ArticlePDF Available

A method to induce stress in human subjects in online research environments

Authors:
  • Cognitive Consultants International (CCI-HQ)

Abstract and Figures

This paper presents a method to induce stress in human subjects during online participation in research studies without the presence of researchers. In this study, participants in the stress-inducing condition ( N = 52, 44%) were asked to answer general knowledge and mathematical questions which people often get wrong, and did so under time pressure as well as receiving feedback. In contrast, participants in the control condition ( N = 66, 56%) did not have time pressure or receive feedback. The stress manipulation was found to be effective, as the reported state anxiety and visual analog scale on stress scores were higher for the stress group than for the non-stress group (both findings, p < 0.001). Consistent findings were found when accounting for trait anxiety as a moderator, with the exception of the state anxiety levels in high trait anxiety group. This stressing method combines the established stress conditions of uncontrollability (such as time pressures) and social evaluative threats (such as negative feedback). In addition, the method contains specific measures (such as a commitment statement and attention check questions) to enhance the internal validity by preventing and detecting cheating or random responses. This method can be deployed through any commonly available online software. It offers a simple and cost-effective way to collect data online – which fits the increasing need to carry out research in virtual and online environments.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Behavior Research Methods
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-022-01915-3
A method toinduce stress inhuman subjects inonline research
environments
MohammedA.Almazrouei1,2,3 · RuthM.Morgan1,2 · ItielE.Dror1,2
Accepted: 20 June 2022
© The Author(s) 2022
Abstract
This paper presents a method to induce stress in human subjects during online participation in research studies without the
presence of researchers. In this study, participants in the stress-inducing condition (N = 52, 44%) were asked to answer gen-
eral knowledge and mathematical questions which people often get wrong, and did so under time pressure as well as receiving
feedback. In contrast, participants in the control condition (N = 66, 56%) did not have time pressure or receive feedback.
The stress manipulation was found to be effective, as the reported state anxiety and visual analog scale on stress scores were
higher for the stress group than for the non-stress group (both findings, p < 0.001). Consistent findings were found when
accounting for trait anxiety as a moderator, with the exception of the state anxiety levels in high trait anxiety group. This
stressing method combines the established stress conditions of uncontrollability (such as time pressures) and social evalua-
tive threats (such as negative feedback). In addition, the method contains specific measures (such as a commitment statement
and attention check questions) to enhance the internal validity by preventing and detecting cheating or random responses.
This method can be deployed through any commonly available online software. It offers a simple and cost-effective way to
collect data online – which fits the increasing need to carry out research in virtual and online environments.
Keywords Online study· Stress· Human subjects· COVID-19· Crowdsourcing
Generating stress in human subjects for research can be a
challenging task (Ferreira, 2019). This is because, on the one
hand, the experimental design needs to effectively generate
stress but, on the other hand, avoid long-term effects on the
participants (Ferreira, 2019). Adding to this challenge is the
variability in how individuals perceive and react to the same
stress factor (Epel etal., 2018; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
It has been observed that using only participants that
can attend and participate in a study in person can have an
impact on the diversity of the participant sample (Upad-
hyay & Lipkovich, 2020). Added to this, the value of being
able to carry out online experiments has been highlighted
particularly during the coronavirus pandemic (Wigginton
etal., 2020) when much of the face-to-face research involv-
ing human subjects was paused worldwide. There has
therefore been growing recognition of the value of creating
opportunities for studies to be delivered online rather than
face-to-face, including stress-inducing studies (Kirschbaum,
2021).
A meta-analysis of 208 laboratory-based stress studies
found that the combination of social–evaluative threats
(when one is judged negatively by others, such as receiving
negative feedback) and uncontrollability (when nothing can
be done to avoid negative consequences or change a situa-
tion, such as having a time limit for completing a task) were
the stress factors that produce the greatest stress response
in human subjects (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). There-
fore, methods that combine social–evaluative threats and
uncontrollability elements, such as the Trier Social Stress
Test (TSST; Kirschbaum etal., 1993), considered the “gold
standard” for inducing experimental stress in human subjects
(Allen etal., 2017; Le etal., 2020), have potential for effec-
tively inducing stress in an online setting.
* Mohammed A. Almazrouei
mohammed.almazrouei@ucl.ac.uk
1 UCL Department ofSecurity andCrime Science, University
College London, 35 Tavistock Square, LondonWC1H9EZ,
UK
2 UCL Centre fortheForensic Sciences, University College
London, 35 Tavistock Square, LondonWC1H9EZ, UK
3 Forensic Evidence Department, Abu Dhabi Police General
Headquarters, AbuDhabi253, UAE
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Several studies have been conducted to try and validate
online versions of TSST, delivered through virtual reality
tools (e.g., Zimmer etal., 2019), and more recently delivered
by video conferencing online (Eagle etal., 2021; Gunnar
etal., 2021; Harvie etal., 2021). However, some of these
Internet-delivered studies did not include a control group
(Eagle etal., 2021; Gunnar etal., 2021), which limits the
opportunity to understand and interpret the outcomes of
the stress manipulation, for example, by not accounting for
potential additional psychological stress as a result of video
conferencing (Riedl, 2021). One study included a control
group (Harvie etal., 2021), but required the (virtual) pres-
ence of at least three experimenters (i.e., the researcher and
two panelists) in each video conferencing session, which
limits online stress studies to live tasks in which the presence
of the researchers is required nevertheless (virtually rather
than in-person).
Therefore, in this study, alternative stressors were consid-
ered that combine social–evaluative threats and uncontrol-
lability yet were still feasibly operationalized in an Internet-
delivered environment without the need of the researchers to
be present. One such stressor is the Trier Mental Challenge
Test Stress Protocol originally developed by Kirschbaum
etal. (1991)—referred to here as the ‘Mental Challenge
Test’. In the Mental Challenge Test, participants are asked
through programmed software to answer a number of arith-
metic questions without a calculator under a time limit and
receive feedback, such as “wrong” for incorrect answers
(Kirschbaum etal., 1991). The studies that utilized the
Mental Challenge Test were computer-assisted, yet, to date
they have been conducted in the presence of the research-
ers (Allendorfer etal., 2014, 2019; Dedovic etal., 2005;
Kirschbaum etal., 1991).
