5. Theme 5: immediate environmental context
Participants described masking as contextually driven. The nature and familiarity of the context and characteristics of the people
present were triggers for masking. Sometimes the environmental context was a direct trigger for the development of masking:
Especially in a new situation, Im very aware of having to like pull back on it, not be myself in that sense, but yeah. Its more
obvious for people when Im when theyre around me 24/7′′ (Participant 8).
However often the context triggered emotional and sensory experiences which in turn drove masking or authenticity:
Like if I just have to spontaneously go out to get a haircut. Its ok when Im doing it [masking] because I focus all of my energy
on doing it. Its usually the period before thats the most worrisome. Its just looking like somethings wrong with me perhaps.
But its also just feeling anxious I think, thats the problem, sometimes you cant really put a nger on it(Participant 10).
6.5.1. Theme 5.1: familiarity of context
Many participants described unfamiliar situations as a trigger for masking, and often related this to higher levels of anxiety and
lower self-condence associated with these unknown contexts. Teenagers felt that they were able to unmask and feel more authentic in
familiar situations where they were more relaxed and comfortable:
If its something where Im going every day, school particularly, it gets easier. So lessons are better. I dont have to worry about
that kind of stuff at school. Occasionally it comes back though usually if Im talking about something new. If its something
rehearsed like public speaking its actually not that bad because its rehearsed. Thats why because Ive done drama before and I
dont really have a problem with projection because its a rehearsed idea (Participant 10).
On the other hand, for some participants, masking became impossible in the most emotionally overwhelming unfamiliar contexts:
Well, if Im really sad I wouldnt have come in the rst place. Because I can mask that in school but I cant mask that in an
actual [new situation]. Because I know about school, I know what lessons I have, know the teachers I have, so I know I can
pretend to be okay in those lessons. Whereas in that social group I didnt know who was going to be there so something could
trigger me and I could cry(Participant 12)
6.5.2. Theme 5.2: characteristics of other people
Participants identied people who they were either more or less likely to mask in the presence of. Most commonly, participants felt
more authentic in their interactions with other autistic people, people they knew well, particularly close friends and family members,
people who knew they were autistic and people who they shared interests with or felt similar to. This related to feeling less anxious and
more relaxed in the company of these people. More spontaneous and less structured interactions felt easier with close friends and
family:
And churchtheres a few kids who have an autism diagnosis. I get on so well with themIts just this circle of complete and
utter transparency and honesty, and it makes my life so much easier (Participant 17)
And they will be more understanding but also its just because I think theres also something a bit because of, the same sort of
ethereal sense of being different is less so youre less on guard so youre less managing your expressions, your voice, your
actions(Participant 10).
On the contrary participants reported feeling more anxious and tense, and masking most commonly with neurotypical people,
strangers, and people who didnt know they were autistic. Some participants reported masking most commonly with neurotypical
people their own age:
Yeah if I meet any new people I think its safer just to mask because otherwise I would seem insane. So its when I meet new
people and when I meet people who are like, you know, the typical neurotypicals, the popular people in my school, Ill mask
then(Participants 20).
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
10
6.5.3. Theme 5.3: environmental characteristics
Several participants often described their masking as contextual; they noted how it was elicited by specic places or situations.
Teenagers noticed that they masked more in busy public places, where their anxiety was higher, and they experienced specic fears
that other people would respond negatively to them or make negative judgements:
Mostly I feel like I need to mask it when Im in a public place. And like when theres loads of crushing people Im like, oh, I
really want to run but I cant run because I wouldnt ap because people are obviously going to stare at me and think, oh, whats
that girl doing running and apping(Participant 18)
Some participants had noticed an increase in masking after they started in a mainstream secondary school. In addition to expe-
riences of bullying and ostracism described in theme one, masking developed through increased anxiety and feelings of difference, in
the context of large numbers of neurotypical peers and sensory overstimulation:
I guess it was just of kind of being around everyone else, sort of going from a little school into a big school and seeing how
everyone else was acting and kind of like feeling that maybe I dont do the same things. I dont walk the same way(Participant
4).
Environments which supported participants to pursue their interests, passions or strengths also supported them to be themselves
and reduced the pressures to mask:
Im still myselfIm more relaxed in my environment, so its, erm, and Im more comfortable as well, and I feel more, its my,
my second home, if youd likeIm doing art stuff I love, and theyve got amazing facilities. So Im really happy and positive
about all of it(Participant 8).
6.5.4. Theme 5.4: intersecting identities
Teenagers also alluded to ways in which being autistic intersected with other aspects of their identity in specic contexts, driving
masking. Masking became more necessary in environments when other peoples norms, expectations or prejudices relating to gender
and class were activated:
I remember trying to act calm or normal in a posh, rich areaso like imagine this everyday person, you know, you see them on
the street. Now imagine them in a restaurant. A rich one. Just like being awkward. With the handkerchief on his lap
(Participant 2).
Well, from, I joined my prep school in year four and I played with the boys most of the time up until about year six, because that
was just more fun, they were like, the girlsconversations were just really boring. But then in year seven, they started like not
hanging around with me as much, so then I had, I wanted to go and try and be popular with the girls. (Participant 20)
6.6. Theme 6: Internal experiences drive masking
Participants described how their mental health, and broader internal emotional and sensory experiences, were involved in the
development of masking. Participants described some of the thoughts, feelings, and physical sensations involved in the development of
and experience of masking, often relating to negative past experiences, negative self-image and social environmental factors (explored
in themes 24).
6.6.1. Theme 6.1: anxiety and worry
Participants described how anxiety contributed to the development of masking. Teenagers named specic fears relating to coming
across as rude or ‘weird, or doing something ‘wrong, and noticed feeling tense and nervous in these situations. Some participants also
described worrying about others noticing they were anxious or upset and developing an ‘emotional maskto hide feelings of anxiety or
distress:
Yeah its just looking like somethings wrong with me perhaps. But its also just feeling anxious I think, thats the problem,
sometimes you cant really put a nger on it. I was always analysing peoples behaviours and facial expressions quite a lot,
usually to determine whether I was doing something wrong myself. So I often got quite an anxiety about that and still do. I dont
know its not particularly tangible, but youre just worried people are going to notice youre feeling anxiousit can get quite
intrusive. (Participant 10)
I felt really awkward. I just sat there eating my chewing gum being like well Im going to try and be cool, I have chewing gum
and have my Starbucks. Try and be cool and just be like I dont care, but I do, Im freaking out inside(Participant 12)
Other participants described specic fears of other people making fun of them or criticising them. This was often based on past
experiences from which masking had developed alongside considerable anxiety, for instance about whether masking will prevent
further criticism:
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
11
Yeah, I think she apologized, but Im still a bit nervous now as to what she still thinks of me going to college. And I really want
to know what she thinks now, if shes changed because Ive tried to work on everything and now I can have a proper con-
versation without being criticised for something. (Participant 11)
Additionally, many participants described feeling relaxed and comfortable in the contexts where they were less likely to mask:
At school. I mean even when were just sat in form at tutor time and we all have all laughs and we all feel like happy and
everyones happy, and were all like connected and happy. Because we all know each other in the class and we all have to deal
with each other in the class. (Participant 12)
6.6.2. Theme 6.2: depression and low mood
Some participants spoke of the inuence of depression or low mood on the development of masking. For some, masking was
motivated by a desire to hide their low mood from others. Participants described how long-term experiences of bullying and ostracism,
alongside being distressing, led to experiences of depression and low mood, which contributed to the development of masking. For one
participant the drawn-out experience of struggling to obtain a diagnosis of autism also played a role in the development of depression
then masking:
When I was undiagnosed you feel very alone because obviously you think youre neurotypical, and everyone else thinks youre
neurotypical, so they expect you to act like youre neurotypical. And I thought I was, but obviously I was not. Like, denitely
was not. So I was very much ostracized by my classmates. My teachers didnt know what to do with me. My parents didnt know
what to do with me. I was very much inert in my own life. [] I felt so relieved afterwards to know that there was a reason for
why I was a little bit different. But then at the same time, I had a bit of an identity crisis. And for two years did my absolute best
to hide it from my classmates (Participant 17)
6.6.3. Theme 6.3: sensory and cognitive overload
Several participants described overwhelming experiences of sensory sensitivities and cognitive overload, particularly when coping
with the demands of the environments described in theme three. They described how masking could be highly physically uncom-
fortable and an additional mental and sensory demand. Specic aspects of masking, for instance forcing oneself to make eye contact,
were described as physically painful, and became harder when participants were already dealing with sensory and cognitive demands:
I always have everything Ive ever done or said, whats going on right now, everything thats going in my head, Im thinking at
the same time Im talking to you All this stuff is because of my brain and because of my emotions and my reactions and
everything that makes my head hurt[Masking] does add to it. I mean, I already have a dozen things to think about, I dont
need like one to two more(Participant 18)
6.7. Theme 7: a self-fullling prophecy
Masking had many consequences for participants; some were direct consequences to the teenagers mental health, and some were
closely related to their mental health.
