ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Although people derive substantial benefit from social connection, they often refrain from talking to strangers because they have pessimistic expectations about how such conversations will go (e.g., they believe they will be rejected or not know what to say). Previous research has attempted but failed to get people to realize that their concerns about talking to strangers are overblown. To reduce people's fears, we developed an intervention in which participants played a week-long scavenger hunt game that involved repeatedly finding, approaching, and talking to strangers. Compared to controls, this minimal, easily replicable treatment made people less pessimistic about the possibility of rejection and more optimistic about their conversational ability—and these benefits persisted for at least a week after the study ended. Daily reports revealed that people's expectations grew more positive and accurate by the day, emphasizing the importance of repeated experience in improving people's attitudes towards talking with strangers.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
0022-1031/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Talking to strangers: A week-long intervention reduces psychological
barriers to social connection
Gillian M. Sandstrom
a
,
*
,
1
, Erica J. Boothby
b
,
1
, Gus Cooney
b
,
1
a
University of Sussex, United Kingdom
b
University of Pennsylvania, United States of America
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Social interaction
Conversation
Intervention
Social connection
ABSTRACT
Although people derive substantial benet from social connection, they often refrain from talking to strangers
because they have pessimistic expectations about how such conversations will go (e.g., they believe they will be
rejected or not know what to say). Previous research has attempted but failed to get people to realize that their
concerns about talking to strangers are overblown. To reduce peoples fears, we developed an intervention in
which participants played a week-long scavenger hunt game that involved repeatedly nding, approaching, and
talking to strangers. Compared to controls, this minimal, easily replicable treatment made people less pessimistic
about the possibility of rejection and more optimistic about their conversational abilityand these benets
persisted for at least a week after the study ended. Daily reports revealed that peoples expectations grew more
positive and accurate by the day, emphasizing the importance of repeated experience in improving peoples
attitudes towards talking with strangers.
Research on well-being, conversation, and belonging has under-
scored the importance of social interaction for peoples health and
happiness (Clark & Watson, 1988; Diener & Seligman, 2002; Hawkley &
Cacioppo, 2010; Helliwell & Putnam, 2004; Holt-Lunstad, Robles, &
Sbarra, 2017; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988; Kahneman, Krueger,
Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, 2004; Mehl, Vazire, Holleran, & Clark,
2010; Myers, 2000; Pavot, Diener, & Fujita, 1990; Sun, Harris, & Vazire,
2020; Vittengl & Holt, 1998; Watson, Clark, McIntyre, & Hamaker,
1992). Despite the benets of social interaction, people seldom strike up
conversations with people they do not know. Instead, people wear
headphones to avoid talking, stay glued to their smartphones in public
places, or pretend not to notice a new coworker they still have not
introduced themselves to (Goffman, 1963; Kushlev, Hunter, Proulx,
Pressman, & Dunn, 2019). These impressive displays of civil inatten-
tion,seemingly innocuous dodges in the moment, can collectively add
up to a behavioral pattern that stymies social interaction before it begins
(Kim, 2012; Zuckerman, Miserandino, & Bernieri, 1983).
Recent research suggests one reason for this behavior: people are
remarkably pessimistic about almost every aspect of talking to strangers.
For example, people expect that others will not be interested in talking
to them (Atir, Wald, & Epley, 2021; Cooney, Boothby, & Schweitzer,
2021; Epley & Schroeder, 2014; Schroeder, Lyons, & Epley, 2021),
people underestimate how much others like them after meeting for the
rst time (Boothby, Cooney, Sandstrom, & Clark, 2018; Mastroianni,
Cooney, Boothby, & Reece, 2021), and people are skeptical of their
ability to start and maintain such conversations (Atir et al., 2021;
Sandstrom & Boothby, 2021). In a striking demonstration of this basic
idea, Epley and Schroeder (2014) approached people waiting for a train
in Chicago and showed that they systematically underestimated how
rewarding it would be to strike up a conversation with a stranger during
their commute. Our aim in this paper is twofold. First, to replicate past
research showing that people underestimate how positively strangers
will react to attempts at social connection. And second, to intervene on
peoples pessimistic beliefs, thereby allowing people to see the possi-
bility of talking to a stranger as a positive opportunity rather than
something to dread.
Fortunately, the same research that demonstrates peoples pessi-
mism about talking to strangers also shows that this pessimism is often
misplacedstrangers are more willing to talk than people anticipate
(Cooney et al., 2021; Epley & Schroeder, 2014; Sandstrom & Boothby,
This paper has been recommended for acceptance by Dr Karina Schumann
* Corresponding author at: School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RH, United Kingdom.
E-mail address: g.sandstrom@sussex.ac.uk (G.M. Sandstrom).
1
All authors contributed equally to this paper.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104356
Received 4 January 2022; Received in revised form 11 May 2022; Accepted 12 May 2022
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
2
2021; Schroeder et al., 2021), conversations tend to go overwhelmingly
better than people predict (Epley & Schroeder, 2014; Sandstrom &
Boothby, 2021; Sandstrom & Dunn, 2014), and people are more
conversationally competent than they expect (Sandstrom & Boothby,
2021; Welker, Walker, Boothby, & Gilovich, 2021). This suggests a
promising intervention strategy: give people concentrated and repeated
practice talking to strangers, so that they may realize their fears are
exaggerated.
There have been surprisingly few attempts to improve peoples at-
titudes towards talking to strangers, and those attempts have proved
only partially successful (e.g., Sandstrom & Boothby, 2021). Here, we
added to this new body of research, and aimed to improve on prior
intervention efforts by creating a more lasting shift in peoples attitudes.
We did so by having people play a scavenger hunt game that prompted
them to engage in repeated conversations with strangers over the course
of a week. Why a whole week?
It would seem that sporadic positive interactions with strangers, like
the ones that people have in their day-to-day lives, do not fully allay
peoples fears, and that the lessons from such positive interactions do
not stick. Perhaps it is too easy for people to discount any single con-
versation that goes well as a pleasant exception to the unpleasant rule.
Or perhaps because people talk to strangers relatively infrequently, it is
difcult to detect a consistent pattern. For these reasons, we aimed to
alter peoples entrenched pessimism by having people repeatedly
practice talking over the course of many consecutive days. Overall, our
aim was to condition people to the (surprisingly positive) reality of
talking to strangers, thereby reducing peoples fears, and increasing
peoples recognition that these conversations typically go quite well.
1. Current research
We ran a week-long, multi-site intervention, in which participants
were prompted by a scavenger hunt app to either start conversations
with, or simply observe, at least one stranger every day for a week. As
the intervention unfolded, we measured peoples fears about rejection,
their beliefs about their conversational ability, their expected
awkwardness and enjoyment, and their beliefs about the impression
they make on strangers. We aimed to nd out whether repeatedly
talking to strangers over the course of a week could make people less
pessimistic about the prospect of talking to strangers.
2. Method
This manuscript reports the results of a time-consuming and
resource-intensive intervention. Two consequences are that: (a) we pre-
registered several research questions that could be answered with the
resulting data; and (b) there were some slight deviations from our pre-
registered plan.
(a) This paper reports the results of one of our pre-registered research
questions (Q1): Do repeated interactions with strangers improve
attitudes towards talking to strangers? (see https://osf.
io/dvqez/). Correspondingly, we analyze only the measures
that are associated with this research question. For example, the
pre-registration contains measures related to whether repeated
interactions with strangers increase social connection, but we do
not address this question in the current manuscript, as it is related
to a different stream of work. See OSF for complete materials.
(b) Analyses. Although we did not conduct the exact analyses that we
had planned, they were conceptually very similar: we used con-
dition, time, and condition x time as predictors of our outcomes,
and focused on the specied comparisons that we pre-registered
(baseline vs. end-of-study, and baseline vs. follow-up). For ana-
lyses of rejection, our count data were over-dispersed, so we used
a negative binomial regression to analyze these data. Upon
further reection we also chose a regression framework to
analyze the other outcomes as well, instead of using the ANOVA
framework that we had pre-registered.
Sample size was determined before any data analysis, and we report
all manipulations and exclusions. Finally, please note that a subset of the
data reported here were included as unpublished data in a meta-analysis
(Sandstrom & Boothby, 2021; see p. 2 of the SOM for more details).
2.1. Participants
A total of 454 people started the study (see Fig. 1). Twenty-one were
removed from analyses due to experimenter error. Of the remaining 433,
136 were removed due to pre-registered exclusion criteria: 68 completed
fewer than four days (12% of participants in the control condition; 17%
of participants in the treatment condition), a rate that is similar to past
intervention studies (e.g., 15% of participants in Fredrickson, Cohn,
Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008 were excluded for not attending enough
sessions or not completing enough weekly reports). See SOM for ana-
lyses of participants who dropped out versus those who nished, and see
Discussion for implications of these ndings. A further 68 participants
failed our honesty check (11% of participants in the control condition,
18% of participants in the treatment condition), a rate that is similar to
an unpublished study in our lab (15%). In the main manuscript, we
analyze the responses of the remaining 286 people (75 male, 209 female,
2 identied otherwise or preferred not to say; M
age
=20.1 years, SD =
2.1 years), but analyses including the participants who failed the
honesty check leave the results unchanged (see the SOM for details).
This sample was recruited from two university campusesone in the
U.S. (N =135) and one in the U.K. (N =151). The majority of partici-
pants received course credit, but some were paid. A sensitivity analysis
using G*Power (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) suggests that
our sample can detect effects where f 0.08 (i.e., small effects). See
SOM for payment details, full details of analyses testing the effects of
attrition and exclusion criteria, additional details about the sensitivity
analysis, and a discussion of self-selection.
2.2. Procedure
We created a scavenger hunt game using a mobile app called
GooseChase (GooseChase Adventures, 2019). Participants used the app
to complete the study. We designed 29 scavenger hunt missionsthat
people could choose from. The goal of each mission was to nd a
stranger with certain characteristics (e.g., nd someone wearing
interesting shoesor nd someone whos drinking a coffee; see Ap-
pendix for full list of missions). Participants received points on the app
and an entry into a draw prize for every mission that they completed.
