PreprintPDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Tabone, W., Happee, R., Garcia, J., Lee, Y. M., Lupetti, M. C., Merat, N., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2023). Augmented reality interfaces for pedestrian-vehicle interactions: An online study. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 94, 170–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2023.02.005 Augmented Reality (AR) technology could be utilised to assist pedestrians in navigating safely through traffic. However, whether potential users would understand and use such AR solutions is currently unknown. Nine novel AR interfaces for pedestrian-vehicle communication, previously developed using an experience-based design method, were evaluated through an online questionnaire study completed by 992 respondents in Germany, the Netherlands, Norway, Sweden, and the United Kingdom. The AR indicated whether it was safe to cross the road in front of an approaching automated vehicle. Each interface was rated for its intuitiveness and convincingness, aesthetics, and usefulness. Moreover, comments were collected for qualitative analysis. The results indicated that interfaces that employed traditional design elements from existing traffic, and head-up displays, received the highest ratings overall. Statistical results also showed that there were no significant effects of country, age, and gender on interface acceptance. Thematic analysis of the textual comments offered detail on each interface design’s stronger and weaker points, and revealed unintended effects of certain designs. In particular, some of the interfaces were commented on as being dangerous or scary, or were criticised that they could be misinterpreted in that they signal that something is wrong with the vehicle, or that they could occlude the view of the vehicle. The current findings highlight the limitations of experience-based design, and the importance of applying legacy design principles and involving target users in design and evaluation. Future research should be conducted in scenarios in which pedestrians actually interact with approaching vehicles.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
Available online 21 February 2023
1369-8478/© 2023 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Augmented reality interfaces for pedestrian-vehicle interactions:
An online study
Wilbert Tabone
a
,
*
, Riender Happee
a
, Jorge García
b
, Yee Mun Lee
b
,
Maria Luce Lupetti
c
, Natasha Merat
b
, Joost de Winter
a
a
Department of Cognitive Robotics, Faculty of Mechanical Engineering, Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands
b
Institute for Transport Studies, Faculty of Environment, University of Leeds, The United Kingdom
c
Department of Human Centred Design, Faculty of Industrial Design Engineering, Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Augmented reality
Pedestrian-vehicle interactions
Vulnerable road users
Automated vehicles
Online questionnaire
User study
Road crossing
ABSTRACT
Augmented Reality (AR) technology could be utilised to assist pedestrians in navigating safely
through trafc. However, whether potential users would understand and use such AR solutions is
currently unknown. Nine novel AR interfaces for pedestrian-vehicle communication, previously
developed using an experience-based design method, were evaluated through an online ques-
tionnaire study completed by 992 respondents in Germany, the Netherlands, Norway, Sweden,
and the United Kingdom. The AR indicated whether it was safe to cross the road in front of an
approaching automated vehicle. Each interface was rated for its intuitiveness and convincingness,
aesthetics, and usefulness. Moreover, comments were collected for qualitative analysis. The re-
sults indicated that interfaces that employed traditional design elements from existing trafc, and
head-up displays, received the highest ratings overall. Statistical results also showed that there
were no signicant effects of country, age, and gender on interface acceptance. Thematic analysis
of the textual comments offered detail on each interface designs stronger and weaker points, and
revealed unintended effects of certain designs. In particular, some of the interfaces were com-
mented on as being dangerous or scary, or were criticised that they could be misinterpreted in
that they signal that something is wrong with the vehicle, or that they could occlude the view of
the vehicle. The current ndings highlight the limitations of experience-based design, and the
importance of applying legacy design principles and involving target users in design and evalu-
ation. Future research should be conducted in scenarios in which pedestrians actually interact
with approaching vehicles.
1. Introduction
Future trafc, in which automated vehicles (AVs) will be driving in city environments, requires transparent communication of the
intentions of the vehicle with interaction partners, such as vulnerable road users (VRUs). In traditional trafc, transparent commu-
nication between vehicles and vulnerable road users is achieved through implicit and explicit cues (Lee et al., 2021; Schieben et al.,
2019). Implicit cues include vehicle speed, dynamics, and gap size, while explicit cues include the horn, hand gestures, and eye
contact. VRUs base their crossing decisions primarily on implicit cues (Dey & Terken, 2017; Lee et al., 2021), whereas explicit cues
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: w.tabone@tudelft.nl (W. Tabone).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Transportation Research Part F:
Psychology and Behaviour
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/trf
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2023.02.005
Received 30 August 2022; Received in revised form 3 February 2023; Accepted 8 February 2023
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
171
tend to be used when implicit cues are ambiguous (Onkhar et al., 2021; Uttley et al., 2020). With the introduction of AVs in the urban
environment, the lack of a driver or attentive passenger may require a different approach to communicating intent from the AV to the
VRU (Ackermans et al., 2020; Carmona et al., 2021; Faas et al., 2020; Hensch et al., 2019). Several communication methodologies have
been proposed to alleviate the problems of AV-VRU interactions. These include the use of smart road infrastructure (L¨
ocken et al.,
2019; Pompigna & Mauro, 2022; Toh et al., 2020), smart vehicle kinematics through the use of vehicle pitch, deceleration, and lateral
position (Bindsch¨
adel et al., 2022; Dietrich et al., 2020; Fuest et al., 2018; Sripada et al., 2021), and external humanmachine in-
terfaces (eHMIs).
Various forms of eHMIs have been developed, including LED strips, LED screens, anthropomorphic elements, actuated robotic
attachments, and projections on the road, amongst others (see Bazilinskyy et al., 2019; De Winter & Dodou, 2022; Dey, Habibovic,
Peging, et al., 2020; Rouchitsas & Alm, 2019, for reviews of such interfaces). Despite their effectiveness in encouraging VRUs to (not)
cross in front of the AVs path, current eHMI designs have some drawbacks, namely if the eHMI needs to signal to a single pedestrian in
a group, or, for text-based eHMIs, if the message is in a language unfamiliar to the pedestrian. Furthermore, so far, there has been no
standardisation of eHMIs, and therefore pedestrians may encounter a variety of different eHMIs on vehicles, which could cause
confusion (Rasouli & Tsotsos, 2020; Tabone, De Winter, et al., 2021), with potentially dangerous consequences.
In an effort to address some of these problems, augmented reality (AR) has been proposed as a new type of communication in
trafc. AR used by individual VRUs can alleviate several issues, especially the one-to-many communication problem, where multiple
actors (vehicles and pedestrians) are present in the environment and it is not clear which actor is communicating to whom. Through
Fig. 1. The nine AR concepts for pedestrian-vehicle interactions designed and developed by Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021). In total, nine AR interface
concepts were developed, each with a yielding and non-yielding state: 1. Augmented zebra crossing, 2. Planes on vehicle, 3. Conspicuous looming
planes (i.e., planes which grew or shrank in size), 4. Field of safe travel, 5. Fixed pedestrian trafc lights, 6. Virtual fence, 7. Phantom car (i.e., a
transparent car which indicates the vehicles predicted future position), 8. Nudge HUD (i.e., a oating text message and icon which informed the
pedestrian whether or not it was safe to cross), 9. Pedestrian trafc lights HUD. Interfaces 1, 4, 5, 6, and 7 are projected on the road surface, while
Interfaces 2 and 3 are projected on the car. Interfaces 8 and 9 are head-locked, i.e., they remain in the users eld of view.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
172
AR, the communication signal could be sent individually and separately to each pedestrian, and does not have to be constrained to the
AV itself but can be presented anywhere in the environment (Tabone, Lee, et al., 2021; Tran et al., 2022).
So far, studies on AR for pedestrian-vehicle interaction consider the driver as the AR user, by highlighting pedestrians and/or
cyclists in front of the vehicle (e.g., Calvi et al., 2020; Colley et al., 2021; Currano et al., 2021; Kim et al., 2018; Pichen et al., 2020).
Such solutions are becoming technologically feasible when considering that the most recent vehicle models already feature AR-based
head-up displays (Volkswagen, 2020). The use of AR by VRUs themselves is still relatively rare and has mostly been constrained to
route navigation tasks (e.g., Bhorkar, 2017; Dancu et al., 2015; Dong et al., 2021; Ginters, 2019), for example as an add-on to Google
Maps (Ranieri, 2020). Only a small, but growing number of studies have examined the use of AR for supporting VRUs in making safe
crossing decisions. Examples include road projections such as zebra crossings, safe paths, and arrows (Hesenius et al., 2018; Li et al.,
2022; Prattic`
o et al., 2021; Tran et al., 2022), visualisation of obstructed vehicles (Matviienko et al., 2022; Von Sawitzky et al., 2020),
visualisation of collision times and conict points (Tong & Jia, 2019), warning signs (Tong & Jia, 2019; Von Sawitzky et al., 2020), and
car overlays (Tran et al., 2022). Using virtual reality, Oudshoorn et al. (2021) developed bioinspired eHMIs for pedestrian-AV
interaction, whereas Mok et al. (2022) developed eHMIs in the form of laser-type rays emitted from the AV. The authors noted that
these types of eHMIs may be hard to physically implement on real AVs, and that AR used by pedestrians (such as through AR glasses or
handheld devices) could be a viable alternative.
It should be noted that most AR concepts for VRUs are still in a conceptual stage (videos, virtual reality), while only a few AR
interfaces for VRUs have been demonstrated on a real road (Maruhn et al., 2020; Tabone, Lee, et al., 2021), or in a laboratory
environment (Matviienko et al., 2022; Prattic`
o et al., 2021; Tran et al., 2022). In Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021), novel AR interfaces for
pedestrian-AV interaction were developed and demonstrated in a real crossing environment. The interfaces were designed to assist
pedestrians in the decision to cross the road in front of an approaching automated vehicle which was either yielding (stopping) or non-
yielding. The interfaces were based on expert perspectives extracted from Tabone, De Winter, et al. (2021) and designed using
theoretically-informed brainstorming sessions (see Fig. 1 for the interfaces). In total, nine AR interfaces were designed, each with a
non-yielding and yielding state, depicted in red and green respectively. These colours were selected based on their high intuitiveness
rating for signalling ‘please (do not) cross(Bazilinskyy et al., 2020).
Three of the interfaces were mapped to the road, four were mapped to the vehicle, and two were head-locked to the users eld of
view. The ones mapped to the road were the augmented zebra crossing, which is a traditional zebra crossing design (1 in Fig. 1), xed
pedestrian trafc lights (5), which depicts a familiar pedestrian trafc light design across the road, and a virtual fence (6), which includes
semi-translucent walls around a zebra-crossing and a gate that opens in the yielding state. The interfaces that were mapped to the
vehicle included the planes on the vehicle (2), which displays a plane on the windshield area of the vehicle, the conspicuous looming plane
(3), which grows or shrinks as the vehicle approaches the pedestrian depending on the AVs yielding state, the eld of safe travel (4)
which projects a eld on the road in front of the vehicle to communicate safety, and the phantom car (7) which projects the vehicles
predicted future motion. The nal two interfaces are head-up displays: the nudge head-up display (HUD) (8), which displays text and
icons, and the pedestrian lights HUD (9), which displays a head-locked version of the pedestrian trafc lights.
In Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021), the interfaces were implemented on a handheld device (iPad Pro 2020) and demonstrated in a real
crossing environment (Fig. 1), but no user study was performed. The concepts were designed using a ‘genius-based design approach
(Saffer, 2010). In contrast to other design approaches, genius design does not involve users as part of the formal research phase.
