ArticlePDF Available

Proactive cognitive control, mathematical cognition and functional activity in the frontal and parietal cortex in primary school children: An fNIRS study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Understanding how children acquire mathematical abilities is fundamental to planning mathematical schooling. This study focuses on the relationships between mathematical cognition, cognition in general and neural foundation in 8 to 9-year-old children. We used additive mathematics tests, cognitive tests determining the tendency for proactive and reactive problem solving and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for functional brain imaging. The ability to engage in proactive control had a stronger association with mathematical performance than other cognitive abilities, such as processing speed, sustained attention and pattern recognition. The fNIRS method identified differences between proactive and reactive control, i.e., the more proactive the children were, the greater the increase in oxygenated hemoglobin in the left lateral prefrontal cortex during reactive beneficiary situations. During a text-based task involving additive reasoning, increased activity in the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex was detected compared to a similar task with supportive spatial-geometric information.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
Available online 10 June 2022
2211-9493/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier GmbH. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Research paper
Proactive cognitive control, mathematical cognition and functional activity
in the frontal and parietal cortex in primary school children: An fNIRS study
Simon Skau
a
,
b
,
*
, Ola Helenius
b
,
c
, Kristoffer Sundberg
b
, Lina Bunketorp-K¨
all
a
,
Hans-Georg Kuhn
a
a
Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
b
Department of Pedagogical, Curricular and Professional Studies, Faculty of Education, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
c
National Center for Mathematics Education, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
ABSTRACT
Understanding how children acquire mathematical abilities is fundamental to planning mathematical schooling. This study focuses on the relationships between
mathematical cognition, cognition in general and neural foundation in 8 to 9-year-old children. We used additive mathematics tests, cognitive tests determining the
tendency for proactive and reactive problem solving and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for functional brain imaging. The ability to engage in pro-
active control had a stronger association with mathematical performance than other cognitive abilities, such as processing speed, sustained attention and pattern
recognition. The fNIRS method identied differences between proactive and reactive control, i.e., the more proactive the children were, the greater the increase in
oxygenated hemoglobin in the left lateral prefrontal cortex during reactive beneciary situations. During a text-based task involving additive reasoning, increased
activity in the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex was detected compared to a similar task with supportive spatial-geometric information.
1. Introduction
Proper understanding of how children acquire mathematical abilities
is fundamental for planning mathematical schooling. A major constitu-
ent of such understanding concerns the relationship between cognitive
abilities and the corresponding neural correlates. An important question
is to what extent the development of mathematical skills and compe-
tences, such as the ability to denote and manipulate numbers arith-
metically, mainly relies on general cognitive abilities [1], domain
specic cognitive abilities that primarily affect the learning of arith-
metic [2] or a mix of abilities that might include specic but
non-mathematical abilities such as abilities associated with language
processing [3].
In this paper we will investigate the relationship between general
cognitive ability, proactive cognitive control, mathematical cognition
and functional activity in the frontal and parietal cortex in children aged
8-9 years old.
Different cognitive control functions such as working memory, in-
hibition, and cognitive exibility have been linked to mathematical
performance [4]. However, most studies of cognitive control are only
designed to test reactive and not proactive cognitive control [5,6]. Ac-
cording to the dual mechanism of control theory (DMC), cognitive
control can be used in a proactive (preparatory) or in a reactive
(stimulus-driven) manner [6]. Proactive cognitive control is crucial to
keeping attention on the essentials of a situation and to prepare for
handling future information, for example applying tools a teacher
explained in a previous assignment.
Children begin to engage in proactive behavior around the age of 5 to
6, but it is not until the age of 8 that children begin to use proactive
control more reliably [7]. The tendency to engage in proactive control
increases with age up to adulthood [8]; hence adults tend to be more
proactive than children [6]. However, studies have shown that the
ability to be proactive decreases towards the onset of adolescence [9]. In
recent studies, the ability to use proactive control was associated with
increased mathematical performance in 6 to 11-year-old children [10,
11].
In adults, a recent meta-analysis has shown that the right medial
frontal gyrus is involved during the reactive but not the proactive mode
and the right inferior frontal gyrus is involved during the proactive but
not the reactive mode [12]. However, only a handful of imaging studies
investigating proactive control in children have been conducted so far.
Moreover, the different age groups (ranging from 5 to 15 years),
different aims and experimental designs of previous studies make it
difcult to draw generalized conclusions. For example, Kamijo and
Masaki found that children with greater physical tness (at 10 years of
age) were more proactive and showed increased activity in the posterior
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: simon.skau@gu.se (S. Skau).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Trends in Neuroscience and Education
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tine
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2022.100180
Received 12 October 2021; Received in revised form 14 May 2022; Accepted 9 June 2022
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
2
parietal cortex (PPC) during reactive tasks than children with lower
tness [13]. In contrast, Strang and Pollak found increased activity in
the inferior frontal gyrus, right inferior parietal cortex, bilateral thal-
amus and insula in 10 and 15-year-olds, when being proactive [14].
However, the involvement of these structures was tied to reward con-
ditions. Even though the information about proactive and reactive
control is difcult to differentiate from the tness of the children and
reward conditions, a general trend of dynamic involvement of PPC
during proactive and reactive control can be supported [7,1315]. More
studies on children are needed to dene the stage of development during
which the prefrontal cortex is involved in differentiating proactive from
reactive control.
For mathematical cognition, a meta-analysis of functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) studies found that children between 9 and 14
years of age involve the right inferior parietal lobule, inferior parietal
sulcus, claustrum, and insula when performing numerical tasks. Simi-
larly, for a calculation task, they also involved the right cingulate gyrus,
bilateral medial frontal gyrus, right precuneus, left claustrum and left
insula [16]. Most studies of both mathematical cognition and proactive
control have been performed using fMRI or electroencephalography
(EEG). For fMRI-based techniques, the limiting environment of the MR
scanner creates experimental restrictions. This raises the question of
ecological validity towards the mathematical tasks in school and
everyday life [16] and since the scanner environment is not
child-friendly due to the loud noise and space limitations, most studies
have been performed in adults [17]. Therefore, limitations regarding
development of mathematical cognition in children exist [18]. In light of
these issues, researchers have argued for the use of functional
near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) in the study of mathematical
cognition [17]. fNIRS is used to detect oxygenated (oxy-Hb) and deox-
ygenated hemoglobin (deoxy-Hb) in a specic brain location, which are
surrogate measures for neuronal activity. Thus, fNIRS and fMRI are
sensitive to the same physiological process. However, while fNIRS has
lower spatial resolution, it provides better temporal resolution [19].
fNIRS is a non-invasive, safe, accessible, quiet and portable brain ac-
tivity monitoring system and compared to EEG and fMRI it is more
robust against motion artifacts. This makes fNIRS well suited for
assessing in children the neural substrates for mathematical cognition
and cognitive control in general [17,20,21].
This exploratory study aims to investigate three relationships: (i) the
relationship between proactive control and mathematical cognition, (ii)
the relationship between proactive control and brain activation and (iii)
the relationship between brain activation and mathematical cognition in
different situations involving children between 8 to 9 years of age. As a
measure of proactive/reactive control, we used a modied version of the
AX-Continuous Performance Test (AX-CPT) (see Fig. 1A and method
section) [10,2224]. We wanted (i) to investigate whether proactive
control was associated more with mathematical performance than pre-
viously established constructs such as pattern recognition, sustained
attention, and processing speed [3,25,26] and (ii) to perform a con-
ceptual replication of previous fMRI imaging studies of proactive and
reactive control with children with fNIRS. In addition, we wanted to
investigate if fNIRS can reveal neural activation differences during
mathematical tasks, where only context of the presentation differs. We
developed three mathematical tests centered around different ways of
performing addition and subtraction that could be used with fNIRS. The
rst test, additive situations, examined the ability to transform mathe-
matical situations into arithmetic language, without the need to perform
any calculations (Fig. 1B). The second and third tests investigated ad-
ditive reasoning, when it is text-embedded or comes with geometric support
(Fig. 1C-D).
Fig. 1. The test done during fNIRS. A) The modied AX-CPT test with an example of the serial stimulus presentation with the arrow indicating the order of pre-
sentation. Subjects press right or left to indicate the correct response to the stimuli presented in the squares. The designations AX (dogprobe followed by cat
stimulus), BY (no dogprobe followed by no catstimulus), AY (dogprobe followed by no catstimulus) and BX (no dog probe followed by catstimulus)
indicate the four probe types that can be generated from image pairs (see Method section for further information). B) Example of the Additive situations task. C)
Example of the Additive reasoning with geometric support task. D) example of Additive reasoning text-based task.
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
3
2. Materials and methods
The present study is part of the larger Gothenburg AMBLE project
(Arena for Mind, Brain, Learning and Environment) which integrates
research from developmental psychology, neuroscience and educational
science.
2.1. Participants
Participants were recruited from the second grade in two Swedish
primary schools located in two different cities. Parents gave their writ-
ten informed consent before the study began and the participants were
told that they could withdraw at any time. In total, 53 children from the
two schools participated in the study (21 from a school in Uppland and
32 from a school in V¨
astra G¨
otaland). The cohort consisted of 30 boys,
23 girls and a mean age of 8.5 years with a standard deviation of 4
months, with an age range between 8.1 and 9.3 years of age. Based on
self-reports, 46 of the children were right-handed, and seven were left-
handed. The study was approved by the Gothenburg Regional Com-
mittee of the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (reference number: 839-
17).