This study presents a method that has been developed for
inducing stress in an online setting, without the presence of
researchers (either in-person or virtually). This method may
enable advancements in stress research, by accessing large
number of international participants rapidly and in a cost-
effective manner. In this method, participants were asked to
answer a number of general knowledge and mathematical
questions selected specifically for this study under stress
conditions of social evaluative threats (such as displaying
negative feedback) and uncontrollability (such as imposing
time limits).
Method
Participants
Data were collected from 120 participants through the Pro-
lific platform in a single session. Two participants in the
stress group withdrew their data and were excluded from
analysis. The final sample consisted of 118 participants, of
whom N = 66, 56% were in the control group and N = 52,
44% in the stress group (see Table1). Thirteen participants
dropped out (n = 11 from the stress group and n = 2 from
the control group). A drop-out is counted when a participant
starts answering the mathematical and general knowledge
questions then drops out by exiting the study.
Stress procedure
Participants signed the consent form and were then given
instructions about the exercise (see Fig.1). The consent
form and instructions were carefully written to offer fully
informed consent, but without revealing the specific aim of
the study (i.e., inducing stress to participants). Then, partici-
pants were randomly allocated into either the stress or the
control group through Qualtrics. The stress group was shown
a warning message that performance was being monitored.
They were then asked to answer a block of eight random
mathematical/general knowledge questions with time limits
and with feedback given (i.e., Stress Block A; see Appendi-
ces A, B and C for further details on the feedback messages
and mathematical/general knowledge questions). If a partici-
pant answered a question incorrectly, a “
WRONG
” message
in red would appear immediately on the screen. Conversely,
a neutral “OK” message appeared in grey if a question was
answered correctly. If the time allocated to the question ran
out, a “
TIME OUT!
” message appeared in red.
At the end of the mathematical/general knowledge ques-
tion block, either a neutral message or a negative message
was given to participants, depending on their performance
Table 1 Demographical information of participants
*The two participants reported PGCE (postgraduate certificate in
education) as their highest completed education. Their data were
coded within the ‘graduate degree’ holders, since PGCE is an
advanced education after the bachelor’s degree
Mean (SD) Range
Age 33.3 (7.0) 25–59
nValid%
Sex
Male 58 49.2
Female 60 50.8
Highest degree completed
High school diploma/ A-levels or equivalent 18 15.3
Technical/ community college 9 7.6
Undergraduate degree (BA/BSc/Other) 46 39.0
Graduate degree (MA/MSc/MPhil/Other) 37 31.4
Doctorate degree (PhD/Other) 6 5.1
Other* 2 1.7
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
(compared to a preset criterion score of three correct
answers). If the participant scored three correct answers or
lower in this block, then a negative message would appear
explicitly comparing the individual score with those of other
participants. This had the potential to further increase the
social evaluative threat component of stress (Dickerson &
Kemeny, 2004; Kirschbaum etal., 1991). If the participant
scored four or more questions correctly in this block, a neu-
tral message would appear that had no reference to indi-
vidual or group performance. This approach was repeated
in two more blocks (i.e., Stress Blocks B and C). The con-
trol group was asked to complete a comparable number and
genre of questions but without feedback or a time limit.
Questions were randomized through Qualtrics. To prevent
and detect cheating or random responses, a range of quality
assurance measures were included, such as adding a com-
mitment statement, including a tool to detect potential bot
responses and attention check questions (see Appendix A).
After three blocks of mathematical/ general knowledge
questions, the participants were asked to complete the state
anxiety scale (Spielberger etal., 1983) and a visual ana-
logue scale on stress, referred to as ‘VAS-stress’ scale from
here onwards. Next, participants were asked to provide their
demographic information of age, sex, and their highest level
of education. Participants were then asked to complete the
trait anxiety scale (Spielberger etal., 1983). At the end of the
experiment, participants were debriefed that this study spe-
cifically aimed to induce momentary stress. In the debrief,
participants were given the opportunity to withdraw their
data without giving a reason and without it affecting the
rights and benefits (such as payment) to which they were
entitled, or it having any negative repercussions for them.
Stress manipulation check
The effectiveness of the stress manipulation was assessed
and validated using two self-reported measures. First, to cap-
ture the situational anxiety levels of participants (i.e., the
anxiety feelings in the present moment; see Appendix D),
the state scale of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
was used (Spielberger etal., 1983). This state anxiety scale
is a validated and commonly used measure for various stress
manipulations (Arora etal., 2010; LeBlanc etal., 2005;
Spielberger etal., 1983; Tanida etal., 2007). The scale con-
sists of 20 statements (e.g., I feel nervous) for which users
indicate their degree of agreement on a 4-point scale, in
regard to how they feel ‘right now’ (score range is from 20 to
80; Spielberger etal., 1983). Second, following the approach
of Le etal. (2020), participants were asked to report their
stress levels on a VAS-stress, retrospectively: “Looking
back, how stressed did you feel throughout answering the
mathematical and general knowledge questions?” The par-
ticipants rated their feelings from 0% (not stressed at all) to
100% (extremely stressed).
Trait anxiety
Participants were also asked to complete the STAI trait anxi-
ety scale (Spielberger etal., 1983; see Appendix E) to ensure
that the background anxiety levels of participants do not
confound the reported state anxiety or VAS-stress levels.
The trait scale consists of 20 statements that measure how
people ‘generally’ feel (score range from 20 to 80). The
STAI manual recommends placing the trait anxiety scale,
after the state anxiety scale if both scales are administered
together, because the former measures a more stable anxiety
construct that should not be affected with situational stress
(Spielberger etal., 1983). Accordingly, the trait anxiety scale
was placed at the end of the experiment.
Results
Overall stress andtrait anxiety
The mean stress levels, as measured by the state anxiety
scale, was significantly higher for the stress group (M =
48.89, SD = 13.01) than for the control group (M = 34.35,
SD = 10.66), M = – 14.54, 95% CI [– 18.85, – 10.22], t(116)
= – 6.67, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = – 1.24. In addition, partici-
pants in the stress group (M = 73.17, SD = 24.01) reported
higher VAS-stress ratings than the control group (M = 30.55,
SD = 22.90). This was also a statistically significant dif-
ference, M = – 42.63, 95% CI [– 51.22, – 34.04], t(116) =
– 9.83, p < 0.001, d = – 1.82. On average, the stress (M =
45.79, SD = 11.30) and non-stress groups (M = 41.58, SD =
Fig. 1 Graphic timeline of the experimental procedure
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
12.37) were comparable in terms of their background stress
(i.e., trait anxiety levels), M = – 4.21 , 95% CI [– 8.59, 0.16],
t(116) = – 1.91, p = 0.059, d = – 0.35.