Instead, the design team relies on personal experience, existing knowledge of human behaviour, the problem space, and human
cognition and psychology (Saffer, 2010). This approach offers the benet of time efciency, coherence of solutions with the original
vision, and the exibility to generate ideas quickly. Yet, such an approach could be contested as it addresses the problem space only
from a designers viewpoint without the involvement of the intended users (Nielsen, 2007).
Although a theoretical evaluation based on nine AR heuristics (Endsley et al., 2017) was performed in Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021), it
is vital that AR concepts are evaluated empirically to assess whether the theoretically informed ideas are valid. Such an empirical
evaluation would assess the viability of the ‘geniusdesign approach in Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021) and whether the designersintended
effects would generalise to potential target users. Conducting a real-world study with the implemented AR prototypes would have been
very difcult at the time of writing due to AR technology limitations, such as outdoor luminance levels that may hinder perception,
latency issues that may lead to visually induced motion sickness, and ocular vergence-accommodation conicts in open spaces (Buker
et al., 2012; Rolland et al., 1995; Wann et al., 1995). Therefore, an online questionnaire study approach with a large number of
participants was selected. A substantial number of previous works have conducted online user surveys to evaluate eHMIs for
pedestrian-AV interaction (e.g., Bai, Legge, Young, Bao, & Zhou, 2021; Bazilinskyy, Dodou, & De Winter, 2020; Bazilinskyy, Kooijman,
Dodou, & De Winter, 2021; Dey et al., 2020; Lau et al., 2021). However, no large-sample survey of AR interfaces for VRU-AV in-
teractions has been conducted so far.
Hence, we attempt to ll this gap and build upon the previous design work reported in Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021) by conducting an
online video-based questionnaire study that investigates user acceptance of the AR interfaces across large numbers of participants,
exploring key moderator variables (e.g., nationality, gender). Ratings of intuitiveness, convincingness, usefulness, aesthetics, and
satisfaction with the interface were captured, which were thought to represent key dimensions of interface quality. These measures
were based on previous studies which explored intuitiveness (Bazilinskyy et al., 2020), usefulness (Adell, 2010), quality of information
(Lau et al., 2021), as well as aestheticism, attractiveness, and visibility (M´
etayer & Coeugnet, 2021). More specically, it was reasoned
that a high-quality AR interface should be easily understood (intuitive) and encourage people to follow up its recommendations
(convincing), and be seen as useful in supporting pedestrian decision-making (usefulness). Furthermore, apart from encouraging
performance, whether people like the AR interface (attractiveness, satisfaction) was seen as relevant, as when people might reject/
disuse an (otherwise useful) AR interface on aesthetic grounds, it will still fail to be effective.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
173
2. Method
In this study, participants viewed videos in a within-subject design, with 9 AR interfaces and 2 yielding behaviours. Participants
rated each video according to a number of criteria. The video content, questionnaire design and procedures, and statistical analysis
methods are explained below.
2.1. Videos
A total of 19 videos (at 30 fps) depicting an approaching AV with a representation of the AR interface in the virtual reality (VR)
environment were created (Fig. 2). More specically, nine videos depicted a yielding AV featuring a green-coloured (RGB: 32, 244, 0)
AR interface, and nine videos depicted a non-yielding AV featuring a red-coloured (RGB: 244, 0, 0) AR interface.
A 19th video was created to depict a non-yielding AV without any interface. The latter was used at the start of the questionnaire to
demonstrate how confusing and dangerous a situation without any form of signal would be, especially if the vehicle does not yield,
while the other 18 videos were shown to participants in the experiment section of the questionnaire.
The videos were created based on a simulation created in a Unity-built VR environment (Unity, 2022). The road environment was
obtained from previous research (e.g., Kaleefathullah et al., 2020) performed in the Highly Immersive Kinematic Experimental
Research (HIKER) simulator located at the University of Leeds (University of Leeds, 2022). The videos mimicked the rst-person view
of a stationary pedestrian considering to cross in front of an approaching vehicle and looking to the right, on a one-way street. A one-
way street was selected in order to standardise the direction of trafc ow, considering that the target population of the study were
from countries with different trafc systems. Other studies focusing on road crossing have also utilised a one-way street scenario (e.g.,
Cavallo et al., 2019; Kaleefathullah et al., 2020; Weber et al., 2019).
Trigger points and speeds were adopted from a study on pedestrian crossing in the HIKER simulator (Kaleefathullah et al., 2020).
The AV, represented by the same car model in each video, spawned out of sight from the eld of view (Fig. 3, Point A) and moved at a
constant speed of 30 mph (48 kph). All interfaces, irrespective of location and state, were triggered when the vehicle reached Point B,
located 43 m from the participant (camera) location at Point E. For yielding AVs, the vehicle started decelerating at a rate of 2.99 m/s
2
at Point C, which is located 33 m from Point E, and it came to a full stop 3 m from Point E, at Point D. In the case of a non-yielding AV,
Fig. 2. The nine AR interfaces presented in a VR environment used for this online questionnaire study. Interfaces 1, 4, 5, 6, and 7 are projected on
the road surface, while Interfaces 2 and 3 are projected on the car. Interfaces 8 and 9 are head-locked. The interfaces were adapted from Tabone,
Lee, et al. (2021).
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
174
the vehicle maintained its initial speed of 30 mph throughout.
Each video started with the camera pointing towards the other end of the crossing (Fig. 4, at time 0 s). The camera then slowly
panned to the right as the vehicle approached from point A, starting at an elapsed time of 0.5 s. At an elapsed time of 2 s, the camera
would have rotated by an angle of 45, and the approaching vehicle and AR interface (regardless of type) could be seen simultaneously.
At 4 s, the camera started to rotate back to the front-facing position, and it stopped rotating at 20to the right for the yielding state
(elapsed time: 9 s), and fully facing the front for the non-yielding AV (elapsed time: 8 s) so that the vehicle could be observed driving
over the crossing area.
In addition to videos, side-by-side images were created per AR interface, for insertion in the questionnaire (see Fig. 5 for an
example). For the yielding AV, the frame where the vehicle came to a complete stop was selected, while for the non-yielding state, the
frame at an elapsed time of 6 s was used so that each screenshot had a similar perspective on the road. The only exception was the side-
by-side comparison of the phantom car, where the screenshots were taken with respect to the location of the phantom car interface,
rather than the actual vehicle, so that both the interface and the vehicle could be seen in the screenshots. The 19 videos produced for
the experiment are included in the Supplementary Material.
2.2. Questionnaire procedure
The online questionnaire was administered to 1500 respondents from Germany, the Netherlands, Norway, Sweden, and the United
Kingdom. These countries were selected based on the geographical locations of the participating partners of the Horizon 2020 SHAPE-
Fig. 3. Virtual environment used in the videos. Each salient point is demarcated by a label, together with the distance (in metres) between each
point. A: spawn point, B: AR interface onset, C: AV deceleration onset, D: stopping point, E: participant location. The participant position is also
marked with a camera icon.
Fig. 4. Screenshot of the camera view for Augmented Zebra Crossing at key timestamps. The screenshot at the top are for the non-yielding state,
while the bottom screenshots correspond to the yielding state.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
175
IT project, which funded this research. These ve European countries also have a strong research base in automated vehicle devel-
opment (Hagenzieker et al., 2020) and are likely candidates for the early deployment of eHMIs and AR interfaces. The questionnaire
was developed in English using the Qualtrics XM (Qualtrics, 2022) survey platform and distributed to representative Internet panels
through the German market research institute INNOFACT AG (Innofact, 2022), which has been used in previous research on the
acceptance of AVs (Nordhoff et al., 2021).
A screening questionnaire, prepared in the national language of each of the target respondents countries, was added by INNO-
FACT, to control for age, gender, and nationality and lter out respondents who were uncomfortable with completing the question-
naire in English. Our requested target sample was an equal distribution across countries, gender, and split between ve (1829, 3039,
4049, 5059, 6069) age groups. INNOFACT ensured that participants only participated using a desktop device, and safeguards
against bots and duplicate respondents were also taken.
The survey ran from February to April 2022, and the respondents were nancially compensated with approximately
3. The study
was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the TU Delft under application number 1984.
2.3. Questionnaire design
2.3.1. Introductory information
First, a brief overview of AR and VR technologies was presented, together with examples of popular AR apps, so that the unfamiliar
respondents would have a clearer picture of what would be discussed in the rest of the questionnaire. This was followed by an example
of what the future could look like with the introduction of AR glasses, a brief introduction to the future urban environment, and the
need for communication between AVs and pedestrians. The problem of having no clear signals from the car due to the lack of a driver
was demonstrated through the baseline video (i.e., without AR interface) of a non-yielding AV. The respondents were provided with an
explanation of the purpose of the study, where the potential of solving the communication issue using AR interfaces would be explored.
2.3.2. Consent
Respondents were provided with a consent section, which contained the experimentersnames, contacts, conditions to participate
(being 18 years or older), the main purpose of the study, and the approximate length of the questionnaire (30 min). It was also
highlighted that there were no risks associated with participation and that the questionnaire was anonymous and voluntary. Re-
spondents were encouraged to close the page if they disagreed. Moreover, a question asking whether the instructions were read and
understood was provided (Q1). If ‘Nowas selected, the questionnaire was terminated.
2.3.3. Demographics
Next, respondents were asked about their identifying gender (Q2), age (Q3), country of residence (Q4), and their highest level of
formal education completed (Q5). Respondents were presented with the Afnity for Technology Interaction (ATI) scale (Franke et al.,
2019) to gauge their afnity with technological systems (Q6). Respondents were then asked if they had ever used VR headsets (Q7) and
AR apps (Q8), and how willing they would be to use AR wearables in general (Q9), specically on the road as a pedestrian (Q10), and
for the specic task of assisting pedestrians in crossing a road in front of an AV (Q11).
The respondents were then asked whether they had ever encountered AVs before (Q12), their daily walking time as pedestrians
(Q13) (as used in Deb et al., 2017), and their primary mode of transportation (Q14). The last part in the demographic section treated
any constraints in personal mobility (Q15) and included a colour blindness test (Q16) (Ishihara, 1917; as used in Bazilinskyy et al.,
2020).
2.3.4. Video presentation of AR interfaces and rating questions
Following a brief introduction to the experiment, participants proceeded to the main part of the study, where the yielding and non-
yielding state of the nine interfaces were presented, together with various rating questions.
Fig. 5. Example of side-by-side image for AR concept 1, Augmented zebra crossing. Left: non-yielding state, Right: yielding state.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
176
The videos from each interface were presented on a separate page, having the title of the respective interface (see Fig. 2). The order
in which the nine interfaces were presented was randomised for each respondent. Each interface page rst presented the non-yielding-
state video, followed by the yielding-state video. The videos auto-played and looped. All 18 videos were presented to each participant.
Below each video depicting a non-yielding AV, the respondents used a 7-point Likert scale (Strongly disagree, Disagree, Somewhat
disagree, Neither agree nor disagree, Somewhat agree, agree, Strongly agree) to rate whether:
The interface in the video above is intuitive for signalling ‘Please do NOT cross the road’” (Intuitiveness: Q17).
The interface in the video above convinced me NOT to cross the road (Convincingness: Q18).
and below each video depicting a yielding AV, the following two questions were asked:
The interface in the video above is intuitive for signalling ‘Please cross the road’” (Intuitiveness: Q19).
The interface in the video above convinced me to cross the road (Convincingness: Q20).
Intuitiveness and convincingness were regarded as two key elements of interface quality, where the former refers to whether the
message is readily understandable, and the latter refers to whether the interface would empower people to cross or not cross the road.
