Conference PaperPDF Available

Stereotypes Contribute to Gender Imbalances in STEM Fields

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Stereotypes Contribute to Gender Imbalances in STEM Fields
Objectives
The gender gap in participation in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics
(STEM) remains a large and persistent educational problem. Women are less likely to major in
STEM fields in college, and less likely to enter STEM careers (U.S. Department of Education,
2017). This gender gap can be traced back to children’s motivation. Girls report lower interest
and confidence in STEM, as early as elementary school (Authors, 2017; Mullis et al., 2008;
Nagy et al., 2010).
Yet STEM fields are not homogenous, and differ in their representation of women. Some
STEM fields have achieved near-equality in gender representation at the college level. For
example, in 2016, women in the United States earned 60% of bachelor’s degrees in biological
sciences and 43% of degrees in math and statistics (U.S. Department of Education, 2017). In
contrast, women’s representation was significantly lower in fields like computer science (19%)
and engineering (20%). Any full explanations for women’s underrepresentation in STEM must
account for these large variations among STEM fields (Ceci et al., 2014). Previous explanations
for women’s underrepresentation have pointed to the math-based nature of STEM fields (e.g.,
Wang et al., 2013), but this does not explain why women are proportionally more likely to major
in math than computer science. Recent evidence suggests that the most likely explanations
involve gender differences in preferences and choices (i.e., motivation) rather than abilities and
performance (Riegle-Crumb et al., 2012).
One potential source of variation in motivation is differences in the cultural stereotypes
about particular STEM fields (Cheryan et al., 2017). There are pervasive cultural stereotypes in
Western countries associating boys and men with STEM (Authors, 2015). Recent research
2
findings have demonstrated that these stereotypes are linked to gender differences in
participation and interest in different academic fields. For example, the more that people within a
field endorse the belief that “natural” talent or genius is required for that field (with “genius
stereotypically linked to males), the lower the proportion of women in the field (Leslie et al.,
2015). These beliefs are more frequent within fields such as computer science and engineering.
Science-gender stereotypes are also correlated with women’s lower participation in science at the
college and professional levels cross-nationally (Miller et al., 2015). Stereotypes may also be
important for children’s budding interests about academic domains. One study found that 6-year-
old children were more likely to endorse stereotypes that boys were better at programming and
robotics, compared to math and science, a pattern that paralleled adult women’s participation in
those fields (Authors, 2017).
We measured adolescents’ gender stereotypes about who is better and their interest across
four STEM fields, including (as a novel contribution of this project) computer science and
engineering. We predicted that girls would report the strongest stereotypes and the lowest
interest for computer science and engineering compared to math and science. We predicted that
adolescents’ stereotypes would correlate with their interest in those specific fields. We also
expected that this relation would be moderated by gender, with stereotypes more negatively
related to interest among girls compared to boys.
Theoretical Framework
Our model (see Figure 1) is rooted in expectancy-value theory, which shows how
individuals’ perceptions of stereotypes and gender influence their expectations of success in a
domain, which in turn influence their interest in engaging with that domain (Eccles, 2011).
Stereotypes about gender (e.g., that girls have less ability in computer science than boys) may
3
make girls less confident that they have the ability to succeed in that field. With lower
confidence and expectations of future success, girls may be less interested in pursuing
opportunities in that field. This model is also consistent with balanced identity theory, in which
children’s gender identity, gender-STEM stereotypes, and STEM self-concepts are organized in
a consistent way (Cvencek et al., 2011; Cvencek et al., 2014). Previous research has found that
girls’ gender stereotypes about ability are correlated with ability self-concepts in STEM fields
including math, physics, computer science, and engineering (Authors, 2017; Passolunghi et al.,
2014; Plante et al., 2013; Steffens et al., 2010). In turn, ability self-concepts predict motivation to
pursue STEM fields (Beyer, 2014; Eccles & Wang, 2016; Simpkins et al., 2006).
