ArticlePDF Available

The Reinforcing Natures of Hyper-Palatable Foods: Behavioral Evidence for Their Reinforcing Properties and the Role of the US Food Industry in Promoting Their Availability

Authors:

Abstract

Purpose of Review Hyper-palatable foods (HPF) may exploit our neurobiological propensities to seek and consume rewarding foods. The review highlights evidence from basic behavioral and neurobiological studies in humans on the reinforcing properties of HPF and consequences of repeated HPF consumption over time. The review also addresses HPF within the context of the US food environment. Recent Findings There is reasonably strong behavioral evidence to indicate that HPF may have reinforcing properties that are similar to drugs of abuse. Evidence indicates that healthy individuals may exhibit greater preference for HPF relative to non-HPF and that powerful cues may develop that indicate the presence of HPF as a reward. Preliminary evidence indicates that elevated HPF intake may yield neurobiological consequences for brain reward neurocircuitry. The US food environment provides wide and easy access to HPF. Conceptualized as a substance, HPF exist unregulated in our environment, similar to the tobacco availability in the 1940s. Parallels have been drawn between food and tobacco company practices; a review of industry documents indicates that tobacco companies owned major US food companies since the 1980s, possibly leading the development and proliferation of HPF. Summary There is reasonably strong evidence to indicate that HPF may have powerful reinforcing properties similar to drugs of abuse; however, more longitudinal work is needed. Critical attention to the factors and drivers of HPF proliferation in the US food system is paramount to conceptualizing the presence of HPF in our food environment and in considering strategies to protect the US population’s health.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Current Addiction Reports
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-022-00417-8
FOOD ADDICTION (E SCHULTE, SECTION EDITOR)
The Reinforcing Natures ofHyper‑Palatable Foods: Behavioral
Evidence forTheir Reinforcing Properties andtheRole oftheUS Food
Industry inPromoting Their Availability
TeraL.Fazzino1,2
Accepted: 10 May 2022
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022
Abstract
Purpose of Review Hyper-palatable foods (HPF) may exploit our neurobiological propensities to seek and consume reward-
ing foods. The review highlights evidence from basic behavioral and neurobiological studies in humans on the reinforcing
properties of HPF and consequences of repeated HPF consumption over time. The review also addresses HPF within the
context of the US food environment.
Recent Findings There is reasonably strong behavioral evidence to indicate that HPF may have reinforcing properties that
are similar to drugs of abuse. Evidence indicates that healthy individuals may exhibit greater preference for HPF relative to
non-HPF and that powerful cues may develop that indicate the presence of HPF as a reward. Preliminary evidence indicates
that elevated HPF intake may yield neurobiological consequences for brain reward neurocircuitry. The US food environment
provides wide and easy access to HPF. Conceptualized as a substance, HPF exist unregulated in our environment, similar
to the tobacco availability in the 1940s. Parallels have been drawn between food and tobacco company practices; a review
of industry documents indicates that tobacco companies owned major US food companies since the 1980s, possibly leading
the development and proliferation of HPF.
Summary There is reasonably strong evidence to indicate that HPF may have powerful reinforcing properties similar to
drugs of abuse; however, more longitudinal work is needed.
Critical attention to the factors and drivers of HPF proliferation in the US food system is paramount to conceptualizing the
presence of HPF in our food environment and in considering strategies to protect the US population’s health.
Keywords Food reward· Food addiction· Reinforcement· Food environment· Policy
Introduction
Food asaReinforcer andHyper‑Palatable Foods
Food consumption promotes our survival. Accordingly, con-
sumption of food is an inherently rewarding process that
serves to increase the likelihood that we consume more food
in the future [1, 2]. There are multiple mechanisms through
which food serves to reinforce our ingestive behavior, which
consist of behavioral learning processes and internal physi-
ological processes. The role of physiological processes,
such as those related to gut-brain signaling, have recently
been reviewed [3, 4]. Behavioral reinforcement processes,
which may have important influences on neurobiological and
physiological processes, will be the focus of this review.
Many foods are palatable in their originally occurring
forms in nature. For example, fresh fruits such as an apple
are typically sweet and pleasant to ingest. Many vegetables,
such as zucchini or broccoli, have savory flavors and may
also be pleasant to ingest. The appealing properties of foods
elicit our approach toward and consumption of food, thereby
promoting our survival [5]. Neurobiologically, consump-
tion of a food (upon initial exposure) activates brain reward
neurocircuitry, which provides a pleasant eating experience
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Food Addiction
* Tera L. Fazzino
tfazzino@ku.edu
1 Department ofPsychology, University ofKansas, 1415
Jayhawk Blvd, 4th Floor, Lawrence, KS66046, USA
2 Cofrin Logan Center forAddiction Research andTreatment,
University ofKansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
and serves to promote further ingestion [6, 7]. With repeated
consumption of a food, cues indicating the presence of a
food (e.g., sweet scent of an apple) activate brain reward
pathways, promoting approach and consumption of the food
[8]. Along with the pleasant and rewarding characteristics
of food, foods that occur in nature typically contain one
primary palatability-inducing nutrient that occurs in com-
bination with nutrients that promote satiety. For example,
sugar in an apple is accompanied by both fiber and water,
which promote satiety and slow the process through which
sugar may be absorbed and digested. Thus overall, a set of
evolutionarily important processes exist that reinforce our
approach toward and consumption of food, and these occur
alongside nutritional safeguards, such as satiety-promoting
nutrients, that together influence our consumption behavior
[9].
Hyper‑Palatable foods
In the current US food system, many available foods have
been modified from their originally occurring forms in
nature. Food modification is not inherently problematic.
However, some foods have been altered in ways that may
serve to exploit our neurobiology and behavioral propen-
sity to approach and consume palatable foods. For example,
hyper-palatable foods (HPF) are foods that have substantial
deviations from their naturally occurring forms, and these
deviations are designed to maximize a food’s rewarding
properties during consumption [10]. In contrast to natu-
rally occurring foods that contain one primary palatability
inducing nutrient, HPF contain combinations of nutrients
that are not typically found in nature (e.g., high quantities of
both sugar and fat) [11]. The combinations of nutrients are
present at thresholds that artificially enhance a food’s palat-
ability and provide a highly rewarding eating experience,
eliciting strong activation in brain reward regions [3, 12].
Furthermore, HPF may delay engagement of physiological
satiety mechanisms and extend eating occasions involving
HPF. Sensory specific satiety is an exposure-based mecha-
nism that serves to bridge food intake with gastro-intestinal
reception and processing of food [13]. Within a meal, each
bite of a food will be experienced as less enjoyable or pleas-
ant than the prior one, which is a learning-based habitua-
tion process that leads to the cessation of consumption [13].
However, sensory-specific satiety is a process that is subject
to variability based on characteristics of a food, and evidence
indicates that HPF may garner weaker sensory specific sati-
ety responses compared to non-HPF, which may facilitate
extended eating occasions [14]. Taken together, HPF may
create a highly rewarding eating experience and may delay
engagement of physiological satiety mechanisms, leading to
excess energy intake.
Many different types of foods may be hyper-palatable.
Our analysis of HPF in the US food system revealed that
HPF are widely available and comprise many different types
of foods and products [10]. For example, some HPF may be
fast foods and desserts, which may be expected to be hyper-
palatable. However, HPF may also many meal-based items,
frozen foods sold in grocery stores, snacks, and even some
foods marketed as healthy due to having reduced calories
or fat [10]. Furthermore, foods identified as related con-
structs in the literature, such as ultra-processed foods and
high energy density foods, may also be hyper-palatable. For
example, ultra-processed foods undergo extensive process-
ing and often contain industrial additives that are designed to
enhance a food’s palatability (e.g., high fructose corn syrup
as a sweetener); thus, many ultra-processed foods may be
hyper-palatable [15]. In addition, foods with elevated energy
density may contain nutrients such as fat and/or sugar that
increase both the caloric density of the food and a food’s pal-
atability [16, 17]. Thus, foods with high energy density may
also have enhanced palatability. To this point, our prior work
has indicated that approximately 50% of foods that are HPF
also have high energy density and that > 60% of HPF may
also be classified as ultra-processed [10, 18]. Thus, foods
that have enhanced palatability are extremely widespread in
our food system and are present across many types and cat-
egories of foods. In this review, the term HPF will be used to
describe foods that may have artificially enhanced palatabil-
ity; however, the foods may be described using other termi-
nology (e.g., high energy density) in their original articles.
Hyper‑Palatable Food asaSubstance
HPF may serve as potent rewards and may yield reinforce-
ment processes that drive habitual intake of HPF, similar to
the reinforcement processes observed with other powerful
reinforcers, such as drugs of abuse. In this regard, Gearhardt
and colleagues have suggested that HPF may be considered
the target substance of food addiction [19, 20]. If viewed
as a substance, HPF may be expected to induce a series
of behavioral and neurobiological changes from repeated
consumption over time, which would serve to drive and
maintain habitual HPF intake. For example, with drugs of
abuse (alcohol, nicotine, opioids, etc.), ample evidence has
indicated that repeated consumption of a substance yields a
series of neurobiological changes that drive continued use
[21, 22]. For example, with repeated substance use, neuroa-
daptive changes occur from repeated and excessive activa-
tion of brain reward neurocircuitry, such that individuals
experience blunted pleasure or reward during use, but also
experience greater motivation or drive to use as they become
hyper-sensitive to substance-related cues in the environment
(sensitization) [22]. As a result, a behavioral pattern occurs
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
in which individuals exhibit strong motivation to use and
engage in compulsive use despite consequences, often while
experiencing less pleasure or rewarding effects while using
[21, 23]. Importantly, the effects are on a continuum and in
a dose–response relationship. Thus, effects may be relevant
for those who engage in a variety of use patterns, from occa-
sional to excessive or daily use [24]. Applied to HPF, similar
behavioral, motivational, and neurobiological changes may
be expected to occur as repeated HPF intake may serve to
dysregulate neurobiological reinforcement mechanisms [25].