2.2. Experimental design
The study was conducted during the 2019 spring term and consisted
of one individual session per child conducted by a researcher and one
whole-class session two weeks later conducted by their teacher. For the
individual session, all tests, including the fNIRS recordings, were per-
formed in a separate room near the childrens classroom. The data was
collected with the participant seated in a chair at a table with a computer
screen. All participants performed the tasks in the same order, starting
with the pen and paper tasks, Symbol Search [27] and Digit Symbol
Coding [27], followed by a reaction time test. After the reaction time
task, the fNIRS cap (EASYCAP GmbH, Germany), with the fNIRS optodes
and detectors attached was carefully placed on the participants head.
Participants performed a modied version of the AX-Continuous Per-
formance Test, followed by the additive situation test and
geometric/text-based test (Fig. 1). The procedure took between 50-60
min in total, including preparations and trial runs for all tasks.
2.3. Measures
Symbol Search and Digit Symbol Coding are subtests within the Pro-
cessing Speed Index in Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children fourth
edition (WISC-IV) [28]. In both tests, the subject was asked to process as
many symbols as possible during 2 min. The raw score was dened as
the number of correctly processed symbols.
Reaction time: the participants were asked to press the space key as
fast as possible on a computer whenever they heard a sound with a 5-sec-
ond interval.
Modied version of AX-Continuous Performance Test (AX-CPT). The
test involved cues called A and B and targets X and Y. We used a child-
adapted version of a setup we used on adults in a previous study [29]
using pictures of animals instead of letters. The participant was pre-
sented with one picture of an animal at a time on the computer screen.
The participant was instructed to respond with the left hand on a
keyboard whenever a picture of a cat (X) preceded by a picture of a dog
was shown (A) and to respond with the right hand on the keyboard for
all other combinations of cue-target pairs, also referred to as probes
(Fig. 1) [30]. Each stimulus (picture of an animal) had two functions; it
served as the target item for the current trial and represented the cue for
the upcoming trial, which results in four different kinds of trials: AX
(dogcat), AY (dogno cat), BX (no dog cat) and BY (no dogno
cat), only the rst (AX: dog followed by cat) trial required a left-hand
response (Fig. 1A). The trials can be categorized as; AX, target trials;
AY, situation benetting from reactive control (or reactive trials for
short); BX, situation benetting from proactive control (or proactive trials
for short); BY, control trials [29]. The following list of animals were used:
dog, cat, elephant, camel, giraffe, shark, buttery, rat. We used three
different pictures of dogs and two different pictures of cats. The trials
were semi-randomized and generated 22 AX, 18 AY, 22 BX and 34 BY
trials.
Participants were asked to answer as fast and correct as possible and
had 4,000 ms to respond before the picture on the screen disappeared.
The time span between response and the next stimulus jittered between
5 and 7 s, with a mean of 6 s. During this time, participants were asked to
focus on a plus sign on the screen. A 30-second break was given after half
of the 96 trials. Raw scores were recorded as reaction time, errors, and
omissions.
From the response time data, we calculated the proactive behavioral
index (PBI) according to the formula below. This has previously been
applied in similar contexts [2224]:
PBI =(AY BX
AY +BX)
A positive PBI value indicates more proactive tendency and a nega-
tive value more reactive tendency. We also calculated the d context
index, which is based on the proportion of accuracy scores for AX and BX
trials. The proportion of correct AX trials is subtracted by the proportion
of errors on BX trials. 100% accuracy on AX trials was replaced with
(2
(1/N)
; N =AX trials), and 100% accuracy for BX trials was replaced
with (1-2
(1/N)
; N =BX trials), to get an unbiased estimation [15]. A
higher dcontext value indicates a higher sensitivity to contextual cues
[15].
2.4. Response time adjustment
In order to adjust the response time based on the error rate, the linear
integrated speed-accuracy score (LISAS) [31] was calculated.
LISAS =RTi+(SRT
SPE)×PEi
Where RTi is the participants mean response time in condition i, PEi
is the participants proportion of errors in condition i; SRT is the par-
ticipants overall RT standard deviation; and SPE is the participants
overall PE standard deviation. Weighting the PE with the RT and PE
standard deviation ratio is done to achieve a similar weight of the two
components, RT and PE.
2.5. Mathematical test for fNIRS imaging
Additive situations. The author, O.H., developed the test based on
Vergnauds theoretical analysis of the psychology of the addition and
subtraction operations [32]. The children were asked to listen to a
description of an additive situation involving two numbers. They were
then asked to choose the arithmetic expressions that described the sit-
uation out of four possible choices presented on the computer screen
(see Fig. 1B). Each arithmetic expression was highlighted with a specic
color and presented at different sites on the screen. The participants
responded by pressing a colored button on a gamepad corresponding to
the specic color of the arithmetic expression, and was located on the
same side on the gamepad as on the screen. There were a total of 17
trials. Each question was verbally presented and, within a few seconds,
repeated once more. The participant had 25 s to answer before the trial
ended and there was 4 to 6 s between the end of a trial and the beginning
of the next. All participants performed the trials in the same order. The
number of correct answers was recorded as a raw score.
Additive reasoning text-based or with geometric support. The test was
developed by author O.H. and contained 20 arithmetical tasks that can
be solved with the addition operation. Half of the tasks were formulated
in natural language (Swedish), describing an additive situation
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
4
(Fig. 1D). The other half of the tasks consisted of similar additive tasks,
matched for arithmetic difculty, but were formulated with the help of a
picture involving spatial/geometric settings, such as length, distance or
movement (Fig. 1C). The two types of tasks were randomly mixed and all
participants performed the tasks in the same order. For each task, par-
ticipants were handed a sheet of paper describing/depicting the task.
The researcher read the task aloud to the participant twice with a short
pause in between. Participants had 25 s to write down the answer before
the sheet of paper was removed. 4-6 s later, the next sheet of paper with
a task was presented. The participants were informed that they could
write on the sheet of paper if they wanted to make sketches or calcu-
lations. The raw scores were dened as the number of correct answers.
2.6. Whole class-based measurements
Basic numeracy and calculations (BANUCA) [33]. The test assessed the
basic non-symbolic and symbolic number sense, subitizing and con-
ceptual subitizing, simple addition and subtraction and the ability to
identify simple number patterns. We also added ten simple multiplica-
tion questions. We used the total score (maximum score of 89 points) as
the outcome.
Additive and multiplicative reasoning (AMR) [34]. The test con-
tained a total of 17 tasks. About half of the tasks require additive
reasoning, such as Jamal and Sara play a game. Sara is on number 11
and Jamal on number 4. How much further ahead is Sara? (accompa-
nied by a picture of the board game with Saras and Jamals positions
shown). Half of the tasks are multiplicative such as There are 3 rabbits
in each house, how many rabbits in total live in the 4 houses?
(accompanied by a picture of 4 houses with the digit 3 on one of them).
Test of Visual Perceptual Skills-III (TVPS-III) [35]. Children were
presented with a gure or pattern and underneath, four more compli-
cated gures. One of the four gures contains the rst gure. Children
are asked to nd and mark that gure. The test contained 18 tasks in
total.
Working memory subtest from Lilla Duvan [36]. The test has a
dual-task format where a child is orally presented with a letter symbol
immediately followed by a straightforward yes/no question (e.g., Can
dogs bark?). The yes/no answer is given using red and green signs.
Following the answer, the task is to recall the presented letter. There
were six tasks with an increasing number of or letters (2-4) to remember.
The correct letters in the correct order yielded two points, whereas
correct letters in incorrect order yielded one point. We only used the
total score (max score is 36) as the outcome.
2.7. fNIRS data acquisition
The fNIRS measurements were performed using a continuous wave
system (NTS, Optical Imaging System, Gowerlabs Ltd., UK) [37], using
two wavelengths (780 and 850nm) to measure changes in the concen-
tration of oxygenated hemoglobin (oxy-Hb), deoxygenated hemoglobin
(deoxy-Hb) and their sum, total hemoglobin (tot-Hb). The system has 16
dual-wavelength sources and 16 detectors. The array used 43 channels
(i.e., source/detector pairs) with a source-detector distance of 30mm
and two short-separation channels with a 10-mm distance, as suggested
from previous studies [38,39]. Short separation channels are only sen-
sitive to hemodynamic activity in the scalp and skull. They are used to
regress out the scalp signal and improve the fNIRS measurements
specicity of hemodynamic responses [38,39]. The optode placements
were designed to encompass both the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and
the parietal cortex (Fig. 2). The fNIRS data was acquired at a sampling
frequency of 10 Hz.
2.8. fNIRS data analysis
The fNIRS data was preprocessed using MATLAB 2018b [40] and the
MATLAB-based fNIRS-processing package HomER2 [41]. The process-
ing pipeline started with pruning the raw data. Channels were rejected if
their mean intensity was below the instruments noise oor (1e-3 A.U.).
Fig. 2. Layout of the fNIRS measurement. A) The cap with fNIRS optodes on a child. B) Visualization of the measurement points/channel (red dots). The region of
interest (ROI); LPPC, lateral posterior parietal cortex; MPPC, medial posterior parietal cortex; LPC, lateral parietal cortex; MPC, medial parietal cortex; DLPFC,
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DMPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; aDLPFC, anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, aDMPFC, anterior dorsomedial prefron-
tal cortex.
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
5
The raw data was then converted to optical density. A high-pass lter of
0.03 Hz was used to correct for drift and a low-pass 0.5 Hz lter to
remove pulse and respiration. The HomER2 functions hmrMotionArti-
fact and hmrMotionCorrectSpline were used to correct for motion arti-
facts. Optical density was converted to hemoglobin concentration with
hmrOD2Conc, using the default values [6.0 6.0] for the partial path-
length factors.