The app allowed participants to see their performance (i.e., the number
of missions they had completed) compared to other participants.
For logistical reasons, participants were recruited in weekly groups.
On Monday, participants were brought to the lab to complete surveys
(start of study and the rst pre-conversation surveys; see Measures), to
receive instructions, and to download and try out the scavenger hunt
app (see Fig. 2; see SOM for more details on the procedure). After the lab
visit, all communication (e.g., daily game codes and reminders to
complete surveys) occurred by text message and email. All members of
that weeks group were assigned to the same condition: either a treat-
ment condition (i.e., nd, approach, and talk to a stranger; N =198), or a
control condition (i.e., nd, approach, but simply observe a stranger; N
=88). Participants in the treatment condition either saw tips in their
mission descriptions (N =98) or not (N =100), which is why the
treatment condition was double the size of the control. As pre-registered,
we collapsed across this factor for the current project, creating a single
treatment condition (see SOM for details). In total, participants in the
treatment condition had 1336 conversations with strangers.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
3
2.3. Measures
For one weekdaily from Monday to Fridayparticipants played a
scavenger hunt game that involved nding a stranger, and then either
talking to (treatment) or observing that person (control). Participants
completed both Generaland Daily surveys.
The General surveys were designed to capture the broader attitudinal
and behavioral changes elicited by the intervention. Participants in both
the treatment and control conditions completed general surveys at the
start of study on Monday (see Fig. 1; Phase 1), at the end of study on
Friday (see Fig. 1; Phase 3), and at follow-up, one week after the inter-
vention had ended (see Fig. 1; Phase 4).
Participants also completed two types of Daily surveys: one at the
beginning of each day, and one after completing each mission (see Fig. 1;
Phases 13). Our primary interest in the daily surveys was to examine
the time course of the intervention, and the underlying psychological
processes of those in the treatment group. Participants in the treatment
group made predictions about how their conversations would go on each
day, in what we refer to as the pre-conversation survey, and reported on
their experiences after completing their daily mission(s) in the post-
conversation survey. To ensure that all participants had a similar expe-
rience, we asked control group participants to also complete the daily
surveys, even though we did not analyze their responses; control par-
ticipants reported on their current mood in the rst survey, and
described the person that they observed in the second survey.
2.3.1. Rejection
We asked participants whether they thought the people they
approached would be willing to talk to them. In the General surveys, we
asked How many people do you think you will need to approach in
order to complete a mission (i.e., get someone to talk to you)?, and
instructed them, for example, to enter 1if they thought the rst person
they approached would talk to them. In the Daily surveys, the pre-
conversation survey asked How many people do you think you will
need to approach in order to complete your mission today (i.e., get
someone to talk to you)?, and instructed people to enter 1if they
thought the rst person they approached would talk to them. In the post-
conversation survey, we asked people to enter 1if the rst person they
approached talked to them, otherwise If the rst person did not want to
talk, please enter the number of people you approached, including the
Fig. 1. Flowchart depicting participant drop-outs and exclusions.
Note. See SOM for analyses of participants who dropped out versus those who nished, and see Discussion for implications of these ndings.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
4
person who did talk to you.For interpretability, we created a measure
of rejection by subtracting one from the number of expected/actual ap-
proaches (e.g., expecting to need to approach two people means
expecting to be rejected by one person).
2.3.2. Conversational ability
We asked participants how skilled they were in talking to strangers
(General survey: It is hard [to start a conversation / to keep a conver-
sation going / to end a conversation] with a stranger; Daily survey: It
[will be / was] hard [to start a conversation / to keep a conversation
going / to end a conversation]). We created two composite measures of
conversational ability, both assessed on a 7-point scale. The rst was
based on the General survey items, which were each measured at three
time points (
α
start_of_study
=0.60,
α
end_of_study
=0.63,
α
follow-up
=0.70),
and the second was based on the Daily surveys (
α
daily
=0.62). Since
these items concern peoples perceptions of difculty,they were
reverse-coded before averaging to yield a measure of conversational
ability.
2.3.3. Awkwardness and enjoyment
We asked people how they felt about talking to strangers. We
assessed both negative and positive feelings, in both the General surveys
and Daily surveys, creating a composite for each: General awkwardness:
I feel [comfortable (reverse-scored) / awkward / nervous] talking to
strangers, (
α
start_of_study
=0.85,
α
end_of_study
=0.83,
α
follow-up
=0.81);
Daily awkwardness: I [will feel/felt] [comfortable (reverse-scored) /
awkward / nervous](
α
daily
=0.82); General enjoyment: I enjoy talk-
ing to strangers,When I talk to strangers, I [nd them interesting / like
them], Talking to strangers feels like [work (reverse-scored) / fun]
(
α
start_of_study
=0.84,
α
end_of_study
=0.84,
α
follow-up
=0.84); Daily enjoy-
ment: I [will enjoy talking to / will like / enjoyed talking to / liked] my
conversation partners,” “I [will nd/found] my conversation partners
interesting,” “The conversations [will feel / felt] like [work (reverse-
scored) / fun](
α
daily
=0.84). The General measures were assessed on a
7-point scale, and the Daily measures were assessed on a 5-point scale.
2.3.4. Positive impression
Participants reported what kind of impression they would make
when talking to strangers (General: The strangers I talk to [like me /
nd me interesting / enjoy talking to me; Daily: My conversation
partners [will like me / will nd me interesting / will enjoy talking to me
/ liked me / found me interesting / enjoyed talking to me.) We then
averaged these measures to create a composite for both the General
surveys (
α
start_of_study
=0.92,
α
end_of_study
=0.90,
α
follow-up
=0.92) and the
Daily surveys (
α
daily
=0.86). The General measure was assessed on a 7-
point scale, and the Daily measure was assessed on a 5-point scale.
2.3.5. Initiating conversations with strangers
Although our study was designed to test for changes in attitudes to-
wards talking to strangers, we included an exploratory measure to test
for changes in behavior: How many strangers have YOU started a con-
versation with in the past 7 days?We included some examples of
conversations with strangers: chatting with the barista, talking to
someone while waiting in a queue/line, talking to a new classmate...
This item was included in the General surveys at the start of study and at
the follow-up; we didnt include it in the end of study survey because we
wanted a measure of spontaneous conversations with strangers, not the
conversations that were required by participation in our study.
2.3.6. Noticing opportunities to talk to strangers
To test whether our intervention affected peoples awareness of op-
portunities to talk to strangers, we also included an exploratory measure
in the General surveys, on a 7-point scale: I notice opportunities to talk
to strangers.
2.3.7. Demographic items
At the beginning of the intervention, people reported their de-
mographic information.
Please see the SOM for information about additional measures that
were assessed but not included in this paper (a couple of items in the
Daily surveys that were not relevant to the research question addressed
in this paper; items in the General surveys that do not match the items in
the Daily surveys), and see OSF for complete materials.
3. Results
3.1. General results: Change in perceived likelihood of rejection
At the start of the study, at the end of the study, and at a follow-up
one week later, participants in the treatment condition predicted how
likely they were to be rejected in their attempts to start a conversation
with a stranger. Due to over-dispersed count data, we used a negative
binomial regression to analyze these data. Because of the nested struc-
ture of the data, we used a mixed-effects model with a random intercept
for participant. We estimated the model using restricted maximum
likelihood. Time (start of study, end of study, or follow-up) and
Fig. 2. Phases of the talking to strangersintervention study.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
5
condition (treatment or control) were included as xed categorical in-
dependent variables (i.e., DV ~ Condition*Time +(1 | ParticipantId).
We report the most relevant post-hoc contrasts with the estimated means
for predicted rejections on the original scale, while using a multivariate t
distribution to correct p-values and condence intervals for multiple
comparisons.
At the end of the week-long intervention, participants in the treat-
ment group expected to be rejected by signicantly fewer people than
they had at the start of the study (p <.001; see Table 1 and Fig. 3).
Additionally, one week after the intervention had ended, participants in
the treatment group still expected to be rejected by fewer people,
showing a lasting effect of our intervention (p <.001).
What about participants in the control group, who simply observed
strangers for a week, but did not actually strike up conversations? They
did not make predictions about rejection at the start of the study, in
order to preserve the integrity of the control condition. However, at the
end of the study, they were asked to imagine continuing the scavenger
hunt for another week, but this time talking to people rather than simply
observing them.
Participants in the control condition predicted that they would be
rejected by as many people as participants in the treatment group had
predicted at the start of the study, ratio of control to treatment =1.11,
95% CI =[0.70, 1.74], p =.97. As expected, the beliefs of participants in
the control group did not change from the end of the study to the follow-
up, ratio of end of study to follow-up =0.89, 95% CI =[0.57, 1.38], p =
.95. At the end of the week-long study and a full week later, participants
in the control condition resembled participants in the treatment condi-
tion at the start of the study, who had overly pessimistic expectations
about the amount of rejection they would experience.
Directly comparing treatment and control conditions revealed that
participants in the treatment group expected signicantly less rejection
compared to those in the control groupboth at the end of the study (p
<.001) and at the follow-up (p <.001).
As shown in the left panel of Fig. 3, it seems that a week-long
intervention involving repeated conversations with strangers substan-
tially reduced peoples fears about rejection compared to controls, and
these reductions persisted for at least one week following the end of the
intervention.
3.2. General results: Change in conversational ability
To examine changes in perceived conversational ability over the
course of the intervention, we used the same model as previously
specied, except here we used a mixed-effects regression instead of a
negative binomial.
As shown in Table 1 and the right panel of Fig. 3, participants in the
treatment group felt more positive about their general ability to talk to
strangers at the end of the study (p <.001), as well as at the follow-up
one week later (p <.001), compared to how they felt at the start of
the study. Meanwhile, participants in the control group did not experi-
ence the same improvement in their perceived ability to carry out a
conversation. At the start of the study, participants in the treatment and
control groups did not differ in their feelings about their ability to talk to
strangers (p =.88), but at the end of the study and a week later, par-
ticipants in the treatment group felt more positive than participants in
the control group about their ability to talk to strangers (p =.01, p =
.004).