Effects may be expected to occur most prominently among
individuals who engage in excess HPF intake and/or who
have individual risk factors or vulnerabilities to the reward-
ing effects of HPF [2628]. However, the reinforcing proper-
ties of HFP would also be expected to be observed on a con-
tinuum, with most of the population exhibiting some level
of approach toward and preferential consumption of HPF,
which may yield passive overconsumption and/or reward-
driven eating behavior. A smaller segment of the population
would be expected to develop compulsive use of HPF due
to preexisting vulnerabilities to the reinforcing properties of
HPF [29]. The evidence for HPF as a potent reinforcer with
similar properties as other substances of abuse is reviewed
herein, with a summary of behavioral evidence first, fol-
lowed by neurobiological evidence in humans. It should
be noted that the foods evaluated in the studies described
below aligned with the premise of hyper-palatability; how-
ever, many used other terms to describe and identify foods.
Behavioral Evidence (Humans)
If HPF serve as potent reinforcers and drive HPF intake
behavior, evidence from basic behavioral studies should
indicate that healthy individuals exhibit preferential valu-
ation and approach toward HPF, relative to foods that are
not HPF (non-HPF). Evidence should also indicate that
with repeated consumption, individuals experience greater
motivation to consume HPF, becoming hyper-sensitive to
HPF cues (sensitization). Overall, there is reasonably strong
evidence to suggest that HPF may be valued and preferred
over non-HPF, and there is preliminary evidence indicating
that repeated HPF intake may elicit enhanced motivation to
consume HPF (sensitization) over time. However, given the
substantial body of literature focused on consequences of
HPF intake (e.g., obesity and food addiction), far fewer basic
behavioral studies have been conducted to establish these
fundamental aspects of HPF as reinforcers among healthy
individuals.
Evidence indicates that healthy individuals may on
average exhibit greater valuation of and preference for
HPF compared to non-HPF. Studies in the literature have
examined preferences for HPF using behavioral tasks that
measure valuation of HPF and non-HPF based on (1) how
much individuals are willing to pay to access an HPF or non-
HPF or (2) how much effort individuals exhibit (e.g., mouse
clicks) to access an HPF or non-HPF. Overall, evidence has
indicated that on average, healthy individuals may be will-
ing to pay more for or exhibit greater behavioral effort to
obtain an HPF vs a non-HPF, indicating greater valuation
and preference for HPF vs non-HPF among healthy samples
of adults [3034], adolescents [3537], and children [38]. In
addition, one study examined preference and choice impul-
sivity for HPF vs non-HPF among healthy adults and used
a standardized definition of HPF to select food items [39].
Similar to the studies of HPF valuation, results indicated
that individuals exhibited greater choice impulsivity that was
specific to HPF, compared to non-HPF [39]. Thus overall,
evidence suggests that healthy individuals on average exhibit
preferential valuation of HPF relative to non-HPF, likely due
to their stronger reinforcing properties.
Evidence also supports the premise that individuals seek
out/approach HPF for their rewarding effects. First, a large
body of evidence has suggested that cues that indicate the
presence of HPF, such as food logos, signs, and related
paraphernalia, may become conditioned stimuli that predict
the availability of HPF [4042]. We navigate our environ-
ments by learning about cues that maximize our chances
of obtaining evolutionarily meaningful rewards (e.g., food)
[40]. Thus, the development of conditioned cues for HPF
speaks to the strength of HPF as a reinforcer and parallels
the cue development processes that occur with substances
[43]. In addition to eliciting approach to HPF cues, prelimi-
nary evidence also indicates that HPF may be sought and
consumed for their rewarding effects. For example, research-
ers have found that one common motive that individuals
seek out HPF is for their rewarding effects, termed enhance-
ment motive in the literature [44, 45]. Consuming HPF for
their rewarding effects has been specifically identified as an
important motive in binge eating when conceptualized on a
continuum from passive overconsumption to loss of control
eating [46]. Furthermore, preliminary evidence indicates
that HPF, when defined using the standardized definition,
comprised the vast majority of calories consumed during
binge eating episodes among a clinical eating disorders
sample, thus indicating that HPF may be primarily targeted
for consumption during clinically significant binge eating
episodes [47]. Overall, evidence indicates that HPF may be
sought out for their rewarding effects, consistent with other
substances of abuse.
Preliminary behavioral evidence from several studies has
suggested that repeated HPF consumption over time may
increase motivation to seek out HPF as individuals become
hyper-sensitive to HPF cues; however, observed effects have
varied across studies and groups examined. For example,
two experimental studies among healthy adults used 14-day
exposure protocols in which participants consumed the same
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
HPF snack daily. Both studies reported that motivation to
consume the HPF, measured behaviorally, increased over
the exposure period for individuals with obesity; however,
motivation to consume HPF decreased among individuals
without obesity [48, 49]. In a longer trial among healthy
adults, findings revealed that motivation to consume HPF
did not change following daily HPF snack intake over
12weeks [50]. The retention of motivation to consume the
HPF snack despite repeated exposure over time was inter-
preted to indicate that HPF may circumvent habituation pro-
cesses that would be expected to occur with repeated expo-
sure over time [50]. Finally, in another 14-day consumption
trial among adolescents, researchers reported the sample
on average exhibited decreases in motivation to consume
HPF following daily HPF intake [37]. However, the small
proportion of adolescents in the sample who experienced
increased motivation to consume HPF had greater weight
gain at 2-year follow-up [37].
Overall, a reasonably strong body of basic behavioral
evidence suggests that on average, healthy children, ado-
lescents, and adults may exhibit greater valuation of HPF
relative to non-HPF and may seek out HPF in response
to cues in the environment. Longitudinal evidence of the
effects of HPF from repeated consumption over time is much
more limited and has indicated that in response to repeated
HPF intake over 14–21days, some individuals may experi-
ence enhanced motivation to consume HPF (sensitization);
however, it is unclear whether the effects may be observed
overall or individuals who may be particularly sensitive to
the rewarding effects of HPF. Thus, more work is needed,
particularly longitudinal consumption studies to characterize
the effects of HPF exposure on potential change in motiva-
tion to consume HPF over time, which may lead to habitual
HPF intake.
Neurobiological Evidence (Humans)
A limited number of studies have examined the basic effects
of HPF consumption on brain reward neurocircuitry (exam-
ined cross-sectionally) and neurobiological changes (exam-
ined longitudinally). Regarding cross-sectional evidence of
HPF intake patterns and brain reward responsivity, most of
the evidence has been indirect. For example, substantial evi-
dence from cross-sectional studies has identified differences
in brain reward responsivity by weight class or BMI [51]. In
these studies, BMI or obesity status has been used as a proxy
to indicate repeated/habitual HPF intake; however, most
studies have not studied the association between HPF intake
and brain reward responsivity explicitly. In this regard, in
a recent meta-analytic review of the literature, researchers
reported that individuals with obesity were found to exhibit
greater responsivity to food cues in brain regions that gov-
ern reward and motivation, suggesting that individuals with
obesity may exhibit sensitization to HPF cues [51]. Only
three prior cross-sectional studies directly examined the
association between HPF intake and brain reward respon-
sivity, all of which were conducted among healthy samples
of adolescents or adults. For example, the studies used fMRI
paradigms and found that greater dietary intake of HPF or
sugar was associated with greater responsivity to HPF cues
in brain reward regions (dorsal and ventral striatum), sug-
gesting sensitization to HPF [52], and reduced responsivity
in striatal regions when consuming an HPF (e.g., a milk-
shake) [53, 54], thus indicating blunted reward respond-
ing during consumption. Overall, more work is needed to
directly examine cross-sectional associations between HPF
consumption and differences in HPF motivation and reward
responsivity.
A small body of evidence from longitudinal studies sug-
gests that neurobiological changes may occur among ado-
lescents and adults from HPF or sugar intake over time.
However, only one prior study measured intake, and the
remainder assumed HPF intake through observed weight
gain. Specifically, Burger etal. [55] provided experimen-
tal evidence that indicated that after consuming a sugar-
sweetened beverage for 21days, adolescents exhibited
marked increases in motivation to consume the beverage
when exposed to cues and decreases in response in reward
regions (e.g., dorsal striatum) during beverage consumption
in fMRI. The large effects sizes suggested sensitization to
the sugar-sweetened beverage cues over time [55]. In addi-
tion, two repeated measures studies examined the associa-
tion between weight or body fat gain, which was assumed
to be a result of repeated HPF intake, and responsivity to
HPF cues and consumption [56, 57]. Findings indicated
that adolescents who gained body fat over a 3-year period
exhibited increased striatal responsivity to HPF cues, sug-
gesting increased motivation to consume HPF [56]. Further-
more, in a sample of adult women, individuals who gained
weight over a 6-month period were found to have reductions
in striatal responsivity to consumption of an HPF during
an fMRI paradigm [57], suggesting blunted responsivity to
HPF reward during consumption. In addition to these stud-
ies, three studies with healthy children [58] and adolescents
[59, 60] identified greater responsivity to HPF cues in brain
reward regions at baseline to be prospectively associated
with weight gain, with the assumption that greater HPF
intake yielded may have contributed to changes in weight
observed longitudinally.