Trait anxiety asastress moderator
Two linear regression models were run to investigate
whether the trait anxiety or the demographical variables (i.e.,
age, sex, and education) moderated the reported state anxiety
or VAS-stress scores. In both models, the trait anxiety was
the only factor (p < 0.001) that moderated the dependent
variables. In addition, trait anxiety was significantly corre-
lated with both state anxiety (r(118) = .55, p < 0.001) and
VAS-stress scale (r(118) = .33, p < 0.001).
Hence, it was necessary to account for trait anxiety, as a
background stress, to further understand the effectiveness
of the online stressor presented here. To do so, participants
were divided into three homogenous groups in terms of
reported trait anxiety levels: low, moderate, and high anxiety
(this approach is similar to Horikawa and Yagi (2012)). The
high anxiety group (N = 35; n = 15 in the control condition
and n = 20 in the stress condition) were those whose trait
scores were 0.5 SD above the mean trait score of 43.43 (SD
= 12.04). Conversely, the low anxiety group (N = 40; n = 27
in the control condition and n = 13 in the stress condition)
were those whose trait scores were 0.5 SD below the mean
trait score. The rest of participants (N = 43; n = 24 in the
control condition and n = 19 in the stress condition) were
classified to have moderate trait anxiety levels.
The state anxiety levels varied significantly between the
stress and control conditions, in the low anxiety group (M
= – 16.00, 95% CI [– 25.77, – 6.23], Welch’s t(13.57) =
– 3.52, p = 0.004, d = – 1.19) and moderate anxiety group
(M = – 12.82, 95% CI [– 17.75, – 7.90], t(41) = – 5.26, p
< .001, d = – 1.61), but not in the high anxiety group (M
= – 7.20, 95% CI [– 15.43, 1.03], t(33) = – 1.78, p = 0.084,
d = – 0.61; Fig.2). However, when comparing the VAS-
stress scores, there were statistical significant differences
in all the three anxiety groups (low anxiety: M = – 35.24,
95% CI [– 54.00, – 16.49], t(38) = – 3.80, p = 0.001, d =
– 1.28; moderate anxiety: M = – 44.21, 95% CI [– 56.30,
– 32.13], t(41) = – 7.39, p < 0.001, d = – 2.27; high anxiety:
M = – 39.87, 95% CI [– 54.77, – 24.96], Welch’s t(21.48)
= – 5.55, p < 0.001, d = – 1.90). Note that Welch’s t test
is used when the assumption of homogeneity of variances
has been violated, as assessed by Levene’s test for equality
of variances.
Performance onstress blocks
The majority (67.3–88.5%) of participants in the stress group
scored three correct responses or less in stress blocks A, B,
and C. This means that those participants received negative
feedback after completing those blocks of questions. One
participant was able to score 7 of 8 questions correctly in
Block C, and no one scored 8 of 8 questions correctly (see
Table2).
Discussion
The stress manipulation was found to be effective in the
sample who participated in this study. The state anxiety and
VAS-stress scores were significantly higher for the stress
group than the control group, with and without account-
ing for trait anxiety as a moderator. The exception was the
state anxiety levels in the high trait anxiety group. Here, the
state anxiety levels in the stress condition were still higher
than the non-stress condition, although the difference was
not statistically significant. One possible explanation is that
the online stress method was not effective enough to induce
momentary stress to already highly anxious participants—a
clear sign of a ceiling effect.
Directly comparing our findings with published studies
on stress-inducing methods can be limited (Narvaez Lin-
ares etal., 2020), especially that the online stressors are by
their very nature less powerful than classical in-person stress
tasks. Variations of TSST in previous research were able
to cause elevations in state anxiety and VAS-stress levels
comparable to the current stressor, but with smaller sample
sizes. For instance, Guez etal. (2016) and Le etal. (2020)
reported large effect sizes of their stressors on state anxi-
ety (η2
p = 0.23, N = 46) and VAS on stress (d = 1.74, N =
76), respectively. This difference in magnitude is likely to be
due to a number of factors that may include the absence of
researchers during the stress-inducing period. Notably, how-
ever, our findings appear to be more in line with the impact
of established stressors that had minimal interactions of
investigators during the stress manipulation (Dedovic etal.,
2005; see Discussion in p. 325).
The stress stimuli selected for this study appear to be
challenging since most participants scored 3 or less ques-
tions correctly. Thus, the selected stress stimuli made it pos-
sible to give negative and potentially stressful feedback to
participants in all three stress blocks. It may also be inferred
from the data that engagement of some participants in
answering the questions in the stress blocks may have been
sustained (e.g., some participants were able to score four,
five, six, or even seven questions correctly in a block, all
of which were above the preset criterion score of three (see
Table2)). However, we cannot rule out the possibility that
this procedure might lead to reduced engagement in some
participants. Future studies should incorporate a considera-
tion of whether low engagement/motivation might influence
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Fig. 2 Mean state anxiety (top) and VAS-stress scores (bottom) for low, moderate, and high trait anxiety participant groups. Error bars reflect
95% confidence intervals
Table 2 Frequency and cumulative percentages of correct responses in stress Blocks A, B, and C
Correct response Stress Block A Stress Block B Stress Block C
N N %N%N%
0 10 19.2 7 13.5 5 9.6
1 21 59.6 19 50.0 5 19.2
2 10 78.8 10 69.2 13 44.2
3 4 86.5 10 88.5 12 67.3
4 3 92.3 5 98.1 7 80.8
5 2 96.2 1 100 6 92.3
6 2 100 0 100 3 98.1
7 0 100 0 100 1 100
8 0 100 0 100 0 100
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
scores if, for example, a cognitive task was used after the
stress induction.
The higher drop-out rate in the stress condition com-
pared with the control condition could be due to a num-
ber of factors, namely the stress manipulation effectively
causing stress and thus reduced motivation to complete the
difficult tasks. The drop-out rate in this study appears to be
higher than other validated stress methods. For instance, in a
recent TSST method that was delivered by Zoom, one of 72
participants discontinued the study during the stress period
(although it is worth noting that a total of 31 participants
dropped out by the end of the experiment for other reasons,
such as not showing up in scheduled sessions; Eagle etal.,
2021).