3.3. General results: Change in awkwardness
Participants in the treatment group felt less awkward about talking
to strangers at the end of the study (p <.001), and a week later (p <
.001), compared to how they felt at the beginning of the study (see
Table 1 and the left panel of Fig. 4). Participants in the control group
reported no change in awkwardness between the beginning of the study
and either the end of the study (p =.54) or a week later (p =.99).
Directly comparing treatment and control conditions revealed no
difference in predicted awkwardness at the start of the study (p =.87),
but participants in the treatment group expected to feel less awkward
than those in the control group, both at the end of the study (p =.05) and
at the follow-up (p =.03).
3.4. General results: Change in enjoyment
Participants in the treatment group expected to enjoy their conver-
sations with strangers more at the end of the intervention (p <.001) and
a week later (p =.002), compared to how they had felt at the start of the
study. Meanwhile, participants in the control group did not report a
change in how much they expected to enjoy conversations with
strangers from the start of the study to the end of the study (p =.18) or to
the one-week follow-up (p =.51).
Directly comparing treatment and control conditions revealed no
difference in how much people expected to enjoy conversations with
strangers at the start of the study (p =.99), but also, unexpectedly, no
difference at the end of the study (p =.83), or at the follow-up (p =.99).
In short, as shown in Table 1 and the middle panel of Fig. 4, par-
ticipants in the treatment condition showed an improvement in their
expectations of how positively they would feel about their conversa-
tions, which lasted a week after the intervention ended, and although
participants in the control condition did not experience this improve-
ment, the difference between treatment and control was not signicant.
3.5. General results: Change in making a positive impression
Participants in the treatment group expected to make a more positive
impression after talking to strangers for a week (p <.001), and a week
after the study had ended (p <.001), compared to the start of the study.
Participants in the control condition exhibited similar, though smaller,
effects; they expected to make a more positive impression when talking
to strangers at the end of the study (p =.01) and a week later (p =.004),
compared to their predictions at the start of the study.
Directly comparing treatment and control conditions revealed no
difference in expectations about making a positive impression when
talking to strangers at the start of the study (p =.94), but also, unex-
pectedly, no difference at the end of the study (p =.63), or at the follow-
up (p =.79).
In sum, as shown in Table 1 and the right panel of Fig. 4, participants
in the treatment condition showed an improvement in their beliefs about
how positive of an impression they made, which lasted a week after the
intervention ended. However, note that participants in the control
condition also showed some improvement. It appears that simply
approaching and observing strangers also made people less pessimistic
about the impressions they would make, perhaps because it lowered
peoples anxiety, although we urge caution in interpreting this unpre-
dicted result.
3.6. Exploratory analyses: Changes in initiating conversations and
noticing opportunities
3.6.1. Initiating conversations
Participants in the treatment condition reported more positive atti-
tudes towards talking to strangers after completing the intervention. Did
these more positive attitudes lead people to have more conversations
with strangers? After removing responses more than three standard
deviations from the mean (e.g., some participants reported talking to
fty or more strangers as part of their job), we found that, at the one-
week follow-up, participants in the treatment group reported having
had more conversations with strangers in the previous week than they
had had in the week prior to the start of the study, p =.01, whereas
participants in the control group reported no change, p =.88 (see
Table 1). However, directly comparing treatment and control conditions
revealed no difference in how many conversations with strangers
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
6
Table 1
Descriptive and inferential statistics for the general survey results.
Measure Time/
Comparison
Treatment Control Treatment vs. Control
Rejection Start of study M =0.89, SE =0.09 n/a n/a
End of Study M =0.23, SE =0.04 M =0.99, SE =0.14 Ratio ¼4.22, 95% CI ¼[2.47, 7.20],
p < .001
Follow-up M =0.32, SE =0.05 M =1.11, SE =0.17 Ratio ¼3.49, 95% CI ¼[2.00, 6.09],
p < .001
Start vs. End Ratio ¼3.82, 95% CI ¼[2.56, 5.69],
p < .001
n/a
Start vs. Follow-
up
Ratio ¼2.81, 95% CI ¼[1.87, 4.22],
p < .001
n/a
Convo ability Start of study M =4.34, SE =0.08 M =4.20, SE =0.12 ΔM = − 0.14, 95% CI =[0.54, 0.25],
t(499) = − 0.97, p =.88, d =0.12
End of Study M =4.77, SE =0.08 M =4.28, SE =0.12 ΔM ¼ ¡0.50, 95% CI ¼[¡0.89,¡0.11],
t(498) ¼ ¡3.38, p ¼.01, d ¼0.43
Follow-up M =4.81, SE =0.09 M =4.27, SE =0.13 ΔM ¼ ¡0.54, 95% CI ¼[¡0.96, ¡0.12],
t(582) ¼ ¡3.44, p ¼.004, d ¼0.46
Start vs. End ΔM ¼ ¡0.43, 95% CI ¼[¡0.64, ¡0.23],
t(506) ¼ ¡5.70,p < .001, d ¼0.37
ΔM = − 0.08, 95% CI =[0.38, 0.22],
t(505) = − 0.70, p =.97, d =0.07
Start vs. Follow-
up
ΔM ¼ ¡0.47, 95% CI ¼[¡0.69, ¡0.25],
t(522) ¼ ¡5.64, p < .001, d ¼0.41
ΔM = − 0.07, 95% CI =[0.40, 0.26],
t(518) = − 0.58, p =.99, d =0.06
Awkwardness Start of study M =3.78, SE =0.09 M =3.94, SE =0.14 ΔM =0.16, 95% CI =[0.28, 0.61],
t(420) =0.97, p =.87, d =0.12
End of Study M =3.34, SE =0.09 M =3.78, SE =0.14 ΔM ¼0.45, 95% CI ¼[0.001, 0.89],
t(420) ¼2.66. p ¼.05, d ¼0.35
Follow-up M =3.45, SE =0.10 M =3.95, SE =0.15 ΔM ¼0.51, 95% CI ¼[0.04, 0.97],
t(490) ¼2.87. p ¼.03, d ¼0.40
Start vs. End ΔM ¼0.45, 95% CI ¼[0.25, 0.64],
t(505) ¼6.18, p < .001, d ¼0.33
ΔM =0.16, 95% CI =[0.12, 0.45],
t(505) =1.51, p =.54, d =0.12
Start vs. Follow-
up
ΔM ¼0.33, 95% CI ¼[0.12, 0.54],
t(516) ¼4.19, p < .001, d ¼0.26
ΔM = − 0.01, 95% CI =[0.32, 0.30],
t(514) = − 0.08, p =.99, d =0.05
Enjoyment Start of study M =4.34, SE =0.08 M =4.37, SE =0.11 ΔM =0.03, 95% CI =[0.33, 0.39],
t(422) =0.20, p =.99, d =0.03
End of Study M =4.70, SE =0.08 M =4.55, SE =0.11 ΔM = − 0.14, 95% CI =[0.50, 0.22],
t(422) = − 1.06. p =.83, d =0.14
Follow-up M =4.58, SE =0.08 M =4.51, SE =0.12 ΔM = − 0.07, 95% CI =[0.44, 0.31],
t(493) = − 0.46. p =.99, d =0.07
Start vs. End ΔM ¼ ¡0.36, 95% CI ¼[¡0.52, ¡0.20],
t(505) ¼ ¡6.13, p < .001, d ¼0.34
ΔM = − 0.19, 95% CI =[0.42, 0.04],
t(505) = − 2.14, p =.18, d =0.17
Start vs. Follow-
up
ΔM ¼ ¡0.24, 95% CI ¼[¡0.41, ¡0.07],
t(516) ¼ ¡3.71, p ¼.002, d ¼0.19
ΔM = − 0.15, 95% CI =[0.40, 0.11],
t(514) = − 1.55, p =.51, d =0.07
Positive
impression
Start of study M =4.25, SE =0.07 M =4.14, SE =0.11 ΔM = − 0.10, 95% CI =[0.45, 0.25],
t(476) = − 0.79, p =.94, d =0.11
End of Study M =4.62, SE =0.07 M =4.44, SE =0.11 ΔM = − 0.18, 95% CI =[0.53, 0.17],
t(476) = − 1.37. p =.63, d =0.18
Follow-up M =4.66, SE =0.08 M =4.50, SE =0.12 ΔM = − 0.16, 95% CI =[0.53, 0.21],
t(557) = − 1.14. p =.79, d =0.14
Start vs. End ΔM ¼ ¡0.38, 95% CI ¼[¡0.55, ¡0.20],
t(506) ¼ ¡5.80, p < .001, d ¼0.38
ΔM ¼ ¡0.30, 95% CI ¼[¡0.56,
¡0.04],
t(506) ¼ ¡3.08, p ¼.01, d ¼0.28
Start vs. Follow-
up
ΔM ¼ ¡0.41, 95% CI ¼[¡0.60, ¡0.22],
t(521) ¼ ¡5.79, p < .001, d ¼0.36
ΔM ¼ ¡0.36, 95% CI ¼[¡0.64,
¡0.08],
t(518) ¼ ¡3.42, p ¼.004, d ¼0.27
Talking to
strangers
Start of study M =4.68, SE =0.33 M =5.70, SE =0.48 ΔM =1.01, 95% CI =[0.43, 2.45],
t(421) =1.73, p =.26, d =0.24
Follow-up M =5.81, SE =0.36 M =5.32, SE =0.53 ΔM = − 0.49, 95% CI =[2.07, 1.10],
t(464) = − 0.76, p =.85, d =0.10
Start vs. Follow-
up
ΔM ¼ ¡1.12, 95% CI ¼[¡2.06, ¡0.18],
t(245) ¼ ¡2.97, p =.01, d ¼0.25
ΔM =0.37, 95% CI =[0.99, 1.73],
t(235) =0.68, p =.88, d =0.08
Noticing
opportunities
Start of study M =3.99, SE =0.11 M =4.03, SE =0.16 ΔM =0.04, 95% CI =[0.48, 0.56],
t(490) =0.20, p =.99, d =0.03
End of Study M =4.45, SE =0.11 M =4.06, SE =0.16 ΔM = − 0.39, 95% CI =[0.91, 0.12],
t(489) = − 2.03, p =.23, d =0.26
Follow-up M =4.42, SE =0.12 M =4.00, SE =0.17 ΔM = − 0.42, 95% CI =[0.97, 0.12],
t(571) = − 2.01, p =.24, d =0.28
Start vs. End ΔM ¼ ¡0.46, 95% CI ¼[¡0.72, ¡0.19],
t(506) ¼ ¡4.62, p < .001, d ¼0.30
ΔM = − 0.02, 95% CI =[0.42, 0.37],
t(505) = − 0.15, p =.99, d =0.02
Start vs. Follow-
up
ΔM ¼ ¡0.43, 95% CI ¼[¡0.71, ¡0.14],
t(521) ¼ ¡3.95, p < .001, d ¼0.29
ΔM =0.03, 95% CI =[0.39, 0.45],
t(517) =0.18, p =.99, d =0.02
Note. Analysis of rejection used a negative binomial regression: descriptives are estimated means for predicted rejections, and comparisons between groups and
between time points are ratios. For all other measures, a mixed-effects regression was used: comparisons between groups and between time points are mean differ-
ences. Signicant effects indicated in bold. We recognize that the best practice for calculating effect sizes for multilevel models is unclear. Nevertheless, in the interest
of supporting meta-analytic work, we report effect sizes (Cohens d) which we calculate based on pairwise comparisons.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
7
participants had had in the week prior to the start of the study, p =.26,or
in the week prior to the follow-up, p =.85.
