- Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
- Learn more
Download available
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports
1‑Octanol emitted by Oecophylla
smaragdina weaver ants
repels and deters oviposition
in Queensland fruit y
Vivek Kempraj*, Soo Jean Park, Donald N. S. Cameron & Phillip W. Taylor
Humans have used weaver ants, Oecophylla smaragdina, as biological control agents to control
insect pests in orchards for many centuries. Over recent decades, the eectiveness of weaver ants as
biological control agents has been attributed in part to deterrent and oviposition inhibiting eects
of kairomones produced by the ants, but the chemical identity of these kairomones has remained
unknown. We have identied the kairomone responsible for deterrence and oviposition inhibition by
O. smaragdina, providing a signicant advance in understanding the chemical basis of their predator/
prey interactions. Olfactometer assays with extracts from weaver ants demonstrated headspace
volatiles to be highly repellent to Queensland fruit y, Bactrocera tryoni. Using electrophysiology
and bioassays, we demonstrate that this repellence is induced by a single compound, 1‑octanol. Of
16 compounds identied in O. smaragdina headspace, only 1‑octanol evoked an electrophysiological
response from B. tryoni antennae. Flies had greatly reduced oviposition and spent signicantly less
time in an olfactometer arm in the presence of 1‑octanol or a synthetic blend of headspace volatiles
containing 1‑octanol than in the presence of a synthetic blend of headspace volatiles without
1‑octanol, or clean air. Taken together, our results demonstrate that 1‑octanol is the functional
kairomone component of O. smaragdina headspace that explains repellence and oviposition
deterrence, and is hence an important contributor to the eectiveness of these ants as biological
control agents.
Weaver ants are voracious predators and have been used as biological control agents for many centuries to
control insect pests in Asia (Oecophylla smaragdina) and in Africa (Oecophylla longinoda). A common practice
in Asia involves establishing a nest on one tree and then connecting it to adjacent trees with bamboo poles,
thus enabling the movement of ants throughout the orchard to forage1,2. Records of this practice can be found
in the 1726 Imperial Encyclopedia of the Ching dynasty and in a regional botany work written by Ji Han in
3042. Weaver ants have been found to be eective in controlling insect pests of mango3–8, cashew9,10, citrus11,12,
coconut13 and cocoa14,15. While direct eects of predation by weaver ants are certainly important, recent studies
have highlighted that repellence and oviposition deterrence induced by chemical emissions (kairomones) from
the ants are also important elements of crop protection conferred by weaver ants5,16. Unidentied weaver ant-
produced kairomones have been found to inhibit oviposition by fruit y pests including Bactrocera jarvisi5, B.
dorsalis and Ceratitis cosyra17,18. In the eld, damage to mango fruits by B. jarvisi is decreased in the presence
of O. smaragdina5. When presented with O. longinoda-exposed and unexposed mango fruits in the absence of
ants, B. dorsalis and C. cosyra land less oen on ant-exposed fruits and if they do land tend to depart quickly
and fail to oviposit17. Volatile olfactory cues from O. smaragdina induce increases in motility (velocity, active
time and distance moved) and reductions in foraging, oviposition and mating propensity in the Queensland
fruit y Bactrocera tryoni19.
While numerous studies have demonstrated fruit y responses to kairomones from weaver ants, and have
strongly implicated such kairomones as an important element of biological control5,17,18, the specic compounds
responsible remain unknown. Weaver ants are known to emit and deposit a diversity of compounds, including
hydrocarbons, esters, fatty acids, terpenes, and alcohols. Hydrocarbons make up ~ 90% of the compounds emit-
ted by O. smaragdina with n-undecane being a highly emitted compound (~ 45%)20. Identifying kairomones
that mediate responses of prey to predators can provide valuable insights to subtle aspects of predator–prey
interactions and can also provide insights into how kairomones aect food webs21,22. Furthermore, when a
OPEN
Applied BioSciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia. *email: vivek.kempraj@gmail.com
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
predator-released kairomone repels or deters oviposition in a pest species, it can have a signicant impact on
pest populations23,24 and may even be developed as an eective pest management tool. In the present study, we
identify a single compound from the headspace volatiles emitted by O. smaragdina that is detected by antennae,
induces repellence, and deters oviposition in B. tryoni. is knowledge signicantly advances understanding of
predator–prey interactions between weaver ants and fruit ies, and lays the foundations for the development of
biologically inspired repellents that could oer a new tool for non-insecticidal, safe, management of economi-
cally important fruit ies.