Participants recruited through crowdsourcing platforms,
as in the current study, appear to have a higher dropout rate
than in-person/offline studies (Stewart etal., 2017; Zhou &
Fishbach, 2016). This may be due a range of factors includ-
ing participants having the ability to preview the study
(Stewart etal., 2017), and potentially returning the study
before completing the tasks and without affecting their repu-
tation score on the crowdsourcing platforms (Palan & Schit-
ter, 2018). Furthermore, there may be fewer barriers to drop-
ping out of an online study due to the anonymity afforded
by the online setting in comparison to dropping out of a live
study (in person, or online but with a video connection with
the researchers). In addition, researchers may not be aware
of participants who have dropped out as they do not count
towards the quota allocated in a crowdsourcing platform,
and thus researchers under-report them in published papers
(Zhou & Fishbach, 2016).
Importantly, drop-outs can be condition-dependent, for
reasons such as experiencing more mental fatigue in one
condition compared to the other (Zhou & Fishbach, 2016).
Though selective attrition can potentially influence inter-
nal validity, we do not feel that this caused a meaningful
impact on our findings, because the remaining randomized
sample sizes in each condition for the method validation
were reasonably comparable (i.e., 56% in comparison to
44%). Nevertheless, it may be beneficial for studies that use
crowdsourcing platforms to include proactive countermeas-
ure strategies (e.g., telling participants upfront that dropping
out could affect the quality of data; Reips, 2000; Zhou &
Fishbach, 2016).
A number of limitations do exist in regard to using this
online stress method that should be addressed in future
studies. First, the findings from this study are based on the
assessment of stress from self-report measures (Nisbett &
Wilson, 1977). Future research can include additional physi-
ological measures, such as the approach taken by Harvie
etal. (2021) who had participants measure their own heart
rate.
Another limitation is that we did not balance the baseline
stress (e.g., via VAS) for both groups. We were concerned
that placing a VAS before the stress manipulation (so we
could balance it across conditions) could impact feelings
and expectations of the participants, and hence impact their
performance (e.g., Christensen-Szalanski & Willham, 1991).
Furthermore, as with any remote online study, there is
no control over what participants do during the exercise.
Despite the effort made by the researchers to control experi-
mental stimuli and set explicit instructions for the exercise,
participants are not monitored and may be carrying out other
activities while taking part in the study (such as doing the
exercise while relaxing on the sofa compared to a desk).
Such variations in behavior in completing the exercise may
have the potential to influence the stress levels of partici-
pants, as opposed to being solely induced by the stress stim-
uli themselves.
Nevertheless, this is the first method that has been
designed and used to induce stress in human participants
effectively online without the presence of the researchers.
It offers a cost-effective and easy-to-use method to induce
momentary stress to human subjects in a controlled man-
ner in an online setting. In addition, by not requiring the
researchers to be agents of stress, the online method also
enables quick access to large participant samples globally
through crowdsourcing platforms (Peer etal., 2017). The
method includes unpredictable social evaluative threats
common in everyday life, including those in professional
domains (e.g., Arora etal. 2010), which means it is a method
that can offer a degree of ecological validity.
Conclusions
This paper presents a new and ecologically valid method to
stress human subjects in an online setting without the pres-
ence of researchers. This method offers a cost-effective way
to collect data from a diverse range of participant cohorts,
which is particularly useful insituations where there is a
need to carry out research in online environments. The
building blocks of this method (such as having specific
measures to enhance data quality collected) could be use-
ful for in a wide range of studies that aim to collect quality
psychological data online.
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplemen-
tary material available at https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 022- 01915-3.
Acknowledgements We acknowledge PhD studentship funding from
Abu Dhabi Police, United Arab Emirates.
Code availability Note applicable
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Data availability The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the
current study are available from the corresponding author on reasona-
ble request. In addition, the programmed study link could be shared for
current and/ or future research on reasonable request. None of the data
or materials for the experiments reported here is available in a publicly
accessible respiratory, and none of the experiments was preregistered.
Declarations
Competing interests The authors declare that there are no known
competing interests or activities that might be seen as influencing this
research.
Ethics approval This study was performed in line with the principles
of the Declaration of Helsinki. This research was approved by the UCL
Research Ethics Committee (Project ID 15395/003).
Consent to participate Informed consent was obtained from all indi-
vidual participants included in the study.
Consent for publication Informed consent was obtained from all indi-
vidual participants included in the study regarding publishing their
data.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
Allen, A. P., Kennedy, P. J., Dockray, S., Cryan, J. F., Dinan, T. G., &
Clarke, G. (2017). The Trier Social Stress Test: Principles and
practice. Neurobiology of Stress, 6, 113–126. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. ynstr. 2016. 11. 001
Allendorfer, J. B., Heyse, H., Mendoza, L., Nelson, E. B., Eliassen,
J. C., Storrs, J. M., & Szaflarski, J. P. (2014). Physiologic and
cortical response to acute psychosocial stress in left temporal lobe
epilepsy—A pilot cross-sectional fMRI study. Epilepsy & Behav-
ior, 36, 115–123. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. yebeh. 2014. 05. 003
Allendorfer, J. B., Nenert, R., Hernando, K. A., DeWolfe, J. L., Pati,
S., Thomas, A. E., Billeaud, N., Martin, R. C., & Szaflarski, J. P.
(2019). FMRI response to acute psychological stress differenti-
ates patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures from healthy
controls – A biochemical and neuroimaging biomarker study.
NeuroImage: Clinical, 24, 101967. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. nicl.
2019. 101967
Arora, S., Sevdalis, N., Nestel, D., Woloshynowych, M., Darzi, A.,
& Kneebone, R. (2010). The impact of stress on surgical perfor-
mance: A systematic review of the literature. Surgery, 147(3),
318–330. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. surg. 2009. 10. 007
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J., & Willham, C. F. (1991). The hind-
sight bias: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 48(1), 147–168. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/
0749- 5978(91) 90010-Q
Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Mahani, N. K., Engert, V., Lupien, S. J.,
& Pruessner, J. C. (2005). The Montreal Imaging Stress Task:
Using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving
and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. Journal
of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 30(5), 319–325.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol
responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory
research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355–391. https:// doi. org/
10. 1037/ 0033- 2909. 130.3. 355
Eagle, D. E., Rash, J. A., Tice, L., & Proeschold-Bell, R. J. (2021).
