Content uploaded by Kay Brauer
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Kay Brauer on Jun 16, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Content uploaded by Kay Brauer
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Kay Brauer on May 30, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
Available online 29 May 2022
0092-6566/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Prole similarities among romantic partners’ character strengths and their
associations with relationship- and life satisfaction
Kay Brauer
a
,
*
, Rebekka Sendatzki
a
, Fabian Gander
b
, Willibald Ruch
c
, Ren´
e T. Proyer
a
a
Martin Luther University Halle-Wittenberg, Germany
b
University of Basel, Switzerland
c
University of Zurich, Switzerland
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Actor-Partner Interdependence Model
Character strengths
Partner similarity
Prole similarity
Romantic relationships
Relationship satisfaction
Values in action
ABSTRACT
We studied the similarity among partners’ character strengths (i.e., positively valued traits) across two studies. In
Study 1, N =68 couples completed the 240-item VIA Inventory of Strengths and in Study 2, N =143 couples
completed a 24-item brief-form and measures of life- and relationship satisfaction. We computed raw, normative,
and distinctive prole similarities for the 24 strengths and found support for partners’ similarity in both studies
(normative: rs ≥0.84; raw: rs ≥0.23; distinctive: rs ≥0.06). Actor-Partner Interdependence Model analyses
(Study 2) provided no evidence for the notion that similarity relates to couples’ satisfaction. We discuss our
ndings regarding prior research, assortative mating preferences, and extensions to the study of partner- and
ideal partner perceptions.
1. Introduction
Romantic relationships are positive institutions that contribute to
well-being and positive experiences (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi,
2014) and most adults strive to establish and maintain close relation-
ships (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Human mating is far from random
and partner similarity relates to the formation and longevity of re-
lationships (e.g., Buss, 1994, 2016). Common wisdom (e.g., “birds of a
feather ock together”) aligns with the similarity attraction hypothesis,
which assumes that people are attracted to others who are similar to
themselves in a variety of variables, including personality traits (e.g.,
Buss, 1994; Byrne, 1971, 1997). Empirical research has provided robust
support for the notion that similarity attracts and that partners are on
average similar at the on-set as well as later phases of relationships (for
an overview, see Luo, 2017; Montoya et al., 2008). While many studies
have examined partner similarity for broad personality traits such as the
big ve and HEXACO traits, research on character strengths in close
relationships has received comparatively little attention thus far, which
is surprising given that several character strengths are among the traits
most sought after in potential partners (e.g., Buss & Barnes, 1986).
Particularly, we are not aware of research on partner similarity in
character strengths. This is surprising as the core criterion for the
denition of character strengths (“A strength contributes to various
fulllments that comprise the good life, for the self and for others.”)
implies that character strengths may be benecial to the partner
(Peterson & Seligman, 2004, p. 17). We aimed to narrow this gap in the
literature by studying the similarity in romantic partners’ proles of
character strengths across two studies. In addition, we used Actor-
Partner Interdependence Model (APIM; Cook & Kenny, 2005) analyses
to study whether prole similarities are associated with relationship
satisfaction (RS) and life satisfaction (LS).
1.1. Character strengths
Character strengths are morally positively valued personality traits
and Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) Values-in-Action (VIA) classica-
tion of strengths and virtues covers 24 character strengths. These are
assigned to six broader virtues (i.e., wisdom, courage, humanity, justice,
temperance, and transcendence). Peterson and Seligman selected the
strengths based on ten criteria (e.g., they must be fullling, morally
valued, or not diminishing others when displayed, for an evaluation of
these criteria, see Ruch & Stahlmann, 2019). The strengths relate
differentially to outcomes such as well-being and LS (Baumann et al.,
2020; Buschor et al., 2013), posttraumatic growth (Peterson et al., 2006)
* Corresponding author at: Emil-Abderhalden-Str. 26-27, 06099 Halle, Germany.
E-mail addresses: kay.brauer@psych.uni-halle.de (K. Brauer), rebekka.sendatzki@psych.uni-halle.de (R. Sendatzki), mail@fabiangander.ch (F. Gander), w.ruch@
psychologie.uzh.ch (W. Ruch), rene.proyer@psych.uni-halle.de (R.T. Proyer).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Research in Personality
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2022.104248
Received 21 December 2021; Received in revised form 21 May 2022; Accepted 25 May 2022
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
2
and growth after collective life events such as the COVID-19 pandemic
(Gander & Wagner, 2021), health behaviors (Proyer et al., 2013), and
work-related outcomes such as job performance (Harzer et al., 2021), to
name but a few.
Seligman’s (2012) PERMA (R indicating “Relationships”) model of
ourishing posits that (positive) relationships are an important foun-
dation for well-being. Empirical ndings support this notion. For
example, studies showed robust associations between self-reports of
engaging in positive relationships and LS and ourishing (Gander et al.,
2017), and daily exercises to increase attention to relationships in day-
to-day life improved well-being and alleviated depressiveness in a
placebo-controlled randomized control study (Gander et al., 2016).
Moreover, Seligman proposes that the use and enactment of strengths
contribute to ourishing in the domain of relationships; particularly for
those strengths that play a role in social settings and might affect how
oneself but also the partner experiences the relationship (e.g., self-
regulation, kindness, and gratitude). In line with the personality-
relationship transaction model (Mund et al., 2016; Neyer & Asen-
dorpf, 2001), which posits that personality traits affect how people
interact with their social environment and considering that character
strengths are dened as fullling, morally valued, and do not diminish
others (Peterson & Seligman, 2004), we argue that character strengths
contribute to the formation and experience of relationships and how
people interact with their partner. Initial ndings support this notion, as,
for example, character strengths predicted being accepted by peers and
experiencing friendship quality even at an early age (Wagner, 2019).
However, the role of the VIA strengths is comparatively understudied in
romantic life.
1.2. Character strengths in romantic life
Although theoretical and empirical research has highlighted the role
of character strengths for, particularly close, relationships (e.g., Peter-
son & Seligman, 2004; Ruch et al., 2018; Seligman, 2012; Wagner,
2019), to the best of our knowledge, only two studies have thus far
examined the VIA character strengths in romantic relationships. Using a
sample of 177 opposite-sex adult couples, Boiman-Meshita and Littman-
Ovadia (2022) tested the associations between the higher-order strength
factors caring (including the character strengths of gratitude, kindness,
love, teamwork, social intelligence, and leadership), self-control (pru-
dence, perseverance, self-regulation, honesty, and humility), and
inquisitiveness (curiosity, creativity, zest, bravery, love of learning, and
hope) and marital satisfaction, intimacy, and burnout. APIM analyses
showed positive actor effects of the strength factors for all study vari-
ables and positive associations with their partner’s RS (i.e., partner ef-
fects). Supplementary analyses showed positive actor effects for the
single strengths (except for appreciation of beauty, gratitude, and humor
in men; prudence in women). Further, they found positive partner ef-
fects of creativity, curiosity, fairness, forgiveness (in women), gratitude
(in women), honesty, hope, humility, judgment, prudence, self-
regulation, social intelligence, teamwork, and zest on RS. Hence, char-
acter strengths relate to indicators of RS in actors and in some cases their
partner’s, which is in accordance with Peterson and Seligman’s (2004)
notion of character strengths being benecial for oneself and the part-
ner. In addition, they found robust similarity among partners’ higher-
order strength factors, with coefcients between 0.21 and 0.27. How-
ever, similarity ndings for single strengths were not reported and as-
sociations between partners’ similarity in strengths and RS were not
tested.
Weber and Ruch (2012) examined the partner similarity of 87
adolescent couples (mean age: 16.5 years) with analyses of self- and
ideal partner ratings of the 24 character strengths assessed with an age-
appropriate measure, the German version of the VIA-Youth (Park &
Peterson, 2006; Ruch et al., 2014). They found overall positive partner
similarity correlations (≥0.20) for creativity, bravery, honesty, zest,
fairness, perseverance, teamwork, fairness, beauty, gratitude, hope, and
spirituality. Moreover, Weber and Ruch tested whether similarity
(measured as absolute differences between partners’ scores in strengths,
i.e., greater differences indicate higher dissimilarity) relates to LS.
Multiple regression analyses, including the strengths of actors (Step 1),
partners (Step 2), and the couples’ dissimilarity scores (Step 3), showed
mixed ndings: For boys, similarity in perseverance, zest, forgiveness,
and humor was associated with their LS, whereas similarity in honesty
and teamwork was associated with girls’ LS. Further, they found
consensus in partners’ desires for an ideal partner’s expressions in the
strengths when computing the rank-order correlation (r =0.89). Weber
and Ruch’s study suggests that adolescents show assortative mating
preferences and partner similarity concerning the VIA strengths.
