Content uploaded by David C Wang
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by David C Wang on Jul 15, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Citation: Lowe, Gabriel B., David C.
Wang, and Eu Gene Chin. 2022.
Experiential Avoidance Mediates the
Relationship between Prayer Type
and Mental Health before and
through the COVID-19 Pandemic.
Religions 13: 652. https://
doi.org/10.3390/rel13070652
Academic Editor: Simon Dein
Received: 10 May 2022
Accepted: 30 June 2022
Published: 15 July 2022
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2022 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
religions
Article
Experiential Avoidance Mediates the Relationship between
Prayer Type and Mental Health before and through the
COVID-19 Pandemic
Gabriel B. Lowe 1, *, David C. Wang 2and Eu Gene Chin 3
1Independent Researcher, Encinitas, CA 92024, USA
2Fuller Theological Seminary, Pasadena, CA 91182, USA; david.wang@biola.edu
3Rosemead School of Psychology, Biola University, La Mirada, CA 90639, USA; eu.gene.chin@biola.edu
*Correspondence: g.lowe3290@gmail.com
Abstract:
The practice of prayer has been shown to predict various mental health outcomes, with
different types of prayer accounting for different outcomes. Considering the numerous stressors
facing seminary students, which have only intensified throughout the COVID-19 pandemic, prayer
may be a common coping strategy for students who study theology, spiritual growth, and leadership.
The present study investigates the role that different types of prayer may have in reducing anxiety,
depression, and work burnout among seminary students. Experiential avoidance is proposed as
a mediator such that specific types of prayer contribute to greater spiritual and characterological
formation through staying engaged in the midst of struggle. Longitudinal data was collected from
564 graduate seminary students from 17 institutions accredited by the Association of Theological
Schools. Based on previous research, we hypothesized that experiential avoidance would mediate
the relationship between colloquial, liturgical, meditative, and petitionary prayer types and the
negative mental health outcomes of anxiety, depression, and work burnout. Results confirmed
significant negative relationships between colloquial, liturgical, and meditative prayer types and all
three mental health indicators, fully mediated by experiential avoidance. Petitionary prayer was not
significantly related to lower levels of mental health. These results indicate that engaging in certain
prayer practices may be a protective factor by facilitating experiential engagement.
Keywords: prayer type; experiential avoidance; seminarians; anxiety; depression; burnout; COVID-19
1. Experiential Avoidance as a Mediator between Prayer Type and Mental Health in a
Seminary Sample Introduction
Clergy operate at the front lines and play vital roles in fostering healthy communities,
especially among minority groups (Wang et al. 2003) and elderly community members
(Weaver et al. 2003). On any given workday, clergy members may serve as ritualists,
pastors, preachers, teachers, organizers, and/or administrators (Milstein et al. (2005)). Con-
sequently, they face many occupational-type hazards due to the complex and public-facing
nature of their roles, including presumptive expectations, personal criticisms, ambiguous
boundaries (Lee and Iverson-Gilbert 2003), loneliness (Terry and Cunningham 2020) and
stress impinged upon their own family connections (Darling et al. 2004). Navigating these
powerful interactive systems (e.g., family pressures, peer expectations, congregant needs)
can be especially challenging and may downregulate the well-being of clergy and con-
gregant members (Grosch and Olsen 2000), resulting in shame and poor mental health
(Blea et al. 2021).
Prior to being clergy, many individuals seek training from seminary institutions to
prepare them for the roles, responsibilities, and challenges of this vocation. In these settings,
students are encouraged to grow in their theological understanding, professional skills,
and spiritual development. Spiritual growth among seminarians is challenging, with
Religions 2022,13, 652. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13070652 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/religions
Religions 2022,13, 652 2 of 22
many citing the rigor of their academic demands and juggling multiple responsibilities
as impediments toward formation of their spiritual lives (Greenman and Siew 2001). In
service of spiritual formation, clergy are often expected to practice and develop prayer
on their own. Broadly defined as a spiritual or religious act of communication between
humans and the divine, Poloma and Pendleton (1989) describe four types of prayers
(i.e., petitionary, colloquial, ritualistic, and meditative). Several studies show that prayer
predicts enhanced mental health through various mechanisms (e.g., feeling like God is
in control;
Jeppsen et al. 2015
), but none have tested experiential avoidance as a potential
causal mechanism.
Experiential avoidance, defined as value-incongruent attempts to escape from or con-
trol uncomfortable or undesirable internal states (e.g., thoughts, feelings), is garnering
increasing amounts of empirical support as a significant contributor to psychopathology and
other negative outcomes, including among samples of religious leaders
(Jankowski et al. 2022b)
.
In a systematic review, Chawla and Ostafin (2007) found that experiential avoidance was
associated with trauma symptoms, substance use relapse, maladaptive coping, poor self-
regulation, and overall mental illness severity. This study contributes to the literature in
a few ways. First, longitudinal data obtained from seminarians across two years of data
collection will establish the temporal precedence of experiential avoidance when examining
indirect effects of prayer on mental health outcomes. Second, as opposed to broad opera-
tionalizations of mental health in previous studies (Black et al. 2015;
Jeppsen et al. 2015
),
this study tracks more specific indices of mental health (i.e., anxiety, depression, and
burnout symptoms) to provide greater specificity in tested models. Results of the study are
discussed in relation to empirical and theological literature, with suggested future research
directions to broaden the scope and deepen our understanding of prayer and well-being
among clergy members.
Clergy Roles and Responsibilities
Clergy often fill many roles within their communities, both explicitly and implic-
itly. Milstein and his colleagues (2005) operationally defined six roles clergy frequently
assume to varying degrees. As a ritualist, they guide ritual practices (e.g., sacraments,
ordinances, etc.), preside over rites of passage (e.g., confirmation, weddings, funerals, etc.),
and lead worship services. The role of pastor refers to the relational duties of caring for the
needs of the congregation at an individual level, whereas the role of preacher refers to the
inspirational and guiding duties of caring for the needs of the congregation at the group
level. One of the more apparent roles is that of teacher, which refers to explicit activities
such as preaching, teaching, and educating. Behind the scenes, clergy also operate as
organizers who coordinate communal activities within the organization as well as engage
with their larger community, and as administrators who oversee the logistics of running
their religious establishment.
In addition to the wide skill set expected of clergy, they face specific stressors within
these varied roles, especially given the public-facing nature of their position. Some of
the occupational hazards identified by previous researchers of this population include
unsolicited criticism, boundary ambiguity, presumptive expectations (Frenk et al. 2013;
Lee and Iverson-Gilbert 2003), loneliness and lack of social support (Darling et al. 2004;
Frenk et al. 2013;Terry and Cunningham 2020), and burnout (Grosch and Olsen 2000).
These stressors extend to the families of clergy as well. Darling and her colleagues (2004)
found that clergy spouses showed similar risk levels for compassion fatigue, and burnout
and experienced higher levels of psychological and physiological stress compared to those
found in clergy. High workload, deep emotional demands, and low social support are
strong predictors of burnout and emotional exhaustion across a spectrum of front-line,
human service workers as well (e.g., medical doctors, police officers, teachers; Seidler et al.
2014). Teaching seminarians how to deal with these real-world problems is difficult, and
preparing them is a more challenging task.
Religions 2022,13, 652 3 of 22
2. Seminary Training
Indeed, those training to be clergy (i.e., seminary students) have plenty of respon-
sibilities to attend to, and they often find themselves in a unique subculture that can be
difficult to describe to those not familiar with theological and spiritual fields of study. For
example, many clergy and seminarians report receiving a “calling” regarding their chosen
field of work or study. While people in other professions, especially helping professions,
may report a similar magnetism towards a particular field, what sets this experience apart
is that the origin is divine (Thomas 1962). Moreover, experiencing a call to vocational
ministry is often assumed or presumed. In two separate studies, all participants responded
affirmatively to experiencing a call to ministry at some point in their life (Civish 2013;
Meek et al. 2003).
Another distinction of seminary training is the aim of developing students in an inte-
grated manner such that they are not only growing in their theological understanding and
professional skills, but in their spirituality as well. The terms “spirituality” and “spiritual
formation” have an abundance of meanings both within and beyond Christian traditions.
A compelling and dynamic conceptualization is that of “relational spirituality” put forth
by Shults and Sandage (2006). At its core is a dialectical posture positing movement be-
tween a rootedness in a spiritual community and tradition and a healthy curiosity seeking
individual expression and intellectual wrestling (Sandage et al. 2011). The impetus of
this movement hopefully leads to development along at least three dimensions: spiritual
formation (an individual’s relationship with God and/or the sacred), characterological
formation (internalization of virtue), and moral formation (outward expression of virtue;
Porter et al. 2019).
The aspiration of promoting the spiritual growth of seminarians is easier said than
done. While interviewing seminarians, Greenman and Siew (2001) reported nearly all
subjects in the study alluded to the potential dampening or disruption to their personal
spiritual lives because of the rigor of their academic demands. Furthermore, the interview-
ers solicited comments regarding public engagement as an expression of spirituality which
adds yet another dimension to which seminarians may be attempting to embody their
vocational calling amidst their academic responsibilities. When helping seminarians thrive
while juggling the various demands, it is important for training institutions to be attentive
to the holistic health of their students.
3. The Mental Health of Seminary Students
While much of the attention has understandably gone towards investigating the
spiritual health and development of seminary students, the mental health of seminary
students is interesting for a variety of reasons. Perhaps most obvious is mental health as
an indicator of their quality of life and well-being which hopefully impacts their ability to
thrive during their seminary journey. It is important, first, to pause and acknowledge the
nuance between “mental health” and “mental illness.” “Mental health” denotes strengths-
based terms such as emotional well-being, effective behaviors, meaningful relationships,
and the ability to cope with stressors (American Psychological Association n.d.). On the
other hand, “mental illness” denotes deficits-based terms such as emotional and cognitive
disturbances and corresponding abnormal manifestations and/or functional impairment
caused by these symptoms (American Psychological Association n.d.). While the two
concepts have been frequently conflated, recent studies confirm that they are indeed
distinct yet correlated axes rather than poles of a single continuum (Keyes 2005;Payton
2009). Although the variables in the present study focus primarily on mental illness,
the results of this study will be considered from a larger perspective that includes ideas
pertaining to mental health as well.