Middle-school students often report counter-stereotypes that girls are better in math and
science than boys (Evans et al., 2011; Martinot & Désert, 2007; Passolunghi et al., 2014; Rowley
et al., 2007). Few studies have examined computer science and engineering stereotypes, yet we
have reason to believe that they may be particularly strong and influential in current society
(Authors, 2015). Middle and high school is also an important time frame for career trajectories.
The choice of whether students intend to pursue STEM careers in particular often occurs during
middle (Tai et al., 2006) or high school (Hall et al., 2011). Thus, we examined students’
stereotypes and interest across both middle and high school.
Partnership and Collaboration
This research project is well-suited for this year’s AERA Theme, “When Researchers and
Organizational Stakeholders Collaborate.” This project is a result of a partnership between the
Authors (university-based researchers) and the Computer Science For Rhode Island (CS4RI)
team. In 2016, the Rhode Island Department of Education’s (RIDE) Office of Innovation reached
out to the Authors to solicit their expert advice on how to improve equity in computer science
4
education while enacting a governor’s initiative to put computer science into all K-12 schools.
We began a Researcher-Practitioner Partnership, in which the Authors conducted research within
Rhode Island schools in close consultation with the CS4RI organization within RIDE. The
CS4RI team is composed of organizational leaders from education, government, and local
industry who have contributed research questions. The Authors have attended CS4RI team
meetings to learn more about the challenges of broadening access to computer science education
in authentic educational settings on a statewide level. Research findings and data have
continuously been shared.
Methods
Participants
Participants were 1,233 students in grades 6 through 12 in a suburban school district in
Rhode Island. A minimum target sample size (N = 882) was chosen based on effect sizes from a
pilot study. Families were sent an opt-out consent form about the research project. Students took
the survey using the online program Qualtrics. Participants were excluded from analyses if they
failed an attention check (“Please mark ‘slightly disagree’ to show that you read this item”),
leaving a final sample of N = 1,041 students. For this sample, 509 students self-identified as
boys, 503 students as girls, and 26 students as using a different word, with 3 missing. Only
children who identified as boys or girls were included in these analyses. In this district, 43% of
students receive free/reduced lunch. Students self-reported their race/ethnicity as 36% White,
26% Latinx, 17% Multiple ethnicities, 10% Asian American, 8% Black/African American; 1%
Other, and 1% Native American, with 1% missing. In terms of maternal education level, 9%
reported no high school degree; 18% high school degree; 38% some college/college degree; 17%
some grad school/graduate degree; and 18% missing.
5
Measures
Stereotypes. Stereotypes were measured with two questions: “How good do you think
most girls are at the following subjects?and “How good do you think most boys are at the
following subjects?” on a Likert scale from 1 (Really not good) to 6 (Really good). Students
separately rated four different fields for each question: math, science, computer coding, and
engineering. We defined computer coding and engineering for the students. The order of the four
fields was random and balanced across participants; each participant saw the same order for all
questions. Stereotype score was calculated by taking the difference between ratings about boys
minus ratings about girls, so that positive scores indicated traditional stereotypes that boys were
better, 0 represented neutral, and negative scores represented counter-stereotypes that girls were
better. Test-retest correlations for middle-schoolers were stable over four months, ps .001.
Interest. Interest was measured with two items, “How much do you agree with the
following statements: I like to do [field] activities,” “I am interested in [field] activities,” on a
Likert scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 6 (Strongly agree). Correlations between the two items
for each field ranged from r = .83 to .88, ps < .001, indicating high reliability, so items were
averaged for each field.
Results
See Tables 1 and 2 for descriptive statistics. One strength of this study is that the
hypotheses and analyses were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework. All results below
support the pre-registered hypotheses.
Correlations between stereotypes and interest. Boys showed positive correlations
between stereotypes and interest in each field (significant for all fields except computer coding),
while girls showed negative correlations for each field (significant for all fields). The gender
6
difference in correlations was significant for each field, all zs > 2.95, ps < .001. See Table 3 for
full correlations, and Table 4 for same-field correlations by school level.