Results
Olfactometer screening of extracts and volatile emissions of O. smaragdina revealed that only head extract and
headspace volatiles repel B. tryoni (Fig.1). Male and female ies spent signicantly more time in the control
arm of olfactometers (Male: 5.129 ± 0.65min (mean ± s.e.m), t = 2.383, df = 19, P = 0.02; Female: 5.885 ± 0.73min,
t = 2.249, df = 19, P = 0.03; Fig.1e) than in the treatment arm containing head extract (Male: 2.916 ± 0.45min;
Female: 3.459 ± 0.54min; Fig. 1e). A similar, but a stronger repellence was observed in response to head-
space volatiles of O. smaragdina. Male and female ies spent signicantly more time in the control arm (Male:
7.620 ± 0.34min; t = 9.839; df = 19; P < 0.001; Female: 6.713 ± 0.73min, t = 9.370, df = 19, P < 0.001; Fig.1f ) than
in the treatment arm containing headspace volatiles (Male: 1.498 ± 0.36min; Female: 0.3755 ± 0.14min; Fig.1f).
Headspace volatiles were explored further by Gas Chromatography-Electroantennographic Detection (GC-
EAD) to identify compounds that might be responsible for repellence of B. tryoni. Both male and female ies
responded very consistently to a single compound in the headspace volatiles, and from Gas chromatograph
mass spectrometry (GC–MS) analysis the electrophysiologically active compound was found to be 1-octanol
(Fig.2). Although we observed a single compound to be electrophysiologically active on antennae, which are
thought to mediate long range olfactory responses, B. tryoni do have olfactory receptors on other body parts
(e.g., maxillary palps)25 and so the possibility remained that other compounds may be detected by organs other
than those on antennae and be responsible for repellence. To conrm the functional eect of 1-octanol as a
repellent, we prepared two synthetic blends of headspace volatiles, one that contained all components includ-
ing 1-octanol (BL+OL) and one that contained all components except 1-octanol (BL−OL). In olfactometer assays,
male and female ies were not repelled by the blend BL−OL, spending similar amounts of time in the control arm
(Male: 4.848 ± 0.53min; t = 0.4238; df = 19; P = 0.67; Female: 4.459 ± 0.28min, t = 1.106, df = 19, P = 0.28; Fig.3a)
and the treatment arm (Male: 4.481 ± 0.55min; Female: 3.922 ± 0.35min). However, when presented with the
blend BL+OL, both male and female ies spent signicantly more time in the control arm (Male: 5.562 ± 0.64min;
t = 4.406; df = 19; P < 0.001; Female: 5.136 ± 0.61min, t = 7.635, df = 19, P < 0.001; Fig.3b) than in the treatment
arm (Male: 1.669 ± 0.47min; Female: 0.366 ± 0.12min). Next, 1-octanol alone was evaluated for its deterrence
of B. tryoni to test whether ies responded to 1-octanol outside the context of ant volatiles. Male and female ies
spent signicantly more time in the control arm (Male: 7.738 ± 0.59min; t = 12.110, df = 19, P < 0.001; Female:
6.354 ± 0.55min; t = 8.299, df = 19, P < 0.001; Fig.3c) than in the treatment arm containing 1-octanol (Male:
0.7765 ± 0.18min; Female: 1.424 ± 0.17min).
In oviposition assays, gravid females laid signicantly more eggs into control agarose plates (113.2 ± 14.05
eggs; mean ± s.e.m) and BL−OL plates (containing all headspace components except 1-octanol) (71.7 ± 6.33 eggs)
than into BL+OL plates (containing all headspace components including 1-octanol) (2.5 ± 0.73 eggs) or OL plates
(containing only 1-octanol) (2.2 ± 0.59 eggs)(F3, 36 = 50.2; P < 0.001; Fig. 4). e presence of 1-octanol almost
completely inhibited oviposition, but it is important also to note that the eect was by contact or short range
olfaction because such inhibition did not carry over to other plates in the same cage. Taken together, our results
demonstrate that 1-octanol is responsible for kairomonal eects of repellence and oviposition deterrence in B.
tryoni that are exposed to olfactory cues from O. smaragdina weaver ants.
Discussion
Kairomones released by predators can signicantly inuence prey species behaviour and life history26–29. Despite
numerous studies demonstrating kairomonal eects of olfactory cues released by predators21,28,30, there are sur-
prisingly few studies providing chemical characterisation of such kairomones31–33.
