ArticlePublisher preview available

Support for Increasing Low-Wage Workers’ Compensation: The Role of Fixed-Growth Mindsets About Intelligence

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Approximately 44% of U.S. workers are low-wage workers. Recent years have witnessed a raging debate about whether to raise their minimum wages. Why do some decision-makers support raising wages and others do not? Ten studies (four preregistered) examined people’s beliefs about the malleability of intelligence as a key antecedent. The more U.S. human resource managers (Study 1) and Indian business owners (Study 2) believed that people’s intelligence can grow (i.e., had a growth mindset), the more they supported increasing low-wage workers’ compensation. In key U.S. swing states (Study 3a), and a nationally representative sample (Study 3b), residents with a more growth mindset were more willing to support ballot propositions increasing the minimum wage and other compensation. Study 4 provided causal evidence. The next two studies confirmed the specificity of the predictor. People’s beliefs about the malleability of intelligence, but not personality (Study 5a) or effort (Study 5b), predicted their support for increasing low-wage workers’ compensation. Study 6 examined multiple potential mechanisms, including empathy, attributions for poverty, and environmental affordances. The relationship between growth mindset and support for raising low-wage workers’ wages was explained by more situational rather than dispositional attributions for poverty. Finally, Studies 7a and 7b replicated the effect of growth mindset on support for increasing low-wage workers’ compensation and provided confirmatory evidence for the mediator—situational, rather than dispositional, attributions of poverty. These findings suggest that growth mindsets about intelligence promote support for increasing low-wage workers’ wages; we discuss the theoretical and practical implications.
Support for Increasing Low-Wage WorkersCompensation: The Role of
Fixed-Growth Mindsets About Intelligence
Shilpa Madan
1
, Anyi Ma
2
, Neeraj Pandey
3
, Aneeta Rattan
4
, and Krishna Savani
5, 6
1
Department of Marketing, Pamplin College of Business, Virginia Tech
2
Management Area, AB Freeman School of Business, Tulane University
3
Amrut Mody School of Management, Ahmedabad University
4
Department of Organisational Behaviour, London Business School
5
Department of Management and Marketing, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University
6
Department of Leadership, Management, and Organization, Nanyang Technological University
Approximately 44% of U.S. workers are low-wage workers. Recent years have witnessed a raging
debate about whether to raise their minimum wages. Why do some decision-makers support raising
wages and others do not? Ten studies (four preregistered) examined peoples beliefs about the malleabil-
ity of intelligence as a key antecedent. The more U.S. human resource managers (Study 1) and Indian
business owners (Study 2) believed that peoples intelligence can grow (i.e., had a growth mindset), the
more they supported increasing low-wage workerscompensation. In key U.S. swing states (Study 3a),
and a nationally representative sample (Study 3b), residents with a more growth mindset were more
willing to support ballot propositions increasing the minimum wage and other compensation. Study 4
provided causal evidence. The next two studies conrmed the specicity of the predictor. Peoples
beliefs about the malleability of intelligence, but not personality (Study 5a) or effort (Study 5b), pre-
dicted their support for increasing low-wage workerscompensation. Study 6 examined multiple poten-
tial mechanisms, including empathy, attributions for poverty, and environmental affordances. The
relationship between growth mindset and support for raising low-wage workerswages was explained
by more situational rather than dispositional attributions for poverty. Finally, Studies 7a and 7b repli-
cated the effect of growth mindset on support for increasing low-wage workerscompensation and pro-
vided conrmatory evidence for the mediatorsituational, rather than dispositional, attributions of
poverty. These ndings suggest that growth mindsets about intelligence promote support for increasing
low-wage workerswages; we discuss the theoretical and practical implications.
Keywords: low-wage workers, lay theories, minimum wage, fixed-growth mindsets, attributions
Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001303.supp
As many as 53 million Americans, or 44% of all workers aged
1864, are employed in low-wage jobs (Ross & Bateman, 2019).
Often, these workers are unable to afford basic necessities, such as
food and shelter, and rely on government programs (e.g., food
stamps, public housing) to meet their basic needs (Belser & Rani,
2010;U.S. Census Bureau, 2013;U.S. Department of Labor,
2013a,2013b). In recent years, perhaps spurred by workersadvo-
cacy and public debate, several entities have taken steps toward
increasing the compensation of low-wage workers. For example,
some cities (e.g., New York City; Washington, D.C.; San Fran-
cisco), states (e.g., Arizona, Colorado, and Maine), and global cor-
porations (e.g., Costco, Ikea, Starbucks, and Whole Foods) have
raised their minimum wages in recent years (Kaufman, 2017;
National Conference of State Legislatures, 2017). Yet, many cor-
porations, localities, and small businesses continue to comply only
with the minimum established compensation standards (Hiltzik,
2020), which have not been raised nationally in the United States
since 2009 (Elwell, 2014).
