ArticlePDF Available

Theory of mind in naturalistic conversations between autistic and typically developing children and adolescents

SAGE Publications Inc
Autism
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Successful social interactions are assumed to depend on theory of mind—the ability to represent others’ mental states—yet most studies of the relation between theory of mind and social-interactive success rely on non-interactive tasks that do not adequately capture the spontaneous engagement of theory of mind, a crucial component of everyday social interactions. We addressed this gap by establishing a novel observational rating scale to measure the spontaneous use of theory of mind (or lack thereof) within naturalistic conversations (conversational ToM; cToM). In 50 age- and gender-matched dyads of autistic and typically developing youth aged 8–16 years (three dyad types: autistic–typically developing, typically developing–typically developing, autistic–autistic), we assessed cToM during 5-min unstructured conversations. We found that ratings on the cToM Negative scale, reflecting theory-of-mind-related violations of neurotypical conversational norms, were negatively associated with two forms of non-interactive theory of mind: visual-affective and spontaneous. In contrast, the cToM Positive scale, reflecting explicit mental state language and perspective-taking, was not associated with non-interactive theory of mind. Furthermore, autistic youth were rated higher than typically developing youth on cToM Negative, but the groups were rated similarly on cToM Positive. Together, these findings provide insight into multiple aspects of theory of mind in conversation and reveal a nuanced picture of the relative strengths and difficulties among autistic youth. Lay abstract Conversation is a key part of everyday social interactions. Previous studies have suggested that conversational skills are related to theory of mind, the ability to think about other people’s mental states, such as beliefs, knowledge, and emotions. Both theory of mind and conversation are common areas of difficulty for autistic people, yet few studies have investigated how people, including autistic people, use theory of mind during conversation. We developed a new way of measuring cToM using two rating scales: cToM Positive captures behaviors that show consideration of a conversation partner’s mental states, such as referring to their thoughts or feelings, whereas cToM Negative captures behaviors that show a lack of theory of mind through violations of neurotypical conversational norms, such as providing too much, too little, or irrelevant information. We measured cToM in 50 pairs of autistic and typically developing children (ages 8–16 years) during 5-min “getting to know you” conversations. Compared to typically developing children, autistic children displayed more frequent cToM Negative behaviors but very similar rates of cToM Positive behaviors. Across both groups, cToM Negative (but not Positive) ratings were related to difficulties in recognizing emotions from facial expressions and a lower tendency to talk about others’ mental states spontaneously (i.e., without being instructed to do so), which suggests that both abilities are important for theory of mind in conversation. Altogether, this study highlights both strengths and difficulties among autistic individuals, and it suggests possible avenues for further research and for improving conversational skills.
This content is subject to copyright.
Theory of mind in naturalistic conversations between autistic
and typically developing children and adolescents
Diana Alkire1,2, Kathryn A. McNaughton1,2, Heather A. Yarger1,2, Deena Shariq1,2, Elizabeth
Redcay1,2
1Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, University of Maryland, College Park
2Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park
Abstract
Successful social interactions are assumed to depend on theory of mind (ToM)—the ability
to represent others’ mental states—yet most studies of the relation between ToM and social-
interactive success rely on non-interactive tasks that do not adequately capture the
spontaneous
engagement of ToM, a crucial component of everyday social interactions. We addressed this
gap by establishing a novel observational rating scale to measure the spontaneous use of
ToM (or lack thereof) within naturalistic conversations (
conversational ToM
; cToM). In 50
age- and gender-matched dyads of autistic (AUT) and typically developing (TD) youth aged
8–16 (three dyad types: AUT-TD, TD-TD, AUT-AUT), we assessed cToM during 5-minute,
unstructured conversations. We found that ratings on the cToM Negative scale, reflecting ToM-
related violations of neurotypical conversational norms, were negatively associated with two
forms of non-interactive ToM: visual-affective and spontaneous. In contrast, the cToM Positive
scale, reflecting explicit mental state language and perspective-taking, was not associated with
non-interactive ToM. Furthermore, autistic youth were rated higher than TD youth on cToM
Negative, but the groups were rated similarly on cToM Positive. Together, these findings provide
insight into multiple aspects of ToM in conversation and reveal a nuanced picture of the relative
strengths and difficulties among autistic youth.
Introduction
Everyday life is saturated with social interactions, and leveraging these interactions to form
high-quality relationships is critical for mental and physical wellbeing (Hedley et al., 2018;
Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010). It is widely assumed that social-interactive success depends on
theory of mind (ToM)—the ability to represent the mental states of others and oneself—
an assumption that drives the influential “mindblindness” account of social impairment in
autism (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). While empirical findings indicate broad, distal relations
between ToM and social outcomes (Bosacki & Astington, 1999; Caputi et al., 2017; Devine
& Hughes, 2013; Peterson et al., 2016), the specific mechanisms driving these relations are
Correspondence: Diana Alkire, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742,
diana.r.alkire@gmail.com.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
HHS Public Access
Author manuscript
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Published in final edited form as:
Autism
. 2023 February ; 27(2): 472–488. doi:10.1177/13623613221103699.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
unclear. That is, how does ToM ability or propensity impact real-world behaviors that in turn
influence social-interactive success?
Answering this question with a useful degree of specificity is hampered by the discrepancy
between standard ways of measuring ToM in the laboratory and the nature of everyday
social interactions. Standard ToM tasks range widely in format, but they share a common
feature: the subject plays the role of an observer rather than a participant in a social
exchange (Begeer et al., 2010; Schilbach et al., 2013). As such, the behaviors directly
elicited by observational tasks—labeling mental states, or verbally predicting or explaining
a character’s actions, based on passive observation—do not easily map onto the behaviors
that typically occur throughout everyday social interactions. Furthermore, most laboratory
tasks include explicit prompts to make ToM inferences, for example, asking participants to
reason about a fictional character’s motivations in a social vignette (e.g., Happé, 1994). Such
prompts rarely occur in everyday interactions; thus, performance on these standard tasks
may not correspond with the tendency to represent the mental states of one’s real-world
interaction partners.
Reliance on explicit, non-interactive measures is especially problematic when it comes to
characterizing ToM abilities in autism. Autistic individuals who pass first-order false belief
tasks nevertheless struggle with everyday mindreading (Frith, 1994; Peterson et al., 2009),
a finding that has prompted the development of several more advanced ToM measures such
as the Strange Stories (Happé, 1994) and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-
Cohen et al., 2001). Yet, even performance on advanced ToM tasks is not consistently
related to social impairment in autism (Alkire et al., 2021; Barendse et al., 2018; Usher
et al., 2015), suggesting that autistic individuals may solve explicit ToM tasks through
compensatory strategies (i.e., verbal or domain-general reasoning) that do not serve them
in naturalistic, interactive contexts (Happé, 1995; Scheeren et al., 2013). Furthermore, the
unstructured nature of everyday social situations (as opposed to highly structured laboratory
tasks) appears to exacerbate ToM difficulties in autism (Ponnet et al., 2008; Roeyers et
al., 2001). Thus, to more accurately characterize real-world ToM and its role in interactive
success, particularly for autistic individuals, there is a need for more direct measures of the
application of ToM within unstructured social interactions.
Naturalistic conversation is an ideal unstructured format in which to measure applied ToM.
Conversation becomes an increasingly prominent feature of peer interactions during middle
childhood to adolescence, an important period for social development in general (Raffaelli
& Duckett, 1989; Rubin et al., 2007). Crucially, successfully navigating a conversation
inherently requires ToM (Roth & Spekman, 1984; Sperber & Wilson, 1987). As such,
the application of ToM within conversation, hereafter termed
conversational ToM
(cToM),
represents a key aspect of interactive social cognition and, potentially, its impairment in
autism.
cToM is apparent in a variety of communicative acts. While some of these acts are overt
indications of ToM, such as explicitly referring to a conversation partner’s mental states,
other speech acts
imply
that the speaker has or has not taken the listener’s perspective into
account. Failing to engage in this perspective-taking can result in statements that are under-
Alkire et al. Page 2
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
or over-informative, irrelevant, ambiguous, or disorganized (Grice, 1975), at least from a
neurotypical perspective. Conversely, speakers engage in cToM when they clarify potential
ambiguities, distinguish between common and privileged ground (i.e., information that is
or is not mutually shared; Clark & Marshall, 1981; Raczaszek-Leonardi et al., 2014), and
repair misunderstandings (Schegloff, 2004). These properties of everyday conversation offer
a promising avenue for investigating how ToM supports social interaction. However, as we
review below, there is a dearth of measures specifically designed to capture cToM.
Behaviors pertaining to cToM are primarily assessed within the discipline of pragmatic
language, which concerns the use of language in social contexts and the ability to infer
meanings that depart from the literal meaning of an utterance. Pragmatic impairment has
been associated with poor social outcomes (Mok et al., 2014; Whitehouse et al., 2009)
and is common in autism (Kalandadze et al., 2018; Ying Sng et al., 2018). However, two
characteristics of the pragmatics literature limit its applicability to studying ToM in real-time
social interactions. First, the broad category of pragmatics includes a range of heterogeneous
skills that vary in the extent to which they depend on ToM. Several authors have proposed a
theoretical division between aspects that require taking the perspective of one’s conversation
partner and those that do not, instead depending solely on structural language (i.e.,
vocabulary and syntax) and contextual reasoning from the listener’s egocentric perspective
(Andrés-Roqueta & Katsos, 2017; Kissine, 2012; O’Neill, 2012). However, many existing
measures of pragmatic ability conflate these heterogeneous aspects (Matthews et al., 2018).
A second limitation of previous pragmatics studies is that, much like ToM studies, they often
employ non-interactive measures. Because these measures may simply reflect knowledge of
social norms (e.g., Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999), and because pragmatic language use is highly
context dependent (Ying Sng et al., 2018), standard laboratory tasks may not adequately
capture one’s true capacity to use ToM within everyday conversations.
Given the limited ecological validity of task-based measures of pragmatic competence,
observational measures of natural language samples are better suited to identifying behaviors
that reflect real-world ToM. For example, the Pragmatic Rating Scale (PRS; Landa et
al., 1992) has been used to characterize pragmatic difficulties in autism that are observed
during interactions with a clinician or experimenter (Greenslade et al., 2019; Lam & Yeung,
2012; Paul et al., 2009) and during free-play among preschoolers (Bauminger-Zviely et
al., 2013). However, because the PRS and similar rating scales are designed to quantify
only pragmatic deficits, they are ill-suited to measuring positive indicators of cToM such
as clarifying ambiguities or explicit perspective-taking. Other observational studies have
quantified discrete ToM-relevant features of conversation, such as the number of relevant
and appropriately informative statements (Capps et al., 1998; Nadig et al., 2010; Tager-
Flusberg & Anderson, 1991). Though focused on limited aspects of discourse, these studies
establish the feasibility of analyzing naturalistic conversation for behavioral indicators of
cToM.
We argued above that non-interactive ToM tasks are limited in their capacity to
measure individuals’ ability and propensity to apply ToM during real-world interactions.
Nevertheless, understanding how performance on established tasks relates to cToM-related
pragmatic abilities could shed light on the cognitive underpinnings of these abilities.