Unfortunately, there is a dearth of research focusing on the mental health of seminari-
ans. Moreover, within this small body of literature, disparate results illuminate the breadth
of trajectories future investigation may take. For example, one study found that seminari-
ans reported higher quality of life than their secular peers in psychophysical, psychosocial,
Religions 2022,13, 652 4 of 22
personal, and metaphysical spheres (Prusak et al. 2021), while another study found that the-
ology students and pastors scored lower on measures of mental health compared to norm
samples (Voltmer et al. 2011). The former study attributed their results to the numerous
studies validating the positive impact of religiosity and spirituality on well-being in the way
of social support, purpose in life, and positive orientation. For example, one longitudinal
study reported a longitudinal relationship between religious/spiritual commitment and
well-being that was mediated by virtue (Jankowski et al. 2022a). On the other hand, the
second study stressed the psychosocial strain incurred in helping professions such as voca-
tional ministry. Similar to the distinction made between mental health and mental illness
(Keyes 2005;Payton 2009), the former study employed measures that generally map onto
mental health (e.g., gratitude, meaning in life, psychological quality in life), while the latter
used instruments that map onto mental illness (e.g., mental health symptoms, physical
health impairments). Thus, while the results appear to be contradictory at the surface level,
these divergent results are not necessarily orthogonal in nature. The pain/strain incurred in
vocational ministry may be heavy, but it may not have to reduce the felt meaning, purpose,
and quality of life of seminary students.
Despite being several decades old, an article by the Rev. Dr. Owen Thomas (1962)
provides insight into the psychological strain that may still resonate with present-day
seminarians. First, he points out that the content of seminary study is both “deeply
personal and existential,” covering topics such as ultimate destiny, human nature, and
redemption. Second, he identifies the “irrational anomaly” of the calling experience (see
Seminary Training section). Finally, because of this calling, doubts about their vocation can
become overbearing, and the pressure to finish seminary is intensified by the shame at the
thought of “failing” their call. These among others are unique psychological stressors that
seminarians may face.
It is also important to recognize contextual differences and individual needs. For
example, Catholic seminarians in Poland report minimal anxiety regarding their career
trajectory since this decision would be made for them (Prusak et al. 2021), whereas Catholic
seminarians in Nigeria expressed a greater desire for more career counseling given the lack
of guidance in their context (Adubale and Aluede 2017). Another specific expressed need
surfacing in the Nigeria study was for financial counseling. Financial burden is a reality for
many seminarians given the rising costs of higher education and the relatively low earning
potential of their chosen field; however, this goes beyond material pragmatics. One study
found that the negative relationship between financial well-being and mental illness, in
the form of depression and anxiety, was mediated by shame (Blea et al. 2021). While the
precise reason for this relationship requires further investigation, it is clear that the stresses
that seminarians face can have a deeply personal impact and contribute to mental illness.
While further research is needed to better understand mental health and illness among
seminarians, it is undoubtedly important for seminarians to develop coping skills and
healthy practices given the prevalence of mental illness among clergy. Multiple studies
have found increased rates of depression and anxiety among clergy members compared
to the general population (Proeschold-Bell et al. 2013;Shaw et al. 2021), and higher levels
of mental illness have been associated with social isolation, loneliness, and critical and
demanding congregations (Proeschold-Bell et al. 2015;Terry and Cunningham 2020). A
longitudinal study by Milstein et al. (2020) helped to clarify the directionality of these
interactions in which greater spiritual well-being predicts lower depressive symptoms,
which in turn predicts less occupational distress. They note that this direction suggests that
while occupational stressors may not necessarily cause depression, depressive symptoms
may increase susceptibility to occupational distress.
The need to help seminarians promote healthy practices and reduce mental illness risk
factors has only become more pronounced due to the increased strain from the COVID-19
pandemic. Research presented by Scott McConnell investigating pastor attrition in the Fall
of 2021 (personal communication, 11 February 2022) found that 50% of pastors feel that the
demands of ministry are greater than they can handle. The proportion of pastors who feel
Religions 2022,13, 652 5 of 22
“frequently overwhelmed” was recorded at 63%, up from 54% in 2015. Corresponding with
other findings regarding pastoral social support, 38% of pastors reported feeling isolated
in their role. These numbers corroborate findings from Barna who found that as of March
2022, 42% of pastors considered quitting full-time ministry within the past year (Barna
Group 2022). While some metrics improved, such as pastors’ sense of financial security and
their feeling of being “on call”, continued efforts are needed to increase clergy resiliency.
4. Prayer Practices and Types
Many occupations place professionals in the unenviable limelight to “practice what
they preach,” lest they be labeled a hypocrite; however, few may feel this pressure more
than clergy for whom this maxim has a quite literal meaning. Prayer, defined as a personal
or corporate act of communication between humans and the divine (e.g., God, the gods,
supernatural entities; Illueca and Doolittle 2020), is a ubiquitous spiritual practice across
many religions and spiritual traditions, and for individuals who seek to lead their commu-
nities in prayer practices, it is reasonably expected that clergy develop their own practice
of this discipline.
Although studies on prayer have begun to pervade the scientific literature, it remains
a difficult phenomenon to study. Methodologically, many previous studies have suffered
from a lack of rigor including design flaws, weak measurement tools, and internal validity
problems (McCullough 1995). From a theological perspective, the primary goal of prayer is
relating to God, which is often not at the center of the research question. While this need
not necessarily exclude the study of prayer, taking into consideration cultural diversity and
inclusion, it behooves researchers to acknowledge the religious and spiritual values at play.
Thus, studies examining prayer practices may focus on secondary goals and tangential
effects. A review of empirical research on the relationship between prayer and health
tends to divide into the following domains: prayer and subjective well-being, prayer as
coping, the effects of intercessory prayer, content of prayer, and prayer and psychiatric
symptoms/disorders (Masters and Spielmans 2007;McCullough 1995). Our study will
focus on prayer and psychiatric symptoms/disorders.
When examining the relationship between prayer and psychiatric symptoms, we will
consider prayer from a multidimensional perspective. The effects of prayer on psychiatric
symptoms/disorders appear to be dependent on one’s image about God (e.g., benevolent
versus vindictive God; Bradshaw et al. 2008), attachment style to God (e.g., avoidance ver-
sus secure attachment; Ellison et al. 2014), type of prayer (e.g., meditative versus petitionary
prayers; Poloma and Pendleton 1989), increased control through God
(Jeppsen et al. 2015)
,
and closeness to God (Jeppsen et al. 2015). With regards to private prayer, Laird et al.
(2004) divided prayer into four types: adoration (i.e., focus on worship of God), confes-
sion (i.e., acknowledging own shortcomings), thanksgiving (i.e., expressions of gratitude),
and supplication (i.e., requests for God to intervene). Covering a broader spectrum of
prayers (beyond private prayers), Poloma and Pendleton (1989) identified four types of
prayers: petitionary, colloquial, ritualistic, and meditative prayer types based on principal
components analysis and demonstrated theorized relationships with external measures of
quality of life. Petitionary prayer refers to requests for specific material needs of self and
others. Colloquial prayer includes talking to God in one’s own words, generally being less
concrete and specific compared to petitionary prayers. Ritualistic prayer refers to prayers
that are read, memorized, and recited. Meditative prayer includes components of intimacy
and personal relationship to God such as adoring, reflecting, and communicating with
the divine.
Poloma and Pendleton (1989,1991) reported that the content and intent of the prayer
type being practiced makes a difference. In their study on the effects of prayer types on
general well-being, they found that frequency of prayer was a weak predictor of well-being
and that experiences during prayer and types of prayer practices engaged in were stronger
predictors of well-being (1991). Furthermore, they found that petitionary prayer was the
only type not related to any measure of quality of life; meditative, colloquial, and ritual
Religions 2022,13, 652 6 of 22
prayer were related to at least one specific subcategory of quality of life (1989). In fact,
petitionary prayer has been found to be consistently associated with negative mental health
outcomes (Black et al. 2015;Jeppsen et al. 2015;Zarzycka and Krok 2021). Upon closer
investigation, several studies have shown that prayer practices predict enhanced mental
health and well-being through various mechanisms including closeness to God and feeling
like God is in control (Jeppsen et al. 2015), disclosure as a means of communicating with
God (Black et al. 2015;Zarzycka and Krok 2021); trust-based beliefs (Pössel et al. 2014), and
cognitive reappraisal (i.e., reframing the source of pain; Dezutter et al. 2011). These run
parallel to many of the studies reviewed by McCullough (1995) that examined prayer as a
coping resource for reducing existing distress, pain, and stress.
5. Experiential Avoidance
So how does prayer move from practice to discipline to character growth? Returning
to the model of spiritual, characterological, and moral formation set forth by Porter and
his colleagues (2019), there is an intermingling of experiencing the Divine, being shaped
in a virtuous direction, and expressing virtues in tangible manifestations. They go on
to posit that there is value to investigating both the end-result of a formative process as
well as the pathways towards those end-results. In this way, the spiritual and character
formation of seminarians is an active process requiring intentional, value-driven, and
flexible engagement practices that facilitate virtue development within the context of both
religious commitment and religious exploration (Bond et al. 2011;Jankowski et al. 2021).
Psychological flexibility in this context enables full contact with the present moment
and persisting and changing behavior in service of spiritual character formation (Bond et al.