Evaluation of a remote, Internet-delivered version of the Trier
Social Stress Test. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
165, 137–144. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. ijpsy cho. 2021. 03. 009
Epel, E. S., Crosswell, A. D., Mayer, S. E., Prather, A. A., Slavich,
G. M., Puterman, E., & Mendes, W. B. (2018). More than a feel-
ing: A unified view of stress measurement for population science.
Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 49, 146–169. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. yfrne. 2018. 03. 001
Ferreira, S. O. (2019). Emotional activation in human beings: Pro-
cedures for experimental stress induction. Psicologia USP, 30,
e180176. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1590/ 0103- 6564e 20180 176
Guez, J., Saar-Ashkenazy, R., Keha, E., & Tiferet-Dweck, C. (2016).
The Effect of Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) on Item and Asso-
ciative Recognition of Words and Pictures in Healthy Partici-
pants. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 507. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/
fpsyg. 2016. 00507
Gunnar, M. R., Reid, B. M., Donzella, B., Miller, Z. R., Gardow, S.,
Tsakonas, N. C., Thomas, K. M., DeJoseph, M., & Bendezú,
J. J. (2021). Validation of an online version of the Trier Social
Stress Test in a study of adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinol-
ogy, 125, 105111. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psyne uen. 2020.
105111
Harvie, H. M. K., Jain, B., Nelson, B. W., Knight, E. L., Roos, L. E.,
& Giuliano, R. J. (2021). Induction of acute stress through an
Internet-delivered Trier Social Stress Test as assessed by pho-
toplethysmography on a smartphone. Stress, 24(6), 1023–1032.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 10253 890. 2021. 19957 14
Horikawa, M., & Yagi, A. (2012). The Relationships among Trait Anxi-
ety, State Anxiety and the Goal Performance of Penalty Shoot-Out
by University Soccer Players. PLoS ONE, 7(4), e35727. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00357 27
Kirschbaum, C. (2021). Why we need an online version of the Trier
Social Stress Test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 125, 105129.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psyne uen. 2021. 105129
Kirschbaum, C., Diedrich, O., Gehrke, J., Wüst, S., & Hellhammer, D.
H. (1991). Cortisol and behavior: The “Trier Mental Challenge
Test” (TMCT)—First evaluation of a new psychological stress
test. In A. Ehlers, W. Fiegenbaum, I. Florin, & J. Margraf (Eds.),
Perspectives and promises of clinical psychology (pp. 67–78).
Springer.
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier
Social Stress Test’ – A Tool for investigating psychobiological
stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology,
28(1–2), 76–81. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1159/ 00011 9004
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.
Springer.
Le, J. T., Watson, P., Begg, D., Albertella, L., & Le Pelley, M. E.
(2020). Physiological and subjective validation of a novel stress
procedure: The Simple Singing Stress Procedure. Behavior
Research Methods. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 020- 01505-1
LeBlanc, V. R., MacDonald, R. D., McArthur, B., King, K., & Lepine,
T. (2005). Paramedic performance in calculating drug dos-
ages following stressful scenarios in a human patient simulator.
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Prehospital Emergency Care, 9(4), 439–444. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1080/ 10903 12050 02552 55
Narvaez Linares, N. F., Charron, V., Ouimet, A. J., Labelle, P. R., &
Plamondon, H. (2020). A systematic review of the Trier Social
Stress Test methodology: Issues in promoting study comparison
and replicable research. Neurobiology of Stress, 13, 100235.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. ynstr. 2020. 100235
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know:
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3),
231–259. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0033- 295X. 84.3. 231
Palan, S., & Schitter, C. (2018). Prolific.ac—A subject pool for online
experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance,
17, 22–27. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jbef. 2017. 12. 004
Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Samat, S., & Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond
the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral
research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 153–
163. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jesp. 2017. 01. 006
Reips, U.-D. (2000). The Web experiment method: Advantages, disad-
vantages, and solutions. Psychological Experiments on the Inter-
net, 89–117. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ B978- 01209 9980-4/ 50005-8
Riedl, R. (2021). On the stress potential of videoconferencing: Defini-
tion and root causes of Zoom fatigue. Electronic Markets. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s12525- 021- 00501-3
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P., & Jacobs,
G. (1983). Manual for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory [Data
set]. Consulting Psychologists Press. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/
t06496- 000
Stewart, N., Chandler, J., & Paolacci, G. (2017). Crowdsourcing Sam-
ples in Cognitive Science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(10),
736–748. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2017. 06. 007
Tanida, M., Katsuyama, M., & Sakatani, K. (2007). Relation between
mental stress-induced prefrontal cortex activity and skin condi-
tions: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Research, 1184,
210–216. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. brain res. 2007. 09. 058
Upadhyay, U. D., & Lipkovich, H. (2020). Using online technologies
to improve diversity and inclusion in cognitive interviews with
young people. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 20(1), 159.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/ s12874- 020- 01024-9
Wigginton, N. S., Cunningham, R. M., Katz, R. H., Lidstrom, M. E.,
Moler, K. A., Wirtz, D., & Zuber, M. T. (2020). Moving aca-
demic research forward during COVID-19. Science, 368(6496),
1190–1192. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1126/ scien ce. abc55 99
Zhou, H., & Fishbach, A. (2016). The pitfall of experimenting on the
web: How unattended selective attrition leads to surprising (yet
false) research conclusions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 111(4), 493–504. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ pspa0
000056
Zimmer, P., Buttlar, B., Halbeisen, G., Walther, E., & Domes, G.
(2019). Virtually stressed? A refined virtual reality adaptation
of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) induces robust endocrine
responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 101, 186–192. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. psyne uen. 2018. 11. 010
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
... The math questions for this experiment are derived from Almazrouei et al. (2023). These included 30 GRE level math questions for the difficult math condition and 30 grade school level math questions for the easy math condition (each participant attempted a random selection of 20 easy or 20 difficult problems). ...
... Second, experienced stress was eliminated as a potential reason for the lower JOLs: participants' state anxiety was not higher after completing the difficult math task compared to after completing the easy math task. This math manipulation was designed to elicit stress (Almazrouei et al., 2023), but it obviously did not work in this instance even though this manipulation did affect participants' memory judgments. ...