Alkire et al. Page 3
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
ToM is multifaceted, and different non-interactive tasks likely tap into several distinct
underlying abilities (Altschuler et al., 2018; Hayward & Homer, 2017; Schaafsma et al.,
2015; Warnell & Redcay, 2019). As measuring every facet of ToM is beyond the scope of
a single study, here we focus on two aspects that are likely instrumental to conversation.
Face-to-face interactions in particular require sensitivity to multimodal cues from one’s
interaction partner, including facial expressions conveying affective information relevant
to the conversation (e.g., interest or confusion). Tasks measuring
visual-affective ToM
(vaToM), the ability to infer emotional states from facial expressions, are widespread
in the general ToM literature and show positive correlations with social functioning in
autism (Trevisan & Birmingham, 2016), yet their connection with conversational skills is
underexplored. vaToM tasks provide participants with a set of emotion words from which
to select the most appropriate label for each facial expression. However, as mentioned
above, real-world social interaction lacks such explicit prompts to reason about mental
states. In contrast,
spontaneous ToM
(sToM), the propensity to attribute mental states in
the absence of explicit prompts1, is commonly measured using social animation tasks in
which participants are asked to describe scenes depicting interactions between geometric
shapes; responses are then coded for the degree of spontaneous mental state attribution
(Abell et al., 2000; Castelli et al., 2002; Klin, 2000). sToM may be another key mechanism
for cToM, allowing one to actively make inferences or predictions about a conversation
partner’s mental states to guide and adjust one’s own communication.
The present study aims to demonstrate the instrumental role of ToM in social interaction
through observational coding of unstructured, face-to-face conversations among autistic and
typically developing (TD) children and adolescents. Guided by theoretical arguments (e.g.,
the “double empathy problem”; Milton, 2012) and empirical work suggesting that dyadic
interactions differ depending on the characteristics of each member, particularly autism
status (Crompton et al., 2020; Morrison et al., 2019), our study includes dyads that were
either matched (both TD or both autistic) or mismatched (one TD, one autistic).
Within this ecologically valid context, we established a novel coding framework for cToM
that builds on prior work by focusing on ToM-relevant behaviors rather than general
pragmatic competence, and by considering both positive and negative indicators of cToM
rather than a deficit-only approach (Clark & Adams, 2020; McCrimmon & Montgomery,
2014). After examining differences between autistic and TD participants on cToM, we
examined cToM in relation to the two aforementioned facets of ToM (measured with
non-interactive tasks) that are especially relevant to conversation: vaToM and sToM. We
hypothesized that both vaToM and sToM would show positive associations with the
tendency to apply ToM in conversation (cToM Positive) and negative associations with ToM-
related violations of conversational norms (cToM Negative) (Hypothesis 1). Finally, in line
with the widespread assumption that ToM plays an instrumental role in social interactions,
1This distinction between explicit and spontaneous
tasks
should not be confused with the distinction between explicit and implicit
cognitive processes
that is common in the ToM literature (e.g., Frith & Frith, 2008). Throughout this paper, we highlight measures
(sToM and cToM) that are spontaneous in that they do not explicitly prompt participants to engage in ToM; we make no claim about
whether the cognitive processes participants may spontaneously engage are explicit or implicit in the sense of the two-systems account
of ToM (Apperly & Butterfill, 2009).
Alkire et al. Page 4
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
we hypothesized that cToM Positive would positively predict interaction success, as rated by
interaction partners, and cToM Negative would negatively predict success (Hypothesis 2).
Methods
Participants
The present study was part of a larger ongoing study approved by the University
of Maryland’s Institutional Review Board (approval number 733144). Participants were
recruited largely based on participation in previous studies in our lab. Additional autistic
participants were recruited through the Interactive Autism Network, Simons Foundation
Powering Autism Research (SPARK), Facebook advertisements, and flyering at local events.
We appreciate obtaining access to recruit participants through SPARK research match
on SFARI Base. Additional TD participants were recruited through the University of
Maryland’s Infant and Child Studies database. Autism diagnoses were confirmed using
the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al.,
2012) administered by a research-reliable clinician. All participants were native English
speakers with a full-scale IQ of at least 80 on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, 2nd
edition (KBIT-2; Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004). TD participants had no diagnosis of any
neurological or psychiatric disorders, or first-degree relatives with autism or schizophrenia.
Additional diagnoses of the autistic participants are reported in the Supplementary
Materials.
The sample size was based on a power analysis using the Actor–Partner Interdependence
Model (APIM) Power Analysis Shiny Application (Ackerman et al., n.d.), which indicated
that a sample of 50 dyads provides 80% power to detect effects with standardized beta
weights of at least 0.3. See Supplementary Materials for additional details.
Dyad members were matched on gender and age (within 1 year or grade level) and
were arranged into three dyad types: AUT-TD (one autistic, one TD participant), TD-TD,
and AUT-AUT. The final sample of 50 dyads included 25 TD-TD (8 female dyads), 18
AUT-TD (4 female dyads), and 7 AUT-AUT (1 female dyad)2; across dyad types, the
sample comprises 68 (20 female) TD and 32 (6 female) AUT individuals, for a total of
100 individuals. Each participant was included in only one dyad. The average age across
the whole sample was 13.34 years, with a range of 8.72–16.91 and a standard deviation
of 1.85 years. Participants’ gender was assessed via parent report: parents were asked to
select between the options of “male” or “female” to indicate their child’s gender. Race
and ethnicity distributions for the whole sample are as follows: 2% Asian, 11% Black or
African American, 65% White, 11% more than one race, 11% not reported; 9% Hispanic or
Latino, 78% not Hispanic or Latino, 13% not reported. Additional participant demographics,
including indicators of socioeconomic status, are reported in Supplementary Table 1.
2We had intended to recruit a balanced number of dyads across the three types; however, data collection was halted indefinitely
by the COVID-19 pandemic. While we were sufficiently powered to test the hypotheses presented in this paper, the limited sample–
particularly of AUT-AUT dyads–precluded us from testing the effect of (or interactions with) dyad type as a main research question.
Alkire et al. Page 5
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Face-to-face Peer Interaction Task
The unstructured conversations took place within a peer interaction paradigm developed
by Usher et al. (2015, 2018). After separately providing informed written consent along
with their caregivers, participants were seated across from each other in a behavioral testing
room in which three video cameras recorded the interaction from multiple angles. The
experimenter told participants they would engage in simple activities together for about 20
minutes, then said, “Before I explain your task, why don’t you get to know each other? I’ll
be back in about five minutes.” This five-minute “Get to Know You” period was followed
by two semi-structured activities. Given that the present study focused on unstructured
conversation, only the Get to Know You portion was coded for cToM. Following these
activities, participants were placed in separate rooms, where they individually completed
questionnaires and additional behavioral tasks.
cToM Rating Scale
Conversations were rated along two scales, Positive and Negative, based on whether
behaviors reflected the presence (Positive) or absence (Negative) of ToM. Categories of
behaviors considered for the cToM rating are described in Supplementary Table 3; where
applicable, real examples from the observed sample are provided. Among other categories,
the Positive scale included explicit references to the partner’s mental state, whereas the
Negative scale included violations of neurotypical conversational norms (e.g., over- or
under-informative statements). The Positive and Negative scales were 6 points each (0 to 5),
with higher numbers representing higher frequencies of behaviors.
The Get to Know You task was coded for cToM by a team of three trained research
assistants supervised by the first author. Coders were masked to participant diagnosis except
for three participants who self-disclosed their autism diagnosis and one who self-disclosed
his lack of autism diagnosis during the conversation task. Furthermore, due to concern that
coders may be biased by participants’ physical appearance or mannerisms such that they
could speculate on diagnosis or form a negative impression (Sasson et al., 2017), coding
was based on extracted audio files rather than video. Prior to coding, conversations were
transcribed from the videos using the Computerized Language Analysis (CLAN) program
(MacWhinney, 2000). Transcripts assisted coding by indicating meaningful nonverbal
gestures (e.g., nodding the head yes or no, shaking hands, etc.), which provided additional
information by which raters could interpret the conversation, and by highlighting mental
state words, which were taken into consideration for the explicit ToM category. All
transcripts were checked for accuracy by a second research assistant or the first author.
Additional details on the coding system, training and coding procedures, and interrater
reliability are reported in the Supplementary Materials. Reliability coefficients for the final
dataset were as follows: Positive scale, Krippendorff’s alpha = 0.81; Negative scale, Finn’s
r
= 0.943. Coefficients of 0.7 or above are considered acceptable for both Krippendorff’s
alpha and Finn’s
r
(Hayes & Krippendorff, 2007; Heyman et al., 2014).
3Finn’s
r
was used to accommodate the skewed distribution of the Negative scale; see Supplementary Materials.
Alkire et al. Page 6
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Control variables
For both hypotheses, analyses included the following variables (in italics) as covariates of no
interest.
Verbal IQ
, consistently linked to performance on ToM tasks (Ronald et al., 2006;
Scheeren et al., 2013), was measured via the KBIT-2 Verbal Intelligence score.
Executive
functioning
(EF), also known to correlate with ToM (Jones et al., 2018; Lecce et al., 2017)
and pragmatic abilities (Matthews et al., 2018), was measured via the Global Executive
Composite on the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, 2nd ed. (Gioia et al.,
2015). Higher scores on this measure correspond to greater impairment.
Age, gender
, and
autism diagnosis
are also associated with variability in ToM skills (Bal et al., 2013; Kirkland
et al., 2013). Finally, given that cToM scores were derived from speech, we controlled for
the amount of speech produced by each individual to ensure that any effects of interest
were specific to cToM and not simply driven by how much an individual talked.
Language
productivity
was operationalized as the total number of words per minute (Scott & Windsor,
2000) spoken by each child during the 5-minute interaction, as calculated by CLAN. For
Hypothesis 1 only, this total was averaged together with the average word count of responses
to the sToM task (described below) to create a language productivity composite.
Hypothesis 1: Non-interactive ToM predicts cToM
After the peer interaction, participants individually completed two non-interactive ToM
tasks: vaToM and sToM.
vaToM was assessed using the Face task from the Cambridge Mindreading Face-Voice
Battery for Children (Golan et al., 2015). Children viewed videos of actors expressing
complex emotions (emotions involving cognitive states) and chose the appropriate label
from among four choices. The task consisted of 27 items representing nine emotional
concepts: unfriendly, disappointed, embarrassed, jealous, loving, nervous, bothered, amused,
and undecided. The measure of interest was accuracy (% correct). See Supplementary
Materials for details on stimulus presentation and task administration.
sToM was assessed using the Frith-Happé Triangles task (Abell et al., 2000). Children
viewed four short animations in which two triangles move in a manner that suggests a
ToM-related interaction. After viewing each video, children were asked to describe the
cartoon while their verbal responses were audio-recorded. Responses were coded for the
frequency of unique and appropriate internal state attributions (mental states, intentions, and
emotions) following guidelines adapted from Rice and Redcay (2015); see Supplementary
Materials. Responses were independently coded by two trained research assistants, with
24% of cases double-coded and discussed with the coding supervisor (a third research
assistant) during weekly meetings until consensus was reached. Interrater reliability was
excellent (Krippendorff’s alpha = 0.85). Scores for each of the three items (excluding a
practice item) were averaged for an overall sToM score.
