ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Research on awe and meaning in life (MIL) is rare. In the current research, we conducted a pre-registered study to examine how awe influences MIL from the perspective of the tripartite model of MIL as well as the construction and detection routes of deriving meaning. The results showed that awe increased MIL via motivating purpose pursuit but decreased MIL by reducing the sense of significance. Overall, awe increased MIL, which was driven mainly by the mediating effect of purpose pursuit. Our findings suggest that awe is not a purely positive emotion, and it affects MIL in a complex way. The implications were discussed.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
RESEARCH PAPER
Accepted: 10 July 2022 / Published online: 6 August 2022
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature B.V. 2022
Tonglin Jiang
tljiang@pku.edu.cn
Yuwan Dai
daiyuwan@pku.edu.cn
Miao Miao
miaomaio@bjmu.edu.cn
1 School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and
Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China
2 Department of Medical Psychology, School of Health Humanities, Peking University, Beijing,
China
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
YuwanDai1· TonglinJiang1· MiaoMiao2
Journal of Happiness Studies (2022) 23:3517–3529
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-022-00559-6
Abstract
Research on awe and meaning in life (MIL) is rare. In the current research, we con-
ducted a pre-registered study to examine how awe inuences MIL from the perspective
of the tripartite model of MIL as well as the construction and detection routes of deriving
meaning. The results showed that awe increased MIL via motivating purpose pursuit but
decreased MIL by reducing the sense of signicance. Overall, awe increased MIL, which
was driven mainly by the mediating eect of purpose pursuit. Our ndings suggest that
awe is not a purely positive emotion, and it aects MIL in a complex way. The implica-
tions were discussed.
Keywords Awe · Meaning in life · Purpose pursuit · Signicance feeling · Coherence
seeking
Awe is a complicated emotion characterized by a perceived sense of vastness and need for
accommodation (Keltner & Haidt, 2003). This perceived vastness comes from observing
something physically or conceptually vast. The need for accommodation refers to the pro-
cess that when the vast stimuli violate people’s common understanding of the world, people
need to change their mental structures to make sense of the stimuli and account for the
deviation between the vast stimuli and one’s current understanding of the world. The emerg-
ing science of awe has accumulated evidence to support a wide range of benecial eects
1 3
Y. Dai et al.
of awe. Awe has been found to increase positive mood, promote prosociality, induce humil-
ity, expand time perception, decrease materialism, improve subjective well-being and life
satisfaction, and enhance the joy and prosocial emotions of the elderly (e.g., Anderson et al.,
2018; Bai et al., 2021; Jiang et al., 2018; Jiang & Sedikides, 2021; Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015;
Pi et al., 2015; Rudd et al., 2012; Stellar et al., 2017; Sturm et al., 2020). In addition, awe
could act as a coping strategy for protecting people from negative incidents such as daily
stress and uncertain waiting periods (Bai et al., 2021; Koh et al., 2019; Rankin et al., 2020).
Awe has also been theorized as a meaning-making emotion (Danvers et al., 2016; Ihm
et al., 2019; Stellar, 2021). However, little research has examined the eects of awe on the
outcomes of meaning-making, such as on the sense of meaning in life (MIL). In the current
research, we aimed to examine the eect of awe on MIL, specically by examining how awe
aects the sense of meaning via three facets of MIL: coherence, purpose, and signicance.
1 Awe as a Meaning-making Emotion
Awe is a meaning-making emotion (Danvers et al., 2016; Ihm et al., 2019; Stellar, 2021)
suggested that awe enables a meaning-making process by helping people establish a grand
framework that integrates people’s life experiences coherently and connects them to a
greater entity. Ihm et al. (2019) suggested that awe enhances the sense of MIL for two pos-
sible reasons. First, people feel awe when they encounter physically or conceptually vast
stimuli that cannot be fully comprehended, thus arousing the need for them to change their
mental structure in order to make sense of the vast stimuli (Keltner & Haidt, 2003). Accom-
modative meaning-making—revising the mental structures and beliefs to have a sense of
meaning—is the cognitive mechanism that could account for why awe enhances MIL (Ihm
et al., 2019). Second, awe also leads to a sense of self-smallness, leading people to feel that
they are small and insignicant (e.g., Bai et al., 2017; Pi et al., 2015; Shiota et al., 2007).
Ihm et al. (2019) suggested that the sense of self-smallness induced by awe reduces people’s
reliance on existing schemas to understand the world, thus promoting a meaning-making
process, which further results in an increased sense of meaning. In contrast, Rivera et al.
(2019) made an opposite suggestion: that self-smallness leads to a decrease in MIL, given
that awe induces the sense that one is small and insignicant.
Empirical research on the relationship between awe and MIL yields inconsistent results.
People who reported more frequent experiences of awe have been found to have a higher
score on MIL (Ihm et al., 2019). In addition, meaningful life events frequently were reported
as the source of awe, which often brought about signicant and lasting changes in people’s
lives (Ihm et al., 2019). However, inducing awe did not aect MIL (Rivera et al., 2019)
found that self-smallness accounted for the negative eect of awe on MIL, whereas hap-
piness accounted for the positive eects of awe on MIL. They suggested that the opposite
mediating eects explained why there was no signicant main eect of awe on MIL (Rivera
et al., 2019).
We suggest that happiness may not stand as a theoretically based mediator for awe’s
eect on MIL. Awe lies in the “upper reaches of pleasure and on the boundary of fear”
(Keltner & Haidt, 2003). Although awe usually includes other positive emotions (Shiota et
al., 2007; Valdesolo & Graham, 2014), positive aect is not the dening prototypical fea-
ture of awe (Keltner & Haidt, 2003). Consistent with this suggestion, Jiang and Sedikides
1 3
3518
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
(2021) found that compared with participants in the control condition, participants in the
awe condition always reported more positive (e.g., happiness and pride) and negative aect
(e.g., fear). Rivera et al. (2019) found that happiness mediated the positive eect of positive
awe on meaning; however, they found no signicant mediating eect of happiness on the
eect of negative awe, induced by threatening stimuli, on MIL (Rivera et al., 2019, Study
4). In comparing awe with amusement, happiness mediated the negative eect of awe on
MIL (Rivera et al., 2019, Study 5). It seems that whether and how happiness mediates the
eect of awe on meaning depends on the kind of awe-inducing stimuli. Moreover, although
happiness and MIL overlap substantially, they have great dierences (McGregor & Little,
1998). Happiness mainly comes from one’s needs and desires being suciently satised,
whereas MIL comes from the judgment that one’s life has purpose and value (Baumeister et
al., 2013). An unhappy but meaningful life or a happy but meaningless life are both possible.
King and Hicks (2021) suggested that other variables moderate the relationship between
positive aect and MIL. Awe is considered predominantly positive, but it often induces a
mix of positive and negative emotions (Bonner & Friedman, 2011; Keltner & Haidt, 2003).
Thus, although happiness is positively associated with MIL (King et al., 2006; Rivera et al.,
2019), suggesting that happiness accounts for the positive eect of awe on MIL seems not
that theoretically convincing.
To summarize, empirical research does not consistently support awe as being a meaning-
making emotion like psychologists have theorized. Whether and how awe impacts MIL
requires further investigation. In the next section, we discuss how awe aects the sense of
meaning via the three facets of MIL.