6.7.1. Theme 7.1: anxiety
As described above, many participants described experiencing anxiety prior to and whilst masking. Participants also described how
anxiety was maintained in the longer term through masking. Suppressing emotions, stimming, and responses to stressors were noted to
maintain anxiety. Masking across multiple contexts had a cumulative effect on anxiety and its impact on the young peoples lives:
Youre worried that people will think youre anxious, so you try not to be anxious, but you just get more anxious. Its a self-
fullling prophecy in a way" (Participant 10)
Participants also explained how in situations where they felt more able to be their authentic selves, they experienced reduced
anxiety and increased condence. Anxiety was particularly reduced through the authenticity experienced in the company of other
autistic people:
It makes me feel less anxious. It does, it increases your self-esteem. It makes you have more energy because youre not spending
so much on putting on an act. (Participant 10)
6.7.2. Theme 7.2: mood
Participants also described how masking lowered their mood and made them feel sad about being unable to be themselves and
being disconnected or different to others. For some this led to feelings of hopelessness about the future, particularly when masking did
not prevent future occurrences of bullying and ostracism:
‘[After masking] I just go into my own world really I get a bit sad I dont know why(Participant 8)
Participants identied how their mood became more positive in situations where their authentic selves came through:
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
12
It gets my spirit a bit uprandomly sing and dance. its kind of more like a way to be sane. (Participant 2)
6.7.3. Theme 7.3 self-image
Another consequence of masking was the long-term impact on self-esteem and self-image, whereby masking reinforces feelings of
difference, shame and low self-condence. Certain participants specically noted how masking negatively impacted their self-image:
I guess it can make me feel a bit annoyed sometimes. I can feel a bit like Im not normal, Im having to, Im having to kind of
watch this. I can feel a bit annoyed afterwards. I guess I can feel a bit sad as well for the same reason(Participant 4)
Some teenagers also found that situations where they were not masking, and others responded positively, had a positive impact on
their self-image:
When I nally told people and they were all so, so great about it. That really impacted me positively. I mean societys all like ‘be
yourselfbut not like that. When I was myself, and even though society would say ‘not like that, everyone else was like, ‘yes,
like that!, it was great. It was very it was very powerful (Participant 17)
6.7.4. Theme 7.4 connection
Participants described how masking increased their feelings of disconnection from other people, which negatively impacted their
mental health. These participants described masking as a lonely experience. By hiding their emotions, identities and authentic selves,
teenagers felt as though there was a barrier between themselves and other people:
Its like youre very close to someone, but very far at the same time, so you can know someones behaviours and such and you
can analyse them becauseI was always analyzing peoples behaviours and facial expressions quite a lot, usually to determine
whether I was doing something wrong myself. So I often got quite an anxiety about that and still do. But at the same time, you
feel like youre always on sort of a different level somewhat (Participant 20).
Teenagers emphasised that they felt most connected to others during times when they were not masking and felt most authentic:
[I feel most connected] when I dont have to like, act, and we can just talk about similar things, or, we can both, like, give pets
cuddles, and sort of nice stuff like that (Participant 7).
6.7.5. Theme 7.5: exhaustion
Several participants described masking as a demanding and draining experience, which took its toll on their mental wellbeing.
Teenagers described how masking took up a lot of energy, physically, mentally and emotionally:
Going outside for something thats not rehearsed can take energy. Like if I just have to spontaneously go out to get a haircut,
which I did this week, can take energy to do that (Participant 10)
‘Its so tiring, trying to constantly be on the ball (Participant 20)
Participants who described masking all day at school explained how the exhaustion impacted them on their return home. They
described how everything ‘came outat home and how this impacted their interactions and relationships with their families, which in
turn impacted on their self-image and mental health:
It all came out. So like, all everything that built up from school it all just come out. And thatd make it worse at home. Kinda like
angry and tearful and annoyed and shouting at my brother things like that (Participant 8).
6.7.6. Theme 7.6: sensory and cognitive overload
Some participants identied stimming as essential for regulating emotions and sensory sensitivities, and explained how suppressing
(i.e., masking) stimming took away an essential emotional regulation tool and mental health coping mechanism:
When youre masking, you cant be yourselfyoull be in situations in which you have to deal with it as a neurotypical person
would. So if youre in a loud situation, you cant block your ears or be distressed because then people will looking at me like
whats she doing?’… its that that will cause a meltdown because you youre getting all of this input and you cant lter it and
its horrible. (Participant 17).
On the other hand, times when these participants were able to express themselves freely, particularly through stimming, they
described an immense emotional, sensory and cognitive release:
Everything inside my brain, just vanishes for a second, and I just feel like Im able to just process everything and all I think
about really is the running. I dont care what happens. I just want to run run run run runit all just vanishes, it all just melts
away suddenly. Because I, all I really want in life is to be free (Participant 18).
6.7.7. Theme 7.7: suicidality
One participant spoke about how the exhaustion and impact of masking took a profound toll on their mental health and how at
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
13
times this led to suicidal thoughts and feelings:
For some people, it will be easier to not exist rather than to exist. There have been countless times in my life where I have felt
that and it has taken me great courage to keep on existing (Participant 17).
Another participant reected on times when copying and mimicking others, while masking, made it difcult to distinguish their
feelings and someone elses feelings. At times, this left them very vulnerable to harm:
I made friends with this girl called [name]. And she was not the best inuence because I was trying to copy people at that time
so erm, she was quite depressed and quite, quite suicidal as well, and she would like self-harm and things. Yeah, we used to say
like that we were one. And, yeah, we used to do everything together, including how we felt about things. I wasnt really sure
what I personally believed in anymore(Participant 20).
6.7.8. Theme 7.8: protection from bullying and ostracism
Participants described a nal consequence of masking; to some extent the mask protected teenagers from further bullying or
ostracism and from the severe emotional consequences of those experiences. Participants commented on the profound negative impact
that bullying and ostracism had on their mental health and when masking helped them to avoid this, they experienced some relief and
satisfaction:
Id come from such a bad place so to be in a place where I was accepted. And a place where I was liked even was just so good
that it didnt matter to me that I was masking, that I was doing it falsely(Participant 17)
And I feel really satised when Im going home because Im like, wow, I impressed them today and they didnt nd me weird,
like(Participant 19)
7. Discussion
The present study sought to understand autistic teenagersexperiences of masking as well as the relationship between masking and
their mental health experiences. The ndings give a broader conceptualisation of masking than currently exists in the literature. Most
participants described experiences of both masking and, conversely, of autistic authenticity, with different developmental and
consequential factors. Masking and authenticity seemed to be two potential (but not necessarily mutually exclusive) responses to being
autistic in a mostly neurotypical world.
Results suggest that the relationship between mental health and masking is not necessarily linear. Autistic teenagersnegative past
experiences of other peoples behaviour and reactions to them, their own negative self-image and various environmental factors were
all described as factors leading to the development of masking. These factors also contributed to high levels of anxiety in social sit-
uations, which in itself drove the development of masking. Experiences of masking and authenticity had further consequences for
mental health, including for anxiety and mood, and related factors including self-image, connectedness, energy levels and sensory
experiences.