Analysis strategy—Due to the dyadic data structure and thus the possibility of non-
independence among variables, actor-partner interdependence models (APIM; Kenny et al.,
2006) were used to estimate the effects of interest (vaToM and sToM) on cToM. The APIM
simultaneously estimates actor effects (e.g., the effect of Person 1’s vaToM on Person 1’s
Alkire et al. Page 7
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
cToM) and partner effects (e.g., the effect of Person 1’s vaToM on Person 2’s cToM); see
Figure 1. We ran separate models with each of the cToM scales as outcomes. Each model
included actor and partner effects of the non-interactive ToM variables, diagnosis (AUT or
TD), verbal IQ, EF, and the language productivity composite; and age (averaged between
dyad members) and gender as dyad-level covariates. The hypothesized effects were the
actor effects of each of the non-interactive ToM variables. Additionally, we ran reduced
versions of these models without partner effects of the individual-level predictors. Results
and interpretations for the reduced models were similar to the full models and are reported
in the Supplementary Materials, along with details on the modeling approach used for
each cToM scale. Because the Negative scale had a highly skewed distribution, leading to
violations of the assumptions of linear models, for this model we instead applied generalized
estimating equations, which make no distributional assumptions and are recommended for
estimating APIMs with non-Gaussian outcomes (Loeys et al., 2014; Loeys & Molenberghs,
2013). All APIM analyses (both hypotheses) were conducted using SPSS Version 26; the
data and syntax used for these analyses are available via the Open Science Framework.
Hypothesis 2: cToM predicts interaction success
Following the peer interaction, participants individually completed six items from the Social
Interaction Evaluation Measure (Berry et al., 1996) assessing interaction quality, that is,
how much the participant enjoyed the interaction and would like to interact again (see
Supplementary Materials for specific items). The summed total of responses to these items
from each participant’s partner served as the operational definition of interaction success.
We predicted that each cToM scale would have a significant effect on interaction success
(positive effect for Positive, negative effect for Negative).
Analysis strategy—For each of the cToM scales, an APIM was estimated using linear
mixed effects modeling in SPSS, with actor and partner cToM as the predictors and
interaction success as the outcome. Covariates included actor and partner effects of verbal
IQ, language productivity, and EF, as well as dyad-level age, gender, and dyad type (TD-TD,
AUT-TD, AUT-AUT). We included dyad type rather than individual diagnosis as a covariate
based on dyadic research suggesting that perceptions may depend on the match or mismatch
between partners’ diagnosis rather than an individual’s diagnosis alone (Morrison et al.,
2019).
We also conducted a planned follow-up analysis examining the effect of vaToM and sToM
on interaction success (see Supplementary Materials).
Community Involvement Statement
Community members were not involved in the design, implementation, or interpretation of
the study.
Alkire et al. Page 8
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Descriptive statistics, group comparisons, and zero-order correlations among all variables
are shown in Supplementary Tables 4–7. Notably, Positive and Negative scales were
uncorrelated (
r
= .00). Figure 2 depicts the distributions of and group differences in each
cToM scale. The Negative scale showed a marked group difference in the expected direction
based on previous literature, with autistic participants rated higher (meaning they displayed
more frequent negative cToM behaviors;
t
(35.14) = 3.25,
p
= 0.003). In contrast, TD and
autistic participants showed very similar distributions on the Positive scale (
t
(59.18) = 0.02,
p
= 0.98).
We also examined the frequency of each category of behavior within the cToM coding
system. Figure 3 depicts these within-category scores and their averages across the full
sample and for each group separately. The most frequent category on the Positive scale
was explicit ToM, followed by distinguishing common versus privileged ground. The other
Positive categories were relatively infrequent, with only a handful of participants scoring 1
or above. Categories on the Negative scale were more comparable in their frequency, with
most participants showing no or very few instances across categories (as reflected in the
skewed distribution of the overall Negative score).
Autistic and TD participants showed comparable rates across all Positive categories. On the
Negative scale, higher rates within each category were represented almost exclusively by
a minority of autistic participants. On average, autistic participants were more likely than
TD participants to violate maxims of quantity, relevance, and manner and to interrupt their
partners in a manner suggesting they were either not attending to their partner’s mental state
or were attending but struggled to integrate this information into their responses.
The distributions of the other key variables in Hypotheses 1 and 2 are plotted by diagnostic
group in Figure 4. Groups did not significantly differ on sToM (
t
(45.83) = −0.22,
p
= 0.83),
but accuracy on the vaToM task was significantly lower in the autistic group (
t
(42.21) =
−3.72,
p
< 0.001). Groups did not significantly differ on interaction quality (participant’s
own rating;
t
(50.23) = 0.63,
p
= 0.53) or interaction success (partner rating;
t
(55.51) = 0.56,
p
= 0.58). Interaction success did not significantly differ across dyad types (
F
(2,97) = 1.07,
p
= 0.35; AUT-TD vs. TD-TD:
t
(80.65) = −0.44,
p
= 0.66; AUT-TD vs. AUT-AUT:
t
(17.19)
= 1.17,
p
= 0.26; AUT-AUT vs. TD-TD:
t
(16.94) = 0.95,
p
= 0.36). The distributions of all
additional variables are plotted by group in Supplementary Figure 1.
Hypothesis 1 Results
For cToM Negative, there was a significant negative actor effect4 of vaToM (
B
= −0.24,
SE
= 0.09,
p
= 0.01) and a significant negative actor effect of sToM (
B
= −0.30,
SE
= 0.13,
p
= 0.02), supporting our hypothesis that non-interactive ToM predicts cToM (Figure 5).
However, this hypothesis was not supported for cToM Positive, for which there were no
4That is, there was a negative effect of an individual’s own vaToM on their own cToM Negative score; see Figure 1.
Alkire et al. Page 9
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
significant actor effects of either vaToM or sToM. Unstandardized regression coefficients for
all effects are reported in Table 1.
Hypothesis 2 Results
To test the hypothesis that cToM predicts interaction success, we estimated linear mixed
effects models with each of the cToM scales as predictors. Contrary to our hypothesis,
there were no significant effects of either cToM scale predicting interaction success
(Supplementary Table 11).
Discussion
This study introduced the cToM coding system, a novel approach to measuring the use
of ToM in social interaction that capitalizes on properties of naturalistic conversation
that inherently involve ToM. We found partial support for our hypotheses in that cToM
(Negative) was correlated with non-interactive ToM; however, cToM was not related
to interactive success. We also demonstrated dissociations between cToM Positive and
Negative scales, and this divergence is relevant to our understanding of cToM in autism.
Divergence between cToM Positive and Negative scales
The Positive and Negative cToM scales—reflecting the presence or absence, respectively, of
ToM-related behaviors—were decidedly uncorrelated (
r
= 0.00). Given that they appear to
capture orthogonal dimensions, it is not surprising that we found divergent results for the
two scales in Hypothesis 1, as well as in the between-group comparison.
Comparing TD and autistic participants on cToM revealed a few noteworthy patterns,
especially considering the prominent mindblindness theory of autism. As expected, at the
group level the autistic participants were rated significantly higher than TD participants on
the Negative scale (reflecting more frequent negative cToM behaviors). Thus, the Negative
scale seems to capture ToM-relevant pragmatic difficulties that have been previously
documented in the autism literature, particularly violations of Gricean maxims (Landa,
2000; Paul et al., 2009; Ying Sng et al., 2018). At the same time, autistic and TD participants
showed highly similar distributions on the Positive scale, and this was also true at the
category level (Figure 3). Consistent with previous findings (reviewed in Bang et al.,
2013), autistic participants used mental state language (“explicit ToM” in our categorization)
to the same extent as TD participants. Furthermore, while explicit ToM was the most
frequent category within the Positive scale, the autistic participants’ scores do not solely
reflect superficial knowledge of mental state concepts; they were also comparable to TD
participants on the second-most frequent category of distinguishing between common
and privileged ground. Behaviors coded in this category include preemptively sharing
information that would plausibly be unknown to one’s partner, or asking whether one’s
partner shares certain knowledge, both of which reflect an active awareness that others
can possess different knowledge states from oneself. Thus, our findings converge with
other evidence that autistic individuals’ ToM abilities are often underestimated (Heasman &
Gillespie, 2018), and that they are aware of common ground and modify their discourse in
response (De Marchena & Eigsti, 2016). Together with the lack of correlation between the
Alkire et al. Page 10
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Positive and Negative scales, this pattern of between-group difference and similarity implies
even those autistic individuals who violate certain neurotypical conversational norms may be
comparable to their TD peers on other aspects of cToM. As such, the abilities captured by
the cToM Positive scale could be recognized as an area of strength that could potentially be
leveraged in social skills interventions (McCrimmon & Montgomery, 2014).
The discrepancy between the Positive and Negative scales also raises the question of how
they may differ in their cognitive demands. Focusing on the dominant category of explicit
ToM, simply referencing a partner’s mental state does not necessarily require switching
between two perspectives (self vs. other) beyond the basic recognition that others have their
own mental states. Thus, it is possible that the behaviors captured by the Positive scale do
not reflect the degree of perspective-taking necessary for scoring low on the Negative scale.
cToM Negative relates to non-interactive ToM
Non-interactive ToM—specifically, visual-affective (vaToM) and spontaneous (sToM)—was
negatively associated with cToM Negative but not Positive. The association between vaToM
and cToM Negative suggests that children who struggle to identify complex emotions
based on facial expressions also tend to violate neurotypical conversational norms, such
as providing too little, too much, or irrelevant information, or expressing themselves
in a confusing manner. It is easy to imagine how this association might play out in
a conversation. For example, a child who does not pick up on her partner’s confused
expression would be unaware that her utterances come across as irrelevant or unclear (in
the absence of verbal feedback to this effect), and thus would not adjust her behavior. In
contrast, explicitly referencing a partner’s mental state could reflect a general (rather than
partner-specific) understanding of diverse mental states, as argued above, and thus might not
necessitate “checking in” with a partner to dynamically integrate feedback from their facial
expressions—hence the lack of association between vaToM and cToM Positive.
The negative association between sToM and cToM Negative is consistent with the
notion that successfully navigating a conversation, and social interaction more broadly,
involves spontaneous mental state attribution. Specifically, participants who described social
animations in more mentalistic terms were rated lower on cToM Negative, suggesting
that these children spontaneously attributed mental states to their partners during the
conversation, and that this allowed them to follow neurotypical conversational norms. The
lack of association between sToM and cToM Positive is somewhat surprising given that our
sToM measure was the frequency of mental state attributions within a verbal description, and
cToM Positive scores were largely driven by explicit mental state language. However, it is
important to consider that while the sToM task did not explicitly prompt children to attribute
mental states, it did require them to give verbal descriptions of the scenes. In contrast, most
mental state attributions made during a naturalistic conversation are almost certainly not
vocalized. Whereas cToM Negative may be related to the general tendency to make mental
state attributions (which may be unvocalized unless prompted), cToM Positive, specifically
the explicit ToM category, indexes the subset of mental state attributions that happen to be
vocalized during the conversation.