Differentiating stereotypes in different STEM fields. Because we were interested in
comparing computer coding and engineering, r(1,036) = .37, to math and science, r(1,035) = .48,
we grouped the fields into pairs for analyses and calculated the average stereotype for both pairs.
To see stereotype means for each field separately, see Figure 2A. Girls reported significantly
stronger stereotypes (that boys are better than girls) for coding/engineering, M = .43, SD = 1.04,
compared to math/science, M = -.40, SD = 0.96, t(502) = -15.58, p < .001, dz = .70. (Boys also
reported stronger stereotypes for coding/engineering than math/science, p < .001.)
Effects of gender and field on interest. Again, we grouped coding/engineering, r(1,038) =
.37, and math/science, r(1,038) = .39, and calculated the average interest for both pairs. To see
means for each field separately, see Figure 2B. A 2 × 2 (Participant gender [girl, boy] × Field
[coding/engineering, math/science]) mixed-model ANOVA on interest revealed a significant
interaction, F(1,1009) = 60.47, p < .001, ηp2 = .06. Simple effects for girls revealed that interest
in coding/engineering, M = 3.50, SD = 1.40, was significantly lower than interest in
math/science, M = 4.01, SD = 1.26, p < .001, dz = .38. In contrast, boys’ interest was higher for
coding/engineering, M = 4.30, SD = 1.26, compared to math/science, M = 4.17, SD = 1.18, p =
.022, dz = .10. Looking at the interaction the other way, the gender difference in math/science
interest, p = .041, d = .13, was smaller than the gender difference in coding/engineering interest,
p < .001, d = .61.
Scholarly Significance
We found that girls had stronger stereotypes linking ability with males and reported lower
interest for computer science/engineering compared to math/science. We also replicated previous
7
research showing that middle-school students believe girls are better than boys at math and
science. Students’ stereotypes and their interest were also directly correlated for each field. Boys
who believed that boys were better than girls at math, science, and engineering reported higher
interest in these fields, similar to a “stereotype lift” effect (Walton & Cohen, 2003). In contrast,
girls who believed that boys were better than girls at each field reported lower interest. Because
these results are correlational, the next step involves experimental follow-ups to assess more
closely the causal effects of stereotypes (planned longitudinal intervention studies).
Improving diversity in STEM is an important educational issue. Women are most
strongly underrepresented in fields like computer science and engineering, which leads to deep
inequities because these are high-paying and high-status jobs. These findings provide insight into
one possible mechanism underlying women’s underrepresentation across STEM fields. These
findings have implications for the design of educational interventions to improve diversity in
computer science education. To have the greatest impact in reducing gender disparities in STEM,
we might want to redirect our efforts into computer science and engineering, rather than solely to
math or science. Successful approaches may include changing girls’ perceptions of computer
science and engineering by broadening the image of who succeeds in these fields, and by giving
girls more opportunities to experience these fields and build self-efficacy (Cheryan et al., 2017).
Changing (and diversifying) messages about who can succeed in STEM can help increase equity
in STEM.
Word Count: 1,999
8
References
Authors. (2015).
Authors. (2017).
Beyer, S. (2014). Why are women underrepresented in Computer Science? Gender differences in
stereotypes, self-efficacy, values, and interests and predictors of future CS course-taking
and grades. Computer Science Education, 24, 153-192.
Ceci, S. J., Ginther, D. K., Kahn, S., & Williams, W. M. (2014). Women in academic science: A
changing landscape. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 15, 75-141.
Cheryan, S., Ziegler, S. A., Montoya, A. K., & Jiang, L. (2017). Why are some STEM fields
more gender balanced than others? Psychological Bulletin, 143, 1-35.
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in
elementary school children. Child Development, 82, 766-779.