... In addition to replicating this surprising outcome, Experiment 2 addressed a design confound in Experiment 1. Specifically, as the difficult math task was designed to cause stress compared to the easy math task (cf. Almazrouei et al., 2023), the former condition included feedback on the correctness of each answer but the latter condition did not. As such, the lower JOLs for the difficult math group in Experiment 1 could have stemmed from the difficulty of the math task, from the feedback, or both. ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies indicate that students’ metacognitive monitoring of their learning can be biased by factors related to the study experience, but less is known about how performance on an unrelated task affects judgments of learning (JOLs) on a subsequent learning task. In four experiments, college students completed either a very difficult or very easy math task (or no math task). All participants then completed the same paired-associates learning task, making a JOL as they studied each item. In all four experiments, participants judged their learning of the paired-associates to be better after completing the easy math task than after completing the difficult math task. This occurred even though participants’ performance on the memory task was not affected by which math task they completed, and occurred with and without feedback on the math task. Differences in JOLs led to differences in restudy time. Difficult math lowered state self-esteem, but it did not mediate the effect on JOLs. The results indicate that performance on a prior, unrelated task can bias students’ JOLs on a subsequent learning task. Such effects are not explained by current theories of metacognitive monitoring, but suggest that students who are studying for multiple exams close together in time (e.g., final exams) might inappropriately judge their learning for one topic after experiencing a test in another topic.
... For this purpose, one of the most widely used tests is the Trier Social Stress Test [13]. However, this paradigm requires the presence of more experimenters, which limits the usability of the test [14]. To handle this issue, Almazrouei and colleagues [14] have developed and validated a stress paradigm (i.e., an online quiz involving general knowledge and mathematical questions, administered with and without time pressure and instant feedback on performance) that does not require the presence of an experimenter. ...
... However, this paradigm requires the presence of more experimenters, which limits the usability of the test [14]. To handle this issue, Almazrouei and colleagues [14] have developed and validated a stress paradigm (i.e., an online quiz involving general knowledge and mathematical questions, administered with and without time pressure and instant feedback on performance) that does not require the presence of an experimenter. According to their findings, the stressful version of the quiz caused a substantial increase in participants' state anxiety and perceived stress level compared to the control version. ...
... To induce stress, we adapted the procedure published by Almazrouei and colleagues [14] to the Hungarian language. Both groups were tasked with answering randomly selected mathematic and general knowledge questions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Proprioceptive accuracy is an important aspect of motor functioning thus understanding how the stress response affects it can broaden our knowledge about the effects of stress on motor performance. There has been published only one quasi-experimental study on this topic to date, reporting a negative association between stress and proprioceptive accuracy. The aim of the present study was to explore whether the stress response influences proprioceptive accuracy in a randomized and controlled experimental setting. Method Participants (Mage = 20.4 yrs, SDage = 1.91 yrs) were randomly assigned to a stress (n = 29) and a control (n = 28) group. Psychological stress was induced via an online quiz involving time pressure and instant feedback on performance. Participants’ perceived (state anxiety) and physiological (heart rate, heart rate variability, skin conductance level) stress response and proprioceptive accuracy (the active and passive version of the Joint Position Reproduction test for the elbow joint) were measured before and after the experimental manipulation. Results The quiz substantially increased only participants’ perceived stress however, proprioceptive accuracy was not impacted by the experimental manipulation. Conclusion Perceived stress does not impact proprioceptive accuracy.
... In the low-pressure condition, participants were asked to complete relatively easy questions, with no time limit and no feedback (see Appendix 1 for details). The aim of this stage was to induce stress feelings in participants by using the method detailed in [36]. This method combines two stress elements, which are common in professional workplaces e.g., [37,38]: social evaluative threats (when one is negatively being judged by others) and uncontrollability (when nothing can be done to control a situation). ...
... Therefore, to ensure the completeness and accuracy of our research, independent data collection is imperative. Questions are asked under artificial induction of emotions according to Almazrouei et al. [35]. ...
... While levels of background noise have increased, stressful timed tasks like standardized tests or testing in general have also been on the rise. These standardized or non-standardized tests often have strict time limits, which greatly increases the induced stress and anxiety on students [8]. With the increasing importance of standardized tests in academics, there has been increasing pressure on students to perform well in timed testing [9]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Noise pollution has been rising alongside urbanization. Literature shows that disorganized background noise decreases attention. Timed testing, an attention-demanding stress task, has become increasingly important in assessing students' academic performance. However, there is insufficient research on how background noise affects performance in timed stress tasks by impacting attention, which this study aims to address. The paper-based SAT math test under increased time pressure was administered twice: once in silence and once with conversational and traffic background noise. Attention is negatively attributed to increasing blink rate, measured using eye landmarks from dLib's machine-learning facial-detection model. First, the study affirms that background noise detriments attention and performance. Attention, through blink rate, is established as an indicator of stress task performance. Second, the study finds that participants whose blink rates increased due to background noise differed in performance compared to those whose blink rates decreased, possibly correlating with their self-perception of noise's impact on attention. Third, using a case study, the study finds that a student with ADHD had enhanced performance and attention from background noise. Fourth, the study finds that although both groups began with similar blink rates, the group exposed to noise had significantly increased blink rate near the end, indicating that noise reduces attention over time. While schools can generally provide quiet settings for timed stress tasks, the study recommends personalized treatments for students based on how noise affects them. Future research can use different attention indices to consolidate this study's findings or conduct this study with different background noises.
... Furthermore, the questions in the stress condition were selected to be more difficult and prone to error than those in the control group in order to increase the level of stress (by increasing the probability of participants making mistakes and receiving negative feedback). Hence, this experimental design included both social evaluative threat (such as feedback messages after answering each question) and uncontrollability stress elements (such as time pressure for answering the questions), as outlined in (Almazrouei et al., 2022). ...
Article
Stress has been shown to have an impact on the quality of decisions made by professionals in a variety of domains. However, there is lack of research examining the impact of stress on forensic decision-making contexts, where experts can face various levels of stress. This exploratory study examines fingerprint decisions made under stress, by novices (N = 115) and fingerprint experts (N = 34). Findings suggested a potentially complex relationship between stress and expert performance. On the one hand, in this study stress seemed to improve the performance of both novices and experts on fingerprint assessments, but mainly for same-source evidence. In contrast, the induced stress appeared to have an impact on risk-taking. When the same-source prints were difficult, a trend emerged with stressed experts taking less risk and reported more inconclusive conclusions with higher confidence than the control group. Furthermore, stress had a significant impact on the overall confidence levels and response times of novices, but not experts. These findings suggest that stress and decision-making tasks are important factors that should be considered when considering optimal working environments for increasing decision quality.