2011). Conversely, psychological inflexibility occurs when people avoid making contact
with private events
1
due to rigid dominance of psychological reactions over chosen values
and contingencies. Experiential avoidance is an instance of psychological inflexibility;
specifically, it occurs when an individual attempts to alter, escape from, or control difficult,
uncomfortable, or undesirable internal states (thoughts, feelings, sensations, etc.), even
when this leads to or would lead to actions that are incongruent with one’s values and goals
(Hayes et al. 2006). Thus, experiential avoidance is diametrically opposed to the notion of
psychological flexibility, which is frequently described in the acceptance and commitment
therapy model for mental illness (Hayes et al. 1999).
Experiential avoidance has often been shown to only create or exacerbate problems or,
ironically, increase interaction with the situation trying to be avoided (Gámez et al. 2011).
A review completed by Chawla and Ostafin (2007) of studies investigating the relationship
between experiential avoidance and psychopathology found that experiential avoidance
was associated with substance use relapse, trauma symptoms, maladaptive coping, poor
self-regulation, and psychiatric symptom severity. Conversely, facing the difficulty with
acceptance and developing cognitive flexibility have been shown to promote health and
work productivity (Bond et al. 2010).
This is congruent with both Christian theology and studies examining faith matu-
rity. Both Jesus and the biblical authors state that life will be filled with difficulty and
suffering, especially for those who choose to follow Christ (Matthew 16:24; John 15:18–20;
2 Corinthians 12:10). A cross-sectional study reported by Knabb and Grigorian-Routon
(2014) found that those with greater faith maturity tended to utilize positive religious
coping strategies such as reaching out to God for support, asking God to help with feelings
of anger, and collaborating with God to take action in the face of adversity. They also
found that the relationship between negative religious coping strategies (i.e., questioning
God’s power, viewing suffering as punishment from God) and depression and anxiety was
partially mediated by experiential avoidance. Spiritual practices that promote acceptance
and psychological flexibility will lead to positive outcomes such as enhanced mental health,
reduced mental illness, and higher levels of maturity and well-being.
Religions 2022,13, 652 7 of 22
6. The Present Study
Because of the centrality of prayer to Christian spirituality, it is not difficult to see how
prayer sits squarely in the spiritual formation domain as a primary means to relate to God
and submit to God’s will. Thus, this study will examine the downstream effects of specific
types of prayers within Porter et al.’s (2019) spiritual and characterological development
framework. Specifically, this study will shed light on the longitudinal direct and indirect
pathways in which prayer may protect against or predispose toward experiencing mental
illness. To this end, experiential avoidance will be examined longitudinally as a means
to track the development of personal virtues resulting from prayer activities. Moreover,
because previous studies investigating prayer operationalize mental health as a general,
broad construct referred to as overall mental health (Black et al. 2015;Jeppsen et al. 2015), we
have chosen to examine the particular relationships between prayer types and depression,
anxiety, and burnout symptoms to provide greater specificity in the literature.
To these ends, we submit the following hypotheses. First, following the example of
Poloma and Pendleton (1989,1991), we hypothesize that the total frequency of prayer
will be a weak predictor of mental illness, and specificity of prayer types will be a greater
indicator of mental health. Second, we hypothesize that colloquial, liturgical (ritual),
and meditative prayer will be negatively related with depression, anxiety, and burnout
symptoms. Given the frequent association of petitionary prayer with negative mental
health outcomes, we hypothesize that petitionary prayer will positively predict depression,
anxiety, and burnout symptoms. Third, building off Hayes and his colleague’s (1999) con-
ceptualization of experiential avoidance, we hypothesize that experiential avoidance will
positively predict depression, anxiety, and burnout symptoms. Fourth, we hypothesize that
colloquial, liturgical, and meditative prayer will negatively predict experiential avoidance,
and petitionary prayer will positively predict experiential avoidance. Fifth, for our primary
model of interest, we hypothesize that experiential avoidance will mediate the relationship
between each of the four prayer types and each of the negative mental health outcomes.
Although measuring the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic was not the primary aim of
this study when data was initially collected in the fall of 2019, the longitudinal nature of
this project provides additional context for the variables of interest as they apply to this
contemporary reality.
7. Method
7.1. Participants
Data were collected from students at 17 seminaries across North America (N = 564;
Mage = 31.49; SD = 11.12; range = 19–71). Participants included non-degree, graduate
degree (e.g., M.A., M.Div.), or certificate-earning students. Among those providing demo-
graphic data, 52.0% identified as male, 47.3% identified as female, and 0.7% identified as
other or chose not to disclose. The majority of the sample identified as White (62.9%), fol-
lowed by Asian (14.3%), Black (8.7%), Hispanic (6.0%), multiracial (6.1%), and other (2.0%).
7.2. Procedure
Upon receiving Institutional Review Board approval, first-term students were re-
cruited during the fall of 2019 via digital and in-person announcements. Participants
provided voluntary consent and completed an online survey containing a variety of mea-
sures discussed below. Students were compensated with a USD 25 gift card for their
participation and were invited in subsequent semesters approximately six months apart to
complete the survey again. Accordingly, participants completed time point 1 measures in
the fall of 2019, time point 2 measures in the spring of 2020, time point 3 measures in the
fall of 2020, and time point 4 measures in the spring of 2021.
Religions 2022,13, 652 8 of 22
8. Measures
8.1. Prayer Types
Frequency engaging in various prayer practices was measured using a scale consisting
of eight items with two items for each prayer type, resulting in four subscales (colloquial,
liturgical, meditative, and petitionary). This scale was based on the factor analysis of
15 prayer activity items formulated by Poloma and Pendleton (1989) in which they identified
four factors (“types of prayer”). “Ritual” prayer was reconceptualized as “liturgical” prayer.
Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often), with
higher scores indicating more frequent practice of the prayer activity associated with the
prayer type and lower scores indicating less frequency. Scores of the two items per subscale
were aggregated to produce a single score for each prayer type. Examples of statements
that participants were prompted to respond to included: “Talk with God as you talk with a
friend” (colloquial), “Recite prayers that you have memorized” (liturgical), “Spend time
being in the presence of God” (meditative), and “Ask God for material things you may
need” (petitionary). For total frequency of prayer, we did not have a separate item as
Poloma and Pendleton (1989,1991) did and instead used an aggregate variable of all
eight items.
8.2. Experiential Avoidance
Experiential avoidance was measured using items from the Acceptance and Action
Questionnaire, version 2 (AAQ-II), a measure designed to measure experiential avoidance
and psychological inflexibility (Bond et al. 2011). Although their factor analysis resulted
in a unidimensional measure centered around the construct of psychological inflexibility,
experiential avoidance is a foundational concept within this construct and reflected in
several of the items. For the purposes of the present study, we used three items that most
closely reflect the concept of experiential avoidance and had high factor loadings: “I’m
afraid of my feelings” (0.70), “I worry about not being able to control my worries and
feelings” (0.71), and “Emotions cause problems in my life” (0.79). Items were rated on a
7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never true)to7(always true), and responses were
aggregated to form a single latent concept.
8.3. Depression
Depression symptoms were measured using the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-
9), a diagnostically validated, self-report screening measure frequently used in clinical
settings (Kroenke et al. 2001;Spitzer et al. 1999). It consists of nine items which ask about
the frequency of depressive symptoms within the past two weeks. Responses for each item
are rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 to 3, with a possible overall scale
range of 0 to 27. Higher scores indicate higher levels of impairment with cutoffs for clinical
use being: 1–4, minimal depression; 5–9, mild depression; 10–14, moderate depression;
15–19, moderately severe depression; and 20–27, severe depression.
8.4. Anxiety
Anxiety symptoms were measured using the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale-
7 (GAD-7), a diagnostically validated, self-report screening measure frequently used in
clinical settings (Spitzer et al. 2006). It consists of seven items which ask about the frequency
of core symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder within the past two weeks. Responses for
each item are rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 to 3, with a possible overall
scale range of 0 to 21. Higher scores indicate higher levels of impairment with cutoffs for
clinical use being: 1–5, mild anxiety; 6–10, moderate anxiety; and 15–21, severe anxiety.
8.5. Work-Related Burnout
Work-related burnout was measured using the corresponding scale of the Copenhagen
Burnout Inventory (CBI; Kristensen et al. 2005). The CBI was developed to have three
sub-dimensions (personal burnout, work-related burnout, and client related burnout) that
Religions 2022,13, 652 9 of 22
could be used independently or integratively. The developers of this scale cite theoretical
and methodological reasons for choosing not to validate the three CBI scales through factor
analysis, one such reason being that the scales may be used to fit with the populations
being studied. They define work-related burnout as “the degree of physical and psycho-
logical fatigue and exhaustion that is perceived by the person as related to his/her work”
(Kristensen et al. 2005). The work-related burnout subscale consists of seven items rated on
a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never/almost never)to5(always/almost always).
In the validation study, scoring ranged from 0 (never/almost never) to 100 (always/almost
always). Example items include “Is your work emotionally exhausting?” and “Do you feel
worn out at the end of the working day?”
9. Statistical Analyses
Data analysis utilized the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version
28. We tested our hypotheses using bivariate and multiple regression models and utilized
Hayes’s (2018) PROCESS macro for SPSS Model 4 to test a series of simple mediation models
with covariates. Data from four time points were analyzed to investigate the longitudinal
relationship between variables. In addition to the proposed model of experiential avoidance
mediating the relationship between each prayer type and each negative mental health
outcome, we tested three other models to confirm the directionality of the relationships. In
the proposed model, the independent variables were each of the four prayer types at Time
1, each tested separately. The dependent variables were depression and anxiety at Time 3,
each tested separately. Burnout was also tested as a dependent variable; however, data for
burnout was only available at Time 4. Experiential avoidance was measured at Time 2 as
the mediator variable.