Overall very limited evidence has directly connected HPF
intake to cross-sectional differences or longitudinal changes
in HPF reward responsivity and motivation. Most research
has used BMI or weight gain and assumed that HPF con-
sumption was the underlying driver. More research is needed
to directly examine the association between HPF intake and
neurobiological changes. While not reviewed herein, the
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
animal literature presents compelling evidence that HPF and
sugar intake may yield substantial neurobiological changes
that are consistent with neurobiological changes observed
with substances of abuse [6164]. Thus, evidence in the
human literature is needed to examine the basic connections
between HPF intake and changes in reward neurocircuitry
among healthy samples of children, adolescents, and adults,
information that is key to understand potential consequences
related to habitual/compulsive HPF intake, obesity, and food
addiction.
In addition to a limited body of evidence, the existing
literature is limited due to inconsistencies in terminology
and definitions used to describe highly reinforcing foods.
Prior studies in the behavioral and neuroimaging literature
used a variety of descriptive definitions (e.g., fast foods)
or terms (e.g., high energy density foods) when referenc-
ing foods that may be particularly reinforcing. Future work
using a standardize definition of foods that have enhanced
reinforcing properties is needed to bring concordance to the
literature. Recently, Fazzino etal. [39] provided a stand-
ardized definition of HPF, which specifies quantitative
thresholds for combinations of nutrients (fat, sugar, carbo-
hydrates, and/or sodium) that are hypothesized to induce
hyper-palatability and as a result may be highly rewarding
to consume. The provision of this definition is a first step in
facilitating research with a standardized definition, which
may strengthen scientific inquiry and policy guidance. In
addition, given that definitions of energy density and ultra-
processed foods do not directly address a food’s palatabil-
ity (but do address other important food characteristics that
may increase their reinforcing properties), it is important for
future research to elucidate the degree to which the defini-
tions identify foods that may be consistent with or may be
vary in their reinforcing properties compared to HPF. Over-
all, the field has substantial work to do to better define and
identify the specific characteristics of foods (e.g., ingredient
combinations and food processing characteristics) that yield
their excessive reinforcing properties, which may strongly
influence our food approach and intake behavior.
Distinction Between HPF asSubstance vs Individual
Differences inVulnerability toHPF
The present review focuses on the reinforcing properties of
HPF, conceptualized as a substance. This premise is distinct
from most of the literature, which has focused on identifying
individual-level differences in vulnerabilities to the effects of
HPF. Most research has focused on identifying subgroups or
characteristics of individuals who may be vulnerable to HPF
effects and may be at risk for developing obesity or food
addiction. For example, ample work from Epstein and col-
leagues has established that individuals differ in the degree
to which they value food as a reward and that individuals
with high food reinforcement may be at greater risk for obe-
sity [6567]. In a similar lens of identifying individual-level
vulnerabilities to HPF, Stice and Burger [68] presented a
theoretical framework for obesity that identified individual-
level differences in sensitivity to HPF cues and neurobio-
logical changes from repeated HPF intake that may combine
to increase obesity risk. Finally, Gearhardt and colleagues
identified food addiction as a clinical phenomenon that is
exhibited among a subset of individuals who may be particu-
larly susceptible to the rewarding effects of HPF and who
demonstrate clinically significant symptoms and impairment
from HPF consumption [69, 70].
Overall, extensive work has been conducted in the field
to identify individual-level vulnerabilities to the rewarding
effects of HPF that may occur among a subset of individuals
in the population. Importantly, HPF may be the substance
that directly facilitates the observed effects and conse-
quences. Thus, it is critical to gain a more thorough scientific
understanding of HPF, defined using a standard definition,
as a substance, in a similar manner as the field of addiction,
which has conducted extensive research to understand the
basic neurobiological and behavioral impacts of substance
use (e.g., alcohol, nicotine, and opioids) on healthy indi-
viduals. Thoroughly understanding the neurobiological and
behavioral changes that may occur from the intake of HPF is
critical to developing prevention and treatment approaches
that recognize the potency of the substance (HPF) and best
address the target mechanism (HPF intake) that may yield
clinical consequences such as obesity and food addiction
among vulnerable individuals.
Hyper‑Palatable Foods intheUS Food
Environment
Behavior Doesn’t Occur inaVacuum: The Food
Environment
HPF are powerful reinforcers; however, use of a substance,
or HPF in this case, is largely influenced by the surrounding
environment. Choice is relative and depends on the avail-
ability of HPF, as well as the other alternatives in the envi-
ronment. In the USA, the food environment is considered
obesogenic and is structured in a manner that maximizes
access to HPF and provides limited access to alternative
foods (e.g., non-HPF such as fresh fruits and vegetables)
[71, 72]. This environmental structure may exacerbate
approach and consumption of HPF as normative eating
behavior (passive overconsumption), as well as potentially
pathological behavior (e.g., food addiction). Thus, the food
environment is structured to exploit our natural behavioral
tendencies to seek out convenience in HPF access, and HPF
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
themselves are designed to exploit our neurobiological pro-
clivities to seek and consume highly rewarding foods. This
picture is grim when considering the degree to which it may
be possible to access and eat foods that do not excessively
activate our brain reward neurocircuitry and the potential
for population-level health consequences, such as obesity
and food addiction.
How Did We Get Here? US Tobacco Company
Involvement intheUS Food System
The US food environment has changed dramatically since
the early 1970s and may have been strongly influenced by
the entrance of US tobacco companies to the food system.
Below, changes in the food system are described first, fol-
lowed by a review of tobacco company involvement in the
food system. Before the 1970s, the food environment in the
USA was largely supplied by smaller, local food produc-
ers, and regional companies [73]. However, in the 1970s
and 1980s, larger food companies developed by absorbing
smaller food producers and food companies and centralizing
food processing and distribution efforts [74]. As a result,
food companies maximized their profits by consolidating
production expenses and distribution efforts while substan-
tially increasing sales in the USA and globally [74, 75]. As
food companies focused on minimizing costs, the companies
also focused on developing food technology and creating
less costly versions of ingredients, such as high fructose corn
syrup, an inexpensive alternative to sugar [76]. As a result,
the US food environment became saturated with inexpen-
sive, easily accessible HPF [75, 77].
The changes observed in the food environment since
the 1970s occurred alongside the entrance of a new player
in the US food system, US tobacco companies. Leading
tobacco companies in the USA, specifically Phillip Mor-
ris and RJ Reynolds, began investing in food companies
in the 1970s and 1980s, which served to compensate for
declining tobacco sales and increased federal regulation
of tobacco products in the USA [78, 79]. By the 1990s,
tobacco companies led the US food market in sales annu-
ally [80]. For example, tobacco company Phillip Morris
entered the US food market in the early 1980s and bought
two major food brands, Kraft and General Foods. By 1985,
Phillip Morris had more than double the US market shares
compared to any other food company [81], a position that
was maintained through the 1990s [80]. By 1999, Phil-
lip Morris brands led the market in sales of 17 out of 20
food product categories, such as frozen foods [78]. The
investment in the food system was extremely profitable
for Phillip Morris; sales from food products were approxi-
mately equal to sales of tobacco products between 1989
and 2001 [78, 82, 83] and helped buoy the company’s prof-
its during a time of intense litigation and profit loss from
tobacco. Phillip Morris-developed food products are still
very present in the food environment today. Kraft remains
a leading food company and is owned by a now-parent
company of Phillip Morris, Altria Group [83, 84]. Thus,
Phillip Morris’s influence and involvement in the food sys-
tem has been a reliable and stable factor in our food system
since the early 1980s.
The RJ Reynolds tobacco company had a similar tra-
jectory of involvement in the food system, although their
food products were specialized to specific market seg-
ments. RJ Reynolds initially bought into the US food
system via the sugar-sweetened beverage market in the
1960s and expanded their ownership to include Nabisco, a
major snack food company, in the 1970s [85, 86]. By 1986,
sales of food and tobacco products for RJ Reynolds were
approximately equal in the company’s portfolio [79], sug-
gesting food sales were a major contributor to RJ Reynolds
revenue in the tobacco litigation era. By 1990, Nabisco
was the largest manufacturer and marketer in cookie and
cracker industry in USA, with market shares that were
2–3 times larger than their closest competitors [79]. Thus,
RJ Reynolds also had a major impact in leading the US
food system through the 1990s, with a specific focus on
specialty snacks and sweetened drinks. However, by the
late 1990s, RJ Reynold’s market shares from food products
dropped, and by the early 2000s, RJ Reynolds divested
from Nabisco ownership, thus relinquishing its broader
influence in the food system [87].
The influence of US tobacco companies in the food sys-
tem has been extensive, and tobacco company leadership
in the food system likely played a major role in shaping
the current US food environment. However, the scientific
evidence identifying the ways in which the tobacco compa-
nies influenced the food system is extremely limited. In two
foundational studies that leveraged primary source docu-
ments provided by the University of California, San Fran-
cisco Industry Documents Library, Nguyen and colleagues
revealed that both RJ Reynolds and Phillip Morris used the
techniques they developed in tobacco product development,
sales, and marketing to develop and market unhealthy food
and beverage products to vulnerable populations in the USA,
specifically children [88], and racial and ethnic minority
groups [89]. Thus, Nguyen and colleagues have provided
the first pieces of evidence to suggest that tobacco ownership
of food companies was likely problematic for public health.