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kapur, M. (2014). Cognitive consistency and math–gender
stereotypes in Singaporean children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 73-
91.
Eccles, J. (2011). Gendered educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles et al.
model of achievement-related choices. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
35, 195-201.
Eccles, J. S., & Wang, M. T. (2016). What motivates females and males to pursue careers in
mathematics and science? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 100-
106.
Evans, A. B., Copping, K. E., Rowley, S. J., & Kurtz-Costes, B. (2011). Academic self-concept
in Black adolescents: Do race and gender stereotypes matter? Self and Identity, 10, 263-
9
277.
Hall, C., Dickerson, J., Batts, D., Kauffmann, P., & Bosse, M. (2011). Are we missing
opportunities to encourage interest in STEM fields? Journal of Technology
Education, 23, 32-46.
Leslie, S.-J., Cimpian, A., Meyer, M., & Freeland, E. (2015). Expectations of brilliance underlie
gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science, 347, 262-265.
Martinot, D., & Désert, M. (2007). Awareness of a gender stereotype, personal beliefs and self-
perceptions regarding math ability: When boys do not surpass girls. Social Psychology of
Education, 10, 455-471.
Miller, D. I., Eagly, A. H., & Linn, M. C. (2015). Women’s representation in science predicts
national gender-science stereotypes: Evidence from 66 nations. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 107, 631-644.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., & Foy, P. (with Olson, J. F., Preuschoff, C., Erberber, E., Arora,
A., & Galia, J.) (2008). TIMSS 2007 international mathematics report: Findings from
IEA’s trends in international mathematics and science study at the fourth and eighth
grades. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College.
Nagy, G., Watt, H. M. G., Eccles, J. S., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Baumert, J. (2010). The
development of students’ mathematics self-concept in relation to gender: Different
countries, different trajectories? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 482-506.
Passolunghi, M. C., Rueda Ferreira, T. I., & Tomasetto, C. (2014). Math-gender stereotypes and
math-related beliefs in childhood and early adolescence. Learning and Individual
Differences, 34, 70-76.
Plante, I., De la Sablonnière, R., Aronson, J. M., & Théorêt, M. (2013). Gender stereotype
10
endorsement and achievement-related outcomes: The role of competence beliefs and task
values. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 225-235.
Riegle-Crumb, C., King, B., Grodsky, E., & Muller, C. (2012). The more things change, the more
they stay the same? Prior achievement fails to explain gender inequality in entry into
STEM college majors over time. American Educational Research Journal, 49, 1048-
1073.
Rowley, S. J., Kurtz
-
Costes, B., Mistry, R., & Feagans, L. (2007). Social status as a predictor of
race and gender stereotypes in late childhood and early adolescence. Social
Development, 16, 150-168.
Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math and science motivation: A
longitudinal examination of the links between choices and beliefs. Developmental
Psychology, 42, 70-83.
Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., & Noack, P. (2010). On the leaky math pipeline: Comparing implicit
math-gender stereotypes and math withdrawal in female and male children and
adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 947-963.
Tai, R. H., Liu, C. Q., Maltese, A. V., & Fan, X. (2006). Planning early for careers in science.
Science, 312, 1143-1144.
U.S. Department of Education (2017). Table 318.30. Bachelor’s, masters, and doctor’s degrees
conferred by postsecondary institutions, by sex of student and discipline division: 2015-
16. National Center for Education Statistics. Retrieved from https://nces.ed.gov/programs
/digest/d17/tables/dt17_318.30.asp
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2003). Stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 39, 456-467.
11
Wang, M.-T., Eccles, J. S., & Kenny, S. (2013). Not lack of ability but more choice: Individual
and gender differences in choice of careers in science, technology, engineering, and
mathematics. Psychological Science, 24, 770-775.