Article
Full-text available
Stress is a prevalent global concern impacting individuals across various life aspects. This paper investigates stress detection using electroencephalographic (EEG) signals, which have proven valuable for studying neural correlates of stress. Stress was induced in students, and physiological data was recorded as part of the experimental setup. Different feature sets were extracted and four machine learning models, including LightGBM, Convolutional Neural Network (CNN), K-Nearest Neighbors (KNN), and Support Vector Machine (SVM), were utilized for classification tasks. The findings indicate that the mean and standard deviation of 19 channels consistently outperform other feature sets. LightGBM demonstrates superior performance across all scenarios compared to CNN, KNN, and SVM. Overall, this study presents an effective stress detection approach using EEG signals and demonstrates the potential of integrating simple statistical features for enhanced classification accuracy. The findings contribute to the advancement of stress monitoring technologies, with potential applications in wearables and BCIs for real-time stress management.
Article
Full-text available
Workplace stress can affect forensic experts’ job satisfaction and performance, which holds financial and other implications for forensic service providers. Therefore, it is important to understand and manage workplace stress, but that is not simple or straightforward. This paper explores stress as a human factor that influences forensic expert decision-making. First, we identify and highlight three factors that mitigate decisions under stress conditions: nature of decision, individual differences, and context of decision. Second, we situate workplace stress in forensic science within the Challenge-Hindrance Stressor Framework. We argue that stressors in forensic science workplaces can have a positive or a negative impact, depending on the type, level, and context of stress. Developing an understanding of the stressors, their sources, and their possible impact can help forensic service providers and researchers to implement context-specific interventions to manage stress at work and optimize expert performance.
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies have demonstrated the feasibility of administering the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) through the internet, with major implications for promoting inclusivity in research participation. However, online TSST studies to date are limited by a lack of control groups and the need for biological measures of stress reactivity that can be fully implemented online. Here, we test smartphone-based photoplethysmography as a measure of heart rate reactivity to an online variant of the TSST. Results demonstrate significant acceleration in heart rate and heightened self-reported stress and anxiety in the TSST condition relative to a placebo version of the TSST. The placebo condition led to a significant increase in self-reported stress and anxiety relative to baseline levels, but this increase was smaller in magnitude than that observed in the TSST condition. These findings highlight the potential for smartphone-based photoplethysmography in internet-delivered studies of cardiac reactivity and demonstrate that it is critical to utilize random assignment to a control or stressor condition when administering acute stress online.
Article
Full-text available
As a consequence of lockdowns due to the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) and the resulting restricted social mobility, several billion people worldwide have recently had to replace physical face-to-face communication with computer-mediated interaction. Notably, the adoption rates of videoconferencing increased significantly in 2020, predominantly because videoconferencing resembles face-to-face interaction. Tools such as Zoom, Microsoft Teams, and Cisco Webex are used by hundreds of millions of people today. Videoconferencing may bring benefits (e.g., saving of travel costs, preservation of environment). However, prolonged and inappropriate use of videoconferencing may also have an enormous stress potential. A new phenomenon and term emerged, Zoom fatigue, a synonym for videoconference fatigue. This paper develops a definition for Zoom fatigue and presents a conceptual framework that explores the major root causes of videoconferencing fatigue and stress. The development of the framework draws upon media naturalness theory and its underlying theorizing is based on research published across various scientific fields, including the disciplines of both behavioral science and neuroscience. Based on this theoretical foundation, hypotheses are outlined. Moreover, implications for research and practice are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Laboratory stress-induction procedures have been critical in illuminating the effects of stress on human health, cognition, and functioning. Here, we present a novel stress induction procedure, the Simple Singing Stress Procedure (SSSP), that overcomes some of the practical challenges and conceptual limitations of existing procedures in measuring the causal influence of stress on psychological variables. In the stress condition of the SSSP, participants were instructed to sing a song in front of the experimenter while being video- and audio-recorded. Participants were also informed that they would have to sing again at the end of the experiment, and that this second performance would later be assessed by a panel of experimenters. Participants in a no-stress condition instead read lyrics in each phase. Our findings revealed that participants in the stress condition showed significantly higher blood pressure immediately following the initial singing session, as well as heightened salivary cortisol at a latency consistent with the initial singing session, than those in the no-stress condition. Our stress procedure also generated elevations in self-reported stress ratings immediately after the first singing session and subsequently in anticipation of the second singing session, relative to the no-stress condition. Collectively, these findings suggest that the SSSP is a simple and effective stress induction procedure that may be a promising alternative to existing protocols.