A missing piece of the puzzle is whether tobacco compa-
nies applied their techniques for creating addictive tobacco
products to develop foods that may be difficult to stop eat-
ing and have addictive properties (i.e., HPF). Thus, another
key piece of evidence needed to expand our understand-
ing of the impact of tobacco company involvement in the
US food system is a potential connection between tobacco
ownership and HPF proliferation in the food environment.
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
This information is needed to fully conceptualize our food
system, to consider ways in which HPF may be designed to
exploit our neurobiology, and implications for addressing
clinically relevant conditions, including obesity and food
addiction.
Conclusion
HPF differ in key ways from naturally occurring foods and
may exploit our neurobiological propensities to seek and
consume rewarding foods. There is reasonably strong behav-
ioral evidence to indicate that HPF may have reinforcing
properties that are similar to drugs of abuse. Strong evidence
indicates that healthy individuals may exhibit greater pref-
erence for and valuation of HPF relative to non-HPF, that
individuals may seek out HPF for their rewarding effects,
and that cues may develop that indicate the presence of
HPF. All of these factors speak to the reinforcing properties
of HPF. More limited evidence indicates that HPF intake
may be associated with greater motivation to consume HPF
(sensitization) and blunted experience of reward during HPF
intake, which may be observed neurobiologically. More
research is needed to understand and characterize longitu-
dinal neurobiological and behavioral changes that may occur
from repeated HPF intake over time among healthy human
samples.
The US food environment has shifted dramatically since
the 1970s and currently provides extensive access to HPF.
The major changes in the US food environment may have
been driven by US tobacco companies, which invested
heavily in the US food system in the early 1980s. Thus, the
leader of the US food system over the past 30years has been
US tobacco companies, which have specialized in creating
addictive tobacco products, and applied their product devel-
opment and marketing efforts to develop and sell food in
the US food system [88]. At this juncture, it is critical for
researchers to investigate whether tobacco companies may
have driven the design and proliferation of HPF in the food
environment, which have marked similarities to other addic-
tive substances. If this connection is established, it would
provide a strong justification for the regulation of HPF at
the federal level. Substances, such as alcohol and nicotine,
are regulated in the US environment because it is recognized
that they may have substantial detrimental effects on vulner-
able groups in the population, including children and those
with risk factors for substance use disorder. This same prem-
ise should be considered for HPF, which also have estab-
lished harmful effects on individuals who are vulnerable to
their rewarding properties (e.g., food addiction and obesity),
and who may suffer an outsized health burden as HPF are
allowed to proliferate the US food environment unregulated.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Competing Interests The author declare no competing interests.
Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent No human or animal
subject data were used in the manuscript.
References
Papers of particular interest, published recently, have
been highlighted as:
• Of importance
1. Sclafani A. Oral and postoral determinants of food reward.
Physiol Behav. 2004;81:773–9.
2. Sclafani A. Post-ingestive positive controls of ingestive behavior.
Appetite. 2001;36:79–83.
3. Small DM, DiFeliceantonio AG. Processed foods and food
reward. Science. American Association for the Advancement of
Science; 2019;363:346–7.
4. de Araujo IE, Schatzker M, Small DM. Rethinking food reward.
Annu Rev Psychol. 2020;71:139–64.
5. Breslin PAS. An evolutionary perspective on food and human
taste. Curr Biol. 2013;23:R409–18.
6. Berridge KC. Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and lik-
ing. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1996;20:1–25.
7. Small DM, Jones-Gotman M, Dagher A. Feeding-induced
dopamine release in dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleas-
antness ratings in healthy human volunteers. Neuroimage.
2003;19:1709–15.
8. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in
reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?
Brain Res Rev. 1998;28:309–69.
9. Gerstein DE, Woodward-Lopez G, Evans AE, Kelsey K,
Drewnowski A. Clarifying concepts about macronutri-
ents’ effects on satiation and satiety. J Am Diet Assoc.
2004;104:1151–3.
10.• Fazzino TL, Rohde K, Sullivan DK. Hyper-palatable foods:
development of a quantitative definition and application to the
US food system database. Obesity. Wiley; 2019;27:1761–8. The
article presented the first definition of hyper-palatable foods
that is standardized and uses quantitative criteria to identify
foods based on nutrient combinations linked to enhanced
palatability. The article also quantified the extensive avail-
ability of hyper-palatable foods in the US food system.
11. Balter V, Braga J, Télouk P, Thackeray JF. Evidence for dietary
change but not landscape use in South African early hominins.
Nature. Nature Publishing Group; 2012;489:558–60.
12. DiFeliceantonio AG, Coppin G, Rigoux L, Edwin Thanarajah
S, Dagher A, Tittgemeyer M, etal. Supra-additive effects of
combining fat and carbohydrate on food reward. Cell Metab.
2018;28:33-44.e3.
13. Rolls BJ. Sensory-specific satiety. Nutr Rev. 1986;44:93–101.
14. Johnson J, Vickers Z. Factors influencing sensory-specific sati-
ety. Appetite. 1992;19:15–31.
15. Monteiro CA, Cannon G, Moubarac J-C, Levy RB, Louzada
MLC, Jaime PC. The UN decade of nutrition, the NOVA food
classification and the trouble with ultra-processing. Public
Health Nutr. 2018;21:5–17.
16. Rolls BJ. The role of energy density in the overconsumption of
fat. J Nutr. 2000;130:268S-271S.
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
17. Rolls BJ. Dietary energy density: applying behavioural science
to weight management. Nutr Bull. Wiley; 2017;42:246–53.
18. Fazzino TL, Dorling JL, Apolzan JW, Martin CK. Meal compo-
sition during an adlibitum buffet meal and longitudinal predic-
tions of weight and percent body fat change: the role of hyper-
palatable, energy dense, and ultra-processed foods. Appetite.
2021;167:105592.
19.• Gearhardt AN, Davis C, Kuschner R, Brownell KD. The addic-
tion potential of hyperpalatable foods. Curr Drug Abuse Rev.
4:140–5. In this article Gearhardt and colleagues discuss the
importance for considering whether certain foods may have
addictive potential, and the relevance of this question for
individual and population health.
20. Schulte EM, Potenza MN, Gearhardt AN. A commentary on
the “eating addiction” versus “food addiction” perspectives on
addictive-like food consumption. Appetite. 2017;115:9–15.
21. Koob GF, Moal ML. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward,
and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97–129.
22. Volkow ND, Morales M. The brain on drugs: from reward to
addiction. Cell. 2015;162:712–25.
23. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving:
an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain
Res Rev. 1993;18:247–91.
24. Lender A, Miedl S, Wilhelm F, Miller J, Blechert J. Love at first
taste: activation in reward-related brain regions during single-
trial naturalistic appetitive conditioning in humans. Physiol
Behav. 2020;224:113014.
25. Gilbert JR, Burger KS. Neuroadaptive processes associated with
palatable food intake: present data and future directions. Curr
Opin Behav Sci. 2016;9:91–6.
26. Volkow ND, Wise RA, Baler R. The dopamine motive system:
implications for drug and food addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci.
Nature Publishing Group; 2017;18:741–52.
27. Temple JL. Behavioral sensitization of the reinforcing value
of food: what food and drugs have in common. Prev Med.
2016;92:90–9.
28. Morales I, Berridge KC. ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ in eating and
food reward: Brain mechanisms and clinical implications. Phys-
iol Behav. 2020;227:113152.
29. Zorrilla EP, Koob GF. Chapter6 - The dark side of compul-
sive eating and food addiction: affective dysregulation, negative
reinforcement, and negative urgency. In: Cottone P, Sabino V,
Moore CF, Koob GF (eds). Compulsive Eating Behavior and
Food Addiction. Academic Press; 2019;p. 115–92. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/ B978-0- 12- 816207- 1. 00006-8.
30. Verdejo-Román J, Vilar-López R, Navas JF, Soriano-Mas C,
Verdejo-García A. Brain reward system’s alterations in response
to food and monetary stimuli in overweight and obese individu-
als. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:666–77.
31. Giesen JCAH, Havermans RC, Douven A, Tekelenburg M,
Jansen A. Will work for snack food: the association of bmi and
snack reinforcement. Obesity. 2010;18:966–70.
32. Giesen JCAH, Havermans RC, Jansen A. Substituting snacks
with strawberries and Sudokus: does restraint matter? Health
Psychol. 2010;29:222–6.
33. Epstein LH, Paluch RA, Carr KA, Temple JL, Bickel WK, MacKillop
J. Reinforcing value and hypothetical behavioral economic demand for
food and their relation to BMI. Eat Behav. 2018;29:120–7.
34. Epstein LH, Stein JS, Paluch RA, MacKillop J, Bickel WK.
Binary components of food reinforcement: amplitude and per-
sistence. Appetite. 2018;120:67–74.
35. Temple JL. Factors that influence the reinforcing value of foods
and beverages. Physiol Behav. 2014;136:97–103.
36. Vervoort L, Clauwaert A, Vandeweghe L, Vangeel J, Van Lippe-
velde W, Goossens L, etal. Factors influencing the reinforcing
value of fruit and unhealthy snacks. Eur J Nutr. 2017;56:2589–98.
37. Temple JL, Ziegler AM, Crandall AK, Mansouri T, Hatzinger
L, Barich R, etal. Sensitization of the reinforcing value of
high energy density foods is associated with increased zBMI
gain in adolescents. Int J Obes. Nature Publishing Group;
2022;46:581–7.