The following are the other models tested to confirm the directionality of the relation-
ships and to test for better model fits. Model 2 used experiential avoidance at Time 1 as the
dependent variable, each prayer type at Time 1 used separately as the mediator variable,
and depression and anxiety at Time 3 and burnout at Time 4 used separately as the depen-
dent variable. Model 3 used experiential avoidance at Time 1 as the dependent variable,
each prayer type at Time 2 used separately as the mediator variable, and depression and
anxiety at Time 3 and burnout at Time 4 used separately as the dependent variable. Model
4 used experiential avoidance at Time 1 as the dependent variable, all prayer type at Time 2
used as parallel mediators, and depression and anxiety at Time 3 and burnout at Time 4
used separately as the dependent variable.
10. Results
Means and standard deviations for the variables studied are displayed in Table 1. The
mean score for the PHQ-9 in the present study (M = 5.87, SD = 5.15) was comparable to
some normative data. A study in Germany found that in non-clinical settings, women had
mean scores of 3.1 (SD = 3.5) and men had mean scores of 2.7 (SD = 3.5; Kocalevent et al.
2013); however, a study in Japan found mean scores of 6.96 (SD = 6.46) among a non-clinical
group and mean scores of 12.42 (SD = 7.57) among those diagnosed with major depressive
disorder (Doi et al. 2018). Some 19 percent of participants scored 10 or higher, which
is regarded as the cutoff score with the highest combined sensitivity and specificity for
major depressive disorder (Levis et al. 2019). The mean score for the GAD-7 in the present
study (M= 4.93, SD = 4.71) was likewise very similar to previous findings of mean scores
among a general population (M= 4.9, SD = 4.8; Spitzer et al. 2006). Some 16 percent of
participants scored 10 or higher which is the cutoff for a potential diagnosis of generalized
anxiety disorder.
Religions 2022,13, 652 10 of 22
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of variables tested in regression models.
M SD
Petitionary Prayer 3.06 0.92
Liturgical Prayer 2.75 1.21
Meditative Prayer 3.83 0.90
Colloquial Prayer 3.87 0.95
Experiential Avoidance 2.97 1.38
Anxiety Symptoms 4.93 4.71
Depression Symptoms 5.87 5.15
Work Burnout 18.37 5.15
Table 2displays the correlation matrix as a preliminary overview for the variables
studied in the proposed model. As expected, the majority of the prayer types correlated
with each other as did all three of the negative mental health outcomes. Petitionary prayer
and liturgical prayer did not correlate with each other. Interestingly, depression symptoms
had a negative relationship with both liturgical prayer (r=
−
0.11; small effect, p< 0.05) and
meditative prayer (r=−0.14; small effect, p< 0.01).
Table 2. Correlations among prayer types, experiential avoidance, and negative mental health.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Petitionary Prayer -
2. Liturgical Prayer 0.058 -
3. Meditative Prayer 0.118 ** 0.293 ** -
4. Colloquial Prayer 0.276 ** 0.109 ** 0.517 ** -
5. Experiential Avoidance −0.024 −0.138 ** −0.146 ** −0.165 ** -
6. Anxiety Symptoms 0.029 −0.079 −0.095 −0.081 0.345 ** -
7. Depression Symptoms −0.078 −0.113 * −0.144 ** −0.093 0.401 ** 0.709 ** -
8. Work Burnout 0.079 −0.051 −0.074 −0.086 0.275 ** 0.405 ** 0.454 ** -
*p< 0.05; ** p< 0.01.
Our first hypothesis predicted that the total prayer score would demonstrate a weak
relationship with anxiety, depression, and burnout. The standardized direct effects on
anxiety (
β
= 0.01, p= 0.93), depression (
β
=
−
0.06, p= 0.25), and work burnout (
β= 0.01
,
p= 0.77) were all insignificant. Interestingly, when we added the mediator variable of
experiential avoidance as in the primary model of interest into the regression model,
the results were significant: anxiety (Table 3, standardized indirect effect =
−
0.08, 95%
CI [
−
0.13,
−
0.03]); depression (Table 3, standardized indirect effect =
−
0.72, 95% CI
[−1.19, −0.35]
); and work burnout (Table 3, standardized indirect effect =
−
0.55, 95% CI
[−0.94, −0.24]). These results partially support our hypothesis.
Our second hypothesis predicted that colloquial, liturgical, and meditative prayer
would negatively predict anxiety, depression, and burnout and that petitionary prayer
would positively predict depression, anxiety, and burnout. For colloquial prayer, the
standardized direct effects on anxiety (
β
= 0.01, p= 0.80), depression (
β
=
−
0.01, p= 0.83),
and work burnout (
β
=
−
0.01, p= 0.82) were all insignificant. The standardized direct
effects of liturgical prayer on anxiety (
β
=
−
0.05, p= 0.37), depression (
β
=
−
0.04,
p= 0.50
),
and work burnout (
β
=
−
0.01, p= 0.89) were all insignificant. For meditative prayer, the
standardized direct effects on anxiety (
β
=
−
0.004, p= 0.94), depression (
β
=
−
0.06,
p= 0.25
),
and work burnout (
β
= 0.01, p= 0.93) were all insignificant. The direct effects between
petitionary prayer and anxiety (
β
= 0.06, p= 0.20), depression (
β
=
−
0.08, p= 0.16), and
work burnout (
β
= 0.10, p= 0.07) were all insignificant. These results did not support
our hypothesis.
Religions 2022,13, 652 11 of 22
Table 3.
Regression models involving total prayer, experiential avoidance and mental health symptoms.
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Anxiety
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Total Prayer a1−0.49 0.12 0.000 c’10.01 0.06 0.929
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b10.16 0.02 0.001
Constant IM1 4.82 0.43 0.000 IY1 0.44 0.24 0.071
R2= 0.050 R2= 0.13
F(2, 333) = 8.68, p< 0.01 F(3, 332) = 17.07, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Depression
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Total Prayer a2−0.49 0.12 0.000 c’2−0.52 0.45 0.245
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b21.47 0.20 0.000
Constant iM24.82 0.43 0.000 iY2203.31 1.86 0.000
R2= 0.050 R2= 0.18
F(2, 333) = 8.68, p< 0.01 F(3, 332) = 24.94, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Work Burnout
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Total Prayer a3−0.54 0.13 0.000 c’30.13 0.47 0.770
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b31.02 0.21 0.000
Constant iM35.02 0.48 0.000 iY317.01 1.94 0.000
R2= 0.058 R2= 0.12
F(2, 272) = 8.41, p< 0.01 F(3, 271) = 11.99, p< 0.01
Note. Estimates represented in the table are unstandardized parameter estimates. The corresponding stan-
dardized indirect effects predicting dependent outcomes as follows: anxiety =
−
0.08, 95% CI [
−
0.13,
−
0.03];
depression = −0.72, 95% CI [−1.19, −0.35]; work burnout = −0.55, 95% CI [−0.94, −0.24].
Our third hypothesis predicted that experiential avoidance will positively predict
depression, anxiety, and burnout symptoms. In each of the models tested, experiential
avoidance positively predicted anxiety: colloquial (Table 4;b
1
= 0.16, p< 0.01), liturgical
(Table 5;b
1
= 0.16, p< 0.01), meditative (Table 6;b
1
= 0.16, p< 0.01), and petitionary
(Table 7;b
1
= 0.16, p< 0.01). Similarly, in each of the models tested, experiential avoidance
positively predicted depression: colloquial (Table 4;b
2
= 1.64, p< 0.01), liturgical (Table 5;
b
2
= 1.62, p< 0.01), meditative (Table 6;b
2
= 1.60, p= 0.01), and petitionary (Table 7;
b2= 1.63
,
p< 0.01)
. Finally, in each of the models, experiential avoidance positively predicted work
burnout: colloquial (Table 4;b
3
= 1.04, p< 0.01), liturgical (Table 5;b
3
= 1.04; p< 0.01),
meditative (Table 6;b
3
= 1.04; p< 0.01), and petitionary (Table 7;b
3
= 1.06, p< 0.01). Our
third hypothesis was fully supported in all models.
Religions 2022,13, 652 12 of 22
Table 4.
Regression models involving colloquial prayer, experiential avoidance, and mental
health symptoms.
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Anxiety
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Colloquial Prayer a1−0.24 0.08 0.002 c’10.01 0.04 0.801
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b10.16 0.03 0.000
Constant IM14.06 0.33 0.000 IY10.42 0.19 0.027
R2= 0.03 R2= 0.13
F(2, 333) = 5.00, p< 0.01 F(3, 332) = 17.09, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Depression
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Colloquial Prayer a2−0.30 0.09 0.001 c’2−0.07 0.32 0.829
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b21.64 0.22 0.000
Constant iM24.07 0.39 0.000 iY2200.91 1.64 0.000
R2= 0.04 R2= 0.19
F(2, 272) = 5.73, p< 0.01 F(3, 271) = 21.31, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Work Burnout
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Colloquial Prayer a3−0.32 0.09 0.000 c’3−0.07 0.30 0.818
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b31.04 0.20 0.000
Constant iM34.19 0.37 0.000 iY318.26 1.49 0.000
R2= 0.01 R2= 0.14
F(2, 293) = 6.77, p< 0.01 F(3, 292) = 15.31, p< 0.01
Note. Estimates represented in the table are unstandardized parameter estimates. The corresponding stan-
dardized indirect effects predicting dependent outcomes as follows: anxiety =
−
0.04, 95% CI [
−
0.07,
−
0.01]
depression = −0.49, 95% CI [−0.86, −0.17]; work burnout = −0.33, 95% CI [−0.60, −0.12].
Table 5.
Regression models involving liturgical prayer, experiential avoidance, and mental
health symptoms.