To calculate the hemodynamic response function (HRF), the
hmrDeconvHRF_DriftSS function in HomER2, which estimates the HRF
by applying a General Linear Model (GLM), was used. To solve the GLM,
a least-square t of a convolution model in which the HRF at each
channel and chromophore was modeled as a series of Gaussian basis
functions, with a spacing and standard deviation of 0.5 s [42]. The
model included polynomial drift regressors up to the 3rd order. The
regression time length for the three mathematical tests was -5 to 25 s and
for the AX-CPT -2 to 12 s. The short separation channel selection for
regression of each long channel was chosen based on the highest cor-
relation to the long channel.
Since the wavelengths used by the Gowers Lab NTS system (780 nm,
850 nm) are less sensitive to deoxy-Hb, only the oxy-Hb data were sta-
tistically analyzed [43,44]. For the mathematical test analysis, each
channel was analyzed individually, while for the AX-CPT, channels were
pooled together into predened regions of interests (ROIs) by averaging
the signals, see Fig. 2. For the mathematical test, the maximum peak
between 3 and 25 s after each stimulus was identied for each channel,
and two s around the peak value were averaged. For the AX-CPT, the
maximum peak between 2 and 10 s after each stimulus was identied for
each channel, and two seconds around the peak value were averaged.
2.9. Statistical analyses
Since the study is exploratory in nature, we choose a Bayes factor
(BF) analysis using the open-source program JASP version 0.10. [45].
We have applied BF
10
as the main criterion, and the interpretation of
BF
10
=3 would be that, given the data, the alternative hypothesis (H
1
) is
3 times more likely than the null hypothesis (H
0
), while BF
10
=0.3 can be
interpreted that, given the data, the H
0
is 3 times more likely than H
1
. A
BF
10
>3 can also be interpreted as the equivalent to a p-value <0.01
[46]. The H
0
is dened in this study as no difference or no association,
depending on the test. Following the praxis of Wagenmakers and col-
leagues [47], a BF
10
in one of the four categories between 3-10, 10-30,
30-100 or above 100 is interpreted as substantial, strong, very strong or
extreme evidence for H
1
, respectively.
For the rst aim of the study, we took the average proportion of
correct answers to the math tests, additive situations, additive reasoning
text-based, additive reasoning with geometric support, BANUCA, and AMR,
denoting it math performance. We then conducted a Bayes factor version
of Pearsons correlation of math performance in relation to PBI, d
context, reaction time, performance on the AX-CPT, Symbol Search, Digit
Symbol Coding, Lilla Duvan, and TVPS-III. For the correlations, we used
the default stretched beta prior width of 1.
For the studys second aim, we used Pearsons r correlation between
peak oxy-Hb concentration in each ROI for AY and BX trials, with the PBI
and dcontext.
For the third aim of the study, a Bayes factor version of paired t-test
was conducted to compare average peak oxy-Hb values for the three
different mathematical situations (additive situations, additive reasoning
with geometric support, additive reasoning text-based) for each channel. We
used a default Cauchy prior of 0.707.
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive result and evaluation of variables
Two children could not undergo fNIRS recording due to discomfort
wearing the cap and were thus excluded from the analysis. Three were
excluded from the AX-CPT analysis since they either were unable to
perform the whole AX-CPT test or had two or fewer correct responses on
the BX trials. One child did not perform Digit Symbol Coding and
Symbol Search; ve did not perform Lilla Duvan, eight did not perform
the AMR, seven did not perform BANUCA, and twelve did not perform
TVPS-III. When analyzing processing speed as measured by the Digit
Symbol Coding and Symbol Search subtests of the WISC-IV processing
speed index, the children had an average processing speed index of 103
points, corresponding to the 58-percentile compared to the age-matched
WISC-IV reference population [28]. Only one child had a processing
speed outside two standard deviations with a processing speed index of
131 points. Additional descriptive data are presented in Table 1. Based
on visual analysis of histogram and Q-Q plots (Supplementary Fig.1), the
following variables were not used in correlation with math performance:
AY error, BX error, BY error, AX omission, AY omission, BX omission, BY
omission.
3.2. Performance in the individual mathematical test
The average correct responses for the additive situation test were
61.4%, the additive reasoning with geometric support 65.6% and the ad-
ditive reasoning text-based 79.0%, indicating that the additive situations
and additive reasoning with geometric support were more difcult tasks
than the additive reasoning text-based. The associations between the three
tests and the two whole class tests of mathematics are shown in Table 2.
When we correlated the performance results for the general cognitive
tests with all mathematical tests, we nd a positive association of d
context with additive reasoning with geometric support (with r =.47 and
BF
10
=56.0) and BANUCA test (with r =.43 and BF
10
=13.27). The
Digit Symbol Coding was positively correlated with additive reasoning
text-based (with r =.473 and BF
10
=77.75), indicating that the higher
the childrens general processing speed, the higher the number of cor-
rect answers on the additive reasoning text-based. The AMR test was
negatively associated with response time on all four probes; AX (r =-.60
and BF
10
=995.4), AY (r =-.49 and BF
10
=39), BX (r =-.51 and BF
10
=
61.9) and BY (r =-.47 and BF
10
=26), indicating that faster responses on
the AX-CPT task are associated with higher scores on the AMR test. None
of the mathematical test results were associated with the Symbol Search
test performance or the PBI (see Table 2).
To evaluate our rst aim, we calculated math performance as the
Table 1
Descriptive statistics of the behavioral variables.
N Mean SD Min Max
Age 51 104.3 3.94 97 112
Reaction time 50 305.1 59.781 211 489
Additive situations 52 10.4 2.30 4 15
Geometric support 52 6.5 2.47 1 10
Text-based 52 7.9 2.07 1 10
AMR 46 78.5 7.05 49 88
BANUCA 45 12.2 2.84 6 18
PBI 50 -0.113 0.09 -0.274 0.102
d context 50 0.698 0.18 0.182 0.924
Lilla Duvan 48 29.1 5.45 14 36
TVPS-III 41 13.7 3.02 5 18
Digit Symbol Coding 52 35.6 6.74 19 52
Symbol Search 52 20.5 3.77 12 30
AX 50 1133.7 338.2 497.7 2084.1
AY 50 1029.2 185.1 686.8 1474.3
BX 50 1316.6 332.7 667.6 2167.8
BY 50 1158.4 280.4 592.8 1711.1
AX error 50 3.940 2.94 0 12
AY error 50 0.580 0.92 0 4
BX error 50 2.620 3.18 0 17
BY error 50 1.060 2.61 0 17
AX omission 50 0.640 0.87 0 3
AY omission 50 0.260 0.63 0 3
BX omission 50 1 1.21 0 5
BY omission 50 0.680 1.26 0 6
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
6
average mathematical performance on all ve tasks and correlated it
with all behavioral variables (Table 2). The overall math performance
was associated with dcontext (r=.051 and BF
10
=43.6), TVPS-III (r=.43
and BF
10
=3.68), Digit Symbol Coding (r=.39 and BF
10
=3.90), and
response time on AY (r=-.41 and BF
10
=4.88) and BX trials (r=-.56 and
BF
10
=128). PBI was not associated with average math performance
with an r=.34 and BF
10
=1.87 (for scatter plots of average math per-
formance with PBI, dcontext, and BX response time see Fig. 3, and for
scatter plots with all other behavioral variables see Supplementary
Fig. 2).
3.3. fNIRS results AX-CPT
The second aim was to associate childrens tendency to use reactive
or proactive control with functional activity in the frontal and parietal
cortex. We performed Pearsons r correlation of both PBI and dcontext
with oxy-Hb in the predetermined ROIs for AY trials and BX trials. All
results are summarized in Table 3. There is evidence for an association
between PBI and oxy-Hb increase in the right LPPC during AY trials (r =
0.491 and BF₁₀ =9.496) indicating that with higher PBI score a larger
increase in oxy-Hb in right PPC is observed during situations beneting
from reactive control (Fig. 4A). The peak oxy-HB in left DLPFC during
BX trials with dcontext showed very strong evidence with an r =-.498 and
BF₁₀ =56.85, indicating that the higher the dcontext score, the less the
left DLPFC was involved during BX trials (Fig. 4B).
3.4. fNIRS results mathematical test
Our third aim was to evaluate if there was a functional difference in
the frontal or parietal cortex between an arithmetic math task without
calculations (additive situation), a text-based task and a task with geo-
metric support. Average oxy-Hb curves for each channel for additive
situation, additive reasoning with geometric support, and text-based, plus t-
values based on the paired t-test for each channel are visualized in Fig. 5,
and degree of freedom, t-value, BF₁₀ and Cohens d are shown in Sup-
plementary Table 1-3.
Both the geometric support and text-based showed higher activity in
the anterior PFC compared to additive situation. For the comparison
between additive reasoning with geometric support and additive situations,
seven of the most anterior channels showed very strong evidence or more
for higher activity during additive reasoning with geometric support than
for additive situations, with an average Cohens d of 0.741 ranging from
0.637 to 0.839. The posterior channel in the right DLPFC also indicated
increased activity for additive reasoning with geometric support with a BF₁₀
=10.36 and Cohens d of 0.622, as well as one channel in the left LPPC
with a BF₁₀ =13.35 and Cohens d of 0.676. For the comparison between
text-based and additive situations, all of the seven most anterior channels
showed very strong to extreme evidence for increased activity during text-
Table 2
Correlation between mathematical tests and behavioral variables.