38. Eagleton SG, Temple JL, Keller KL, Marini ME, Savage JS. The
relative reinforcing value of cookies is higher among head start
preschoolers with obesity. Front Psychol. 2021;12:653762.
39. Fazzino T, Bjorlie K, Rohde K, Smith A, Yi R. Choices between
money and hyper-palatable food: choice impulsivity and eating
behavior. Health Psychol. in press;
40. Bouton ME. Learning and the persistence of appetite: extinc-
tion and the motivation to eat and overeat. Physiol Behav.
2011;103:51–8.
41 Boswell RG, Kober H. Food cue reactivity and craving pre-
dict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes Rev.
2016;17:159–77.
42. Belfort-DeAguiar R, Seo D. Food cues and obesity: overpower-
ing hormones and energy balance regulation. Curr Obes Rep.
2018;7:122–9.
43. Flagel SB, Akil H, Robinson TE. Individual differences in the
attribution of incentive salience to reward-related cues: implica-
tions for addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:139–48.
44 Burgess EE, Turan B, Lokken KL, Morse A, Boggiano MM.
Profiling motives behind hedonic eating. Preliminary validation
of the palatable eating motives scale. Appetite. 2014;72:66–72.
45. Boggiano MM. Palatable eating motives scale in a college popu-
lation: distribution of scores and scores associated with greater
BMI and binge-eating. Eat Behav. 2016;21:95–8.
46. Fazzino TL, Raheel A, Peppercorn N, Forbush K, Kirby T,
Sher KJ, etal. Motives for drinking alcohol and eating palat-
able foods: an evaluation of shared mechanisms and associations
with drinking and binge eating. Addict Behav. 2018;85:113–9.
47. Bjorlie K, Forbush KT, Chapa DAN, Richson BN, Johnson SN,
Fazzino TL. Hyper-palatable food consumption during binge-
eating episodes: a comparison of intake during binge eating and
restricting. Int J Eat Disord. 2022;55(5):688–96. https:// doi. org/
10. 1002/ eat. 23692.
48. Clark EN, Dewey AM, Temple JL. Effects of daily snack food
intake on food reinforcement depend on body mass index and
energy density. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91:300–8.
49. Temple JL, Bulkley AM, Badawy RL, Krause N, McCann S,
Epstein LH. Differential effects of daily snack food intake on the
reinforcing value of food in obese and nonobese women. Am J
Clin Nutr. 2009;90:304–13.
50. Tey SL, Brown RC, Gray AR, Chisholm AW, Delahunty CM.
Long-term consumption of high energy-dense snack foods on sen-
sory-specific satiety and intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:1038–47.
51. Devoto F, Zapparoli L, Bonandrini R, Berlingeri M, Ferrulli A,
Luzi L, etal. Hungry brains: a meta-analytical review of brain
activation imaging studies on food perception and appetite in
obese individuals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;94:271–85.
52. Dorton HM, Luo S, Monterosso JR, Page KA. Influences of
dietary added sugar consumption on striatal food-cue reactivity
and postprandial GLP-1 response. Front Psychiatry. 2018;8:297.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyt. 2017. 00297.
53. Burger KS, Stice E. Frequent ice cream consumption is associ-
ated with reduced striatal response to receipt of an ice cream–
based milkshake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:810–7.
54. Stice E, Spoor S, Bohon C, Veldhuizen MG, Small DM. Relation
of reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obe-
sity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Abnorm
Psychol. 2008;117:924–35.
55. Burger KS. Frontostriatal and behavioral adaptations to daily
sugar-sweetened beverage intake: a randomized controlled trial.
Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;105:555–63.
Current Addiction Reports
1 3
56. Stice E, Yokum S. Gain in body fat is associated with increased
striatal response to palatable food cues, whereas body fat stabil-
ity is associated with decreased striatal response. J Neurosci.
2016;36:6949–56.
57. Stice E, Yokum S, Blum K, Bohon C. Weight gain is associ-
ated with reduced striatal response to palatable food. J Neurosci.
2010;30:13105–9.
58. Masterson TD, Bobak C, Rapuano KM, Shearrer GE, Gilbert-
Diamond D. Association between regional brain volumes and
BMI z-score change over one year in children. PLOS ONE. Pub-
lic Library of Science; 2019;14:e0221995.
59. Yokum S, Ng J, Stice E. Attentional bias to food images associ-
ated with elevated weight and future weight gain: an fMRI study.
Obesity (Silver Spring). 2011;19:1775–83.
60. Stice E, Burger KS, Yokum S. Reward region responsivity pre-
dicts future weight gain and moderating effects of the TaqIA
allele. J Neurosci. 2015;35:10316–24.
61. Zorrilla EP, Kreisler AD, Bagsic SR. Intermittent Extended
Access Rodent Models of Compulsive Eating. In: Avena NM
(ed). Anim Models Eat Disord. New York: Springer US; 2021;p.
133–62. Available from: https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ 978-1- 0716-
0924-8_8. Accessed 21 Mar 2022.
62. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Addiction-like reward dysfunction and
compulsive eating in obese rats: Role for dopamine D2 recep-
tors. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:635–41.
63. Colantuoni C, Rada P, McCarthy J, Patten C, Avena NM,
Chadeayne A, etal. Evidence that intermittent, excessive
sugar intake causes endogenous opioid dependence. Obes Res.
2002;10:478–88.
64. Lalanza JF, Snoeren EMS. The cafeteria diet: a standardized
protocol and its effects on behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2021;122:92–119.
65. Epstein LH, Leddy JJ. Food reinforcement. Appetite.
2006;46:22–5.
66. Epstein LH, Leddy JJ, Temple JL, Faith MS. Food rein-
forcement and eating: a multilevel analysis. Psychol Bull.
2007;133:884–906.
67. Epstein LH, Lin H, Carr KA, Fletcher KD. Food reinforce-
ment and obesity: psychological Moderators. Appetite.
2012;58:157–62.
68. Stice E, Burger K. Neural vulnerability factors for obesity. Clin
Psychol Rev.2019;68:38–53.https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cpr. 2018.
12. 002.
69. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary valida-
tion of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite. 2009;52:430–6.
70 Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Development of the
Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0. Psychol Addict Behav.
2016;30:113–21.
71. Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT,
Moodie ML, etal. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by
global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011;378:804–14.
72. Swinburn B, Kraak V, Rutter H, Vandevijvere S, Lobstein T,
Sacks G, etal. Strengthening of accountability systems to create
healthy food environments and reduce global obesity. Lancet.
2015;385:2534–45.
73. Lyson TA. Civic Agriculture: Reconnecting Farm, Food, and
Community. University Press of New England, Lebanon.
74. Lyson T, Lewis RA. Stalking the wily multinational: power
and control in the US food system. Agric Hum Values.
2000;17:199–208.
75. Stuckler D, Nestle M. Big food, food systems, and global health.
PLOS Med. Public Library of Science; 2012;9:e1001242.
76. Alfranca O, Rama R, von Tunzelmann N. Technological fields
and concentration of innovation among food and beverage mul-
tinationals. Int Food Agribus Manag Rev. International Food and
Agribusiness Management Association; 2003;05:1–14.
77. Nestle M. Food politics: how the food industry influences nutri-
tion and health. Food Polit. University of California Press; 2013
[cited 2022 Mar 31]. Available from: https:// doi. org/ 10. 1525/
97805 20955 066/ html.
78. Phillip Morris Companies, Inc. Philip Morris Companies Inc.
1999 Annual Report- University of California San Francisco
Industry Documents Library [Internet]. 1999. https:// www. indus
trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ tobac co/ docs/# id= yjhj0 223. Accessed 15
June 2021.
79. RJR Nabisco. Securities and exchange commission form 10-K
annual report pursuant to section13 OR 15 (D) of the securities
exchange act of 1934 (340000). RJR NABISCO HOLDINGS
CORP. 1991. Available from: https:// www. indus trydo cumen ts.
ucsf. edu/ docs/# id= sfdv0 082. Accessed 29 Mar 2022.
80. Phillip Morris. PM Plan Overivew 1994–1998. 1993.
81. psgh0120 - Bernstein Research Equity Portfolio Strategy SP... -
Industry Documents Library. 1991. Available from: https:// www.
indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ docs/# id= psgh0 120. Accessed 15
June 2021.
82. Phillip Morris Companies, Inc. Philip Morris Companies Inc.
five year plan 1989–1993. 1989. Available from: https:// www.
indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ docs/# id= ymbl0 000. Accessed 16
June 2021.
83. Altria Group, Inc. pfnw0189 - 10-K Annual report pursuant to
section13 and 1... - Industry Documents Library. 2001. Avail-
able from: https:// www. indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ docs/# id=
pfnw0 189. Accessed 16 June 2021.
84. Altria Group, Inc. kkhj0223 - Form 10-K annual report pursuant
to section13 ... - Industry Documents Library. 2007. Available
from: https:// www. indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ docs/# id= kkhj0
223. Accessed 29 Mar 2022.
85. RJ Reynolds. myng0099 - RJ Reynolds Industries 1979 (790000)
Annual REP... - Industry Documents Library. 1979. Avail-
able from: https:// www. indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ docs/# id=
myng0 099. Accessed 29 Mar 2022.