3.6.2. Noticing opportunities
At the end of the intervention, and after the week-long follow-up,
participants in the treatment group (but not participants in the control
group, ps >.99) noticed more opportunities for conversations with
strangers than they had at the start of the study, p <.001 (see Table 1).
However, directly comparing treatment and control conditions revealed
no difference in how many opportunities were noticed overall, ps >.22.
Although our studies were not designed to examine behavior change,
these measures provide suggestive evidence that changes in attitudes
may ultimately lead people to strike up more conversations with
strangers in their everyday life.
3.7. Daily results: Predictions vs. experiences
After undergoing our intervention, participants in our treatment
condition displayed less pessimistic attitudes towards talking to
strangers. This pattern of results was true across a number of different
measures. Here, we focus on the daily changes in our measures, in order
to answer two critical questions:
(1) What is the time course of our intervention on peoples beliefs?
(2) How do peoples beliefs compare to reality?
The answers to these questions provide some evidence for the psy-
chological processes responsible for the changes we saw as a result of our
intervention.
Regarding the rst question, if peoples beliefs changed gradually
over the course of the week, this would suggest that repeated experience
is a necessary component of the intervention, as we hypothesized.
Alternatively, it is possible that just one or two conversations with a
stranger were sufcient to bring about the observed changes.
Regarding the second question, we know that peoples predictions
grew less pessimistic over the course of the week, but it is important to
Fig. 3. General results for perceived likelihood of rejection and perceived conversational ability.
Note. The effect of a talking to strangersintervention on peoples perceived likelihood of rejection (i.e., peoples beliefs about how many strangers would reject
them before they could nd someone to talk to) and peoples perceived conversational ability (7-point scale). Error bars represent 95% condence intervals.
Fig. 4. General results for awkwardness, enjoyment, and making a positive impression.
Note. The effect of a talking to strangersintervention on peoples feelings of awkwardness and enjoyment when talking to strangers, and peoples perceptions of
how positive of an impression they made on their partners (all 7-point scales). Error bars represent 95% condence intervals.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
8
consider these predictions in relation to peoples actual experiences.
Using peoples beliefs about the possibility of rejection as an example, if
the actual number of rejections people experienced was low, this would
suggest that our intervention worked by bringing peoples beliefs more
in line with reality over the course of the week. On the other hand, if the
actual number of rejections was high, this would suggest that instead our
intervention worked by making people feel more condent in their
ability to approach people, regardless of reality. Finally, the number of
actual rejections people experienced may, in fact, have decreased over
the course of the week; this would suggest that our intervention worked
by making people better at approaching strangers and avoiding
rejection.
3.8. Daily results: Change in perceived likelihood of rejection
Daily surveys consisted of peoples pre-conversation predictions
about the number of rejections they would experience on a particular
day, and their post-conversation reports of the actual number of re-
jections they experienced that day. Again, we used a mixed-effects
binomial regression estimated using restricted maximum likelihood,
with a random intercept for participant, rating type (predicted vs.
actual) as a xed categorical independent variable, and time (day of the
week) as a xed continuous independent variable (i.e., DV ~ Rat-
ingType*Day +(1 | ParticipantId).
Directly comparing daily predicted and actual rejections revealed
that participants expected signicantly more rejection than they expe-
rienced (p <.001; see Table 2). Indeed, on the rst day of the study, only
40% of participants in the treatment condition thought the rst person
they approached would talk to them, when in fact participants managed
to have a conversation with the rst person they approached 92% of the
time. Over the course of the study, of the 1336 conversations that par-
ticipants had with strangers, 1164 (87%) occurred with the rst person
that participants approached.
Critically, there was also a signicant rating type x time interaction,
p <.001. Follow-up tests revealed that peoples predicted rejections
decreased over time (p <.001), whereas the number of rejections people
actually experienced did not change over time (p =.37). In other words,
as shown in the left panel of Fig. 5, the number of rejections people
actually experienced while trying to talk to strangers was consistently
low, and peoples predictions about the number of rejections they would
face decreased over the course of the week to more accurately reect
reality.
3.9. Daily results: Change in conversational ability
Overall, participants expected lower conversational ability than they
actually reported having after talking to strangers. Critically, there was
also a signicant rating type x time interaction (p <.001), suggesting
that the gap between peoples predictions about their own conversa-
tional ability and their actual conversational ability varied over the
course of the week. Follow-up tests revealed that peoples predicted
ability increased over time (p <.001), while their actual ability also
increased but at a lower rate (p <.001; see the right panel of Fig. 5). In
short, peoples predictions about their own conversation ability, which
had been overly pessimistic, grew more optimistic as the intervention
wore on.
3.10. Daily results: Change in awkwardness
Overall, participants expected to feel more awkward than they
actually did. This was qualied by a signicant rating type x time
interaction (p =.003). Follow-up tests revealed that peoples predicted
awkwardness decreased over time (p <.001), while their actual
awkwardness also decreased but at a lower rate (p <.001; see left panel
of Fig. 6). Once again, our week-long intervention appeared to narrow
the gap between peoples overly pessimistic predictions and their actual
experiences.
3.11. Daily results: Change in enjoyment
Participants expected to feel less enjoyment than they actually
experienced. However, the rating type x time interaction was not sig-
nicant (p =.37). Unlike the previous measures, our intervention did
not have a discernible effect on the gap between peoples predicted
enjoyment and their actual enjoyment (see middle panel of Fig. 6). This
effect is surprising given the strong effects that our intervention had on
awkwardness, and future research might fruitfully explore why negative
affect was more strongly inuenced by repeatedly talking to strangers.
3.12. Daily results: Change in making a positive impression
Finally, participants expected to make a less positive impression than
they actually thought they made, on reection. The rating type x time
interaction was again not signicant (p =.15). Follow-up tests revealed
that peoples predicted positive impression and their actual positive
impression both increased over time (see right panel of Fig. 6). These
results are consistent with the results from the general survey, which
also showed increased positivity from participants in both the treatment
and control groups, suggesting the possible benets of simply observing
strangers.
Overall, the results from the daily surveys are broadly consistent
with the proposal that our intervention worked, in part, by repeatedly
conditioning people to the reality of talking to strangers. Strangers are
typically open to talking, and peoples conversations with strangers
typically go quite well. As such, our week-long intervention made peo-
ples predictions about talking to strangers steadily more positive,
thereby reducing important barriers to social connection.
4. Discussion
People are remarkably pessimistic about the prospect of talking to
strangers. Our novel talking to strangersintervention aimed to reduce
peoples fears about talking to strangers by prompting them to repeat-
edly have conversations with strangers over the course of ve days. By
the end of the study, participants in our treatment group reported
signicantly more positive attitudes towards talking to strangers: They
anticipated less rejection, reported less awkwardness, and felt more
condent in their conversational ability compared to controlspositive
effects that persisted for at least one week after the intervention had
ended.
Our intervention also improved peoples enjoyment and the im-
pressions people thought they made on strangers. Note, however, that
participants in the control condition also experienced modest im-
provements on these two measures, suggesting that even simply
observing strangers may have some benets.
Finally, our analysis of the daily surveys highlighted the dimensions
on which our intervention seemed most effective: fear of rejection,
perceived conversational ability, and awkwardness. Such analyses
revealed that peoples overly pessimistic expectations grew more posi-
tive and more accurate by the day, emphasizing the importance of
repeated experience in improving peoples attitudes towards talking
with strangers.
4.1. What features contributed to the effectiveness of our intervention?
4.1.1. Repeated practice
Our intervention prompted people to repeatedly strike up conversa-
tions with strangers over the course of a week, which proved highly
effective in reducing peoples fears about talking to strangers. But could
our intervention have achieved the same results in a less time-intensive
manner? The answer appears to be nopeoples beliefs changed grad-
ually over the course of the week, as people engaged in more
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
9
conversations with strangers. Moreover, past research suggests that after
a single pleasant conversation with a stranger, people fail to generalize
their experience, instead returning to their more pessimistic expecta-
tions when anticipating another such conversation with someone new
(Sandstrom & Boothby, 2021). Repeated practice appears to be a critical
ingredient in our interventions success.