Widespread stereotypes characterize these workersjobs as
low-skilled or unskilled, and the workers themselves are stereo-
typed as lacking intelligence or competence (Fiske et al., 2002,p.
885). If peoples judgments of low-wage workerscompensation
are based in part on their views of the workersintelligence or
Shilpa Madan https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8224-177X
Krishna Savani https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6934-1917
We received feedback from participants at the Society for Personality
and Social Psychology Conference in 2017 and 2022 and the Academy of
Management Conference in 2014, 2015, and 2022. We thank Carol Dweck
and Lisa Shu for helpful feedback on previous drafts, and Andrea Low,
Dayana Bulchand, Velvetina Lim, Yu Ding, and Janice Foo for generous
research assistance. The study materials, data, and analysis code are
available at https://osf.io/jtqmv/ (Madan, Ma, et al., 2022).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Krishna
Savani, Department of Management and Marketing, The Hong Kong
Polytechnic University, Li Ka Shing Tower, Hung Hom, Kowloon, Hong
Kong. Email: krishna.savani@polyu.edu.hk
1
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
©2022 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 0096-3445 https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001303
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2023, Vol. 152, No. 4, 935–955
935
This article was published Online First October 31, 2022.
... In one series of studies, people who endorsed fixed mindsets about whether people can change the kind of person they are (i.e., kind of person mindsets) were less supportive of immigration policies that provided support for resettling refugees, as they believed refugees would be unlikely to assimilate into their society (Madan et al., 2019). Similarly, people who endorsed fixed intelligence mindsets were less likely to support policies to increase wages for low-wage workers, who are stereotyped as having relatively low levels of intellectual ability (Madan et al., 2023). These findings illustrate that when people believe that aspects of other people are relatively unchangeable, they are more likely to support existing policies and thus, in many cases, to preserve aspects of the existing social system that contribute to certain groups' marginalization. ...
... Integration of multiple measures of system legitimization allows for a relatively robust examination of the relationship between intelligence mindsets and system legitimizing beliefs and policy preferences. By meta-analyzing these data, we aim to contribute to an emerging body of literature illustrating how individuals' mindsets shape their preferences for society (e.g., Madan et al., 2019Madan et al., , 2020Madan et al., , 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Growth and fixed mindsets (i.e., beliefs about whether people’s abilities can be developed) shape how people interpret and respond to events in their social worlds. The present work examines how these mindsets relate to individuals’ likelihood of legitimizing racial and socioeconomic inequities in education. Meta-analyses of 20 original studies including K-12 teachers and staff members (k = 8; N ≈ 2001), college students (k = 7; N ≈ 2725), and American adults (k = 5; N ≈ 1792) demonstrated that the belief that intelligence is an unchangeable characteristic (i.e., fixed mindset) consistently predicted greater likelihood of legitimizing educational inequities via five different measures of legitimization. These results suggest that fixed mindsets about intelligence are likely to undermine policy-driven efforts to change inequitable educational systems, and that one pathway toward educational equity involves attending to prevailing beliefs about the possibilities of growing one’s intelligence.
... Possessing a growth mindset is associated with numerous anti-prejudice outcomes including, willingness to reading information about intergroup biases (Kern et al., 2023) and more positive expectation associated with confronting bias in the workplace (Rattan & Dweck, 2018). Growth mindset is also associated with social justice position, such as support for resettling refugees (Madan et al., 2019), integration of immigrants (Lou & Noels, 2020), more just compensation for low-wage workers (Madan et al., 2023), and a lower tendency to legitimize inequities in educational settings (Brady et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
We explore how the growth mindset relates to intercultural and global citizenship competencies in three Southeast Asian countries (Indonesia, Philippines, Thailand). Using data from the global competencies self-report survey and cognitive assessment of PISA 2018, we hypothesized that intercultural attitudes (i.e., openness, respect, global mindedness) will be positively associated with cognitive global competence, and that growth mindset will amplify this positive association, consistent with research on the benefits of possessing a growth mindset. Separate hierarchical multiple regression analyses for each country indicated partial but substantial support for the hypotheses. In all countries, growth mindset boosted the relationship between global mindedness and global competence; growth mindset also enhanced the relationship between respect and global competence, except in Indonesia. Growth mindset did not enhance the positive association between openness and global competence. The results provide additional support for the benefits of growth mindset in a learning domain that has previously not been extensively studied; specifically, in strengthening the links between intercultural attitudes and global competency, but not for the more surface attitude (openness). The results indicate how the psychological mechanisms associated with growth mindset could create pathways to strengthening global citizenship competencies.