86. RJR Nabisco. rscv0082 - Securities and exchange commission
form 10-K. - Truth Tobacco Industry Documents . 1987. Avail-
able from: https:// www. indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ tobac co/
docs/# id= rscv0 082. Accessed 29 Mar 2022.
87. RJR Nabisco. njhg0188 - RJR NABISCO 1997 (970000) Operat-
ing plan. - Industry Documents Library. 1996. Available from:
https:// www. indus trydo cumen ts. ucsf. edu/ docs/# id= njhg0 188.
Accessed 26 Jan 2022.
88• Nguyen KH, Glantz SA, Palmer CN, Schmidt LA. Tobacco
industry involvement in children’s sugary drinks market. BMJ.
2019;364:l736. In this article, Nguyen and colleagues pre-
sent the first evidence of tobacco industry involvement in
the US food industry, in which tobacco company practices
related to food marketing and product development were
directly applied to developing and marketing sugary drinks
to children.
89. Nguyen KH, Glantz SA, Palmer CN, Schmidt LA. Transferring
racial/ethnic marketing strategies from tobacco to food corpora-
tions: Philip Morris and kraft general foods. Am J Public Health.
American Public Health Association; 2020;110:329–36.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
... Collectively, PM-and RJR-owned companies dominated the US food system between the late 1980s to the early 2000s [14][15][16][17]; thus, companies that specialized in creating addictive tobacco products led the development of the US food system for > 20 years (see Fazzino [18] for a detailed review of industry activities). In this regard, evidence has revealed that RJR and PM directly applied their tobacco product formulation strategies to their food company practices [8,9]. ...
... Prior studies have described how certain foods may have addictive qualities similar to those of tobacco products and other substances of abuse [18,19]. Such foods, termed hyper-palatable foods (HPF), may be an addictive agent that tobacco companies developed in their food products. ...
... Our findings indicate that tobacco companies were consistently involved in the dissemination of HPF into the US food system between 1988 and 2001, a time during which tobacco-owned food companies led the US food industry [15][16][17][18]37]. The three types of HPF have distinct formulations (fat and sodium HPF; fat and sugar HPF; carbohydrates and sodium HPF), and our evidence suggests that tobacco companies may have selectively targeted the promotion and dissemination of two types of HPF, specifically fat and sodium HPF and carbohydrate and sodium HPF. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and aims: US tobacco companies owned leading US food companies from 1980 to 2001. We measured whether hyper-palatable foods (HPF) were disproportionately developed in tobacco-owned food companies, resulting in substantial tobacco-related influence on the US food system. Design: The study involved a review of primary industry documents to identify food brands that were tobacco company-owned. Data sets from the US Department of Agriculture were integrated to facilitate longitudinal analyses estimating the degree to which foods were formulated to be hyper-palatable, based on tobacco ownership. Setting and cases: United States Department of Agriculture data sets were used to identify HPF foods that were (n = 105) and were not (n = 587) owned by US tobacco companies from 1988 to 2001. Measurements: A standardized definition from Fazzino et al. (2019) was used to identify HPF. HPF items were identified overall and by HPF group: fat and sodium HPF, fat and sugar HPF and carbohydrates and sodium HPF. Findings: Tobacco-owned foods were 29% more likely to be classified as fat and sodium HPF and 80% more likely to be classified as carbohydrate and sodium HPF than foods that were not tobacco-owned between 1988 and 2001 (P-values = 0.005-0.009). The availability of fat and sodium HPF (> 57%) and carbohydrate and sodium HPF (> 17%) was high in 2018 regardless of prior tobacco-ownership status, suggesting widespread saturation into the food system. Conclusions: Tobacco companies appear to have selectively disseminated hyper-palatable foods into the US food system between 1988 and 2001.
... A logical first step is that the behaviors that are recommended for change should be reinforcing so that people are motivated to engage in those behaviors. Dietary change can be challenging as food is a powerful primary reinforcer, and many high energydense (HED) foods that are related to obesity are highly reinforcing [179,180]. Food reinforcement is both cross-sectionally [181,182] and prospectively positively related to obesity [183,184]. Food reinforcement is also positively related to energy intake using a variety of approaches to measure dietary intake [185][186][187]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background There are large individual differences in weight loss and maintenance. Metabolic testing can provide phenotypical information that can be used to personalize treatment so that people remain in negative energy balance during weight loss and remain in energy balance during maintenance. Behavioral testing can assess the reinforcing value and change in the temporal window related to the personalized diet and exercise program to motivate people to maintain engagement in healthier eating and activity programs. Objective Provide an expository overview of how metabolic testing can be used to personalize weight control. Ideas about incorporating behavioral economic concepts are also included. Methods A broad overview of how resting metabolic rate, thermic effect of food and respiratory quotient can be used to improve weight control. Also discussed are behavioral economic principles that can maximize adherence to diet and activity protocols. Results Research suggests that measuring metabolic rate can be used to set calorie goals for weight loss and maintenance, thermic effect of food to increase energy expenditure, and respiratory quotient to guide macronutrient composition of the diet and maximize fat loss. Developing programs that foster a strong motivation to eat healthier and be active can maximize treatment success. Conclusion Incorporating metabolic measures can personalize behavioral weight loss programs, and the use of behavioral economic principles can increase the probability of adherence and long‐term success in weight control.
... An often-discussed example is hyperpalatable food (Li et al., 2018;. This is processed food that has been engineered to be extremely palatable and satisfying (Fazzino, 2022). Such food could be seen as efficiently packaging tasty calories with impressive storage capabilities. ...
... An often-discussed example is hyperpalatable food (Li et al., 2018;. This is processed food that has been engineered to be extremely palatable and satisfying (Fazzino, 2022). Such food could be seen as efficiently packaging tasty calories with impressive storage capabilities. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Technological inventions create environments altered from the environments of human evolution. I examine humans' ability to adapt to evolutionary mismatches by scrutinizing historical and current cases of technological disruptions. Using a historical case method, how technology altered parent-child relationships is analyzed in the very different cases of the Agricultural and Industrial Revolutions. Less harm occurred in the former because caregivers rather than strangers supervised children. Parents were able to situate the farm labor as training for adult subsistence, a parenting goal that aligns with evolved human nature. I examine technologies that were not harmful, such as contraception. Contraceptive technology is aligned with human nature, specifically, the desire to invest in parenting when resources are sufficient. But contraception ushered in altered gender roles, returning power to women reminiscent of what they had before resource hoarding was made possible with agriculture. This alteration widely destabilized Western societies, leading to the so-called 'culture wars' which are especially acute in the U.S. The analyzed historical cases allow the construction of a taxonomy of types of harm caused by technological mismatch. Themes included mismatch causing interpersonal vs. societal disruptions, and technologies sometimes rectifying evolutionary mismatches from an earlier historical period. Drawing on these insights, I argue that robots might not be a mismatch but could aid humans by ameliorating some of the stresses of modernity.
... There is an overwhelming agreement in the scientific literature and global health guidelines that physical exercise has acute and chronic benefits for diverse aspects of health 1-6 . While some people choose to exercise over other social and recreational activities 7 , many people underexercise and overeat highly palatable food (HPF) that is widely available in many societies [8][9][10] . This is considered a global health problem 1, 11,12 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the well-known health benefits of physical activity, many people underexercise; what drives the prioritization of exercise over alternative options is unclear. We developed a task that enabled us to study how mice freely and rapidly alternate between wheel running and other voluntary activities, such as eating palatable food. When multiple alternatives were available, mice chose to spend a substantial amount of time wheel running without any extrinsic reward and maintained this behavior even when palatable food was added as an option. Causal manipulations and correlative analyses of appetitive and consummatory processes revealed this preference for wheel running to be instantiated by hypothalamic hypocretin/orexin neurons (HONs). The effect of HON manipulations on wheel running and eating was strongly context-dependent, being the largest in the scenario where both options were available. Overall, these data suggest that HON activity enables an eat–run arbitration that results in choosing exercise over food.
... Hyper-palatable foods (HPF) contain combinations of palatability-inducing nutrients (fat, sugar, sodium, and/or carbohydrates) at unnatural thresholds (28) . As a result, HPF provide a highly rewarding eating experience and delay physiological satiety responses (29) , explaining why HPF intake has been associated with overeating and obesity-related outcomes (30,31) . Foods that are not hyper-palatable typically contain one main palatability inducing nutrient (e.g., sugar) combined with satiety promoting ingredients (water, fibre), such as fresh, whole fruits and vegetables (28) . ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Food taxation can improve diets by making unhealthy foods more expensive and by making healthy foods cheaper. In the Netherlands, a political window of opportunity arose in December 2021 to reduce the Value Added Tax on fruits and vegetables to zero percent. The policy is now facing institutional friction along several fronts, however, delaying and potentially averting its implementation. We analysed this institutional friction to inform future food tax policies. Design We qualitatively analysed open-access fiscal and health experts’ position papers about benefits and downsides of the zero-rate that were discussed with members of parliament in June 2023. Setting The Netherlands. Participants Not applicable. Results Health and fiscal experts expressed noticeably different viewpoints towards the utility of the zero-rate. One important argument fiscal experts based their negative advice upon, pertained the legal restrictions for distinguishing between healthier and unhealthier forms of fruits and vegetables (i.e., the principle of neutrality). A zero-rate Value Added Tax on unhealthier forms of fruits and vegetables, e.g., processed cucumber, mixed with salt and sugar, would be undesirable, but differentiating between raw and processed cucumber would offend the neutrality principle. Conclusions The Dutch attempt to give fruits and vegetables a tax break highlights the need for crystal-clear food classifications when designing food tax policies. Public health nutritionists should combine classifications based on caloric density, palatability, degree of processing, and nutrient content to provide a database for evidence-informed tax differentiation according to food item healthfulness.
