Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)
gaze is conspicuous
at ecologically‑relevant distances
Will Whitham1,2*, Steven J. Schapiro2, Jolyon Troscianko3 & Jessica L. Yorzinski1
Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) sclera appear much darker than the white sclera of human eyes, to such
a degree that the direction of chimpanzee gaze may be concealed from conspecics. Recent debate
surrounding this topic has produced mixed results, with some evidence suggesting that (1) primate
gaze is indeed concealed from their conspecics, and (2) gaze colouration is among the suite of traits
that distinguish uniquely social and cooperative humans from other primates (the cooperative eye
hypothesis). Using a visual modelling approach that properly accounts for specic‑specic vision, we
reexamined this topic to estimate the extent to which chimpanzee eye coloration is discriminable. We
photographed the faces of captive chimpanzees and quantied the discriminability of their pupil, iris,
sclera, and surrounding skin. We considered biases of cameras, lighting conditions, and commercial
photography software along with primate visual acuity, colour sensitivity, and discrimination ability.
Our visual modeling of chimpanzee eye coloration suggests that chimpanzee gaze is visible to
conspecics at a range of distances (within approximately 10 m) appropriate for many species‑typical
behaviours. We also found that chimpanzee gaze is discriminable to the visual system of primates
that chimpanzees prey upon, Colobus monkeys. Chimpanzee sclera colour does not eectively conceal
gaze, and we discuss this result with regard to the cooperative eye hypothesis, the evolution of
primate eye colouration, and methodological best practices for future primate visual ecology research.
e colours and patterns of primate faces are remarkably diverse, and are related to primate social ecology in
ways that suggest adaptive, communicative functions1. For example, more varied, complex, and colourful facial
patterning is most common in Old World primates that live in larger social groups2. Eye colours appear exemplary
of this relationship between morphology and social ecology. e sizes, shapes, and colours of primate eyes are
highly variable across species in ways that potentially aect their perceptibility and utility for communication3.
e cooperative eye hypothesis suggests that primate eyes are adapted to reveal or conceal gaze information4.
According to this hypothesis, lighter primate sclera, like those of humans, are discriminable, conspicuous, and
useful for gaze perception whereas darker primate sclera, like those of chimpanzees, are dark, obscured, and
camouaged in ways that impede gaze perception. e hypothesis suggests that these divergent phenotypes are
the product of evolutionary pressures to cooperate and communicate prosocially (with highly discriminable
gaze) or to avoid doing so (with eye colouration that conceals intraspecic cues), and that eye colouration thus
provides a window into the evolutionary history of primate social ecology. In support of this hypothesis, natural
human sclera colours are indeed highly perceptible, communicative cues. Newborn infants attend preferen-
tially to individuals who gaze directly at them5, and exhibit rudimentary gaze following6. By 18-months-of-age,
humans infants use gaze cues functionally to nd hidden items7. Human adults detect gaze information more
easily when sclera are naturally coloured, rather than darker or the same colour as the iris, across multiple light
environments8–10. e extent to which dark sclera in nonhuman primates are cryptic is less clear. Chimpanzees
have relatively dark sclera and follow the gaze of conspecics in their social group11 as well as the gaze of a com-
puterized conspecic on a computer monitor12. However, it is unclear in both of these cases whether chimpanzees
use conspecic eyes specically, rather than the head or body, to guide gaze following. More recent experimenta-
tion suggests that experimentally-enculturated chimpanzees are capable of discriminating computerized images
of averted chimpanzee eyes from images of chimpanzee eyes directed forward, albeit with less success than
when making the same discrimination of human eye images or chimpanzee eye images with reversed polarity13.
e cooperative eye hypothesis makes predictions about both primate eye colours and primate perception.