This emphasis on repeated experience is reminiscent of cognitive
behavioral therapies (Butler, Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006), but
while our intervention does rely on repeated behavioral experience, the
cognitivecomponent is not explicitly present. In other words, people
in our intervention were not explicitly prompted to challenge or
restructure their thoughts about strangersalthough we suspect that
people may have done so naturally over the course of the week.
Intriguingly, there is evidence that explicit cognitive restructuring is less
important than practice for the treatment of anxiety and social phobias
in particular (Clark, 1995; Longmore & Worrell, 2007).
4.1.2. Gamication
In light of peoples aversion to talking to strangers, we suspect that
part of the effectiveness of our intervention may be attributable to the
fact that it was delivered via a scavenger hunt game. We drew on several
features of gamication to encourage participants to repeatedly
perform a behavior that they normally tend to avoid altogether. For
example, we gave participants a choice of which missions to tackle each
day, and participants reported appreciating this freedom, which may
have helped them see talking to strangers as a challenge rather than a
threat. Anecdotally, participants also seemed to enjoy the points they
received for each mission, and kept an eye on the leaderboard that
showed their relative ranking each week, which may have increased
their motivation to talk to more strangers. Our research thus adds to
existing literature that has harnessed gamication features to encourage
people to adopt behaviors that support health and well-being (Johnson
et al., 2016).
Table 2
Descriptive and inferential statistics for the daily survey results.
Measure Time/Comparison Predicted Actual Predicted vs. Actual
Rejection Monday M =0.65 M =0.06
Tuesday M =0.47 M =0.15
Wednesday M =0.36 M =0.14
Thursday M =0.41 M =0.10
Friday M =0.29 M =0.12
Overall M =0.43, SE =0.04 M =0.11, SE =0.01 Ratio ¼3.75, 95% CI ¼[3.18, 4.43],
z ¼15.58, p < .001
Estimated simple slopes & Rating type
x time interaction
b ¼ ¡0.18, SE ¼0.03,
95% CI ¼[¡0.25, ¡0.12],
z ¼ ¡5.47, p < .001
b =0.04, SE =0.05,
95% CI =[0.05, 0.14],
z =0.90, p =.37
b ¼0.23, SE ¼0.06,
95% CI ¼[0.11, 0.34],
z ¼3.87, p < .001
Conversational
ability
Monday M =4.18 M =5.01
Tuesday M =4.39 M =5.10
Wednesday M =4.72 M =5.31
Thursday M =4.72 M =5.31
Friday M =4.94 M =5.32
Overall M =4.58, SE =0.06 M =5.19, SE =0.06 ΔM ¼ ¡0.61, 95% CI ¼[¡0.69, ¡0.53],
t(2069) ¼-15.55, p < .001
Estimated simple slopes & Rating type
x time interaction
b ¼0.19, SE ¼0.02,
95% CI ¼[0.15, 0.23],
t(2052) ¼9.23, p < .001
b ¼0.09, SE ¼0.02,
95% CI ¼[0.06, 0.12],
t(2065) ¼5.12, p < .001
b ¼ ¡0.10, SE ¼0.03,
95% CI ¼[¡0.15, ¡0.05],
t(2057.23) ¼ ¡3.72, p < .001
Awkwardness Monday M =3.10 M =2.47
Tuesday M =2.82 M =2.27
Wednesday M =2.59 M =2.17
Thursday M =2.59 M =2.14
Friday M =2.42 M =2.06
Overall M =2.71, SE =0.04 M =2.24, SE =0.04 ΔM ¼0.48, 95% CI ¼[0.42, 0.53],
t(2070) ¼16.13, p < .001
Estimated simple slopes & Rating type
x time interaction
b ¼ ¡0.16, SE ¼0.02,
95% CI ¼[¡0.19, ¡0.13],
t(2054) ¼ ¡10.46, p < .001
b ¼ ¡0.10, SE ¼0.01,
95% CI ¼[¡0.13, ¡0.07],
t(2066) ¼ ¡7.69, p < .001
b ¼0.06, SE ¼0.02,
95% CI ¼[0.02, 0.10],
t(2058.78) ¼3.00, p ¼.003
Enjoyment Monday M =3.26 M =3.60
Tuesday M =3.28 M =3.58
Wednesday M =3.28 M =3.61
Thursday M =3.26 M =3.62
Friday M =3.35 M =3.76
Overall M =3.28, SE =0.03 M =3.63, SE =0.03 ΔM ¼ ¡0.35, 95% CI ¼[¡0.40, ¡0.29],
t(2083) ¼-12.71, p < .001
Estimated simple slopes & Rating type
x time interaction
b =0.02, SE =0.01,
95% CI =[0.01, 0.04],
t(2056) =1.16, p =.25
b ¼0.03, SE ¼0.01,
95% CI ¼[0.01, 0.06],
t(2077) ¼2.74, p ¼.01
b =0.02, SE =0.02,
95% CI =[0.02, 0.05],
t(2066) =0.89, p =.37
Positive impression Monday M =3.21 M =3.43
Tuesday M =3.32 M =3.46
Wednesday M =3.24 M =3.54
Thursday M =3.28 M =3.51
Friday M =3.36 M =3.65
Overall M =3.28, SE =0.03 M =3.51, SE =0.03 ΔM ¼ ¡0.23, 95% CI ¼[¡0.28, ¡0.18],
t(2080) ¼-9.18, p < .001
Estimated simple slopes & Rating type
x time interaction
b ¼0.03, SE ¼0.01,
95% CI ¼[0.001, 0.05],
t(2054) ¼1.98, p ¼.05
b ¼0.05, SE ¼0.01,
95% CI ¼[0.03, 0.07],
t(2073) ¼4.57, p < .001
b =0.03, SE =0.02,
95% CI =[0.01, 0.06],
t(2061) =1.45, p =.15
Note. Analysis of rejection used a mixed-effects binomial regression: descriptives are estimated means for predicted and actual rejections, and comparisons between
predicted and actual are ratios. For all other measures, a mixed-effects regression was used: comparisons between predicted and actual are tests of the mean difference.
The rating type x time interaction is reported in the predicted vs. actual column; follow-up simple slopes analyses are reported in the predicted and actual columns.
Signicant effects indicated in bold.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
10
4.2. Limitations of the current research
Although gamication may have made the intervention more
engaging, the use of gamication poses a limitation: we do not know the
extent to which the effects generalize to other less gamied ways of
delivering the intervention. While gamication may have contributed to
peoples motivation, as long as an intervention has some mechanism to
ensure participant compliance in actually having conversations, we have
no reason to doubt that similar results would be observed.
As with most eld research that requires participants to complete
repeated tasks over time, our study suffered from some attrition. We
carried out extensive analyses (see SOM), nding that participants who
dropped out did not differ from those who nished the study on a range
of personality traits and general attitudes towards talking to strangers.
However, there is some evidence that drop-outs reported more negative
experiences than nishers during their rst day of conversations. That
said, most participants who eventually dropped out of the study per-
sisted beyond the rst day, at which point their responses on our pri-
mary dependent variables became statistically indistinguishable from
those of participants who completed the study.
Finally, one unexpected limitation is that our intervention appeared
to reduce negative outcomes, such as fear of rejection, more strongly
than it improved positive outcomes, such as peoples perceptions of the
impressions they made on their partners. This is a limitation in that this
pattern was not predicted, and we do not know exactly why it happened.
On the other hand, assuming such a pattern is consistent, it suggests
theoretical and practical implications that will allow future research to
make more precise predictions, either when trying to modify our
intervention for stronger effects, or more broadly, in future attempts to
explore the underlying psychology of initial interactions with strangers.
Fig. 5. Daily results for predicted likelihood of rejection and conversational ability and actual number of rejections and perceived conversational ability.
Note. The effect of a talking to strangersintervention on peoples daily predictions about the likelihood of rejection compared to the actual number of rejections
they experienced, and peoples daily predictions about their ability to carry out a conversation compared to their post-conversation reections about their ability (7-
point scale). Error bars represent 95% condence intervals.
Fig. 6. Daily results for predicted and actual awkwardness, enjoyment, and making a positive impression.
Note. The effect of a talking to strangersintervention on peoples daily predictions about their awkwardness, their enjoyment, and the positivity of the impression
they will make on their partner, compared to their actual experience reported after they nished their conversations each day (all on 5-point scales). Error bars
represent 95% condence intervals.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
11
4.3. Implications for encouraging more conversations with strangers
When people feel more optimistic about talking to strangers, do they
actually initiate more conversations with strangers? Models of behavior
change emphasize factors such as peoples attitudes, perceptions of the
norms, and perceived behavioral control as important predictors (e.g.,
Ajzen, 1985). Our intervention shifted measures that could be inter-
preted as indicators of peoples attitudes (enjoyment), perceptions of the
norms (rejection), and perceived behavioral control (conversational
ability). It is therefore not unreasonable to think that our intervention
might also shift peoples actual behavior. And indeed, we found pre-
liminary support for this hypothesis: participants in the treatment con-
dition reported talking to more strangers in the week following the study
compared to the week prior to the study. However, looking back, we
wish our instructions had been more specic about exactly what type of
conversations we were interested in (i.e., social, not instrumental); more
than one participant mentioned that they worked in retail and had
talked to dozens of customers. Further, our study was not primarily
designed to assess behavior change; we simply asked people to
remember how many conversations they had initiated over the course of
a week, which is inevitably an imprecise measure. These results were
also exploratory and so we urge caution in interpreting them, leaving the
question of prolonged behavior change to future research.
4.4. Implications for loneliness
Our intervention may have special relevance now, as a growing
number of scientists and public health ofcials are raising the alarm
about increasing levels of loneliness, and the dire health consequences
(Buecker, Mund, Chwastek, Sostmann, & Luhmann, 2021; Holt-Lunstad,
Smith, Baker, Harris, & Stephenson, 2015). Emerging research has
documented several promising classes of interventions to address lone-
liness, including increasing opportunities for social interaction,
addressing maladaptive social cognition, and improving social skills
(Masi, Chen, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2011). Our intervention does not
fall neatly into one of these categories; rather, it ts in several.