Taken together, the ndings of these studies support the notion that
individual differences in character strengths relate to how people
experience their relationship and provide preliminary support for the
notion that partners are similar in their strengths. However, an analysis
of the similarity in the full proles of the strengths and an examination of
their associations with relationship outcomes in a well-powered adult
sample is thus far missing.
1.3. Partner similarity in romantic couples
The role of partner similarity has received major interest in research
on romantic relationships. Studies testing the degree of partner simi-
larity showed systematic similarity across a wide range of variables, and
similarity has been argued to play a role for initial romantic attraction,
satisfaction with and the longevity of relationships, and the heritability
of traits (e.g., Buss, 1994, 2016; Byrne, 1971, 1997; Luo, 2017; Watson
et al., 2004, 2014; Weidmann et al., 2016). Hence, the study of similarity
concerns two distinct questions; namely, the description (i.e., “how
similar are partners?”) and the effects of similarity (i.e., “does similarity
relate to outcomes such as romantic interest, relationship satisfaction, or
longevity of a relationship?”).
Description of similarity. The description of similarity concerns
quantifying the degree of similarity among partners. Luo (2017) sug-
gests distinguishing between assortative mating, partner similarity, and
potential mechanisms for why partners match non-randomly: “Whereas
A[ssortative]M[ating] specically refers to initial partner similarity
evident at the beginning of the relationship that reects active or passive
assortment, (couple) similarity refers to partner similarity at any point of
the relationship, which may result from initial assortment and/or
development within the relationship” (p. 1). For both assortative mating
and partner similarity, there is robust evidence for the systematic sim-
ilarity of partners concerning various characteristics, including psy-
chologically relevant variables such as attitudes, values, and personality
traits (see e.g., Buss, 1994, 2016; Luo, 2017; Montoya et al., 2008;
Weidmann et al., 2016). Luo’s literature review shows that the magni-
tude of partner similarity correlation coefcients depends on what
variables are studied, with similarity correlation coefcients in the
range of 0.70 ≤r ≤0.90 for age, 0.40 ≤r ≤0.60 for education, 0.40 ≤r
≤0.70 for attitudes, 0.10 ≤r ≤0.40 for values, and consistently positive
but comparatively weaker similarities for personality traits (≤0.30). For
the latter, ndings apply to broad traits (e.g., big ve; Weidmann et al.,
2016) as well as narrow traits (e.g., adult playfulness, power, and dis-
positions toward ridicule and being laughed at; e.g., Brauer & Proyer,
2018; K¨
orner & Schütz, 2021; Proyer et al., 2019).
Effects of similarity. The consequences of partner similarity have been
discussed from genetic, social, and psychological perspectives (for
overviews see e.g., Buss, 2016; Luo, 2017). Here, we focus on the psy-
chological consequences of partner similarity in personality traits. It has
been argued that similarity provides partners with shared ways of
perceiving and dealing with their social environment. Thus, similar
behaviors and experiences are expected to decrease the likelihood of
conict among partners, thereby contributing to greater RS and, in the
long-term, a lower probability of dissolving the relationship (Rammstedt
et al., 2013). Studies testing the associations between similarity and
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
3
outcomes have provided mixed ndings. When controlling for main
effects of both partners’ personality traits, cross-sectional and longitu-
dinal studies showed positive associations between partner similarity in
broad and narrow personality traits and the outcomes of RS and LS, but
effect sizes are small to negligible (e.g., Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Brauer
et al., 2021; Chopik & Lucas, 2019; Decuyper et al., 2012; Dyrenforth
et al., 2010; Furler et al., 2013; Humbad et al., 2013; Proyer et al., 2019;
van Scheppingen et al., 2019; see also Luo, 2017; Weidmann et al.,
2016). Rammstedt and colleagues (2013) used an alternative approach
to study how similarity relates to break-up, as they compared the sim-
ilarity of stable and separated couples’ big ve traits (N
total
=4,809
couples) across two assessments over a 4-year interval. Couples who
dissolved their relationship at Wave 2 showed lower initial similarity
and became less similar over time (0.10 ≤Δr ≤0.24) than couples who
did not break up. These “surviving” couples were characterized by
higher initial similarity at Wave 1 and the absence of change in simi-
larity over time (Δr ≤0.01). Taking these ndings together, partners’
similarity in personality traits has typically shown small associations
with indicators of satisfaction beyond the existence of actor and partner
effects, but Rammstedt et al.’s ndings suggest that similarity might
relate to long-term consequences for relationships. Also, when studying
similarity at the on-set of relationships, meta-analytic ndings showed
that similarity predicts romantic interest and attraction to a potential
partner (Montoya et al., 2008).
Similarity in traits and proles. Finally, two major approaches to
assessing similarity are distinguished: trait wise approaches examine
partner similarity concerning a single trait and allow inferences about
similarity across persons in a single trait (i.e., variable-centered anal-
ysis). While this approach is informative at the ne-grained level, it only
captures a fraction of the full set of personality traits, in our case
strengths, that describe a person more comprehensively. To achieve a
more comprehensive assessment, one is interested in testing the simi-
larity among a set of variables, the prole of traits (i.e., person-centered;
in dyadic studies: couple-centered analyses). Testing partners’ similarity
among proles allows for the comprehensive and simultaneous analysis
of the full information that characterize the partners in a couple (Furr,
2008; Rogers et al., 2018). Technically, the difference between both
approaches is that the trait wise approach analyzes the similarity across
all couples concerning a single attribute, whereas the prole approach
analyzes a single couple concerning a set of attributes. The latter is done
for each couple and then averaged across all couples to inform about the
average prole similarity. We argue that testing proles is of particular
interest when studying partners’ similarity in the congurations of their
24 strengths.
1.4. The present study
The VIA strengths have been theoretically and empirically linked to
how people experience and maintain their relationships; for example, in
friendships and couples of different age groups (Boiman-Meshita &
Littman-Ovadia, 2022; Seligman, 2012; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi,
2014; Wagner, 2019; Weber & Ruch, 2012). We aimed to extend the
research on character strengths in romantic relationships by addressing
partner similarity across two independent studies. The aim of Study 1
was to test the magnitude of similarity in couples’ proles of their
character strengths using the full 240-item VIA-IS questionnaire. In
Study 2, we aimed to replicate the ndings of Study 1 using a brief form
of the strengths questionnaire and additionally to test how similarity is
associated with RS and LS in couples.
We expected that partners would share similar expressions of their
positively valued behaviors, thinking, and views as described in the 24
strengths of the VIA classication. Our rationale for this notion is based
on two assumptions. First, Boiman-Meshita and Littman-Ovadia’s
(2022) ndings indicate the existence of partner similarity in the range
reported in the literature on personality and values, with coefcients
between 0.20 and 0.30 (Luo, 2017). Also, Weber and Ruch’s (2012)
ndings from adolescents suggest that character strengths are desired in
potential partners and that adolescent couples are similar in their self-
reports. Further, Wagner (2019) showed that character strengths
(particularly kindness, fairness, humor, and honesty) are desired by
adolescents when they describe what they seek for in a potential best
friend and that similarity among friends’ strengths is related to higher
friendship quality. Hence, initial evidence suggests that close relation-
ships are characterized by dyadic similarity and a desire for those
character strengths in close others to be expressed similarly. Secondly,
research testing partner preferences independently from a theoretical
framework of personality trait classications found that traits and at-
tributes such as kindness, creativity, good sense of humor, and play-
fulness are among the most desired traits in potential partners (e.g., Buss
& Barnes, 1986; Chick et al., 2020; Watson et al., 2014). One might
argue that these attributes resemble Peterson and Seligman’s (2004)
character strengths to a certain degree, and we speculate that morally
positively valued traits could be sought for in partners in similar ways.
Moreover, people’s preferences are related to their self-reports but also
to their actual spouses’ self-reports, in line with the similarity attraction
hypothesis (e.g., Botwin et al., 1997; Buss & Barnes, 1986; Byrne, 1997;
Chick et al., 2020; Watson et al., 2014; Weber & Ruch, 2012).
For the description of the partner similarity, we focused on studying
the prole similarity in partners’ full sets of the 24 strengths. We used
Furr’s (2008) approach to prole analysis that allows differentiating
between the raw prole similarity and the distinctive prole similarity.