The video subsection containing the yielding and non-yielding videos along with the respective intuitiveness and convincingness
items was followed by a side-by-side screenshot of the interfaces states. A matrix table was presented with a 5-point descriptor scale
(Q21) for interpretability, where the respondents had to rate the following:
Do you think that the interface was triggered too early or too late? (too early too late) (Q21.1)
Do you think that the interface is too small or too large? (too small too large) (Q21.2)
How clear (understandable) was the interface to you?(very unclear very clear) (Q21.3)
How visually attractive is this interface to you?(very unattractive very attractive) (Q21.4)
Q17Q21 were inspired from previous work which looked at perceived quality/clarity of information (Bazilinskyy, Dodou, & De
Winter, 2020; Rahman, Lesch, Horrey, & Strawderman, 2017; Adell, 2010; Lau et al., 2021), and attractiveness, aestheticism, ease of
understanding, and the adequacy of information, amongst others (M´
etayer & Coeugnet, 2021).
Each interface page ended with a 9-item acceptance scale (Van Der Laan et al., 1997) to collect further ratings on facets of use-
fulness and satisfaction (Q22.1Q22.9). A free text area (Q23) was added to let respondents elaborate on their ratings, Please add a
few words to justify your choices above (eg. comment on the shape, colour, functionality, and the clarity of the interface).
2.3.5. Final questions
The nal section of the questionnaire opened with a question on whether such AR interfaces would be useful for crossing the road in
future trafc (Q24). This query was followed by three side-by-side screenshots contrasting various interface elements, and the
following three statements:
I prefer interfaces mapped to the street rather than on the vehicle (Q25)
I prefer interfaces with text rather than interfaces with just graphical elements (Q26)
I prefer interfaces that move around with my head rather than interfaces that stay xed(Q27), to which the respondent was
answered with a 5-point Likert agreement scale from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree.
The penultimate question related to whether the respondent would like to have the ability to customise the interfaces (Q28). The
nal question once again asked whether the respondent would be willing to use such interfaces as an aid for crossing after having seen
all examples, assuming that they own AR glasses (Q29).
2.4. Analysis
Mean item scores for the AR interfaces in their yielding and non-yielding states were computed and visualized in scatter plots,
together with 95 % condence intervals. The condence intervals were computed by applying a correction for within-subjects effects
of the nine AR interfaces, according to a method presented by Morey (2008).
Differences between ratings of AR interfaces were examined using a repeated-measures ANOVA with an alpha level of 0.05. This
was followed by paired-samples t-tests. Here, an alpha value of 0.005 was used to reduce the occurrence of false positives, compared to
the more commonly used alpha value of 0.05 (Benjamin et al., 2018). It should be noted that because our sample size was large, even
small within-subject differences between the AR interfaces were strongly signicant.
For the assessment of the effects of the moderator variables (gender, age group, educational level), a repeated-measures ANOVA
was used with the AR interface as a within-subject variable and the moderator variable subgroup (e.g., male, female) as a between-
subjects variable (alpha =0.05). Additionally, statistical comparisons between ratings for AR interfaces between participant groups
(e.g., males vs females) were performed using independent-samples t-tests (alpha =0.005).
Apart from testing differences between AR interfaces and the effects of moderator variables, Pearson product-moment correlation
coefcients among item scores were computed to evaluate redundancy among items. Highly correlated items were aggregated to form
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
177
a composite score.
The textual responses were evaluated through thematic analysis (Kiger & Varpio, 2020). All text responses were read, with re-
sponses copied into a separate document if a common theme emerged. For example, if multiple participants commented that a
particular interface was ‘slow, then all comments with such a statement were extracted and placed in a text document under the
section pertaining to the AR interface. Following the collation of all comments, four comments per interface (two per positive and two
per negative valence were selected), depending on which theme was featured the most in that interfaces comment section.
3. Results
In total, 1500 participants answered the questionnaire. An initial quality ltering process was carried out to remove respondents
who did not complete the entire questionnaire (n =357) or answered ‘no to the consent item (Q1) (n =39). Next, the recorded
duration in seconds was used to omit the top 10 % of fastest respondents (i.e., those who completed the questionnaire in 593 s or less, n
=110), since the fastest respondents are likely to yield relatively low-quality data (De Winter & Hancock, 2015). The resulting sample
size was 992 (492 males, 491 females, 8 non-binary, 1 not specied). Within the resulting sample, the median time to complete the
questionnaire was 23.3 min (25th percentile =16.4 min, 75th percentile =33.6 min).
General characteristics of the 992 retained respondents were as follows:
Fig. 6. Scatter plot of intuitiveness ratings (mean of Q17 and Q19) and convincingness ratings (mean of Q18 and Q20) per AR interface. In this
gure, ratings for the yielding and non-yielding states were averaged. The error bars represent 95% condence intervals.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
178
Country: 202 were from Germany, 197 were from the Netherlands, 184 were from Norway, 197 were from Sweden, and 212 were
from the United Kingdom (Q4).
Age: The age (Q3) ranged from 18 to 69 (M =45.10, SD =14.17).
Education: 54 % (n =536) indicated that they went to university, 25 % (n =246) attended trade or vocational school, whereas 21 %
(n =210) indicated ‘none of these(Q5).
Constraints: 17 % (n =170) reported some form of mobility constraint (Q15).
Constraints: 3 % (n =32) were considered colour blind as they submitted three or more incorrect answers (Bazilinskyy et al., 2020)
for the six-item Ishihara colour blindness test (Q16).
The results regarding AR and VR use indicated the following:
42 % of respondents had used a VR headset before (Q7).
45 % had used AR apps before (Q8).
On a scale of 1 (Strongly unwilling) to 5 (Strongly willing), the mean response to How willing would you be to use AR glasses?
(Q9) was 3.59 (SD =1.04).
For How willing would you be to use AR glasses on the road as a pedestrian(Q10), the mean was 3.10 (SD =1.13).
For How willing would you be to use AR glasses on the road if these warn you about how safe it is to cross in front of a self-driving
car?(Q11), the mean was 3.30 (SD =1.12).
Since the goal of this research was to perform a population-level evaluation of the AR interfaces, colour blind participants or
participants with a mobility constraint were not excluded from the analysis.
3.1. Ratings of videos depicting AR interfaces
Table S1 in the Supplementary material shows the means across the 992 respondents for the 17 items for each of the nine AR
interfaces. From this table, it can be seen that there are clear redundancies among the items, with some AR interfaces producing
considerably higher ratings than others on almost all of the 17 items.
In an attempt to better understand item redundancy, several correlational analyses were performed. In particular, Fig. 6 shows the
mean intuitiveness ratings (Q17, Q19) and convincingness ratings (Q18, Q20) for the nine AR interfaces. The ratings were very highly
correlated (r =0.998), indicating that the intuitiveness and convincingness questions yielded nearly the same information. Fig. 6 also
shows that the Nudge HUD scored highest, followed by the Augmented zebra crossing, Fixed pedestrian lights, Pedestrian lights HUD, and
Virtual fence. The Phantom car yielded the lowest ratings.
In the same vein, Fig. 7 shows the averaged intuitiveness and convincingness rating for the nine AR interfaces for yielding AVs
versus non-yielding AVs. Again, a strong association (r =0.93) is seen, indicating that the AR interfaces were rated similarly regardless
of whether the vehicle was stopping or not. We performed a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA of the averaged intuitiveness and
convincingness rating with AR interface and yielding state as within-subject factors. Results showed a signicant effect of the AR
interface, F(8,7928) =197.4, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.17, but not of yielding state F(1, 991) =0.12, p =0.728, partial
η
2
=0.00. There
was, however, a signicant AR interface ×yielding state interaction, F(8, 7928) =41.5, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.04. Follow-up paired-
samples t-tests showed that several AR interfaces (i.e., Augmented zebra crossing, Field of safe travel, Fixed pedestrian lights, Nudge HUD,
Pedestrian lights HUD) yielded somewhat higher ratings for the non-yielding state than for the yielding state (p <0.005 according to
paired-samples t-tests). The Virtual fence and the Planes on vehicle, on the other hand, were rated statistically signicantly higher for
yielding AVs than for non-yielding AV.
A correlation matrix (Fig. 8) of the mean ratings for each interface revealed strong associations between all 17 measured items,
except for the small/large item (Q21, Item 1) and early/late item (Q21, Item 2). The correlation coefcients between the means of the
15 other items ranged from r =0.862 (for irritating/likeable [Q22, Item 6] vs sleep-inducing/raising alertness [Q22, Item 9]) to r =
0.999 (unpleasant/pleasant [Q22, Item 2] vs irritating/likeable [Q22, Item 6]).
3.2. Descriptor scale (Q21), acceptance scale (Q22), and composite score
Because correlation coefcients between items were very high, it was decided to compute a composite score of the 15 strongly-
correlated items (unit-weight method, see DiStefano et al., 2009).
1
More specically, for each AR interface, a 992 participant ×15
matrix was available. The matrices were concatenated, yielding an 8928 ×15 matrix, and subsequently standardised, so that the item
mean was 0 and the standard deviation was 1. The scores of the 15 items were summed, thus producing an 8928-long vector, which
was then standardised. Finally, the 8928-long vector was partitioned back to the nine interfaces, so that a composite score was
available for each participant and AR interface. Fig. 8 shows that the mean composite score correlated very strongly with each of its
1
An inspection of the eigenvalues of the correlation matrix (9 AR interfaces ×15 items) showed strong uni-dimensionality. More specically, the
rst component explained 96.5% of the variance in the participant means, and the corresponding Cronbachs alpha value was 0.990. Additionally,
the correlation matrix at the participant level (992 participants x 15 items) showed strong uni-dimensionality as well, with the rst component
explaining 67.6% of the variance in the means of the 9 AR interfaces, and Cronbachs alpha being 0.962.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
179
dening items, which conrms that the composite score captures a large amount of the variance (96.5 %) in the mean ratings of the
nine AR interfaces. The strongest correlations between the composite score and the individual items (r =0.997, 0.998) occurred for the
items useful/useless (Q22.1), bad/good (Q22.3), and worthless/assisting (Q22.7). This suggests that the meaning of the composite
score is well described by the colloquial phrase ‘whether the AR interface is good or not.
The mean and standard deviation of the composite score per AR interface are shown in Table 1. The ndings align with the above
results (Figs. 6 and 7) that the Nudge HUD was most favoured while the Phantom car was least favoured. A one-way repeated-measures
ANOVA of the composite score showed a signicant effect of the AR interface, F(8,7928) =195.0, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.16. A total
of 32 of 36 pairs of AR interfaces were statistically signicantly different from each other (p <0.005), see Table 1.
3.3. Assessment of moderator variables
Gender: Fig. S2 in the supplementary material shows a strong correlation (r =0.980) between the mean composite scores for male
and female respondents. A repeated-measures ANOVA of the composite score, with the AR interface as a within-subject factor and
gender (male or female) as a between-subjects factor showed a signicant effect of AR interface, F(8, 7848) =192.6, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.16, and no signicant effect of gender, F(1, 981) =0.36, p =0.547, partial
η
2
=0.00, but a signicant AR interface ×gender
interaction, F(8, 7848) =2.00, p =0.043, partial
η
2
=0.00. The interaction effect was extremely small, however, and scores for the
nine AR interfaces did not differ signicantly between males and females. More specically, independent-samples t-tests for the nine
AR interfaces yielded p-values between 0.087 and 0.952 (Conspicuous looming planes: Mean (SD) males/females: 0.40 (1.03)/0.29
Fig. 7. Scatter plot of averaged intuitiveness and convincingness ratings of the yielding state (mean of Q19 & Q20) versus the non-yielding state
(mean of Q17 & Q18) of each AR interface. The error bars represent 95% condence intervals.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
180
Fig. 8. Correlation matrix for the means of the scores of the AR interfaces (n =9). Responses to Q22 Items 1, 2, 4, 5, 7, 9 were reversed with respect
to the questionnaire. The variables are sorted based on hierarchical clustering, i.e., similarity with the other variables.