One approach to studying the cooperative eye hypothesis measures primate eye colours, then estimates whether
OPEN
1Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA. 2Department
of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, TX, USA. 3Centre for Ecology and
Conservation, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK. *email: wwhitham@tamu.edu
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
primates are likely to distinguish among those colours. For example, evidence that a species’ iris colors are
highly discriminable from its sclera colors would suggest that the species is capable of using conspecic eyes as
an informative cue in its social ecology. To this end, recent research has sought to quantify ape eye morphology
using commercial photo-editing soware and archival or publicly available photographs to estimate the relative
conspicuousness of ape eye colours. Perea-García and colleagues reported that chimpanzee sclera are conspicu-
ous, counter to the predictions of the cooperative eye hypothesis14. Mearing and Koops failed to replicate this
result using the same method, reporting that chimpanzee and bonobo sclera are indeed cryptic15. Mearing and
colleagues went on to report that phylogenetic analyses of 15 primate species suggested that patterns of scleral
pigmentation (e.g., in chimpanzees) and depigmentation (e.g., in humans) were fundamentally related to spe-
cies sociality, and were most likely to be functional adaptations for social cooperation or competition16. Other
researchers have included the relative degree of scleral exposure, the width/height ratio of the eye, and other
morphological measurements into such analyses. Mayhew & Gómez reported that the averted gaze of gorillas
(Gorilla gorilla) reveals similar amounts of visible sclera as a human’s averted gaze, and is not heavily pigmented in
many individuals17. Caspar and colleagues quantied ocular pigmentation and eye morphology across 15 homi-
noid and reported three unique phenotypic clusters: one that was uniquely human (bright, exposed sclera), one
that included gibbons, siamangs, and chimpanzees (cryptic, hidden sclera), and one that included the remaining
apes (variable pigmentation and morphology with brighter, more exposed sclera than the second cluster)18. Kano
etal. performed sophisticated image analyses on images of seven great ape species and reported that whereas
human eye outlines and iris colours were much more visually distinct in humans than other apes, ape eyes are
broadly conspicuous rather than cryptic19.
Taken together, these studies demonstrate wide variability in eye morphology of primates, with uncertain
functional signicance. A key limitation of these studies (noted by19) is their omission of several key dimensions
of animal perception. In particular, dierent species have dierent capacities to discriminate colour (e.g., sclera
colours that are discriminable to human perception are not necessarily discriminable to other species) and to
resolve ne detail (e.g., a given sclera may be conspicuous up to 5m in one species but only up to 2m in another
species). And, all species’ ability to resolve ne detail decreases with increasing distance (e.g., sclera that are
conspicuous at 5m may be cryptic at 10m within a given species)20,21. Furthermore, photographs of unknown
origin and uncontrolled lighting conditions, and soware that are tuned for human perception, are poor ts
for empirical modeling of nonhuman animal perception22,23. Because any hypothesized relationship between
ape eye morphology and social cognition depends on a conspecic observer’s ability to perceive and act on any
cues the eye morphology produces, analyses that account for the unique perception of the observer species are a
necessary part of understanding any hypothetical selection pressures for primate gaze colouration. For example,
species-specic visual modeling reveals that the gaze colouration of a New World monkey species, Sapajus apella,
is uniquely suited for signaling to conspecics and prospective predators, but not to prey24.
We tested the cooperative eye hypothesis using modern tools for modeling the species-specic and distance-
dependent determinants of animal perception with colour calibrated photographs of chimpanzee faces. Speci-
cally, we tested whether a perceptual model based on the chimpanzee visual system can discriminate among the
colours of four regions of interest (ROIs) involved in perceiving gaze—chimpanzee iris, pupil, sclera, and skin
(Fig.1)—at simulated distances up to 16m. Because the relative perceptibility of chimpanzee gaze may also aect
chimpanzees’ facility in interspecic interactions, we also tested whether the visual phenotype of chimpanzees’
primate prey, the colobus monkey (genus Colobus), can perceive chimpanzee gaze information. In doing so, we
oer a more complete account of how chimpanzee eye colour is perceived by chimpanzees and other primates.
Results
Perceptual modeling suggests that the chromatic (color) information of chimpanzee iris, sclera, and pupil are
not discriminable from each other at any distance (∆S discriminability measure values at all distances < thresh-
old value 3), nor are they discriminable from chimpanzee face skin, for either species (Fig.2). e achromatic
(luminance) information of these same regions are discriminable from each other (∆S discriminability measure
values > threshold value 3) by both species. Using the lower-bound of the highest posterior density interval as
criteria for discrimination (see “Methods”), chimpanzees and colobus can discriminate the iris from the pupil
using achromatic information when they are less than approximately 7m and 6m away, respectively. Similarly,
chimpanzees can discriminate the sclera from the pupil, iris, or skin when they are approximately 20m, 24m,
and 29m away, respectively. And, colobus can discriminate the sclera from the pupil, iris, or hair when they are
approximately 21m, 16m, and 21m away, respectively. e ability of chimpanzees and colobus to discriminate
ROIs is similar because they have similar color sensitivities and acuity (see “Methods”).