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Anxiety
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Liturgical Prayer a1−0.18 0.06 0.004 c’1−0.03 0.03 0.365
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b10.16 0.03 0.000
Constant IM13.72 0.25 0.000 IY10.55 0.15 0.000
R2= 0.03 R2= 0.14
F(2, 333) = 4.57, p = 0.011 F(3, 332) = 17.38, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Depression
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Liturgical Prayer a2−0.19 0.07 0.007 c’2−0.17 0.25 0.499
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b21.62 0.21 0.000
Constant iM23.53 0.29 0.000 iY2201.19 1.28 0.000
R2= 0.03 R2= 0.19
F(2, 272) = 3.81, p= 0.023 F(3, 271) = 21.48, p< 0.01
Religions 2022,13, 652 13 of 22
Table 5. Cont.
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Work Burnout
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Liturgical Prayer a3−0.21 0.06 0.001 c’3−0.03 0.22 0.893
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b31.04 0.19 0.000
Constant iM33.63 0.27 0.000 iY318.08 1.15 0.000
R2= 0.03 R2= 0.14
F(2, 293) = 5.24, p< 0.01 F(3, 292) = 15.29, p< 0.01
Note. Estimates represented in the table are unstandardized parameter estimates. The corresponding stan-
dardized indirect effects predicting dependent outcomes as follows: anxiety =
−
0.03, 95% CI [
−
0.05,
−
0.01];
depression = −0.30, 95% CI [−0.55, −0.08]; work burnout = −0.22, 95% CI [−0.37, −0.09].
Table 6.
Regression models involving meditative prayer, experiential avoidance, and mental
health symptoms.
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Anxiety
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Meditative Prayer a1−0.25 0.08 0.003 c’1−0.003 0.04 0.937
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b10.16 0.03 0.000
Constant IM14.10 0.35 0.000 IY10.47 0.19 0.017
R2= 0.03 R2= 0.13
F(2, 333) = 4.85, p< 0.01 F(3, 332) = 17.07, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Depression
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Meditative Prayer a2−0.29 0.09 0.002 c’2−0.38 0.33 0.246
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b21.60 0.21 0.000
Constant iM24.09 0.41 0.000 iY2202.22 1.68 0.000
R2= 0.04 R2= 0.19
F(2, 272) = 5.28, p< 0.01 F(3, 271) = 21.85, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Work Burnout
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Meditative Prayer a3−0.31 0.09 0.001 c’3−0.03 0.30 0.929
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b31.04 0.20 0.000
Constant iM34.20 0.39 0.000 iY318.09 1.54 0.000
R2= 0.04 R2= 0.14
F(2, 293) = 6.10, p< 0.01 F(3, 292) = 15.29, p< 0.01
Note. Estimates represented in the table are unstandardized parameter estimates. The corresponding stan-
dardized indirect effects predicting dependent outcomes as follows: anxiety =
−
0.04, 95% CI [
−
0.07,
−
0.01];
depression = −0.46, 95% CI [−0.82, −0.17]; work burnout = −0.32, 95% CI [−0.55, −0.13].
Religions 2022,13, 652 14 of 22
Table 7.
Regression models involving petitionary prayer, experiential avoidance, and mental
health symptoms.
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Anxiety
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Petitionary Prayer a1−0.07 0.08 0.395 c’10.05 0.04 0.204
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b10.16 0.03 0.000
Constant IM13.42 0.31 0.000 IY10.30 0.17 0.077
R2= 0.004 R2= 0.14
F(2, 333) = 0.69, p= 0.50 F(3, 332) = 17.69, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Depression
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Petitionary Prayer a2−0.06 0.09 0.474 c’2−0.46 0.33 0.161
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b21.63 0.21 0.000
Constant iM23.18 0.35 0.000 iY2202.01 1.39 0.000
R2= 0.003 R2= 0.20
F(2, 272) = 0.46, p= 0.64 F(3, 271) = 22.10, p< 0.01
Consequent Variable
(M) Experiential Avoidance (Y) Work Burnout
Antecedent variable Coeff SE p Coeff SE p
(X) Petitionary Prayer a3−0.06 0.09 0.532 c’30.55 0.30 0.071
(M) Experiential Avoidance - - - b31.06 0.19 0.000
Constant iM33.20 0.34 0.000 iY316.37 1.26 0.000
R2= 0.002 R2= 0.14
F(2, 293) = 0.22, p= 0.80 F(3, 292) = 16.55, p< 0.01
Note. Estimates represented in the table are unstandardized parameter estimates. The corresponding stan-
dardized indirect effects predicting dependent outcomes as follows: anxiety =
−
0.01, 95% CI [
−
0.04, 0.02];
depression = −0.11, 95% CI [−0.46, 0.21]; work burnout = −0.06, 95% CI [−0.29, 0.14].
Our fourth hypothesis predicted that colloquial, liturgical, and meditative prayer
would negatively predict experiential avoidance, and petitionary prayer would positively
predict experiential avoidance. For all three negative mental health outcomes, colloquial
prayer negatively predicted experiential avoidance: anxiety (Table 4;
a1=−0.24
,p< 0.01),
depression (Table 4;
a2=−0.30
,
p< 0.01
), and work burnout (Table 4;a
3
=
−
0.32, p< 0.01).
Liturgical prayer also negatively predicted experiential avoidance for all three negative
mental health outcomes: anxiety (Table 5;a
1
=
−
0.18, p< 0.01), depression (Table 5;
a2=−0.19,
p< 0.01), and work burnout (Table 5;a
3
=
−
0.21, p< 0.01). Likewise, meditative
prayer negatively predicted experiential avoidance for all three negative mental health
outcomes: anxiety (Table 6;a
1
=
−
0.25, p< 0.01), depression (Table 6;a
2
=
−
0.29, p< 0.01),
and work burnout (Table 6;a
3
=
−
0.31, p< 0.01). In the case of petitionary prayer, its effect
on experiential avoidance was insignificant for all three negative mental health outcomes:
anxiety (Table 7;a
1
=
−
0.07, p= 0.40), depression (Table 7;a
2
=
−
0.06, p= 0.47), and work
burnout (Table 7;a
3
=
−
0.06, p= 0.53). These results mostly supported our hypothesis with
only petitionary prayer failing to reach a significant level.
Finally, our fifth hypothesis predicted that experiential avoidance would mediate
the relationship between each of the four prayer types and each of the negative mental
health outcomes. For the first set of models, experiential avoidance significantly medi-
ated the relationship between colloquial prayer and anxiety (Table 4; standardized in-
direct
effect = −0.04
, 95% CI [
−
0.07,
−
0.01]), depression (Table 4; standardized indirect
effect = −0.49
, 95% CI [
−
0.86,
−
0.17]), and work burnout (Table 4; standardized indirect
effect =
−
0.33, 95% CI [
−
0.60,
−
0.12]). Because the direct effects of colloquial prayer on
Religions 2022,13, 652 15 of 22
all mental health outcomes were not statistically significant (see c’1, c’2, and c’3in Table 4),
experiential avoidance fully mediated the aforementioned relationships, suggesting that
anxiety, depression and work burnout symptoms are influenced by engagement with col-
loquial prayer through a causal sequence in which greater engagement with colloquial
prayer leads to less experiential avoidance, which in turn, putatively decreases anxiety,
depression, and burnout symptoms.
Similarly, experiential avoidance fully mediated the relationship between liturgical
prayer and anxiety (Table 5; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.03, 95% CI [
−
0.05,
−
0.01]),
depression (Table 5; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.30, 95% CI [
−
0.55,
−
0.08]), and work
burnout (Table 5; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.22, 95% CI [
−
0.37,
−
0.09]). Because the
direct effects of liturgical prayer on all three negative mental health outcomes were not
statistically significant (see c’
1,
c’
2,
and c’
3
in Table 5), experiential avoidance fully mediated
the aforementioned relationships, suggesting that anxiety, depression, and work burnout
symptoms are influenced by engagement with liturgical prayer through a causal sequence
in which greater engagement with liturgical prayer leads to less experiential avoidance,
which in turn, putatively decreases anxiety, depression, and burnout symptoms.
For meditative prayer, experiential avoidance also fully mediated the relationship with
anxiety (Table 6; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.04, 95% CI [
−
0.07,
−
0.01]), depression
(Table 6; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.46, 95% CI [
−
0.82,
−
0.17]), and experiential
avoidance (Table 6; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.32, 95% CI [
−
0.55,
−
0.13]). Because
the direct effects of meditative prayer on all three negative mental health outcomes were
not statistically significant (see c’
1,
c’
2,
and c’
3
in Table 6), experiential avoidance fully
mediated the aforementioned relationships, suggesting that anxiety, depression, and work
burnout symptoms are influenced by engagement with meditative prayer through a causal
sequence in which greater engagement with meditative prayer leads to less experiential
avoidance, which in turn, putatively decreases anxiety, depression, and burnout symptoms.
Finally, in the case of petitionary prayer, we did not find a significant mediation effect
for anxiety (Table 7standardized indirect effect =
−
0.01, 95% CI [
−
0.04, 0.02]), depression
(Table 7; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.11, 95% CI [
−
0.46, 0.21]), or work burnout
(Table 7; standardized indirect effect =
−
0.06, 95% CI [
−
0.29, 0.14]). Because the direct
effects between petitionary prayer on all three negative mental health outcomes were not
statistically significant as well (see c’
1,
c’
2,
and c’
3
in Table 7), greater engagement with
petitionary prayer appears to have no direct or indirect downstream effects on experiential
avoidance of anxiety, depression, or work burnout symptoms.
11. Discussion
The aim of the present study was to determine the relationship between different types
of prayer, experiential avoidance, and negative mental health outcomes among seminary
students. Following the lead of Poloma and Pendleton (1989,1991), our first test was to
examine the overall effect of prayer frequency on mental illness. Similar to their findings,
no significant direct effect between total prayer frequency and anxiety, depression, or work
burnout was detected. However, when experiential avoidance was added as a mediator, the
relationship between total prayer frequency and mental illness became significant. It should
be noted that we did not use a separate variable for prayer frequency as did Poloma and
Pendleton which may mean we examined slightly different constructs. Nevertheless, the
significance of these results bodes well for understanding the longitudinal effects of prayer
on mental illness. Our subsequent hypotheses delved into the specificity of individual
prayer types.