Distinctive prole similarity adjusts for stereotype effects (normative-
ness) and describes the degree of similarity in partners’ deviations from
the average person. Due to controlling for normativeness, coefcients
from distinctive prole analyses are typically lower than raw prole
correlation estimates, but they allow for a less biased assessment of
similarity (Rogers et al., 2018). Prior studies illustrated the importance
of removing stereotype effects. For example, Rogers et al. (2018) re-
ported a high raw prole similarity for the big ve traits (r =0.39) that
was reduced to 0.02 after removing normativeness. The same has been
found when studying proles of narrower traits such as dispositions
toward ridicule and being laughed at (r
raw
=0.51, r
distinctive
=0.27;
Brauer & Proyer, 2018) and facets of adult playfulness (r
raw
=0.55,
r
distinctive
=0.12; Proyer et al., 2019). Although distinctive prole sim-
ilarities of broad and narrow personality traits are typically numerically
small, they are consistently positive and robust in the sense that 95%
condence intervals exclude zero, thus suggesting the existence of
congural similarity among partners’ deviations from the average per-
son. Finally, the normative prole describes the prole of the average
person. In studies of opposite-sex couples, the normative prole simi-
larity is computed separately for men and women and informs about the
similarity between the stereotypical proles of men and women. Typi-
cally, normative proles are highly correlated (≈0.90; e.g., Rogers
et al., 2018). Taking prior ndings on couples’ prole similarities into
account, we expected to nd positive prole similarities, with numeri-
cally small distinctive prole similarity coefcients above chance (i.e.,
their condence intervals exceeding zero). In addition to the prole
analyses, we examined the trait wise similarity of the 24 strengths in an
exploratory fashion across both studies.
The second aim of Study 2 was to test whether partner similarity in
strengths robustly relates to RS and LS. The literature has shown that
prole similarity indices outperform single trait absolute differences
scores in terms of predictive validity (e.g., Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Furler
et al., 2013; for a discussion of the disadvantages of absolute difference
scores as estimates of similarity see also Edwards, 2001). Thus, we
examined whether couples’ similarity in the proles of strengths is
associated with RS and LS over and above actor and partner effects. We
used the APIM (Cook & Kenny, 2005) to account for the partners’
interdependence in predictor and outcome variables and to control for
actor- and partner effects of the strengths on RS and LS. In line with the
literature on associations between partner similarity and indicators of
satisfaction (e.g., Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Furler et al., 2013; Humbad
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
4
et al., 2013; Luo, 2017; Weidmann et al., 2016), we also expected pos-
itive associations of minor size between prole similarity and partners’
satisfaction.
Taken together, the data from both studies allowed us to examine the
questions of similarity in strengths among couples by using two assess-
ment approaches (i.e., brief- and full forms), to replicate the ndings
across samples and methods, and to examine the associations between
similarity and the outcomes of RS and LS in couples.
2. Study 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Sample
We collected data of 68 opposite-sex couples with men aged 23 to 76
years (M =49.0, SD =12.5) and women aged 21 to 84 years (M =46.9,
SD =12.5). Most couples were married (72.1%). Close to two thirds of
the sample held a degree from a university or a university of applied
sciences (61.8%), 23.6% nished high school qualifying them to attend
a university or a university of applied sciences, 12.5% completed
vocational training, 1.5% completed secondary education, and one
person did not nish secondary education. In this sample, we did not
assess information on cohabitation or relationship duration.
2.1.2. Instruments
The Values-in-Action Inventory of Strengths (VIA-IS; Peterson et al.,
2005; German version by Ruch et al., 2010) is the 240-item self-report
instrument for the assessment of the 24 character strengths from the
VIA classication. Each strength is assessed with 10 items. All items are
answered on a 5-point Likert-style scale ranging from 1 =“very much
unlike me” to 5 =“very much like me.” A sample item is “I nd the world
a very interesting place” (curiosity). Ruch et al. (2010) report good in-
ternal consistency (0.71 ≤
α
≤0.90) and retest-reliability (r
tt
≥0.65 for
6-month intervals and ≥0.62 for 9-month intervals) and provide evi-
dence for criterion validity.
2.1.3. Procedure
Data collection was carried out before the COVID-19 pandemic and
participants provided informed consent. The study was approved by the
local ethics committee. The data analyzed here are a subsample of 5,521
participants who took part in an online training program for character
strengths that was advertised in online forums, newspaper articles, and
mailing lists. Inclusion criteria were being ≥18 years of age, no con-
sumption of illegal drugs, and currently not being in psychotherapeutic
treatment. Participants were asked to provide the email address of their
partner if they also participated in the training program. Based on this
information, a total of 68 opposite-sex couples could be matched. Par-
ticipants received no nancial compensation, but individualized feed-
back on their character strengths at the end of the program. The data
reported here partially overlap with those reported in Wagner et al.
(2020), who investigated the relationship between character strengths
and PERMA but did not examine dyadic relationships.
2.1.4. Data analysis
We used two approaches to prole similarity. First, we correlated the
rank order of the strengths of the men and women. This allows to derive
an estimate of how the relative rankings of the strengths are associated
among men and women on average. Secondly, we used Furr’s (2008)
approach to examine the within-couple prole similarity (i.e., each
couple is denoted by a similarity coefcient). Furr suggests analyzing
three types of proles and, accordingly, three types of similarity. First,
normative proles are based on the men’s and women’s respective sample
means in the strengths, thus reecting the expressions of strengths for
the average man and average woman. The normative prole similarity
describes how the average man and the average woman converge in
their strengths. Next, we computed raw prole similarities for each couple
by correlating the couples’ man’s and woman’s respective responses to
the 240 items of the VIA-IS, which informs about the overlap of partners’
raw proles. Thirdly, we computed distinctive proles by mean-centering
the women’s and men’s scores on their respective sample means. Thus,
the distinctive proles describe deviations from the average man’s and
average woman’s prole and the distinctive prole similarity informs
about partners’ similarity in deviations from the stereotypical prole.
This procedure adjusts the proles for normativeness and controls for
stereotype effects (Furr, 2008; Rogers et al., 2018). After computing the
raw and distinctive prole similarity correlations for each couple, we
computed the mean of the similarity coefcients across all couples on
basis of Fisher r-to-z transformed values. The mean value was trans-
formed back by z-to-r transformation and informs about the average
similarity in the sample (i.e., across all couples). We examined the sta-
tistical signicance of the prole similarities with one-sample t-tests
(test value =0; two-tailed). As a supplementary analysis, we examined
the trait wise similarities in single strengths by computing bivariate
correlations between the men’s and women’s VIA-IS scale scores. For all
correlation analyses, we computed bootstrapped (k =5,000 samples)
95% condence intervals (CI) and assumed statistical signicance of a
coefcient when the CI excludes zero and when p <.05 (two-tailed).
Sample size rationale. We determined the sample size appropriate for
our research based on the rationale of our main analyses; namely, testing
the prole similarity in couple-centered analyses. For each couple, we
computed the prole correlation on basis of each partner’s responses to
the N =240 items of the VIA-IS. Thus, our analyses rely on the number of
items that constitute a prole (Furr, 2008; Rogers et al., 2018), and each
prole correlation is computed on basis of 240 observations. Simulation
studies showed that this sample size provides stable estimates of cor-
relations, showing minor uctuations around
ρ
(Sch¨
onbrodt & Perugini,
2013). After computing the prole correlations for each couple, result-
ing in 68 prole correlation coefcients, we tested the mean prole
correlation among all couples by averaging the stable correlations using
Fisher r-to-z transformation. This allowed us to derive a robust initial
estimate of the average prole similarity among couples.
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Preliminary analyses
The internal consistencies of the VIA-IS scales were in line with prior
ndings on the German-language version (e.g., Ruch et al., 2010), with
α
coefcients between 0.62 (self-regulation) and 0.93 (spirituality;
α
median
=0.76). The mean and SD values of the VIA-IS scales were comparable
to Ruch et al.’s (2010) ndings. On average, men and women showed
small mean differences in their strengths (Cohen’s ds ≤0.44) except for
a moderate effect size in judgment (d =0.62) with men reporting greater
expressions than women (see Fig. 1). All coefcients of the descriptive
statistics and internal consistencies are displayed in ESM A.
As a preliminary analysis, we inspected the similarity in the single
strengths. We found statistically signicant partner similarity in ve of
the 24 strengths, namely zest, gratitude, hope, humor, and spirituality
(rs ≥0.24, ps ≤.048, 95% CIs not including zero; see Table 1).