2 Awe, MIL, Coherence Seeking, Purpose Pursuit, and Signicance
Feeling
MIL is a construct that has long been studied as unidimensional subjective appraisals (e.g.,
George & Park, 2016a; Heintzelman & King, 2014; Steger et al., 2006). This simplied
research approach suers from conceptual ambiguity and fails to tap into the complexity
of MIL. Recently, researchers have converged on tripartite models of MIL involving coher-
ence, purpose, and signicance (Costin & Vignoles, 2020; George & Park, 2016a, b; King &
Hicks, 2021; Martela & Steger, 2016). Coherence means comprehensibility, predictability,
and order—it is about the feelings that one’s experiences and the world around make sense
(Heintzelman & King, 2014). However, self-related coherence and outside world coherence
are dierent. People could feel uncertain about themselves without feeling uncertain about
the world around them (Costin & Vignoles, 2020) suggested that a sense of meaning focuses
on one’s personal life and identity process; thus, self-related coherence is the dening sense-
making facet of MIL. Purpose refers to one’s directions and goals in life. Signicance refers
to the sense that one’s life is valuable and worth living (Costin & Vignoles, 2020; Heintzel-
man & King, 2014; George & Park, 2016a; Martela & Steger, 2016). Coherence, purpose,
and signicance are three bases upon which people judge their lives as meaningful (MIL
judgments; Costin & Vignoles, 2020); George and Park (2016b) found that purpose, signi-
cance, and coherence together accounted for 60–71% of the variance in MIL judgments, as
measured by dierent scales.
1 3
3519
Y. Dai et al.
As MIL judgment comprises purpose, signicance, and coherence, it is reasonable to
suggest that factors that inuence purpose, signicance, and coherence could further inu-
ence MIL judgment. Awe stands out as one of these potential factors. King and Hicks (2009)
distinguished two ways in which people may derive MIL: meaning detection and mean-
ing construction. Meaning detection involves an assimilation process of incorporating new
experiences into one’s mental structure. In contrast, meaning construction works when peo-
ple are faced with chaotic events, which involves people adjusting their mental structure to
accommodate the new stimuli (King & Hicks, 2009). Because awe is a triggering experience
that initiates the accommodation process, we hypothesize that awe may inuence the three
facets of MIL dierently via the two routes of meaning construction and meaning detection.
First, we suggest that awe aects coherence via the meaning-construction route. As dis-
cussed above, people feel awe when their current mental structure cannot accommodate the
vast stimuli, inspiring a need for them to adjust their mental structure for accommodation
(Keltner & Haidt, 2003). Hence, it is reasonable to suggest that awe motivates people to
seek coherence but does not instill a sense of coherence. Empirical research supports this
suggestion. For example, Valdesolo and Graham (2014) found that experimentally induced
awe increased the tendency to seek order and pattern in events, and intolerance for uncer-
tainty mediated this eect. MIL judgement requires a personal framework for making sense
of one’s life as a coherent whole. Seeking coherence promotes the development of such a
personal framework of meaning (Danvers et al., 2016; Ihm et al., 2019). Thus, we expect
that awe initiates the accommodative process of seeking coherence, which in turn contrib-
utes to MIL.
Second, awe impacts purpose via the meaning-construction route. The experience of
awe always makes people ponder deeper questions and reevaluate their goals and purpose
in life (Bonner & Friedman, 2011; Danvers & Shiota, 2017; Schneider, 2009). Awe is also a
self-transcendent emotion. Its self-transcendent nature allows individuals to set aside their
day-to-day concerns and reach out beyond their self-boundary (Jiang et al., 2018; Jiang &
Sedikides, 2021; Reed, 1991), which liberates individuals from the constraints of daily life
and fosters a sense of autonomy. Autonomy enables a sense of freedom to follow one’s own
heart and encourages people to pursue their intrinsic goals (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Consistent
with these suggestions, Jiang and Sedikides (2021) further found that awe motivated people
to pursue the authentic self, as manifested by their important goals and values. In addi-
tion, awe is an approach-oriented emotion (Emmerich & Rigotti, 2017; Impett et al., 2013),
which is conducive to pursuing one’s purpose. Thus, we suggest that awe motivates purpose
pursuit, rather than directly increases the sense of purpose. As meaningful life is expressed
as the pursuit of personally signicant goals (e.g., Costin & Vignoles, 2020; Emmons,
2003), we expect awe enhances MIL judgment via purpose pursuit.
Third, awe aects the sense of signicance via the meaning-detection route. The vastness
associated with awe makes people aware of how small they are. The sense of insignicance
or smallness is well incorporated into people’s mental structure in the face of awe. A grow-
ing body of empirical research has lent support to awe’s small-self eect (Bai et al., 2017;
Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015; Pi et al., 2015; Shiota et al., 2007), which was found consis-
tently in both independent and collectivistic cultures (Bai et al., 2017). Thus, we suggest
that awe triggers feelings of insignicance and smallness. Signicance refers to feelings
of importance and self-worth (George & Park, 2016a; Heintzelman & King, 2014; Martela
& Steger, 2016). The sense of insignicance induced by awe may decrease MIL judgment,
1 3
3520
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
given that an important aspect of MIL is the feeling of signicance. Consistent with this
suggestion, Rivera et al. (2019) found that self-smallness accounts for the negative eects
of awe on MIL. Thus, we predict that awe decreases MIL judgment via its negative eect
on signicance feeling.
To summarize, we hypothesize that awe increases MIL via coherence seeking and pur-
pose pursuit, but decreases MIL via reducing signicance feeling. Because the indirect
eects are competing, we made no specic hypothesis about the total eect of awe on MIL.
3 Overview of the Current Research
In the current research, from the perspective of the meaning-construction and meaning-detec-
tion routes of deriving MIL, we tested whether induced awe motivates coherence seeking
and purpose pursuit and reduces the sense of signicance, as well as whether awe inuences
MIL judgment via increasing purpose pursuit and coherence seeking, and decreasing sig-
nicance feeling. We received ethical approval from the corresponding author’s institution.
4 Method
4.1 Participants and Design
We used a video clip to manipulate awe, a common method of inducing awe (Algoe &
Haidt, 2009; Jiang & Sedikides, 2021; Pi et al., 2015; Prade & Saroglou, 2016), as well as
scales to measure coherence seeking, purpose pursuit, sense of signicance, and MIL. The
experiment was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the
Ethics Committee of the corresponding author’s institution. We preregistered this study on
Aspredicted (https://aspredicted.org/id535.pdf).
We tested 300 Chinese participants on Credamo, a reliable platform to recruit partici-
pants similar to Qualtrics Online Sample or Amazon Mechanical Turk (Gong et al., 2020;
Jiang & Sedikides, 2021). We excluded 18 for failing the attention check questions, leaving
282 participants (127 men, 155 women; Mage = 28.15, SDage = 6.63) in the nal sample.
Among them, 276 were Han Chinese, and six were from other ethnicities; 9.93% had a
middle school, high school, or college (preparatory or apprenticeship) degree, 79.79% had a
bachelor’s degree, and 10.28% had a master’s or PhD degree. We randomly allocated them
to the experimental (n = 135) or control (n = 147) condition.
4.2 Procedure and Materials
In the experimental condition, the participants watched a 302-s video (validated by Hornsey
et al., 2018; Jiang & Sedikides, 2021), illustrating the comparative sizes of the Earth and
other celestial bodies. In the control condition, the participants watched a 302-s video illus-
trating how to make a table. Afterward, the participants answered two questions—“To what
extent are you feeling awe?” and “To what extent did the video you just watched make you
feel awe?”—on a 7-point scale, M = 5.06, SD = 1.69, r(282) = 0.93, p < .001 (1 = not at all,
7 = extremely, Koh et al., 2019), to check whether our awe induction was eective or not.