The second goal was to replicate previous findings linking prayer types with mental
health outcomes. While we did find significant indirect effects between certain prayer
types and negative mental health, we did not find a significant direct effect as previous
studies have found (Black et al. 2015;Jeppsen et al. 2015,2022). One possibility for why
this occurred is that our study used instruments for particular mental health ailments
(i.e., GAD-7, PHQ-9, CBI) rather than a general measure for overall mental health. It may
Religions 2022,13, 652 16 of 22
be that identifying such a specific relationship is beyond the scope of our measures, if
such a relationship exists at all. Additionally, our study utilized longitudinal data rather
than a cross-sectional approach which may weaken the direct effect that prayer practices
have on mental states many months into the future. Although Park et al. (2018) did find
statistically significant predictive relationships between positive and negative religious
coping and well-being in a longitudinal study, religious coping also encompasses a broader
range of coping styles beyond prayer, including but not limited to reappraisals of the
stressful situation, seeking control through individual initiatives, seeking support from
others, and/or religious helping (Pargament et al. 2001).
The third hypothesis that we pursued validated the conceptualization set forth by
Hayes and his colleagues (1999) that experiential avoidance leads to negative mental health
outcomes. This was fully supported in all the models that were tested. These results
confirm the notion that attempting to avoid uncomfortable or difficult situations and
feelings serves to exacerbate the challenge of managing mental ailments. One explanation
involves conceptualizing emotional avoidance as an emotional discordant stress response
(Leonidou and Panayiotou 2021). Specifically, individuals high in dispositional experiential
avoidance appear to experience a more intense perception of subjective arousal (controlling
for objective physiological activation) during emotional exposures and cope by not being
fully aware of their responses or the significance of these types of private events. This
inaccurate assessment of stress may lead to a mismatch between coping strategies and
the type and severity of stressors. Furthermore, when people mitigate or escape difficulty
through avoidance, they deny themselves the opportunity to develop the psychological
skills and fortitude to grow more capable and resilient. The cost of experiential avoidance is
not only that the problem is postponed, but also that we choose incongruent and inauthentic
replacements for our true goals and values. These physiological, phenomenological, and
behavioral mechanisms represent possible pathways in which experiential avoidance leads
to psychopathology (Hayes et al. 1996).
The next hypothesis tested was that colloquial, liturgical, and meditative prayer would
negatively predict experiential avoidance and that petitionary prayer would positively
predict experiential avoidance. This was mostly supported by our results with petitionary
prayer being the only model that was not significant. Our interpretation is that colloquial,
liturgical, and meditative prayer help to remain engaged with the present moment in the
face of adversity. We posit that petitionary prayer, while it is still a widely encouraged
practice in Scripture, can inadvertently lead to experiential avoidance if it is utilized
habitually as a wishful attempt to escape. Petitionary prayer has been deemed a less mature
form of prayer by some (Black et al. 2015;Poloma and Gallup 1991), and in previous
studies, it has been frequently connected with poor mental health outcomes (Black et al.
2015;Jeppsen et al. 2015;Zarzycka and Krok 2021). While our results did not demonstrate
a positive relationship between petitionary prayer and experiential avoidance, the lack
of significance indicates that more investigation needs to be carried out to illuminate
the virtues and pitfalls of this particular practice. Furthermore, future studies should
continue to explore directionality to differentiate from individuals whose mental illness
may predispose them to utilize petitionary prayer in response to their situation.
Our primary investigatory question was exploring the possibility of experiential
avoidance mediating the relationship between prayer types and negative mental health. As
with our fourth hypothesis, the models for colloquial, liturgical, and meditative prayer were
all significant, and the models for petitionary prayer were insignificant. Thus, colloquial,
liturgical, and meditative prayer appears to predict lower experiential avoidance over time,
which putatively downregulates anxiety, depression, and burnout symptoms later in life.
Additionally, the significance of the mediated model of experiential avoidance on total
prayer frequency and mental illness suggests that greater engagement through prayer has a
positive effect over time. The fact that the direct effects of all four types of prayer and total
prayer on negative mental health were insignificant may help to interpret the longitudinal
nature of this relationship. We theorize that the significance of the indirect relationship
Religions 2022,13, 652 17 of 22
over the various time points is indicative of the characterological growth that can occur
through these prayer practices which can aid in remaining engaged in the midst of struggle.
This perseverance may be due to the intentionality of being disciplined in these practices,
the buffering effects of receiving divine or social support, both, or even something more.
Previous studies have consistently demonstrated the positive impact of social support for
pastors and seminarians, and specifically colloquial and meditative prayer types have been
theorized to tap into those relational needs in the human–divine relationship (Black et al.
2015;Jeppsen et al. 2015).
While it is convenient to group the types of prayer together based on the model results
or previous findings, it is worth speculating on their individual relationships to experiential
avoidance and mental health. Although colloquial prayer may feel uncomfortably informal
to some, it is precisely that lack of pretense that allows a person to present their current
situation to God in an authentic way. Theologically speaking, there is nothing about us
that is hidden from God and trying to hide behind pretense or formality is just another
form of experiential avoidance. Moreover, communicating with God in a colloquial manner
promotes relational intimacy. Jesus himself encouraged his followers to address his Father
as their own, which is an invitation to bring everything before God with a sense of freedom
(Matthew 6:9).
Liturgical prayer may be more or less familiar depending on a person’s tradition
and background, and it can include a variety of practices including repeating a well-
known prayer (e.g., the Lord’s prayer), reading from a book of prayers, and praying a
memorized prayer. In their study of prayer type and mental health outcomes, Black and
her colleagues (2015) hypothesized and found that ritual prayer was not significantly
related to mental health through self-disclosure as a mediator. Although this makes sense
given their theoretical approach, prayer is a multi-layered activity that goes beyond just
communicating with God, though that is a primary purpose. Thus, our decision to reframe
“ritual” prayer as “liturgical” helps to reflect the nuances and impact of this prayer type,
even if those words may be interchangeable in some situations. While there is indeed
a repetitive aspect of liturgical prayer that can become rote and hackneyed if one is not
careful, it can also be deeply connecting and resonant as well. For some, repeating the
prayers that have been prayed for centuries is rooting and comforting. It is a way to join
with the multitude of saints — past, present, and future. It can also be promoting unity
within the Church, adding an “amen” of agreement to a prayer that resonates with your
own current experiences.
Meditative prayer may seem like escape to some and engagement to others. Indeed,
anything has the potential to be turned into avoidance. We suggest that meditative prayer
can be richly engaging, especially when seen from the vantage point of mindful attention
to the present moment. Mindfulness practices are well-documented in the psychological
literature as an effective coping strategy and promoter of well-being. Even though mind-
fulness and meditation are not always synonymous, there can often be overlap. A main
thrust behind the robustness of mindfulness is its emphasis on acceptance of the self and
the present moment. This acceptance coupled with a meditative attention to core truths
and values can be a positive and potent combination. Applied to meditative prayer and
experiential avoidance, the practice of simply basking in the presence of God and allowing
space to listen and reflect can help to loosen the grip of anxiety and control. Although
meditative prayer may not be engaging in a flashy or obvious way, it can be a powerful
way of engaging with a difficult situation while releasing the final say to God.
Petitionary prayer seems to have acquired a poor reputation over the several studies
that have been referenced, and the lack of significant results in the present study does
not help the case. Nevertheless, we are hesitant to label petitionary prayer as immature
or deficient. The present study, for example, used items that measure petitionary prayer
reference supplication for material things for self (“How often do you ask God for material
things you may need?”) or others (“Ask for material things your friends or relatives may
need?”). Although different from Poloma and Pendleton’s (1989) original definition, one
Religions 2022,13, 652 18 of 22
wonders whether petitionary prayer could encompass other non-materialistic outcomes
(e.g., increased faith, hope, and love) and whether this would demonstrate different rela-
tionships with experiential avoidance and mental health outcomes. Furthermore, because
Scripture encourages prayer, it is possible that the value of petitionary prayer could be
hidden among other virtues that have not been highlighted in these studies. For example,
petitionary prayer may be more an exercise in faith and faithfulness rather than a measure
of avoidance versus engagement. Indeed, previous studies have found feeling close to
God and feeling like they have control through God (God-mediated control) appear to
be function as mediators between prayer supplication and well-being, with the former
having stronger explanatory ability compared to the latter (Jeppsen et al. 2015). Conversely,
another possibility is that petitionary prayer practiced selfishly or inconsiderately has the
tendency to draw out pathology in ways that the other prayer types avoid. If a person
asks for deliverance or a change in their circumstances but is unwilling or unable to be
introspective about their role to play, that petition could become a form of experiential
avoidance. Furthermore, although not specific to petitionary prayer, the effects of prayer
frequency on psychiatric symptoms/disorders appears to be dependent on one’s image
about God (e.g., benevolent versus vindictive God; Bradshaw et al. 2008) and attachment
style to God (e.g., avoidance versus secure attachment; Ellison et al. 2014). Future research
could shed further light into these possible mediation and moderation effects of petitionary
prayer on mental health outcomes.
We hope that the results of this study will encourage and support students, religious
professionals, and training institutions to continue to do the difficult work of spiritual,
character, and moral formation (Porter et al. 2019). As helpful as it is to have longitudinal
data to shed light on the process dynamics of spiritual growth and development, one does
not need a research study to know that character is formed over the course of a lifetime. To
that end, we implore spiritual and religious leaders to reflect deeply on not just how to teach
spiritual practices, but also how those spiritual practices form our thoughts, perceptions,
and behaviors. We also hope that this research encourages individualized attention in
mentorship and discipleship. To try passing on the findings of this study by painting with
broad brushstrokes is to risk falling into the trap of shaping behavior without forming
character. As has been alluded to several times and demonstrated widely, formation occurs
in the context of relationships (Shults and Sandage 2006). A final exhortation is to not lose
heart if prayer practices include discomfort. Psychological flexibility comes with growing
pains, and engagement is often much more disruptive and unsettling than avoidance.