1
As in
Weber and Ruch (2012), we also found inclinations to similarity in
creativity (r =0.18), teamwork (r =0.21), and appreciation of beauty (r
=0.19), but coefcients were not statistically signicant (95% CI
including zeros and ps ≤.092) and should not be overinterpreted
without replication. Against expectations, we found initial evidence for
complementarity in the strengths of judgment (r =-0.30, 95% CI [-0.47,
-0.12], p =.013) and social intelligence (r =-0.25, 95% CI [-0.43, -0.05]
p =.043).
1
We also found a statistically signicant (p =.014) similarity coefcient of r
=0.29 for love, but the standard error was comparatively large (0.15) and the
bootstrapped 95% CI included zero. The nding awaits replication before
interpretation.
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
5
2.2.2. Prole similarity
First, we examined the rank order similarity between men and
women, which was robustly positive (r =0.88, 95% CI [0.74, 0.96], p <
.001; see ESM A for the ranks). This aligns well with the robust
normative prole similarity (r =0.87, 95% CI [0.84, 0.90], p <.001)
and the visual inspection of trajectories of men’s and women’s strengths
(Fig. 1). Thus, the expressions of the strengths were similar for the
average man and the average woman.
Secondly, we tested the raw and distinctive prole similarity using
the full set of the 240 VIA-IS items for each couple. On average, we found
robustly positive raw prole similarity (M
r
=0.23, 95% CI[0.20, 0.26],
t
67
=14.97, p <.001; SD
r
=0.13; ranging between -0.04 and 0.48). As
expected, the distinctive prole similarity decreased when controlling
for normativeness (M
r
=0.06, 95% CI [0.02, 0.09], t
67
=3.36, p =.001;
SD
r
=0.14; ranging between -0.22 and 0.44).
2.3. Discussion
Our ndings extended the study of character strengths in couples by
providing initial evidence for partner similarity in proles of the VIA
strengths. As expected, the normative prole similarity indicated that
the average man and woman show highly similar proles of strengths,
whereas the within-couple prole similarities revealed numerically low,
but above chance (i.e., 95% CIs exclude zero), similarity coefcients
after controlling for normativeness. The distinctive prole similarity
coefcient found for the strengths exceeded those reported for the big
ve traits (Rogers et al., 2018), but ts well into prior ndings of nar-
rower traits such as adult playfulness and dispositions toward ridicule
and being laughed at (Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Proyer et al., 2019). In
conclusion, on average, couples showed positive but numerically small
distinctive prole similarity. Furthermore, the range and SDs of the
similarity coefcients indicated that couples vary in their similarity,
Fig. 1. Normative Proles of Character Strengths Assessed With the 240-item VIA-IS for the Women and Men in Study 1.
Table 1
Similarity Correlation Coefcients with Bootstrapped (k =5,000 Samples) 95%
Condence Intervals in Study 1 and 2 (N =68 and 143).
Study 1
(240-item VIA-IS)
Study 2
(24-item CSRF)
Prole similarity
Normative 0.87*** [0.84, 0.90] 0.84*** [0.51, 0.95]
Raw 0.23*** [0.20, 0.26] 0.27*** [0.22, 0.23]
Distinctive 0.06** [0.02, 0.09] 0.10*** [0.05, 0.15]
Trait wise similarity
Creativity 0.18 [-0.07, 0.40] -0.06 [-0.20, 0.08]
Curiosity -0.04 [-0.26, 0.18] 0.11 [-0.08, 0.30]
Judgement -0.30* [-0.47, -0.12] 0.08 [-0.09, 0.25]
Love of learning -0.07 [-0.27, 0.13] -0.01 [-0.17, 0.16]
Perspective -0.08 [-0.33, 0.17] 0.10 [-0.06, 0.26]
Bravery -0.03 [-0.21, 0.15] 0.22** [0.04, 0.38]
Perseverance -0.16 [-0.39, 0.08] 0.06 [-0.12, 0.23]
Honesty -0.02 [-0.26, 0.23] 0.25** [0.06, 0.42]
Zest 0.24* [0.03, 0.43] 0.12 [-0.05, 0.29]
Love 0.29* [-0.02, 0.55] 0.23** [0.05, 0.40]
Kindness 0.24 [0.02, 0.46] 0.07 [-0.13, 0.28]
Social
intelligence
-0.25* [-0.43, -0.05] 0.10 [-0.08, 0.28]
Teamwork 0.21 [-0.02, 0.41] 0.28*** [0.13, 0.43]
Fairness 0.04 [-0.20, 0.27] 0.47*** [0.30, 0.62]
Leadership -0.12 [-0.39, 0.17] 0.01 [-0.16, 0.18]
Forgiveness -0.05 [-0.33, 0.22] 0.12 [-0.06, 0.29]
Humility -0.01 [-0.30, 0.28] 0.15 [-0.05, 0.33]
Prudence -0.13 [-0.38, 0.13] 0.14 [-0.04, 0.32]
Self-regulation 0.12 [-0.15, 0.37] 0.08 [-0.08, 0.23]
Beauty 0.19 [-0.03, 0.39] 0.14 [-0.01, 0.29]
Gratitude 0.43*** [0.25, 0.58] 0.28*** [0.12, 0.44]
Hope 0.35** [0.19, 0.51] 0.20* [0.05, 0.35]
Humor 0.27* [>0.00, 0.52] 0.09 [-0.08, 0.26]
Spirituality 0.54*** [0.34, 0.70] 0.47*** [0.30, 0.61]
Note. Coefcients in boldface indicate that 95% CIs do not include zero. *p <.05.
**p <.01. ***p <.001. Two-tailed.
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
6
which poses the follow-up question of whether similarity relates to in-
dicators of RS and LS (i.e., are similar couples happier than less similar
couples?). When it comes to the similarities in single strengths, our
ndings converged well with those reported for adolescent couples
(Weber & Ruch, 2012), both numerically and conceptually, regarding
which strengths indicated partner similarity. Against expectations, we
found dissimilarity among partners’ expressions in the strengths of
judgment and social intelligence, which might also have reduced the
prole similarities.
While the sample size was appropriate for the prole similarity an-
alyses, it is desirable to replicate the ndings in a larger sample. Further,
it must be noted that our sample was comparatively old. Since similarity
is typically higher in older couples (e.g., Brauer et al., 2021; Rammstedt
& Schupp, 2008), replication in a sample comprising younger couples is
also desirable. Therefore, the main ambitions behind Study 2 were to
replicate our ndings and to extend the analyses to test associations
between prole similarity and indicators of relationship- and life
satisfaction.
3. Study 2
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Sample
Our sample comprised 143 opposite-sex couples with an average
relationship duration of 5.0 years (SD =6.5; median =3.0; range: 1
month to 44.3 years). Most couples (78.3%) lived together and 11.9%
were married. The average age was M =26.0 years (SD =7.3) in women
and 28.9 years (SD =8.3) in men. Approximately half of the sample were
students (52.1%), 42.3% were employed, and the remainder (5.6%)
were in vocational training, unemployed, or retired. The educational
level was high, as 45.0% held an academic degree, 38.1% nished high
school qualifying them to attend university, 9.1% completed vocational
training, 5.2% held a regular high school diploma, and 2.1% responded
with “other.”
3.1.2. Instruments
The Character Strengths Rating Form (CSRF) by Ruch et al. (2014) is a
brief measure of Peterson and Seligman’s (2014) VIA-classication. For
each strength, a brief description is provided, which is then rated on a 9-
point Likert-type scale (1 =very much unlike me; 9 =very much like me). A
sample item is “Creativity (originality, ingenuity): Creative people have
a highly developed thinking about novel and productive ways to solve
problems and often have creative and original ideas. They do not content
themselves with conventional solutions if there are better solutions.”
The CSRF showed high convergence with the VIA-IS and relationships
with external measures, and demographic variables were highly com-
parable to those of the VIA-IS (Ruch et al., 2014).
The Relationship Quality Questionnaire (RQQ; Siffert & Bodenmann,
2010) is a 26-item questionnaire that assesses six dimensions of RS:
Fascination (3 items; i.e., being attracted to and fascinated by the part-
ner; “I admire many things about my partner”), Engagement (5 items; i.e.,
investing into the relationship; “I invest in our relationship”), Sexuality
(5 items; i.e., sexual satisfaction in the partnership; “Our partnership is
sexually satisfying for me”), Future (5 items; i.e., expecting a stable
relationship with the partner; “I think that our couple relationship has a
future”), Mistrust (3 items; “Sometimes I distrust my partner”), and
Constraint (5 items; i.e., feeling constrained in one’s autonomy by the
partner[ship]; “I feel restricted in our partnership”). Further, we esti-
mated a latent global RS factor by using the scale scores as indicators.