Article
One approach to alleviate poverty is to identify and allow consumers to purchase poverty alleviation products (PAPs) from low‐income agricultural producers in rural areas. We explore whether consumers' attribution of the cause of poverty affects their purchase intentions for PAPs. We differentiate consumer attribution into situational and dispositional and find that situational attribution does not affect purchase intentions, whereas dispositional attribution negatively does. Furthermore, we find that consumer attitudes toward the government's consumption poverty alleviation (CPA) policies may mediate the direct impact of poverty attribution on PAP purchase intention. Consumers generally value CPA policies consistent with their attribution and the mediation effect strengthens the direct effects. This study offers insights into consumer purchase of products designed to fight poverty and provides policy recommendations that can mobilize poverty reduction through product development and consumer endorsement.
Article
El presente artículo de revisión tuvo como objetivo describir al salario emocional desde la perspectiva organizacional en países de América Latina y establecer las principales características y medios para lograr su implementación efectiva, del proceso de revisión sistemática a 80 artículos científicos en las bases de búsqueda de Scopus y Web of Science, se seleccionaron para la elaboración del presente artículo de revisión 60 investigaciones por su pertinencia y relevancia, empleando los principios de ética investigativa, resultando que la implementación del salario emocional en países de América Latina, dependerá del sector, el país y la adaptabilidad organizacional. Sin embargo, del análisis realizado es indudable que el salario emocional es una estrategia de suma importancia que genera bienestar emocional y psicológico; permitiendo el incremento del desempeño laboral en los colaboradores. Finalmente, se concluye que esta se caracteriza por la flexibilidad y la generación de un ambiente laboral positivo, además de promover el desarrollo profesional y personal de los colaboradores, fomentando la adquisición de competencias organizacionales.
Article
Full-text available
People are excessively confident that they can judge others’ characteristics from their appearance. This research identifies a novel antecedent of this phenomenon. Ten studies (N=2,967, four pre-registered) find that the more people believe that appearance reveals character, the more confident they are in their appearance-based judgments, and therefore, the more they support the use of facial profiling technologies in law enforcement, education, and business. Specifically, people who believe that appearance reveals character support the use of facial profiling in general (Studies 1a-1b), and even when they themselves are the target of profiling (Studies 1c-1d). Experimentally inducing people to believe that appearance reveals character increases their support for facial profiling (Study 2) because it increases their confidence in the ability to make appearance-based judgments (Study 3). An intervention that undermines people’s confidence in their appearance-based judgments reduces their support for facial profiling (Study 4). The relationship between the lay theory and support for facial profiling is weaker among people with a growth mindset about personality, as facial profiling presumes a relatively unchanging character (Study 5a). This relationship is also weaker among people who believe in free will, as facial profiling presumes that individuals have limited free will (Study 5b). The appearance reveals character lay theory is a stronger predictor of support for profiling than analogous beliefs in other domains, such as the belief that FacebookTM likes reveal personality (Study 6). These findings identify a novel lay theory that underpins people’s meta-cognitions about their confidence in appearance-related judgments and their policy positions.
Chapter
Full-text available
Beliefs play a central role in human development. For instance, a growth mindset--a belief about the malleability of intelligence--can shape how adolescents interpret and respond to academic difficulties and how they subsequently navigate the educational system. But do usually-adaptive beliefs have the same effects for adolescents regardless of the contexts they are in? Answering this question can reveal new insights into classic developmental questions about continuity and change. Here we present the Mindset x Context framework and we apply this model to the instructive case of growth mindset interventions. We show that teaching students a growth mindset is most effective in educational contexts that provide affordances for a growth mindset; that is, contexts that permit and encourage students to view ability as developable and to act on that belief. This evidence contradicts the "beliefs alone" hypothesis, which holds that teaching adolescents a growth mindset is enough and that students can profit from these beliefs in almost any context, even unsupportive ones. The Mindset x Context framework leads to the realization that in order to produce more widespread and lasting change, we must complement the belief-changing interventions that have been aimed at students with new interventions that guide teachers toward classroom policies and practices that allow students' growth mindset beliefs to take root and yield benefits.
Article
Full-text available
Amazon's Mechanical Turk is widely used for data collection; however, data quality may be declining due to the use of virtual private servers to fraudulently gain access to studies. Unfortunately, we know little about the scale and consequence of this fraud, and tools for social scientists to detect and prevent this fraud are underdeveloped. We first analyze 38 studies and show that this fraud is not new, but has increased recently. We then show that these fraudulent respondents provide particularly low-quality data and can weaken treatment effects. Finally, we provide two solutions: an easy-to-use application for identifying fraud in the existing datasets and a method for blocking fraudulent respondents in Qualtrics surveys.