1 3
3521
Y. Dai et al.
Next, we measured coherence seeking, purpose pursuit, signicance feeling, and MIL
by adapting the Multidimensional Meaning in Life Scale (Costin & Vignoles, 2020). The
scale has four dimensions: sense of coherence, purpose, mattering, and MIL judgments. We
rephrased the sentences to capture coherence seeking (e.g., “I would like to make sense of
the things that happen in my life”), purpose pursuit (e.g., “I am motivated to pursue what I
am trying to accomplish in my life”), and signicance feeling (e.g., “I feel whether my life
ever existed matters even in the grand scheme of the universe,” reverse-coded), and used the
original items to measure MIL judgements (e.g., “My life as a whole has meaning”). Par-
ticipants had to indicate their agreement with the items. The scores of the items from each
dimension were averaged to index coherence seeking (1 = strongly disagree, 9 = strongly
agree; M = 6.23, SD = 1.04, α = 0.53), purpose pursuit (1 = strongly disagree, 9 = strongly
agree; M = 6.98, SD = 1.30, α = 0.86), signicance feeling (1 = strongly disagree, 9 = strongly
agree; M = 5.10, SD = 1.56, α = 0.77), and MIL (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree;
M = 5.70, SD = 1.08, α = 0.87). The items in the subscales were presented in separate random
order for each participant.
5 Results and Discussion
5.1 Awe Manipulation Check
We present descriptive statistics in Table 1. As predicted, the experimental participants felt
more awe than the control participants did, F(1, 280) = 249.38, p < .001, ηp
2 = 0.471.
5.2 Coherence Seeking, Purpose Pursuit, Significance Feeling, and MIL
Participants in the experimental condition reported greater willingness to pursue purpose,
F(1, 280) = 5.68, p = .018, ηp
2 = 0.020, but less sense of signicance, F(1, 280) = 32.98,
p < .001, ηp
2 = 0.105, than those in the control condition. No dierence was found in coher-
ence seeking, F(1, 280) = 1.41, p = .24, ηp
2 = 0.005. In addition, the participants in the experi-
mental condition reported more sense of MIL than those in the control condition did, F(1,
280) = 5.16, p = .024, ηp
2 = 0.018.
5.3 Mediational Analysis
Next, we tested whether purpose pursuit, signicance feeling, and coherence seeking medi-
ated the eect of awe on MIL. We ran a bootstrapping mediational analysis (Hayes, 2018;
PROCESS 3.5, Model 4) with 5,000 interactions. We entered the awe manipulation as an
Awe Control
Mean SD Mean SD
Awe 6.27 0.72 3.95 1.56
Coherence seeking 6.30 1.08 6.15 1.01
Purpose pursuit 7.17 1.22 6.80 1.35
Signicance feeling 4.58 1.69 5.59 1.25
MIL 5.85 1.06 5.56 1.09
Table 1 Descriptive Statistics
1 3
3522
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
independent variable (1 = experimental condition, 0 = control condition); coherence seeking,
purpose pursuit, and signicance feeling as mediators; and MIL as the dependent variable.
The indirect eect of coherence seeking was not signicant, b = 0.05, SE = 0.05, 95% CI
[ 0.0323, 0.1489]; the indirect eect of purpose pursuit was signicant, b = 0.14, SE = 0.07,
95% CI [0.0249, 0.2833]; and the indirect eect of signicance feeling was signicant,
b = 0.08, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [ 0.1560, 0.0219] (see Fig. 1). The indirect eect of purpose
pursuit was larger than the indirect eect of signicance feeling, which could account for
that the overall eect of awe on MIL was positive.
These results suggested that awe motivated people to pursue purpose, which further con-
tributed to their sense of MIL. Meanwhile, awe also made people feel insignicance, which
was further detrimental to their sense of MIL. Coherence seeking could not account for the
eect of awe on the sense of MIL.
6 Discussion
Awe has been identied as “the ultimate humanistic moment” (Konecni, 2005, p. 27), in
which people are challenged to look beyond themselves and ponder deep questions about
the MIL. Coherence, purpose, and signicance are three bases upon which people make
MIL judgments (Costin & Vignoles, 2020). However, whether and how the three facets of
MIL contribute to the relationship between awe and MIL was not empirically examined in
prior research. Drawing upon the tripartite model of MIL (e.g., Costin & Vignoles, 2020)
as well as the meaning-construction and meaning-detection routes proposed by King and
Hicks (2009), we found that awe motivated people to pursue their purpose and reduced
their feelings of signicance, which further had competing eects on MIL. Specically, we
found that awe increased MIL judgment via motivating purpose seeking but decreased MIL
judgment via reducing the sense of signicance, which is consistent with previous nd-
ings that MIL was positively associated with an approach orientation (Steger et al., 2008)
Fig. 1 The Eect of Awe, Mediated by Coherence Seeking, Purpose Pursuit, and Signicant Feeling, on
Meaning in Life. Unstandardized coecients displayed
1 3
3523
Y. Dai et al.
and was negatively associated with a sense of insignicance and smallness (Rivera et al.,
2019). In contrast to Rivera’s (2019) ndings, we found that awe increased MIL, which was
driven mainly by the mediating eect of purpose pursuit. To our knowledge, the current
research provides the rst empirical evidence examining the eect of awe on MIL from the
perspective of the tripartite model of MIL. These ndings advance our knowledge about the
psychological mechanisms underlying the eect of awe and MIL.
Contrary to our hypothesis, in the current research, we did not nd that awe motivated
coherence seeking. This may be because feeling awe makes people realize that something
exists beyond their comprehensibility. Awe induces humility (Stellar et al., 2018), which
refers to transcending oneself and accepting one’s limits and place in the grand scheme of
things (Kesebir, 2014). Feeling awe makes people feel humble, accurately as well as less
defensively appraise their own characteristics, acknowledge their limitations, and accept
what happens in their life. Thus, their motivation to seek order and comprehensibility in
their personal life might be attenuated. Supporting this suggestion, dispositional awe and
induced awe have been found to be negatively related to, or decrease the need for cognitive
closure (Lv et al., 2021; Shiota et al., 2007). Awe also has been proposed to promote per-
sonal growth and transformative change, which is closely related to MIL, via augmenting
the capacity to accept ambiguity (Armstrong & Detweiler-Bedel, 2008; Bonner & Fried-
man, 2011). These pieces of evidence yield some plausible explanations about why awe
did not motivate coherence seeking. However, our research just provides a single source of
evidence. More systematic research is needed.
Our ndings that awe did not motivate coherence seeking do not speak against Valdesolo
and Graham’s (2014) ndings. In their research, awe motivated people to seek a pattern
and order in the outside world, which is consistent with ndings showing that awe was
positively associated with openness and curiosity (Anderson et al., 2020; Rudd et al., 2018).
However, coherence seeking of MIL concerns seeking predictability and order in one’s per-
sonal life (Costin & Vignoles, 2020). Not seeking predictability and order in one’s personal
life in the wake of awe may reect the self-transcendent nature and humility aspect of awe
(e.g., Jiang & Sedikides, 2021; Stellar et al., 2018). Together with Valdesolo and Graham’s
(2014) ndings, our research indicates that awe might have divergent impacts on coherence
seeking in the outside world and self-related coherence seeking.