Although various denitions have been proposed (Hull et al., 2017; Lai et al., 2017; Livingston et al., 2019; Pearson & Rose, 2021),
within the literature, the conceptualisation of masking is in its infancy (Ai et al., 2022). To date, masking has been mostly limited in
denition to conscious behavioural or cognitive strategies. The current ndings point towards a broader understanding of masking as
something multifaceted, emotionally and psychologically rooted, which develops partially subconsciously within a
social-environmental context. Participants described masking less as a choice or decision, but more as an anxiety-driven response to
others behaviour and attitudes towards them, or to uncomfortable and inhospitable environments. This ts with some previous
ndings that masking involves subconscious components (Bargiela et al., 2016), and arguments that masking is a response to not being
accepted by the neurotypical world (Pearson & Rose, 2021). Participants did not limit their descriptions to social strategies or the
camouaging of autistic traits; masking included the suppression of emotion, reactions, sensory sensitivities, opinions and other as-
pects of identity such as class and gender. This reects a growing awareness of the intersecting impact of multiple stigmatised identities
for many autistic people (Botha & Frost, 2018).
This study identied some similar themes to those reported in a previous mixed-methods study of masking in autistic and non-
autistic young people (Bernardin et al., 2021), including the experience of negative emotions when masking, and feeling expecta-
tion to mask due to other peoples reactions. However, the present study extended previous ndings to identify specic characteristics
of the environment and the individual which might contribute to the association between masking and mental health, as well as
proposing for the rst time that mental health problems, particularly anxiety, might motivate masking as well as being a consequence
for autistic teenagers.
Participantsdescriptions of anxiety as a driver of masking is in line with previous qualitative research into masking in adults. Hull
et al. (2017) found motivations for masking included high anxiety in relation to staying safe, ‘assimilating, and nding social
acceptance. The current study also replicated previous ndings that autistic people mask their anxiety, and other emotional reactions,
in social situations, due to fears of the consequences of sharing their true feelings (Tierney et al., 2016).
The interviews highlighted that negative past experiences of how other people behave towards autistic teenagers impacts upon the
development of both masking and mental health. Previous research has highlighted the negative impact of bullying and victimisation
on autistic young peoples mental health (Grifths et al., 2019). Avoiding negative behaviours from others has previously been
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
14
identied as an external factor driving masking (Bargiela et al., 2016; Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Hull et al., 2017; Livingston
et al., 2019). The current interviews also emphasised the range of behaviours and reactions from others, including bullying, ostracism,
alienation, rejection, criticism, and ignorance, which can contribute to the development of masking both directly, and indirectly
through their impact on mental health, particularly anxiety.
The current study also showed how when autistic teenagers receive messages from family, peers, school, and wider society that they
are ‘decientsocially, this inuences their self-image and self-esteem, and drives the development of masking. These ndings t with
Milton (2012) ‘double empathy problemwhich states that autistic and neurotypical people may both experience difculties in un-
derstanding one anothers sociality due to differences in neurology and communication styles (Crompton & Ropar, 2020), however
‘decit-basednarratives of autism place the expectation to understand and adapt to the differences on autistic people only. The in-
terviews in this study show how autistic teenagers can internalise the ‘decit narrative and this can lead to the development of
masking in order to shoulder the burden of double empathy problems in social situations.
Mandy (2019) suggested that an environment that is inhospitable towards autistic people could be another factor involved in the
relationship between masking and mental health. Participants in the present study identied environmental factors that drove
masking, both directly and indirectly through the anxiety and sensory distress caused. Previous research has captured autistic peoples
sense of themselves as a social (stigmatised) minority in the company of neurotypical friends and family and how this drove the need to
conform to neurotypical expectations (Botha, Dibb, & Frost, 2020; Crompton, Hallett et al., 2020). Getting by in a ‘neurotypical world
has been identied in previous research as a cause of masking (Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019). The current ndings support this;
participants reported that they masked the most in environments where social norms were most strongly held and enforced (i.e.
mainstream secondary schools), they were therefore most expected to ‘act neurotypical and they were most likely to experience
anxiety and sensory distress. This was especially the case in the company of neurotypical people and in environments ‘built for
neurotypicals which were particularly sensorially overwhelming or unpredictable. This would t with the social model of disability
(Shakespeare, 2006), whereby the environment excludes and impairs the individual. Previous research has shown that non-autistic
people can form immediate negative impressions of autistic people which can impact the way non-autistic people interact with
autistic people (Sasson et al., 2017), supporting the observations of our participants that others treated them differently because of
their autism.
Participants descriptions of the mental health-related consequences of masking support a number of existing ndings in the
literature. Anxiety was involved in each stage of the process of masking; it was identied as a trigger, a description of the emotional
experience of masking and a consequence. An association between masking and anxiety has been established in previous work (Cage &
Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Hull et al., 2021; Livingston et al., 2019) and qualitative studies have identied the pressure of ‘successful
masking as highly anxiety provoking (Hull et al., 2017; Livingston, Shah and Happe, 2019). Participants in the current study described
how the masking of emotions, sensory distress and stimming reinforced anxiety. Exhaustion was another mental health related
consequence of masking frequently cited by participants in the current and previous work (Bargiela et al., 2016; Hull et al., 2017;
Livingstone, Shah, & Happ´
e, 2019; Cage et al., 2018; Rees & Pearson, 2021). In line with this, autistic burnout, characterised as chronic
exhaustion, loss of skills, and reduced tolerance to stimulus, is reported to stem from an inability to obtain relief from the pressures,
expectations and stress associated with masking (Raymaker et al., 2020). Other consequences of masking replicate ndings from
previous research including low mood and depression (Cage et al., 2018), disconnection from others (Hull et al., 2017; Livingston
et al., 2019) and suicidality (Cassidy et al., 2018).
Cage et al. (2018) suggested that masking may mediate the relationship between negative behaviour from others such as bullying
and low autism acceptance, and consequent anxiety and depression, although no mediating role of masking was found in the rela-
tionship between experience of stigma and mental health issues (Perry et al., 2021). The current interviews indicate that bullying and
low autism acceptance impact anxiety and mood directly, but for many young people masking reinforces and worsens anxiety, low
mood, exhaustion, disconnection and poor self-image.
Previous research in the area has suggested that some autistic adults nd that their ‘compensationstrategies are successful and key
to having fullling life experiences and that future research should seek to understand which strategies are most useful and ‘how their
success can be maximised(Livingston, Shah & Happe, 2019). The current study does not support these conclusions in all the autistic
young people we interviewed, who described that many of the potentially ‘successful outcomes of masking actually represented
avoiding negative responses from others. This suggests the focus of research should be on promoting acceptance of autistic authen-
ticity, rather than encouraging masking. ‘Successfulmasking cannot be deemed so without considering the negative, and in some
cases traumatic life events, which may drive an individual to mask, or the potential longer-term consequences for mental health, self-
image and identity. Encouraging the suppression of the self and enactment of ‘social normalcy reinforces disabling and dis-
empowering ‘decit-basednarratives of autism messages which young people internalise about themselves. Thomas (2020) compares
neurodivergent masking to the Finding Nemo character Dorys strategy to ‘just keep swimming and concludes that, ‘No amount of
swimming[is] enough if the world will not also adjust to meet you.
The current study also explored the relationship between authenticity and mental health and found that experiences of being
accepted, supported and valued drove authenticity, a positive self-image and a range of positive mental health consequences. Indeed,
existing research suggests that authenticity is positively associated with higher self-esteem and life satisfaction (Goldman & Kernis,
2002). Authenticity is also reported as key for recovery from autistic burnout (Raymaker et al., 2020). In the current study, many
participants were able to be their most authentic self in the company of other autistic people, and found these interactions and
friendships particularly energising, positive and connecting. This replicates existing research ndings that autistic people feel best
understood, and a sense of belonging, with other autistic people (Crompton, Hallett et al., 2020). Our ndings suggest that authenticity
may be an alternative outcome to masking for some autistic young people in some conditions; future research should look more
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
15
explicitly at how both masking and authenticity impact autistic peoples mental health and are impacted by this.
It is also interesting to consider the present ndings in relation to research into autistic peoples self-knowledge and theory of own
mind. Autistic people may introspect in different ways to neurotypical people; some ndings suggest that autistic young people nd it
more difcult than their neurotypical peers to recall autobiographical examples relating to knowledge of ones own personality traits
(Robinson, Howlin, & Russell, 2017). In line with this, several participants in the current study noted difculties in reecting upon
times when they were masking as opposed to ‘being myself. This raises important questions about the qualitative nature and expe-
rience of authenticity, as opposed to masking, for autistic people. Introspection differences, and alexithymia, may be important factors
in understanding how masking might develop subconsciously.