Although olfactory cues produced by weaver ants (O. smaragdina in Asia and Australia and O. longinoda
in Africa) have been known to have a strong repellent eect on fruit ies3–8,11,12, the present study is the rst to
chemically identify and demonstrate eects of kairomonal components. Active compounds in the headspace
appear to originate in the head of O. smaragdina. 1-octanol was found to be the only one of 16 compounds in the
headspace to elicit electrophysiological responses in B. tryoni antennae, and the kairomonal function of 1-octanol
as a repellent and oviposition deterrent was demonstrated using bioassays that presented headspace blends with
and without 1-octanol. GC–MS analysis of the body extracts and volatile emissions revealed 1-octanol in the head
extract and headspace volatiles. 1-Octanol was previously reported from head extracts and mandibular glands
of O. smaragdina20,34,35, and also in headspace20. Previous studies have found that 1-octanol in honeybee alarm
pheromone repels the parasitic mite, Varroa jacobsoni36. Octanol (unspecied conguration) has been reported
as a minor component in the endocrine secretions of cockroaches, although no function has been identied37.
1-Octanol in essential oils of plants has been reported as a biting deterrent in the mosquito Aedes aegypti38 and
as an oviposition deterrent in the Asian corn borer, Ostrinia furnacalis39. A recent study has shown 1-octanol to
be a major component of the alarm pheromones in a mammal, the Bank vole Myodes glareolus40. However, the
function of 1-octanol in O. smaragdina is currently unknown. Given the alarm pheromone function of 1-octanol
in honeybees, also a social hymenopteran, a similar function warrants investigation in O. smaragdina.
Gravid B. tryoni rely on fruit volatiles when detecting and choosing fruits for oviposition41–43. We used
γ-octalactone, a strong oviposition stimulant of B. tryoni44 and a short-range attractant in some tephritid fruit
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
ies45, to establish a high baseline of oviposition in order to demonstrate the substantial inhibitory eects of
1-octanol. In oviposition assays, 1-octanol in the presence or absence of other O. smaragdina headspace com-
ponents over-rode the oviposition-stimulating eect of γ-octalactone resulting in very low levels of oviposition.
Identifying and characterizing predator-released kairomones paves the way for more detailed studies of how
prey behaviour and food web structure can be aected by such public information. Identifying the predator-
released kairomones that inuence oviposition by B. tryoni and other fruit ies also provides foundations for the
development of new, sustainable, pest management tools. Kairomone-based repellents and oviposition deterrents,
such as 1-octanol, could potentially be exploited to protect crops and reduce reliance on environmentally harmful
insecticides. In addition to eects on gravid female B. tryoni, 1-octanol was found to be repellent to male ies
and could potentially contribute to reduced mating in pest populations.
Figure1. Behavioural response of male and female B. tryoni to O. smaragdina body extracts and volatiles. (A)
cuticular compounds; CH, (B) Dufour gland; DG, (C) poison gland; PG, (D) trail extract; TR (E) head extract;
HD and (F) headspace volatiles; HS were tested. Only head extract and headspace volatiles signicantly repelled
ies. Male and female ies spent signicantly more time in control arm (Yeast Hydrolysate; YH) than the
treatment arm (Yeast Hydrolysate + Head extract or Yeast Hydrolysate + Headspace voltiles). Error bar represent
s.e.m. Signicant dierence was analysed by paired t-test (see Results).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Materials and methods
Insects. Bactrocera tryoni were obtained from a colony that originated from infested fruit collected in central
coastal New South Wales and had been maintained in a controlled environment laboratory (25 ± 0.5°C, 65 ± 5%
RH, photoperiod of 11.5:0.5:11.5:0.5 light: dusk: dark: dawn) at Macquarie University for 32 generations. From
emergence, adult ies were fed yeast hydrolysate, sugar and water adlibitum and were used in experiments when
10 to 15days old, when sexually mature46. Major workers of O. smaragdina were collected from ve dierent
colonies in the vicinity of Mareeba Research Facility, Department of Agriculture and Fisheries, QLD, Australia
(17.00724°S, 145.42984°E).
Chemicals. Authentic standards of 1-hexanol, decane,p-cymene, D-limonene, γ-terpinene, 1-octanol, dihy-
dromyrcenol, undecane, nonanal, dodecane, tridecane, 1-tetradecene, tetradecane, pentadecane, hexadecane,
heptadecane (all known components of emissions produced by O. smaragdina)20 and hexane were purchased
from Sigma-Aldrich. All chemicals were of analytical grade (≥ 98% purity) and were used without further puri-
cation.