Discussion
Using a visual modelling approach, regions of chimpanzee eyes relevant to gaze perception are discriminable
from each other and from surrounding skin to both chimpanzees and colobus. Some discriminations (iris versus
skin, and pupil versus skin) are dicult at any distance, while any discrimination involving chimpanzee sclera
is likely possible at distances of 10m or greater. While we do not know the specic facial regions that chimpan-
zees and colobus rely on to inform their gaze discriminations, our analyses suggest that they can use contrasts
between the sclera and pupil, iris or surrounding skin to evaluate gaze. is result conforms to previous research
in which human experimenters reliably identied chimpanzee gaze direction when they were within 10m of
the chimpanzees25. A 10–20m distance at which chimpanzee sclera are conspicuous is likely to have many con-
sequences for chimpanzee ecology. Many chimpanzee social behaviours—mating, ghting, grooming, feeding,
play—necessarily occur within this distance. Outside this distance, it may be adaptive for gaze information to be
more cryptic. Similarly, predators benet by having their gaze cues hidden as they approach prey, as many prey
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure1. Chimpanzee face. (a) A sample photograph of a chimpanzee. (b) Enlarged view of the regions of the
chimpanzee’s eye region. (c) Example ROIs for this photograph with pupil outlined in red, iris in blue, sclera in
green, and skin in yellow. We omitted highlights in the ocular media, like the visible reection of the sky, from
all ROIs.
Figure2. Chromatic and achromatic ∆S values for contrasts among the eye regions shaded in the top-right
inset. A ∆S value greater than 3 at some distance suggest that the two ROI can be discriminated from each other.
Shaded regions around lines are Bayesian 94% Highest Posterior Density intervals around ∆S estimates. Tick
intervals on the x-axis are log-scaled.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
species decide whether to remain in place or ee on the basis of predator gaze26–28. It would be adaptive for chim-
panzee gaze cues to remain indiscriminable to Colobus monkeys as the apes approach, but the 10–20m distance
at which Colobus may discriminate chimpanzee gaze is likely benecial to the monkeys. is dynamic perhaps
underpins chimpanzee strategies for hunting Colobus that rely on coordinated pursuit rather than ambush29.
It is also probable that other determinants, aside from gaze perception, contribute to the evolution of facial
colouration. e colours of primate hair and skin demonstrably vary with a primate’s native geography, cli-
mate, and diurnality; the relationship between these determinants and social ecology is inconsistent1,2,30. ese
non-social determinants of primate colour extend even to the regions immediately surrounding the eyes, with
some Neotropical primates exhibiting dark eye masks that likely function as defense from UV radiation1. A
phylogenetically-controlled analysis of eye colour across the primate order that uses proper animal colouration
techniques could provide evidence that primate eye colour is broadly adapted to its social ecology. e coopera-
tive eye hypothesis would be supported if lighter sclera colours were visible to conspecics or predators at greater
distances than darker sclera. Absent such evidence, hypotheses that lighter sclera colours are of great functional
signicance in intraspecic signaling, or that the evolution of human sclera suggests a uniquely hyper-cooperative
evolutionary trajectory relative to other primates, are unsupported by data from primate eye colours.
Primate eyes vary along several morphological continua, and this extensive variability accordingly yields
high variability in the contexts in which primate eyes and gaze directions are discriminable. Our study mod-
eled and emphasized the distance-dependent and species-specic nature of visual perception. Future research
may extend these techniques with additional context—of how perception of chimpanzee eye colours changes in
dierent lighting environments, or of how discriminable the amount of visible sclera available during species-
typical social interactions is likely to be. e cooperative eye hypothesis regards chimpanzee sclera as exemplary
of cryptic eye colouration. Our analyses, along with recent work on the topic13,14,19 conrm that this is not so to
the chimpanzee visual system, and oer substantive context using established tools for studying animal sensory
ecology. Additional studies will be necessary to experimentally determine whether chimpanzees can discriminate
gaze and at what distance they can make these discriminations, while accounting for their specic visual system.