Although it is easy to be preoccupied by the immediate struggle, the benefit comes in the
form of long-term mental health.
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
Prayer is inherently a difficult construct to study. It is often a very private activity,
shaped by culture and tradition, and can be difficult to operationalize for the sake of
study. While we feel confident in the items chosen for our prayer type measures for the
purposes of this study, future studies may benefit from more extensive measures to draw
out further nuances of the multi-dimensions of prayer. For example, the effects of group
prayer versus control on mental health appears promising, with experimental evidence
suggesting positive effects of communal prayer at post- (Boelens et al. 2009) and 1-year
follow-up (Boelens et al. 2012) time points. Given the accumulating evidence for benefits
attained through social support (Ellison et al. 2017;Bartkowski et al. 2017), differentiating
the effects of private versus communal prayer could be a rich area of study. Participants
may also have been tempted to answer items in a socially desirable manner so as not
to appear spiritually immature. There is also the possibility of testing fatigue given the
comprehensive nature of the survey containing over 100 items and taking approximately
90 minutes to complete.
It is also important to acknowledge that while data was collected from 17 seminaries,
there is an immense amount of diversity within Christianity that could not be fully captured
Religions 2022,13, 652 19 of 22
in this study. While we attempted to use inclusive, ecumenical language in this study to
increase the potential for utility across denominational lines, we recognize that we are
contextualized within our own biases and relational circles. Future studies may consider
replicating or extending the findings of this study within different traditions or with an even
more comprehensive sample. Moreover, some studies have also compared models across
other religions which may help increase understanding of cultural differences (Jeppsen et al.
2022). Seminary students continue to be an understudied population, and more research
is needed to further understand the unique challenges they face and how to best prepare
them for religious professions.
We also recommend future studies to incorporate positive psychology into their inves-
tigation of how to help seminary students thrive. As Proeschold-Bell and her colleagues
(2015) have highlighted, mental health and mental illness are correlated, yet separate,
continuums, and the integration of both will help provide a more complete understanding
of human flourishing. Recognizing this distinction provides some advantages. It enhances
precision by revealing significant relationships that might exist on one construct but not
the other, thereby being easily missed if not considered, and it helps to provide a more
complete picture of a model that benefits from the explanatory variance of both sides
(Proeschold-Bell et al. 2015). While we focused on experiential avoidance, anxiety, depres-
sion, and work burnout, we recognize that health is not just the absence of ailments but
also the presence of strengths, resilience, and virtue.
Author Contributions:
G.B.L., D.C.W. and E.G.C.; writing—review and editing, G.B.L., D.C.W. and
E.G.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This study received generous funding from the John Templeton Foundation (#61515).
Institutional Review Board Statement:
This study was approved by the IRB of Biola University
(Protocol #SS19-014_SE).
Informed Consent Statement:
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Conflicts of Interest:
The authors declare no potential conflict of interest with respect to the research,
authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Note
1
Psychological inflexibility can occur when private events are positive emotions (e.g., avoid feeling elated for fear of disappoint-
ment) or when neutral or pleasant internal events decrease people’s sensitivity to value-related goals (e.g., fantasizing about a
promotion at work which decreases their ability to respond to important responsibilities; Bond et al. 2011).
References
Adubale, Andrew A., and Oyaziwo Aluede. 2017. A survey of counselling needs of seminarians in Catholic major seminaries in
Nigeria. Asia Pacific Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy 8: 29–40. [CrossRef]
American Psychological Association. n.d. Mental Health. In APA Dictionary of Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association, Available online: https://dictionary.apa.org/mental-health (accessed on 6 June 2022).
Barna Group. 2022. Pastors Share Top Reasons They’ve Considered Quitting Ministry in the Past Year; Ventura: Barna Group. Available
online: https://www.barna.com/research/pastors-quitting-ministry/ (accessed on 27 April 2022).
Bartkowski, John. P., Gabriel. A. Acevedo, and Harriet Van Loggerenberg. 2017. Prayer, meditation, and anxiety: Durkheim revisited.
Religions 8: 191. [CrossRef]
Black, Stephanie Winkeljohn, Patrick Pössel, Benjamin D. Jeppsen, Annie C. Bjerg, and Don T. Wooldridge. 2015. Disclosure During
Private Prayer as a Mediator Between Prayer Type and Mental Health in an Adult Christian Sample. Journal of Religion and Health
54: 540–53. [CrossRef]
Blea, Jordan, David C. Wang, Christina L. Kim, Gabriel Lowe, Jesika Austad, Melabi Amponsah, and Nicholle Johnston. 2021. The
Experience of Financial Well-Being, Shame, and Mental Health Outcomes in Seminary Students. Pastoral Psychology 70: 299–314.
[CrossRef]
Boelens, Peter A., Roy R. Reeves, William H. Replogle, and Harold G. Koenig. 2009. A randomized trial of the effect of prayer on
depression and anxiety. The International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 39: 377–92. [CrossRef]
Boelens, Peter A., Roy R. Reeves, William H. Replogle, and Harold G. Koenig. 2012. The effect of prayer on depression and anxiety:
Maintenance of positive influence one year after prayer intervention. The International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 43: 85–98.
[CrossRef]
Religions 2022,13, 652 20 of 22
Bond, Frank W., Paul E. Flaxman, Marc J. P. M. van Veldhoven, and Michal Biron. 2010. The impact of psychological flexibility and
acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) on health and productivity at work. In Contemporary Occupational Health Psychology:
Global Perspectives on Research and Practice, Vol 1. Edited by Jonathan Houdmont and Stavroula Leka. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell,
pp. 296–313. [CrossRef]
Bond, Frank W., Steven C. Hayes, Ruth A. Baer, Kenneth M. Carpenter, Nigel Guenole, Holly K. Orcutt, Tom Waltz, and Robert
D. Zettle. 2011. Preliminary Psychometric Properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire–II: A Revised Measure of
Psychological Inflexibility and Experiential Avoidance. Behavior Therapy 42: 676–88. [CrossRef]
Bradshaw, Matt, Christopher G. Ellison, and Kevin J. Flannelly. 2008. Prayer, God imagery, and symptoms of psychopathology. Journal
for the Scientific Study of Religion 47: 644–59. [CrossRef]
Chawla, Neharika, and Brian Ostafin. 2007. Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An
empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology 63: 871–90. [CrossRef]
Civish, Gayle. 2013. The calling: A study of personal spiritual experiences of seminary students. Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health
15: 186–214. [CrossRef]
Darling, Carol A., E. Wayne Hill, and Lenore M. McWey. 2004. Understanding stress and quality of life for clergy and clergy spouses.
Stress & Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress 20: 261–77.
Dezutter, Jessie, Amy Wachholtz, and Jozef Corveleyn. 2011. Prayer and pain: The mediating role of positive re-appraisal. Journal of
Behavioral Medicine 34: 542–49. [CrossRef]
Doi, Satomi, Masaya Ito, Yoshitake Takebayashi, Kumiko Muramatsu, and Masaru Horikoshi. 2018. Factorial validity and invariance
of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-9 among clinical and non-clinical populations. PLoS ONE 13: e0199235. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
Ellison, Christopher G., Matt Bradshaw, Kevin J. Flannelly, and Kathleen C. Galek. 2014. Prayer, attachment to God, and symptoms of
anxiety-related disorders among US adults. Sociology of Religion 75: 208–33. [CrossRef]
Ellison, Christopher G., Reed T. DeAngelis, and Metin Güven. 2017. Does religious involvement mitigate the effects of major
discrimination on the mental health of African Americans? Findings from the Nashville Stress and Health Study. Religions 8: 195.
[CrossRef]
Frenk, Steven M., Sarah A. Mustillo, Elizabeth G. Hooten, and Keith G. Meador. 2013. The Clergy Occupational Distress Index (CODI):
Background and Findings from Two Samples of Clergy. Journal of Religion and Health 52: 397–407. [CrossRef]
Gámez, Wakiza, Michael Chmielewski, Roman Kotov, Camilo Ruggero, and David Watson. 2011. Development of a measure of
experiential avoidance: The Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment 23: 692–713.
[CrossRef]
Greenman, Jeffrey P., and Yau Siew. 2001. Spirituality and public character: A qualitative cross-sectional study of Master of Divinity
students in Toronto. Theological Education 38: 1–20.
Grosch, William N., and David C. Olsen. 2000. Clergy Burnout: An Integrative Approach. Journal of Clinical Psychology 56: 619–32.
[CrossRef]
Hayes, Andrew F. 2018. Partial, conditional, and moderated mediation: Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Communication
Monographs 85: 4–40. [CrossRef]
Hayes, Steven C., Jason B. Luoma, Frank W. Bond, Akihiko Masuda, and Jason Lillis. 2006. Acceptance and commitment therapy:
Model, processes and outcomes. Behavior Research and Therapy 44: 1–25. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hayes, Steven C., Kelly G. Wilson, Elizabeth V. Gifford, Victoria M. Follette, and Kirk Strosahl. 1996. Experiential avoidance and
behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
64: 1152–68. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hayes, Steven C., Kirk Strosahl, and Kelly G. Wilson. 1999. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior
Change. New York: Guilford Press.
Illueca, Marta, and Benjamin R. Doolittle. 2020. The use of prayer in the management of pain: A systematic review. Journal of Religion
and Health 59: 681–699. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jankowski, Peter J., Sam Murphy, Jaclyn Johnson, Steven J. Sandage, David C. Wang, and James Tomlinson. 2022a. The influence of
experiential avoidance, humility and patients on the association between religious/spiritual exploration and well-being. Journal
of Happiness Studies 23: 2137–56. Available online: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-021-00488- w (accessed on 28 March 2022).