Table 1
Means with standard deviations in parentheses for the composite scores (z-scores) (n =992). Also shown are results for pairwise comparisons.
No AR interface Composite score 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 Augmented zebra crossing 0.32 (0.89)
2 Planes on vehicle 0.26 (1.01) x
3 Conspicuous looming planes 0.35 (1.00) x x
4 Field of safe travel 0.12 (1.00) x x
5 Fixed pedestrian lights 0.28 (0.88) x x x
6 Virtual fence 0.04 (1.00) x x x x x
7 Phantom car 0.52 (1.05) x x x x x x
8 Nudge HUD 0.37 (0.85) x x x x x x
9 Pedestrian lights HUD 0.25 (0.86) x x x x x x
Note. ‘xmarks pairs of conditions that are statistically signicantly different from each other, computed using paired-samples t-tests (df =991).
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
181
(0.97), t(981) = 1.71, p =0.087; Nudge HUD: Mean (SD) males/females: 0.37 (0.84)/0.37 (0.85), t(981) = 0.06, p =0.952).
Country: The composite score of each interface was examined across the respondentscountries of residence (Fig. 9). The mean
composite scores of the nine AR interfaces correlated again strongly. More specically, for the 10 pairs of countries, correlations
ranged between r =0.972 (between Germany and Sweden) and r =0.992 (between Norway and Sweden). A repeated-measures
ANOVA of the composite score, with the AR interface as within-subject factor and country as between-subjects factor showed a sig-
nicant effect of AR interface, F(8, 7896) =194.1, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.16, and no signicant effect of country, F(4, 987) =0.82, p
=0.515, partial
η
2
=0.00, and no signicant AR interface ×country interaction, F(32, 7896) =0.69, p =0.902, partial
η
2
=0.00.
Age: A repeated-measures ANOVA of the composite score, with the AR interface as a within-subject factor and age (45 or younger vs
46 or older) as a between-subjects factor showed a signicant effect of AR interface, F(8, 7920) =195.2, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.16,
and no signicant effect of age group, F(1, 990) =0.44, p =0.506, partial
η
2
=0.00, and no signicant AR interface ×age group
interaction, F(8, 7920) =1.52, p =0.143, partial
η
2
=0.00. The corresponding scatter plot is found in the supplementary material
(Fig. S3).
Education: A repeated-measures ANOVA of the composite score, with the AR interface as a within-subject factor and educational
attainment (university degree, trade/technical/vocational training, none of these) as a between-subjects factor showed a signicant
effect of AR interface, F(8, 7912) =167.8, p <0.001, partial
η
2
=0.15, and no signicant effect of education, F(2, 989) =0.72, p =
0.489, partial
η
2
=0.00, and no signicant AR interface ×education interaction, F(16, 7912) =0.98, p =0.476, partial
η
2
=0.00. The
corresponding scatter plots are found in the supplementary material (Figs. S4 and S5).
It is noteworthy that although the overall composite score (i.e., averaged across the nine AR interfaces) did not correlate signi-
cantly with gender (r =0.01 [1 =male, 2 =female]), age (r =0.02), the highest level of education completed (r =0.04, [1 =university
degree, 2 =trade/technical/vocational training, 3 =none of these]), having ever used a VR headset (Q7; r = 0.01 [1 =no, 2 =yes]),
or having ever used AR apps or games (Q8; r =0.02 [1 =no, 2 =yes]), it did correlate moderately with willingness to use AR glasses (r
=0.33, 0.32, and 0.35 for Q9, Q10, and Q11, respectively) and with the ATI scale of technology afnity (Q6; r =0.22). It is also
noteworthy that older participants were less likely to have ever used VR (Q7; r = 0.30) or AR (Q8; r = 0.44, respectively).
3.4. Textual responses (Q23)
An average of 46 comments were extracted per interface. The subset of comments was further ltered down to retain four
informative comments per concept, split equally between positive and negative valence (Table 2). These nal selected comments were
deemed representative of some of the major themes that arose per concept.
Fig. 9. Bar plots of the composite score of each interface per respondentscountry. The standard deviation across respondents for the 45 depicted
AR interface ×country combinations ranges between 0.78 and 1.12.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
182
Table 2
Sample of four comments per interface, split based on positive or negative sentiment. Spelling and grammar mistakes were not corrected.
AR Interface Positive Comments Negative Comments
Augmented zebra
crossing
A good idea. The zebra crossing is familiar to every-one. The big
red cross over the crossing should make it clear not to cross.
Its clear what the images mean but it doesnt ll me with
condence regarding when it would be safe to cross the road. I think
if you are not looking at the approaching vehicle you will always be
in danger because you are not aware as to what it is doing, moving or
stopping.
Very clear and presumably understandable by most people
including children once the different colours are explained to
them.
The video signalling do not cross the road, I think is very clear.
However, the video signalling that it is safe to cross is not so clear.
The green lines either side of the pedestrian crossing did not
immediately make me think it was safe, a green tick symbol maybe
wouldve been better.
Planes on vehicle [C]orrect colours for alert and safeness. [T]he walking man on the green background made sense but the
hand on the red background was unclear. i didnt like it moving with
the car, would prefer it to be in your face []
[B]etter variant because the size stays the same and symbols are
clearer.
The problem with this signal, is that it just signals something about
the car, not about the pedestrians.
Conspicuous
looming planes
Very effective, the colour and hand signal stands out well. [T]he colours are still very clear to understand: red for warning and
green for no danger BUT as the vehicle approaches from the right side
(around the corner) it was difcult so identify the signs written on the
coloured boxes, it was kind of a weird perspective and therefore
irritating. [A]s the stop/go signs where moving with the car and
where not xedat the top of my AR glasses, I had to think twice if
these instructions were meant for me as a pedestrian or if there was
another issues not concerning me.
[T]he warning one was much better than the yielding one as the
logo became larger as potential danger increased. [T]he change in
size of the yielding one was hardly noticeable.
I wondered when something would actually appear in the screen. It
took forever before I realised the notication was actually on the car
itself. I nd this visualisation absolutely useless.
Field of safe travel I think it is somewhat useful as it shows the path of the vehicle. [T]he green corridor has me confused, you see the car coming, with
a corridor ahead, that makes me think it will drive on instead of
stop.
There was good warning time to let me know whether I was to
cross or not. I also liked how the red and green showed up a good
distance off too. Very clear.
The beam in the ‘stop videolooks more like a red carpet, which I
guess is something every-one would like to walk on.
Fixed pedestrian
lights
This interface has been familiar and useful to me for as long as I
remember, using it is highly intuitive and I see no need to alter it.
I think the sign for triggered too late for the non-yielding state,
which would be more of a problem as I might already have started my
journey across the street which can be a risk if the vehicle expects
pedestrians to stand still. Otherwise the sign with a pole is very much
familiar to me in my cultural context and therefore easily
understood.
[T]he interface is very clear/understandable as trafc lights are
common in everyday lifeit includes people who are not able to
readit seems like a ‘no energyinteraction for me as I already know
everything I need to know and do not have to think about it.
The signals are good, but optically too small and might well be
overseen depending on the device holder (age, sight) or the
background (lots of distraction on the street).
Virtual fence It creates a safe feeling by creating a virtual wall. I like the crossing part of this as previously stated, but pairing it with
walls is really confusing. When you just see the red one, you
immediately think they are walls to stop the car from going through
and it looks like you are being given access through the crossing. The
green one is better, but together confusing.
Very clear in terms of the obvious colour difference but also in the
size of the warnings. Very functional!
I realised in all examples so far, I enjoy the green signs more. I found
this red one being wayyyyy too big and it literally made me jump
when it appeared. It was also not clear to me that it signalled do not
cross, except the red colour. When I could compare it with the green
sign which was more intuitive it was clear that red meant stop. Before
that I saw the red more as a frame/hallway around the zebra
crossing.
Phantom car [T]he phantom was very fast and clear and really did signal the
options I had its sustainable as well.
[D]ont like the look. reminds me of a video game. so I guess it can
be dangerous cause you feel like in a game.
Really good looking and easily understandable. [T]he trouble is its just a bit too attractive and your brain does what
it always does when you see something really attractive (particularly
cars) and it goes WOW!When it does that it sort of sucks up all of
your attention and you actually pay less attention to the other car.
You almost forget about it.
Nudge HUD I liked this one. People are pretty used to something similar to a
notication like this and the colour +text makes it even easier to
understand it.
[] I feel the non-yielding state should specify do not crossas
opposed to just stating a vehicle is approaching. The yielding state
clearly states safe to cross so the message is much clearer with no
room for misinterpretation.
(continued on next page)
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
183
3.5. Preferred AR interfaces and use of augmented reality in trafc
The results of the nal questionnaire section (Table 3) showed that 66 % of respondents felt that communication using AR in-
terfaces in future trafc would be useful (Q24). Furthermore, 72 % preferred interfaces mapped to the street over those on the vehicle
(Q25), 52 % preferred interfaces that included text rather than just graphical elements (Q26), and 51 % preferred head-locked over
world-locked interfaces (Q27). Moreover, 62 % would like to have the ability to customise the AR interfaces (Q28), and 47 % indicated
they would likely use AR interfaces as an aid for crossing in front of vehicles if they owned AR glasses (Q29).
4. Discussion
An online questionnaire study, aiming to evaluate nine AR interfaces for pedestrian-vehicle interaction, resulted in 992 valid re-
spondents. Respondents were asked to rate the interfaces, presented in videos, on several qualities such as intuitiveness, convinc-
ingness, aesthetics, usefulness, and satisfaction.
4.1. Interface preference by respondents
When considering the intuitiveness and convincingness ratings (Figs. 6 and 7), and the composite score in Table 1, it can be asserted
that AR interfaces that incorporated traditional trafc elements (Augmented zebra crossing, Fixed pedestrian lights, and Pedestrian lights
HUD) and those that were head-locked performed better than the others. In addition, respondents indicated their preference for head-
locked interfaces in the nal responses of the questionnaire (Table 3).
The ‘geniusdesign approach yielded a number of AR interfaces that were theoretically interesting but awed from a users point of
view. The ndings can retrospectively be explained by legacy design principles, which some AR interfaces adhered to and others did
not (see Wickens et al., 2004, for thirteen established principles of display design). For example, although the Phantom car was
designed to adhere to the principle of predictive aiding (since it showed the future position of the car), and the Field of safe travel
adhered to the principle of ecological interface design (Kadar & Shaw, 2000; Tabone, Lee, et al., 2021; Waldenstr¨
om, 2011), these two
interfaces may have failed to comply with other design principles, such as redundancy gain (these interfaces displayed a coloured
element, but no redundant icon or text), the proximity compatibility principle (it may be hard to perceptually separate the Phantom car
from the real car), and the principle of top-down processing (participants are likely unfamiliar with these concepts). The most
Table 2 (continued )
AR Interface Positive Comments Negative Comments
This again empowers the user to make a choice based on their
actions, not based on what the car is doing. It is much bigger then
some, but in some ways less distracting. More functional.
This example is clear enough, but a busy road is not like this. Except
of cars, it can be running pets, pedestrians, bicycles coming from
behindIt is dangerous to rely on this system, I think.
Pedestrian lights
HUD
The best so far because you get the information in the same
direction so you are looking for incoming trafc.