Methods
Photograph collection. We photographed captive chimpanzees housed at e University of Texas’ MD
Anderson Cancer Center’s Michale E. Keeling Center for Comparative Medicine and Research in April and
May of 2021. All work was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the Keeling Center
(IACUC approval number 0894-RN01), followed the guidelines of the Institute of Medicine on the use of chim-
panzees in research, complied with the Society for Neuroscience Policy on Ethics, and reported in accordance
with ARRIVE guidelines31. We used a Sony α7 II (ILCEm2) mirrorless digital camera (24 megapixel sensor) and
Sony lens with 70–350mm focal length for all photography. To standardize lighting as much as possible across
photographs we photographed animals outside at a distance of approximately 10m, in articial shade, only on
clear, sunny days (between 10:27 and 15:16) using the same aperture (f/6.3) and focal length (350mm) for all
photography. Because it was not feasible to place a photography reectance standard in the same lighting con-
ditions as unrestrained chimpanzees, we photographed a grey concrete region of the animals’ enclosure in the
same lighting as the animal moments aer taking each photograph of an animal (i.e., the sequential method32,33.
To identify the grey value appropriate for each concrete region of the animals’ enclosure, we took additional
photographs of the concrete alongside a 20% grey reectance standard (Spectralon). A total of 76 photos (11
chimpanzees, 1–23 photos per chimpanzee) were used for all analyses. Skin and iris ROIs were identiable in
every photograph, sclera ROIs were identiable in 73 photographs, and pupil ROIs were identiable in 23 pho-
tographs (due primarily to highlights in the ocular media masking pupil colouration).
Cone catch conversion and acuity correction. We used the micaToolbox (v.2.2.2) plugin for ImageJ
(v.1.53e) to transform the colour and luminance data of the RAW photographs (.ARW format) into forms rep-
resentative of chimpanzee and colobus colour vision22. Briey, a linearized set of pixel values was extracted
from each RAW photograph and normalized for dierences in lighting using the concrete grey standard photo-
graph taken immediately aer each chimpanzee photograph. en, the RGB sensitivities of our camera sensor
(included in a stock version of micaToolbox) were mapped to chimpanzee and colobus monkey colour sensitivi-
ties using a linear regression, resulting in pixel values representative of the animals’ colour vision phenotype.
We used the micaToolbox instantiation of AcuityView to transform photographs in ways representative of these
species’ vision at distances of 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 m20,34; Fig.3). e parameters of chimpanzee and colobus
vision on which these operations are based, and relevant citations, are listed in Table1.
Receptor noise limited modeling. We used chromatic41 and achromatic42 receptor noise limited (RNL)
modeling procedures to estimate the discriminability of eye and face colours of the transformed chimpanzee
photographs. is modeling yields psychometric distances (∆S) among regions of interest (ROI) that predict
whether or not ROI are likely discriminable from each other. We used a conservative criterion of 3 ∆S through-
out, such that ∆S > 3 are likely discriminable. Additional parameters of chimpanzee and colobus vision on which
RNL depends are listed in Table1.
Statistical modeling. In order to quantify uncertainty around ∆S values, we used ∆S values to estimate
94% Highest Posterior Density intervals (HPD) using a Bayesian logistic regression with parameter estimates for
each ROI contrast, vision phenotype, and distance. ese HPD intervals estimate the likeliest 94% of values for
an ROI contrast, and act as an easily interpreted decision criteria: if an HPD estimate for an ROI contrast is less
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
than or includes 3 ∆S, the ROI are plausibly not discriminable from each other. e regression also allowed for
inferences about the eect of distance beyond the range of values that were explicitly modeled using AcuityView.
Data availability
All data that were used as regression model input are publicly available in an OSF repository at https:// osf. io/
ap74f/? view_ only= 3da59 b82af 3d4a9 e9e4c 200b9 58c53 be.
Received: 17 February 2022; Accepted: 23 May 2022
References
1. Santana, S. E., Alfaro, J. L. & Alfaro, M. E. Adaptive evolution of facial colour patterns in Neotropical primates. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol.
Sci. 279, 2204–2211 (2012).
Figure3. Visualizations of the eect of cone catch conversion and acuity correction steps on a chimpanzee eye
as simulated in chimpanzee, colobus, and human vision phenotypes at simulated distances of 4, 8, and 16m. e
human vision phenotype is included only as a point of reference, and was not included in any analyses.