... Recent national survey estimates have indicated that typical dietary energy density among the US population is >1.8, suggesting that typical diets may be composed of many HED foods [18]. Each of the three definitions have been linked to patterns of overeating and heightened risk for obesity [5,9,[19][20][21], implicating their prominence in the American diet. However, it remains largely unclear the degree to which the definitions, although distinct, may identify common foods (e.g., pizza) and the degree to which they may identify unique foods that may promote obesity risk. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Ultraprocessed foods (UPF), hyper‐palatable foods (HPF), and high energy density (HED) foods may contribute to obesity risk. All have distinct definitions; however, it is unknown the degree to which they may identify overlapping or distinct foods. This study examined the availability of UPF, HPF, and HED foods in the US food system from 1988 to 2018 and the degree of distinction across definitions. Methods Four data sets representing the US food system (1988, 2001, 2006, 2018) from the US Department of Agriculture were analyzed. UPF were identified based on the extent of industrialized processing. HPF were identified using the standardized definition that specifies palatability‐inducing nutrient combinations. HED was characterized as >2.0 kcal/g. Results Across years, 58% to 65% of foods were classified as UPF, 55% to 69% as HPF, and 37% to 47% as HED. Prevalence of UPF, HPF, and HED foods was higher in 2018 versus 1988 ( p values < 0.001); HPF evidenced the largest increase (14%) and UPF evidenced the smallest (4%) over time. There was moderate to high overlap in foods (40%–70%) across definitions. Conclusions Together, UPF, HPF, and HED foods comprise most foods in the US food supply. Changes in availability varied across definitions, with substantial increases in HPF and HED and relative stability of UPF.
... Not all foods are equally addictive. After concluding and analyzing the chemical characteristics of foods that may cause addictive behavior, Fazzino (2022) found that the word "hyperpalatable foods" (HPFs) can be used to summarize. HPFs are foods that have significant deviations from their natural form that are designed to maximize the beneficial and rewarding properties of the food during consumption. ...
Article
Full-text available
The impact of sugary foods on public health has contributed to the development of low‐sugar and sugar‐substituted products, and sugar reduction has become a major challenge for the food industry. There is growing empirical evidence that odor can enhance the perception of sweetness without increasing the caloric load. This current review summarizes the researches on odor‐induced sweetness enhancement published in recent years and discusses the mechanisms and influencing factors of odor‐sweetness interactions. In addition, by combing existing studies, this paper also summarizes the research methods and strategies to investigate odor‐induced sweetness enhancement. Finally, the feasibility of synergistic enhancement of sweetness through the superposition of odor with other senses (texture, visual, etc.) is also discussed and analyzed. In conclusion, odor‐induced sweetness enhancement may present an alternative or complementary approach for developing foods with less sugar.
... Today, the penalties for driving while intoxicated are far stiffer than they were in the 1970s and 80s, but there is much more attention to sophisticated treatment plans today, interventions that didn't exist back then. Given the increasing research on the addictive properties of ultra-processed foods [107], and the comforting allure of hyperpalatable but unhealthy foods [108], we can imagine preventive and treatment interventions. Years ago, probation officer Barbara Reed said that trying to correct a person's behavior in the absence of essential nutrients is like trying to ride a bicycle without filling the tires with air, it won't work. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the ongoing series of interviews, Challenges Advisory Board member and Nova Institute for Health Fellow Alan C. Logan meets with thought leaders, scientists, scholars, healthcare professionals, artisans, and visionaries concerned about health at the scales of persons, places, and the planet. Here, Dr Stephen J. Schoenthaler of California State University, Stanislaus, responds to a set of questions posed by Challenges. For over forty years, Dr. Schoenthaler has been at the forefront of the research connecting nutrition to behavior and mental health. In particular, Dr. Schoenthaler’s work has examined relationships between dietary patterns, nutritional support, and behaviors that might otherwise be associated with criminality and aggression. Although the idea that nutrition is a factor in juvenile delinquency was popularized in the 1950s, the area received little scientific attention. In the 1970s, the idea that nutrition could influence behavior gained national attention in the US but was largely dismissed as “fringe”, especially by those connected to the ultra-processed food industries. Today, relationships between diet and behavior are part of the robust field called “nutritional psychiatry”; emerging studies demonstrate clear societal implications, including those within the criminal justice system. Here, Dr. Schoenthaler discusses how we got here and updates Challenges on where the field has moved, with an eye toward future possibilities. Dr. Schoenthaler reflects on the early influences that shaped his interest in the field and discusses the ways in which this research, especially in the context of criminal justice, is related to the many interconnected challenges of our time.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Choice impulsivity may influence eating behavior. The study tested whether choice impulsivity, termed delay discounting, may be related to food generally, or may be specific to hyper-palatable foods (HPF). The study also determined whether a discounting task with choices between money and food may have utility in predicting obesity-related outcomes. Method: Participants (N = 284) completed a task that assessed choices between smaller reward available immediately and larger reward available later. Single commodity conditions presented choices between amounts of HPF, non-HPF, or money (e.g., HPF now vs. HPF later). Cross-commodity conditions presented choices between money and food commodities (e.g., money now vs. HPF later; money now vs. non-HPF later). Results: There were no significant differences in discounting of HPF and non-HPF in single commodity conditions (Mean ln[k] difference = .40, p = .058). In the cross-commodity conditions holding money constant as the immediate reward, individuals discounted HPF significantly less than non-HPF (Mean ln[k] difference = .92; p = .0001). In regression analyses, individuals with excess HPF intake, greater HPF craving, and higher BMIs were more likely to choose HPF immediately, when money was the delayed reward (p values = .003 to .008). Conclusions: Choice impulsivity may be specific to foods that are hyper-palatable. Results suggest that individuals with excess HPF intake, higher HPF craving, and higher BMIs may exhibit a general tendency toward impulsive decision making that may be exacerbated in an obesogenic environment that provides wide access to HPF. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Background/Objectives Characterizing behavioral phenotypes that predict increased zBMI gain during adolescence could identify novel intervention targets and prevent the development of obesity. The purpose of this study was to determine if sensitization of the relative reinforcing value (RRV) of high (HED) or low energy density (LED) foods predicts adolescent weight gain trajectories. A secondary aim was to test the hypothesis that relationships between sensitization of the RRV of food and weight change are moderated by delay discounting (DD). Subjects/Methods We conducted a prospective, longitudinal cohort study in 201 boys and girls with an average zBMI of 0.4, who began the study between the ages of 12 and 14 years and completed the study 2 years later. Participants completed five laboratory visits where the RRV of HED and LED, and DD were assessed at a baseline (visits 1, 2, and 4) and then RRV was measured again after participants consumed a portion of the same HED and LED food for 2 weeks (visits 3 and 5; order counterbalanced). Increases (>1) in the RRV from baseline to post-daily intake were categorized as “sensitization” and decreases (≤1) were categorized as “satiation.” Participants returned to the laboratory for follow-up visits at 6, 15, and 24 months to have height and weight taken and to complete additional assessments. Results Sensitization to HED food was associated with a greater zBMI change over time ( β = 0.0070; p = 0.035). There was no impact of sensitization to LED food or interaction between sensitization to HED and LED food on zBMI change and no moderation of DD on the relationship between HED sensitization and zBMI change (all p > 0.05). Conclusion Our prior work showed that sensitization to HED food is cross-sectionally associated with greater zBMI. This study extends this work by demonstrating that sensitization to HED food prospectively predicts increased zBMI gain over time in adolescents without obesity. Future studies should determine if sensitization can be modified or reduced through behavioral intervention. Trial registration Clinicaltrials.gov: NCT04027608.
Article
Full-text available
Background Foods that increase obesity risk are ubiquitous in the US food environment. Such foods may be the target of hedonic eating, which may facilitate weight gain and lead to obesity. The study tested whether meal composition during an ad libitum buffet meal was associated with 1-year weight and percent body fat changes among healthy younger adults without obesity. Hyper-palatable foods (HPF) were the study focus; comparisons were conducted with high energy dense (HED) and ultra-processed foods (UPF). Design Younger adults without obesity (N = 82; 43% male; mean age 26.8) completed an ad libitum buffet meal and provided body composition measurements at baseline and 1-year follow up. Multiple regression models tested associations between the proportion of the target food consumed (HPF, HED, or UPF) during the ad libitum meal and 1) weight change and 2) percent body fat change. The proportion of HPF was characterized by HPF group, specifically carbohydrate and sodium (CSOD) foods or fat and sodium (FSOD) foods. Results Participants who consumed a greater proportion of CSOD HPF in their ad libitum buffet meals had significantly greater weight change (b = 0.354, p = .003) and percent body fat change (b = 0.247, p = .036) at 1-year follow up. In contrast, no significant associations were found between the proportion of FSOD HPF, HED, or UPF consumed and anthropometric outcomes (p values = .099-.938). Conclusions Eating a greater proportion of hyper-palatable CSOD foods ad libitum appears to be a pattern of hedonic eating, which may increase an individual's risk for weight and body fat gain in early adulthood.