Siffert and Bodenmann (2010) reported good internal consistency co-
efcients (between 0.75 and 0.94), good factorial validity using
conrmatory factor analyses, and convergent validity (robust correla-
tions with alternative measures of RS). In this study, the internal con-
sistencies were 0.84 (Future and Sexuality), 0.79 (Constraint and
Mistrust), 0.77 (Fascination), and 0.70 (Engagement).
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985; German
version by Glaesmer et al., 2011) is a global measure of subjective life
satisfaction. The ve items are answered on a 7-point Likert-like scale (1
=strongly disagree, 7 =strongly agree) and summed up to a total score. A
sample item is “If I could live my life over, I would change almost
nothing.” The original scale has very good psychometric properties (
α
=
0.87; two-months retest-correlation r
tt
=0.82) and convergent validity
(e.g., strong correlations with other subjective well-being scales; Diener
et al., 1985). Also, the German version is characterized by high internal
consistency (0.92) and a unidimensional factor structure (Glaesmer
et al., 2011). In our study, the internal consistency was 0.84.
3.1.3. Procedure
We advertised the study online on websites of the department of our
university and on the website of a popular German psychology magazine
(Psychology Today) under the title “personality traits in romantic re-
lationships.” The advertisements contained the link to the online ques-
tionnaire (hosted on https://www.soscisurvey.de) and the inclusion
criteria, which stated that both partners of a couple should complete the
questionnaire independently from each other and being ≥18 years of
age. Participants did not receive nancial compensation, but psychology
students were given the opportunity to earn course credit. Partners
generated a couple code on basis of their names and years of birth to
match the data within dyads. Data collection was carried out before the
COVID-19 pandemic.
3.1.4. Data analysis
As in Study 1, we computed the trait and prole similarity co-
efcients. In addition to the analyses used in Study 1, we tested whether
prole similarity relates to LS and RS. We used the Actor-Partner
Interdependence Model (Cook & Kenny, 2005; see Fig. 2) to control
for actor and partner effects
2
of the 24 character strengths on RS and LS
and also account for partners’ interdependence in predictor and
outcome variables. As in earlier studies (e.g., Brauer & Proyer, 2018;
Furler et al., 2013), we used the raw and distinctive prole coefcients
as predictor variables in the APIM to estimate associations with RS and
LS for both partners, respectively (see Fig. 2). We computed the analyses
in Mplus 8.4 (Muth´
en & Muth´
en, 2017) using the maximum-likelihood
estimator and bootstrapped standard errors (k =1,000 samples). In
line with Cook and Kenny (2005), we report the effect parameters
describing the association between similarity and RS and LS as unstan-
dardized coefcients b. We tested whether effects of similarity differed
for men and women by computing constrained (i.e., setting effect pa-
rameters equal) and saturated (i.e., estimating effects for men and
women freely) models and comparing their t with
χ
2
difference tests.
Fig. 2. Actor-Partner Interdependence Model to Examine Similarity Effects.
Note. –Actor effects. – –Partner effects. –Similarity effects. RS =Relationship
satisfaction. LS =Life satisfaction.
2
Note that we use the terms “effect” and “predictor” in accordance with the
language use of the APIM literature. Because our data were cross-sectional, we
cannot draw causal conclusions but only examine associations.
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
7
We accepted the parsimonious model (i.e., same effects for men and
women) when the
χ
2
difference test was not statistically signicant (p >
.20; see Cook & Kenny, 2005).
Sample size rationale. As in Study 1, we determined the sample size
based on the requirements of our main analysis. Again, we used couple-
centered analyses for the description of the prole similarity and
computed the prole similarity for each couple on basis of each part-
ner’s 24 responses to the brief measure of character strengths, resulting
in 143 prole correlation coefcients that we computed in accordance
with Study 1. In addition to the analyses used in Study 1, we tested the
associations between couples’ prole similarity and outcomes (i.e., RS
and LS) with the APIM, which requires the variable-centered approach.
Thus, the dyad is the unit of analysis, and the number of couples is of
interest for sample size considerations in this study. We aimed to meet
Ledermann and Kenny’s (2017) sample size recommendation of 100 to
150 couples for the APIM analyses.
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Preliminary analyses
Preliminary analyses showed that the means and SDs of the RQQ and
SWLS were comparable to prior research in German-speaking samples
(see ESM B; e.g., Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Glaesmer et al., 2011). In line
with prior dyadic studies (Furler et al., 2013; K¨
orner & Schütz, 2021),
partners showed interdependence in the RQQ facets and SWLS (0.32 ≤
rs ≤0.51) while the mean differences among men and women were
negligible (ds ≤0.29; see ESM B for all coefcients). The expressions in
the CSRF aligned with previous ndings from German-speaking samples
(e.g., Ruch et al., 2014; see ESM A).
We tested the trait wise similarity in the single strengths and found
robust similarity for eight strengths (rs ≥0.20, 95% CIs not including
zero, ps ≤.019; Table 1), namely, bravery, honesty, love, teamwork,
fairness, gratitude, hope, and spirituality.
3.2.2. Prole similarity
We examined the correlation between the ranks of men’s and
women’s character strengths. There was a strong convergence in the
relative rankings of men’s and women’s strengths (r =0.70, 95% CI
[0.47, 0.84], p <.001). This is also reected in the normative prole
similarity, which showed high positive associations (r =0.84, 95% CI
[0.51, 0.95], p <.001), indicating that, on average, men and women
showed similar expressions of the VIA strengths. The visual inspection of
the average proles of the men and women (Fig. 3) and effect size an-
alyses (ESM A) showed that, on average, the strengths converged well,
except for men reporting greater expressions than women in judgment
(d =0.49), bravery (d =0.70), leadership (d =0.46), and self-regulation
(d =0.62).
When testing within-couple similarities, we found positive raw
prole similarity (M
r
=0.27, 95% CI [0.22, 0.32], t
142
=10.11, p <.001;
SD
r
=0.27, ranging between -0.42 and 0.95). As expected, the distinc-
tive prole similarity showed numerically lower overlap among part-
ners, but the similarity was still positive and above chance after
controlling for stereotype effects (M
r
=0.10, 95% CI [0.05, 0.15], t
142
=
3.81, p <.001; SD
r
=0.28, ranging between -0.57 and 0.85).
3.2.3. Associations between prole similarity and satisfaction
We tested the associations between raw and distinctive prole sim-
ilarities and RS and LS using APIM analyses. We did not nd evidence of
statistically signicant associations between prole similarities in
character strengths and indicators of RS and LS over and above actor and
partner effects (all bs ≤|0.55|, ps ≥.176; see ESM C for all coefcients).
Thus, partner’s prole similarities were unrelated to satisfaction in
couples.
4. General discussion
We aimed at narrowing a gap in the literature by describing adult
couples’ partner similarity in the proles of character strengths (Peter-
son & Seligman, 2004), and testing whether similarity relates to rela-
tionship satisfaction (RS) and life satisfaction (LS). Overall, our ndings
converged well across both of our studies and also with initial ndings
from adolescent couples (Weber & Ruch, 2012). In line with Weber and
Ruch (2012), we found high rank-order and normative prole similarity
correlations. Thus, the relative expressions (rank-order) and average
expressions (normative proles) are highly similar for men’s and
women’s character strengths. This ts into the literature on the negli-
gible gender differences in the VIA strengths (Heintz et al., 2019) and
high normative prole similarity associations that also exist for broad
and narrow personality traits (e.g., Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Rogers et al.,
2018). More importantly, the within-couple associations showed the
expected positive similarity across the raw and distinctive proles across
studies. The similarity of partners’ proles of the 24 VIA strengths was
expectedly positive, irrespective of using the full 240-item VIA-IS (Study
1) or the brief 24-item form (CSRF; Study 2). Moreover, after adjusting
for stereotype effects (i.e., distinctive prole similarity; Furr, 2008;
Rogers et al., 2018), partners’ deviations from the average person
converged well above chance (i.e., 95% CIs did not contain zero), thus,
indicating congural similarity in partners’ proles of strengths. Across
both studies and measures, the effect sizes replicated very well. As ex-
pected, the distinctive prole similarities were numerically small. In
comparison, they exceeded those reported for the broad big ve per-
sonality traits (Rogers et al., 2018) and were comparable to coefcients
reported for narrower traits such as adult playfulness and dispositions
toward ridicule and being laughed at (Brauer & Proyer, 2018; Brauer
et al., 2021; Proyer et al., 2019). Our ndings could be interpreted as
preliminary evidence for the notion that similarity can attract when it
comes to Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) set of 24 positively valued
traits.