Alkire et al. Page 11
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Altogether, these results add to the relatively sparse literature linking diverse aspects of ToM
with conversational skills.
cToM does not predict partner-reported interaction success
Contrary to our hypothesis, neither cToM scale was a significant predictor of interaction
success, as measured by partner ratings of interaction quality. Two aspects of our
methodology must be considered when interpreting these null findings.
First, this study focused on the specific context of a single interaction with an unfamiliar
peer. This setup was meant to represent the type of real-world scenario that, if successful,
would lead to further interactions and eventually the formation of a long-term relationship.
Autistic individuals often find interacting with unfamiliar peers particularly challenging,
as they struggle to apply their social knowledge (including skills learned through explicit
training) to novel situations (Bauminger-Zviely et al., 2013; Usher et al., 2015). As
such, we expected our paradigm to be more effective at eliciting cToM difficulties than
would interacting with familiar individuals. However, it is possible that cToM plays a
more important role in the perceived quality of interactions between familiar compared
to unfamiliar partners. That is, as two people gain more experience with each other, one
might expect the other to make more accurate mental state attributions about oneself, and
thus failure to do so would be more striking. More generally, while the present study
suggests that cToM is not a significant factor in how individuals judge the quality of a single
interaction, the full impact of ToM on relationship quality likely plays out over repeated
interactions. Future studies could explore this possibility by following dyads longitudinally
and tracking their interaction patterns, including cToM, and relationship outcomes over time.
Second, our choice of partner self-report as the measure of interaction success may have
limited our ability to detect a true effect of cToM, as social desirability bias may have led
some children to underreport any dissatisfaction with the interaction. We do not believe
this was a major concern, as there is a decent spread of scores on this measure (Figure 4).
Furthermore, in a post hoc exploration of a subset of participants’ future partner preferences
(i.e., whether they would prefer the same or a different partner if they were to participate
in another session; see Supplementary Materials), which they completed at the end of
the session while the experimenter was out of the room, the extent to which participants
preferred the same partner correlated positively with their earlier report of interaction quality
(
r
= 0.42,
p
< 0.001). This suggests that our measure of interaction success is a reasonable
proxy for the participants’ motivation to interact with their partners again, a prerequisite for
long-term relationship formation. Nevertheless, future studies could incorporate alternative
measures of peer acceptance, such as classroom sociometric ratings, that may be stronger
indicators of real-world interaction success.
General limitations and future directions
A few additional caveats apply to our discussion of the overall study. First, while our sample
of 50 dyads allowed sufficient power to test our hypotheses, a larger sample would likely
produce more robust and reproducible results. Furthermore, the relatively low proportion
of autistic participants (32% of the total sample) likely limited the variability we observed
Alkire et al. Page 12
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
in the cToM Negative scale, on which only a handful of autistic participants received
high ratings. This in turn may have limited our ability to detect the predicted effects.
The generalizability of our findings to the wider autistic population is also limited, as we
excluded individuals with IQs below 80 or who are minimally verbal, and our autistic
sample was 84% White. It is also possible that our findings would not generalize to age
groups outside of the middle childhood to adolescent range. Additionally, while there is
growing recognition that autistic females often differ from their male counterparts in their
experience and behavioral presentation of autism, including social behaviors (Dean et al.,
2017; Mandy & Lai, 2017; Wood-Downie et al., 2021), our sample was predominantly
male. Further research with larger and more balanced samples should assess whether gender
moderates the relations under study, and whether gender interacts with autism diagnosis
to predict cToM or interaction success. We also recognize the limitation that we assessed
gender via parent- and not self-report and provided only binary options. In line with growing
recognition of a broader spectrum of gender identities, particularly among the autistic
population (Corbett et al., 2022; Strang et al., 2020), future studies should include nonbinary
and other options for gender and pose this question to the participants themselves.
Future studies should also further explore AUT-AUT interactions, which constituted only a
small portion of our sample. Recent work has suggested that AUT-AUT interactions may
qualitatively differ from TD-TD or TD-AUT interactions, including features relevant to
cToM such as a generous assumption of common ground (Heasman & Gillespie, 2019),
and in ways that impact interaction success (Granieri et al., 2020; Morrison et al., 2019).
Such findings support the idea of the double empathy problem (Milton, 2012), in which
social interaction difficulties are attributed not solely to the autistic individual, but to a
breakdown in communication resulting from the different experiences and expectations of
autistic and neurotypical people (Crompton et al., 2021). Thus, rather than being a static
trait of an individual, cToM may be intrinsically tied to the dyadic context created between
an individual and a particular partner, and an important aspect of this context may be the
match or mismatch in autism status. As such, future studies of cToM should be sufficiently
powered to investigate the effect of dyad type (i.e., match or mismatch) on both cToM and
interaction success.
Other limitations relate to the cToM measure itself. Because coding was based on the
audio and transcripts of the conversations, we were unable to capture potentially relevant
features like facial expressions and eye gaze; future iterations of the cToM system could
be expanded to include the visual modality. Furthermore, any measure based on third-party
ratings has the limitation of not directly accessing an individual’s subjective experience.
We encourage future studies using the cToM coding system to collect self-report measures
immediately following the conversation to provide complementary information about how
people perceive their own use of ToM in conversation. Ratings about an individual’s cToM
from their conversation partner would be similarly valuable and may be more likely to
uncover associations between cToM and partner-rated interaction success.
Further research is also needed to validate and refine the cToM rating scales. Beyond
the above-discussed divergence between the Positive and Negative scales, there may be
heterogeneity within each scale in terms of the cognitive processes they reflect. Future
Alkire et al. Page 13
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
research using a larger sample, and perhaps a slightly more structured interactive task
that elicits higher rates of the categories that were infrequent in the present study (e.g.,
misunderstandings, explicit perspective-taking, and irony comprehension), could employ
factor analysis to characterize the pattern of correlations among the categories. Uncovering
the latent dimensional structure of cToM could enable more precise assessments of its
relations with non-interactive ToM, interaction success, and other constructs.
Finally, our cToM coding system was developed without input from autistic perspectives
and thus is biased to reflect the norms of the dominant neurotypical culture; these norms
may not be as valued by autistic individuals. Future studies could involve autistic individuals
in adapting the cToM system to better capture how autistic individuals consider (or do not
consider) each other’s perspectives while interacting, and how important this is to their
perceptions of interaction quality.
Conclusion
We introduced the cToM coding system, addressing the need for interactive, ecologically
valid measures of ToM. While we did not find evidence that this novel measure improves on
standard ToM measures in predicting interaction success, the cToM is a useful framework
for characterizing the various ways in which ToM- related difficulties show up in naturalistic
conversations between TD and autistic individuals. Crucially, the divergence between the
Positive and Negative scales reveals the multidimensionality of ToM in conversation. This
finding is valuable in two respects. First, it adds to mounting evidence of divergence
between distinct components of ToM in general (Schaafsma et al., 2015; Warnell & Redcay,
2019). Second, it refines our understanding of ToM and pragmatic difficulties among
autistic individuals, as even those who struggle with ToM-related violations of neurotypical
conversational norms can nevertheless display typical levels of other forms of mental state
representation, such as those indexed by the cToM Positive scale. Furthermore, our finding
that cToM Negative relates to two forms of non-interactive ToM can inform future studies
of the more basic processes that support the application of ToM in social interactions.
Altogether, the present study provides a springboard for further investigation into the
mechanisms and consequences of ToM-related behavior within naturalistic conversations.
Supplementary Material
Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
We thank the members of the cToM coding team: Ryan Regars, Aliceann Trostle, Daniel Friedman-Brown,
and Ming Yuan; Katie Beverley, Hema Clarence, Diana Grant, Sarah Gray, Alex Kalomiris, Dahye Kang, and
Manasvinee Mayil Vahanan for their assistance with transcription and coding; Jacqueline Thomas, Alexandra
Hickey, Tina Nguyen, Micah Plotkin, Kathryn Bouvier-Weinberg, Matthew Kiely, Ryan Stadler, Aranje
Sripanjalingam, Dominic Smith-DiLeo, Avi Warshawsky, Maddie Reiter, Bess Bloomer, Miranda Sapoznik, Nicole
Chapman, and Aiste Cechaviciute for their assistance with data collection; Dr. Lauren Usher for conceiving of
the Get to Know You task; and Drs. Peter Carruthers, Jude Cassidy, and Edward Lemay for their advice on study
design, methods, and data analysis. Finally, we thank the participants and their families for making this research
possible.
Alkire et al. Page 14
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Funding
Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National
Institutes of Health under Award number R01MH107441. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and
does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.
References
Abell F, Happé F, & Frith U (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to
animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 1–16.
10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00014-9
Ackerman RA, Ledermann T, & Kenny DA (n.d.). Power analysis for the Actor-
Partner Interdependence Model. Unpublished manuscript. https://robert-ackerman.shinyapps.io/
APIMPowerR/
Alkire D, Warnell KR, Kirby LA, Moraczewski D, & Redcay E (2021). Explaining Variance in Social
Symptoms of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 51(4), 1249–1265. 10.1007/s10803-020-04598-x [PubMed: 32676827]
Altschuler M, Sideridis G, Kala S, Warshawsky M, Gilbert R, Carroll D, … Faja S (2018). Measuring
Individual Differences in Cognitive, Affective, and Spontaneous Theory of Mind Among School-
Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
48(11), 3945–3957. 10.1007/s10803-018-3663-1 [PubMed: 29971661]
Andrés-Roqueta C, & Katsos N (2017). The Contribution of Grammar, Vocabulary and Theory of
Mind in Pragmatic Language Competence in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Frontiers
in Psychology, 8(JUN), 996. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00996 [PubMed: 28663734]
Apperly IA, & Butterfill SA (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-
like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/
2009-18254-013 [PubMed: 19839692]
Bal E, Yerys BE, Sokoloff JL, Celano MJ, Kenworthy L, Giedd JN, & Wallace GL (2013). Do
social attribution skills improve with age in children with high functioning autism spectrum
disorders? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(1), 9–16. 10.1016/j.rasd.2012.07.004
[PubMed: 23130085]
Bang J, Burns J, & Nadig A (2013). Brief report: Conveying subjective experience in
conversation: Production of mental state terms and personal narratives in individuals with high
functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 1732–1740. 10.1007/
s10803-012-1716-4 [PubMed: 23179342]
Barendse EM, Hendriks MPH, Thoonen G, Aldenkamp AP, & Kessels RPC (2018). Social behaviour
and social cognition in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD):
two sides of the same coin? Cognitive Processing, 19(4), 545–555. 10.1007/s10339-018-0866-5
[PubMed: 29959562]
Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, & Frith U (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?
Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 [PubMed: 2934210]
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Spong A, Scahill V, & Lawson J (2001). Are intuitive physics
and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of
Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5(1), 47–78.
Bauminger-Zviely N, Karin E, Kimhi Y, & Agam-Ben-Artzi G (2013). Spontaneous peer conversation
in preschoolers with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder versus typical development. Wiley
Online Library, 55(4), 363–373. 10.1111/jcpp.12158
Begeer S, Malle BF, Nieuwland MS, & Keysar B (2010). Using theory of mind to represent
and take part in social interactions: Comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and
typically developing controls. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 104–122.