Our research speaks to the complexity of awe. We found that awe had divergent and even
opposing eects on dierent facets of MIL. More importantly, we found that although awe
has competing eects on MIL, the insignicance feeling triggered by awe did not mask the
overall eects of awe on MIL. Such ndings imply that as a self-transcendent emotion with
signicant psychological benets (e.g., Bai et al., 2021; Jiang & Sedikides, 2021; Rudd et
al., 2012), awe does not work as a purely positive emotion like most positive psychologi-
cal research examines it as being. There might be a complicated mechanism underlying the
eect of awe on certain outcomes.
Our research has practical implications. Awe has a wide range of psychological ben-
ets, such as improving mental health, well-being, and prosociality (e.g., Anderson et al.,
2018; Bai et al., 2021; Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015). Emerging research has studied awe as a
buer with which to cope with negative incidents (e.g., Bai et al., 2021; Koh et al., 2019;
Rankin et al., 2020). Our ndings suggest that psychological practitioners should take cau-
tious steps in incorporating awe as a strategy to enhance MIL in their practice. Given the
current ndings, awe promotes MIL. However, awe is not a positive emotion with purely
1 3
3524
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
positive eects. Practitioners who seek to foster MIL via awe should focus on bolstering
the purpose pursuit or on inhibiting the threatening eect of insignicance sense. To inhibit
the threatening eect of insignicance feelings, it is imperative to dierentiate the sense of
smallness and insignicance from the decreased sense of self-salience, which are related
yet distinguishable feelings in the wake of awe. The sense of smallness and insignicance
comes from perceived vastness (Pi et al., 2015). Decreased sense of self-salience refers
to reducing the awareness of the self as a separate and distinct entity, which arises from
shifting attention away from the self and outward to a larger entity (Stellar, 2021). It is the
reducing sense of self-salience that makes feeling small and insignicant less threatening in
the wake of awe (Stellar, 2021). Our current ndings lend credence to the relatively smaller
threatening eect of feeling insignicance. Thus, practitioners could try to reduce the threat-
ening eect of feeling insignicance by augmenting the decreased sense of self-salience.
7 Limitations and Future Directions
Awe may inuence MIL via other routes. For example, self-transcendence is a source of
MIL (Frankl, 1973). Awe is a self-transcendent emotion (Chirico & Yaden, 2018; Jiang
& Sedikides, 2021). It is reasonable to expect that awe may increase MIL via self-tran-
scendence. In addition, religion is a powerful source of meaning in life for many people.
Research has found that awe promotes spirituality and religiosity (Saroglou et al., 2008; Van
Cappellen & Saroglou, 2012), which will also lead to increased MIL. Future research could
test these possibilities.
In the current research, we found the four-item coherence-seeking measure had rela-
tively low internal consistency (α = 0.53, r = .35, ω = 0.70). Previous research suggested that
for short scales with less than ve items, a Cronbach’s alpha exceeding 0.50 is acceptable
(Hinton et al., 2014). In addition, when estimating the reliability of short scales, the main
inter-item correlations should be within a recommended optimal range from 0.20 to 0.40
(Piedmont, 2004), and omega is a more reasonable index (Hayes & Coutts, 2020). Overall,
the reliability of the four-item coherence-seeking measure is acceptable. Nevertheless, we
conducted an item-to-total correlation and an exploratory factor analysis. The results sug-
gested that the fourth item (“I would like to live a life that feels like a sequence of uncon-
nected events”) should be deleted for better internal consistency. After removing the fourth
item, the results consistently supported the nonsignicant main eect of awe on coherence
seeking as well as the nonsignicant mediating eect of coherence seeking,1 suggesting
that the nonsignicant results could not be attributed to the coherence-seeking measure.
Future research should investigate this issue with other valid and reliable coherence-seeking
measurements.
Awe is a complex emotion. The experience of awe could have dierent variants depend-
ing on the dierent characteristics of inducing stimuli. (Keltner & Haidt, 2003). In the cur-
rent research, we induced awe with the video depicting the comparative sizes of the Earth
and other celestial bodies, which is a commonly-used awe-inducing stimulus (e.g., Hornsey
et al., 2018; Jiang & Sedikides, 2021). Future research could extend these ndings by inves-
tigating whether other awe-inducing stimuli have the same competing eects on MIL via
purpose pursuit and the sense of insignicance as we found in current research. Specically,
1 We report the results in the supplementary materials.
1 3
3525
Y. Dai et al.
we thought it worth investigating awe induced by threatening experience (Gordon et al.,
2017). Awe induced by threatening experience has been found to lead to lower self-control
and a sense of powerlessness due to the associated threat appraisals (Gordon et al., 2017). It
is likely that the awe induced by threatening experience may not motivate purpose pursuit,
and hence may not foster MIL. This warrants careful examination in future studies.
Future research could examine how individual dierences impact the ndings we
observed in current research. For example, religious beliefs may be one of the factors worth
investigating. Comparing those without religious beliefs, people with religious beliefs
might have a strong sense of purpose (Galek et al., 2015). The boosting eect of awe on
MIL via purpose would be strengthened, whereas the detrimental eect of awe on MIL via
insignicance feelings would be weakened for those with religious beliefs.
In the current research, we focused on the sense of present meaning without considering
searching for meaning, another important aspect of meaning in life (Steger et al., 2008). Our
aim was to investigate how awe impacts MIL via three facets of MIL. The tripartite model
of MIL we drew upon and the Multidimensional MIL Scale we used as the measurement
(Costin & Vignoles, 2020) do not speak to the searching for meaning aspect. Thus, search-
ing for meaning is beyond the scope of current research. Searching for meaning and present
meaning was not linearly associated (Steger, 2012). Whether and how awe impacts search-
ing for meaning merits investigation.
8 Conclusions
Research on awe as a positive emotion and its positive consequences has ourished in the
last few years. We found that awe promoted MIL via increasing purpose pursuit and decreas-
ing the sense of signicance. These ndings carry theoretical and practical implications by
suggesting that awe is a complex feeling despite its positive valence. MIL, along with other
outcomes, may respond to awe experience in a complex way.
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-
022-00559-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Authors’ Contribution Tonglin Jiang and Yuwan Dai contributed to the study conception and design. Material
preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by Yuwan Dai and Tonglin Jiang. The rst draft of
the manuscript was written by Tonglin Jiang and all authors commented on previous versions of the manu-
script. All authors read and approved the nal manuscript.
Funding This research was supported by National Natural Science Foundation for Young Scholars awarded
to Tonglin Jiang by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No.: 32000767). We conrm
that we have mentioned all organizations that funded our research in the Acknowledgements section of my
submission, including grant numbers where appropriate.
Data Availability We conrm that all data and materials support our published claims and comply with eld
standards. And the datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the
corresponding author on reasonable request.
Declarations
Conflict of Interest We conrm that we have no known competing nancial interests or personal relationships
that could have appeared to inuence the work reported in this paper.
1 3
3526
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
Ethics Approval The experiment was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the
Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology, Peking University.
Consent to Participate Informed consent was obtained from each participant before the experiment.
References
Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: the ‘other-praising’ emotions of eleva-
tion, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105–127. https://doi.
org/10.1080/17439760802650519
Anderson, C. L., Dixson, D. D., Monroy, M., & Keltner, D. (2020). Are awe-prone people more curious?