In Study 2, we tested the associations between couples’ prole sim-
ilarities and indicators of satisfaction in APIM analyses (Cook & Kenny,
2005). In line with the majority of studies that tested whether partner
similarity in personality traits relates to outcomes beyond actor- and
partner effects, we found positive but negligible effect sizes (see e.g.,
Brauer et al., 2021; Chopik & Lucas, 2019; Dyrenforth et al., 2010;
Furler et al., 2013; Humbad et al., 2013; Proyer et al., 2019; van
Scheppingen et al., 2019; Weidmann et al., 2016). Thus, although
partners resemble each other in their proles of strengths, similarity is
unrelated to satisfaction (i.e., similar couples do not report greater
satisfaction than less similar couples).
In addition to the proles, we also examined partner similarity in
single strengths (trait wise analysis). We found that across our studies
and Weber and Ruch’s (2012), three strengths emerged as robustly
similar among partners, independent of the samples’ age and the mea-
sure used; namely, gratitude, hope, and spirituality. Also, there was
convergence across our and Weber and Ruch’s (2012) studies concern-
ing partner similarity in love (all coefcients ≥0.19; average correlation
of 0.24 across studies [n =298 couples]), teamwork (all coefcients ≥
0.20; average similarity correlation of 0.23 across studies), and zest (all
coefcients ≥0.12; average correlation of 0.23 across studies).
Considering that the former strengths focus on emotional expressiveness
(so-called strengths of the heart; Peterson, 2006), we preliminarily
conclude that partners share an understanding of such heart-related
strengths that contribute to interpersonal understanding and behaviors
that might support the maintenance of close relationships, whereas
strengths that focus on intellectual restraint (strengths of the mind; e.g.,
self-regulation, learning, and perspective) are less interdependent and
more individually expressed across partners. However, integrating our
ndings from prole and trait wise similarity analyses, it could be
argued that although no single strength of the mind yielded statistically
signicant trait wise similarity, partners show on average similarity in
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
8
the conguration across the full set of these strengths as well. Overall, the
effect sizes of the discussed trait wise similarity correlations were in the
expected range that is typically reported for personality traits and values
(Luo, 2017). An exception was spirituality, which showed the compar-
atively strongest effect sizes across studies. However, this aligned well
with prior ndings that showed that inclinations to religion and reli-
giousness are consistently characterized by notably high partner simi-
larity coefcients (rs ≈0.50; Luo, 2017; Watson et al., 2004, 2014).
Because our data are of cross-sectional nature, we cannot examine
the mechanisms behind the existence of partner similarity. In line with
Luo (2017), at least two possible explanations could account for the
existence of partner similarity: While assortative mating assumes that
partners share initial similarity, convergence assumes that partners
become similar over time. Meta-analytic ndings (Montoya et al., 2008)
and literature reviews (Luo, 2017) suggest that there is robust evidence
for the notion that assortative mating explains partner similarity. Also,
the literature suggests that there is little evidence for congruence, i.e.,
the notion that couples grow more similar over time (Luo, 2017; see also
Rammstedt et al., 2013; Watson et al., 2014). It could be argued that
convergence does not account for partner similarity in strengths since
our similarity ndings did not considerably differ across the older (Study
1) and younger (Study 2) couples. However, longitudinal data are
needed to clarify the trajectories of couples’ similarity across time, and
our ndings on this question can only be viewed as preliminary.
3
Further, initial similarity contributes to providing the basis for estab-
lishing a romantic relationship as it predicts romantic attraction and
-interest. A mechanism that supports initial assortative preferences is the
notion of active and passive choices during the mating process. Prior
studies addressing the VIA strengths and attributes that resemble the
strengths showed that people seek for morally valued traits when
describing their ideal close friend and partner, and that these prefer-
ences converge with actual expressions of the friend’s or partner’s
strengths (Wagner, 2019; Weber & Ruch, 2012; see also Buss, 2016; Buss
& Barnes, 1986; Chick et al., 2020; Watson et al., 2014). Drawing on
these ndings, it is feasible that people have certain preferences
regarding an ideal partner’s strengths; for example, they desire higher
expressions of honesty, humor, love, and kindness, whereas spirituality,
love of learning, perseverance, and leadership are less desired attributes
in idealized partners (Weber & Ruch, 2012), and people actively seek a
partner that overlaps with their ideal. Prior research has provided evi-
dence that people can accurately perceive the strengths of others (Ruch
et al., 2010), which is the prerequisite for identifying the strengths of a
potential partner. However, it is unclear whether people nd a partner
who possesses their ideal traits, idealize their partner according to their
desires, or adjust their perceptions of their partner to match their ideal
partner (Michelangelo phenomenon; e.g., Bühler et al., 2020; see Rusbult
et al., 2009 for an overview). Collecting partner- and ideal partner re-
ports additionally would help addressing this research question in future
studies as they add incremental value over and beyond self-reports
(Brauer et al., 2021) and allow examining the overlap between these
levels of perceptions on the intra- and interpersonal level (e.g., whether
one’s partner view converges with their ideal partner desires; Decuyper
et al., 2012). Decuyper and colleagues (2012) showed that the assumed
similarity is uniquely associated with RS and more predictive of RS than
the partners’ actual similarity. Finally, other mechanisms such as social
homogamy (i.e., partners sharing social and geographical backgrounds;
Luo, 2017) might also play a role. For example, those high in spirituality
might visit institutions such as a church more frequently than those low
in spirituality, thus nding a partner with similar expressions of
Fig. 3. Normative Proles of Character Strengths Assessed With the 24-item CSRF for the Women and Men in Study 2.
3
One might argue that analyzing couple similarity and relationship duration
with the data of Study 2 could clarify this question, but we omitted these an-
alyses because they often produce misleading ndings and conclusions (for a
discussion, see Brauer et al., 2022).
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
9
spirituality might be more likely in a shared background. Market forces
might also play a role: if someone is characterized by a prole of
consensually desired traits, they have more options of choosing partners
than someone who is characterized by a less favorable prole of
strengths and therefore has fewer options of choosing partners (Luo,
2017). Further research is needed to learn more about the mechanisms
behind partner similarity and partner selection regarding character
strengths.
Our ndings only provide preliminary knowledge by using cross-
sectional data, and we encourage follow-up studies that directly aim
at uncovering the potential developmental trajectories and effects of
partner similarity. We propose several future directions for the study of
strengths in romantic life. For example, longitudinal studies have shown
the stability of strengths over time (Gander et al., 2020), but also indi-
cated that people change in their strengths naturally and, when being
exposed to interventions, deliberately (Harzer & Ruch, 2016; Proyer
et al., 2015). It can be assumed that such changes affect partner simi-
larity depending on whether changes co-occur in partners or whether
changes are limited to only one partner of the dyad. Changes in strengths
go along with changes in well-being in individuals (Gander et al., 2020),
but no data exist for dyadic change in strengths and its effects on
satisfaction. Considering that our ndings indicate interdependence of
partners’ single strengths and proles, it could be speculated that the
dyadic interdependence affects the individuals’ change (e.g., Chopik
et al., 2018; Mund et al., 2016). Thus, extension to longitudinal data on
the dyadic development of the strengths is desirable to learn more about
the trajectories of similarity in couples’ strengths over time as well as its
effects on relationship outcomes and relationship dissolution. Future
research might also examine the consequences of partner similarity on
the heritability of the strengths. Steger et al., 2007 found support for the
existence of a genetic component to individual differences in the
strengths (14 to 59% genetic effects; median =42%). However, Steger
et al.’s classical twin design did not allow for modeling effects of
parental similarity on heritability and assumed random assortative
mating. A replication and extension to include estimates of assortative
mating is desirable to learn more about the role of partner similarity in
the heritability of the strengths and its comparison to other individual
difference variables (e.g., Borkenau et al., 2001; Buss, 2016; Kandler
et al., 2012; Luo, 2017; Vinkhuyzen et al., 2012).
Although we did not nd direct associations between partner simi-
larity and RS and LS, similarity could affect third variables that might
mediate or moderate associations. For example, strengths are robust
predictors of stress coping and buffer negative effects on satisfaction
(Harzer & Ruch, 2015), and it could be speculated that both partners’
strengths contribute to dyadic coping with relationship-related and in-
dividual problems and stressors (e.g., Herzberg, 2013). We encourage
further work on the potential practical relevance of taking proles into
account when working with clients. This may help when counseling
clients, discussing their (dis-)similarity in character strengths and how
they can capitalize on their strengths in their daily lives.