12
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for STEM–Gender Stereotypes
Overall
Girls
Boys
Gender
Field
SD
M
SD
M
SD
difference
Math
1.24
-.42
1.19
-.36
1.29
p = .40
Science
1.09
-.37
1.02
-.20
1.15
p = .011
Computer coding
1.29
.34
1.21
.55
1.35
p = .008
Engineering
1.27
.52
1.25
.68
1.29
p = .041
Note. Range: -5 (girls a lot better) to 5 (boys a lot better). All measures were significantly
different from neutral (0), ps < .001.
13
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics for Interest
Overall
Girls
Boys
Gender
Field
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
difference
Math
3.81*
1.55
3.78*
1.57
3.84*
1.53
p = .54
Science
4.38*
1.36
4.24*
1.44
4.50*
1.28
p = .003
Computer coding
3.84*
1.56
3.45
1.54
4.21*
1.48
p < .001
Engineering
3.97*
1.49
3.54
1.50
4.40*
1.36
p < .001
Note. Range: 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree) for items including “I like to do [field]
activities.”
Overall significantly different from scale midpoint: *p < .001
14
Table 3
Correlations Between Stereotypes and Interest
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
1. Math stereotypes
.50***
.21***
.18***
-.13**
-.07
-.03
-.02
2. Science stereotypes
.26***
.27***
.20***
-.01
-.09*
-.01
-.01
3. Computer coding
stereotypes
.12**
.30***
.43***
-.02
-.09*
-.14**
-.11*
4. Engineering
stereotypes
.12**
.14**
.53***
-.05
-.06
-.08
-.18***
5. Math interest
.22***
.06
.07
.04
.40***
.32***
.39***
6. Science interest
.02
.13**
.07
.00
.40***
.36***
.42***
7. Computer coding
interest
.01
.00
.05
.00
.23***
.34***
.70***
8. Engineering interest
.00
.09*
.14**
.11*
.32***
.43***
.56***
Note. Girls (N = 503) are above the diagonal, and boys (Ns = 506-508) are below the diagonal.
Stereotypes are difference scores for ratings about boys minus ratings about girls. Results shown are from
students (84%) who passed an attention check question in the survey. Same-field correlations are in black,
and different-field correlations are in gray.
*p .05, **p .01, ***p .001
15
Table 4
Same-Field Stereotype-Interest Correlations by Gender and School Level
Math
Science
Computer
Coding
Engineering
Middle
Girls
-.12
-.14*
-.08
-.20***
Boys
.30***
.11
.06
.06
High
Girls
-.14*
-.06
-.16*
-.16*
Boys
.14*
.14*
.02
.14*
Note. *p .05, **p .01, ***p .001
16
Figure 1. Theoretical model showing how stereotypes influence interest in STEM.
INTEREST
ABILITY
STEREOTYPES
ABILITY SELF-CONCEPT
(Will I be good at this?)
17
Figure 2. (A) Stereotypes and (B) Interest by gender and field. Error bars are +/ SE.
-1
-0.5
0
0.5
1
Math Science Computer
Coding
Engineering
Stereotypes
A
Boys
Girls
Boys better
Girls better
1
2
3
4
5
6
Math Science Computer
Coding
Engineering
Interest
Boys
Girls
Strongly
Strongly not
Slightly
Slightly not
B
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Women obtain more than half of U.S. undergraduate degrees in biology, chemistry, and mathematics, yet they earn less than 20% of computer science, engineering, and physics undergraduate degrees (National Science Foundation, 2014a). Gender differences in interest in computer science, engineering, and physics appear even before college. Why are women represented in some science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields more than others? We conduct a critical review of the most commonly cited factors explaining gender disparities in STEM participation and investigate whether these factors explain differential gender participation across STEM fields. Math performance and discrimination influence who enters STEM, but there is little evidence to date that these factors explain why women’s underrepresentation is relatively worse in some STEM fields. We introduce a model with three overarching factors to explain the larger gender gaps in participation in computer science, engineering, and physics than in biology, chemistry, and mathematics: (a) masculine cultures that signal a lower sense of belonging to women than men, (b) a lack of sufficient early experience with computer science, engineering, and physics, and (c) gender gaps in self-efficacy. Efforts to increase women’s participation in computer science, engineering, and physics may benefit from changing masculine cultures and providing students with early experiences that signal equally to both girls and boys that they belong and can succeed in these fields.