The relationship between dispositional awe, curiosity, and academic outcomes. Journal of Personality,
88(4), 762–779. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12524
Anderson, C. L., Monroy, M., & Keltner, D. (2018). Awe in nature heals: Evidence from military veterans,
at-risk youth, and college students. Emotion, 18(8), 1195–1202. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000442
Armstrong, T., & Detweiler-Bedell, B. (2008). Beauty as an Emotion: The Exhilarating Prospect of Mas-
tering a Challenging World. Review of General Psychology, 12(4), 305–329. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0012558
Bai, Y., Maruskin, L. A., Chen, S., Gordon, A. M., Stellar, J. E., McNeil, G. D., Peng, K., & Keltner, D.
(2017). Awe, the diminished self, and collective engagement: Universals and cultural variations in the
small self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113 (2), 185–209. https://doi.org/10.1037/
pspa0000087
Bai, Y., Ocampo, J., Jin, G., Chen, S., Benet-Martinez, V., Monroy, M., Anderson, C., & Keltner, D. (2021).
Awe, daily stress, and elevated life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4),
837–860. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000267
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., Aaker, J. L., & Garbinsky, E. N. (2013). Some key dierences between a
happy life and a meaningful life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(6), 505–516. https://doi.org/10
.1080/17439760.2013.830764
Bonner, E. T., & Friedman, H. L. (2011). A conceptual clarication of the experience of awe: An interpreta-
tive phenomenological analysis. The Humanistic Psychologist, 39(3), 222–235. https://doi.org/10.108
0/08873267.2011.593372
Chirico, A., & Yaden, D. B. (2018). Awe: A self-transcendent and sometimes transformative emotion. In H.
C. Lench (Ed.), The function of emotions (pp. 221–233). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.
org/10.1007/978-3-319-77619-4
Costin, V., & Vignoles, V. L. (2020). Meaning is about mattering: Evaluating coherence, purpose, and exis-
tential mattering as precursors of meaning in life judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 118 (4), 864–884. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000225
Danvers, A. F., O’Neil, M. J., & Shiota, M. N. (2016). The Mind of the “Happy Warrior”: Eudaimonia, Awe,
and the Search for Meaning in Life. In J. Vittersø (Ed.), Handbook of Eudaimonic Well-Being (pp.
323–335). Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-42445-3_21
Danvers, A. F., & Shiota, M. N. (2017). Going o script: Eects of awe on memory for script-typical and
-irrelevant narrative detail. Emotion, 17(6), 938–952. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000277
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “What” and “Why” of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the
Self-Determination of Behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11 (4), 227–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15327965pli1104_01
Emmerich, A. I., & Rigotti, T. (2017). Reciprocal Relations between Work-Related Authenticity and Intrinsic
Motivation, Work Ability and Depressivity: A Two-Wave Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00307
Emmons, R. A. (2003). Personal goals, life meaning, and virtue: Wellsprings of a positive life. In C. L. M.
Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 105–128). Ameri-
can Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10594-005
Frankl, V. E. (1973). The doctor and the soul: From therapy to logotherapy. New York: Random House
Galek, K., Flannelly, K. J., Ellison, C. G., Silton, N. R., & Jankowski, K. R. B. (2015). Religion, meaning and
purpose, and mental health. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 7(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0037887
George, L. S., & Park, C. L. (2016a). Meaning in Life as Comprehension, Purpose, and Mattering: Toward
Integration and New Research Questions. Review of General Psychology, 20(3), 205–220. https://doi.
org/10.1037/gpr0000077
1 3
3527
Y. Dai et al.
George, L. S., & Park, C. L. (2016b). The Multidimensional Existential Meaning Scale: A tripartite approach
to measuring meaning in life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 12(6), 613–627. https://doi.org/10.1
080/17439760.2016.1209546
Gong, S., Lu, J. G., Schaubroeck, J. M., Li, Q., Zhou, Q., & Qian, X. (2020). Polluted psyche: Is the eect of
air pollution on unethical behavior more physiological or psychological? Psychological Science, 31(8),
1040–1047. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620943835
Gordon, A. M., Stellar, J. E., Anderson, C. L., McNeil, G. D., Loew, D., & Keltner, D. J. (2017). The dark
side of the sublime: Distinguishing a threat-based variant of awe. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 113 (2), 310–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000120
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Quantication, inference, and
interpretation. Communication Monographs, 85(1), 4–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.135
2100
Hayes, A. F., & Coutts, J. J. (2020). Use Omega Rather than Cronbach’s Alpha for Estimating Reliability.
But… Communication Methods and Measures, 14(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2020.1
718629
Heintzelman, S. J., & King, L. A. (2014). Life is pretty meaningful. American Psychologist, 69(6), 561–574.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035049
Hinton, P. R., McMurray, I., & Brownlow, C. (2014). SPSS Explained (2nd ed.). Routledge
Hornsey, M. J., Faulkner, C., Crimston, D., & Moreton, S. (2018). A microscopic dot on a microscopic dot:
Self-esteem buers the negative eects of exposure to the enormity of the universe. Journal of Experi-
mental Social Psychology, 76, 198–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.02.009
Ihm, E. D., Paloutzian, R. F., van Elk, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2019). Awe as a meaning-making emotion: On
the evolution of awe and the origin of religions. In J. R. Feierman, & L. Oviedo (Eds.), The Evolution
of Religion, Religiosity and Theology (pp. 138–153). Routledge
Impett, E. A., Javam, L., Le, B. M., ASYABI-ESHGHI, B., & Kogan, A. (2013). The joys of genuine giving:
Approach and avoidance sacrice motivation and authenticity. Personal Relationships, 20(4), 740–754.
https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12012
Jiang, L., Yin, J., Mei, D., Zhu, H., & Zhou, X. (2018). Awe Weakens the Desire for Money. Journal of Pacic
Rim Psychology, 12, e4. https://doi.org/10.1017/prp.2017.27
Jiang, T., & Sedikides, C. (2021). Awe motivates authentic-self pursuit via self-transcendence: Implications
for prosociality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000381.