8. Limitations
The present study has several limitations. Firstly, qualitative research is limited in its capacity to address issues of causality. The
associations presented above, depicting the relationship between masking and mental health, and in contrast between authenticity and
mental health, are not generalisable to a wider population of autistic young people at this stage. However, as was the aim of this study,
the ndings have generated a broader understanding of some of the factors likely to be involved in the relationship between autistic
teenagers experiences of masking and mental health (namely a sense of authenticity and acceptance from others), which can be
investigated further in future research.
Secondly, while the sample was diverse in some respects (equal numbers of teenagers identifying as male and female, geographical
spread of participants), in other ways it was not. There was minimal diversity in the sample in terms of race and gender identity, which
limits the breadth of perspectives gathered in this study. In addition, many other aspects of identity including sexual orientation, class,
religion or culture were not asked about. These limitations mean that perspectives on how neurotype and other aspects of identity
might intersect, and specically how being autistic and from another minoritised or marginalised community might impact experi-
ences and consequences of masking, were missed.
A third limitation is the reliance on verbal qualitative data to understand something that is sometimes intangible, still developing
and not fully conscious. It is possible that some teenagers may have limited conscious awareness of masking and its consequences.
Additionally, many autistic people experience alexithymia, a difculty identifying and describing feelings (Milosavljevic et al., 2016).
Many participants in the present study articulated their emotional experienced in great detail, but others found this more difcult, and
therefore the experiences of each young person may not have been richly captured. Giving participants the opportunity to write or
draw responses to questions may have overcome this limitation, and future research should explore alternative methods to discuss and
measure masking. Such methods might also enable the perspectives of autistic people with intellectual disability to be elicited, a group
who have been previously excluded from research into masking (Cook et al., 2021).
9. Implications
The current study describes the relationship between masking and mental health in autistic teenagers, highlighting the role of
othersbehaviour and reactions, social-environmental factors, and self-image.
Firstly, future research should investigate the development and consequences of masking for young people who hold multiple
marginalised group memberships i.e. they are neurodivergent and from a Black, Asian or other minority ethnic background. It will be
important to investigate how other forms of discrimination, such as racism or transphobia, impact the development of masking.
Secondly, current research strongly indicates that authentic self-expression and presentation has multiple positive mental health
consequences for autistic teenagers. Future research is needed to develop and test therapeutic interventions, at an individual, family
and system level, to support autistic adolescents to develop a positive identity and self-image as an autistic person.
Thirdly, one of most frequently given consequences of masking, in the current and previous research, is exhaustion and burnout.
Research has begun to better dene burnout (Raymaker et al., 2020) and the next steps should be to empirically test the relationship
between masking, burnout and long term physical and mental health consequences. Research should also aim to develop and test
interventions to prevent and support autistic people experiencing burnout.
Fourthly, denitions of masking in the literature need to be expanded to focus more on how autistic people themselves concep-
tualise the experience, as a holistic experience, rather than purely a set of cognitive or behavioural strategies. Work should be un-
dertaken with autistic people to explore how aspects of stigmatization, additional marginalized identities, and the impact on wellbeing
might be included in denitions of masking.
Finally, further clinical research is needed to understand the short- and long-term impact of behavioural interventions, such as
Positive Behavioural Support (PBS), on both masking and mental health. For instance, an evaluation of the use of PBS is needed to
understand if and how behavioural intervention might be reinforcing masking and suppressing authentic expression or emotional
regulation (e.g. stimming).
Findings from this research also have several clinical implications. Firstly, therapeutic support for autistic young people experi-
encing mental health difculties must consider their access to environments which are likely to support and drive their authentic
expression and interaction; for instance time with other autistic people, contexts which will nurture, value and support the young
persons skills and interests, and environments which can meet young peoples sensory needs and encourage sensory expression.
Secondly, therapeutic support should consider supporting young people to explore dropping the mask and discovering authenticity
in safe contexts. Considerations of intersectionality here are important; particularly in contexts where authentic expression may pose
risks to safety for young people who face racist or transphobic discrimination or abuse. Mindfulness is one strategy which could
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
16
empower autistic young people to become more aware of their internal experiences which drive both masking and authenticity, which
could slowly enable them to step back and make more conscious decisions (Stark, 2020). However, it is essential that clinical in-
terventions do not place all the responsibility on autistic young people to develop skills to ‘stop masking. The difcult experiences and
negative social narratives which drive masking cannot be ignored in any therapeutic intervention. A whole system approach should be
taken whereby external social or environmental factors such as bullying, which are driving or reinforcing masking and mental health
difculties, should be addressed. Research suggests that autism acceptance training for non-autistic people reduces explicit biases
around autism (Jones, DeBrabander, & Sasson, 2021), demonstrating the importance of interventions for non-autistic people.
Thirdly, the long-term consequences of social skills training interventions for autistic young people need to be considered and
interventions which encourage autistic young people towards masking and away from authenticity should be discontinued. Research
has suggested that social skills training programmes do not achieve their aim of improving wellbeing by teaching young people the
skills to form meaningful friendships (e.g. Gates, Kang, & Lerner, 2017). It has been suggested that this is because the groups inhibit
young peoples authenticity and increase autism stigma (Bottema-Beutel, Park, & Kim, 2018). The current research supports the
discontinuation of social skills groups and the development of groups to support autistic young people to develop positive and
authentic autistic identity.
10. Conclusion
This qualitative study described for the rst time relationships between masking, authenticity, and mental health in autistic young
people. Young people described how environmental factors could trigger masking directly, and through their impact on mental health,
and that masking in turn impacted mental health. The characteristics of environments that promote a sense of authenticity for autistic
young people were also discussed. The ndings emphasise the need to consider masking within the context of the individuals broader
identity and the environment in which they exist.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance of Ben Hannon and Aaron Wiener-Blotner with data collection.
Funding
LC was supported by funding from Health Education England. LH is currently supported by the Elizabeth Blackwell Institute,
University of Bristol, the Wellcome Trust and the Rosetrees Trust, but was not at the time this research was conducted. WMs work is
funded by the National Institute for Health Research, Autistica, the Medical Research Council, the European Research Council and
Dunhill Medical Trust. The funding sources had no role in any stage of the research.
Declaration of competing interest
This study was conducted as part of LCs Doctorate in Clinical Psychology at University College London. LC, LH and WM con-
ceptualised the study. LC collected the data and performed the initial analyses; LC and LH drafted the manuscript. All authors
contributed to the design of the study, analysis and interpretation, and revised the manuscript.
References
Ai, W., Cunningham, W. A., & Lai, M.-C. (2022). Reconsidering autistic ‘camouagingas transactional impression management. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.05.002
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub.
Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., & Charman, T. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort
of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). The Lancet, 368(9531), 210215. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69041-7
Bargiela, S., Steward, R., & Mandy, W. (2016). The experiences of late-diagnosed women with autism spectrum conditions: An investigation of the female autism
phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(10), 32813294. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8
Barker, C., Pistrang, N., & Elliott, R. (2016). Research methods in clinical psychology. An introduction for students and practitioners (third ed.). Chichester: John Wiley,.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2019, April 30). The concept of neurodiversity is dividing the autism community [Blog post]. Retrieved from https://blogs.scienticamerican.com/
observations/the-concept-of-neurodiversity-is-dividing-the-autism-community/.
Berg, K. L., Shiu, C. S., Acharya, K., Stolbach, B. C., & Msall, M. E. (2016). Disparities in adversity among children with autism spectrum disorder: a population-based
study. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 58(11), 11241131. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13161
Bernardin, C. J., Mason, E., Lewis, T., & Kanne, S. (2021). You Must Become a Chameleon to Survive: Adolescent Experiences of Camouaging, 2021 51:12 Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(12), 44224435. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10803-021-04912-1.
Botha, M., Dibb, B., & Frost, D.M. (2020). Autism is me: an investigation of how autistic individuals make sense of autism and stigma. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09687599.2020.1822782.