Article
Full-text available
The relative reinforcing value (RRV) of food measures how hard someone will work for a high-energy-dense (HED) food when an alternative reward is concurrently available. Higher RRV for HED food has been linked to obesity, yet this association has not been examined in low-income preschool-age children. Further, the development of individual differences in the RRV of food in early childhood is poorly understood. This cross-sectional study tested the hypothesis that the RRV of HED (cookies) to low-energy-dense (LED; fruit) food would be greater in children with obesity compared to children without obesity in a sample of 130 low-income 3- to 5-year-olds enrolled in Head Start classrooms in Central Pennsylvania. In addition, we examined individual differences in the RRV of food by child characteristics (i.e., age, sex, and reward sensitivity) and food security status. The RRV of food was measured on concurrent progressive-ratio schedules of reinforcement. RRV outcomes included the last schedule reached (breakpoint) for cookies (cookie Pmax) and fruit (fruit Pmax), the breakpoint for cookies in proportion to the total breakpoint for cookies and fruit combined (RRV cookie), and response rates (responses per minute). Parents completed the 18-item food security module to assess household food security status and the Behavioral Activation System scale to assess reward sensitivity. Pearson’s correlations and mixed models assessed associations between continuous and discrete child characteristics with RRV outcomes, respectively. Two-way mixed effects interaction models examined age and sex as moderators of the association between RRV and Body Mass Index z-scores (BMIZ). Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05. Children with obesity (17%) had a greater cookie Pmax [F (1, 121) = 4.95, p = 0.03], higher RRV cookie [F (1, 121) = 4.28, p = 0.04], and responded at a faster rate for cookies [F (1, 121) = 17.27, p < 0.001] compared to children without obesity. Children with higher cookie response rates had higher BMIZ (r = 0.26, p < 0.01); and RRV cookie was positively associated with BMIZ for older children (5-year-olds: t = 2.40, p = 0.02) and boys (t = 2.55, p = 0.01), but not younger children or girls. The RRV of food did not differ by household food security status. Low-income children with obesity showed greater motivation to work for cookies than fruit compared to their peers without obesity. The RRV of HED food may be an important contributor to increased weight status in boys and future research is needed to better understand developmental trajectories of the RRV of food across childhood.
Article
Full-text available
Obesity is a major health risk, with junk food consumption playing a central role in weight gain, because of its high palatability and high-energy nutrients. The Cafeteria (CAF) diet model for animal experiments consists of the same tasty but unhealthy food products that people eat (e.g. hot dogs and muffins), and considers variety, novelty and secondary food features, such as smell and texture. This model, therefore, mimics human eating patterns better than other models. In this paper, we systematically review studies that have used a CAF diet in behavioral experiments and propose a standardized CAF diet protocol. The proposed diet is ad libitum and voluntary; combines different textures, nutrients and tastes, including salty and sweet products; and it is rotated and varied. Our summary of the behavioral effects of CAF diet show that it alters meal patterns, reduces the hedonic value of other rewards, and tends to reduce stress and spatial memory. So far, no clear effects of CAF diet were found on locomotor activity, impulsivity, coping and social behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Palatable food can trigger appetitive responses, such as salivation and approach tendencies. Though evolutionarily functional, these conditioned responses can encourage overeating and obesity when food is abundant. The current study examines the neural correlates of ‘denovo’ Pavlovian appetitive conditioning, pairing one class of unknown objects (conditioned stimuli, CS) with their sweet taste (unconditioned stimulus, US) during a single trial. To do so, 23 participants consumed unknown (marzipan) objects of one particular color (CS+) while only interacting with control stimuli of different color and shape (CS-). After this single-trial conditioning procedure, participants viewed and rated images of the marzipan figures and the control objects during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Relative to the CS-, the CS+ elicited stronger activation in the dorsal striatum, a brain region associated with cue-reward coupling. Furthermore, conditioning effects in subjective ‘craving’, defined as increased palatability and desire to eat, were observed, and these were positively related to conditioning effects in the amygdala, a brain region associated with the need-dependent value of a reward. Thus, the study identified reward-related brain regions involved in single-trial appetitive learning, thereby providing a potential mechanism that contributes to the etiology of food craving. These findings might help to understand clinically relevant food cravings in individuals with eating or weight related concerns and might support the development of extinction based treatments. •Pavlovian appetitive conditioning can occur after a single conditioning session •A single trial of appetitive conditioning was sufficient to increase ‘craving’ •A general conditioning effect was found in the dorsal striatum •The conditioning effect on ‘craving’ was related to amygdala activation •These conditioning effects may contribute to the etiology of food craving
Article
Full-text available
Objectives. To investigate the transfer of marketing knowledge and infrastructure for targeting racial/ethnic minorities from the tobacco to the food and beverage industry in the United States. Methods. We analyzed internal industry documents between April 2018 and April 2019 from the University of California San Francisco Truth Tobacco Industry Documents Library, triangulated with other sources. Results. In the 1980s, Philip Morris Companies purchased General Foods and Kraft Foods and created Kraft General Foods. Through centralized marketing initiatives, Philip Morris Companies directly transferred expertise, personnel, and resources from its tobacco to its food subsidiaries, creating a racial/ethnic minority–targeted food and beverage marketing program modeled on its successful cigarette program. When Philip Morris Companies sold Kraft General Foods in 2007, Kraft General Foods had a “fully integrated” minority marketing program that combined target marketing with racial/ethnic events promotion, racial/ethnic media outreach, and corporate donations to racial/ethnic leadership groups, making it a food industry leader. Conclusions. The tobacco industry directly transferred racial/ethnic minority marketing knowledge and infrastructure to food and beverage companies. Given the substantial growth of food and beverage corporations, their targeting of vulnerable populations, and obesity-related disparities, public policy and community action is needed to address corporate target marketing.
Article
Objective: The study aim was to elucidate the degree to which hyper-palatable foods (HPF) are consumed during binge episodes compared to restricting episodes, and to test the association between HPF intake during each episode and respective episode frequency. Method: This study was a secondary analysis of data from a larger study on eating disorders. The present sample included adults (N = 147, 83% women) diagnosed with sub-threshold (41%) or full-threshold (59%) bulimia nervosa (BN). Foods consumed during binge and restricting episodes were assessed using the Eating Pathology Symptoms Inventory-Clinician Rated Version. A standardized definition of HPF developed previously was applied to foods consumed during binge and restricting episodes. A Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test was used to test the difference between total caloric intake from HPF (KcalHPF) and percentage of caloric intake from HPF (PercHPF) during binge episodes relative to restricting episodes. Four linear regression models tested HPF intake (KcalHPF and PercHPF) during both episode types (binge and restricting) as predictors of respective episode frequency. Results: There was a significant difference between median KcalHPF (1846.6 vs. 279.6; Z = -13.38, p < .001) and PercHPF during binge compared to restricting episodes (95% vs. 61%; Z = -7.35, p < .001). Regression analyses demonstrated that KcalHPF during binge episodes was significantly associated with binge episode frequency (B = 0.002; p < .001), but not PercHPF (p = .287). Discussion: Results suggest that HPF may be primarily consumed during binge episodes among individuals with BN, and may be associated with greater binge-eating frequency. Public significance: Findings from the current study support an underlying assumption of theoretical models of binge eating, suggesting that highly rewarding, hyper-palatable foods (HPF), may constitute the vast majority of energy intake during binge-eating episodes. Additionally, a substantial amount of energy intake from HPF may occur during restricting episodes among people with bulimia nervosa. Greater HPF intake during binge eating may also be associated with binge-eating severity.
Chapter
Compulsive eating is prevalent in binge-type eating disorders, present in some obese individuals, and often conceptualized in relation to the recently operationalized construct of food addiction. Compulsive eating putatively involves escalated intake of highly preferred foods, finickiness toward otherwise acceptable alternatives, increased effort and time spent to obtain preferred foods, eating behavior despite incorrect or adverse outcomes, and eating of palatable food in order to soothe abstinence- and stress-induced negative emotional states. We review theoretical and empirical bases for an opponent-process affective dysregulation model of compulsive eating, adapted from the addiction field, whereby intermittent, extended access to palatable food progressively dampens reward circuitry and potentiates activation of stress circuitry. We then detail corresponding protocols for two rodent models of intermittent, extended access to palatable food, describe methods for assessing compulsive-like outcomes, and discuss possible adaptations that can be used to understand better the prevention, biology, and treatment of compulsive eating.
Article
It is becoming clearer how neurobiological mechanisms generate ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ components of food reward. Mesocorticolimbic mechanisms that enhance ‘liking’ include brain hedonic hotspots, which are specialized subregions that are uniquely able to causally amplify the hedonic impact of palatable tastes. Hedonic hotspots are found in nucleus accumbens medial shell, ventral pallidum, orbitofrontal cortex, insula cortex, and brainstem. In turn, a much larger mesocorticolimbic circuitry generates ‘wanting’ or incentive motivation to obtain and consume food rewards. Hedonic and motivational circuitry interact together and with hypothalamic homeostatic circuitry, allowing relevant physiological hunger and satiety states to modulate ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ for food rewards. In some conditions such as drug addiction, ‘wanting’ is known to dramatically detach from ‘liking’ for the same reward, and this may also occur in over-eating disorders. Via incentive sensitization, ‘wanting’ selectively becomes higher, especially when triggered by reward cues when encountered in vulnerable states of stress, etc. Emerging evidence suggests that some cases of obesity and binge eating disorders may reflect an incentive-sensitization brain signature of cue hyper-reactivity, causing excessive ‘wanting’ to eat. Future findings on the neurobiological bases of ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ can continue to improve understanding of both normal food reward and causes of clinical eating disorders.