Advance online publication
Joye, Y., & Bolderdijk, J. W. (2015). An exploratory study into the eects of extraordinary nature on emo-
tions, mood, and prosociality. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01577
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and
Emotion, 17(2), 297–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930302297
Kesebir, P. (2014). A quiet ego quiets death anxiety: Humility as an existential anxiety buer. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 106(4), 610–623. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035814
King, L. A., & Hicks, J. A. (2009). Detecting and constructing meaning in life events. The Journal of Positive
Psychology, 4(5), 317–330. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760902992316
King, L. A., & Hicks, J. A. (2021). The Science of Meaning in Life. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1),
561–584. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-122921
King, L. A., Hicks, J. A., Krull, J. L., & del Gaiso, A. K. (2006). Positive aect and the experience of
meaning in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(1), 179–196. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.179
Koh, A. H. Q., Tong, E. M. W., & Yuen, A. Y. L. (2019). The buering eect of awe on negative aect
towards lost possessions. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 14(2), 156–165. https://doi.org/10.1080
/17439760.2017.1388431
Konecni, V. J. (2005). The aesthetic trinity: Awe, being moved, thrills. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts,
5(2), 27–44
Lv, Y., Shi, J., Yu, F., & Zhang, C. (2021). The eect of awe on natural risk-taking preferences: The role of
need for closure. Current Psychology, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01758-9
Martela, F., & Steger, M. F. (2016). The three meanings of meaning in life: Distinguishing coherence, pur-
pose, and signicance. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 11 (5), 531–545. https://doi.org/10.1080/1
7439760.2015.1137623
McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and
being yourself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 494–512. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.494
Piedmont, R. L. (2004). Spiritual Transcendence as a Predictor of Psychosocial Outcome From an Out-
patient Substance Abuse Program. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18(3), 213–222. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0893-164x.18.3.213
1 3
3528
Uncovering the Effects of Awe on Meaning in Life
Pi, P. K., Dietze, P., Feinberg, M., Stancato, D. M., & Keltner, D. (2015). Awe, the small self, and proso-
cial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(6), 883–899. https://doi.org/10.1037/
pspi0000018
Prade, C., & Saroglou, V. (2016). Awe’s eects on generosity and helping. The Journal of Positive Psychol-
ogy, 11(5), 522–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1127992
Rankin, K., Andrews, S. E., & Sweeny, K. (2020). Awe-full uncertainty: Easing discomfort during waiting
periods. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 15(3), 338–347. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2019
.1615106
Reed, P. G. (1991). Self-Transcendence and Mental Health in Oldest-Old Adults. Nursing Research, 40(1),
https://doi.org/10.1097/00006199-199101000-00002
Rivera, G. N., Vess, M., Hicks, J. A., & Routledge, C. (2019). Awe and meaning: Elucidating complex eects
of awe experiences on meaning in life. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(2), 392–405. https://
doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2604
Rudd, M., Hildebrand, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). Inspired to Create: Awe Enhances Openness to Learning
and the Desire for Experiential Creation. Journal of Marketing Research, 55(5), 766–781. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0022243718802853
Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe Expands People’s Perception of Time, Alters Deci-
sion Making, and Enhances Well-Being. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1130–1136. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0956797612438731
Saroglou, V., Buxant, C., & Tilquin, J. (2008). Positive emotions as leading to religion and spirituality. The
Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(3), 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760801998737
Schneider, K. J. (2009). Awakening to Awe: Personal Stories of Profound Transformation. Jason Aronson, Inc
Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and eects on
self-concept. Cognition and Emotion, 21(5), 944–963. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600923668
Steger, M. F. (2012). Making meaning in life. Psychological Inquiry, 23(4), 381–385. https://doi.org/10.108
0/1047840X.2012.720832
Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the
presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53(1), 80–93. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80
Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., & Lorentz, D. (2008). Understanding the Search for Meaning
in Life: Personality, Cognitive Style, and the Dynamic Between Seeking and Experiencing Meaning.
Journal of Personality, 76(2), 199–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x
Stellar, J. E. (2021). Awe helps us remember why it is important to forget the self. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 1501(1), 81–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14577
Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A., Anderson, C. L., Pi, P. K., McNeil, G. D., & Keltner, D. (2018). Awe and humility.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114 (2), 258–269. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000109
Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A. M., Pi, P. K., Cordaro, D., Anderson, C. L., Bai, Y., Maruskin, L. A., & Kelt-
ner, D. (2017). Self-Transcendent Emotions and Their Social Functions: Compassion, Gratitude,
and Awe Bind Us to Others Through Prosociality. Emotion Review, 9(3), 200–207. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1754073916684557
Sturm, V. E., Datta, S., Roy, A. R. K., Sible, I. J., Kosik, E. L., Veziris, C. R., Chow, T. E., Morris, N.
A., Neuhaus, J., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L., Holley, S. R., & Keltner, D. (2020). Big smile, small
self: Awe walks promote prosocial positive emotions in older adults. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/
emo0000876. Advance online publication
Valdesolo, P., & Graham, J. (2014). Awe, Uncertainty, and Agency Detection. Psychological Science, 25(1),
170–178. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613501884
Van Cappellen, P., & Saroglou, V. (2012). Awe activates religious and spiritual feelings and behavioral inten-
tions. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 4(3), 223–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025986
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
1 3
3529
... Accounts for awe, as Rachel Carson wrote in The Edge of the Sea, often help people be aware of their meaning in life. Recently, researchers have begun to investigate the relationship between awe and meaning in life (Dai et al., 2022;Danvers et al., 2016;Ihm et al., 2019;Zhao et al., 2019). Although psychologists have suggested awe is a meaning-making emotion, existing findings regarding awe's effect on meaning in life are still mixed (Dai et al., 2022;Kim et al., 2022;. ...
... Recently, researchers have begun to investigate the relationship between awe and meaning in life (Dai et al., 2022;Danvers et al., 2016;Ihm et al., 2019;Zhao et al., 2019). Although psychologists have suggested awe is a meaning-making emotion, existing findings regarding awe's effect on meaning in life are still mixed (Dai et al., 2022;Kim et al., 2022;. Therefore, empirical research is still needed to clarify how and when awe contributes to meaning in life. ...
... Kim et al. (2022) found that inducing awe did not impact meaning in life. In contrast, Dai et al. (2022) found that awe induction increased meaning in life. found inconsistent effects. ...
Article
Full-text available
Awe is theoretically proposed as a meaning-making emotion. However, empirical evidence has shown that awe has mixed effects on meaning in life. The explanations for such complicated results have been limited. To fill this gap, in this research, we aimed to clarify how and when awe contributes to meaning in life. In six studies (N = 1,115), we examined the indirect effect of awe on meaning in life through authentic-self pursuit as well as trait authenticity’s moderating effect on this indirect effect. We consistently found a positive indirect effect of awe on meaning in life via authentic-self pursuit (Studies 1–3 and Study 5), which arised beyond happiness and self-smallness (Studies 2a, 2b, and 3) and also held for awe brought on by a threatening experience (Study 3). Moreover, we found that manipulating authentic-self pursuit improved meaning in life (Study 4). Importantly, the main effect of awe on meaning in life and indirect effect of awe on meaning in life through authentic-self pursuit were significant for those with low to average rather than high trait authenticity (Study 5). These findings facilitate the understanding of awe as a meaning-making emotion.
... Existing research substantiates this assertion. First, as a meaning-making emotion, awe promotes a sense of meaning and purpose in life, thereby bolstering optimism (Dai et al., 2022;Ihm et al., 2019;. Research has shown that awe promotes meaning in life by motivating the pursuit of authentic self and purpose-seeking (Dai et al., 2022;. ...
... First, as a meaning-making emotion, awe promotes a sense of meaning and purpose in life, thereby bolstering optimism (Dai et al., 2022;Ihm et al., 2019;. Research has shown that awe promotes meaning in life by motivating the pursuit of authentic self and purpose-seeking (Dai et al., 2022;. A purpose-driven life has been associated with heightened optimism (Krok & Telka, 2019;Lee et al., 2006;Yu & Chang, 2019). ...
... Study 1 involved 226 full-time employees (average age = 28.22) who work daily with AI to participate in a between-subjects online experiment via Credamo, a Chinese online platform (Dai et al., 2022;Liang et al., 2022;Zhou et al., 2022). The experiment involved a 2 (high HAI-C job insecurity vs. low HAI-C job insecurity) × 2 (workplace mindfulness training vs. control) between-subject design. ...
... Study 2 aims to examine the total hypothesized model and compensate for the lack of external validity of study 1. We recruited participants through an online platform, Credamo, widely used in China for conducting surveys (Dai et al., 2022;Liang et al., 2022;Zhou et al., 2022). Each participant (employee) who filled out all surveys received CNY16 (approximately US$2.32). ...