Botha, M., & Frost, D. M. (2018). Extending the minority stress model to understand mental health problems experienced by the autistic population. Society and Mental
Health, October, 038. https://doi.org/10.1177/2156869318804297
Bottema-Beutel, K., Park, H., & Kim, S. Y. (2018). Commentary on social skills training curricula for individuals with ASD: social interaction, authenticity, and stigma.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(3), 953964. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3400-1
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77101. https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2019). Reecting on reexive thematic analysis. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 11(4), 589597. https://doi.org/10.1080/
2159676X.2019.1628806
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
17
Brede, J., Babb, C., Jones, C., Elliott, M., Zanker, C., Tchanturia, K., Mandy, W. (2020). For Me, the anorexia is just a symptom, and the cause is the autism:
Investigating restrictive eating disorders in autistic women. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, 42804296. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-
04479-3
Brugha, T., Cooper, S., McManus, S., Purdon, S., Smith, J., Scott, F.J., Spiers, N., Tyrer, F., & the NHS Information Centre Community and Mental Health Team.
(2012). Estimating the Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Conditions in Adults Extending the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Health and Social Care
Information Centre: London.
Brugha, T., McManus, S., Meltzer, H., Smith, J., Scott, F., Purdon, S., Harris, J., & Bankart, J. (2009). Autism Spectrum Disorders in Adults Living in Households
Throughout England. Report from the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey 2007. The NHS Information Centre: London.
Cage, E., Di Monaco, J., & Newell, V. (2018). Experiences of autism acceptance and mental health in autistic adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48
(2), 473484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3342-7
Cage, E., & Troxell-Whitman, Z. (2019). Understanding the reasons, contexts and costs of camouaging for autistic adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 49(5), 18991911. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x
Cassidy, S., Bradley, L., Shaw, R., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2018). Risk markers for suicidality in autistic adults. Molecular Autism, 9(1), 42. https://doi.org/10.1186/
s13229-018-0226-4
Cath, D. C., Ran, N., Smit, J. H., Van Balkom, A. J., & Comijs, H. C. (2008). Symptom overlap between autism spectrum disorder, generalized social anxiety disorder
and obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults: a preliminary case-controlled study. Psychopathology, 41(2), 101110. https://doi.org/10.1159/000111555
Cook, J., Hull, L., Crane, L., & Mandy, W. (2021). Camouaging in autism: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 89, Article 102080. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102080
Crompton, C. J., Hallett, S., Ropar, D., Flynn, E., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2020). ‘I never realised everybody felt as happy as I do when I am around autistic people: A
thematic analysis of autistic adultsrelationships with autistic and neurotypical friends and family. Autism Advanced Online Publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1362361320908976
Crompton, C. J., Ropar, D., Vans-Williams, C. V. M., Flynn, E. G., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2020). Autistic peer to peer information transfer is highly effective. Autism, 24
(7), 17041712. https://doi.org/10.31219/OSF.IO/J4KNX
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2013). Identication of risk loci with shared effects on ve major psychiatric disorders: A genome-
wide analysis. The Lancet, 381(9875), 13711379. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62129-1
Gates, J. A., Kang, E., & Lerner, M. D. (2017). Efcacy of group social skills interventions for youth with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 164181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.01.006
Goldman, B. M., & Kernis, M. H. (2002). The role of authenticity in healthy psychological functioning and subjective well-being. Annals of the American Psychotherapy
Association, 5(6), 1820.
Grifths, S., Allison, C., Kenny, R., Holt, R., Smith, P., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2019). The vulnerability experiences quotient (VEQ): A study of vulnerability, mental health
and life satisfaction in autistic adults. Autism Research, 12(10), 15161528. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2162
Hollocks, M. J., Jones, C. R., Pickles, A., Baird, G., Happ´
e, F., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2014). The association between social cognition and executive functioning
and symptoms of anxiety and depression in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 7(2), 216228. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1361
Hull, L., Levy, L., Lai, M. C., Petrides, K. V., Cohen, S. B., Allison, C., Mandy, W. (2021a). Is social camouaging associated with anxiety and depression in autistic
adults. Molecular Autism, 113. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00421-1
Hull, L., Mandy, W., Lai, M. C., Baron-Cohen, S., Allison, C., Smith, P., & Petrides, K. V. (2019). Development and validation of the camouaging autistic traits
questionnaire (CAT-Q). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(3), 819833. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x
Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., Allison, C., Smith, P., Baron-Cohen, S., Lai, M. C., & Mandy, W. (2017). Putting on my best normal: Social camouaging in adults with
autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and developmental Disorders, 47(8), 25192534. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5
Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., & Mandy, W. (2021b). Cognitive PREDICTORS OF SELF-REPORTED CAMOUFLAGING IN AUTISTIC ADolescents. Autism Research, 14(3),
523532. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2407
Jones, D. R., DeBrabander, K. M., & Sasson, N. J. (2021). Effects of autism acceptance training on explicit and implicit biases toward autism, 136236132098489
Autism. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320984896.
Kim, J. A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L., & Wilson, F. J. (2000). The prevalence of anxiety and mood problems among children with autism and Asperger
syndrome. Autism, 4(2), 117132. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361300004002002
Kirkovski, M., Enticott, P. G., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2013). A review of the role of female gender in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and developmental
Disorders, 43(11), 25842603. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1811-1
Lai, M. C., Kassee, C., Besney, R., Bonato, S., Hull, L., Mandy, W., & Ameis, S. H. (2019). Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population:
a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry, 6(10), 819829. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Ruigrok, A. N., Chakrabarti, B., Auyeung, B., & Szatmari, P. (2017). Quantifying and exploring camouaging in men and women with
autism. Autism, 21(6), 690702. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316671012
Lawson, W. B. (2020). Adaptive morphing and coping with social threat in autism: An autistic perspective. Journal of Intellectual Disability - Diagnosis and Treatment, 8
(3), 519526. https://doi.org/10.6000/2292-2598.2020.08.03.29
Livingston, L. A., Colvert, E., Social Relationships Study Team, Bolton, P., & Happ´
e, F. (2019). Good social skills despite poor theory of mind: Exploring compensation
in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(1), 102110. https://doi.org/10.1111/jc
Livingston, L. A., Shah, P., & Happ´
e, F. (2019). Compensatory strategies below the behavioural surface in autism: a qualitative study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(9),
766777. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30224-X
Mandy, W. (2019). Social camouaging in autism: Is it time to lose the mask? Autism, 23(8), 18791881. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319878559
Mazzone, L., Postorino, V., De Peppo, L., Fatta, L., Lucarelli, V., Reale, L., & Vicari, S. (2013). Mood symptoms in children and adolescents with autism spectrum
disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(11), 36993708. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.034
Milosavljevic, B., Leno, V. C., Simonoff, E., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Jones, C. R., & Happ´
e, F. (2016). Alexithymia in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: its
relationship to internalising difculties, sensory modulation and social cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 13541367. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10803-015-2670-8
Milton, D. (2013c) Natures answer to over-conformity: a deconstruction of pathological demand avoidance. Autism Experts. Accessed from: http://autismexperts.
blogspot.co.uk/2013/03/naturesanswer-to-over-conformity.html.
Milton, D. (2019). Beyond tokenism: Autistic people in autism research. The Psychologist, 32, 23 (Retrieved from) https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-32/
october-2019/beyond-tokenism-autistic-people-autism-research.
Milton, D. E. (2012). On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem. Disability & Society, 27(6), 883887. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09687599.2012.710008
Ozsivadjian, A., Knott, F., & Magiati, I. (2012). Parent and child perspectives on the nature of anxiety in children and young people with autism spectrum disorders: a
focus group study. Autism, 16(2), 107121. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361311431703
Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (second ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Pearson, A., & Rees, J. (2021). ‘Masking is life: Experiences of masking in autistic and non-autistic adults. Autism in Adulthood, 00(00), 19. https://doi.org/
10.31219/osf.io/t6vp2
Pearson, A., & Rose, K. (2021). A conceptual analysis of autistic masking: understanding the narrative of stigma and the illusion of choice. Autism in Adulthood, 3(1).
https://www.liebertpub.com/doi/abs/10.1089/aut.2020.0043.