Limitations. Our ndings should be interpreted with certain limita-
tions. First, we only tested German-speaking opposite-sex couples with
comparatively high educational status, and extension to other de-
mographic groups (e.g., non-German-speaking and same-sex couples)
would contribute to generalizability. Secondly, our data are solely based
on self-reported character strengths, whereas multi-method assessment
approaches (e.g., daily diary data on the enactment of strengths; Gander
et al., 2021) would extend the validity of the ndings. Thirdly, our data
do not allow to draw causal conclusions, but only provide a cross-
sectional snapshot of the partners’ similarity. Fourthly, we have not
examined the similarity-satisfaction associations for single strengths. We
argue that an independently collected and larger sample is needed to
address this aim by using Dyadic Response Surface Analyses
(Sch¨
onbrodt et al., 2018) as an extension of the APIM for modeling the
main effects of the strengths and dyadic trait-similarity effects on in-
dicators of satisfaction. Fifthly, we have not controlled for partner
similarity in broader traits. For example, openness to new experiences
and honesty-humility are highly preferred traits in potential partners
(Liu et al., 2018), and future research should examine partner similarity
(and preferences) under consideration of the relationship between the
strengths and broader traits (see e.g., Harzer et al., 2021).
Despite these limitations, we think that our studies are a fruitful
starting point for future research on the role of similarity in strengths of
character in romantic relationships. Overall, our data suggest that (I)
partners are similar regarding their character strengths proles, (II)
especially regarding strengths entailing emotional expressiveness
(strengths of the heart), but that (III) similarity does not yield an addi-
tional robust association with relationship- or life satisfaction beyond
the main effects of partners’ character strengths.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing nancial
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to inuence
the work reported in this paper.
Acknowledgment
This study was not preregistered. We are grateful to Rico Crucius for
his help in collecting the data, and to Anna Ramona R¨
oßner and Linnea
Marie Vierow for their help with preliminary analyses.
Funding statement
This research did not receive any specic grant from funding
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-prot sectors.
Open Data
All data, syntaxes, and materials are openly available in the Open
Science Framework under https://osf.io/b4qy3/.
Open materials
All materials of this study are openly available and links with per-
manent identiers are linked in the manuscript.
Appendix A. Supplementary material
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jrp.2022.104248.
References
Baumann, D., Ruch, W., Margelisch, K., Gander, F., & Wagner, L. (2020). Character
strengths and life satisfaction in later life: An analysis of different living conditions.
Applied Research in Quality of Life, 15, 329–347. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-
018-9689-x
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117,
497–529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
Boiman-Meshita, M., & Littman-Ovadia, H. (2022). Is it me or you? An actor-partner
examination of the relationship between partners’ character strengths and marital
quality. Journal of Happiness Studies, 23, 195–210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-
021-00394-1
Borkenau, P., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M. (2001). Genetic and
environmental inuences on observed personality: Evidence from the German
Observational Study of Adult Twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
655–668. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.655
Botwin, M. D., Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Personality and mate
preferences: Five factors in mate selection and marital satisfaction. Journal of
Personality, 65, 107–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00531.x
Brauer, K., & Proyer, R. T. (2018). To love and laugh: Testing actor-, partner-, and
similarity effects of dispositions towards ridicule and being laughed at on
relationship satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 76, 165–176. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.08.008
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
10
Brauer, K., Sendatzki, R., & Proyer, R. T. (2021). Testing the associations between
dispositions toward ridicule and being laughed at and romantic jealousy in couples:
An APIM analysis. Journal of Personality, 89, 883–898. https://doi.org/10.1111/
jopy.12621
Brauer, K., Sendatzki, R., & Proyer, R. T. (2022). A primer on studying effects of
relationship duration in dyadic research: Contrasting cross-sectional and
longitudinal approaches. Advance online publication Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654075221074677.
Brauer, K., Sendatzki, R., Scherrer, T., Chick, G., & Proyer, R. T. (2021). Revisiting adult
playfulness and relationship satisfaction: APIM analyses of middle-aged and older
couples. Advance online publication International Journal of Applied Positive
Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41042-021-00058-8.
Bühler, J. L., Finkenauer, C., & Grob, A. (2020). A dyadic personality perspective on the
Michelangelo phenomenon: How personality traits relate to people’s ideal selves and
their personal growth in romantic relationships. Journal of Research in Personality,
86, Article 103943. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103943
Buschor, C., Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2013). Self- and peer-rated character strengths:
How do they relate to satisfaction with life and orientations to happiness? Journal of
Positive Psychology, 8, 116–127. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.758305
Buss, D. M. (1994). The strategies of human mating. American Scientist, 82, 238–249.
Buss, D. M. (2016). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. Basic Books.
Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 559–570. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.50.3.559
Byrne, D. E. (1971). The attraction paradigm. Academic Press.
Byrne, D. E. (1997). An overview (and underview) of research and theory within the
attraction paradigm. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 417–431.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2F0265407597143008
Chick, G., Proyer, R. T., Purrington, A., & Yarnal, C. (2020). Do birds of a playful feather
ock together? Playfulness and assortative mating. American Journal of Play, 12,
178–215.
Chopik, W. J., Kim, E. S., & Smith, J. (2018). An examination of dyadic changes in
optimism and physical health over time. Health Psychology, 37, 42–50. https://doi.
org/10.1037/hea0000549
Chopik, W. J., & Lucas, R. E. (2019). Actor, partner, and similarity effects of personality
on global and experienced well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 78,
249–261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.12.008
Cook, W. L., & Kenny, D. A. (2005). The actor-partner interdependence model: A model
of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 29, 101–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250444000405
Decuyper, M., De Bolle, M., & De Fruyt, F. (2012). Personality similarity, perceptual
accuracy, and relationship satisfaction in dating and married couples. Personal
Relationships, 19, 128–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01344.x
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Grifn, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life
scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15327752jpa4901_13
Dyrenforth, P. S., Kashy, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Lucas, R. E. (2010). Predicting
relationship and life satisfaction from personality in nationally representative
samples from three countries: The relative importance of actor, partner, and
similarity effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 690–702. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0020385
Edwards, J. R. (2001). Ten difference score myths. Organizational Research Methods, 4,
265–287. https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810143005
Furler, K., Gomez, V., & Grob, A. (2013). Personality similarity and life satisfaction in
couples. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 369–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jrp.2013.03.002
Furr, R. M. (2008). A framework for prole similarity: Integrating similarity,
normativeness, and distinctiveness. Journal of Personality, 76, 1267–1316. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00521.x
Gander, F., Hofmann, J., Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2020). Character strengths–Stability,
change, and relationships with well-being changes. Applied Research in Quality of Life,
15, 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-018-9690-4
Gander, F., Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2016). Positive psychology interventions
addressing pleasure, engagement, meaning, positive relationships, and
accomplishment increase well-being and ameliorate depressive symptoms: A
randomized, placebo-controlled online study. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 686. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00686
Gander, F., Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2017). The subjective assessment of
accomplishment and positive relationships: Initial validation and correlative and
experimental evidence for their association with well-being. Journal of Happiness
Studies, 18, 743–763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9751-z
Gander, F., & Wagner, L. (2021). Character growth following collective life events: A
study on perceived and measured changes in character strengths during the rst
wave of the COVID-19 pandemic. Advance online publication European Journal of
Personality. https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070211040975.
Gander, F., Wagner, L., Amann, L., & Ruch, W. (2021). What are character strengths good
for? A daily diary study on character strengths enactment. Advance online
publication The Journal of Positive Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/
17439760.2021.1926532.
Glaesmer, H., Grande, G., Braehler, E., & Roth, M. (2011). The German version of the
Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS): Psychometric properties, validity, and
population-based norms. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 27, 127–132.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000058
Harzer, C., Bezuglova, N., & Weber, M. (2021). Incremental validity of character
strengths as predictors of job performance beyond general mental ability and the Big
Five. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 590. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.518369
Harzer, C., & Ruch, W. (2015). The relationships of character strengths with coping,
work-related stress, and job satisfaction. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 165. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00165
Harzer, C., & Ruch, W. (2016). Your strengths are calling: Preliminary results of a web-
based strengths intervention to increase calling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17,
2237–2256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9692-y
Heintz, S., Kramm, C., & Ruch, W. (2019). A meta-analysis of gender differences in
character strengths and age, nation, and measure as moderators. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 14, 103–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2017.1414297
Herzberg, P. Y. (2013). Coping in relationships: The interplay between individual and
dyadic coping and their effects on relationship satisfaction. Anxiety, Stress & Coping,
26, 136–153. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2012.655726
Humbad, M. N., Donnellan, M. B., Iacono, W. G., McGue, M., & Burt, S. A. (2013).