Math P(n=39) Additive situation (n=52) Text-based(n=52) Geometric support(n=52) BANUCA(n=46) AMR(n=45)
Variables r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀
PBI (n¼50) .34 1.87 .02 0.17 .19 0.43 .22 0.58 .08 0.21 .20 0.44
dcontext (n¼50) .51 43.6 .22 0.60 .36 4.33 .47 56.0 .43 13.27 .33 2.05
Reaction time .03 0.20 -.17 0.34 .09 0.21 -.08 0.20 .00 0.19 .23 0.57
Lilla Duvan (n¼48) .17 0.34 .00 0.18 .12 0.25 .14 0.28 .20 0.45 .30 1.35
TVPS-III (n¼41) .43 3.68 .34 1.99 .15 0.30 .37 3.03 .25 0.62 .42 4.42
DSC (n¼52) .39 3.90 .14 0.28 .47 77.7 .38 8.92 .37 4.38 .22 0.51
SS (n¼52) .33 1.68 .19 0.42 .32 2.66 .30 1.66 .29 1.17 .18 0.37
Age (n¼51) .17 0.34 .05 0.18 .06 0.19 .17 0.34 .02 0.18 .03 0.19
AX (n¼50) -.31 1.13 .08 0.21 -.37 5.83 -.29 1.36 -.22 0.52 -.60 995.4
AY (n¼50) -.41 4.88 -.09 0.21 -.19 0.43 -.18 0.37 -.06 0.20 -.49 39.0
BX (n¼50) -.56 128 -.07 0.20 -.29 1.41 -.31 1.80 -.09 0.22 -.51 61.9
BY (n¼50) -.36 2.48 -.03 0.18 -.32 2.25 -.20 0.45 -.17 0.35 -.47 26.0
AX E (n¼50) -.42 6.59 -.12 0.25 -.37 6.70 -.31 2.18 -.61 >1000 -.20 0.44
Math P
Additive situation .61 871
Text-based .83 >1000 .21 0.56
Geometric support .90 >1000 .42 18.3 .68 >1000
BANUCA .72 >1000 .35 3.20 .70 >1000 .64 >1000
AMR .63 >1000 .10 0.23 .59 >1000 .42 10.5 .32 1.50
Numbers in bold signify a Bayes factor (BF
10
) over 3. >1000 signies a BF
10
over 1000; PBI, proactive behavioral index; DSC, Digit Symbol Coding; SS, Symbol Search;
AMR, Additive and multiplicative reasoning; BANUCA, Basic numeracy and calculations; TVPS-III, Test of Visual Perceptual Skill-III.
Fig. 3. Scatter plot for average math performance. A) PBI (proactive behavioral index), B) d context, and C) response time for BX trials.
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
7
based situation, with an average Cohens d of 0.9840 ranging from 0.811
to 1.185. The four channels in DMPFC (around Fz) had an average
Cohens d of 0.693, ranging from 0.598 to 0.849. A channel in the DLPFC
with BF₁₀ =137 and Cohens d of 0.745, and two channels in the left
LPPC with BF₁₀ =39.82 and Cohens d of 0.781, BF₁₀ =13.61 and
Cohens d of 0.662.
When comparing the text-based with geometric support, one channel
showed evidence for a difference in the anterior DMPFC with a BF₁₀ =
3.696 and Cohens d of 0.411.
4. Discussion
The present study had three aims 1) to investigate whether the ten-
dency to engage in proactive cognitive control is more associated with
mathematical performance than processing speed, sustained attention
and pattern recognition; 2) to investigate the functional activity of
proactive control in the frontal and parietal cortex; 3) to explore the
functional difference in the frontal or parietal cortex during arithmetic
tasks, a text-based mathematical task and a mathematical task with
geometric support in 8 and 9-year-old children.
4.1. Proactive cognitive control
Cognitive control has been a reliable predictor of mathematical
performance and development of mathematical cognition [4]. The most
consistent predictor has been visual spatial working memory [48], fol-
lowed by other cognitive domains such as inhibition, cognitive exi-
bility and processing speed [4]. The ability to engage in proactive
control is dependent on the developmental trajectory as well as the in-
dividualsworking memory capacity and uid intelligence [8,49]. Since
the default design of most cognitive control tasks emphasizes a reactive
mode or strategy [5,6], few studies of cognitive control have focused on
the ability to engage in proactive control. In a recent study, Kubota et al.
observed that proactive ability, as measured by the d context index,
made a unique contribution in predicting mathematical performance
over working memory, inhibition and cognitive exibility [10]. Our
result corroborates this nding, with very strong evidence for a relation
between dcontext and mathematical performance indicating a relation
between a childs ability to focus on the cue (A or B) in the AX-CPT and
mathematical performance. There was no substantial association be-
tween PBI and mathematical performance, contrary to the ndings of
Wang et al. [11] which found that PBI explained additional variance in
mathematical performance beyond the effect of working memory.
However, in our sample there was an association between response
time on the BX trials, processing speed during a situation benetting
from proactive control, and mathematical performance. Based on the
spread of the PBI, only a handful of children had a positive value on the
PBI i.e., tendency to be more proactive, meaning that the children could
rather be viewed as engaging in a reactive mode. If two children engage
in a reactive mode and are presented with an image of a cat (in the BX
trial), the child that is faster in recalling the previous image (not a dog),
will have a faster response time on BX than a child that is slower in
recalling the previous image. That type of quickness of short-term
Table 3
Correlation for oxy-Hb in all ROIs with PBI and d context.
PBI dcontext
AY BX AY BX
r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀ r BF₁₀
Left DLPFC
(n¼44)
.20 0.44 -.01 0.18 -.22 0.53 -.49 56.85
DMPFC (n¼41) .16 0.32 .01 0.19 -.13 0.27 -.41 6.11
Right DLPFC
(n¼44)
.08 0.21 -.04 0.19 -.29 1.14 -.07 0.21
Left aDLPFC
(n¼48)
-.05 0.19 -.10 0.23 -.16 0.33 -.11 0.24
aDMPFC
(n¼48)
.09 0.21 -.07 0.20 .06 0.19 -.06 0.19
Right aDLPFC
(n¼48)
-.02 0.18 .01 0.18 -.18 0.39 -.10 0.23
Left LPPC
(n¼34)
.06 0.22 -.19 0.37 .00 0.21 .11 0.25
Left MPPC
(n¼31)
.13 0.28 -.29 0.76 -.08 0.24 .01 0.22
Left LPC (n¼38) .06 0.21 -.18 0.36 -.16 0.33 -.16 0.33
Left MPC
(n¼33)
.01 0.21 -.03 0.22 -.09 0.24 -.19 0.37
Right MPPC
(n¼40)
-.05 0.20 -.15 0.30 .17 0.35 .05 0.20
Right LPPC
(n¼31)
.49 9.49 -.19 0.37 .16 0.33 -.21 0.42
Right MPC
(n¼40)
.17 0.34 -.08 0.22 .05 0.20 -.07 0.22
Right LPC
(n¼37)
.18 0.35 -.04 0.21 .07 0.22 -.09 0.23
Numbers in bold signify a Bayes factor (BF10) over 3; PBI, proactive behavioral
index; LPPC, lateral posterior parietal cortex; MPPC, medial posterior parietal
cortex; LPC, lateral parietal cortex; MPC, medial parietal cortex; DLPFC,
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DMPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; aDLPFC,
anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, aDMPFC, anterior dorsomedial pre-
frontal cortex
Fig. 4. Scatter plot peak oxy-Hb and proactive control indices. A) x-axis is proactive behavioral index (PBI) and data points are peak oxy-Hb during AY trials in right
posterior partial cortex. B) x-axis is dcontext and data points are peak oxy-Hb during BX trials in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
8
memory access or processing speed in recalling rules and recently
presented relevant information, even in a reactive mode, could be
benecial when facing a new mathematical problem. Consider a typical
task like Nilla has 6 owers and Nicke has 5 owers, how many do they
have together? It is not until the second part of the sentence that the
instruction for how to deal with the numbers is given. The second part of
the question could have been “…how many more owers does Nilla
have? which would have triggered another mathematical operation.
When listening to, or reading the task, the child must wait until the end
to receive information on how to deal with the numerical information
presented earlier. It is possible that if the process of retrieving the pre-
vious information is too slow, then the child forgets or mixes up which
operation to carry out. This may explain why BX compared to the other
trial types (AX, AY and BY) has a strong association with math perfor-
mance, which needs to be investigated in future studies.
When interpreting behavioral results, the relationship between
response time (efciency) and accuracy (effectiveness) should be
considered [50]. There is usually a trade-off relationship, i.e., a fast
response time leads to lower accuracy or high accuracy leads to slower
response time. However, in this sample, there was a positive association
between response time and accuracy, violating this trade-off principle
(Supplementary Table 4). Not only did the more reactive children
respond slower on proactive trials, but they also made more errors. As
shown in the method section, the d context is based on the accuracy
score of AX and BX trials, whereas the PBI is based on the response time
on AY and BX trials. To see if the complementary information in both
response time and accuracy could be combined, we performed a sec-
ondary analysis where we used the LISAS adjustments to the response
time [31,51]. However, the LISAS-adjusted PBI did not correlate more
with average mathematic performance and the adjusted BX response
time had an even lower association with mathematical performance.
There are many possible explanations for why the adjusted BX and
adjusted PBI did not co-vary to the same extent as the d context with
mathematical performance. For example, only the more reactive chil-
dren potentially disrupted the trade-off relationship for BX trials and not
the more proactive children, which means that an adjustment would
penalize the children with slower reaction time on BX trials, making the
difference greater and less nuanced as hoped. Further investigation is
required to determine whether this is a general feature of development
or whether response time adjustments are not suitable for reactive and
proactive testing. One reason for the dcontext with mathematical per-
formance might also be that the dcontext variable taps into an aspect of
proactivity that simply is particularly useful when dealing with the type
of mathematical tasks that are so popular in mathematics tests. In the
example with Nicke and Nilla above, it would obviously be advanta-
geous to already plan ahead after seeing the rst numerical items and
prepare to process information about the mathematical operation e.g.,
addition, subtraction (or something else).