Collection of body & gland extracts, volatile emissions, and trail extracts. Cuticular com-
pounds, head extracts, gland extracts (Dufour and poison glands), headspace volatiles and trail extracts of O.
smaragdina were collected as described by Kempraj etal.20. For cuticular compounds, individual ants (n = 100)
were dipped in 10mL of hexane for 10s. For head extracts, heads of ants (n = 10) were removed with dissection
scissors and immediately placed in 1.5mL of hexane in a glass vial for 24h. e extraction time for cuticular
compounds and head extract was crucial in achieving dierentiation in the compounds extracted. e extended
extraction time for head extracts enabled extraction of glandular compounds present in the head (mandibular
glands, intramandibular glands, propharyngeal and postpharyngeal glands), whereas the short extraction time
for cuticular compounds was enough to extract compounds on the cuticle without signicant extractions from
glands. For gland extracts, Dufour and poison glands were dissected from the abdomen and remnant tissues
were carefully removed using ne forceps. Clean glands (n = 10) were immediately placed into 1.5mL of hexane
in a glass vial. Glands were extracted by standing the vial at room temperature for 24h. Headspace volatiles
present in the air surrounding the ants was collected using an air entrainment system. Ten ants were placed in a
cylindrical glass chamber (120mL capacity) with an inlet and outlet and were allowed to acclimatize for 30min
prior to collection of volatiles. A charcoal lter was connected to the inlet (4mm ID) of the glass chamber using
Tygon tubing (E-3603). e outlet of the glass chamber was connected to a Tenax tube (50mg, Scientic Instru-
ment Services Inc, Tenax-GR Mesh 60/80, packed in 6 × 50mm glass tubes) tted to a screw cap with O-ring.
Nine chambers containing ants and one empty control chamber were set up for each run. Headspace volatiles
were adsorbed onto Tenax at a ow rate of 0.5 L/min for 30min by pulling air from the outlet using a pump
(KNF Pumps, Model no. NMP850.1.2KNDCB, Switzerland). For trail extracts, we found a metal fence that
served as a regular path to transport food and other materials to the nest by O. smaragdina. Prior to collection,
the section of metal fence (ca.3m) that the ants used to commute was rinsed with acetone (100mL) to remove
pre-existing trail chemicals. e ants were allowed to make a trail on the rinsed section of the mesh for 24h.
Between 2 and 4pm Standard Australian Time (when weaver ants are highly active) the metal wire was rinsed,
section by section, with a total of 100mL hexane into a 500mL glass beaker. e trail wash was concentrated
under a gentle stream of clean air down to approximately 10mL. All collections were at least ten replicates and
stored at 4°C until further processing. Samples of body extracts and gland extracts were treated with a drying
agent (sodium sulfate) and by gravity ltration with a glass wool plugged Pasteur pipette to remove water and
debris. Samples free from water and debris were concentrated under a gentle stream of nitrogen gas. Cuticular
compound samples were concentrated to 1mL while Dufour’s gland, poison gland and head samples were con-
centrated to 0.5mL. Trail samples were ltered to remove solid matter and concentrated to 1mL under a gentle
stream of nitrogen gas. Headspace volatile samples did not require further processing. All processed samples
were stored at − 20°C until analysis.
Figure2. Representative GC-EAD recording of male and female B. tryoni response to headspace volatiles of
O. smaragdina. In both male and female ies the FID peak marked as ‘1-octanol’ was the only compound that
elicited consistent response.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Gas chromatography mass spectrometry (GC–MS) analysis. GC–MS analysis of all samples were
carried out on a Shimadzu GC–MS TQ8030 spectrometer equipped with a split/splitless injector and SH RTX-
5MS (30m × 0.25mm, 0.25µm lm) fused silica capillary column. Carrier gas was helium (99.999%) at a ow
rate of 1mL/min. An aliquot of 1 µL was injected in splitless mode, with injector temperature set at 270°C. e
temperature program was as follows: 50°C for 1min, increased to 280°C at 10°C min−1 and increased to 300°C
at 5°C min−1. e ion source and transfer line temperatures were 200°C and 290°C respectively. e ionization
method was electron impact at a voltage of 70eV. Spectra were obtained over a mass range of m/z 45–650. For
the identication of compounds, mass fragmentation patterns were compared with NIST library (NIST17-1,
NIST17-2, NIST17s) and Kovats retention indices were compared with literature values. e identities of the
compounds were conrmed by comparing retention index and fragmentation patterns of each compound with
authentic standards.
Figure3. Behavioural response of male and female B. tryoni to synthetic blends with (BL+OL) or without
(BL−OL) 1-octanol, and 1-octanol alone (OL). In behavioral assays using synthetic blend of headspace voltiles
without 1- octanol, male and female ies spent similar time in control (YH; Yeast hydrolysate) and treatment
(YH + BL−OL) arms. However, in behavioural assays using a synthetic blend of headspace volatiles with 1-octanol
or 1-octanol alone, male and female ies spent signicantly (P < 0.001) more time in control (YH) than
treatment (YH + BL+OL) arms. Error bar represent s.e.m. Signicant dierence was analysed by paired t-test (see
Results).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Electrophysiology. Coupled Gas Chromatography-Electroantennographic Detection (GC-EAD) record-
ings were made using Ag-glass microelectrodes lled with electroconductive gel (Spectra 360, Parker Laborato-
ries Inc., USA) (n = 6). A male or gravid femaleof B. tryoni was subdued by chilling, and the head was separated
from the body using a microscalpel. e base of the head was then xed to the tip of the gel-lled indierent
electrode. e tip of an antenna was placed in contact with the recording electrode and was slightly inserted into
the gel to stabilize the antenna. e signals were passed through a high impedance amplier (UN-06, Syntech,
Hilversum, e Netherlands). Headspace samples were tested by injecting of 1µl of sample into the GC column.