Perry, E., Mandy, W., Hull, L., Cage, E., & Holloway, R. (2021). Understanding camouaging as a response to autism-related stigma: A Social Identity Theory
approach. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 0123456789, 137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04987-w
L. Chapman et al.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 99 (2022) 102069
18
Pickard, H., Rijsdijk, F., Happ´
e, F., & Mandy, W. (2017). Are social and communication difculties a risk factor for the development of social anxiety? Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(4), 344351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.01.007
Rai, D., Culpin, I., Heuvelman, H., Magnusson, C. M., Carpenter, P., Jones, H. J., & Pearson, R. M. (2018). Association of autistic traits with depression from childhood
to age 18 years. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(8), 835843. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1323
Ratto, A. B., Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Bascom, J., Wieckowski, A. T., White, S. W., & Scarpa, A. (2018). What about the girls? Sex-based differences in autistic traits
and adaptive skills. Journal of Autism and developmental Disorders, 48(5), 16981711. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3413-9
Raymaker, D. M., Teo, A. R., Steckler, N. A., Lentz, B., Scharer, M., Delos Santos, A., & Nicolaidis, C. (2020). Having all of your internal resources exhausted beyond
measure and being left with no clean-up crew: Dening autistic burnout. Autism in Adulthood, 2(2), 112. https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2019.0079
Robinson, S., Howlin, P., & Russell, A. (2017). Personality traits, autobiographical memory and knowledge of self and others: A comparative study in young people
with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(3), 357367.
Rylaarsdam, L. E., & Guemez Gamboa, A. (2019). Genetic Causes and Modiers in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 13(1). https://doi.org/
10.3389/fncel.2019.00385
Sasson, N. J., Faso, D. J., Nugent, J., Lovell, S., Kennedy, D. P., & Grossman, R. B. (2017). Neurotypical peers are less willing to interact with those with autism based
on thin slice judgments. Scientic Reports, 7, 40700. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep40700
Shakespeare, T. (2006). The social model of disability. In L. J. Davis (Ed.), The disability studies reader (pp. 197204). New York & London: Routledge.
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence,
comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921929. https://doi.
org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f
Stark, E. (2020). Dropping the mask. The Psychologist. https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/dropping-mask.
Thomas, L. (2020). Just keep swimming: Finding dory. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(1), 2728. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30489-4
Tierney, S., Burns, J., & Kilbey, E. (2016). Looking behind the mask: Social coping strategies of girls on the autistic spectrum. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23,
7383. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.11.013
Tseng, M. H., Fu, C. P., Cermak, S. A., Lu, L., & Shieh, J. Y. (2011). Emotional and behavioral problems in preschool children with autism: Relationship with sensory
processing dysfunction. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 14411450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.02.004
Walker, N. (2014, September 27). Neurodiversity: Some basic terms and denitions [Blog post]. Retrieved from https://neurocosmopolitanism.com/neurodiversity-
some-basic-terms-denitions/.
Willig, C. (1999). Beyond appearances: A critical realist approach to social constructionism. In D. J. Nightingale, & J. Cromby (Eds.), Social constructionist psychology: A
critical analysis of theory and practice (pp. 3751). Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
L. Chapman et al.
... The feeling that one's time, efforts, and participation are worthy of being considered By becoming aware of their social differences, they developed more self-compassion and respect (Chapman et al., 2022) Value Contributions made by an individual are recognized as beneficial to the social interaction ...
... In my mind, anyway… I feel different but at the same time I feel like I want to fit in… I just want to be with others. But I don't want to change myself fundamentally" (Chapman et al., 2022) Support Individuals receive intentional and personalized accommodations to ease their ability to participate Participants also valued times when they had explicitly been included, supported in a social situation, or when someone stood up for them to someone else. These experiences helped teenagers feel more confident to honestly express what they were feeling (Miles, et al., 2019) Welcome People want an individual's presence and participation and invite them to contribute Many described the school environment as unwelcoming, a detriment to their well-being, and a barrier to future aspirations (Tesfaye et al., 2023) Acknowledgement The group recognizes of the contributions an individual makes It was apparent that the school environment was not adapted to support the needs of those who were interviewed; in particular, many youth felt that teachers did not acknowledge their difficulties or help overcome them (Tesfaye et al., 2023) ...
... A cultural stereotype that conflated autism with ID [intellectual disability] was also absorbed by some participants and interestingly left them with a sense of not recognising autism themselves when unable to connect with stereotyped ways of being (Mo et al., 2022) Social influences Challenges or differences that arise in social interactions involving the autistic individual "I've realised like why kids never used to like me, because my social skills weren't good enough. But now that they've got better so hopefully no one will criticise me on them because I've had teachers at school criticise me on them" (Chapman et al., 2022) Qualitative Experiences of Belonging Primary themes are denoted by section headers in the below section, while secondary themes are denoted by italics in the text under each header. Of note, section headers, indicating primary themes, are italicized to conform to required formatting guidelines and not to suggest that the primary themes are also secondary themes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many autistic adolescents experience difficulties finding belonging; however, the current understanding of belonging is from non-autistic perspectives. A stronger understanding of belonging from the autistic perspective is critical to improve autistic adolescents’ well-being. We systematically reviewed articles published from January 2013 to May 2023 investigating autistic adolescents’ understanding and experiences with belonging. Nine studies met inclusion criteria (n = 245), and themes across respondents were identified using inductive methods. Autistic adolescents identified components essential for belonging, including authenticity, similarity, reciprocity, and meaning (e.g., autonomy, respect, support, acceptance). Adolescents’ conceptualizations of belonging highlight their desire to be themselves while finding deep connections with others. Society can continue to move towards an environment that supports autistic adolescents and acknowledges their individuality.
... We have demonstrated the characteristic data of the included studies ( Table 1). The final included articles were published in six different countries, with Australia with the most studies (Arnold et al., 2023a, b;Higgins et al., 2021;Mantzalas et al., 2022a;Mantzalas, Richdale, & Dissanayake, Mantzalas et al., 2022a, b, 2023, followed by the United States (Hale & Sanders, 2023;Raymaker et al., 2020;Richards et al., 2023), United Kingdom (Cage & McManemy, 2022;Chapman et al., 2022), Canada (Phung et al., 2021), South Africa (Mahony &O'Ryan, 2022), andPoland (Pyszkowska et al., 2023). Due to the novelty of the topic, the publication year varied from 2020 to 2023. ...
... Sample sizes varied from 1 to 455. Moreover, while most studies focused on adult ASD, some assessed adolescent ASD (Chapman et al., 2022;Phung et al., 2021). We also could not perform a quality assessment due to the high heterogeneity in study designs (Word cloud is presented in Fig. 2). ...
... We collected the limitations of the included studies. A frequent limitation among the studies is the lack of diversity among participants in terms of age, sex, socioeconomic status, social support, race, ethnicity, gender, and sexuality, as reported in five studies (Chapman et al., 2022;Higgins et al., 2021;Mantzalas et al., 2022;Phung et al., 2021;Raymaker et al., 2020). Additionally, seven studies reported the limitation of insufficient data to conclude about autistic burnout (Arnold et al., 2023a, b;Cage & McManemy, 2022; ...
Article
Full-text available
Autistic people have long talked about autistic burnout in social media yet there is limited data available in the academic literature, and its definition is still under debate. This scoping review aims to explore the key concepts related to autistic burnout and identify gaps in the current knowledge of this condition. We conducted a comprehensive search across PubMed, Scopus, Embase, Web of Science, and PsycINFO databases to gather data on autistic burnout. The extracted data from 14 included studies fell into six themes, namely Subjective Understanding of Burnout (four subthemes), Burnout Characterization (three subthemes), Evaluation, Co-occurrence and Misidentification, Biological Mechanisms, and Solutions. The Subjective Understanding of Burnout represents the internal point of view (POV) on autistic burnout (experiences and feelings of people who have personally encountered burnout), while the rest fall into the category of external POV (observations or assessments from outside people or experts who observed autistic burnout in affected individuals). Despite the growing literature on autistic burnout, our study reveals several gaps in current knowledge, such as its epidemiology, protective and risk factors, consequences, and educational plans and guidelines to improve awareness and support. Separating findings by the internal and external POVs makes up one of the strengths of this study.
... 4 Participants described feeling different from their peers, and engaging in impression management, masking, and camouflaging to fit in, and avoid bullying and victimization. 11,34,35 The strategies used also align with those reported in previous studies with both young people and adults, including mimicking the clothing or facial expressions of others, and minimizing emotional responses. [35][36][37] Engaging in camouflaging led to difficulties with identity, although some found that this eased as they got older and came to be more accepting of being autistic. ...
... 11,34,35 The strategies used also align with those reported in previous studies with both young people and adults, including mimicking the clothing or facial expressions of others, and minimizing emotional responses. [35][36][37] Engaging in camouflaging led to difficulties with identity, although some found that this eased as they got older and came to be more accepting of being autistic. ...