4.2. Functional imaging
The PBI scores, which indicate proactive or reactive tendencies in
behavior, are based on reaction times and our results suggest that the
less reactive (i.e., more proactive) children tend to be, the more they use
the right LPPC in reactive situations. This is in accordance with other
imaging studies with children [7,1315]. We can conrm the usefulness
of fNIRS for studying the neural correlates of cognitive control in chil-
dren. The analysis of d´context, which is based on response accuracy,
indicates that the less a child utilizes contextual cues to solve upcoming
tasks, the more the left DLPFC is activated during BX trials. The intuition
behind the dcontext index is that, if participants have been observant of
the cues (A or B) then they should have similar accuracy score on the AX
and the BX trials. However, if participants did not pay attention to the
cues, then there should be a difference in accuracy for AX and BX trials.
These assumptions correspond well with our data, where children with
lower d´context score appear to have, on their successful BX trials, higher
Fig. 5. Peak oxy-Hb during mathematical tests. The upper row shows channel layout, from the transverse view, with paired t-value difference between additive
reasoning with geometric support (AR-G) vs additive situations (AS) (left), additive reasoning text-based (AR-T) vs. AS (middle) and AR-T vs AR-G (right). Lower row
shows average oxy-Hb curve for all channels during AS on the left, during AR-G in the middle and AR-T on the right. Black dots indicate 10/20 landmarks. White
marks are short separation channels.
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
9
activity in the left DLPFC, possibly due to the added cognitive load of
remembering what the previous cue was.
4.3. Mathematical reasoning
For the third aim, both the text-based task and tasks with geometric
support evoked a larger increase of oxy-Hb in the anterior PFC and the
left parietal cortex than during the additive situations task (Fig. 5).
There was one channel, located over DMPFC, showing a substantially
larger increase in oxy-Hb for additive reasoning text-based compared to
additive reasoning with geometric support. Also, other channels in the left
latera parietal cortex and PFC showed a larger difference but were not
sufcient to reach substantial evidence (see Supplementary Table 3).
These results are in line with other fNIRS studies that found evidence
of an increase in oxy-Hb concentration in the parietal cortex for text-
embedded mathematical questions compared to numerical calcula-
tions for 9- and 10-year-olds [52,53]. Similar ndings were described in
adults [54]; although, none of the studies measured oxy-Hb in the PFC.
Overall, these results show promise for studying the difference between
different mathematical tasks with fNIRS. However, a recent review of
fNIRS and mathematical cognition in children (age 9 to 16 year of age)
found only six studies using fNIRS with some having a low number of
participants (n=8 and n=14) [17].
Literature on arithmetical development supports the participation of
a larger network involving PFC, PPC (including angular gyrus, inferior
parietal sulcus and precuneus), along with deeper brains structures not
detectable with fNIRS [16, 55]. With increased prociency and less
reliance on cognitive control, the involvement of PFC decreases and PPC
increases [55]. In order to reduce differences in the cognitive load be-
tween the mathematical tasks, all questions were read aloud twice to the
children [56]. However, several differences remain. The additive situ-
ation test involved answering by pressing a button while the additive
reasoning with geometric support and additive reasoning text-based required
written answers. Moreover, children had to solve the tasks in additive
reasoning with geometric support and text-based test, while for the additive
situation test, children were presented with alternatives, imposing
different cognitive loads. Based purely on the fNIRS data, a possible
interpretation is that even though children in second grade have learned
how to read, text-based information in a mathematical task adds to the
cognitive load. The potential answer options in additive situations and
the geometric information on the geometric support task could function
similar to gestures, which have been shown to reduce cognitive load
during mathematical tasks for 9-year-olds [57]. Additionally, according
to the gateway hypothesis [58], the rostral PFC/frontal polar area,
which in our studies showed increased activity during additive reasoning
text-based compared to additive reasoning with geometric support, is
involved when shifting from attending to external information to
attending to internal representations, as part of increased metacognitive
demand.
However, the cognitive load interpretation does not align with the
behavioral data, where error rates of additive situations and additive
reasoning with geometric support were close to equivalent, but still higher
compared to additive reasoning text based. Error rate is usually an indi-
cator for the difculty level of a task and higher difculty might suggest
that more cognitive resources are involved to solve the tasks. More
likely, in line with fMRI studies on children, the additive situations did
not induce any calculation, but instead induced an answer based on a
sense of familiarity. Studies using fMRI have found that medial frontal
cortex and right parietal cortex, especially, inferior parietal sulcus, show
increased functional activity during calculation [16], which was not
present in the additive situations test. Our analysis did not detect any
difference for additive reasoning with geometric support and text-based
compared to additive situations in the right PPC, however such a differ-
ence was detected in the medial prefrontal cortex and left PPC, involving
the left inferior parietal sulcus. It is typical for the performance on
different mathematical tests at this age to correlate relatively highly
with each other as well as with cognitive tests [59]. However, it is worth
recognizing that the test performance for additive situations only corre-
lated with additive reasoning with geometric support and possibly with
BANUCA (see Table 2), but not with any other mathematical or cogni-
tive test leading us to the abductive conclusion that the task is not as
rmly associated with general cognitive level or general mathematical
performance as the other tests.
5. Limitations
For some brain areas, especially for the parietal cortex, there was a
relatively high loss of data due to a higher incidence of signal drop-out
and the removal of very weak signals in the preprocessing stage. The
source of the weaker signal was usually the absorption of light by hair
between the optodes and the scalp. Dense and thick hair, especially at
the top of the head, made it more challenging to gain a sufcient signal
for estimation of the hemodynamic response. Our experimental design
allowed for a few min per child to optimize the fNIRS signal, which
resulted in some loss of data.
During the fNIRS mathematical tests (additive situation, additive
reasoning with geometric support and additive reasoning text-based)
response time was not saved, which would have been valuable infor-
mation to evaluate both the hemodynamic response and how difcult
the children perceived the test to be. Since the individual test session and
the whole class sessions were not performed during the same week, for
some children we lack data on the whole class test.
Based on the function for partial pathlength factor from [60], rec-
ommended values for this age group and wavelength would be [5.6 4.6]
instead of [6.0 6.0] which were used in this study. However, since the
group is homogenous with respect to age (SD of 3.9 month and age range
of 15 months), the within group comparison should not be affected.
Due to the time constraint, digitizing the placement of the fNIRS
optodes was not done. We cannot control for variability of the placement
of the channels. This variability is not a concern for the ROI analysis,
where several channels are pooled together, but is a possible concern for
the single channel comparison. However, head size measurements were
taken before the experiment and cap sizes 50, 52, 54 or 56 were used to
t the childs head as accurately as possible using face and head land-
marks in order to get measurements where intended.
6. Conclusion
In the present exploratory study we were able to corroborate previ-
ous studies, where the ability to engage in proactive control co-varies
with mathematical performance better than other cognitive abilities,
such as processing speed, sustained attention, and pattern recognition.
With fNIRS, we could also conceptually replicate previous studies using
other imaging modalities, showing that more proactive children have an
increased activity in posterior parietal cortex during reactive situations.
For the mathematical part we found that children tended to activate the
prefrontal cortex more during text-based mathematical tasks compared
to tasks that also had a visual geometric support. Although the fNIRS
result is in line with the hypothesis that text-based tasks involve a higher
cognitive load, the behavioral result did not support this interpretation.
Future studies are needed to evaluate whether geometrical support re-
duces the cognitive load during mathematical tasks.
Ethics
The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in
Gothenburg (reference number: 839-17).
The parents gave their written informed consent before their chil-
dren took part in the study, and the participants were told that they
could withdraw at any time.
All data were anonymous.
S. Skau et al.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education 28 (2022) 100180
10
Funding
The study was funded by grants from the Swedish Research Council
(Vetenskapsrådet 712-2014-2468, VR-MH 2019-01637) and the STENA
Foundation for Culture and Health.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing nancial
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to inuence
the work reported in this paper.
Supplementary materials
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in
the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.tine.2022.100180.
References
[1] D. Szucs, et al., Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and
inhibition impairment, Cortex 49 (10) (2013) 26742688.
[2] K. Landerl, A. Bevan, B. Butterworth, Developmental dyscalculia and basic
numerical capacities: a study of 89-year-old students, Cognition 93 (2) (2004)
99125.
[3] D.C. Geary, et al., Mathematical Cognition decits in children with learning
disabilities and persistent low achievement: a ve-year prospective study, J. Educ.
Psychol. 104 (1) (2012) 206223.
[4] H.L. St Clair-Thompson, S.E. Gathercole, Executive functions and achievements in
school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory, Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59
(4) (2006) 745759.
[5] A.R. Aron, From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer
model for stopping inappropriate responses, Biol. Psychiatry 69 (12) (2011)
e55e68.
[6] T.S. Braver, The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms
framework, Trends Cogn. Sci. 16 (2) (2012) 106113.
[7] N. Chevalier, et al., Metacognitive processes in executive control development: the
case of reactive and proactive control, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27 (6) (2015) 11251136.
[8] P.W. Van Gerven, et al., Switch hands! Mapping proactive and reactive cognitive
control across the life span, Dev. Psychol. 52 (6) (2016) 960971.
[9] N.R. Polizzotto, et al., Normal development of context processing using the AXCPT
paradigm, PLoS One 13 (5) (2018), e0197812.
[10] M. Kubota, et al., Consistent use of proactive control and relation with academic
achievement in childhood, Cognition (2020) 203.