Euent from the GC column was simultaneously directed to the antennal preparation and the GC detector
at a split ratio of 1.5:1, respectively. Separation of compounds was achieved on a Agilent GC 7890B equipped
with a split/splitless injector and a ame ionization detector (FID), using an HP-5 column (30m, 0.32mm ID,
0.25μm lm, Agilent, CA, US). e carrier gas was hydrogen (99.999%) (BOC, North Ryde, NSW, Australia) at
a ow rate of 3.0mL/min. e injector temperature was 270°C. e oven temperature was maintained at 45°C
for 2min, and then increased to 250°C at 10°C min−1. e outputs from the EAG amplier and the FID were
monitored simultaneously by GcEad soware ver. 1.2.5 (Syntech, Kirchzarten, Germany). Peaks eluting from
the GC column were judged to be active if they elicited EAD activity in six or more of the ten coupled runs. e
identities of FID peaks were conrmed by GC–MS (Shimadzu TQ8030) operating at the same GC conditions
with the same type of column (5% diphenyl and 95% dimethyl polysiloxane).
Preparation of synthetic blends of headspace volatiles. GC–MS results of weaver ant headspace
samples guided the preparation of two synthetic blends. e 16 identied headspace compounds20 were used to
prepare two synthetic blends that matched the relative abundance of compounds in the natural blend. One syn-
thetic blend contained all the headspace components including 1-octanol (BL+OL) (BL = Blend; OL = 1-octanol),
while the other synthetic blend contained all the headspace components except 1-octanol (BL−OL). Stock solu-
tions of the headspace compounds with a concentration range of 5.0–10.0mg/mL in hexane were prepared
in 10mL volumetric asks. e stock solutions were run through GC to obtain response factors for the given
concentration. e response factor of undecane was used as a reference to adjust the volumes of each compound
added to the synthetic blend. e calculated volumes of the compounds were added to a 10mL volumetric ask.
e ask was lled with hexane to the mark and inverted several times to mix the blend well. e synthetic blend
was run through GC to conrm if the relative gas chromatographic (GC) intensities of the compounds were
consistent with that in the natural headspace volatile extract. Preparing a synthetic blend and comparing GC
intensities were repeated several times until the relative GC intensities were consistent with that in the natural
headspace volatile extract. e concentration of undecane, the reference compound, was arbitrary each time but
in a range of 10.0 to 15.0μg/mL. e GC conditions used in this process were the same as the above GC–MS
analysis, except that 1μl of sample was injected at split mode (a ratio of 1:60).
Olfactometer bioassays. An acrylic four-arm olfactometer (120mm diameter; see Fig.S1) was used to
assess behavioural responses of male and female B.tryoni to extracts of cuticle, Dufour gland, Poison gland,
Trail and head and volatile emissions of weaver ants as well as synthetic blends (BL+OL, BL−OL) or 1-octanol (OL)
alone. Prior to each experiment, olfactometers were washed with a non-ionic detergent solution, rinsed with
ethanol and distilled water, and le to air dry. Experiments were conducted in a controlled environment room
(25 ± 2°C, 60% RH). To provide traction for the walking insects, lter paper (Whatmann No. 1, 12cm diameter)
was placed on the oor of the central area. e room was illuminated from above by uniform lighting from white
LED lights. Individual ies(10–15days old, without access to food over the preceding 24h, but with access to
Figure4. 1-octanol inhibited oviposition by gravid B. tryoni females. Gravid females were presented with
agarose plates containing oviposition stimulant (OS) alone (control), agarose plates containing OS + synthetic
blend of headspace volatiles excluding 1-octanol (BL−OL), agarose plates containing OS + synthetic blend of
headspace volatiles including 1-octanol (BL+OL) and agarose plates containing OS + 1-octanol (OL). Signicantly
more eggs were laid into control and BL−OL than into BL+OL and OL. Error bars represent s.e.m. Signicant
dierence is denoted by dierent letters (ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test; P < 0.001; n = 10; see Results).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
water) were introduced to the olfactometer through a hole in the oor. Each y was given 5min to acclimatize
in the olfactometer, aer which the experiment was run for 10min. e olfactometer was rotated 90° aer each
replicate to eliminate any directional bias. Air was drawn through the central hole at 200ml min−1and subse-
quently exhausted into the room. e central arena of the olfactometer was divided into four discrete odour
elds corresponding to each of four inlet arms. A choice test was performed that used two opposite arms and
the other two arms were closed and were not used in the test. One arm was for treatment and the opposite arm
was control. Test samples (extracts, volatile emissions, BL+OL, BL−OL or OL-1.17% v/v (10μl; the concentration
of 1-octanol used was similar to the concentration of 1-octanol present in the natural headspace sample)20 and
yeast hydrolysate solution (YH; 6% w/v, 10μl, a feeding stimulant) were tested individually. e test sample was
pipetted onto lter paper strips that were placed into the treatment cylinder through which air was drawn to one
arm of the olfactometer, while the cylinder through which air was drawn to the control arm of the olfactometer
contained only YH (10μl). Fly activity was video recorded. e time spent in each arm was analysed using
BORIS soware ver. 7.9.647. Twenty replicates were conducted for each type of sample.