Article
Full-text available
Currently, employees are not being replaced by artificial intelligence (AI), but they are facing increasing pressure to adapt and master new AI-related skills—regardless of their attitude to AI collaboration. Drawing on the job demands–resources model (JD-R) and Probst’s (2002) framework, we explain how and when job insecurity related to human–AI collaboration (HAI-C) influences employees’ tech-learning anxiety and subsequent work–life-related outcomes. Additionally, we examine whether workplace mindfulness can mitigate the negative effect of HAI-C job insecurity. We conducted an online experiment (study 1: N = 226) and a three-wave lagged survey (study 2: N = 350) with Chinese employees who daily work with AI. Our results show that HAI-C job insecurity positively relates to HAI-C tech-learning anxiety and subsequently affects employees’ creative performance, informal field-based learning, well-being, and psychological health. Workplace mindfulness played a crucial role in mitigating the negative effect of HAI-C job insecurity on tech-learning anxiety. Specifically, for employees with higher workplace mindfulness, the indirect effects of HAI-C job insecurity on work–life outcomes through tech-learning anxiety were weaker. Our findings have significant implications for both theory and practice concerning HAI-C systems.
... Next, awe was induced using a video-based manipulation as in Study 2. However, to broaden the scope of our investigation, we used a different awe-inducing video that featured cosmic and celestial scenes. In the control condition, participants watched a video of the same length (180 seconds) that demonstrated how to make a table (Dai et al., 2022). Subsequently, participants rated their feelings of awe (M = 5.15, SD = 1.86) and happiness (M = 4.06, SD = 1.54) on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). ...
Article
Full-text available
Awe, a self-transcendent emotion, has been theoretically posited as a precursor to wise reasoning. However, direct empirical evidence supporting this relationship and the underlying mechanism has been limited. In four studies (N = 3700), we examined the relationship between awe and wise reasoning, as well as the mediating effect of self-transcendence, employing cross-sectional, longitudinal , and experimental designs. We consistently found that awe had a lagged effect on (Study 1), enhanced (Studies 2 & 3), and was associated with (Study 4) wise reasoning. Furthermore, self-transcendence mediated this relationship (Studies 3 & 4). The impact of awe on wise reasoning and mediating effect of self-transcendence could not solely be attributed to awe's predominantly positive nature, and the mediation model was established beyond the influence of self-smallness (Studies 3-4). These findings contribute to understanding the emotional trigger of wise reasoning, the cognitive implications of awe, and its role in promoting wise conflict resolution.
... Previous research on awe and wellbeing has followed the traditional binary perspective of well-being, emphasizing the enhancement of positivity and the reduction of negativity. Specifically, awe has been shown to promote a number of positive outcomes, such as improving positive mood (e.g., Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015;Sturm et al., 2022), enhancing the sense of meaning in life (Dai et al., 2022;, motivating authentic self-pursuit (Jiang & Sedikides, 2022), and encouraging humility (Stellar et al., 2018). Additionally, awe can also act as a buffer against various negative outcomes by reducing stress related to daily hassles (Bai et al., 2021), mitigating social pain resulting from ostracism (Yuan, Guo, et al., 2024), and alleviating negative affect stemming from intrapersonal distress, such as the loss of personal belongings (Koh et al., 2019) and the experience of waiting (Rankin et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Awe has been shown to promote well-being through various mechanisms (see Monroy & Keltner, 2023). In this research, we propose a novel perspective for the well-being benefits of awe: Awe fosters equanimity—a balanced state of mind toward all experiences of any valence—and we document how this works, namely, through temporal distancing. Across seven studies, using a combination of experiments, big data analytics, and intervention methods, we provide support for our hypotheses. In Studies 1–3, induced awe increased equanimity, indexed by a self-report scale (Study 1), a decrease in emotional reactivity (Study 2), and an unbiased behavioral approach to positive and negative experiences (Study 3). In Studies 4–6, awe increased equanimity via temporal distancing. This effect persisted beyond self-diminishment (i.e., feeling small and insignificant) and proved to be cross-culturally robust (Study 4). An analysis of almost 200,000 posts on social media (Study 5) revealed that the proposed mediation model manifested in ecologically rich contexts. Study 6 provided causal evidence for the mediation model. Finally, in a 5-day awe intervention (Study 7), awe increased psychological and physical well-being, with equanimity accounting for these benefits. Taken together, these findings reveal that awe cultivates a balanced state of mind by shifting one’s temporal perspective on life events.
... Similarly, awe might promote people to re-evaluate their existing frameworks of meaning and assimilate new viewpoints 99 , and can attenuate the emphasis on the mundane self (everyday aspects of one's identity focused on routine tasks and concerns), thereby accentuating the core constituents of identity, often referred to as the 'true self' (deeper, core aspects of self aligned with one's values and authentic expression) 99 . However, the relationship between awe and global meaning in life (a sense of meaning in life) is not straightforward 26,47,100,101 . Empirical accounts posit that awe increases meaning in life through the elicitation of happiness, but also decreases it by making people feel small and insignificant, rendering the relationship intricate 47 . ...
... MMILS is, therefore, a four-dimensional tool for assessing MIL. Although MMILS had been used in different cultures and age groups (Dai et al., 2022;Kim et al., 2022), its structural stability has not been evaluated directly. ...
Article
Meaning in life (MIL) plays a critical role in promoting physical and mental health. Given the multidimensional nature of MIL, further research is needed to distinguish between different MIL components. The present study focused on three similar but distinct multidimensional measures (the Multidimensional Existential Meaning Scale, MEMS; the Multidimensional MIL Scale, MMILS; the Quadripartite Existential Meaning Scale, QEMS), aiming to validate the Chinese versions of these measures and to compare their predictive effects on subjective well-being, indicated by positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction. Data were collected from two Chinese samples. Sample 1 (N = 393) was used for factor analysis, while Sample 2 (N = 447) was used for the evaluation of measurement invariance, reliability, and predictive relationships of different MIL measures on subjective well-being. Results showed that all three MIL measurements (MEMS, QEMS, and modified MMILS) demonstrated good reliability and validity and positively predicted subjective well-being. Moreover, QEMS showed significant incremental validity in predicting both positive affect and negative affect when controlling for the effects of the other MIL measures. These findings suggest that compared with MEMS and MMILS, QEMS may be a more suitable multidimensional MIL measure in the Chinese context. Further research is needed to examine these findings in other cultural contexts.
... Recent research supports this association. Dai et al. (2022) found that awe increased POM overall, mainly by motivating purpose pursuit. Rivera et al. (2020) found that awe diminished an individual's sense of significance, thereby motivating them to pursue more meaning (i.e., SFM) to satisfy the need for meaning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although individuals with higher trait awe (the tendency to experience awe) are known to be happier and more prosocial, there is limited understanding of the mechanisms underlying these complex relationships. This study uses network analysis to explore dimension‐level relationships between trait awe, meaning in life, subjective well‐being (SWB), and prosocial tendency in a joint network and to explore the bridging role of meaning in life in the network. A total of 538 adults (53.2% females; Mage = 19.86 ± 1.51) completed the survey. The network revealed unique and intricate connections between the dimensions of trait awe, meaning in life (i.e., the presence of and the search for meaning, abbreviated as POM and SFM), subjective happiness and life satisfaction (SWB), and prosocial tendency (i.e., willingness to donate money and volunteer time). Trait awe exhibited direct links to subjective happiness, life satisfaction, and prosocial tendency to donate money. Moreover, through POM and SFM, trait awe also exhibited indirect links to each dimension of SWB and prosocial tendency. Within the global network, POM was further identified as acting as a bridge node with the highest bridge strength and closeness, indicating that POM could efficiently transmit influences within the entire network. These findings highlight the distinct contributions of meaning in life to understanding the relationships between trait awe, SWB, and prosocial tendency, and provide valuable insights for improving SWB and fostering prosocial tendencies.