10.1080/17405620903024263
Berry DS, Hansen JS, & Berry S (1996). Positive Affect, Negative Affect, and Social Interaction.
In Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 71, Issue 4). https://psycnet.apa.org/record/
1996-06401-014
Alkire et al. Page 15
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Bosacki S, & Astington JW (1999). Theory of Mind in Preadolescence: Relations Between
Social Understanding and Social Competence. Social Development, 8(2), 237–255.
10.1111/1467-9507.00093
Capps L, Kehres J, & Sigman M (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and
children with developmental delays. Autism. 10.1177/1362361398024002
Caputi M, Pantaleo G, & Scaini S (2017). Do Feelings of Loneliness Mediate the
Relationship between Sociocognitive Understanding and Depressive Symptoms During Late
Childhood and Early Adolescence? Journal of Genetic Psychology, 178(4), 207–216.
10.1080/00221325.2017.1317629 [PubMed: 28682188]
Carrow-Woolfolk E (1999). CASL: Comprehensive assessment of spoken language.
Castelli F, Frith C, Happé F, & Frith U (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms
for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125(8), 1839–1849. 10.1093/brain/
awf189 [PubMed: 12135974]
Clark HH, & Marshall CR (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. Elements of Discourse
Understanding, 10–63.
Clark M, & Adams D (2020). The self-identified positive attributes and favourite activities of
children on the autism spectrum. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 72, 101512. 10.1016/
J.RASD.2020.101512
Corbett BA, Muscatello RA, Klemencic ME, West M, Kim A, & Strang JF (2022). Greater gender
diversity among autistic children by self-report and parent-report. Autism, 136236132210853.
10.1177/13623613221085337
Crompton CJ, DeBrabander K, Heasman B, Milton D, & Sasson NJ (2021). Double Empathy:
Why Autistic People Are Often Misunderstood. Frontiers for Young Minds, 9. 10.3389/
FRYM.2021.554875
Crompton CJ, Ropar D, Evans-Williams CVM, Flynn EG, & Fletcher-Watson S (2020). Autistic
peer-to-peer information transfer is highly effective. Autism. 10.1177/1362361320919286
De Marchena A, & Eigsti IM (2016). The art of common ground: emergence of a complex pragmatic
language skill in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language, 43(1),
43–80. 10.1017/S0305000915000070 [PubMed: 25708810]
Dean M, Harwood R, & Kasari C (2017). The art of camouflage: Gender differences in the
social behaviors of girls and boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(6), 678–689.
10.1177/1362361316671845 [PubMed: 27899709]
Devine RT, & Hughes C (2013). Silent Films and Strange Stories: Theory of Mind, Gender, and Social
Experiences in Middle Childhood. Child Development, 84(3), 989–1003. 10.1111/cdev.12017
[PubMed: 23199139]
Frith U (1994). Autism and theory of mind in everyday life. Social Development, 3(2), 108–124.
10.1111/j.1467-9507.1994.tb00031.x
Frith CD, & Frith U (2008). Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Cognition. Neuron, 60, 503–510.
10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.032 [PubMed: 18995826]
Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, & Kenworthy L (2015). BRIEF-2: Behavior Rating Inventory of
Executive Function. Psychological Assessment Resources.
Golan O, Sinai-Gavrilov Y, & Baron-Cohen S (2015). The Cambridge Mindreading Face-Voice
Battery for Children (CAM-C): complex emotion recognition in children with and without autism
spectrum conditions. Molecular Autism, 6, 22. 10.1186/s13229-015-0018-z [PubMed: 25932320]
Granieri JE, McNair ML, Gerber AH, Reifler RF, & Lerner MD (2020). Atypical social
communication is associated with positive initial impressions among peers with autism spectrum
disorder. Autism, 24(7), 1841–1848. 10.1177/1362361320924906 [PubMed: 32498545]
Greenslade KJ, Utter EA, & Landa RJ (2019). Predictors of Pragmatic Communication in School-Age
Siblings of Children with ASD and Low-Risk Controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 49(4), 1352–1365. 10.1007/s10803-018-3837-x [PubMed: 30488153]
Grice HP (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole P & Morgan J (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics: Speech
Acts (pp. 41–58). Academic Press.
Alkire et al. Page 16
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Happé FGE (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts
and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154. 10.1007/BF02172093 [PubMed: 8040158]
Happé FGE (1995). The Role of Age and Verbal Ability in the Theory of Mind Task Performance of
Subjects with Autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855. 10.2307/1131954 [PubMed: 7789204]
Hayes AF, & Krippendorff K (2007). Answering the Call for a Standard Reliability Measure for
Coding Data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1(1), 77–89. 10.1080/19312450709336664
Hayward EO, & Homer BD (2017). Reliability and validity of advanced theory-of-mind measures
in middle childhood and adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 10.1111/
bjdp.12186
Heasman B, & Gillespie A (2018). Perspective-taking is two-sided: Misunderstandings between
people with Asperger’s syndrome and their family members. Autism, 22(6), 740–750.
10.1177/1362361317708287 [PubMed: 28683569]
Heasman B, & Gillespie A (2019). Neurodivergent intersubjectivity: Distinctive features of how
autistic people create shared understanding. Autism, 23(4), 910–921. 10.1177/1362361318785172
[PubMed: 30073872]
Hedley D, Uljarević M, Wilmot M, Richdale A, & Dissanayake C (2018). Understanding depression
and thoughts of self-harm in autism: A potential mechanism involving loneliness. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 46, 1–7. 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.11.003
Heyman R, Lorber M, Eddy J, & West T (2014). Behavioral observation and coding. In Reis HT
& Judd CM (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp.
345–372). Cambridge University Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2014-12227-014
Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, & Layton JB (2010). Social Relationships and Mortality Risk: A Meta-
analytic Review. PLOS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316 [PubMed:
20668659]
Jones CRG, Simonoff E, Baird G, Pickles A, Marsden AJS, Tregay J, … Charman T (2018). The
association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum
disorder. Autism Research, 11(1). 10.1002/aur.1873
Kalandadze T, Norbury C, Nærland T, & Næss KAB (2018). Figurative language comprehension
in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analytic review. Autism, 22(2), 99–117.
10.1177/1362361316668652 [PubMed: 27899711]
Kaufman AS, & Kaufman NL (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. Pearson, Inc.
10.1002/9780470373699.speced1189
Kenny D, Kashy D, & Cook W (2006). Dyadic Data Analysis. Guilford.
Kirkland RA, Peterson E, Baker C, & Pulos S (2013). Meta-analysis reveals adult female superiority in
“Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test.” Article in North American Journal of Psychology. Retrieved
from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/260712981
Kissine M (2012). Pragmatics, Cognitive Flexibility and Autism Spectrum Disorders. Mind &
Language, 27(1), 1–28. 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01433.x
Klin A (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism
and Asperger syndrome: The Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
and Allied Disciplines, 41(7), 831–846. 10.1111/1469-7610.00671 [PubMed: 11079426]
Lam YG, & Yeung SSS (2012). Towards a convergent account of pragmatic language deficits in
children with high-functioning autism: Depicting the phenotype using the Pragmatic Rating Scale.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 792–797. 10.1016/J.RASD.2011.08.004
Landa R (2000). Social language use in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism In: Klin F.
Volkmar, & Sparrow S.. Asperger syndrome.
Landa R, Piven J, Wzorek M, … J. G-P, & 1992, U. (1992). Social
language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychological Medicine, 22, 245–
254. https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/psychological-medicine/article/social-language-use-
in-parents-of-autistic-individuals/BB9E0F4712E0A6B585E510B628F97E47 [PubMed: 1574562]
Lecce S, Bianco F, Devine RT, & Hughes C (2017). Relations between theory of mind and executive
function in middle childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 163. 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.06.011
Alkire et al. Page 17
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Loeys T, Cook W, De Smet O, Wietzker A, & Buysse A (2014). The actor-partner interdependence
model for categorical dyadic data: A user-friendly guide to GEE. Personal Relationships, 21(2),
225–241. 10.1111/pere.12028
Loeys T, & Molenberghs G (2013). Modeling actor and partner effects in dyadic data when outcomes
are categorical. Psychological Methods, 18(2), 220–236. 10.1037/a0030640 [PubMed: 23458720]
Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, & Bishop S (2012). Autism diagnostic observation
schedule (2nd ed.). Western Psychological Corporation.
MacWhinney B (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. 3rd Edition. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Mandy W, & Lai MC (2017). Towards sex- and gender-informed autism research. In Autism (Vol. 21,
Issue 6, pp. 643–645). SAGE Publications Ltd. 10.1177/1362361317706904 [PubMed: 28749233]
Matthews D, Biney H, & Abbot-Smith K (2018). Individual Differences in Children’s
Pragmatic Ability: A Review of Associations with Formal Language, Social Cognition,
and Executive Functions. Language Learning and Development, 14(3), 186–223.
10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584
McCrimmon AW, & Montgomery JM (2014). Resilience-Based Perspectives for Autism Spectrum
Disorder. 375–396. 10.1007/978-1-4939-0542-3_17
Milton DEM (2012). On the ontological status of autism: The “double empathy problem.” Disability
and Society, 27(6), 883–887. 10.1080/09687599.2012.710008
Mok PLH, Pickles A, Durkin K, & Conti-Ramsden G (2014). Longitudinal trajectories of peer
relations in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 55(5), 516–527. 10.1111/jcpp.12190 [PubMed: 24410167]
Morrison KE, Debrabander KM, Jones DR, Faso DJ, Ackerman RA, & Sasson NJ (2019). Outcomes
of real-world social interaction for autistic adults paired with autistic compared to typically
developing partners. Autism. 10.1177/1362361319892701
Nadig A, Lee I, Singh L, Bosshart K, & Ozonoff S (2010). How does the topic of conversation affect
verbal exchange and eye gaze? A comparison between typical development and high-functioning
autism. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2730–2739. 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2010.05.020
[PubMed: 20493890]
O’Neill DK (2012). Components of pragmatic ability and children’s pragmatic language development.
In Schmid H (Ed.), Cognitive Pragmatics (pp. 261–287). Walter de Gruyter. https://
books.google.com/books?
hl=en&lr=&id=thBQGNSSQj0C&oi=fnd&pg=PA261&dq=%22Components+of+pragmatic+abilit
y+and+children’s+pragmatic+language+development%22&ots=6TZvVDNi_n&sig=SBxUMmnZ
vh0wGfvYHe3-9uXzRLc
Paul R, Orlovski SM, Marcinko HC, & Volkmar F (2009). Conversational Behaviors in Youth with
High-functioning ASD and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
39(1), 115–125. 10.1007/s10803-008-0607-1 [PubMed: 18607708]
Peterson CC, Garnett M, Kelly A, & Attwood T (2009). Everyday social and conversation
applications of theory-of-mind understanding by children with autism-spectrum disorders
or typical development. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 18(2), 105–115. 10.1007/
s00787-008-0711-y [PubMed: 18810310]
Peterson C, Slaughter V, Moore C, & Wellman HM (2016). Peer social skills and theory of mind
in children with autism, deafness, or typical development. Developmental Psychology, 52(1).