Quantifying the association between personality similarity and marital adjustment
using prole correlations: A cautionary tale. Journal of Research in Personality, 47,
97–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.09.007
Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., & Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political
orientation: The roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative mating,
and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 633–645. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0025560
K¨
orner, R., & Schütz, A. (2021). Power in romantic relationships: How positional and
experienced power are associated with relationship quality. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 38, 2653–2777. https://doi.org/10.1177/
02654075211017670
Ledermann, T., & Kenny, D. A. (2017). Analyzing dyadic data with multilevel modeling
versus structural equation modeling: A tale of two methods. Journal of Family
Psychology, 31, 442–452. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000290
Liu, J., Ludeke, S., Haubrich, J., Gondan-Rochon, M., & Zettler, I. (2018). Similar to and/
or better than oneself? Singles’ ideal partner personality descriptions. European
Journal of Personality, 32, 443–458. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2159
Luo, S. (2017). Assortative mating and couple similarity: Patterns, mechanisms, and
consequences. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11, Article e12337. https://
doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12337
Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., & Kirchner, J. (2008). Is actual similarity necessary for
attraction? A meta-analysis of actual and perceived similarity. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 25, 889–922. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407508096700
Mund, M., Finn, C., Hagemeyer, B., & Neyer, F. J. (2016). Understanding dynamic
transactions between personality traits and partner relationships. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 25, 411–416. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416659458
Muth´
en, L. K., & Muth´
en, B. O. (2017). Mplus User’s Guide (Version 8). Muth´
en &
Muth´
en.
Neyer, F. J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Personality–relationship transaction in young
adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1190–1204. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1190
Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2006). Moral competence and character strengths among
adolescents: The development and validation of the Values in Action Inventory of
Strengths for Youth. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 891–909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
adolescence.2006.04.011
Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. Oxford University Press.
Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Assessment of character strengths. In:
G. P. Koocher, J. C. Norcross, & S.S. Hill III (Eds.), Psychologists’ desk reference (2nd
ed., Vol. 3, pp. 93-98). Oxford University Press.
Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Greater strengths of character and
recovery from illness. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 7-26. doi: 10.1080/
17439760500372739.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and
classication. Oxford University Press.
Proyer, R. T., Brauer, K., Wolf, A., & Chick, G. (2019). Adult playfulness and relationship
satisfaction: An APIM analysis of romantic couples. Journal of Research in Personality,
79, 40–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.02.001
Proyer, R. T., Gander, F., Wellenzohn, S., & Ruch, W. (2013). What good are character
strengths beyond subjective well-being? The contribution of the good character on
self-reported health-oriented behavior, physical tness, and the subjective health
status. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 222–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/
17439760.2013.777767
Proyer, R. T., Gander, F., Wellenzohn, S., & Ruch, W. (2015). Strengths-based positive
psychology interventions: A randomized placebo-controlled online trial on long-term
effects for a signature strengths-vs. a lesser strengths-intervention. Frontiers in
Psychology, 6, 456. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00456
Rammstedt, B., & Schupp, J. (2008). Only the congruent survive–Personality similarities
in couples. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 533–535. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.007
Rammstedt, B., Spinath, F. M., Richter, D., & Schupp, J. (2013). Partnership longevity
and personality congruence in couples. Personality and Individual Differences, 54,
832–835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.12.007
Rogers, K. H., Wood, D., & Furr, R. M. (2018). Assessment of similarity and self-other
agreement in dyadic relationships: A guide to best practices. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 35, 112–134. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407517712615
Ruch, W., Gander, F., Platt, T., & Hofmann, J. (2018). Team roles: Their relationships to
character strengths and job satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 13,
190–199. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1257051
Ruch, W., Martínez-Martí, M. L., Proyer, R. T., & Harzer, C. (2014). The Character
Strengths Rating Form (CSRF): Development and initial assessment of a 24-item
rating scale to assess character strengths. Personality and Individual Differences, 68,
53–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.03.042
K. Brauer et al.
Journal of Research in Personality 99 (2022) 104248
11
Ruch, W., Proyer, R. T., Harzer, C., Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2010).
Values in Action Inventory of Strengths (VIA-IS): Adaptation and validation of the
German version and the development of a peer-rating form. Journal of Individual
Differences, 31, 138–149. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000022
Ruch, W., & Stahlmann, A. G. (2019). 15 years after Peterson and Seligman (2004): A
brief narrative review of the research on the 12 criteria for character strengths–The
forgotten treasure of the VIA classication. In M. Brohm-Badry, C. Peifer,
J. M. Greve, & B. Berend (Eds.), Zusammen wachsen–F¨
orderung der positiv-
psychologischen Entwicklung von Individuen, Organisationen und Gesellschaft [Growing
together-Promoting positive psychological development of individuals, organizations, and
society] (pp. 142–172). Pabst Science Publishers.
Ruch, W., Weber, M., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2014). Character strengths in children and
adolescents: Reliability and initial validity of the German Values in Action Inventory
of Strengths for Youth (German VIA-Youth). European Journal of Psychological
Assessment, 30, 57–64. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000169
Rusbult, C. E., Finkel, E. J., & Kumashiro, M. (2009). The Michelangelo phenomenon.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 305–309. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1467-8721.2009.01657.x
Sch¨
onbrodt, F. D., Humberg, S., & Nestler, S. (2018). Testing similarity effects with
dyadic response surface analysis. European Journal of Personality, 32, 627–641.
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2169
Sch¨
onbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize?
Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 609–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jrp.2013.05.009
Seligman, M. E. P. (2012). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-
being. Simon and Schuster.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). Positive psychology: An introduction.
In M. Csikszentmihalyi (Ed.), Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology (pp.
279–298). Springer.
Siffert, A., & Bodenmann, G. (2010). Entwicklung eines neuen multidimensionalen
Fragebogens zur Erfassung der Partnerschaftsqualit¨
at (FPQ) [Development of a new
multidimensional questionnaire for the assessment of relationship quality (FPQ)].
Journal of Family Research, 22, 242–252. https://doi.org/10.20377/jfr-277
Steger, M. F., Hicks, B. M., Kashdan, T. B., Krueger, R. F., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2007).
Genetic and environmental inuences on the positive traits of the values in action
classication, and biometric covariance with normal personality. Journal of Research
in Personality, 41, 524–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.002
van Scheppingen, M. A., Chopik, W. J., Bleidorn, W., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2019).
Longitudinal actor, partner, and similarity effects of personality on well-being.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117, e51–e70. https://doi.org/10.1037/
pspp0000211
Vinkhuyzen, A. A., Van Der Sluis, S., Maes, H. H., & Posthuma, D. (2012). Reconsidering
the heritability of intelligence in adulthood: Taking assortative mating and cultural
transmission into account. Behavior Genetics, 42, 187–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10519-011-9507-9
Wagner, L. (2019). Good character is what we look for in a friend: Character strengths
are positively related to peer acceptance and friendship quality in early adolescents.
The Journal of Early Adolescence, 39, 864–903. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0272431618791286
Wagner, L., Gander, F., Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2020). Character strengths and
PERMA: Investigating the relationships of character strengths with a
multidimensional framework of well-being. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 15,
307–328. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-018-9695-z
Watson, D., Beer, A., & McDade-Montez, E. (2014). The role of active assortment in
spousal similarity. Journal of Personality, 82, 116–129. https://doi.org/10.1111/
jopy.12039
Watson, D., Klohnen, E. C., Casillas, A., Simms, E. N., Haig, J., & Berry, D. S. (2004).
Match makers and deal breakers: Analyses of assortative mating in newlywed
couples. Journal of Personality, 72, 1029–1068. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-
3506.2004.00289.x
Weber, M., & Ruch, W. (2012). The role of character strengths in adolescent romantic
relationships: An initial study on partner selection and mates’ life satisfaction.
Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1537–1546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
adolescence.2012.06.002
Weidmann, R., Ledermann, T., & Grob, A. (2016). The interdependence of personality
and satisfaction in couples. European Psychologist, 21, 284–295. https://doi.org/
10.1027/1016-9040/a000261
K. Brauer et al.