Article
Full-text available
Personal Projects Analysis (B. R. Little, 1983) was adapted to examine relations between participants’ appraisals of their goal characteristics and orthogonal happiness and meaning factors that emerged from factor analyses of diverse well-being measures. In two studies with 146 and 179 university students, goal efficacy was associated with happiness and goal integrity was associated with meaning. A new technique for classifying participants according to emergent identity themes is introduced. In both studies, identity-compensatory predictors of happiness were apparent. Agentic participants were happiest if their goals were supported by others, communal participants were happiest if their goals were fun, and hedonistic participants were happiest if their goals were being accomplished. The distinction between happiness and meaning is emphasized, and the tension between efficacy and integrity is discussed. Developmental implications are discussed with reference to results from archival data from a sample of senior managers.
Article
Full-text available
We propose that the emotion of awe (i.e., challenge that exceeds the scope of one's mental structures, requiring cognitive accommodation) awakens self-transcendence (i.e., reaching beyond one's self-boundary), which in turn invigorates pursuit of the authentic self (i.e., alignment with one's true self). This process has implications for prosociality. We supported our theoretical model in 14 studies (N = 4,438) using distinct awe manipulations or measures, employing different assessments of authentic-self pursuit, testing participants both in laboratory and field settings, and involving samples from both collectivistic and individualistic cultures. In Studies 1-2 (N = 828), dispositional awe was positively associated with authentic-self pursuit and induced awe motivated authentic-self pursuit. In Studies 2-9 (N = 2,461), dispositional awe was positively associated with, and induced awe strengthened, authentic-self pursuit via self-transcendence. These effects were independent of pride and happiness. In Study 10 (N = 281), self-smallness (i.e., a sense of self as small and insignificant), albeit induced by awe, did not account for the unique effects of awe on authentic-self pursuit via self-transcendence. Finally, in Studies 11-14 (N = 868), awe-induced authentic-self pursuit was linked with higher general prosociality, but lower inauthentic prosociality. The findings invite a reexamination of awe's relation with the self, while highlighting the complexity and intricacy of that relation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Awe alters people’s worldviews and encourages individuals to explore novel experiences. Guided by awe as an emotion, across three studies (N = 896), we tested the effect of awe on natural risk-taking preferences (NRTPs) and the mediating role of the need for closure (NFC). In Study 1, trait awe predicted NRTPs beyond other positive emotions (e.g., amusement). In follow-up studies elicited by videos, positive awe (relative to diverse control states) decreased NFC and natural risk perceptions (NRPs) and increased NRTPs (Studies 2 and 3). Conversely, negative awe reduced preferences for natural risk-taking but did not significantly increase the NFC or NRPs (Study 3). Moreover, NFC mediated the effect of positive awe on NRTPs in Study 2; however, in Study 3, the mediation of ∆NFC (variation of NFC) was not supported for positive or negative awe. The findings supported the key role of positive awe in promoting NRTPs and in reducing NFC, distinguished it from general positive emotion (e.g., amusement and joy) and its negatively valenced variant: negative awe.
Article
Full-text available
It is widely assumed that experiences of awe transform the meaning of daily stresses. Across six studies we tested whether and how the experience of awe is associated with reduced daily stress levels in the moment and, in so doing, leads to elevated life satisfaction. We first documented that individuals who tend to experience greater awe on a daily basis (Study 1) or who report higher levels of trait-like awe (Study 2) report lower levels of daily stress, even after controlling for other positive emotions. In follow-up experiments, after primed with awe (compared with amusement, joy, and pride), individuals reported lower levels of daily stress (Studies 3 and 5) and exhibited lower levels of sympathetic autonomic arousal when talking about their daily stresses (Study 4). Finally, in a naturalistic study, participants who took in an awe-inspiring view at the top of a 200-foot tower reported reduced levels of daily stress and central everyday concerns (Study 6). Mediation analyses revealed that (a) the association between awe and reduced daily stress can be explained by an appraisal of vastness vis-à-vis the self and (b) that the relationship between awe and decreased daily stress levels helps explain awe's positive influence upon life satisfaction. Overall, these findings suggest that experiencing awe can put daily stressors into perspective in the moment and, in so doing, increase well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Awe is a self‐transcendent emotion that exerts a powerful impact on the self. Through diminishing the ego, awe may help cultivate interconnection, wisdom, meaning, and purpose.
Article
Full-text available
Aging into later life is often accompanied by social disconnection, anxiety, and sadness. Negative emotions are self-focused states with detrimental effects on aging and longevity. Awe-a positive emotion elicited when in the presence of vast things not immediately understood-reduces self-focus, promotes social connection, and fosters prosocial actions by encouraging a "small self." We investigated the emotional benefits of a novel "awe walk" intervention in healthy older adults. Sixty participants took weekly 15-min outdoor walks for 8 weeks; participants were randomly assigned to an awe walk group, which oriented them to experience awe during their walks, or to a control walk group. Participants took photographs of themselves during each walk and rated their emotional experience. Each day, they reported on their daily emotional experience outside of the walk context. Participants also completed pre- and postintervention measures of anxiety, depression, and life satisfaction. Compared with participants who took control walks, those who took awe walks experienced greater awe during their walks and exhibited an increasingly "small self" in their photographs over time. They reported greater joy and prosocial positive emotions during their walks and displayed increasing smile intensity over the study. Outside of the walk context, participants who took awe walks reported greater increases in daily prosocial positive emotions and greater decreases in daily distress over time. Postintervention anxiety, depression, and life satisfaction did not change from baseline in either group. These results suggest cultivating awe enhances positive emotions that foster social connection and diminishes negative emotions that hasten decline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Meaning in life has long been a mystery of human existence. In this review, we seek to demystify this construct. Focusing on the subjective experience of meaning in life, we review how it has been measured and briefly describe its correlates. Then we review evidence that meaning in life, for all its mystery, is a rather commonplace experience. We then define the construct and review its constituent facets: comprehension/coherence, purpose, and existential mattering/significance. We review the many experiences that have been shown to enhance meaning in life and close by considering important remaining research questions about this fascinating topic. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology, Volume 72 is January 4, 2021. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Article
Cronbach’s alpha (α) is a widely-used measure of reliability used to quantify the amount of random measurement error that exists in a sum score or average generated by a multi-item measurement scale. Yet methodologists have warned that α is not an optimal measure of reliability relative to its more general form, McDonald’s omega ( ω). Among other reasons, that the computation of ω is not available as an option in many popular statistics programs and requires items loadings from a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) have probably hindered more widespread adoption. After a bit of discussion of α versus ω, we illustrate the computation of ω using two structural equation modeling programs (Mplus and AMOS) and the MBESS package for R. We then describe a macro for SPSS and SAS (OMEGA) that calculates ω in two ways without relying on the estimation of loadings or error variances using CFA. We show that it produces estimates of ω that are nearly identical to when using CFA-based estimates of item loadings and error variances. We also discuss the use of the OMEGA macro for certain forms of item analysis and brief form construction based on the removal of items from a longer scale.