[11] C. Wang, B. Li, Y. Yao, Proactive control mediates the relationship between
working memory and math ability in early childhood, Front. Psychol. (1612)
(2021) 12.
[12] G. Gavazzi, et al., Contiguity of proactive and reactive inhibitory brain areas: a
cognitive model based on ALE meta-analyses, Brain Imaging Behav. (2020).
[13] K. Kamijo, H. Masaki, Fitness and ERP indices of cognitive control mode during
task preparation in preadolescent children, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10 (2016) 441.
[14] N.M. Strang, S.D. Pollak, Developmental continuity in reward-related
enhancement of cognitive control, Dev. Cognitive Neurosci. 10 (2014) 3443.
[15] S.V. Troller-Renfree, G.A. Buzzell, N.A. Fox, Changes in working memory inuence
the transition from reactive to proactive cognitive control during childhood, Dev.
Sci. 23 (6) (2020) p. n/a-n/a.
[16] M. Arsalidou, et al., Brain areas associated with numbers and calculations in
children: Meta-analyses of fMRI studies, Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 30 (2018) 239250.
[17] M. Soltanlou, et al., Applications of Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS)
in studying cognitive development: the case of mathematics and language, Front.
Psychol. 9 (2018) 277.
[18] D. Ansari, Neurocognitive approaches to developmental disorders of numerical and
mathematical cognition: the perils of neglecting the role of development, Learn.
Individ. Differ. 20 (2) (2010) 123129.
[19] V. Quaresima, S. Bisconti, M. Ferrari, A brief review on the use of functional near-
infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for language imaging studies in human newborns
and adults, Brain Lang. 121 (2) (2012) 7989.
[20] S. Rossi, et al., Shedding light on words and sentences: near-infrared spectroscopy
in language research, Brain Lang. 121 (2) (2012) 152163.
[21] Y. Moriguchi, K. Hiraki, Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young
children: a review of NIRS studies, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7 (DEC) (2013).
[22] S.G. Ryman, et al., Proactive and reactive cognitive control rely on exible use of
the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, Hum. Brain Mapp. 40 (3) (2019) 955966.
[23] C. Gonthier, et al., Inducing proactive control shifts in the AX-CPT, Front. Psychol.
7 (2016) 1822.
[24] T.S. Braver, et al., Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human
prefrontal cortex, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106 (18) (2009) 7351.
[25] R.L. Peterson, et al., Cognitive prediction of reading, math, and attention: shared
and unique inuences, J. Learn. Disabil. 50 (4) (2017) 408421.
[26] J. Cui, et al., Visual form perception can be a cognitive correlate of lower level
math categories for teenagers, Front. Psychol. 8 (2017) 1336.
[27] D. Wechsler, Wechsler Adut Intelligence Scale - Fourth Edition, swedish edition,
Pearson Assessment, Stockholm, 2010.
[28] D. Wechsler, Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children - Fourth Edition, swedish
edition, Pearson Assessment, Stockholm, 2003.
[29] S. Skau, et al., Different properties of the hemodynamic response and its relation to
trait mental fatigue and proactive cognitive control, Neuroimage: Rep. 1 (3)
(2021), 100038.
[30] C.H. Chatham, M.J. Frank, Y. Munakata, Pupillometric and behavioral markers of a
developmental shift in the temporal dynamics of cognitive control, in: Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106, 2009,
pp. 55295533.
[31] A. Vandierendonck, A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy
measures of performance: a rejoinder on the binning procedure, Behav. Res.
Methods 49 (2) (2017) 653673.
[32] T.P. Carpenter, J.M. Moser, T.A. Romberg, Addition and Subtraction: A Cognitive
Perspective, L. Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, N.J., 1982.
[33] R¨
as¨
anen, P., BANUCA: Basic Numerical and Calculation abilities =Lukuk¨
asitteen
ja Laskutaidon Hallinnan Testi, ed. P. R¨
as¨
anen. 2005, Jyv¨
askyl¨
a: Niilo M¨
aki
Instituutti (NMI).
[34] T. Nunes, et al., The scheme of correspondence and its role in childrens
mathematics, Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2 (7) (2010) 8399.
[35] N.A. Martin, TVPS-3: Test of Visual Perceptual Skills, Academic Therapy
Publications, Novato, CA, 2006.
[36] U. Wolff, Lilla DUVAN: Dyslexiscreening f¨
or Årskurs 3, 5 Och 7, Hogrefe
Psykologif¨
orlaget Stockholm, 2010.
[37] N.L. Everdell, et al., A frequency multiplexed near-infrared topography system for
imaging functional activation in the brain, Rev. Sci. Instrum. 76 (9) (2005).
[38] L. Gagnon, et al., Improved recovery of the hemodynamic response in diffuse
optical imaging using short optode separations and state-space modeling,
Neuroimage 56 (3) (2011) 13621371.
[39] S. Brigadoi, R.J. Cooper, How short is short? Optimum source-detector distance for
short-separation channels in functional near-infrared spectroscopy,
Neurophotonics 2 (2) (2015), 025005.
[40] MATLAB, The MathWorks, Inc, Massachusetts, United States, 2014.
[41] T.J. Huppert, et al., HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-
infrared spectroscopy of the brain, Appl. Opt. 48 (10) (2009) D280D298.
[42] J.C. Ye, et al., NIRS-SPM: statistical parametric mapping for near-infrared
spectroscopy, Neuroimage 44 (2) (2009) 428447.
[43] H. Sato, et al., A NIRS-fMRI investigation of prefrontal cortex activity during a
working memory task, Neuroimage 83 (2013) 158173.
[44] K. Uludag, et al., Separability and cross talk: optimizing dual wavelength
combinations for near-infrared spectroscopy of the adult head, Neuroimage 22 (2)
(2004) 583589.
[45] M. Marsman, E.-J. Wagenmakers, Bayesian benets with JASP, Eur. J. Dev.
Psychol. 14 (5) (2017) 545555.
[46] M. Jeon, P. De Boeck, Decision qualities of Bayes factor and p value-based
hypothesis testing, Psychol. Methods 22 (2) (2017) 340360.
[47] E.J. Wagenmakers, et al., Why psychologists must change the way they analyze
their data: the case of psi: comment on Bem (2011), J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100 (3)
(2011) 426432.
[48] K. Allen, S. Higgins, J. Adams, The relationship between visuospatial working
memory and mathematical performance in school-aged children: a systematic
review, Educ. Psychol. Rev. 31 (3) (2019) 509531.
[49] L.L. Richmond, T.S. Redick, T.S. Braver, Remembering to prepare: the benets (and
costs) of high working memory capacity, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41 (6)
(2015) 17641777.
[50] M.W. Eysenck, et al., Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control
theory, Emotion 7 (2) (2007) 336353.
[51] A. Vandierendonck, Further tests of the utility of integrated speed-accuracy
measures in task switching, J. Cogn. 1 (1) (2018) 8.
[52] A. Obersteiner, et al., Bringing brain imaging to the school to assess arithmetic
problem solving: chances and limitations in combining educational and
neuroscientic research, ZDM 42 (6) (2010) 541554.
[53] T. Dresler, et al., Arithmetic tasks in different formats and their inuence on
behavior and brain oxygenation as assessed with near-infrared spectroscopy
(NIRS): a study involving primary and secondary school children, J. Neural.
Transm. 116 (12) (2009) 16891700.
[54] M. Richter, et al., Changes in cortical blood oxygenation during arithmetical tasks
measured by near-infrared spectroscopy, J. Neural Transm. 116 (3) (2009)
267273.
[55] L. Peters, B. De Smedt, Arithmetic in the developing brain: A review of brain
imaging studies, Dev. Cognitive Neurosci. 30 (2018) 265279.
[56] S. Gillmor, J. Poggio, S. Embretson, Effects of reducing the cognitive load of
mathematics test items on student performance, Numeracy 8 (1) (2015).
[57] S. Goldin-Meadow, et al., Explaining math: gesturing lightens the load, Psychol.
Sci. 12 (6) (2001) 516522.
[58] P.W. Burgess, I. Dumontheil, S.J. Gilbert, The gateway hypothesis of rostral
prefrontal cortex (area 10) function, Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 (7) (2007) 290298.
[59] L.S. Fuchs, et al., The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic,
algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems, J. Educ. Psychol. 98 (1)
(2006) 2943.
[60] F. Scholkmann, M. Wolf, General equation for the differential pathlength factor of
the frontal human head depending on wavelength and age, J. Biomed. Opt. 18 (10)
(2013), 105004.
S. Skau et al.
... Neural activity observation while using learning materials such as game is becoming prominent to understand student's engagement in study [13]. This allows researchers to investigate the cognitive process behind learning and enhance our understanding of how the brain responds to educational stimuli [37,54]. The fNIRS contributes significantly in the field of neural activity research in education sector because it supports longitudinal studies to enhance customized learning experience [32,37,54] in different environments including classrooms and laboratories [20] that has. ...
... This allows researchers to investigate the cognitive process behind learning and enhance our understanding of how the brain responds to educational stimuli [37,54]. The fNIRS contributes significantly in the field of neural activity research in education sector because it supports longitudinal studies to enhance customized learning experience [32,37,54] in different environments including classrooms and laboratories [20] that has. Evaluating neural activity yields significant insights into the brain's information processing and the Manuscript submitted to ACM regions activated during learning tasks; however, the domain of brain signal analysis remains still developing, as cognitive processes are inferred indirectly from variables such as task performance and hemodynamic responses. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This scoping review analyzes the use of Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) in game-based learning (GBL) settings, providing a thorough examination of contemporary trends and approaches.Employing the PRISMA framework, an initial collection of 956 articles was methodically screened, resulting in 18 research papers that satisfied the inclusion criteria. Each chosen study was assessed based on many criteria, including measurable outcomes, equipment characteristics, and study design. The review categorizes fNIRS-based GBL research into two primary types: cognitive response studies, which analyze how the brain function during tasks and comparative studies, which evaluate finding across different study materials or methods based on neural activities. The analysis includes learning platforms, gaming devices, and various fNIRS devices that has been used. Additionally, study designs and data collection methodologies were reviewed to evaluate their impact on research results. A comprehensive analysis outlines the specifications of fNIRS devices used in diverse studies, including yearly publication trends categorized by learning type, gaming equipment, fNIRS study classification, and outcome measures such as learning improvements and cerebral pattern analysis. Furthermore, the study design and analysis techniques are detailed alongside the number of studies in each category, emphasizing methodological trends and analytical strategies.