... The DPs' pattern of 'masking' failures of face recognition strikes us as resembling masking reported by Autistic participants [66] or individuals with ADHD [67]. Masking is an exhausting and costly cognitive effort, often leading to negative mental-health consequences [68,69] and burnout [70,71]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This mixed methods study examined the real-world experiences of living with developmental prosopagnosia (face blindness), a lifelong neurodevelopmental condition that severely affects the ability to recognise faces despite otherwise normal vision, IQ and memory. Twenty-nine UK based adults with confirmed face recognition difficulties completed an online survey describing and quantifying their experiences of living with poor face recognition. Although the majority (62%) of participants reported being able to recognise their immediate family, e.g., parent, partner, or child, strikingly 35% reported being unable to reliably recognise their immediate family members out of context. Even fewer (45%), reported always being able to recognise their three closest friends when encountering them unexpectedly, highlighting that DP commonly affects the recognition of highly familiar faces with whom individuals have close emotional relationships. Furthermore, participants who reported being able or unable to recognise their immediate family showed no significant difference in objective face memory ability. More than two thirds of participants (65.5%) reported being able to recognise fewer than 10 familiar faces (with the most common response being none), far below typical abilities. Thematic framework analysis highlighted how low public, professional, and employer awareness of developmental prosopagnosia presented challenges across multiple domains including seeking diagnosis, social and family relationships and workplaces. Driven largely by concerns about negative evaluation by others, most participants employed a range of highly effortful, though error prone, strategies to disguise and compensate for their face recognition difficulties. Some of the strategies described may help explain why many individuals can perform within typical norms on laboratory face processing tests despite their clear difficulties in everyday life and highlight the need for ecologically valid tests. Participants’ highest priorities for future research were improved awareness of developmental prosopagnosia and interventions to improve their face recognition ability.
... Although more accurate mentalizing skills might make it easier to understand, predict, and perhaps even imitate neurotypical social behavior, camouflaging one's autistic traits and ways of communicating may lead to feelings of inauthenticity and disconnection in relationships (Chapman et al., 2022). Camouflaging is more prevalent among autistic women and girls (Cook et al., 2021), a demographic that was underrepresented in studies in this review. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research suggests notable heterogeneity in the social-cognitive abilities of autistic people (i.e., the ability to perceive and understand others’ mental states, including their thoughts, emotions, beliefs, and intentions); yet, only recently have researchers begun to examine how these abilities directly relate to the social well-being of autistic youth. This review sought to systematically evaluate and synthesize descriptive and intervention studies (n = 46) examining the association between different aspects of social cognition (emotion recognition, affective, and cognitive mentalizing) and social connectedness (social relationships, adversity, and loneliness) in autistic individuals. The findings are generally mixed, with studies limited to a predominantly verbal subset of the autistic population; nonetheless, several important patterns emerged. More accurate mentalizing, when accounting for other social skills, appears associated with having more and closer social relationships and, in a smaller literature, reduced loneliness, but whether social-cognitive interventions improve these outcomes is less clear. By contrast, both descriptive and intervention studies suggest that social-cognitive abilities are associated with reduced social adversity. Future work is needed to build upon these findings and develop approaches that better reflect this complexity in order to support autistic people in developing fulfilling social connections, whether through skill building and/or fostering inclusive, supportive social settings.
... In their review, 15 empirical studies of 10 interventions were identified, and the results of these studies indicated that interventions that aimed to reduce echolalia did decrease levels of echolalia. With the rise of the neurodiversity movement and autistic selfadvocates, researchers and clinicians are now getting firsthand perspectives on the downsides of eliminating echolalia, such as a sense of needing to hide one's identity (Chapman et al., 2022;Cohn et al., 2024). Alternatively, when speechlanguage pathologists honor and support all forms of communication, including echolalia, it demonstrates respect for their client's sense of identity and agency (Donaldson et al., 2023) and can improve the overall quality of care (Dorsey et al., 2020;Gaddy & Crow, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Although echolalic speech is found in typical development, echolalia is most commonly associated with autism. As such, echolalia has frequently been the focus of various interventions aimed at autistic children. Recent research and the voices of autistic self-advocates indicate that echolalia serves as a meaningful form of communication or functionally supports self-regulation. As a result, the literature on how, or whether, to target echolalia in therapy is full of philosophically diverse approaches. To support evidence-based practice, the purpose of this scoping review was to inform speech-language clinicians about current research on interventions targeting echolalia and how disagreement on this topic may be affecting the autistic individuals we serve. Method Search terms were entered into seven databases utilizing a key word search, and 74 total articles were included in this scoping review. Data on the intervention approach, outcomes, and participant demographics were then extracted for analysis. Results Most interventions utilized principles of applied behavior analysis. Nearly all studies reported decreased echolalia; very few reported increased speech output or communicative attempts. Demographically, most participants were male and White/Caucasian, if sex, race, or ethnic data were reported at all. Conclusions Overwhelmingly, the literature on interventions focused on echolalia had the explicit aim of reducing echolalic speech, a functional form of communication. Autistic self-advocates have made it clear that they are opposed to interventions that seek to eliminate autistic traits, including echolalia. When implementing evidence-based practice that considers scientific evidence alongside client values, speech-language clinicians should avoid interventions aimed at reducing echolalia. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28536404
Article
Full-text available
Positive behaviour support (PBS) is a dynamic approach historically influenced by advances in applied science and social movements. Recently, the neurodiversity perspective, a social movement emerging from the autistic community, has raised sharp criticisms about applied behaviour analysis (ABA), causing some advocates to reject any ABA-based approach, including PBS. Like prior disability social movements that have helped to shape the values and practices of PBS, we believe that the neurodiversity perspective provides an important source of social validity for enhancing PBS practices. In this paper we describe key concerns around ABA-based interventions within four thematic areas and explore ways in which PBS appears to align with the neurodiversity perspective or diverge as currently practiced at the individual level. Considering gaps, discrepancies or implementation challenges, we make recommendations within each theme for how PBS can improve or extend its practice in line with the neurodiversity perspective and PBS values.
Article
Evidence suggests that neurodivergent pupils are disproportionately impacted by school distress and attendance difficulties in mainstream schools in the United Kingdom. Research on school distress and attendance has predominantly consulted parents and teachers and neglected the pupil voice. This qualitative, online interview study involved neurodivergent adolescents (n = 30; 11–16 years), who were currently experiencing school distress and attendance difficulties, to understand their lived experiences. Using thematic analysis, the interviews highlighted that there was significant variability, but multiple factors such as sensory differences, uncertainty at school, social challenges, poor neurodiversity understanding and a lack of an appropriate alternative to mainstream schools, commonly coalesced to create distress. The wide-ranging impacts of school distress extended far beyond attendance. The young people did not feel that the current education system met their needs. The results contribute to considering how best to support neurodivergent pupils experiencing school distress and attendance difficulties in mainstream schools. Lay Abstract There is currently a UK school attendance crisis and research suggests that neurodivergent pupils are particularly impacted. Neurodivergent pupils can experience emotional distress at school more often than their neurotypical peers and this distress can be associated with attendance difficulties. Research has mostly consulted parents and teachers, but the current study interviewed 30 neurodivergent 11- to 16-year-olds in UK mainstream secondary schools who were all experiencing school distress and attendance difficulties with the aim of learning from their experiences. The young people highlighted multiple factors such as sensory differences, frequent uncertainty at school, social challenges and a lack of neurodiversity awareness (to name a few), that could all contribute to their experiences, but that school distress could vary significantly between pupils. Some pupils struggled to get to school in the mornings and others were anxious in the evenings. Some young people self-harmed or experienced significant emotional wellbeing difficulties. Young people did not feel understood or supported, but they did not feel that there was a viable alternative to their current school. The results contribute to considering how best to approach the current UK school attendance crisis in mainstream schools that we know to be disproportionately impacting neurodivergent pupils.
Article
Full-text available
Camouflaging refers to strategies used by autistic people to mask or hide social difficulties. The current study draws on Social Identity Theory to examine the relationship between camouflaging and autism-related stigma, testing the hypothesis that camouflaging represents an individualistic strategy in response to stigma. Two hundred and twenty-three autistic adults completed an online survey measuring perceived autism-related stigma, individualistic and collective strategies, camouflaging and mental wellbeing. Results indicated that higher camouflaging was positively associated with autism-related stigma and both individualistic and collective strategy use. Autism-related stigma was associated with lower wellbeing however this relationship was not mediated by camouflaging. These findings demonstrate how stigma contributes to camouflaging and highlight the complexities of navigating autistic identity while still camouflaging.