Table 1. Parameters of chimpanzee and colobus vision. † 0.22912878. †† 0.0572822. a Cone sensitivities from35;
acuity estimate from36; receptor noise estimated with cone type ratio 1:16:21 for SW:MW:LW from37. b Cone
sensitivities from38 that suggested uniform colour vision among Old World monkeys; acuity from macaque
estimate of39; receptor noise estimated with cone type ratio 1:16:16 for SW:MW:LW from40.
Wavelength of peak sensitivity for
cone type Receptor noise
Phenotype SW MW LW Acuity (cyc/deg) SW MW LW
Chimpanzeea430 535 562 72 ~ 0.229† ~ 0.057†† 0.05
Colobusb430 535 562 53.6 0.2 0.05 0.05
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
2. Santana, S. E., Alfaro, J. L., Noonan, A. & Alfaro, M. E. Adaptive response to sociality and ecology drives the diversication of
facial colour patterns in catarrhines. Nat. Commun. 4, 25 (2013).
3. Kobayashi, H. & Kohshima, S. Unique morphology of the human eye and its adaptive meaning: Comparative studies on external
morphology of the primate eye. J. Hum. Evol. 40, 419–435 (2001).
4. Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H. & Call, J. Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants:
e cooperative eye hypothesis. J. Hum. Evol. 52, 314–320 (2007).
5. Farroni, T. et al. Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Eects of contrast polarity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102,
17245–17250 (2005).
6. Farroni, T., Massaccesi, S., Pividori, D. & Johnson, M. H. Gaze following in newborns. Infancy 5, 39–60 (2004).
7. Itakura, S. & Tanaka, M. Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan
(Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 119–126 (1998).
8. Yorzinski, J. L., orstenson, C. A. & Nguyen, T. P. Sclera and iris color interact to inuence gaze perception. Front. Psychol. 12,
1–11 (2021).
9. Yorzinski, J. L., Harbourne, A. & ompson, W. Sclera color in humans facilitates gaze perception during daytime and nighttime.
PLoSOne 16, 1–15 (2021).
10. Yorzinski, J. L. & Miller, J. Sclera color enhances gaze perception in humans. PLoSOne 15, 1–14 (2020).
11. Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecics. Anim. Behav. 55, 1063–1069 (1998).
12. Kano, F. & Call, J. Cross-species variation in gaze following and conspecic preference among great apes, human infants and adults.
Anim. Behav. 91, 137–150 (2014).
13. Kano, F., Kawaguchi, Y. & Yeow, H. Experimental evidence for the gaze-signaling hypothesis: White sclera enhances the visibility
of eye gaze direction in humans and chimpanzees. bioRxiv 2021.09.21.461201 (2021).
14. Perea-García, J. O., Kret, M. E., Monteiro, A. & Hobaiter, C. Scleral pigmentation leads to conspicuous, not cryptic, eye morphol-
ogy in chimpanzees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 116, 19248–19250 (2019).
15. Mearing, A. S. & Koops, K. Quantifying gaze conspicuousness: Are humans distinct from chimpanzees and bonobos ?. J. Hum.
Evol. 157, 103043 (2021).
16. Mearing, A. S., Burkart, J. M., Dunn, J., Street, S. E. & Koops, K. e evolutionary origins of primate scleral coloration. bioRxiv
40, 2021.07.25.453695 (2021).
17. Mayhew, J. A. & Gómez, J. C. Gorillas with white sclera: A naturally occurring variation in a morphological trait linked to social
cognitive functions. Am. J. Primatol. 77, 869–877 (2015).
18. Caspar, K. R., Biggemann, M., Geissmann, T. & Begall, S. Ocular pigmentation in humans, great apes, and gibbons is not suggestive
of communicative functions. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–14 (2021).
19. Kano, F. et al. What is unique about the human eye? Comparative image analysis on the external eye morphology of human and
nonhuman great apes. Evol. Hum. Behav. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. evolh umbeh av. 2021. 12. 004 (2021).
20. Caves, E. M. & Johnsen, S. AcuityView: An r package for portraying the eects of visual acuity on scenes observed by an animal.
Methods Ecol. Evol. 9, 793–797 (2018).
21. Osorio, D. & Vorobyev, M. Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: Adaptations for luminance and colour vision.
Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 272, 1745–1752 (2005).
22. Troscianko, J. & Stevens, M. Image calibration and analysis toolbox—a free soware suite for objectively measuring reectance,
colour and pattern. Methods Ecol. Evol. 6, 1320–1331 (2015).