First, our intervention increases opportunities for social interaction
by making people more aware of the opportunities that already exist in
their daily lives. Indeed, after our intervention, participants in the
experimental condition (but not the control condition) reported noticing
more opportunities to talk to strangers. Secondly, our intervention may
have facilitated the disruption of maladaptive cognition, allowing peo-
ple to develop an alternative, less pessimistic narrative about initial
interactions (e.g., Talking to strangers is much better than I expectI
can do this.). Finally, while our intervention did not explicitly instruct
people on how to improve their social skills, people did feel more
conversationally skilled by the end of the intervention. More research is
needed to determine the effects of our intervention on loneliness, but we
can be certain that our intervention did lead people to make connections
that continued beyond the study itself: 41% of participants in the
treatment condition reported exchanging contact information and
following up with at least one of their conversation partnersa prom-
ising start.
4.5. Conclusion
Ultimately, our novel talking to strangers intervention was suc-
cessful, providing evidence that repeated experience talking to strangers
can reduce peoples fears about talking to strangers, and make them
more accurate in their predictions about future conversations.
At its heart, our intervention is simple: it involves repeatedly
approaching and talking to strangers. As such, this intervention is
something that many people could self-administer. We encourage
readers to try it, despite any natural instinct to avoid such inter-
actionswhich even the authors confess to sharing. As our research
shows, these conversations really do get easier with practice, and the
experience will be more positive than you expect.
Open practices
Materials, data and analysis scripts are available here: (https://osf.
io/b76gf/).
Author contributions
All authors contributed equally to this paper. All authors developed
the study concept and contributed to the study design. G.M.S and E.J.B
collected the data. All authors analyzed the data and drafted the
manuscript.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Meredith Anderer, Shaaba Lotun, Megan Rodri-
guez, Kendra Sober, Qian Sun, Zihao Tian, and Charlotte Walden for
assistance with data collection and study management. This project was
funded by a Small Research Grant from the British Academy (Grant
SG162524; to G.M.S.).
Appendix
Table A1
Scavenger hunt missions.
Name Instructions
Al Fresco Find someone outdoors, and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
All Smiles Find someone who seems friendly and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Artsy Find someone who looks artistic and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Blue Mood Find someone who looks sad and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Bossy Pants Find someone who looks like a leader and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Caffeination Station Find a barista/server and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Coffee Break Find someone whos drinking a coffee and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Do Gooder Find someone who seems like a nice or kind person and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Fashionista Find someone whos accessorizing (e.g., wearing a scarf, hat) and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Fun Fabric Find someone wearing stand-out print (e.g., stripes, animal-print) and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Graphic Tee Find someone who is wearing an interesting shirt and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Hot Find someone whom you nd attractive and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Hungry Find someone whos eating and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Inked Up Find someone who has a tattoo and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Inside Find someone indoors and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Jock Find someone sporty and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
(continued on next page)
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 102 (2022) 104356
12
Table A1 (continued )
Name Instructions
KickinIt Find someone who is wearing interesting shoes and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Line Up Find someone whos waiting in a queue/line and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Manscape Find someone who has a beard/goatee/etc. and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Minion Find someone who is wearing a uniform and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Nailed It Find someone who has funky nails (e.g., unusual shade, fancy design) and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Next Gen Find someone whos from a different generation than you and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
On Top Find someone who is wearing a hat and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Ray of Sunshine Find someone who looks happy and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Sexy Find someone whose gender differs from yours and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Skin Deep Find someone whose skin tone is different from yours and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Twins Find someone wearing the same thing as you (hair style, shirt, shoes, etc.) and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Unicorn Find someone who has eye-catching hair (e.g., pink tips), dyed hair, or a cool hair style and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Wild Card x 2 Find anyone of your choosing and [talk to/observe] them for a few minutes.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104356.
References
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl, &
J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 1139).
Heidelberg: Springer.
Atir, S., Wald, K., & Epley, N. (2021). Conversation is surprisingly informative. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Boothby, E. J., Cooney, G., Sandstrom, G. M., & Clark, M. S. (2018). The liking gap in
conversations: Do people like us more than we think? Psychological Science, 29(11),
17421756.
Buecker, S., Mund, M., Chwastek, S., Sostmann, M., & Luhmann, M. (2021). Is loneliness
in emerging adults increasing over time? A preregistered cross-temporal meta-
analysis and systematic review. Psychological Bulletin, 147(8), 787805.
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical status of
cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clinical Psychology Review,
26, 1731.
Clark, D. A. (1995). Perceived limitations of standard cognitive therapy: A consideration
of efforts to revise Becks theory and therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 9,
153172.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life
events and self-reported mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
296308. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.296
Cooney, G., Boothby, E. J., & Schweitzer, M. (2021). Overestimating otherspreferences for
homophily in initial conversations: Are dissimilar others more interested in talking than we
think? Manuscript submitted for publication.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1),
8184.
Epley, N., & Schroeder, J. (2014). Mistakenly seeking solitude. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 143(5), 1980.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A exible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
Research Methods, 39(2), 175191.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts
build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build
consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5),
1045.
Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places. Simon and Schuster.
GooseChase Adventures. (2019). GooseChase [Mobile application software]. Retrieved
from http://itunes.apple.com.
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical
review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40(2),
218227.
Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of wellbeing. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449),
14351446.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Robles, T. F., & Sbarra, D. A. (2017). Advancing social connection as a
public health priority in the United States. American Psychologist, 72(6), 517.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness
and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives
on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227237.
House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health.
Science, 241(4865), 540545.
Johnson, D., Deterding, S., Kuhn, K. A., Staneva, A., Stoyanov, S., & Hides, L. (2016).
Gamication for health and wellbeing: A systematic review of the literature. Internet
Interventions, 6, 89106.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004).
A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction
method. Science, 306, 17761780. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572
Kim, E. C. (2012). Nonsocial transient behavior: Social disengagement on the greyhound
bus. Symbolic Interaction, 35(3), 267283.
Kushlev, K., Hunter, J. F., Proulx, J., Pressman, S. D., & Dunn, E. (2019). Smartphones
reduce smiles between strangers. Computers in Human Behavior, 91, 1216.
Longmore, R. J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to challenge thoughts in cognitive
behavior therapy? Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 173187.
Masi, C. M., Chen, H. Y., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). A meta-analysis of
interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(3),
219266.
Mastroianni, A., Cooney, G., Boothby, E. J., & Reece, A. G. (2021). The liking gap in
groups and teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 162,
109122.
Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Holleran, S. E., & Clark, C. S. (2010). Eavesdropping on
happiness: Well-being is related to having less small talk and more substantive
conversations. Psychological Science, 21(4), 539541.
Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist,
55(1), 56.
Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1990). Extraversion and happiness. Personality and
Individual Differences, 11, 12991306. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(90)
90157-M
Sandstrom, G. M., & Boothby, E. J. (2021). Why do people avoid talking to strangers? A
mini meta-analysis of predicted fears and actual experiences talking to a stranger.
Self and Identity, 20(1), 4771.
Sandstrom, G. M., & Dunn, E. W. (2014). Is efciency overrated? Minimal social
interactions lead to belonging and positive affect. Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 5(4), 437442.
Schroeder, J., Lyons, D., & Epley, N. (2021). Hello, stranger? Pleasant conversations are
preceded by concerns about starting one. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General.
https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2021-90811-001.
Sun, J., Harris, K., & Vazire, S. (2020). Is well-being associated with the quantity and
quality of social interactions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(6),
14781496.
Vittengl, J. R., & Holt, C. S. (1998). A time-series diary study of mood and social
interaction. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 255275. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
1022388123550
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., McIntyre, C. W., & Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and
social activity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 10111025. https://
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.1011
Welker, J. T., Walker, C., Boothby, E. J., & Gilovich, T. (2021). Pessimistic assessments of
ability in everyday conversation. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Zuckerman, M., Miserandino, M., & Bernieri, F. (1983). Civil inattention existsIn
elevators. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(4), 578586.
Gillian M. Sandstrom is a Senior Lecturer in Psychology at the University of Sussex.
Erica J. Boothby is a Senior Lecturer in the Operations, Information, & Decisions
Department at The Wharton School at the University of Pennsylvania.
Gus Cooney is a Senior Lecturer in the Operations, Information, & Decisions Department
at The Wharton School at the University of Pennsylvania.
G.M. Sandstrom et al.
... For example, weak ties may include relationships with coworkers, neighbors, extended family, or acquaintances one meets regularly. Although people are often more reluctant to interact with weak ties and strangers compared to strong ties (Sandstrom et al., 2022), such interactions can contribute to wellbeing and satisfy belongingness needs (J. L. Hirsch & Clark, 2019;Sandstrom & Dunn, 2014;Van Lange & Columbus, 2021). ...
... Overall, people tend to prefer interacting with people whom they also interacted with in the past (Meijerink-Bosman et al., 2023;Sandstrom et al., 2022). Accordingly, the availability of strong and weak ties in people's social networks is anticipated to influence their day-to-day social interactions, as interactions with familiar individuals are viewed as more easily available, less risky, and requiring less energy than interactions with strangers (Huxhold et al., 2022;Sandstrom et al., 2022). ...
... Overall, people tend to prefer interacting with people whom they also interacted with in the past (Meijerink-Bosman et al., 2023;Sandstrom et al., 2022). Accordingly, the availability of strong and weak ties in people's social networks is anticipated to influence their day-to-day social interactions, as interactions with familiar individuals are viewed as more easily available, less risky, and requiring less energy than interactions with strangers (Huxhold et al., 2022;Sandstrom et al., 2022). Conversely, compared to people with larger social networks, people with smaller social networks have access to a smaller pool of interaction partners, may receive fewer offers for social interactions, and may find themselves for longer periods of time in situations where they cannot immediately find available interaction partners. ...