Article
Full-text available
In the past 40 years, the proportion of women in science courses and careers has dramatically increased in some nations but not in others. Our research investigated how national differences in women's science participation related to gender-science stereotypes that associate science with men more than women. Data from ϳ350,000 participants in 66 nations indicated that higher female enrollment in tertiary science education (community college or above) related to weaker explicit and implicit national gender-science stereotypes. Higher female employment in the researcher workforce related to weaker explicit, but not implicit, gender-science stereotypes. These relationships remained after controlling for many theoretically relevant covariates. Even nations with high overall gender equity (e.g., the Netherlands) had strong gender-science stereotypes if men dominated science fields specifically. In addition, the relationship between women's educational enrollment in science and implicit gender-science stereotypes was stronger for college-educated participants than participants without college education. Implications for instructional practices and educational policies are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Much has been written in the past two decades about women in academic science careers, but this literature is contradictory. Many analyses have revealed a level playing field, with men and women faring equally, whereas other analyses have suggested numerous areas in which the playing field is not level. The only widely-agreed-upon conclusion is that women are underrepresented in college majors, graduate school programs, and the professoriate in those fields that are the most mathematically intensive, such as geoscience, engineering, economics, mathematics/computer science, and the physical sciences. In other scientific fields (psychology, life science, social science), women are found in much higher percentages. In this monograph, we undertake extensive life-course analyses comparing the trajectories of women and men in math-intensive fields with those of their counterparts in non-math-intensive fields in which women are close to parity with or even exceed the number of men. We begin by examining early-childhood differences in spatial processing and follow this through quantitative performance in middle childhood and adolescence, including high school coursework. We then focus on the transition of the sexes from high school to college major, then to graduate school, and, finally, to careers in academic science. The results of our myriad analyses reveal that early sex differences in spatial and mathematical reasoning need not stem from biological bases, that the gap between average female and male math ability is narrowing (suggesting strong environmental influences), and that sex differences in math ability at the right tail show variation over time and across nationalities, ethnicities, and other factors, indicating that the ratio of males to females at the right tail can and does change. We find that gender differences in attitudes toward and expectations about math careers and ability (controlling for actual ability) are evident by kindergarten and increase thereafter, leading to lower female propensities to major in math-intensive subjects in college but higher female propensities to major in non-math-intensive sciences, with overall science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) majors at 50% female for more than a decade. Post-college, although men with majors in math-intensive subjects have historically chosen and completed PhDs in these fields more often than women, the gap has recently narrowed by two thirds; among non-math-intensive STEM majors, women are more likely than men to go into health and other people-related occupations instead of pursuing PhDs. Importantly, of those who obtain doctorates in math-intensive fields, men and women entering the professoriate have equivalent access to tenure-track academic jobs in science, and they persist and are remunerated at comparable rates—with some caveats that we discuss. The transition from graduate programs to assistant professorships shows more pipeline leakage in the fields in which women are already very prevalent (psychology, life science, social science) than in the math-intensive fields in which they are underrepresented but in which the number of females holding assistant professorships is at least commensurate with (if not greater than) that of males. That is, invitations to interview for tenure-track positions in math-intensive fields—as well as actual employment offers—reveal that female PhD applicants fare at least as well as their male counterparts in math-intensive fields. Along these same lines, our analyses reveal that manuscript reviewing and grant funding are gender neutral: Male and female authors and principal investigators are equally likely to have their manuscripts accepted by journal editors and their grants funded, with only very occasional exceptions. There are no compelling sex differences in hours worked or average citations per publication, but there is an overall male advantage in productivity. We attempt to reconcile these results amid the disparate claims made regarding their causes, examining sex differences in citations, hours worked, and interests. We conclude by suggesting that although in the past, gender discrimination was an important cause of women’s underrepresentation in scientific academic careers, this claim has continued to be invoked after it has ceased being a valid cause of women’s underrepresentation in math-intensive fields. Consequently, current barriers to women’s full participation in mathematically intensive academic science fields are rooted in pre-college factors and the subsequent likelihood of majoring in these fields, and future research should focus on these barriers rather than misdirecting attention toward historical barriers that no longer account for women’s underrepresentation in academic science.