Article
Full-text available
Background There is inconsistent evidence for a clear pattern of association between ‘camouflaging’ (strategies used to mask and/or compensate for autism characteristics during social interactions) and mental health. Methods This study explored the relationship between self-reported camouflaging and generalised anxiety, depression, and social anxiety in a large sample of autistic adults and, for the first time, explored the moderating effect of gender, in an online survey. Results Overall, camouflaging was associated with greater symptoms of generalised anxiety, depression, and social anxiety, although only to a small extent beyond the contribution of autistic traits and age. Camouflaging more strongly predicted generalised and social anxiety than depression. No interaction between camouflaging and gender was found. Limitations These results cannot be generalised to autistic people with intellectual disability, or autistic children and young people. The sample did not include sufficient numbers of non-binary people to run separate analyses; therefore, it is possible that camouflaging impacts mental health differently in this population. Conclusions The findings suggest that camouflaging is a risk factor for mental health problems in autistic adults without intellectual disability, regardless of gender. We also identified levels of camouflaging at which risk of mental health problems is highest, suggesting clinicians should be particularly aware of mental health problems in those who score at or above these levels.
Article
Full-text available
Camouflaging includes strategies used by individuals to mask or hide autistic traits. Research has shown that both autistic and neurotypical individuals engage in camouflaging and that there may be sex differences in the reasons for camouflaging in autistic adults. The purpose of this qualitative study was to extend previous research on the lived experience of camouflaging through exploring camouflaging motivations and consequences in autistic and neurotypical adolescents through both questionnaires (n = 132) and semi-structured interviews (n = 19). Results revealed trends in camouflaging motivations and consequences by diagnosis and sex, as well as by sex within the autistic group. These findings further inform our understanding of camouflaging and why it may be reported as particularly detrimental for autistic females.
Article
Full-text available
Non-autistic adults often hold explicit and implicit biases toward autism that contribute to personal and professional challenges for autistic people. Although previous research indicates that non-autistic adults with higher autism knowledge and familiarity express more inclusionary attitudes, it remains unclear whether training programs designed to promote autism acceptance and understanding affect subsequent implicit and explicit biases toward autism. In this study, non-autistic adults (N = 238) completed an autism acceptance training featuring factual information and engaging first-person narratives, a general mental health training not mentioning autism, or a no-training control, then responded to surveys assessing their autism knowledge, stigma, and impressions of autistic adults, and completed a novel implicit association task about autism. Non-autistic adults in the autism acceptance training condition reported more positive impressions of autistic adults, demonstrated fewer misconceptions and lower stigma about autism, endorsed higher expectations of autistic abilities, and expressed greater social interest in hypothetical and real autistic people. However, training had no effect on implicit biases, with non-autistic adults associating autism-related labels with unpleasant personal attributes regardless of training condition. These findings suggest that the autism acceptance training program in this study, designed to increase autism knowledge and familiarity among non-autistic people, holds promise for reducing explicit but not implicit biases toward autism. Lay abstract Autistic adults face prejudice from non-autistic people. They are often judged unfairly and left out of social activities because of their differences. This can make it difficult for autistic people to make friends and find jobs. Some training programs have tried to teach autistic people to act more like non-autistic people to help them gain acceptance. Fewer have focused on teaching non-autistic people how to be more autism friendly. In this study, we used a short training video that teaches people about autism. The video was created with the help of autistic adults and included clips of real autistic people. We found that non-autistic people who watched this video had better knowledge about autism and showed more autism-friendly attitudes than those who watched a video about mental health or those who did not watch any video. They were more open to having a relationship with an autistic person and had more positive beliefs about autism. However, our video did not affect people’s unconscious attitudes about autism. People in our study connected autism with unpleasant traits, even if they had watched the autism training video. This suggests that teaching non-autistic people about autism may promote more autism-friendly attitudes, but some beliefs may be harder to change.
Article
Full-text available
Camouflaging involves masking and/or compensating for autistic characteristics and has been identified in autistic individuals through a variety of different methods. Individual variation in the extent, processes and outcomes of camouflaging has been reported in autistic adults, and there has been some investigation of camouflaging by autistic adolescents. This study was conducted to better understand how some of these individual differences emerge, by examining potential mechanisms (theory of mind, executive function, intelligence quotient and age) involved in camouflaging by 58 autistic adolescents aged 13–18 years (29 females, 29 males). Fewer executive function difficulties predicted greater use of total camouflaging strategies and the compensation subscale, but not the masking or assimilation subscales; no other predictors reached statistical significance. These findings suggest that individual differences in executive function ability may underlie variation in the use of camouflaging by adolescents. The total variance explained in the model was small, suggesting the need to examine other factors which may underpin camouflaging. The implications of this finding for the relationship between camouflaging and well‐being are discussed, along with the distinction between attempts to camouflage and the efficacy of those attempts. Lay Summary Camouflaging involves hiding your autism or finding ways around difficulties in order to fit in during social situations. This study found that autistic teenagers with good executive function abilities camouflage their autism more than those who struggle with executive function (which includes planning, goal‐direction and memory). This may have implications for teenagers' mental health and their social functioning.
Article
Social performances pervade human interactions. Some autistic people describe their social performances as ‘camouflaging’ and engage in these performances to mitigate social challenges and survive in the neurotypical world. Here, we reconsider autistic camouflaging under the unifying framework of impression management (IM) by examining overlapping and unique motivations, neurocognitive mechanisms, and consequences. Predictive coding and Bayesian principles are synthesized into a computational model of IM that applies to autistic and neurotypical people. Throughout, we emphasize the inherently transactional, context-dependent nature of IM, the distinct computational challenges faced by autistic people, and the psychological toll that compelled IM can take. Viewing camouflaging through this lens highlights the pressing needs to change societal attitudes, destigmatize autism, refine social skills-building programs for autistic individuals, and integrate these programs with environment-focused support.
Article
Some autistic people employ strategies and behaviours to cope with the everyday social world, thereby ‘camouflaging’ their autistic differences and difficulties. This review aimed to systematically appraise and synthesise the current evidence base pertaining to autistic camouflaging. Following a systematic search of eight databases, 29 studies quantifying camouflaging in children and adults with autism diagnoses or high levels of autistic traits were reviewed. The multiple methods used to measure camouflaging broadly fell under two different approaches: internal-external discrepancy or self-report. These approaches appear to relate to two distinct but potentially connected elements of camouflaging: observable behavioural presentations and self-perceived camouflaging efforts. While significant variation was noted across individual study findings, much the existing literature supported three preliminary findings about the nature of autistic camouflaging: (1) adults with more self-reported autistic traits report greater engagement in camouflaging; (2) sex and gender differences exist in camouflaging; and (3) higher self-reported camouflaging is associated with worse mental health outcomes. However, the research base was limited regarding participant characterisation and representativeness, which suggests that conclusions cannot be applied to the autistic community as a whole. We propose priorities for future research in refining the current understanding of camouflaging and improving measurement methods.
Article
Article
Autistic masking is an emerging research area that focuses on understanding the conscious or unconscious suppression of natural autistic responses and adoption of alternatives across a range of domains. It is suggested that masking may relate to negative outcomes for autistic people, including late/missed diagnosis, mental health issues, burnout, and suicidality. This makes it essential to understand what masking is, and why it occurs. In this conceptual analysis, we suggest that masking is an unsurprising response to the deficit narrative and accompanying stigma that has developed around autism. We outline how classical social theory (i.e., social identity theory) can help us to understand how and why people mask by situating masking in the social context in which it develops. We draw upon the literature on stigma and marginalization to examine how masking might intersect with different aspects of identity (e.g., gender). We argue that although masking might contribute toward disparities in diagnosis, it is important that we do not impose gender norms and stereotypes by associating masking with a “female autism phenotype.” Finally we provide recommendations for future research, stressing the need for increased understanding of the different ways that autism may present in different people (e.g., internalizing and externalizing) and intersectionality. We suggest that masking is examined through a sociodevelopmental lens, taking into account factors that contribute toward the initial development of the mask and that drive its maintenance.