... Whereas it was thought that children younger than 6 years were unable to engage proactive control 49 , there is now consensus regarding that younger children can engage this more demanding form of control under certain conditions 44,50,51 . Importantly, it seems that proactive control is an essential capacity for adaptive controlled behaviours engagement with proactive control being more involved than other cognitive capacities (e.g., sustained attention) in academic skills 52 , and potentially benefitting more than other cognitive control skills from specific metacognitive training 3 . For these reasons, proactive control is becoming the hottest cognitive control ability studied in developmental research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive control development across childhood is critical for later academic achievement. Despite recent advances in the comprehension of how the context influences cognitive control development, no study has ever addressed whether one of the most frequent contextual features of children’s lives (i.e., the presence of another person) impacts control engagement. Here, 123 Chinese children aged 5 and 9 years-old performed, either in the presence of an experimenter or alone, an AX-CPT, a task assessing reactive and proactive control. We found that children were overall negatively affected by the experimenter presence in terms of latencies but not of accuracy. Further, when analysing the trial types separately, we observed that this effect mainly concerned trials requiring children to engage more proactive control and was greater for younger than older children. These results indicate that direct social factors such as the presence of an unfamiliar experimenter seem to modulate cognitive control performance. Future research should continue to examine these effects in the light of the numerous existing social presence theories in order to unravel what are the cognitive mechanisms affected by social presence in childhood.
... However, real-world full-scalp measurements have limitations due to cumbersome hardware, optical losses from hair, and user adherence [16,17]. Furthermore, mapping of the entire brain may not be necessary for a cognitive assessment; comparative studies have shown that most activity happens in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [18][19][20]. The PFC is responsible for working memory, mental imagery, and higher cognitive functions [21][22][23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
From learning environments to battlefields to marketing teams, the desire to measure cognition and cognitive fatigue in real time has been a grand challenge in optimizing human performance. Near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) is an effective optical technique for measuring changes in subdermal hemodynamics, and it has been championed as a more practical method for monitoring brain function compared to MRI. This study reports on an innovative functional NIRS (fNIRS) sensor that integrates the entire system into a compact and wearable device, enabling long-term monitoring of patients. The device provides unrestricted mobility to the user with a Bluetooth connection for settings configuration and data transmission. A connected device, such as a smartphone or laptop equipped with the appropriate interface software, collects raw data, then stores and generates real-time analyses. Tests confirm the sensor is sensitive to oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin changes on the forehead region, which indicate neuronal activity and provide information for brain activity monitoring studies.
... It effectively monitored cerebral oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2), deoxygenated hemoglobin (HbR), and Total-Hb signals. Given its heightened sensitivity to changes in local blood flow in the brain compared to HbR, HbO2 was selected as the indicator reflecting neural activation levels in this study 59,60 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Sporting experience plays a pivotal role in shaping exercise habits, with a mutually reinforcing relationship that enhances cognitive performance. The acknowledged plasticity of cognition driven by sports necessitates a comprehensive examination. Hence, this study delves into the dynamic intricacies of the prefrontal cortex, exploring the impact of orienteering experience on cognitive performance. Our findings contribute empirical evidence regarding the functional activation of specific brain regions bridging the nexus between experiential factors and cognitive capabilities. In this cross-sectional study, a cohort of forty-nine athletes was enrolled to meticulously examine behavioral variances and prefrontal cortex dynamics among orienteering athletes of varying experience levels across diverse non-specialized scenarios. These investigations involved the utilization of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to detect alterations in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2). The high-experience expert group exhibited neurological efficiency, demonstrating significantly diminished brain activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal, left ventral lateral prefrontal, and right orbitofrontal regions compared to the low-experience group. Within the low-experience novice group, superior performance in the spatial memory task was observed compared to the mental rotation task, with consistently lower reaction times across all conditions compared to the high-experience group. Notably, cerebral blood oxygenation activation exhibited a significant reduction in the high-experience expert group compared to the low-experience novice group, irrespective of task type. The dorsolateral prefrontal lobe exhibited activation upon task onset, irrespective of experience level. Correct rates in the spatial memory task were consistently higher than those in the mental rotation task, while brain region activation was significantly greater during the mental rotation task than the spatial memory task.” This study elucidates disparities in prefrontal cortex dynamics between highly seasoned experts and neophyte novices, showcasing a cognitive edge within the highly experienced cohort and a spatial memory advantage in the inexperienced group. Our findings contribute to the comprehension of the neural mechanisms that underlie the observed cognitive advantage and provide insights into the forebrain resources mobilized by orienteering experience during spatial cognitive tasks.”
Article
Modern neuroscience is making significant progress in the study of brain functions, which can be of great importance for the education. However, there is a gap between current neuroscientific evidence on how the brain learns and its direct application in classrooms. What can neuroscientists, psychologists, and educators do to improve the interaction between neuroscience and education? In this article, we attempt to answer this question by examining the essentials of educational neuroscience as an interdisciplinary field of research at the intersection of neuroscience, pedagogy, and cognitive science, which seeks to translate research on the neural mechanisms of learning into educational practice and understand the impact of education on the learner's brain. In the first part of the article we describe the origins and current progress of neuroscience in education, discuss terminological uncertainty in the Russian scientific literature on the relationship between neuroscience and education, as well as possible and, in our opinion, most promising ways of interaction between psychology, pedagogy and neuroscience. In the second part of the article we analyze the main directions of contemporary research in the field of neuroscience, based on the Scopus database of scientific information.
Article
Full-text available
Doll play may provide opportunities for children to rehearse social interactions, even when playing alone. Previous research has found that the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) was more engaged when children played with dolls alone, compared to playing with tablet games alone. Children's use of internal state language (ISL) about others was also associated with pSTS activity. As differences in social cognition are frequently observed in autistic people, we were interested in the brain and language correlates of doll play in children with varying levels of autistic traits. We investigated children's (N = 57, mean age = 6.72, SD = 1.53) use of ISL and their pSTS brain activity using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) as they played with dolls and tablet games, both alone and with a social partner. We also investigated whether there were any effects of autistic traits using the parent-report Autism Spectrum Quotient-Children's Version (AQ-Child). We found that the left pSTS was engaged more as children played with dolls or a tablet with a partner, and when playing with dolls alone, compared to when playing with a tablet alone. Relations between language and neural correlates of social processing were distinct based on the degree of autistic traits. For children with fewer autistic traits, greater pSTS activity was associated with using ISL about others. For children with more autistic traits, greater pSTS activity was associated with experimenter talk during solo play. These divergent pathways highlight the importance of embracing neurodiversity in children's play patterns to best support their development through play.
Article
Objective Taking orienteering as an example, this study aimed to reveal the effects of mental rotation on orienteers’ map representation and their brain processing characteristics. Methods Functional near-infrared spectroscopic imaging (fNIRS) was used to explore the behavioral performance and cortical oxyhemoglobin concentration changes of map-represented cognitive processing in orienteering athletes under two task conditions: normal and rotational orientation. Results Compared to that in the normal orientation, athletes’ task performance in the rotated orientation condition was significantly decreased, as evidenced by a decrease in correct rate and an increase in reaction time; in the normal orientation condition, blood oxygen activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal lobe was significantly greater than that in the ventral prefrontal lobe, which was significantly correlated with the correct rate. With rotating orientation, the brain oxygen average of each region of interest was enhanced, and the brain region specifically processed was the ventral prefrontal lobe, specifically correlating with the correct rate. Conclusions Mental rotation constrains the map representation ability of athletes, and map representation in rotational orientation requires more functional brain activity for information processing. Ventral lateral prefrontal lobe activation plays an important role in the map representation task in rotational orientation.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The Dual Mechanism of Control theory proposes that we use our cognitive control in a proactive and reactive mode. The proactive mode is used to optimize performance in more challenging tasks and is thought to be a more demanding activity. Trait mental fatigue is the overall disposition of mental fatigability and sensation of mental weariness and exhaustion. In the present study, we investigated the association between trait mental fatigue, proactive control, and different properties of the hemodynamic response in the frontal and parietal cortex of healthy adults. Method 30 healthy adults answered the Mental Fatigue Scale (MFS), performed the AX-Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT), a test of proactive cognitive control which generates the proactive behavioral index (PBI), and underwent functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure functional activity in the frontal and parietal cortex while being tested on the AX-CPT. Result There was a Spearman's correlation ρ = −0.462 between MFS and the PBI, indicating an association between trait mental fatigue and a tendency to rely on reactive cognitive control. The exploratory analysis indicated that higher trait mental fatigue was associated with higher oxygenated hemoglobin concentrations (oxy-Hb) and total hemoglobin (tot-Hb) from the initial dip to up to the peak during reactive situations. This was observed bilaterally in the medial posterior partial (MPPC) cortex and the right lateral palatal cortex (LPC) for oxy-Hb. In contrast, for tot-Hb, this pattern was also observed in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and right anterior DLPFC, bilateral MPPC, right lateral PPC, and right parietal cortex. Individuals with a more reactive response type showed during proactive situations a higher concentration of oxy-Hb and tot-Hb in the right MPPC and the right LPPC for oxy-Hb for the latter part of the hemodynamic response. Discussion/conclusion Higher trait mental fatigue is associated with a tendency to favor reactive over proactive cognitive control in healthy adults. fNIRS imaging detected the involvement of several brain regions in trait mental fatigue. However, further research is needed to determine whether the hemodynamic response function could be linked to suboptimal neurovascular coupling.