Article
Full-text available
Women's participation and attitudes to talent Some scientific disciplines have lower percentages of women in academia than others. Leslie et al. hypothesized that general attitudes about the discipline would reflect the representation of women in those fields (see the Perspective by Penner). Surveys revealed that some fields are believed to require attributes such as brilliance and genius, whereas other fields are believed to require more empathy or hard work. In fields where people thought that raw talent was required, academic departments had lower percentages of women. Science , this issue p. 262 ; see also p. 234
Article
Full-text available
This study addresses why women are underrepresented in Computer Science (CS). Data from 1319 American first-year college students (872 female and 447 male) indicate that gender differences in computer self-efficacy, stereotypes, interests, values, interpersonal orientation, and personality exist. If students had had a positive experience in their first CS course, they had a stronger intention to take another CS course. A subset of 128 students (68 females and 60 males) took a CS course up to one year later. Students who were interested in CS, had high computer self-efficacy, were low in family orientation, low in conscientiousness, and low in openness to experiences were more likely to take CS courses. Furthermore, individuals who were highly conscientious and low in relational-interdependent self-construal earned the highest CS grades. Efforts to improve women’s representation in CS should bear these results in mind.
Article
Attracting and retaining students into the STEM fields (science, technology, engineering and math) has become a national concern. This study looked at high school students’ self-reports of career influences, parents’ knowledge of STEM fields, teachers’ knowledge of STEM fields and college students in engineering self-reports about career influences and when they chose engineering as their major. High school participants were students (118) attending a summer academy funded by a grant from the National Science Foundation. High school students reported personal interest, parents, earning potential and teachers as highly influential in their career considerations. However, parents and school personnel were often unaware of the career choices in STEM areas, especially in engineering. Roughly half of the engineering students reported having chosen their major while in high school. Findings suggested that critical points in making adolescents aware of and developing an interest in STEM careers were often overlooked.
Article
Drawing on Eccles’ expectancy-value model of achievement-related choices, we examined the personal aptitudes and motivational beliefs at 12th grade that move individuals toward or away from science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) occupations at age 29. In the first set of analyses, occupational and lifestyle values, math ability self-concepts, family demographics, and high school course-taking more strongly predicted both individual and gender differences in the likelihood of entering STEM careers than math scores on the Differential Aptitude Test. In the second set of analyses, individual and gender differences in career decisions within STEM disciplines (health, biological, and medical sciences (HBMS) versus mathematics, physical, engineering, and computer sciences (MPECS)) were best predicted by occupational values (i.e. preferences for work that were people oriented and altruistic predicted entrance into HBMS instead of MPECS careers). Females were less likely to hold the beliefs that predicted selection of STEM in general, but those who did choose STEM were more likely to select HBMS than MPECS.
Article
This article investigates the empirical basis for often-repeated arguments that gender differences in entrance into STEM majors are largely explained by disparities in prior achievement. Analyses use data from three national cohorts of college matriculates across three decades to consider differences across several indicators of high school math and science achievement at the mean and also at the top of the test distribution. Analyses also examine the different comparative advantages men and women enjoy in math/science vs. English/reading. Regardless of how prior achievement is measured, very little of the strong and persistent gender gap in physical science and engineering majors over time is explained. Findings highlight the limitations of theories focusing on gender differences in skills and suggest directions for future research.