Article
To better understand the cognitive antecedents of teachers’ stress in the school setting, the effects of teachers’ beliefs regarding intelligence and their causal attributions of students’ academic performance on teachers’ job stress were examined in the current study. We recruited 271 teachers who voluntarily filled out an anonymous questionnaire. Findings demonstrated that teachers who endorsed the entity theory of intelligence tended to attribute students’ poor performance to students’ inability but not lack of effort. Results showed a positive relationship between teachers’ fixed view of intelligence and job stress, and this relationship was mediated by teachers’ attributing students’ poor performance to students’ lack of ability, even after controlling for teacher efficacy. We suggest to promote the psychological health of teachers by incorporating intelligence mindset and attribution training programs in preservice teacher education, professional development, and cognitive behavioral stress intervention for in‐service teachers.
Article
Decades of research have shown that people’s mindsets beliefs—their beliefs about the fixedness or malleability of talent, ability, and intelligence—can powerfully influence their motivation, engagement, and performance. This article explores the role of mindsets in organizational contexts. We start by describing the evolution of mindset theory and research and review why mindsets matter for people’s workplace outcomes. We discuss some of the most common growth mindset misconceptions—termed “false growth mindset”—that emerged as the fixed and growth mindset became popularized and (mis)applied in educational settings. We review literature on the situations that move people between their fixed and growth mindsets. Finally, we review new research on organizational mindsets and how organizations’ mindset culture—communicated through its norms, policies, practices, and leadership messages—influences people’s motivation and behavior in the workplace. We outline open theoretical and methodological questions as well as promising future directions for a forward-looking research agenda on mindsets at work. We suggest that extending mindset research—at the personal and organizational levels—to workplace contexts may shed new light on classic organizational behavior questions such as how to create more positive, innovative, and ethical organizational cultures; how to increase employee engagement; and how to reduce group-based disparities and inequalities in organizations.
Article
The goal of the current manuscript is to embed the theory of mindsets about malleability in workplace contexts. We first define fixed-growth mindsets and the methods that have to date been used to study them. We then briefly review the domains in which mindsets have been documented to shape outcomes meaningfully, linking each to exciting research questions that we hope will soon be studied in workplace contexts. We also highlight some of the fascinating, new questions scholars can study by considering how mindsets might shape outcomes across a diversity of workplaces (e.g., the workforce of low wage and vulnerable populations). We further propose that studying mindsets in workplace contexts can develop mindset theory. We first ask whether workplace contexts provide opportunities to test for moderation on mindset expression. Second, we see opportunity for studying moderation of mindset processes – evaluating whether the psychological processes through which mindsets shape outcomes may differ based on contextual factors that vary across workplaces. We argue that investigating these possibilities will advance both the theory of mindsets about malleability and the study of human flourishing in the workplace. We invite scholars to join us in this endeavour.
Article
The novel Coronavirus that spread around the world in early 2020 triggered a global pandemic and economic downturn that affected nearly everyone. Yet the crisis had a disproportionate impact on the poor and revealed how easily working-class individuals' financial security can be destabilised by factors beyond personal control. In a pre-registered longitudinal study of Americans (N = 233) spanning April 2019 to May 2020, we tested whether the pandemic altered beliefs about the extent to which poverty is caused by external forces and internal dispositions and support for economic inequality. Over this timespan, participants revealed a shift in their attributions for poverty, reporting that poverty is more strongly impacted by external-situational causes and less by internal-dispositional causes. However, we did not detect an overall mean-level change in opposition to inequality or support for government intervention. Instead, only for those who most strongly recognized the negative impact of COVID-19 did changes in poverty attributions translate to decreased support for inequality, and increased support for government intervention to help the poor.
Article
Growth mindsets are increasingly used to promote learning, development, and health. The increased popularity resulted in scrutiny and disputes about utility. The current work reviews a perspective critical to the debate. Namely, we focus on emerging research that examines both the favorable and potentially adverse consequences of growth mindset messaging in stigma-relevant contexts. This double-edged sword model merges the mindset perspective with attribution theory and the psychological essentialism literature. In stigmatizing contexts and in isolation, growth mindsets can indirectly predict less positive outcomes, via personal responsibility for the problem, but more positive outcomes, via expectations for the potential to manage conditions in the future. Programmatic research illustrates how to tailor growth mindset messages and interventions, to avoid the potential costs of blame, yet keep the benefits of self-efficacy and weakened essentialism.