23. Stevens, M., Párraga, C. A., Cuthill, I. C., Partridge, J. C. & Troscianko, T. S. Using digital photography to study animal coloration.
Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 90, 211–237 (2007).
24. Whitham, W., Schapiro, S. J., Troscianko, J. & Yorzinski, J. L. e gaze of a social monkey is perceptible to conspecics and preda-
tors but not prey. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 20, 10 (2002).
25. B ethell, E. J., Vick, S. & Bard, K. A. Measurement of eye-gaze in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am. J. Primatol. 69, 562–575 (2007).
26. Sreekar, R. & Quader, S. Inuence of gaze and directness of approach on the escape responses of the Indian rock lizard, Psam-
mophilus dorsalis (Gray, 1831). J. Biosci. 38, 829–833 (2013).
27. Lee, S. et al. Direct look from a predator shortens the risk-assessment time by prey. PLoSOne 8, 1–7 (2013).
28. Carter, J., Lyons, N. J., Cole, H. L. & Goldsmith, A. R. Subtle cues of predation risk: Starlings respond to a predator’s direction of
eye-gaze. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 275, 1709–1715 (2008).
29. Newton-Fisher, N. E. Chimpanzee hunting. Behav. Handb. Paleoanthropol. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ 978-3- 540- 33761-4_ 42. (2007).
30. Caro, T. et al. e evolution of primate coloration revisited. Behav. Ecol. 32, 555–567 (2021).
31. Kilkenny, C., Browne, W., Cuthill, I. C., Emerson, M. & Altman, D. G. Animal research: Reporting invivo experiments: e
ARRIVE guidelines. Br. J. Pharmacol. 160, 1577–1579 (2010).
32. Bergman, T. J. & Beehner, J. C. A simple method for measuring colour in wild animals: Validation and use on chest patch colour
in geladas (eropithecus gelada). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 94, 231–240 (2008).
33. Stevens, M., Stoddard, M. C. & Higham, J. P. Studying primate color: Towards visual system-dependent methods. Int. J. Primatol.
30, 893–917 (2009).
34. van den Berg, C. P., Troscianko, J., Endler, J. A., Marshall, N. J. & Cheney, K. L. Quantitative Colour Pattern Analysis (QCPA): A
comprehensive framework for the analysis of colour patterns in nature. Methods Ecol. Evol. 11, 316–332 (2020).
35. Deeb, S. S., Jorgensen, A. L., Battisti, L., Iwasaki, L. & Motulsky, A. G. Sequence divergence of the red and green visual pigments
in great apes and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 7262–7266 (1994).
36. Matsuzawa, T. Form perception and visual acuity. Folia Primatol. Int. J. Primatol. 55, 24–32 (1990).
37. Jacobs, G. H., Deegan, J. F. & Moran, J. L. ERG measurements of the spectral sensitivity of common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes).
Vis. Res. 36, 2587–2594 (1996).
38. Jacobs, G. H. & Deegan, J. F. Uniformity of colour vision in Old World monkeys. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 266, 2023–2028 (1999).
39. Kemp, A. D. & Christopher Kirk, E. Eye size and visual acuity inuence vestibular anatomy in mammals. Anat. Rec. 297, 781–790
(2014).
40. Osorio, D., Smith, A. C., Vorobyev, M. & Buchanan-Smith, H. M. Detection of fruit and the selection of primate visual pigments
for color vision. Am. Nat. 164, 696–708 (2004).
41. Vorobyev, M. & Osorio, D. Receptor noise as a determinant of colour threshoIds. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 265, 351–358 (1998).
42. Siddiqi, A., Cronin, T. W., Loew, E. R., Vorobyev, M. & Summers, K. Interspecic and intraspecic views of color signals in the
strawberry poison frog Dendrobates pumilio. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 2471–2485 (2004).
Acknowledgements
e research was supported by National Science Foundation (BCS #1926327), the College of Agriculture and
Life Sciences at Texas A&M University, and Texas A&M AgriLife Research funding to J.Y. and W.W.; and S.S.
were supported by the National Science Foundation funding (BCS #1926327).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:9249 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13273-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Author contributions
W.W.: data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, soware, visualization, writing—original dra;
S.S.: funding acquisition, project administration, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing; J.T.: fund-
ing acquisition, methodology, soware, supervision, writing—review and editing; J.Y.: conceptualization, funding
acquisition, methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing.
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to W.W.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
© e Author(s) 2022
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.