Very nice.
This is a lot clearer since it already relies on trafc rules that are now
established in our society. I still have the feeling though that even if it
is green that you would hold back a little bit with crossing the road
since the car drives pretty fast towards you and I would only cross the
street if the car is completely still.
Because the interface uses an image that I am already acquainted
with (as are most members of the general public, including children
and senior citizens) I found it to be very effective in indicating to me
whether I could or could not cross the road safely.
The image is clearly recognisable as one which indicates whether or
not to cross. My only concern is that it is too small. It actually took me
a few seconds to work out where it was. It could, of course,be that in
time users would automatically focus on that part of their vision, and
see the signal, but for this test, I found it worrying.
Table 3
Descriptive statistics (i.e., means (M), standard deviations (SD), and relative frequencies) for the nal questions.
Question M SD Relative Frequencies
Strongly
disagree (1)
Disagree
(2)
Neither agree
nor disagree (3)
Agree
(4)
Strongly
Agree (5)
In future trafc, the communication from AR interfaces would
be useful for crossing the road (Q24)
3.70 1.02 4.7 % 6.7 % 23.1 % 45.4 % 20.2 %
I prefer interfaces mapped to the street rather than on the
vehicle (Q25)
3.98 0.98 1.9 % 6.1 % 19.5 % 37.3 % 35.2 %
I prefer interfaces with text rather than interfaces with just
graphical elements (Q26)
3.44 1.09 5.3 % 14.0 % 28.5 % 35.4 % 16.7 %
I prefer interfaces that move around with my head rather than
interfaces that stay xed (Q27)
3.38 1.13 7.3 % 14.3 % 27.5 % 35.0 % 15.9 %
I would like to have the ability to customise these AR interfaces
(Q28)
3.71 0.95 3.0 % 5.4 % 29.5 % 41.4 % 20.6 %
Now that I have seen these interfaces, if I own AR glasses, I am
likely to use such interfaces as an aid for crossing in front of
vehicles (Q29)
3.30 1.10 8.9 % 11.6 % 32.9 % 34.4 % 12.3 %
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
184
successful AR concepts, such as the Augmented zebra crossing and Pedestrian lights did adhere to the latter three principles, as described
by Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021). The current observations also highlight the importance of involving the target user earlier on in the
process through the use of a user-centred design methodology (Gulliksen et al., 2003) and to not rely on genius design only. The
involvement of the target user early in the process could be achieved through focus groups, interviews, and card sorting, among other
methods (Norman, 2013).
On the technical side, it is to be noted that the different AR interfaces involve different sensor and computational requirements (for
an overview, see Tabone, Lee, et al., 2021). For instance, AR interfaces presented on the AV itself would have to rely on computer-
vision techniques on the pedestrians side, or vehicle-to-pedestrian communication of the AVs position and speed. The nudge in-
terfaces, however, are considerably simpler and would only require the wireless communication of the AVs stopping intent to the
pedestrian. These different sensor requirements were not presented to the respondents, nor were they considered in the evaluation of
the AR interfaces.
Additionally, our study found that the means of questionnaire items were very strongly correlated and that the 15 acceptance-
related items, in the aggregate, were well-represented by a single composite score. A recommendation that follows is that future
research into the population-level mean acceptance of HMI concepts could just as well use a single acceptance item (such as a ve-point
scale ranging from bad to good) instead of multiple acceptance-related items. This nding aligns with previous research on the
acceptance of automated driving systems, which indicated that different acceptance dimensions are hardly distinguishable and that a
single factor of acceptance provides a better representation of the data (De Winter & Nordhoff, 2022; Nees & Zhang, 2020).
There were, however, two items that did not correlate strongly with the composite score, namely items related to the physical
parameters of interface size and timing. As shown in Table S1 in the Supplementary Material, all nine AR interfaces yielded equivalent
ratings (between 2.91 and 3.12) on the scale from 1 (too early) to 5 (too late) (Q21, Item 1), which can be explained by the fact that all
interfaces were triggered at the same moment in the video. The small differences may have been caused by proximity (e.g., Field of safe
travel extends in front of the car, a sort of tongue protruding forward along the road; Gibson & Crooks, 1938, p. 454), which might
give participants the illusion that the interface was triggered early. The size ratings (Q21, Item 2) were also close to the midpoint for
the nine interfaces, i.e., between 2.56 for the Pedestrian lights HUD and 3.37 for the Virtual fence. The differences in perceived size can
also be explained by the actual size of the interfaces (see Fig. 2).
In the aggregate, different groups of participants reached similar conclusions on what they deemed to be a ‘goodinterface, i.e.,
results were similar regardless of gender, age, or country. Anecdotally, it is often believed that there are major cultural differences
among pedestrians in that an eHMI that is found to work well in one country may not be received well in another country (see quotes of
B¨
argman, Hagenzieker, Krems and Ackerman, and Stanton in Tabone, De Winter, et al., 2021). The results of the present study suggest
that these cultural differences are less strong as may be believed, at least for the ve European countries under investigation. Our
ndings mirror those of others (Bazilinskyy et al., 2019; Singer et al., 2022) who found cross-cultural robustness of eHMIs in a larger
number of countries on different continents.
While the online questionnaire was generally well distributed across the set quotas, it should be noted that the represented
countries of residence were exclusively Western and Northern European. Therefore, cultural differences may have been relatively
small. Several studies reported differences between the perceived clarity of eHMIs among participants from China versus Western
Europe (Joisten et al., 2021; Lanzer et al., 2020; Weber et al., 2019). Whether or not cultural differences become apparent may depend
on the clarity of the task instructions in the experiment and participantsprior expectations rather than the eHMI content itself, as
noted by Singer et al. (2022).
4.2. Free-text comments
The textual inputs and opinions of the respondents were varied. Some respondents reported that the interfaces on the road surface
could distract pedestrians from approaching vehicles, while others considered interfaces on the vehicle a hazard, since these blocked
the visibility of the oncoming vehicle. In a number of instances, respondents indicated that they preferred the non-yielding state over
the yielding state, with the former being regarded as more clear and intuitive. In fact, the intuitiveness and convincingness, as shown in
Fig. 7, tended to favor the non-yielding state, except for a number of interfaces (Planes on vehicle, Virtual fence). Respondents described
the yielding state for Virtual fence as clearer, but some labelled the non-yielding state as dangerous because the presence of a zebra
crossing might tempt pedestrians to cross irrespective of the red gate. Similarly, the non-yielding state for the Field of safe travel was
labelled as potentially dangerous by some because it looked like a red carpet that invited them to walk on it.
Another prevalent theme was that some respondents felt that at times it was not clear to whom the communication referred, i.e., the
pedestrian or the vehicle itself. For example, the hand symbol on the Planes on vehicle was sometimes misinterpreted as indicating a
problem with the vehicle. The Planes on vehicle and Conspicuous looming planes interfaces, which project planes on the vehicle, drew
concerns about a blocked view of the vehicle, yet at the same time, the looming planes concept was commended for its clarity in
communicating danger. These observations reveal the issue of unintended effects resulting from ‘genius designs, where the intention
is not fully grasped by the user. Our ndings resonate with broader issues in human factors, namely that the actual, rather than
presumed, impact of new technology is usually quite surprising, unintended, and even counterproductive(Woods & Dekker, 2000, p. 276).
Similar to the observations derived from the statistical analysis, the interfaces based on more traditional trafc elements were
labelled as more understandable and intuitive due to familiar symbology (e.g., zebra crossing, trafc light). The ‘worst performing
interface (Phantom car), while commended for its aesthetic qualities, received various critical descriptions, such as ‘confusing,
‘frightening, ‘scary, ‘startling, ‘spooky, and ‘unclear. In fact, some described the interface as a video game, which in a sense
conrms the original design direction of the Phantom car concept from Tabone, Lee, et al. (2021), where the idea of ghost cars from
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
185
racing video games was drawn upon.
The HUD interfaces were praised for being ‘logical, ‘visible, ‘clear, and ‘perfectto capture the attention of distracted pedestrians.
However, it was also stated that HUDs could be a distraction from other hazards, especially when text is used (for further discussion on
text-based eHMIs, see Bazilinskyy et al., 2019). Moreover, a number of respondents complained that the text was in English, and that
this would be a danger for pedestrians unfamiliar with the language. The latter feedback resonates with an advantage of AR
communication, where personalization of the interface could solve the language issue. In fact, 62 % of the respondents were in favour
of such a possibility. Finally, a number of times, respondents suggested that they would still rely on the vehicle coming to a full stop
before making any decision, conrming that implicit communication plays an important role in shaping pedestrian decisions (Lee
et al., 2021).
4.3. Limitations and future work
Although the online questionnaire was distributed to a wide respondent pool, the analysis revealed that more than half of the
respondents (54 %) reported having attained a university degree. Research suggests that university graduates are more inclined to-
wards the adoption and usage of technology (Burton-Jones & Hubona, 2005; Nielsen & Haustein, 2018). At the same time, we found
strong convergence in ratings for participants with and without a university degree, suggesting that educational level was not an
important moderator of the current ndings (see Figs. S4 and S5). A possible reason is that participants were not asked to understand or
use complex technology; instead, the present task was largely-one of perceptual nature.
A further limitation is that the high correlation of acceptance-related items may have arisen from the uniform questionnaire format,
giving rise to acquiescence bias. However, this limitation may not be signicant as the acceptance scale (Q22) contained reversed items
(from high to low, and from low to high), yet these items still correlated very strongly with the responses to the intuitiveness and
convincingness items.
A number of free-text comments mentioned drivers being blinded by the interfaces that appeared on the car, indicating that those
respondents did not fully grasp what AR technology is. Additionally, there were instances where the terms ‘ARand ‘VR were used
interchangeably in the comments, with a number of respondents expressing total opposition towards wearing ‘VR headsetswhen they
walk around outside. This confusion may have been caused by the fact that participants only saw VR videos of AR concepts, rather than
experiencing AR themselves. That said, such confusion would only have affected the overall understanding of AR, and probably not the
relative differences in the participantsassessments of the nine AR concepts.
Many respondent comments were unusable in the thematic analysis. While gibberish text entries were uncommon, many of the
textual comments were too brief to provide useful information (e.g., This one was clear). This highlights a limitation of online
studies, where there is the risk that some respondents do not thoroughly read the information provided at the beginning or aim to
complete the questionnaire items quickly. A further limitation of online studies with videos is that, while offering high repeatability,
they do not offer high ecological validity and present only low perceived risk to participants (for a similar discussion, see Fuest et al.,
2020; Petzoldt et al., 2018; Tabone, De Winter, et al., 2021).
A further limitation was that the environment consisted of a one-way road, with only one vehicle. The addition of more trafc, with
varying trajectories, would add more natural cues to the testing environment. It can be hypothesized that the Nudge HUD will be
particularly effective when multiple vehicles approach from different directions since the Nudge HUD does not require the pedestrian to
distribute attention across those vehicles. In contrast, the Planes on vehicle require the pedestrian to rst locate the planes in the
environment before crossing, which may be time-consuming and inefcient. A potential advantage of Planes on vehicle, on the other
hand, is that it may prevent overreliance in situations of e.g., vehicle-to-pedestrian communication failure. Another limitation was the
lack of environmental sound, and the fact that participants were not asked to interact with the scene (e.g., to indicate when it is safe to
cross). To better understand the behaviour of users of such interfaces, ecological validity must be increased. Therefore, in the future,
the stimuli could be presented to the participants in a virtual simulation environment and ultimately, in the real world.