10.1037/a0039833
Ponnet K, Buysse A, Roeyers H, & De Clercq A (2008). Mind-reading in young adults with ASD:
Does structure matter? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 905–918. 10.1007/
s10803-007-0462-5 [PubMed: 17929156]
Raczaszek-Leonardi J, Debska A, & Sochanowicz A (2014). Pooling the ground: understanding
and coordination in collective sense making. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(OCT), 1233. 10.3389/
fpsyg.2014.01233 [PubMed: 25426087]
Raffaelli M, & Duckett E (1989). “We were just talking …”: Conversations in early adolescence.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18(6), 567–582. 10.1007/BF02139074 [PubMed: 24272126]
Alkire et al. Page 18
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Rice K, & Redcay E (2015). Spontaneous mentalizing captures variability in the cortical thickness of
social brain regions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(3), 327–334. 10.1093/scan/
nsu081 [PubMed: 24847726]
Roeyers H, Buysse A, Ponnet K, & Pichal B (2001). Advancing Advanced Mind-reading Tests:
Empathic Accuracy in Adults with a Pervasive Developmental Disorder. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 271–278. 10.1017/S0021963001006680 [PubMed: 11280423]
Ronald A, Viding E, Happé F, & Plomin R (2006). Individual differences in theory of mind
ability in middle childhood and links with verbal ability and autistic traits: a twin study. Social
Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 412–425. 10.1080/17470910601068088 [PubMed: 18633802]
Roth FP, & Spekman NJ (1984). Assessing the pragmtic abilities of children: Part 1. Organizational
framework and assessment parameters. In Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders (Vol. 49, Issue
1). 10.1044/jshd.4901.02
Rubin KH, Bukowski WM, & Parker JG (2007). Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. In
Damon W & Lerner RM (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. http://
doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0310
Sasson NJ, Faso DJ, Nugent J, Lovell S, Kennedy DP, & Grossman RB (2017). Neurotypical Peers
are Less Willing to Interact with Those with Autism based on Thin Slice Judgments. Scientific
Reports, 7(1), 1–10. 10.1038/srep40700 [PubMed: 28127051]
Schaafsma SM, Pfaff DW, Spunt RP, & Adolphs R (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing
theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 65–72. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007 [PubMed:
25496670]
Scheeren AM, De Rosnay M, Koot HM, & Begeer S (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high-
functioning autism spectrum disorder. Wiley Online Library, 54(6), 628–635. 10.1111/jcpp.12007
Schegloff EA (2004). Conversation analysis and socially shared cognition. In Perspectives on socially
shared cognition. (pp. 150–171). 10.1037/10096-007
Schilbach L, Timmermans B, Reddy V, Costall A, Bente G, Schlicht T, & Vogeley K (2013).
Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(04), 393–414. http://
journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0140525X12000660 [PubMed: 23883742]
Scott CM, & Windsor J (2000). General Language Performance Measures in Spoken and Written
Narrative and Expository Discourse of School-Age Children with Language Learning Disabilities.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 324–339. 10.1044/jslhr.4302.324
Sperber D, & Wilson D (1987). Précis of Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 10(04), 697. 10.1017/s0140525x00055345
Strang JF, van der Miesen AIR, Caplan R, Hughes C, daVanport S, & Lai MC (2020). Both sex- and
gender-related factors should be considered in autism research and clinical practice. Autism, 24(3),
539–543. 10.1177/1362361320913192 [PubMed: 32299242]
Tager-Flusberg H, & Anderson M (1991). The Development of Contingent Discourse Ability in
Autistic Children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1123–1134. 10.1111/
j.1469-7610.1991.tb00353.x [PubMed: 1838537]
Trevisan DA, & Birmingham E (2016). Are emotion recognition abilities related to everyday social
functioning in ASD? A meta-analysis. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 32, 24–42.
10.1016/j.rasd.2016.08.004
Usher LV, Burrows CA, Schwartz CB, & Henderson HA (2015). Social competence with an unfamiliar
peer in children and adolescents with high functioning autism: Measurement and individual
differences. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 17, 25–39. 10.1016/j.rasd.2015.05.005
[PubMed: 26161136]
Usher LV, Burrows CA, Messinger DS, & Henderson HA (2018). Metaperception in Adolescents With
and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(2),
533–548. 10.1007/s10803-017-3356-1 [PubMed: 29080926]
Warnell KR, & Redcay E (2019). Minimal coherence among varied theory of mind measures in
childhood and adulthood. Cognition, 191, 103997. 10.1016/J.COGNITION.2019.06.009
Whitehouse AJO, Watt HJ, Line EA, & Bishop DVM (2009). Adult psychosocial outcomes of children
with specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and autism. International
Alkire et al. Page 19
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(4), 511–528. 10.1080/13682820802708098
[PubMed: 19340628]
Wood-Downie H, Wong B, Kovshoff H, Cortese S, & Hadwin JA (2021). Research Review:
A systematic review and meta-analysis of sex/gender differences in social interaction and
communication in autistic and nonautistic children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 62(8), 922–936. 10.1111/JCPP.13337 [PubMed: 33137209]
Ying Sng C, Carter M, & Stephenson J (2018). A systematic review of the comparative pragmatic
differences in conversational skills of individuals with autism. Autism & Developmental Language
Impairments, 3, 239694151880380. 10.1177/2396941518803806
Alkire et al. Page 20
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
Schematic of the actor-partner interdependence model.
Alkire et al. Page 21
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Figure 2.
Distributions of the cToM scales among the full sample (leftmost graphs) and by diagnostic
group. AUT = autistic, TD = typically developing
Alkire et al. Page 22
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Figure 3.
Category-level analysis of cToM Positive and Negative scales. Within each scale, sample-
wide averages are displayed (with individual data points superimposed) above boxplots
comparing the distributions within diagnostic group. On the Positive scale, “Following
maxims” refers to consistently following the maxims of quantity, relevance, and manner
throughout the conversation; each child was assigned a global score of either 1 (consistent
throughout), 0.5 (followed maxims for about half the conversation), or 0 (rarely followed
maxims).
Alkire et al. Page 23
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Figure 4.
Distributions plotted by diagnostic group for (A) non-interactive ToM and (B) interaction
quality (participant’s own rating) and interaction success (partner rating). Interaction success
is also plotted by dyad type. vaToM = visual-affective theory of mind. sToM = spontaneous
theory of mind. ***
p
< 0.001 ns = not significant (
p
> 0.1)
Alkire et al. Page 24
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Figure 5.
Hypothesis 1 results for cToM Negative. B = unstandardized beta coefficient. vaToM =
visual-affective ToM, sToM = spontaneous ToM. *
p
< 0.05 **
p
< 0.01 ns =
p
> 0.1
Alkire et al. Page 25
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript Author Manuscript
Alkire et al. Page 26
Table 1.
Hypothesis 1: Parameter estimates of non-interactive ToM predicting cToM
cToM Negative cToM Positive
Effectsa B SE 95% CI
(lower, upper) P B SE 95% CI
(lower, upper) P
vaToM
Actor −0.24
**
0.09 −0.42, −0.07 0.01 −0.17 0.15 −0.46, 0.11 0.24
Partner 0.07 0.12 −0.17, 0.31 0.55 0.03 0.14 −0.25, 0.31 0.84
sToM
Actor −0.30
*
0.13 −0.56, −0.04 0.02 −0.01 0.18 −0.35, 0.34 0.98
Partner 0.02 0.12 −0.27, 0.22 0.84 −0.23 0.18 −0.58, 0.12 0.20
Verbal IQ
Actor −0.01 0.01 −0.03, 0.00 0.13 −0.00 0.01 −0.03, 0.02 0.69
Partner −0.01 0.01 −0.03, 0.00 0.10 −0.01 0.01 −0.04, 0.01 0.20
LangProd
Actor 0.75
**
0.16 0.43, 1.06 0.00 0.64
**
0.22 0.20, 1.08 0.00
Partner 0.03 0.17 −0.31, 0.37 0.86 0.20 0.22 −0.24, 0.64 0.36
EF
Actor 0.01 0.01 −.01, 0.03 0.22 0.00 0.01 −0.01, 0.02 0.61
Partner −0.01 0.01 −0.03, 0.01 0.56 0.01 0.01 −0.00, 0.03 0.13
Dx (AUT)
Actor 0.28 0.35 −0.40, 0.97 0.42 −0.48 0.54 −1.54, 0.58 0.37
Partner 0.74 0.55 −0.36, 1.83 0.18 −0.78 0.54 −1.84, 0.28 0.15
Dyad age −0.06 0.08 −0.22, 0.10 0.46 0.22
#
0.12 −0.02, 0.46 0.07
Dyad gender (F) 0.38 0.31 −0.22, 0.98 0.22 0.32 0.39 −0.45, 1.09 0.41
a
Hypothesized effects in bold
*p
< 0.05
** p
< 0.01
#p
< 0.1
B
= unstandardized beta coefficient,
SE
= standard error, CI = confidence interval, LangProd = language productivity composite (see Methods), EF
= executive functioning (higher scores correspond to greater impairment), Dx = diagnosis
Autism
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2024 February 01.
... Across a range of studies, the term "DEP" is used, despite explicitly describing and measuring a range of entirely different social cognitive constructs. This includes but is not limited to research on theory of mind (e.g., Alkire et al., 2023), emotion recognition (e.g., Hand et al., 2023), "shared understanding" (e.g., Chapple et al., 2021;Heasman & Gillespie, 2019;G. L. Williams et al., 2021), perspective taking (e.g., DeNigris et al., 2018;Heasman & Gillespie, 2018), social perception (e.g., Jones et al., 2021;Morrison, DeBrabander, Jones, Faso, et al., 2020), and observable social behavior (e.g., Chen et al., 2021Chen et al., , 2022a. ...
... Second, most studies on the DEP have involved directly studying social interactions between autistic and neurotypical people. These have either observed interaction in real-world settings (e.g., Chen et al., 2021Chen et al., , 2022aChen et al., , 2022bHeasman & Gillespie, 2019) or the lab 4 (e.g., Alkire et al., 2023;Crompton, Sharp, et al., 2020;Jones et al., 2021;Morrison, DeBrabander, Jones, Faso, et al., 2020;G. L. Williams et al., 2021). ...