[CrossRef]
Jankowski, Peter J., Steven J. Sandage, David C. Wang, and Peter C. Hill. 2021. Relational spirituality profiles and flourishing among
emerging religious leaders. Journal of Positive Psychology. [CrossRef]
Jankowski, Peter J., Steven J. Sandage, David C. Wang, and Sarah Crabtree. 2022b. Virtues as mediators of the associations between
religious/spiritual commitment and well-being. Applied Research in the Quality of Life. Available online: https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11482-022-10046-y (accessed on 28 March 2022).
Jeppsen, Benjamin, Patrick Pössel, Stephanie W. Black, Annie Bjerg, and Don Wooldridge. 2015. Closeness and Control: Exploring the
Relationship Between Prayer and Mental Health. Counseling & Values 60: 164–85. [CrossRef]
Jeppsen, Benjamin, Stephanie W. Black, Patrick Pössel, and David H. Rosmarin. 2022. Does closeness to God mediate the relationship
between prayer and mental health in Christian, Jewish, and Muslim samples? Mental Health, Religion & Culture 25: 99–112.
[CrossRef]
Religions 2022,13, 652 21 of 22
Keyes, Corey L. M. 2005. Mental Illness and/or Mental Health? Investigating Axioms of the Complete State Model of Health. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology 73: 539–48. [CrossRef]
Knabb, Joshua J., and Anna Grigorian-Routon. 2014. The role of experiential avoidance in the relationship between faith maturity,
religious coping, and psychological adjustment among Christian university students. Mental Health, Religion & Culture 17: 458–69.
[CrossRef]
Kocalevent, Rüya-Daniela, Andreas Hinz, and Elmar Brähler. 2013. Standardization of the depression screener Patient Health
Questionnaire (PHQ-9) in the general population. General Hospital Psychiatry 35: 551–55. [CrossRef]
Kristensen, Tage, Marianne Borritz, Ebbe Villadsen, and Karl Christensen. 2005. The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory: A new tool for
the assessment of burnout. Work & Stress 19: 192–207.
Kroenke, Kurt, Robert L. Spitzer, and Janet B. Williams. 2001. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of
General Internal Medicine 16: 606–13. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Laird, Seven P., Charles R. Snyder, Michael A. Rapoff, and Sam Green. 2004. Measuring Private Prayer: Development, Validation, and
Clinical Application of the Multidimensional Prayer Inventory. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 14: 251–72.
[CrossRef]
Lee, Cameron, and Judith Iverson-Gilbert. 2003. Demand, Support, and Perception in Family-Related Stress among Protestant Clergy.
Family Relations 52: 249–57. [CrossRef]
Leonidou, Chrysanth, and Georgia Panayiotou. 2021. Can we predict experiential avoidance by measuring subjective and physiological
emotional arousal? Current Psychology, 1–13. [CrossRef]
Levis, Brooke, Andrea Benedetti, and Brett D. Thombs. 2019. Accuracy of Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) for screening to
detect major depression: Individual participant data meta-analysis. BMJ British Medical Journal 365: l1476. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Masters, Kevin S., and Glen I. Spielmans. 2007. Prayer and health: Review, meta-analysis, and research agenda. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine 30: 329–38. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McCullough, Michael E. 1995. Prayer and Health: Conceptual Issues, Research Review, and Research Agenda. Journal of Psychology &
Theology 23: 15–29.
Meek, Katheryn Rhoads, Mark R. McMinn, Craig M. Brower, Todd D. Burnett, Barrett W. McRay, Michael L. Ramey, David W. Swanson,
and Dennise D. Villa. 2003. Maintaining personal resiliency: Lessons learned from evangelical Protestant clergy. Journal of
Psychology & Theology 31: 339–47.
Milstein, Glen, Gary Kennedy, M. Bruce, Kevin Flannelly, Nancy Chelchowski, and L. Bone. 2005. The Clergy’s Role in Reducing
Stigma: Elder Patients’ Views. World Psychiatry 4: 26–32.
Milstein, Glen, Celia F. Hybels, and Rae Jean Proeschold-Bell. 2020. A prospective study of clergy spiritual well-being, depressive
symptoms, and occupational distress. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 12: 409–16. [CrossRef]
Pargament, Kenneth I., Nalini Tarakeshwar, Christopher G. Ellison, and Keith M. Wulff. 2001. Religious coping among the religious:
The relationships between religious coping and well-being in a national sample of Presbyterian clergy, elders, and members.
Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 40: 497–513. [CrossRef]
Park, Crystal L., Cheryl L. Holt, Daisy Le, Juliette Christie, and Beverly R. Williams. 2018. Positive and negative religious coping
styles as prospective predictors of well-being in African Americans. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 10: 318–26. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
Payton, Andrew R. 2009. Mental health, mental illness, and psychological distress: Same continuum or distinct phenomena? Journal of
Health & Social Behavior 50: 213–27. [CrossRef]
Poloma, Margaret M., and Brian F. Pendleton. 1989. Exploring Types of Prayer and Quality of Life: A Research Note. Review of Religious
Research 31: 46–53. [CrossRef]
Poloma, Margaret M., and Brian F. Pendleton. 1991. The effects of prayer and prayer experiences on measures of general well being.
Journal of Psychology & Theology 19: 71–83.
Poloma, Margaret M., and George H. Gallup. 1991. Varieties of Prayer. A Survey Report. Philadelphia: Trinity Press.
Porter, Steven L., Steven J. Sandage, David C. Wang, and Peter C. Hill. 2019. Measuring the Spiritual, Character, and Moral Formation
of Seminarians: In Search of a Meta-Theory of Spiritual Change. Journal of Spiritual Formation & Soul Care 12: 5–24. [CrossRef]
Pössel, Patrick, Stephanie W. Black, Annie C. Bjerg, Benjamin D. Jeppsen, and Don T. Wooldridge. 2014. Do Trust-Based Beliefs Mediate
the Associations of Frequency of Private Prayer with Mental Health? A Cross-Sectional Study. Journal of Religion and Health 53:
904–16. [CrossRef]
Proeschold-Bell, Rae Jean, Andrew Miles, Matthew Toth, Christopher Adams, Bruce W. Smith, and David Toole. 2013. Using effort-
reward imbalance theory to understand high rates of depression and anxiety among clergy. The Journal of Primary Prevention 34:
439–53. [CrossRef]
Proeschold-Bell, Rae Jean, Ashley Eisenberg, Christopher Adams, Bruce Smith, Sara LeGrand, and Amber Wilk. 2015. The Glory of
God is a Human Being Fully Alive: Predictors of Positive Versus Negative Mental Health Among Clergy. Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion 54: 702–21. [CrossRef]
Prusak, Jacek, Krzysztof Kwapis, Barbara Pilecka, Agnieszka Chemperek, Agnieszka Krawczyk, Marcin Jabło´nski, and Krzysztof
Nowakowski. 2021. The quality of life, meaning in life, positive orientation to life and gratitude of Catholic seminarians in
Poland: A comparative analysis. Archive for the Psychology of Religion 43: 78–94. [CrossRef]
Religions 2022,13, 652 22 of 22
Sandage, Steven J., Peter C. Hill, and Deanne C. Vaubel. 2011. Generativity, relational spirituality, gratitude, and mental health:
Relationships and pathways. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 21: 1–16. [CrossRef]
Seidler, Andreas, Marleen Thinschmidt, Stephanie Deckert, Francisca Then, Janice Hegewald, Karen Nieuwenhuijsen, and Steffi G.
Riedel-Heller. 2014. The role of psychosocial working conditions on burnout and its core component emotional exhaustion–a
systematic review. Journal of Occupational Medicine and Toxicology 9: 1–13. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Shaw, Martin A. L., Roy Lukman, Linda W. Simmons, and Ramona Reynolds. 2021. Clergy Wholeness Study: How Occupational
Distress, Depression, and Social Support Inform the Health of Clergy. The Journal of Pastoral Care & Counseling 75: 23–32. [CrossRef]
Shults, F. LeRon, and Steven J. Sandage. 2006. Transforming Spirituality: Integrating Theology and Psychology. Ada: Baker Academic.
Spitzer, Robert L., Kurt Kroenke, and Janet B. W. Williams. 1999. Validation and Utility of a Self-report Version of PRIME-MD: The
PHQ Primary Care Study. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association 282: 1737–44. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Spitzer, Robert L., Kurt Kroenke, Janet B. W. Williams, and Bernd Löwe. 2006. A Brief Measure for Assessing Generalized Anxiety
Disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine 166: 1092–97. [CrossRef]
Terry, J. Drake, and Christopher J. L. Cunningham. 2020. The Sacred and Stressed: Testing a Model of Clergy Health. Journal of Religion
and Health 59: 1541–66. [CrossRef]
Thomas, Owen C. 1962. Psychological pressures on the seminarian. The Journal of Pastoral Care 16: 95–97.
Voltmer, Edgar, Christine Thomas, and Claudia Spahn. 2011. Psychosocial health and spirituality of theology students and pastors of
the German Seventh-Day Adventist Church. Review of Religious Research 52: 290–305.
Wang, Philip S., Patricia A. Berglund, and Ronald C. Kessler. 2003. Patterns and correlates of contacting clergy for mental disorders in
the United States. Health Services Research 38: 647–73. [CrossRef]
Weaver, Andrew J., Kevin J. Flannelly, Laura T. Flannelly, and Julia. E. Oppenheimer. 2003. Collaboration between clergy and mental
health professionals: A review of professional health care journals from 1980 through 1999. Counseling and Values 47: 162–71.
[CrossRef]
Zarzycka, Beata, and Dariusz Krok. 2021. Disclosure to God as a Mediator Between Private Prayer and Psychological Well-Being in a
Christian Sample. Journal of Religion and Health 60: 1083–95. [CrossRef] [PubMed]