6. Conclusion
Nine augmented reality interfaces for pedestrian-vehicle interaction were presented in a video-based online study that yielded 992
respondents from Germany, the Netherlands, Norway, Sweden, and the United Kingdom. Each interface was shown in its non-yielding
and yielding states at a pedestrian crossing area represented in a VR environment. Respondents were asked to rate each interface based
on its intuitiveness and convincingness in communicating whether or not a vehicle would yield. Other ratings related to functional and
aesthetic qualities, usefulness, and satisfaction.
Statistical and qualitative thematic analysis indicated that respondents preferred head-locked interfaces over their world-locked
counterparts, with interfaces employing traditional trafc interface elements receiving higher ratings than others. These results
indicated that legacy design principles performed better than designs generated through an expert-based approach (‘geniusdesign),
further highlighting the importance of involving the user early in the process. A further qualitative analysis provided more context to
the ratings, such as the preference of the non-yielding state over the yielding state for a number of interfaces, preference towards
traditional trafc symbols, and reliance on implicit cues.
Responses related to the general use of interfaces indicated a preference for interfaces that are mapped to the street instead of the
vehicle. Moreover, respondents preferred interfaces that make use of text compared to interfaces that use just graphical elements, and
interfaces that are head-locked rather than world-locked. Most respondents also indicated that they would like to personalise the AR
interfaces, and that communication using AR interfaces in future trafc would be useful.
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
186
Although the current online study offered an indication of what kinds of AR interfaces, placement in the world, and design elements
are more suitable for pedestrian-vehicle interactions, there are limitations related to the ecological validity dimension of the study. In
order to better understand the behaviour of potential users of the system, in the future, the ecological validity of such a user evaluation
should be increased.
The practical implications of the present study depend on the progression in vehicle automation and communication, and in AR. It
seems plausible that computers will become increasingly compact, and that the use of AR, either via handheld or head-mounted
devices will become increasingly feasible in the real world. At the same time, questions about inclusivity, affordability, and user
acceptance remain to be addressed, as discussed by Tabone, De Winter, et al. (2021). A likely way forward is that the use of AR for
pedestrians will see its introduction rst in professional transportation contexts (e.g., warehouses, airport personnel) before becoming
available to the general public.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Wilbert Tabone: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Software,
Visualization, Writing original draft, Writing review & editing. Riender Happee: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing
review & editing, Supervision. Jorge García: Resources, Software. Yee Mun Lee: Conceptualization, Methodology. Maria Luce
Lupetti: Conceptualization. Natasha Merat: Conceptualization, Methodology. Joost de Winter: Conceptualization, Investigation,
Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing original draft, Writing review & editing, Supervision.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing nancial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to
inuence the work reported in this paper.
Data availability
A link to the data has been included in the manuscript le.
Acknowledgements
This project has received funding from the European Unions Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie
Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No. 860410.
Appendix A. Supplementary material
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2023.02.005.
References
Ackermans, S., Dey, D., Ruijten, P., Cuijpers, R. H., & Peging, B. (2020). The effects of explicit intention communication, conspicuous sensors, and pedestrian attitude
in interactions with automated vehicles. In Proceedings of the 2020 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems. https://doi.org/10.1145/
3313831.3376197
Adell, E. (2010). Acceptance of driver support systems. In Proceedings of the European conference on human centred design for intelligent transport systems, Berlin,
Germany (pp. 475486).
Bai, S., Legge, D. D., Young, A., Bao, S., & Zhou, F. (2021). Investigating external interaction modality and design between automated vehicles and pedestrians at
crossings. In Proceedings of the 2021 IEEE international intelligent transportation systems conference (ITSC). https://doi.org/10.1109/ITSC48978.2021.9564867
Bazilinskyy, P., Dodou, D., & De Winter, J. (2019). Survey on eHMI concepts: The effect of text, color, and perspective. Transportation Research Part F: Trafc
Psychology and Behaviour, 67, 175194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2019.10.013
Bazilinskyy, P., Kooijman, L., Dodou, D., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2021). How should external Human-Machine Interfaces behave? Examining the effects of colour,
position, message, activation distance, vehicle yielding, and visual distraction among 1,434 participants. Applied Ergonomics, 95, Article 103450. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.apergo.2021.103450
Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E. J., Berk, R., Johnson, V. E. (2018). Redene statistical signicance. Nature Human.
Behaviour, 2, 610. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0189-z
Bazilinskyy, P., Dodou, D., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2020). External human-machine interfaces: Which of 729 colors is best for signaling ‘Please (do not) cross? In IEEE
international conference on systems, man and cybernetics (SMC) (pp. 37213728), Toronto, Canada. https://doi.org/10.1109/SMC42975.2020.9282998.
Bhorkar, G. (2017). A survey of augmented reality navigation. arXiv. https://arxiv.org/abs/1708.05006.
Bindsch¨
adel, J., Krems, I., & Kiesel, A. (2022). Active vehicle pitch motion for communication in automated driving. Transportation Research Part F: Trafc Psychology
and Behaviour, 87, 279294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2022.04.011
Buker, T. J., Vincenzi, D. A., & Deaton, J. E. (2012). The effect of apparent latency on simulator sickness while using a see-through helmet-mounted display: Reducing
apparent latency with predictive compensation. Human Factors, 54, 235249. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720811428734
Burton-Jones, A., & Hubona, G. S. (2005). Individual differences and usage behavior: Revisiting a technology acceptance model assumption. ACM SIGMIS Database:
The DATABASE for Advances in Information Systems, 36, 5877. https://doi.org/10.1145/1066149.1066155
Calvi, A., DAmico, F., Ferrante, C., & Ciampoli, L. B. (2020). Effectiveness of augmented reality warnings on driving behaviour whilst approaching pedestrian
crossings: A driving simulator study. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 147, Article 105760. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105760
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
187
Carmona, J., Guindel, C., Garcia, F., & De la Escalera, A. (2021). eHMI: Review and guidelines for deployment on autonomous vehicles. Sensors, 21, 2912. https://doi.
org/10.3390/s21092912
Cavallo, V., Dommes, A., Dang, N. T., & Vienne, F. (2019). A street-crossing simulator for studying and training pedestrians. Transportation Research Part F: Trafc
Psychology and Behaviour, 61, 217228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.04.012
Colley, M., Eder, B., Rixen, J. O., & Rukzio, E. (2021). Effects of semantic segmentation visualization on trust, situation awareness, and cognitive load in highly
automated vehicles. In Proceedings of the 2021 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems. https://doi.org/10.1145/3411764.3445351
Currano, R., Park, S. Y., Moore, D. J., Lyons, K., & Sirkin, D. (2021). Little road driving hud: Heads-up display complexity inuences driversperceptions of automated
vehicles. In Proceedings of the 2021 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems, Yokohama Japan. https://doi.org/10.1145/3411764.3445575.
Dancu, A., Vechev, V., Ünlüer, A. A., Nilson, S., Nygren, O., Eliasson, S., Barjonet, J.-E., Marshall, J., & Fjeld, M. (2015). Gesture bike: Examining projection surfaces
and turn signal systems for urban cycling. In Proceedings of the 2015 international conference on interactive tabletops & surfaces (pp. 151159). https://doi.org/
10.1145/2817721.2817748
De Winter, J., & Dodou, D. (2022). External human-machine interfaces: Gimmick or necessity. Transportation Research Interdisciplinary Perspectives, 15, Article 100643.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trip.2022.100643
De Winter, J. C. F., & Hancock, P. A. (2015). Reections on the 1951 Fitts list: Do humans believe now that machines surpass them? In N. V. Las Vegas (Ed.),
Proceedings of the 6th international conference on applied human factors and ergonomics (AHFE) (pp. 53345341). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.641
De Winter, J., & Nordhoff, S. (2022). Acceptance of conditionally automated cars: Just one factor? Transportation Research Interdisciplinary Perspectives, 15, Article
100645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trip.2022.100645
Deb, S., Strawderman, L., Carruth, D. W., DuBien, J., Smith, B., & Garrison, T. M. (2017). Development and validation of a questionnaire to assess pedestrian
receptivity toward fully autonomous vehicles. Transportation Research Part C: Emerging Technologies, 84, 178195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trc.2017.08.029
Dey, D., & Terken, J. (2017). Pedestrian interaction with vehicles: roles of explicit and implicit communication. In Proceedings of the 9th international conference on
automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications, Oldenburg, Germany, 109113. https://doi.org/10.1145/3122986.3123009.
Dey, D., Habibovic, A., Peging, B., Martens, M., & Terken, J. (2020b). Color and animation preferences for a light band eHMI in interactions between automated
vehicles and pedestrians. In Proceedings of the 2020 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems, Honolulu, HI. https://doi.org/10.1145/3313831.
3376325.
Dey, D., Habibovic, A., L¨
ocken, A., Wintersberger, P., Peging, B., Riener, A., Martens, M., & Terken, J. (2020). Taming the eHMI jungle: A classication taxonomy to
guide, compare, and assess the design principles of automated vehiclesexternal human-machine interfaces. Transportation Research Interdisciplinary Perspectives,
7, Article 100174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trip.2020.100174
Dietrich, A., Tondera, M., & Bengler, K. (2020). Automated vehicles in urban trafc: The effect of kinematics and eHMIs on pedestrian crossing behavior. Advances in
Transportation Studies, 2020, 7384.
DiStefano, C., Zhu, M., & Mindrila, D. (2009). Understanding and using factor scores: Considerations for the applied researcher. Practical Assessment, Research, and
Evaluation, 14, 20. https://doi.org/10.7275/da8t-4g52
Dong, W., Wu, Y., Qin, T., Bian, X., Zhao, Y., He, Y., Xu, Y., & Yu, C. (2021). What is the difference between augmented reality and 2D navigation electronic maps in
pedestrian waynding? Cartography and Geographic Information Science, 48, 225240. https://doi.org/10.1080/15230406.2021.1871646
Endsley, T. C., Sprehn, K. A., Brill, R. M., Ryan, K. J., Vincent, E. C., & Martin, J. M. (2017). Augmented reality design heuristics: Designing for dynamic interactions.
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 61, 21002104. https://doi.org/10.1177/154193121360200
Faas, S. M., Mathis, L.-A., & Baumann, M. (2020). External HMI for self-driving vehicles: Which information shall be displayed? Transportation Research Part F: Trafc
Psychology and Behaviour, 68, 171186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2019.12.009
Franke, T., Attig, C., & Wessel, D. (2019). A personal resource for technology interaction: Development and validation of the afnity for technology interaction (ATI)
scale. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 35, 456467. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2018.1456150
Fuest, T., Michalowski, L., Tr¨
aris, L., Bellem, H., & Bengler, K. (2018). Using the driving behavior of an automated vehicle to communicate intentions a Wizard of Oz
study. In Proceedings of the 2018 21st international conference on intelligent transportation systems (ITSC), Maui, HI (pp. 35963601). https://doi.org/10.1109/ITSC.
2018.8569486.
Fuest, T., Schmidt, E., & Bengler, K. (2020). Comparison of methods to evaluate the inuence of an automated vehicles driving behavior on pedestrians: Wizard of Oz,
virtual reality, and video. Information, 11, 291. https://doi.org/10.3390/info11060291
Gibson, J. J., & Crooks, L. E. (1938). A theoretical eld-analysis of automobile-driving. The American Journal of Psychology, 51, 453471. https://doi.org/10.2307/
1416145
Ginters, E. (2019). Augmented reality use for cycling quality improvement. Procedia Computer Science, 149, 167176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2019.01.120
Gulliksen, J., G¨
oransson, B., Boivie, I., Blomkvist, S., Persson, J., & Cajander, Å. (2003). Key principles for user-centred systems design. Behaviour & Information
Technology, 22, 397409. https://doi.org/10.1080/01449290310001624329
Hagenzieker, M., Boersma, R., Velasco, P. N., Ozturker, M., Zubin, I., & Heikoop, D. (2020). Automated buses in Europe: An inventory of pilots. Delft University of
Technology. Technical Report.