... This follows a general trend in autism research, and clinical science more widely, whereby measures developed in the general population are used without fully considering their conceptual and psychometric validity in clinical populations and for testing clinically relevant theories (e.g., see example in Z. J. Williams & Gotham, 2021). Even where measures used in studies regarding the DEP were specifically developed for this purpose (e.g., Alkire et al., 2023;Chen et al., 2021Chen et al., , 2022aChen et al., , 2022bSasson et al., 2017), there has been little effort to formally quantify their validity (e.g., convergent/divergent validity) and other psychometric properties (e.g., measurement invariance, test-retest reliability). Several empirical studies related to the DEP have also relied heavily on data collected in student populations (e.g., DeNigris et al., 2018;Jones et al., 2021; and/or largely male samples (e.g., Chen et al., 2022b;Finke, 2023;Heasman & Gillespie, 2018;Jones et al., 2021;Morrison, DeBrabander, Jones, Faso, et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Work on the “double empathy problem” (DEP) is rapidly growing in academic and applied settings (e.g., clinical practice). It is most popular in research on conditions, like autism, which are characterized by social cognitive difficulties. Drawing from this literature, we propose that, while research on the DEP has the potential to improve understanding of both typical and atypical social processes, it represents a striking example of a weak derivation chain in psychological science. The DEP is poorly conceptualized, and we find that it is being conflated with many other constructs (i.e., reflecting the “jingle–jangle” fallacy). We provide examples to show how this underlies serious problems with translating theoretical claims into empirical predictions and evidence. To start tackling these problems, we propose that DEP research needs reconsideration, particularly through a better synthesis with the cognitive neuroscience literature on social interaction. Overall, we argue for a strengthening of the derivation chain pertaining to the DEP, toward more robust research on (a)typical social cognition. Until then, we caution against the translation of DEP research into applied settings.
... For example, one study used LENA recording devices to capture children's language in an autism intervention classroom where possible social partners were peers and teachers (Trembath et al., 2019). Another study more explicitly looked at how autistic children interact with autistic versus nonautistic peers by having ASD-ASD, non-ASD-non-ASD, and ASD-non-ASD dyads engage in get-to-know-you conversations (Alkire et al., 2023). This study used unfamiliar social partners, but just over half of studies employed familiar social partners (n = 33, 55.93%). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and aims Caregiver reports and standardized assessments have been the primary methods used to study language development in autism. However, these forms of measurement are often coarse, complicated by floor effects and reporter bias, and limited by the fact that they only capture how children can use language at a single moment in time, rather than how children actually use language during everyday interactions. These limitations have led to recent calls for the use of natural language sampling (NLS) as a fine-grained, developmentally appropriate, and contextually relevant measure of everyday communication. The number of studies using NLS to study language in autism has increased substantially in the last 15 years, resulting in a wide array of sampling methods and measures. Given both the increasing prevalence of NLS methods in the autism literature and the variability in sampling approaches and measures, this scoping review addresses the following questions: 1. What populations have been studied using NLS? 2. Which data collection methods are most prevalent in NLS research? 3. How are measures of language derived from NLS? Method Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines, a search for studies published in the last 15 years across three databases was conducted. After removing duplicates, 4,671 titles and abstracts were screened and 59 papers met inclusion criteria. Sample characteristics, natural language collection methods, and derived measures were extracted and tabled for each study. The most prevalent NLS methods and measures in autism language research are reviewed and the benefits and drawbacks of various methods are discussed. Main contribution This scoping review highlights subgroups of the autistic population that have been underrepresented in NLS studies—in particular, minimally/nonspeaking school-aged autistic children. This article also examines means to collect a “naturalistic” sample of language. Notably, studies did not address whether autistic children exhibit different social communication skills when talking to different types of social partners. Broadly, research has underreported key methodological details, making comparisons across studies difficult. Conclusions This review highlights the appropriate use of NLS across development in autism and makes recommendations for NLS future research. Implications Additional work is needed to address the gaps described in this article and replicate previous findings to identify patterns of natural language across the literature.
... Theory of Mind is a developmental component that enables children to understand emotions and the mental states of others. Children with ASD often struggle to represent others' ``mental states,'' which impedes their ability to engage in reciprocal communication and social interaction (Alkire et al., 2023;Fitzpatrick et al., 2018;Risnawati et al., 2015Risnawati et al., , 2023. As a result, children with ASD often appear disinterested in social engagement, prefer solitude, and are more inclined to interact with inanimate objects or engage in repetitive activities (Davison et al., 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Daily Living Skills (ADLS) are essential for the independence and overall development of children. However, for children with autism, these skills often present significant challenges. This quantitative study examines the factors influencing the development of ADLS in autistic children, with a focus on gender, parental self-efficacy, and grandparent involvement. A total of 96 children with autism, aged 4 to 8 years, were recruited from therapy centers and special education schools in Indonesia. Two instruments were used for data collection: a parental self-efficacy scale adapted from Bandura's theory, and the WeeFIM Scale developed by Slomine to assess children’s ADLS performance. Pearson correlation analysis revealed a significant positive relationship between parental self-efficacy and the development of children’s ADLS, indicating that higher levels of parental confidence are associated with better daily functioning in children. Gender differences were also found, with girls outperforming boys in ADLS mastery. Interestingly, children raised in dual-caregiving environments involving grandparents demonstrated lower ADLS performance compared to those raised primarily by their parents. These findings highlight the importance of strengthening parental self-efficacy through targeted training and support programs. The study also emphasizes the influence of cultural practices, particularly the role of extended family in caregiving, which may impact children's skill development. Future research should further investigate the mechanisms through which grandparent involvement affects ADLS and explore these dynamics using longitudinal designs and cross-cultural comparisons. This study advocates for culturally responsive interventions that support both parents and extended family members in nurturing the daily living skills of children with autism.
... The ToM involves a framework that consists of two main dimensions, including the ability to recognize and differentiate cues in one's immediate social environment, particularly the ability to understand the mental state of others and draw inferences about these indicators, namely the ability to participate in cognitive processes associated with comprehending the thoughts and emotions of others [12,13]. Empirical evidence indicates that the correlation between ToM and mental functions continues to develop until late adolescence [14]. Royka and Santos identified two separate elements within the ToM. ...
Article
When partners coordinate their movement in time and space to reach a goal, they perform joint action, an important part of every interaction. Joint action involves motor abilities and socio-cognitive skills like theory of mind. Autistic children’s lower joint motor coordination (joint action) abilities as well as their motor functioning and theory of mind difficulties may interfere with efficient peer interaction. However, the shared contribution of motor and theory of mind to partners’ joint action was not yet explored. This study investigated those contributors (motor and theory of mind) along with group and age differences in 84 autistic children ages 6–16 years and 64 non-autistic children matched by age, sex, and IQ across three age-groups: early-childhood, preadolescence, and adolescence. Basic and advanced theory of mind skills and most motor tasks were higher among adolescents versus early-childhood. However, the autistic group consistently underperformed the non-autistic group in basic and advanced theory of mind levels and in all gross- and fine-motor tasks across all age-groups, revealing unique motor development characteristics in autism. A significant joint full mediation effect emerged for motor and theory of mind skills on joint action performance in both study groups. Understanding that motor and theory of mind skills together underlie joint action opens up a new channel of intervention to facilitate peer interaction. Lay abstract When two or more people move together in a coordinated way at the same time and in the same place, they perform “joint action,” which is an important part of everyday social interaction. Joint action involves the activation of both motor skills and the social-cognitive understanding of others’ thoughts, feelings, and desires—their ability to hold “Theory of Mind.” Motor functioning and Theory of Mind may be challenging for autistic individuals. We wanted to investigate how motor skills and the ability to understand others’ minds develop in autistic and non-autistic children and adolescents and to explore how these skills contribute to joint action performance. We compared 84 autistic children with 64 non-autistic children matched by age, sex, and IQ. Among these 6- to 16-year-olds, we examined three age-groups: early-childhood, preadolescence, and adolescence. We found that older participants, both in the autistic and non-autistic groups, showed better abilities than younger participants in basic and advanced Theory of Mind skills and in most motor tasks. However, non-autistic children outperformed autistic children in Theory of Mind (at basic and advanced levels) and also in all gross-motor and fine-motor tasks, across all age-groups. The autistic group’s motor patterns were characterized by greater variability in tasks’ rated difficulty levels compared to their non-autistic peers, who showed more intact, uniform patterns. Both motor and Theory of Mind skills were found to significantly impact joint action performance in both study groups. These findings are important for understanding joint action’s underlying mechanisms and for refining social intervention programs for autistic individuals.
Article
Article
There has been a recent surge of naturalistic methodology to assess complex topics in psychology and neuroscience. Such methods are lauded for their increased ecological validity, aiming to bridge a gap between highly controlled experimental design and purely observational studies. However, these measures present challenges in establishing construct validity. One domain in which this has emerged is research on theory of mind: the ability to infer others’ thoughts and emotions. Traditional measures utilize rigid methodology which suffer from ceiling effects and may fail to fully capture how individuals engage theory of mind in everyday interactions. In the present study, we validate and test a novel approach utilizing a naturalistic task to assess theory of mind. Participants watched a mockumentary-style show while using a joystick to provide continuous, real-time theory of mind judgments. A baseline sample’s ratings were used to establish a “ground truth” for the judgments. Ratings from separate young and older adult samples were compared against the ground truth to create similarity scores. This similarity score was compared against two independent tasks to assess construct validity: an explicit judgment performance-based paradigm, and a neuroimaging paradigm assessing response to a static measure of theory of mind. The similarity metric did not have ceiling effects and was significantly positively related to both the performance-based and neural measures. It also replicated age effects that other theory of mind measures demonstrate. Together, our multimodal approach provided convergent evidence that dynamic measures of behavior can yield robust and rigorous assessments of complex psychological processes.
Article
Full-text available
Autistic youth often experience challenges in interactions with neurotypical peers. One factor that may influence successful interactions with peers is interpersonal synchrony, or the degree to which interacting individuals align their behaviors (e.g. facial expressions) over time. Autistic and neurotypical youth were paired together into three dyad types: autistic participants paired with autistic participants (AUT-AUT), autistic participants paired with neurotypical participants (AUT-NT), and neurotypical participants with neurotypical participants (NT-NT). Dyads participated in a free conversation task and a video-watching task. We tested whether smiling synchronization differed between AUT-AUT, AUT-NT, and NT-NT dyads. We further tested if smiling synchronization predicted youth-reported interaction enjoyment. AUT-NT dyads had significantly reduced smiling synchronization compared with NT-NT dyads. Smiling synchronization also predicted multiple aspects of participant-rated interaction enjoyment, such as the desire to interact with the peer partner again, above and beyond the overall amount of smiling in the interaction. These findings indicate links between smiling synchronization and interaction enjoyment for autistic and neurotypical youth. Identifying opportunities to synchronize or share positive affect in interactions may promote more enjoyable interactions for both autistic and neurotypical youth. Lay abstract For autistic and neurotypical youth, having positive social interactions with other youth is an important part of well-being. Other researchers have found that one factor that can make people feel like social interactions have gone well is synchronization. Synchronization happens when peoples’ body movements and facial expressions align while they’re interacting. We focus on smiling synchronization here because other studies have found that when neurotypical individuals synchronize their smiles more in a social interaction, they say they enjoy that social interaction more. However, no studies have directly tested whether smiling synchrony influences social interaction enjoyment in autistic and neurotypical youth. We measured smiling synchrony in pairs of interacting autistic and neurotypical youth who were meeting each other for the first time. Some pairs were autistic youth interacting with other autistic youth (autistic with autistic participant pairs), some pairs were autistic youth interacting with neurotypical youth (autistic with neurotypical participant pairs), and other pairs were neurotypical youth interacting with neurotypical youth (neurotypical with neurotypical participant pairs). We found that autistic with neurotypical participant pairs had lower smiling synchrony than neurotypical with neurotypical participant pairs. Youth who were in dyads that had more smiling synchrony said they enjoyed interacting with their partner more and that they wanted to interact with their partner again. Our research shows that smiling synchrony is one part of interactions between autistic and neurotypical youth that influences how well youth say the interaction went. Identifying natural opportunities for autistic and neurotypical youth to share positive feelings could be one way to promote positive social interactions between autistic and neurotypical youth.