Article
Full-text available
Based on the dual mechanisms of control (DMC) theory, there are two distinct mechanisms of cognitive control, proactive and reactive control. Importantly, accumulating evidence indicates that there is a developmental shift from predominantly using reactive control to proactive control during childhood, and the engagement of proactive control emerges as early as 5–7 years old. However, less is known about whether and how proactive control at this early age stage is associated with children’s other cognitive abilities such as working memory and math ability. To address this issue, the current study recruited 98 Chinese children under 5–7 years old. Among them, a total of 81 children (mean age = 6.29 years) contributed useable data for the assessments of cognitive control, working memory, and math ability. The results revealed that children at this age period predominantly employed a pattern of proactive control during an AX-Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT). Moreover, the proactive control index estimated by this task was positively associated with both working memory and math performance. Further regression analysis showed that proactive control accounted for significant additional variance in predicting math performance after controlling for working memory. Most interestingly, mediation analysis showed that proactive control significantly mediated the association between working memory and math performance. This suggests that as working memory increases so does proactive control, which may in turn improve math ability in early childhood. Our findings may have important implications for educational practice.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive control is a critical feature in adapting our behavior to environmental and internal demands with two types of inhibition having been identified, namely the proactive and the reactive. Aiming to shed light on their respective neural correlates, we decided to focus on the cerebral activity before or after presentation of the target demanding a subject’s stop as a way to separate the proactive from the reactive components associated with the tasks. Accordingly, we performed three Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analyses of fMRI studies exploring proactive and reactive inhibitory phases of cognitive control. For this purpose, we searched for fMRI studies investigating brain activity preceding or following target stimuli. Eight studies (291 subjects, 101 foci) were identified for the proactive analysis. Five of these studies and those previously analyzed by others (348 subjects, 199 foci) were meta-analyzed to explore the neural correlates of reactive inhibition. Overall, our results showed different networks for the two inhibitory components. Notably, we observed a contiguity between areas in the right inferior frontal gyrus pertaining to proactive inhibition and in the right middle frontal gyrus regarding reactive inhibition. These neural correlates allow proposal of a new comprehensive model of cognitive control.
Article
Full-text available
As children become older, they better maintain task-relevant information in preparation of upcoming cognitive demands. This is referred to as proactive control, which is a key component of cognitive control development. However, it is still uncertain whether children engage in proactive control consistently across different contexts and how proactive control relates to academic abilities. This study used two common tasks—the AX Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT) and the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm (CTS)—to examine whether proactive control engagement in 102 children (age range: 6.91–10.91 years) converges between the two tasks and predicts academic abilities. Proactive control indices modestly correlated between tasks in higher but not lower working-memory children, suggesting that consistency in proactive control engagement across contexts is relatively low during childhood but increases with working memory capacity. Further, working memory (but not verbal speed) predicted proactive control engagement in both tasks. While proactive control as measured by each task predicted math and reading performance, only proactive control measured by CTS additionally predicted reasoning, suggesting that proactive control can be used as a proxy for academic achievements.
Article
Full-text available
The body of research surrounding the relationship between visuospatial working memory (VSWM) and mathematics performance remains in its infancy. However, it is an area generating increasing interest as the performance of school leavers comes under constant scrutiny. In order to develop a coherent understanding of the literature to date, all available literature reporting on the relationship between VSWM and mathematics performance was included in a systematic, thematic analysis of effect sizes. Results show a significant influence of the use of a standardised mathematics measure, however, no influence of the type of VSWM or mathematics being assessed, on the effect sizes generated. Crucially, the overall effect size is positive, demonstrating a positive association between VSWM and mathematics performance. The greatest implications of the review are on researchers investigating the relationship between VSWM and mathematics performance. The review also highlights as yet under-researched areas with scope for future research.
Article
Full-text available
The role of ventral versus dorsolateral prefrontal regions in instantiating proactive and reactive cognitive control remains actively debated, with few studies parsing cue versus probe‐related activity. Rapid sampling (460 ms), long cue–probe delays, and advanced analytic techniques (deconvolution) were therefore used to quantify the magnitude and variability of neural responses during the AX Continuous Performance Test (AX‐CPT; N = 46) in humans. Behavioral results indicated slower reaction times during reactive cognitive control (AY trials) in conjunction with decreased accuracy and increased variability for proactive cognitive control (BX trials). The anterior insula/ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (aI/VLPFC) was commonly activated across comparisons of both proactive and reactive cognitive control. In contrast, activity within the dorsomedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was limited to reactive cognitive control. The instantiation of proactive cognitive control during the probe period was also associated with sparse neural activation relative to baseline, potentially as a result of the high degree of neural and behavioral variability observed across individuals. Specifically, the variability of the hemodynamic response function (HRF) within motor circuitry increased after the presentation of B relative to A cues (i.e., late in HRF) and persisted throughout the B probe period. Finally, increased activation of right aI/VLPFC during the cue period was associated with decreased motor circuit activity during BX probes, suggesting a possible role for the aI/VLPFC in proactive suppression of neural responses. Considered collectively, current results highlight the flexible role of the VLPFC in implementing cognitive control during the AX‐CPT task but suggest large individual differences in proactive cognitive control strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Context processing involves a flexible and continually updated representation of task relevant information and is a core aspect of cognitive control. The expectancy AX Continuous Performance Test (AXCPT) was designed to specifically measure context processing and has been widely applied to elucidate mechanisms of cognitive control and their impairments in conditions such as aging and schizophrenia. Here we present a large-sample, cross-sectional study of context processing aimed at characterizing its normal development from childhood to early adulthood (8 to 22 years old). We track the age-related changes in the standard AXCPT performance measures and also investigate their validity using detailed data-driven method. We show how critical maturational changes in context processing can be validly tracked from mid-adolescence onward with increasing reliance on preparatory, proactive strategies well into early adulthood. However, the early maturation from childhood into adolescence showed a sharp, two-fold discontinuity: while standard measures provide partially conflicting results suggesting an early worsening of proactive strategies, further analyses do not support their validity during this period. Our findings advocate the existence of multiple preparatory strategies that cannot be captured by indices that assume a simple dichotomy of proactive vs. reactive strategies. When evaluating context processing differences over development or in clinical populations, we advocate the explicit testing of the assumptions underlying standard AXCPT indices through complementary data-driven methods.
Article
Full-text available
In this review, we aim to highlight the application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) as a useful neuroimaging technique for the investigation of cognitive development. We focus on brain activation changes during the development of mathematics and language skills in schoolchildren. We discuss how technical limitations of common neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have resulted in our limited understanding of neural changes during development, while fNIRS would be a suitable and child-friendly method to examine cognitive development. Moreover, this technique enables us to go to schools to collect large samples of data from children in ecologically valid settings. Furthermore, we report findings of fNIRS studies in the fields of mathematics and language, followed by a discussion of the outlook of fNIRS in these fields. We suggest fNIRS as an additional technique to track brain activation changes in the field of educational neuroscience.
Article
Full-text available
Speed and accuracy of performance are central to many theoretical accounts of cognitive processing. In recent years, several integrated performance measures have been proposed. A comparative study of the available measures [Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 653–673. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0721-5] concluded that three of the measures, namely inverse efficiency score, rate correct score, and linear integrated speed-accuracy score achieved a balanced integration of speed and accuracy. As a follow-up on that study, these three measures were examined in data analyses from 13 (published and unpublished) experiments in the context of task switching. The correlations of the effect sizes in these integrated scores with the effect sizes obtained in latency and accuracy were high, but varied across the three integrated measures. The efficiency to detect effects supported by the speed and accuracy data was examined by means of signal detection analyses. The three measures efficiently detected effects present in either speed or accuracy, but the rate correct score was less efficient than the other two measures and it signalled a larger number of strong effects unsupported by the speed and accuracy data. It is concluded that while the rate correct score is better avoided, and the usage of the inverse efficiency score should be restricted to data with low overall error rates, the linear integrated speed-accuracy score proves to be valid.
Article
Cognitive control develops rapidly over the first decade of life, with one of the dominant changes being a transition from reliance on “as‐needed” control (reactive control) to a more planful, sustained form of control (proactive control). While the emergence of proactive control is important for mature behavior, we know little about how this transition takes place, the neural correlates of this transition, and whether development of executive functions influences the ability to adopt a proactive control strategy. The present study addresses these questions, focusing on the transition from reactive to proactive control in a cross‐sectional sample of 79 children – forty‐one 5‐year‐olds and thirty‐eight 9‐year‐olds. Children completed an adapted version of the AX‐Continuous Performance Task while electroencephalography was recorded and a standardized executive function battery was administered. Results revealed 5‐year‐olds predominantly employed reactive strategies, while 9‐year‐olds used proactive strategies. Use of proactive control was predicted by working memory ability, above and beyond other executive functions. Moreover, when enacting proactive control, greater increases in neural activity underlying working memory updating were observed; links between working memory ability and proactive control strategy use were mediated by such neural activity. These results provide convergent evidence that the transition from reactive to proactive control may be dependent on age‐related changes in neurocognitive indices of working memory and that working memory may influence adopting a proactive control strategy.