Article
Full-text available
Current psychological theories on daily social interactions emphasize individual differences yet are underspecified regarding contextual factors. We aim to extend this research by examining how two context factors shape social interactions in daily life: how many relationships people maintain and how densely people live together. In Study 1, 307 German participants (Mage = 39.44 years, SDage = 14.14) answered up to 20 experience sampling questionnaires regarding their social interactions over 2 days. In Study 2, 313 German participants (Mage = 48.96 years, SDage = 15.54) summarized their daily interactions in daily diaries for 14 days. Participants reported on their social network size and the social density (i.e., household and neighborhood density) of their living situations. Mobile sensing provided additional measures of social interactions and network size. The results showed that participants living in densely populated households transitioned faster from solitude to social interactions but slower from social interactions to solitude. Participants living in dwellings with more homes also transitioned slower from solitude to social interactions. Contrary to the hypothesis, social network size was inconsistently linked with transitions from solitude to social interactions and vice versa. Furthermore, current social desires predicted subsequent social interactions within days, but not across days—irrespective of individuals’ social network size or social density. Together the results point out that people live their daily life in social contexts, which contribute to how they engage in social interactions. The findings thus call for a greater integration of contextual factors in personality theories of social interactions.
... In other cases, as implied by the findings of this study, L1 speakers might feel especially insecure about how much they are liked by their L2-speaking interlocutor, so they avoid communicating with that interlocutor in the future. If metaperception is indeed confirmed as a factor in avoidance behaviors for students in an academic setting, such behaviors (including the underlying metaperception bias) would appear to be relatively easy to address, for instance, through awareness-raising or intervention activities (Sandstrom, Boothby, & Cooney, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
People are frequently concerned about the impressions they make on others (referred to as metaperceptions), but their insights are often inaccurate. Illustrating the phenomenon called the liking gap, speakers interacting in their first language (L1) and second language (L2) tend to underestimate how much they are liked by their interlocutor, and these judgments often predict their desire to engage in future interaction and collaboration. To understand the scope of this bias and its consequences, we focused on L1-L2 dyadic interaction, examining metaperception as a potential barrier to conversations between university students. We recruited 58 previously unacquainted university students to perform a 10-min academic discussion task between one L1 and one L2 speaker. Afterward, the speakers (a) assessed each other's interpersonal liking, speaking skill, and interactional behavior; (b) provided their metaperceptions of their interlocutor's assessments of the same dimensions; and (c) estimated their interest in future interaction with the same interlocutor. All speakers showed a reliable metaperception bias to underestimate their interpersonal liking, speaking skill, and interactional behavior. However, only L1 speakers' desire to engage in future interaction was associated with their metaperceptions of interpersonal liking. We discuss implications of this finding for understanding and promoting academic communication.
... Studies have suggested that miscalibrated social expectations are modulated by personal experiences (e.g., Duffy et al., 2018;Zelenski et al., 2013). For example, research has shown that individuals' expectations of future conversations with strangers improved (e.g., they expected a lower likelihood of rejection) after a 1-week intervention in which they talked to strangers (Sandstrom et al., 2022). Similarly, individuals high in trait-empathic concern, who approach others in need more often (Batson, 2010), may better appreciate the benefits of engaging in supportive behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has suggested that empathic concern may affect cultural differences in social support-seeking. However, neither the mechanisms through which empathic concern promotes support-seeking nor the explanations for cultural differences in empathic concern are clear. This study attempted to address these questions by conducting three studies in Japan and the United States. The results showed that Japanese participants reported having lower trait-empathic concern and seeking less social support in dealing with stress than European Americans. Study 1 found that trait-empathic concern mediated the cultural differences in support-seeking by increasing beliefs about others’ prosocial willingness. Using a controlled set of stressful scenarios, Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1. Additionally, Study 2 showed that Japanese participants reported greater endorsement of the causal repressive suffering construal than European Americans, partly accounting for cultural differences in trait-empathic concern. Using an experimental design, Study 3 showed that primed empathic concern increased support-seeking in coping with follow-up stress across cultures. These findings contribute to our understanding of the role of empathic concern in support-seeking and cultural differences in empathic concern.
... Past research sometimes narrowly defines friends as only "close" or "best" friends [14,15]. However, there is increasing evidence that interactions with weak ties (e.g., casual friends) can be supportive of health and wellness [45,46]. In the present investigation, 55%-60% of names listed were best or close friends, but this leaves a considerable number of "just" friends (~23%). ...
Article
Full-text available
Friendship is critical for individuals’ well-being, but recent efforts to characterize Americans’ friendship have suggested that these relationships are in peril. The present study is a report on the methods and results of three surveys from the American Friendship Project (AFP). The goal of the AFP is to be the most accurate and most complete account of American friendship as well as its health and change over time. The AFP reports on five critical facets of social health as it relates to friendship: 1) the structural factors of friendship (e.g., who are they, how many); 2) friendship quality (e.g., satisfaction, closeness); 3) social support from friends; 4) the quantity of online and offline communication; and 5) well-being (e.g., life satisfaction, loneliness, connection). Data was collected from two national samples of American adults in 2022 and 2023 and from a large sample of college students across three universities in 2022. The key finding from this investigation is, compared to the discouraging results of other recent surveys, Americans reported having more friends and fewer were friendless. AFP results also suggest that face-to-face (FtF) meetings among friends are quite common, as are telephone calls and text messaging. College student and adult samples reported very similar attitudes and experiences with friendship overall, but students were more likely to meet friends at school and to keep them for a shorter length of time. Another key finding is Americans long for greater closeness with friends; though over 75% were satisfied with the number of friends they had, over 40% felt they were not as close to their friends as they would like. Overall, the AFP is a rich source of data that can be used to answer a multitude of questions about friendship and its connection to well-being.
... Numerous studies demonstrate that individuals with stronger social relationships report greater subjective well-being than those with fewer or weaker relationships (Diener & Seligman, 2002;Helliwell, Layard, & Sachs, 2019). In addition, a growing body of research suggests that humans derive emotional benefits from even minimal social interactions with strangers (e.g., Epley & Schroeder, 2014;Sandstrom & Dunn, 2014;Sandstrom, Boothby, & Cooney, 2022), likely because social inclusion offered a survival advantage in our evolutionary past (i.e., safety, resources, offspring). As a result, humans have tools to facilitate social functioning, such as the ability to quickly and accurately understand the emotions and personality of others (Funder, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research suggests that people can predict others’ personality and emotions through various channels, but it is unknown whether strangers can accurately predict a target’s trait happiness from a brief written personal memory, and whether some content is more useful than others. Given the importance of understanding others, we conducted two pre-registered investigations of whether third-party readers could estimate the happiness of 5,169 targets who wrote a brief (average words = 61) personal or prosocial spending memory. We detected a small positive correlation between targets’ trait happiness and readers’ ratings in both conditions, with a larger correlation often found in the personal condition. We then conducted exploratory analyses to probe why certain spending memories facilitate greater trait happiness accuracy by examining word usage
... One intervention, however, has been shown to lastingly change people's attitudes about talking to strangers. This intervention involves participants playing a scavenger hunt game in which they complete a "mission" every day for a week: talking to a stranger in the experimental condition, or observing a stranger in the control condition 45 . At the end of the week, participants in the experimental condition were less worried about rejection, and more confident in their ability to start and maintain a conversation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social relationships provide one of the most reliable paths to happiness, but relationships can fade for various reasons. While it does not take much to reinitiate contact, here we find that people are surprisingly reluctant to do so. Specifically, most people reported losing touch with an old friend yet expressed little interest in reaching out (Studies 1-2, N s = 401 and 199). Moreover, fewer than one third of participants sent a message to an old friend, even when they wanted to, thought the friend would be appreciative, had the friend’s contact information, and were given time to draft and send a message (Studies 3-4, N s = 453 and 604). One reason for this reluctance may be that old friends feel like strangers. Supporting this possibility, participants were no more willing to reach out to an old friend than they were to talk to a stranger (Study 5, N = 288), and were less willing to contact old friends who felt more like strangers (Study 6, N = 319). Therefore, in Study 7 ( N = 194), we adapted an intervention shown to ease anxieties about talking to strangers and found that it increased the number of people who reached out to an old friend by two-thirds.
... It is also possible that the intervention created a new habit in participants. For instance, in a study prompting repeated conversations with strangers for a week, participants noticed more opportunities to engage with strangers even after the intervention ended (Sandstrom et al., 2022). As such, after our 2-week intervention prompting repeated prosocial engagement, participants might have noticed more opportunities for prosocial engagement in their surroundings, leading to more interactions with their recipients. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is an urgent need for effective and easily accessible interventions targeting young adults’ social connection. This study tests whether engaging in prosocial behavior can mitigate social disconnection using an acts of kindness intervention that can be easily integrated into people’s daily routine. University students were randomly assigned to one of two kinds of 14-day kindness exercises (regular or anonymous) or an active control activity. 388 participants completed diary assessments of social contact and loneliness before and after the intervention. Results showed that the intervention promoting prosocial engagement increased social contact (especially with close others) and reduced daily loneliness for lonely participants. Anonymous kindness did not yield these outcomes, suggesting that direct contact with recipients may be an active ingredient driving such effects. This research provides a self-delivered and low-cost intervention that holds promise to reduce both objective social isolation and subjective feelings of loneliness among young adults.
... Critically, the information participants used was largely positive. We assume that this positivity stems from the fact that people generally seem to enjoy interacting with strangers despite their negative expectations (Dunn et al., 2007;Epley & Schroeder, 2014;Sandstrom et al., 2022). Thus, meta-perceivers and the people they interact with experience more positive emotion over time, which would in turn give rise to increasingly positive metaliking and liking judgments. ...