Oviposition assay. To determine whether 1-octanol is key in deterring oviposition by gravid female ies,
oviposition responses of gravid females were assessed using agarose plates containing an oviposition stimulant
(OS; γ-octalactone)44. Number of eggs oviposited on agarose plates containing synthetic blends of weaver ant
headspace volatiles (BL+OL, BL−OL; 10µl) or 1-octanol (OL; 1.17% v/v in hexane; 10µl) was compared with
number of eggs oviposited on agarose plates containing OS alone (control). Agarose (0.8g in 100ml water) was
melted in a microwave oven, and then cooled to ~ 60°C. OS (0.05% v/v in hexane; 10µl) alone or in combination
with BL+OL, BL−OL or OL (10µl) was added. is mixture was poured into pre-cooled Petri dishes, covered, and
stored for 10min at 4°C. Agarose plates containing OS alone (control) and OS combined with BL+OL, BL−OL and
OL were all provided to gravid females at the same time as a multiple-choice test (50 gravid females; 13–15days
old from mixed sex cages) in mesh cages (45 × 45 × 45cm, BugDorm-4S4545). e plates were placed at four
corners of the mesh cage and were separated by ~ 40cm from each other. Aer 24h, eggs laid in each plate were
counted under a dissecting microscope (Olympus SZX12, Japan). Ten replicates of the assay were conducted.
Statistical analysis. Data from olfactometer assays were subjected to paired t tests to assess whether the
amount of time spent by ies in the olfactometer arms diered signicantly between control and treatment.
Data from oviposition assays were subjected to one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparison test
to compare the treatments. Statistical analysis was preformed using GraphPad Prism, version 9.0 (GraphPad
Soware LLC, USA).
Data availability
e datasets generated and analysed during this current study are available from ResearchGate (https:// doi. org/
10. 13140/ RG.2. 2. 20780. 74882).
Received: 6 February 2022; Accepted: 8 September 2022
References
1. Leston, D. & Leston, D. e ant mosaic-tropical tree crops and the limiting of pests and diseases. PANS Pest Artic. News Summ.
19, 311–341 (1973).
2. Huang, H. T. & Yang, P. A tropical ant is used to control insect pests in southern China. Bioscience 37, 665–671 (1987).
3. Peng, R. K. & Christian, K. e weaver ant, Oecophylla smaragdina (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), an eective biological control
agent of the red-banded thrips, Selenothrips rubrocinctus (ysanoptera: ripidae) in mango crops in the Northern Territory of
Australia. Int. J. Pest Manag. 50, 107–114 (2004).
4. Peng, R. K. & Christian, K. e control ecacy of the weaver ant, Oecophylla smaragdina (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), on the
mango leaopper, Idioscopus nitidulus (Hemiptera: Cicadellidea) in mango orchards in the northern territory). Int. J. Pest Manag.
51, 297–304 (2005).
5. Peng, R. K. & Christian, K. Eective control of Jarvis’s fruit y, Bactrocera jarvisi (Diptera: Tephritidae), by the weaver ant, Oeco-
phylla smaragdina (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), in mango orchards in the Northern Territory of Australia. Int. J. Pest Manag. 52,
275–282 (2006).
6. Peng, R. K. & Christian, K. e eect of the weaver ant, Oecophylla smaragdina (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), on the mango seed
weevil, Sternochetus mangiferae (Coleoptera: Curculionidae), in mango orchards in the Northern Territory of Australia. Int. J. Pest
Manag. 53, 15–24 (2007).
7. Blüthgen, N. & Stork, N. E. Ant mosaics in a tropical rainforest in Australia and elsewhere: A critical review. Aust. Ecol. 32, 93–104
(2007).