Hensch, A.-C., Neumann, I., Beggiato, M., Halama, J., & Krems, J. F. (2019). Effects of a light-based communication approach as an external HMI for Automated
Vehicles A Wizard-of-Oz study. Transactions on Transport Sciences, 10, 1832. https://doi.org/10.5507/tots.2019.012
Lau, M., Le, D. H., & Oehl, M. (2021). Design of external human-machine interfaces for different automated vehicle types for the interaction with pedestrians on a
shared space. In: N. L. Black, W. P. Neumann, & I. Noy (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st congress of the international ergonomics association (IEA 2021) (pp. 710717).
Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-74608-7_87.
Hesenius, M., B¨
orsting, I., Meyer, O., & Gruhn, V. (2018). Dont panic! Guiding pedestrians in autonomous trafc with augmented reality. In Proceedings of the 20th
international conference on human-computer interaction with mobile devices and services adjunct, Barcelona, Spain (pp. 261268). https://doi.org/10.1145/3236112.
3236148.
Innofact. (2022). INNOFACT AG Das MarktVORSPRUNGSinstitut. https://www.innofact.com.
Ishihara, S. (1917). Tests for colour-blindness. Tokyo: Kanehara Shuppan Co., Ltd.
Joisten, P., Liu, Z., Theobald, N., Webler, A., & Abendroth, B. (2021). Communication of automated vehicles and pedestrian groups: An intercultural study on
pedestriansstreet crossing decisions. In Proceedings of MuC ‘21: mensch und computer 2021, Ingolstadt, Germany (pp. 4953). https://doi.org/10.1145/3473856.
3474004.
Kadar, E. E., & Shaw, R. E. (2000). Toward an ecological eld theory of perceptual control of locomotion. Ecological Psychology, 12, 141180. https://doi.org/
10.1207/S15326969ECO1202_02
Kaleefathullah, A. A., Merat, N., Lee, Y. M., Eisma, Y. B., Madigan, R., Garcia, J., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2020). External humanmachine interfaces can be misleading:
An examination of trust development and misuse in a CAVE-based pedestrian simulation environment. Human Factors. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0018720820970751
Kiger, M. E., & Varpio, L. (2020). Thematic analysis of qualitative data: AMEE Guide No. 131. Medical Teacher, 42, 846854. https://doi.org/10.1080/
0142159X.2020.1755030
Kim, H., Gabbard, J. L., Anon, A. M., & Misu, T. (2018). Driver behavior and performance with augmented reality pedestrian collision warning: An outdoor user study.
IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 24, 15151524. https://doi.org/10.1109/TVCG.2018.2793680
Lanzer, M., Babel, F., Yan, F., Zhang, B., You, F., Wang, J., & Baumann, M. (2020). Designing communication strategies of autonomous vehicles with pedestrians: an
intercultural study. In Proceedings of the 12th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications, Virtual Event (pp. 122131).
https://doi.org/10.1145/3409120.3410653.
Lee, Y. M., Madigan, R., Giles, O., Garach-Morcillo, L., Markkula, G., Fox, C., Camara, F., Rothmueller, M., Vendelbo-Larsen, S. A., Rasmussen, P. H., Dietrich, A.,
Nathanael, D., Portouli, V., Schieben, A., & Merat, N. (2021). Road users rarely use explicit communication when interacting in todays trafc: Implications for
automated vehicles. Cognition, Technology & Work, 23, 367380. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-020-00635-y
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
188
Li, Y., Kamalasanan, V., Batista, M., & Sester, M. (2022). Improving pedestrian priority via grouping and virtual lanes. arXiv. https://arxiv.org/abs/2205.08783.
L¨
ocken, A., Golling, C., & Riener, A. (2019). How should automated vehicles interact with pedestrians? A comparative analysis of interaction concepts in virtual
reality. In Proceedings of the 11th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications, Utrecht, The Netherlands (pp. 262274).
https://doi.org/10.1145/3342197.3344544.
Maruhn, P., Dietrich, A., Prasch, L., & Schneider, S. (2020). Analyzing pedestrian behavior in augmented realityproof of concept. In Proceedings of the 2020 IEEE
conference on virtual reality and 3D user interfaces (VR), Atlanta, GA (pp. 313321). https://doi.org/10.1109/VR46266.2020.00051.
Matviienko, A., Müller, F., Sch¨
on, D., Seesemann, P., Günther, S., & Mühlh¨
auser, M. (2022). BikeAR: Understanding cyclistscrossing decision-making at uncontrolled
intersections using Augmented Reality. In Proceedings of the CHI conference on human factors in computing systems, New Orleans, LA. https://doi.org/10.1145/
3491102.3517560.
M´
etayer, N., & Coeugnet, S. (2021). Improving the experience in the pedestrians interaction with an autonomous vehicle: An ergonomic comparison of external HMI.
Applied Ergonomics, 96, Article 103478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2021.103478
Mok, C. S., Bazilinskyy, P., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2022). Stopping by looking: A driver-pedestrian interaction study in a coupled simulator using head-mounted
displays with eye-tracking. Applied Ergonomics, 105, Article 103825. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2022.103825
Morey, R. D. (2008). Condence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 6164. 10.
20982/tqmp.04.2.p061.
Nees, M., & Zhang, J. (2020). Acceptance of highly automated vehicles: A factor analysis approach. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9qcjp.
Nielsen, T. A. S., & Haustein, S. (2018). On sceptics and enthusiasts: What are the expectations towards self-driving cars? Transport Policy, 66, 4955. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.tranpol.2018.03.004
Nielsen, J. (2007). The myth of the genius designer. https://www.nngroup.com/articles/the-myth-of-the-genius-designer.
Nordhoff, S., Beuster, A., Kessel, T., Bjorvatn, A., Innamaa, S., Lehtonen, E., Malin, F., Madigan, R., Lee, Y.-M., Merat, N., & Louw, T. (2021). Annual quantitative survey
about user acceptance towards ADAS and vehicle automation (Deliverable D7.1 of L3Pilot project). European Commission. https://l3pilot.eu/downloads#c81.
Norman, D. (2013). The design of everyday things (Revised and expanded edition.). Basic Books.
Onkhar, V., Bazilinskyy, P., Stapel, J. C. J., Dodou, D., Gavrila, D., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2021). Towards the detection of driverpedestrian eye contact. Pervasive and
Mobile Computing, 76, Article 101455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmcj.2021.101455
Oudshoorn, M., De Winter, J. C. F., Bazilinskyy, P., & Dodou, D. (2021). Bio-inspired intent communication for automated vehicles. Transportation Research Part F:
Trafc Psychology and Behaviour, 80, 127140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2021.03.021
Petzoldt, T., Schleinitz, K., & Banse, R. (2018). Potential safety effects of a frontal brake light for motor vehicles. IET Intelligent Transport Systems, 12, 449453. https://
doi.org/10.1049/iet-its.2017.0321
Pichen, J., Yan, F., & Baumann, M. (2020). Towards a cooperative driver-vehicle interface: Enhancing driversperception of cyclists through Augmented Reality. In
Proceedings of the 2020 IEEE intelligent vehicles symposium (IV), Las Vegas, NV (pp. 18271832). https://doi.org/10.1109/IV47402.2020.9304621.
Pompigna, A., & Mauro, R. (2022). Smart roads: A state of the art of highways innovations in the Smart Age. Engineering Science and Technology, an International
Journal, 25, Article 100986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jestch.2021.04.005
Prattic`
o, F. G., Lamberti, F., Cannav`
o, A., Morra, L., & Montuschi, P. (2021). Comparing state-of-the-art and emerging Augmented Reality interfaces for autonomous
vehicle-to-pedestrian communication. IEEE Transactions on Vehicular Technology, 70, 11571168. https://doi.org/10.1109/TVT.2021.3054312
Qualtrics. (2022). Qualtrics XM//The leading experience management software. https://www.qualtrics.com.
Rahman, M. M., Lesch, M. F., Horrey, W. J., & Strawderman, L. (2017). Assessing the utility of TAM, TPB, and UTAUT for advanced driver assistance systems. Accident
Analysis & Prevention, 108, 361373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.09.011
Ranieri, M. (2020). A new sense of direction with Live View. https://blog.google/products/maps/new-sense-direction-live-view.
Rasouli, A., & Tsotsos, J. K. (2020). Autonomous vehicles that interact with pedestrians: A survey of theory and practice. IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation
Systems, 21, 900918. https://doi.org/10.1109/TITS.2019.2901817
Rolland, J. P., Gibson, W., & Ariely, D. (1995). Towards quantifying depth and size perception in virtual environments. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual
Environments, 4, 2449. https://doi.org/10.1162/pres.1995.4.1.24
Rouchitsas, A., & Alm, H. (2019). External humanmachine interfaces for autonomous vehicle-to-pedestrian communication: A review of empirical work. Frontiers in
Psychology, 10, 2757. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02757
Saffer, D. (2010). Designing for interaction: Creating innovative applications and devices. Berkeley, CA: New Riders.
Schieben, A., Wilbrink, M., Kettwich, C., Dodiya, J., Weber, F., Sorokin, L., Lee, Y.-M., Madigan, R., Markkula, G., Merat, N., Dietrich, A., & Kaup, M. (2019). Testing
external HMI designs for automated vehiclesAn overview on user study results from the EU project interACT. 9. Tagung Automatisiertes Fahren.
Singer, T., Kobbert, J., Zandi, B., & Khanh, T. Q. (2022). Displaying the driving state of automated vehicles to other road users: An international, virtual reality-based
study as a rst step for the harmonized regulations of novel signaling devices. IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation Systems, 23, 29042918. https://doi.
org/10.1109/TITS.2020.3032777
Sripada, A., Bazilinskyy, P., & De Winter, J. (2021). Automated vehicles that communicate implicitly: Examining the use of lateral position within the lane.
Ergonomics, 64, 14161428. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2021.1925353
Tabone, W., De Winter, J. C. F., Ackermann, C., B¨
argman, J., Baumann, M., Deb, S., Emmenegger, C., Habibovic, A., Hagenzieker, M., Hancock, P. A., Happee, R.,
Krems, J., Lee, J. D., Martens, M., Merat, N., Norman, D. A., Sheridan, T. B., & Stanton, N. A. (2021). Vulnerable road users and the coming wave of automated
vehicles: Expert perspectives. Transportation Research Interdisciplinary Perspectives, 9, Article 100293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trip.2020.100293
Tabone, W., Lee, Y. M., Merat, N., Happee, R., & De Winter, J. C. F. (2021). Towards future pedestrian-vehicle interactions: Introducing theoretically-supported AR
prototypes. In Proceedings of the 13th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications. https://doi.org/10.1145/
3409118.3475149
Toh, C. K., Sanguesa, J. A., Cano, J. C., & Martinez, F. J. (2020). Advances in smart roads for future smart cities. Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical,
Physical and Engineering Sciences, 476, 20190439. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspa.2019.0439
Tong, Y., & Jia, B. (2019). An Augmented-Reality-based warning interface for pedestrians: User interface design and evaluation. Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 63, 18341838. https://doi.org/10.1177/1071181319631413
Tran, T. T. M., Parker, C., Wang, Y., & Tomitsch, M. (2022). Designing wearable augmented reality concepts to support scalability in autonomous vehiclepedestrian
interaction. Frontiers in Computer Science, 4, Article 866516. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomp.2022.866516
Unity. (2022). Unity real-time development platform | 3D, 2D VR & AR engine. https://www.unity.com.