Article
Full-text available
Feeling accepted by others is a fundamental human motive and an important marker of successful social interactions. This interpersonal perception, known as meta-liking, is especially relevant during adolescence, when peer relationships deepen and expand. However, knowledge is limited regarding meta-liking formation in initial social interactions. This study investigated whether adolescents (N = 293, Mage = 15.48, 61.10% female) have default expectations for meta-liking at zero acquaintance and how these judgments are updated during initial group interactions. Specifically, we used latent change models to examine how personality traits predicted initial meta-liking and whether personality and social interaction experiences were linked to changes in meta-liking judgments throughout an interaction. Our findings revealed three key insights: First, meta-liking increased gradually over the course of the interaction, with substantial individual differences in both default meta-liking and change scores. Second, extraversion, neuroticism, and self-esteem predicted initial meta-liking. Third, liking others was also linked to initial meta-liking and early changes, while meta-liking changes toward the end of the interaction occurred independent of all these features and were not predicted by expressive behaviors of interaction partners. This study represents a first empirical test of default expectations and updates in meta-liking based on personality characteristics and social interaction experiences in initial social interactions. We discuss our results in terms of a broader framework for understanding how metaperceptions are formed and updated early in the acquaintance process.
... Critically, the information participants used was largely positive. We assume that this positivity stems from the fact that people seem to enjoy interacting with strangers despite their negative expectations (Dunn et al., 2007;Epley & Schroeder, 2014;Sandstrom et al., 2022). Thus, meta-perceivers and the people they interact with experience more positive emotion of time which would in turn give rise to increasingly more positive meta-liking and liking judgments. ...
... Importantly, this perspective reveals a possibly suboptimal behavior: in prospect, consumers prefer the role that is less demanding but that, in retrospect, provides lower reflective benefits. This finding is complementary to the research showing that consumers often overweigh the potential (social) costs associated with social behaviors and fail to foresee the post-behavior benefits that such investments may yield (Epley & Schroeder, 2014;Sandstrom et al., 2022). As in that work, we also identify both an individual difference factor (extroversion) and a simple intervention (recoupling costs and benefits) that may reduce hosting aversion, potentially enhancing well-being. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we define and conceptualize two commonly assumed roles that have not yet been studied in the marketing literature—that of a host and a guest. We examine consumers’ preference for, and consequences of, assuming each role across three studies. We find that consumers generally prefer being guests (vs. hosts) due to a greater focus on the costs (vs. benefits) associated with the host role. This aversion to hosting is attenuated for extroverts and when consumers are encouraged to recouple the benefits and costs associated with hosting. Importantly, we also find that being a host involves meaningful reflective benefits, as consumers report greater retrospective enjoyment of events they hosted, compared with those they attended as guests), suggesting that consumers’ a priori preference for the less demanding guest role may be misguided.
Article
Full-text available
Daily mood ratings and corresponding diary entries were studied to determine relations between common events and two independent mood factors—Positive Affect (PA) and Negative Affect (NA)—in a sample of 18 young adults over a 3-month period. In an extension of findings from earlier interindividual studies, PA (enthusiastic, delighted vs. sluggish, drowsy) was found to be associated with a wide range of daily events, whereas fewer correlations were found between these events and NA (distressed, nervous, angry vs. calm, relaxed). The relation between high PA and reported social interactions (particularly physically active social events) was especially robust, and its effects were noted repeatedly; NA was unrelated to social activity. As hypothesized, high NA was associated with physical problems; contrary to expectations, low PA also tended to be correlated with health complaints. Overall, the results reaffirm the importance of assessing NA and PA independently and suggest that PA is an interesting and important dimension that deserves more research attention. Theoretical considerations and clinical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Judged by the sheer amount of global media coverage, loneliness rates seem to be an increasingly urgent societal concern. From the late 1970s onward, the life experiences of emerging adults have been changing massively due to societal developments such as increased fragmentation of social relationships, greater mobility opportunities, and changes in communication due to technological innovations. These societal developments might have coincided with an increase in loneliness in emerging adults. In the present preregistered cross-temporal meta-analysis, we examined whether loneliness levels in emerging adults have changed over the last 43 years. Our analysis is based on 449 means from 345 studies with 437 independent samples and a total of 124,855 emerging adults who completed the University of California Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale between 1976 and 2019. Averaged across all studies, loneliness levels linearly increased with increasing calendar years (β = .224, 95% CI [.138, .309]). This increase corresponds to 0.56 standard deviations on the UCLA Loneliness Scale over the 43-year studied period. Overall, the results imply that loneliness can be a rising concern in emerging adulthood. Although the frequently used term “loneliness epidemic” seems exaggerated, emerging adults should therefore not be overlooked when designing interventions against loneliness.
Article
Full-text available
Connecting with others makes people happier, but strangers in close proximity often ignore each other. Prior research (Epley & Schroeder, 2014) suggested this social disconnection stems from people misunderstanding how pleasant it would be to talk with strangers. Extending these prior results, in a field experiment with London-area train commuters, those assigned to talk with a stranger reported having a significantly more positive experience, and learning significantly more, than those assigned to a solitude or control condition. Commuters also expected a more positive experience if they talked to a stranger than in the solitude or control conditions. A second experiment explored why commuters nevertheless avoid conversation even when it is generally pleasant. Commuters predicted that trying to have a conversation would be less pleasant than actually having one because they anticipated that others would be uninterested in talking. These experiments clarify the precise aspects of social interaction that may be misunderstood. People may avoid pleasant conversations with strangers because of miscalibrated concerns about starting them. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
People are often reluctant to talk to strangers, despite the fact that they are happier when they do so. We investigate this apparent paradox, meta-analyzing pre-conversation predictions and post-conversation experiences across seven studies (N = 2304). We examine: fears of not enjoying the conversation, not liking one’s partner, and lacking conversational skills; fears of the partner not enjoying the conversation, not liking oneself, and lacking conversational skills. We examine the relative strength of these fears, and show that the fears are related to talking behavior. We report evidence that people’s fears are overblown. Finally, we report two interventions designed to reduce fears: conversation tips, and the experience of a pleasant conversation. Ultimately, this research shows that conversations go better than expected.
Article
Full-text available
Social relationships are often touted as critical for well-being. However, the vast majority of studies on social relationships have relied on self-report measures of both social interactions and well-being, which makes it difficult to disentangle true associations from shared method variance. To address this gap, we assessed the quantity and quality of social interactions using both self-report and observer-based measures in everyday life. Participants (N = 256; 3,206 observations) wore the Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), an unobtrusive audio recorder, and completed experience sampling method self-reports of their momentary social interactions, happiness, and feelings of social connectedness, 4 times each day for 1 week. Observers rated the quantity and quality of participants' social interactions based on the EAR recordings from the same time points. Quantity of social interactions was robustly associated with greater well-being in the moment and on average, whether they were measured with self-reports or observer reports. Conversational (conversational depth and self-disclosure) and relational (knowing and liking one's interaction partners) aspects of social interaction quality were also generally associated with greater well-being, but the effects were larger and more consistent for self-reported (vs. observer-reported) quality variables, within-person (vs. between-person) associations, and for predicting social connectedness (vs. happiness). Finally, although most associations were similar for introverts and extraverts, our exploratory results suggest that introverts may experience greater boosts in social connectedness, relative to extraverts, when engaging in deeper conversations. This study provides compelling multimethod evidence supporting the link between more frequent and deeper social interactions and well-being. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Having conversations with new people is an important and rewarding part of social life. Yet conversations can also be intimidating and anxiety provoking, and this makes people wonder and worry about what their conversation partners really think of them. Are people accurate in their estimates? We found that following interactions, people systematically underestimated how much their conversation partners liked them and enjoyed their company, an illusion we call the liking gap. We observed the liking gap as strangers got acquainted in the laboratory, as first-year college students got to know their dorm mates, and as formerly unacquainted members of the general public got to know each other during a personal development workshop. The liking gap persisted in conversations of varying lengths and even lasted for several months, as college dorm mates developed new relationships. Our studies suggest that after people have conversations, they are liked more than they know.
Article
Full-text available
Background Compared to traditional persuasive technology and health games, gamification is posited to offer several advantages for motivating behaviour change for health and well-being, and increasingly used. Yet little is known about its effectiveness. Aims We aimed to assess the amount and quality of empirical support for the advantages and effectiveness of gamification applied to health and well-being. Methods We identified seven potential advantages of gamification from existing research and conducted a systematic literature review of empirical studies on gamification for health and well-being, assessing quality of evidence, effect type, and application domain. Results We identified 19 papers that report empirical evidence on the effect of gamification on health and well-being. 59% reported positive, 41% mixed effects, with mostly moderate or lower quality of evidence provided. Results were clear for health-related behaviours, but mixed for cognitive outcomes. Conclusions The current state of evidence supports that gamification can have a positive impact in health and wellbeing, particularly for health behaviours. However several studies report mixed or neutral effect. Findings need to be interpreted with caution due to the relatively small number of studies and methodological limitations of many studies (e.g., a lack of comparison of gamified interventions to non-gamified versions of the intervention).
Article
Conversation is one of the most common ways of establishing social connection and satisfying the need to belong. But despite spending considerable time talking to others, many people report that engaging in informal conversation with anyone other than close friends and family makes them anxious. In this research, we explored people's assessments of their conversational ability. In Studies 1a–1c, we found that people are relatively pessimistic about their skills in conversation when compared to other common activities. We also provide support for the hypothesis that this pessimism is driven by a tendency to not engage in the usual pattern of self‐serving attributions when it comes to the positive and negative moments of conversations. Instead, people attribute the low points of a conversation more to themselves than to the other person (Studies 2 and 3). We discuss the origins of this attributional pattern, as well as other potential mechanisms underlying conversational pessimism, in the General Discussion.
Article
Every relationship begins with a conversation. Past research suggests that after initial conversations, there exists a liking gap: people underestimate how much their partners like them. We extend this finding by providing evidence that it arises in conversations among small groups (Study 1), continues to exist in engineering teams working on a project together (Study 2), and is linked to important consequences for teams' ability to work together in a sample of working adults (Study 3). Additional evidence suggests that the liking gap is largest for peer relationships and that it is determined in part by the extent to which people focus on negative aspects of the impressions they make on others. Group conversations and team interactions often leave people feeling uncertain about where they stand with others, but our studies suggest that people are liked more than they know.