8. Davidson, D. W., Lessard, J. P., Bernau, C. R. & Cook, S. C. e tropical ant mosaic in a primary Bornean rain forest. Biotropica
39, 468–475 (2007).
9. Peng, R. K., Christian, K. & Gibb, K. e eect of colony isolation of the predacious ant, Oecophylla smaragdina (f.) (Hymenoptera:
Formicidae), on protection of cashew plantations from insect pests. Int. J. Pest Manag. 45, 189–194 (1999).
10. Peng, R. K., Christian, K. & Gibb, K. Ecology of the fruit spotting bug, Amblypelta lutescens lutescens Distant (Hemiptera: Coreidae)
in cashew plantations, with particular reference to the potential for its biological control. Aust. J. Entomol. 44, 45–51 (2005).
11. Van Mele, P. & Cuc, N. T. T. Evolution and status of Oecophylla smaragdina (Fabricius) as a pest control agent in citrus in the
Mekong Delta, Vietnam. Int. J. Pest Manag. 46, 295–301 (2000).
12. Van Mele, P., Cuc, N. T. T. & VanHuis, A. Direct and indirect inuences of the weaver ant Oecophylla smaragdina on citrus farmers’
pest perceptions and management practices in the Mekong Delta, Vietnam. Int. J. Pest Manag. 48, 225–232 (2002).
13. Kumaresan, V. Prevention of rhinoceros beetle (Oryctes rhinoceros) in coconut palm using red ants. J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc. 93,
308–309 (1996).
14. Way, M. J. & Khoo, K. C. Relationships between Helopeltis theobromae damage and ants with special reference to Malaysian cocoa
small holdings. J. Plant Prot. Trop. 6, 1–11 (1989).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
15. Way, M. J. & Khoo, K. C. Colony dispersion and nesting habits of the ants, Dolichoderus thoracicus and Oecophylla smaragdina
(Hymenoptera: Formicidae), in relation to their success as biological control agents on cocoa. Bull. Entomol. Res. 81, 341–350
(1991).
16. Van Mele, P. Biological Control with the Weaver Ant, Oecophylla longinoda, in Africa: A Review of Research and Development
Eorts to Link Farmers to Organic Markets. Outlooks Pest Manag. 19, 180–183 (2008).
17. Van Mele, P., Vayssieres, J. F., Tellingen, E. V. & Vrolijks, J. Eects of an African weaver ant, Oecophylla longinoda, in controlling
mango fruit ies (Diptera: Tephritidae) in Benin. J. Econ. Entomol. 100, 695–701 (2007).
18. Van Mele, P., Vayssieres, J. F., Adandonon, A. & Sinzogan, A. Ant cues aect the oviposition behaviour of fruit ies (Diptera:
Tephritidae) in Africa. Physiol. Entomol. 34, 256–261 (2009).
19. Kempraj, V., Park, S. P. & Taylor, P. W. Forewarned is forearmed: Queensland fruit ies detect olfactory cues from predators and
respond with predator-specic behaviour. Sci. Rep. 10, 7297 (2020).
20. Kempraj, V., Park, S. J. & Faveri, D. S. Overlooked scents: Chemical prole of soma, volatile emissions and trails of the green tree
ant, Oecophylla smaragdina. Molecules 25, 2112 (2020).
21. Burks, R. L. & Lodge, D. M. Cued in: Advances and opportunities in freshwater chemical ecology. J. Chem. Ecol. 28, 1901–1917
(2002).
22. Fink, P. Ecological functions of volatile organic compounds in aquatic systems. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiol. 40, 155–168 (2007).
23. Spencer, M., Blaustein, L. & Cohen, J. E. Oviposition habitat selection by mosquitoes (Culiseta longiareolata) and consequences
for population size. Ecology 83, 669–679 (2002).
24. Binckley, C. A. & Resetarits, W. J. Habitat selection determines abundance, richness and species composition of beetles in aquatic
communities. Biol. Lett. 1, 370–374 (2005).
25. Biswas, M. J. H., Mainali, B., Park, S. J., Taylor, P. & Rempoulakis, P. Electrophysiological responses to cuelure of raspberry ketone-
fed Queensland fruit y. J. Econ. Entomol. 113, 2832–2839 (2020).
26. Kats, L. B. & Dill, L. M. e scent of death: Chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5, 361–394
(1998).
27. Brönmark, C. & Hansson, L. A. Chemical communication in aquatic systems: An introduction. Oikos 88, 103–109 (2000).
28. Dicke, M. & Grostal, P. Chemical detection of natural enemies by arthropods: An ecological perspective. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 32,
1–23 (2001).