University of Leeds. (2022). Highly Immersive Kinematic Experimental Research (HIKER) pedestrian lab. https://uolds.leeds.ac.uk/facility/hiker-lab.
Uttley, J., Lee, Y. M., Madigan, R., & Merat, N. (2020). Road user interactions in a shared space setting: Priority and communication in a UK car park. Transportation
Research Part F: Trafc Psychology and Behaviour, 72, 3246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2020.05.004
Van Der Laan, J. D., Heino, A., & De Waard, D. (1997). A simple procedure for the assessment of acceptance of advanced transport telematics. Transportation Research
Part C: Emerging Technologies, 5, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0968-090X(96)00025-3
Volkswagen. (2020). From the luxury class to the compact segment: the augmented reality head-up display. https://www.volkswagen-newsroom.com/en/press-
releases/from-the-luxury-class-to-the-compact-segment-the-augmented-reality-head-up-display-6730.
Von Sawitzky, T., Wintersberger, P., L¨
ocken, A., Frison, A. K., & Riener, A. (2020). Augmentation concepts with HUDs for cyclists to improve road safety in shared
spaces. In Extended abstracts of the 2020 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems. https://doi.org/10.1145/3334480.3383022
Waldenstr¨
om, C. (2011). Visualizing the eld of safe travel increases performance in a naval movement task. In Proceedings of the 2011 IEEE international multi-
disciplinary conference on cognitive methods in situation awareness and decision support (CogSIMA), Miami Beach (pp. 252256). https://doi.org/10.1109/
COGSIMA.2011.5753454
Wann, J. P., Rushton, S., & Mon-Williams, M. (1995). Natural problems for stereoscopic depth perception in virtual environments. Vision Research, 35, 27312736.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(95)00018-U
W. Tabone et al.
Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour 94 (2023) 170–189
189
Weber, F., Chadowitz, R., Schmidt, K., Messerschmidt, J., & Fuest, T. (2019). Crossing the street across the globe: A study on the effects of eHMI on pedestrians in the
US, Germany and China. In H. Kr¨
omker (Ed.), HCI in mobility, transport, and automotive systems. HCII 2019 (pp. 515530). Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.
1007/978-3-030-22666-4_37.
Wickens, C. D., Gordon, S. E., & Liu, Y. (2004). An introduction to human factors engineering. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Woods, D., & Dekker, S. (2000). Anticipating the effects of technological change: A new era of dynamics for human factors. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 1,
272282. https://doi.org/10.1080/14639220110037452
W. Tabone et al.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
The last few years have seen a wealth of research on external human-machine interfaces (eHMIs). It has been argued that eHMIs are vital because they fill the social interaction void that arises with the introduction of automated vehicles (AVs). However, there is still much discussion about whether eHMIs are needed. The present article surveys arguments for and against eHMIs. We list three arguments against eHMIs: (1) Implicit communication dominates pedestrian-AV interaction, and there is no social interaction void to be filled, (2) There is a large variety of eHMI concepts and a lack of standardization and consensus, and (3) eHMIs may elicit various negative effects such as distraction, confusion, and overreliance. Next, we present five reasons why eHMIs may be useful or required: (1) eHMIs can make planned actions of the AV visible, thereby increasing the efficiency of pedestrian-AV interaction, (2) Participants value an eHMI compared to no eHMI, (3) eHMIs do not have to be limited to showing instructions or the AV's planned actions; showing the AV mode or the AV's cooperative or detection capabilities are other uses of eHMIs, (4) Recent research shows that driver eye contact is important in traffic, and a social interaction void thus exists, and (5) A large portion of pedestrian-vehicle accidents in current traffic is caused by unclear implicit communication, suggesting that pedestrians may benefit from explicit eHMIs. It is hoped that this article contributes to the critical discussion of whether eHMIs are needed and how they should be designed.
Article
Full-text available
Automated vehicles (AVs) can perform low-level control tasks but are not always capable of proper decision-making. This paper presents a concept of eye-based maneuver control for AV-pedestrian interaction. Previously, it was unknown whether the AV should conduct a stopping maneuver when the driver looks at the pedestrian or looks away from the pedestrian. A two-agent experiment was conducted using two head-mounted displays with integrated eye-tracking. Seventeen pairs of participants (pedestrian and driver) each interacted in a road crossing scenario. The pedestrians' task was to hold a button when they felt safe to cross the road, and the drivers' task was to direct their gaze according to instructions. Participants completed three 16-trial blocks: (1) Baseline, in which the AV was pre-programmed to yield or not yield, (2) Look to Yield (LTY), in which the AV yielded when the driver looked at the pedestrian, and (3) Look Away to Yield (LATY), in which the AV yielded when the driver did not look at the pedestrian. The driver's eye movements in the LTY and LATY conditions were visualized using a virtual light beam. Crossing performance was assessed based on whether the pedestrian held the button when the AV yielded and released the button when the AV did not yield. Furthermore, the pedes-trians' and drivers' acceptance of the mappings was measured through a questionnaire. The results showed that the LTY and LATY mappings yielded better crossing performance than Baseline. Furthermore, the LTY condition was best accepted by drivers and pedestrians. Eye-tracking analyses indicated that the LTY and LATY mappings attracted the pedestrian's attention, while pedestrians still distributed their attention between the AV and a second vehicle approaching from the other direction. In conclusion, LTY control may be a promising means of AV control at intersections before full automation is technologically feasible.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Cycling has become increasingly popular as a means of transportation. However, cyclists remain a highly vulnerable group of road users. According to accident reports, one of the most dangerous situations for cyclists are uncontrolled intersections, where cars approach from both directions. To address this issue and assist cyclists in crossing decision-making at uncontrolled intersections, we designed two visualizations that: (1) highlight occluded cars through an X-ray vision and (2) depict the remaining time the intersection is safe to cross via a Countdown. To investigate the efficiency of these visualizations, we proposed an Augmented Reality simulation as a novel evaluation method, in which the above visualizations are represented as AR, and conducted a controlled experiment with 24 participants indoors. We found that the X-ray ensures a fast selection of shorter gaps between cars, while the Countdown facilitates a feeling of safety and provides a better intersection overview.
Article
Full-text available
Wearable augmented reality (AR) offers new ways for supporting the interaction between autonomous vehicles (AVs) and pedestrians due to its ability to integrate timely and contextually relevant data into the user's field of view. This article presents novel wearable AR concepts that assist crossing pedestrians in multi-vehicle scenarios where several AVs frequent the road from both directions. Three concepts with different communication approaches for signaling responses from multiple AVs to a crossing request, as well as a conventional pedestrian push button, were simulated and tested within a virtual reality environment. The results showed that wearable AR is a promising way to reduce crossing pedestrians' cognitive load when the design offers both individual AV responses and a clear signal to cross. The willingness of pedestrians to adopt a wearable AR solution, however, is subject to different factors, including costs, data privacy, technical defects, liability risks, maintenance duties, and form factors. We further found that all participants favored sending a crossing request to AVs rather than waiting for the vehicles to detect their intentions-pointing to an important gap and opportunity in the current AV-pedestrian interaction literature.
Technical Report
Full-text available
One of the main objectives of the L3Pilot was the large-scale piloting of Automated Driving Functions (ADFs) with a focus on level 3 functions. Since the development of ADFs, especially at SAE L3, is fairly well progressed, the aim was to pilot the functions, and to study user preferences, reactions and willingness to use vehicles equipped with AD applications. To this purpose, a largescale L3Pilot Global User Acceptance Survey was administered to 27,970 respondents from 17 countries to provide a comprehensive picture of user acceptance, and identify major challenges related to L3 automation in this field. This study represents the first long-term and global study on user acceptance, attitudes and expectations around automated driving, with a focus on L3 technology.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The future urban environment may consist of mixed traffic in which pedestrians interact with automated vehicles (AVs). However, it is still unclear how AVs should communicate their intentions to pedestrians. Augmented reality (AR) technology could transform the future of interactions between pedestrians and AVs by offering targeted and individualized communication. This paper presents nine prototypes of AR concepts for pedestrian-AV interaction that are implemented and demonstrated in a real crossing environment. Each concept was based on expert perspectives and designed using theoretically-informed brainstorming sessions. Prototypes were implemented in Unity MARS and subsequently tested on an un-marked road using a standalone iPad Pro with LiDAR functionality. Despite the limitations of the technology, this paper offers an indication of how future AR systems may support future pedestrian-AV interactions.
Article
Full-text available
Non-verbal communication, such as eye contact between drivers and pedestrians, has been regarded as one way to reduce accident risk. So far, studies have assumed rather than objectively measured the occurrence of eye contact. We address this research gap by developing an eye contact detection method and testing it in an indoor experiment with scripted driver-pedestrian interactions at a pedestrian crossing. Thirty participants acted as a pedestrian either standing on an imaginary curb or crossing an imaginary one-lane road in front of a stationary vehicle with an experimenter in the driver’s seat. In half of the trials, pedestrians were instructed to make eye contact with the driver; in the other half, they were prohibited from doing so. Both parties’ gaze was recorded using eye trackers. An in-vehicle stereo camera recorded the car’s point of view, a head-mounted camera recorded the pedestrian’s point of view, and the location of the driver’s and pedestrian’s eyes was estimated using image recognition. We demonstrate that eye contact can be detected by measuring the angles between the vector joining the estimated location of the driver’s and pedestrian’s eyes, and the pedestrian’s and driver’s instantaneous gaze directions, respectively, and identifying whether these angles fall below a threshold of 4°. We achieved 100% correct classification of the trials involving eye contact and those without eye contact, based on measured eye contact duration. The proposed eye contact detection method may be useful for future research into eye contact.
Article
The number of automated vehicles (AVs) is expected to successively increase in the near future. This development has a considerable impact on the informal communication between AVs and pedestrians. Informal communication with the driver will become obsolete during the interaction with AVs. Literature suggests that external human machine interfaces (eHMIs) might substitute the communication between drivers and pedestrians. In the study, we additionally test a recently discussed type of communication in terms of artificial vehicle motion, namely active pitch motion, as an informal communication cue for AVs. N = 54 participants approached AVs in a virtual inner-city traffic environment. We explored the effect of three communication concepts: an artificial vehicle motion, namely active pitch motion, eHMI and the combination of both. Moreover, vehicle types (sports car, limousine, SUV) were varied. A mixed-method approach was applied to investigate the participantś crossing behavior and subjective safety feeling. Furthermore, eye movement parameters were recorded as indicators for mental workload. The results revealed that any communication concept drove beneficial effects on the crossing behavior. The participants crossed the road earlier when an active pitch motion was present, as this was interpreted as a stronger braking. Further, the eHMI and a combination of eHMI and active pitch motion had a positive effect on the crossing behavior. The active pitch motion showed no effect on the subjective safety feeling, while eHMI and the combination enhanced the pedestrianś safety feeling while crossing. The use of communication resulted in less mental workload, as evidenced by eye-tracking parameters. Variations of vehicle types did not result in significant main effects but revealed interactions between parameters. The active pitch motion revealed no learning. In contrast, it took participants several trials for the eHMI and the combination condition to affect their crossing behavior. To sum up, this study indicates that communication between AVs and pedestrians can benefit from the consideration of vehicle motion.