Article
Full-text available
Background: We aimed to assess the feasibility of using multiple technologies to recruit and conduct cognitive interviews among young people across the United States to test items measuring sexual and reproductive empowerment. We sought to understand whether these methods could achieve a diverse sample of participants. With more researchers turning to approaches that maintain social distancing in the context of COVID-19, it has become more pressing to refine these remote research methods. Methods: We used several online sites to recruit for and conduct cognitive testing of survey items. To recruit potential participants we advertised the study on the free online bulletin board, Craigslist, and the free online social network, Reddit. Interested participants completed an online Qualtrics screening form. To maximize diversity, we purposefully selected individuals to invite for participation. We used the video meeting platform, Zoom, to conduct the cognitive interviews. The interviewer opened a document with the items to be tested, shared the screen with the participant, and gave them control of the mouse and keyboard. After the participant self-administered the survey, the interviewer asked about interpretation and comprehension. After completion of the interviews we sent participants a follow-up survey about their impressions of the research methods and technologies used. We describe the processes, the advantages and disadvantages, and offer recommendations for researchers. Results: We recruited and interviewed 30 young people from a range of regions, gender identities, sexual orientations, ages, education, and experiences with sexual activity. These methods allowed us to recruit a purposefully selected diverse sample in terms of race/ethnicity and region. It also may have offered potential participants a feeling of safety and anonymity leading to greater participation from gay, lesbian, and transgender people who would not have agreed to participate in-person. Conducting the interviews using video chat may also have facilitated the inclusion of individuals who would not volunteer for in-person meetings. Disadvantages of video interviewing included participant challenges to finding a private space for the interview and problems with electronic devices. Conclusions: Online technologies can be used to achieve a diverse sample of research participants, contributing to research findings that better respond to young people's unique identities and situations.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated psychological stress response in the brain regions involved in emotion-motor-executive control in psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES). 12 PNES patients and 12 healthy controls (HCs) underwent stress task and resting state functional MRI (fMRI), mood and quality of life (QOL) assessments, and measurements of salivary cortisol, alpha-amylase, and heart rate. Group differences were assessed, and we correlated beta values from a priori selected brain regions showing stress task fMRI group differences with other stress response measures. We also used the regions showing stress task fMRI group differences as seeds for resting state functional connectivity (rs-FC) analysis. Mood and QOL were worse in PNES versus HCs. Physiological and assessment measures were similar except 'Planful Problem Solving' coping that was greater for HCs (p = .043). Perceived stress associated negatively with heart rate change (rs = -0.74, p = .0063). There was stress fMRI hyporeactivity in left/right amygdala and left hippocampus in PNES versus HCs (corrected p < .05). PNES exhibited a positive association between alpha-amylase change and right amygdala activation (rs = 0.71, p = .010). PNES versus HCs exhibited greater right amygdala rs-FC to left precentral and inferior/middle frontal gyri (corrected p < .05). Our findings of fMRI hyporeactivity to psychological stress, along with greater emotion-motor-executive control network rs-FC in PNES when compared to HCs suggest a dysregulation in stress response circuitry in PNES.
Article
The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) is a widely used, reliable, and ecologically valid method for inducing acute stress under controlled conditions. Traditionally, the TSST is administered with staff physically present with participants, which limits the participant populations that can be exposed to the TSST. We describe an adaptation of the TSST to remote, online delivery over video-conferencing, which we call the internet-delivered Trier Social Stress Test (iTSST). This adaption has participants use wearable, self-administered ECG monitors received and returned via mail. Fifty participants were recruited to take part in a pilot study evaluating stress-reduction interventions and completed the iTSST at two occasions separated by approximately 12 weeks. Perceived stress and heart rate variability (HRV) were measured during both administrations of the iTSST. Forty-one participants completed both assessments and were included in the set of analyses. Both administrations were characterized by an increase in self-reported stress and reduction in self-reported relaxation from the resting phase to the speech task, which returned to baseline during recovery. In terms of HRV, we observed a significant parasympathetic response to the iTSST in 90% of participants, evidenced by a decrease in RMSSD and increase in heart rate from resting to the speech task, which recovered during the recovery phase. In terms of repeatability, there was little evidence of habituation and the iTSST elicited a stress response during both the initial administration and the 12-week follow-up. While the utility is limited by the lack of a measure of sympathetic and HPA-axis activity, the iTSST represents a promising research tool when physically interacting with participants is not feasible.
Article
Introduction The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) is the most widely used protocol for activating a stress response of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis and other stress-mediating systems. A number of variants of the TSST exist, including ones for children, groups, and virtual reality. All of these versions, though, require in-person assessment. The COVID-19 pandemic has made in-person assessment impossible or extremely difficult and potentially dangerous. The purpose of this study was to validate a completely remote, online, version of the TSST for children. Method A sample of 68 (27 female) 15- and 16-year old participants were administered the TSST-Online (TSST-OL) during the late afternoon hours (3–6 p.m. start time). The participants, judges (one male, one female), and experimenter (female) all joined the assessment from their own homes via the online platform, ZOOM™. Two sessions were conducted, one to obtain consent, explain procedures, work with the family to arrange the computer and room set-up for the TSST-OL and one within two weeks to conduct the procedure. The participants were trained to take their own saliva samples and a saliva sampling kit was mailed to the home in between the first and second session. The samples were then mailed to the researchers within a day of collection. The participant was observed during saliva collection to determine correct procedures were followed. Salivary cortisol, salivary α-amylase and self-reports of stress were measured multiple times over the second session. Results rmANOVAs yielded a significant effect of trials, for cortisol, F(1.37,90.46) = 15.13, p = .001, sAA, F(2.75,146.68) = 6.91, p = .001, and self-rated stress, F(3.43,222.69) = 118.73, p = .001. There were no significant sex by trials interactions for any measure, although females reported more stress than males, F(1,65) = 9.14, p = .004. For cortisol, from baseline to expected peak (30 min after the onset of speech preparation), the Cohen’s effect size was dz = 0.57. Using 1.5 nmol/l (or 0.54 μg/dl) as the criterion for a response (Miller, Plessow, Kirschaum, & Stalder, 2013), 63% of the participants produced a significant increase in cortisol. Conclusions The responses to the TSST-OL are consistent with in-person responses among children and adolescents (see recent meta-analysis (Seddon et al., 2020). The protocol is a viable way of assessing reactivity of the HPA axis and other stress systems without needing to bring the participant into the research laboratory. This method will be useful during periods of widespread infection. It should also work to study populations who all live too far from the research laboratory to be assessed in person.
Article
A gradual, stepwise approach to reopening, informed by public health expertise, will be essential
Article
Since its development in 1993, the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) has been used widely as a psychosocial stress paradigm to activate the sympathetic nervous system and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPAA) stress systems, stimulating physiological functions (e.g. heart rate) and cortisol secretion. Several methodological variations introduced over the years have led the scientific community to question replication between studies. In this systematic review, we used the Preferred Reporting Items of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) to synthesize procedure-related data available about the TSST protocol to highlight commonalities and differences across studies. We noted significant discrepancies across studies in how researchers applied the TSST protocol. In particular, we highlight variations in testing procedures (e.g., number of judges, initial number in the arithmetic task, time of the collected saliva samples for cortisol) and discuss possible misinterpretation in comparing findings from studies failing to control for variables or using a modified version from the original protocol. Further, we recommend that researchers use a standardized background questionnaire when using the TSST to identify factors that may influence physiological measurements in tandem with a summary of this review as a protocol guide. More systematic implementation and detailed reporting of TSST methodology will promote study replication, optimize comparison of findings, and foster an informed understanding of factors affecting responses to social stressors in healthy people and those with pathological conditions.