Article
Full-text available
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is diagnosed on the basis of speech and communication differences, amongst other symptoms. Since conversations are essential for building connections with others, it is important to understand the exact nature of differences between autistic and non-autistic verbal behaviour and evaluate the potential of these differences for diagnostics. In this study, we recorded dyadic conversations and used automated extraction of speech and interactional turn-taking features of 54 non-autistic and 26 autistic participants. The extracted speech and turn-taking parameters showed high potential as a diagnostic marker. A linear support vector machine was able to predict the dyad type with 76.2% balanced accuracy (sensitivity: 73.8%, specificity: 78.6%), suggesting that digitally assisted diagnostics could significantly enhance the current clinical diagnostic process due to their objectivity and scalability. In group comparisons on the individual and dyadic level, we found that autistic interaction partners talked slower and in a more monotonous manner than non-autistic interaction partners and that mixed dyads consisting of an autistic and a non-autistic participant had increased periods of silence, and the intensity, i.e. loudness, of their speech was more synchronous.
Article
Full-text available
Emerging research suggests overrepresentation of gender diversity among autistic youth. Previous gender diversity research with autistic children has relied on parent-report based on a single question. The Gender Diversity Screening Questionnaire–Self-Report and Parent-Report assessed gender diversity experiences from 244 children (140 autism spectrum disorder and 104 typically developing), between 10 and 13 years, and their parents. Parent-report Child Behavior Checklist Item-110, “Wishes to be the opposite sex,” was also collected. Autistic children endorsed higher Gender Diversity Screening Questionnaire–Self-Report Binary Gender Diversity, t(223.21) = –2.83, adjusted p = 0.02, d = –0.35, and Nonbinary Gender Diversity, t(191.15) = –3.79, adjusted p = 0.001, d = –0.46, than typically developing children. Similarly, for Gender Diversity Screening Questionnaire–Parent-Report, there was a significant gender-body incongruence difference between the groups, t(189.59) = –2.28, adjusted p = 0.05, d = –0.30. Within-group analyses revealed that parents of autistic females-assigned-at-birth reported significantly more gender-body incongruence than males-assigned-at-birth, t(32.91) = –3.78, p < 0.001, d = –1.11. Moreover, distinct within-group profiles of gender experiences and internalizing symptoms were revealed, showing associations between gender profiles and symptoms of anxiety, depression, and suicidality. Results extend reports showing increased rates of gender diversity in autistic children based on both self-report and parent-report, underscoring the need to better understand and support the unique and complex needs of autistic children who experience gender diversity. Lay abstract Gender diversity broadly refers to the way in which an individual experiences (expressions and/or identities) their gender distinctly to that which would be expected based upon social norms for their gender assigned at birth. Recent research has shown a higher representation of gender diversity among autistic youth. Previous research in this area has relied on parent-report based on a single question from the Child Behavior Checklist Item-110, asking whether their child “Wishes to be the opposite sex.” The Gender Diversity Screening Questionnaire Self-Report and Parent-Report were used to assess the experience of gender diversity in 244 children (140 autism spectrum disorder and 104 typically developing) between 10 and 13 years. The Item-110 was also collected. Results showed that autistic children endorsed much higher rates of Binary Gender Diversity (less identification with their designated sex and more with the other binary sex) and Nonbinary Gender Diversity (identification as neither male nor female) than typically developing children. Similarly, parents of autistic children reported significantly more gender-body incongruence experienced by their child than parents of typically developing children. Specifically, parents of autistic females-assigned-at-birth reported significantly more gender-body incongruence than autistic males-assigned-at-birth. Parent- and self-report measures were largely related. Moreover, statistical comparisons between and within the groups revealed associations between gender profiles and symptoms of anxiety, depression, and suicidality. Results extend previous reports showing increased rates of gender diversity in autistic children, now based on both self-report and parent-report, and highlight the need to better understand and support the unique and complex needs of autistic children who experience gender diversity.
Article
Full-text available
Autism affects how someone makes sense of the world around them. About 1–2% of people are autistic. You might have an autistic classmate or family member, or maybe you are autistic. Autistic people might communicate differently than people who are not autistic. This means that it can be difficult for other people to understand what autistic people are trying to say or what they mean. We tend to think that people who are not autistic might be more successful at understanding other people, but in fact, autistic people may be better understood by other autistic people. We will examine and explain some research that has explored how autistic and non-autistic people communicate with each other and explore how this research fits with a theory called the double empathy problem. Understanding what makes interaction comfortable and easy for different people can help us all understand each other better.
Article
Full-text available
Background Evidence increasingly suggests that ASD manifests differently in females than males. Previous reviews investigating sex/gender differences in social interaction and social communication have focused at the level of broad constructs (e.g. comparing algorithm scores from pre‐existing diagnostic instruments) and have typically reported no significant differences between males and females. However, a number of individual studies have found sex/gender differences in narrow construct domains. Methods We conducted a systematic review and random effects model meta‐analyses (in January 2019 and updated January 2020) that investigated sex/gender differences in narrow construct measures of social communication and interaction in autistic and nonautistic children and adolescents, and adults. Study quality was appraised using the Appraisal Tool for Cross‐Sectional Studies (AXIS, BMJ Open, 6, 2016, 1). Results Across 16 studies (including 2,730 participants), the analysis found that female (vs. male) individuals with ASD had significantly better social interaction and social communication skills (SMD = 0.39, p < .001), which was reflective of a similar sex/gender profile in nonautistic individuals (SMD = 0.35, p < .001). Nonautistic males had significantly better social interaction and communication than males with ASD (SMD = 0.77, p < .001). Nonautistic females also had significantly better social interaction and communication than females with ASD (SMD = 0.72, p <.001). Nonautistic males had better social interaction and communication than females with ASD, though this difference was not significant (SMD = 0.30, p = .07). Conclusions This systematic review and meta‐analysis highlighted important sex/gender differences in social interaction and communication for individuals with ASD, likely not captured by pre‐existing diagnostic instruments, which potentially contribute to the under recognition of autism in females, and may need to be reflected in the diagnostic process.
Article
Full-text available
The social symptoms of autism spectrum disorder are likely influenced by multiple psychological processes, yet most previous studies have focused on a single social domain. In school-aged autistic children (n = 49), we compared the amount of variance in social symptoms uniquely explained by theory of mind (ToM), biological motion perception, empathy, social reward, and social anxiety. Parent-reported emotional contagion—the aspect of empathy in which one shares another’s emotion—emerged as the most important predictor, explaining 11–14% of the variance in social symptoms, with higher levels of emotional contagion predicting lower social symptom severity. Our findings highlight the role of mutual emotional experiences in social-interactive success, as well as the limitations of standard measures of ToM and social processing in general.
Article
Full-text available
Atypical social communication is a key indicator of autism spectrum disorder and has long been presumed to interfere with friendship formation and first impressions among typically developing youth. However, emerging literature suggests that such atypicalities may function differently among groups of peers with autism spectrum disorder. The current study aimed to investigate the relationship between atypical social communication patterns and first impression sociometric ratings by peers in groups of youth with autism spectrum disorder. Findings suggest that, contrary to typically developing individuals, several forms of atypical communication among youth with autism spectrum disorder are associated with more positive first impressions by others with autism spectrum disorder. This suggests that interventions designed to increase friendships among youth with autism spectrum disorder may benefit from reframing their approach to addressing atypical social communication.
Article
Full-text available
Effective information transfer requires social communication skills. As autism is clinically defined by social communication deficits, it may be expected that information transfer between autistic people would be particularly deficient. However, the Double Empathy theory would suggest that communication difficulties arise from a mismatch in neurotype; and thus information transfer between autistic people may be more successful than information transfer between an autistic and a non-autistic person. We investigate this by examining information transfer between autistic adults, non-autistic adults and mixed autistic-with-non-autistic pairs. Initial participants were told a story which they recounted to a second participant, who recounted the story to a third participant and so on, along a ‘diffusion chain’ of eight participants (n = 72). We found a significantly steeper decline in detail retention in the mixed chains, while autistic chains did not significantly differ from non-autistic chains. Participant rapport ratings revealed significantly lower scores for mixed chains. These results challenge the diagnostic criterion that autistic people lack the skills to interact successfully. Rather, autistic people effectively share information with each other. Information transfer selectively degrades more quickly in mixed pairs, in parallel with a reduction in rapport. Lay abstract Sharing information with other people relies on the ability to communicate well. Autism is defined clinically by deficits in social communication. It may therefore be expected that autistic people find it difficult to share information with other people. We wanted to find out whether this was the case, and whether it was different when autistic people were sharing information with other autistic people or with non-autistic people. We recruited nine groups, each with eight people. In three of the groups, everyone was autistic; in three of the groups, everyone was non-autistic; and three of the groups were mixed groups where half the group was autistic and half the group was non-autistic. We told one person in each group a story and asked them to share it with another person, and for that person to share it again and so on, until everyone in the group had heard the story. We then looked at how many details of the story had been shared at each stage. We found that autistic people share information with other autistic people as well as non-autistic people do with other non-autistic people. However, when there are mixed groups of autistic and non-autistic people, much less information is shared. Participants were also asked how they felt they had got on with the other person in the interaction. The people in the mixed groups also experienced lower rapport with the person they were sharing the story with. This finding is important as it shows that autistic people have the skills to share information well with one another and experience good rapport, and that there are selective problems when autistic and non-autistic people are interacting.
Article
Open access until 12th March 2020: https://authors.elsevier.com/c/1aRta6E7eDIBlx Background: When autism is viewed through a deficit lens the strengths, positive attributes and interests of individuals on the spectrum can be overshadowed. A strengths-based focus counteracts the deficit view that is traditionally associated with developmental disabilities. More strength-based research is needed in the field to shift the emphasis from difficulties, to the positive attributes and interests of individuals on autism spectrum. Method: Eighty-three children on the autism spectrum responded to the following questions: “What do you like most about yourself?”, “What are you absolutely best at?” and “What do you enjoy the most?” Results: Similar responses were collated into themes within the data using content analysis. When asked “What do you like most about yourself?” a good friend or person to be around (n = 15; 18.5%) and I am good at particular things (n = 14; 16.8%) were the most common themes. Children identified that they were “absolutely best at” physical activity (n = 20; 24%) and maths/science (n = 13; 15.6%). Overall, technology and gaming (n = 42; 50.6%) and social interaction were the most frequently reported activities (n = 29; 34.9%) in response to “What do you enjoy most?” Conclusions: Self-report studies provide individuals on the spectrum with the opportunity to have their voice included in the literature and to express and share their attributes, strengths and interests with others. Further work is needed to explore the impact of such positive self-descriptions on an individual’s positive sense of self and self-confidence.