29. Van Donk, E. Chemical information transfer in freshwater plankton. Ecol. Inform. 2, 112–120 (2007).
30. Pohnert, G., Steinke, M. & Tollrian, R. Chemical cues, defence metabolites and the shaping of pelagic interspecic interactions.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 22, 198–204 (2007).
31. Tollrian, R. & von Elert, E. Enrichment and purication of Chaoborus kairomone from water: Further steps toward its chemical
characterization. Limnol. Oceanogr. 39, 788–796 (1994).
32. Von Elert, E. & Loose, C. J. Predator-induced diel vertical migration in Daphnia: Enrichment and preliminary chemical charac-
terization of a kairomone exuded by sh. J. Chem. Ecol. 22, 885–895 (1996).
33. Kusch, J. Self-recognition as the original function of an amoeban defense-inducing kairomone. Ecology 80, 715–720 (1999).
34. Peerzada, N., Pakkiyaretnam, T. & Renaud, S. Volatile constituents of the Green Ant Oecophylla smaragdina. Agric. Biol. Chem.
54, 3335–3336 (1990).
35. Keegans, S. J., Billen, J. & Morgan, E. D. Volatile secretions of the green tree ant Oecophylla smaragdina (Hymenoptera: Formicidae).
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part B Biochem. 100, 681–685 (1991).
36. Kraus, B. eects of honey-bee alarm pheromone compounds on the behaviour of Varroa jacobsoni. Apidologie 21, 127–134 (1990).
37. Brossut, R. Allomonal secretions in cockroaches. J. Chem. Ecol. 9, 143–158 (1983).
38. Ali, A. et al. Insecticidal and biting deterrent activities of Magnolia grandiora essential oil and selected pure compounds against
Aedes aegypti. Molecules 25, 1359 (2020).
39. Yu, J., Yang, B., Chang, Y., Zhang, Y. & Wang, G. Identication of a general odorant receptor for repellents in the Asian corn borer
Ostrinia furnacalis. Front. Physiol. 11, 1–43. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fphys. 2020. 00176 (2020).
40. Sievert, T. et al. Bank vole alarm pheromone chemistry and eects in the eld. Oecologia 196, 667–677 (2021).
41. Fletcher, B. S. & Watson, C. A. e ovipositional response of the tephritid fruit y, Dacus tryoni to 2-Chloroethanol in laboratory
bioassays. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 67, 21–23 (1974).
42. Eisemann, C. H. & Rice, M. J. Attractants for the gravid Queensland fruit y Dacus tryoni. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 62, 125–130 (1992).
43. Devescovi, F., Hurtado, J. & Taylor, W. P. Mating-induced changes in responses of female Queensland fruit y to male pheromones
and fruit: A mechanism for mating-induced sexual inhibition. J. Insect Physiol. 129, 104195 (2021).
44. Kempraj, V., Park, S. P. & Taylor, P. W. γ-Octalactone, an eective oviposition stimulant of Bactro cera tryoni. J. Appl. Entomol. 143,
1205–1209 (2019).
45. Jayanthi, P. D. K. et al. Oviposition site-selection by Bactrocera dorsalis is mediated through an innate recognition template tuned
to γ-octalactone. PLoS ONE 9, e85764 (2014).
46. Weldon, C. V. & Taylor, P. W. Sexual development of wild and mass-reared male Queensland fruit y in response to natural food
sources. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 139, 17–24 (2011).
47. Friard, O. & Gamba, M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging soware for video/audio coding and live observations.
Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330 (2016).
Acknowledgements
e authors thank Dr. Stefano De Faveri, Department of Agriculture & Fisheries, Mareeba, Queensland for
providing lab space and equipment for work with green tree ants. is research was supported by funds from
Australian Research Council Industrial Transformation Training Centre for Fruit Fly Biosecurity Innovation
(Project IC150100026) including a PhD scholarship for V.K. and a Research Fellowship for S.J.P. is research
received additional support from the SITplus collaborative fruit y program. Project Raising Q-y Sterile Insect
Technique to World Standard (HG14033) is funded by the Hort Frontiers Fruit Fly Fund, part of the Hort
Frontiers strategic partnership initiative developed by Hort Innovation, with co-investment from Macquarie
University and contributions from the Australian Government.
Author contributions
V.K. and P.W.T designed the study, V.K, S.J.P and D.C conducted bioassays and other experiments. V.K. wrote
the rst dra of the manuscript and all authors contributed to the nal version of the manuscript.
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
9
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:15768 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20102-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Additional information
Supplementary Information e online version contains supplementary material available at https:// doi. org/
10. 1038/ s41598- 022- 20102-0.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to V.K.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
© e Author(s) 2022
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Content uploaded by Vivek Kempraj
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Vivek Kempraj on Sep 22, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.