ArticlePDF Available

Two Models of the Development of Social Withdrawal and Social Anxiety in Childhood and Adolescence: Progress and Blind Spots

Authors:

Abstract

This commentary features a review of two recently reformulated models of the development of child and adolescent: (1) social withdrawal by Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano 2021, and (2) social anxiety by Spence and Rapee 2016. The articles that present these reformulated models now cover advances made during the prior 12 to 18 years of research, including increased knowledge of genetic vulnerability to anxiety and longitudinal patterns of development, and acknowledgement of multiple pathways towards and away from the development of social withdrawal or social anxiety (i.e., equifinality, multifinality). However, these reformulated models also contain several blind spots. The model of social withdrawal development would be improved by explicitly referring to peer treatment (not only attitudinal peer rejection), especially peer exclusion; and incorporating the potential development of clinically significant anxiety in childhood (not only adolescence) and delays in developmental milestones in adulthood. The model of social anxiety development would be improved by featuring social withdrawal as a proximal affective-behavioral profile (rather than a temperament) and drawing upon the literature on social withdrawal and its links to peer relations. Overall, there is a continuing lack of integration be-tween developmental and clinical research and models of the development of social withdrawal and social anxiety.
Citation: Gazelle, H. Two Models of
the Development of Social
Withdrawal and Social Anxiety in
Childhood and Adolescence:
Progress and Blind Spots. Children
2022,9, 734. https://doi.org/
10.3390/children9050734
Academic Editor: Jason Gilliland
Received: 30 March 2022
Accepted: 13 May 2022
Published: 17 May 2022
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2022 by the author.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
children
Perspective
Two Models of the Development of Social Withdrawal and
Social Anxiety in Childhood and Adolescence: Progress and
Blind Spots
Heidi Gazelle
Human Development and Family Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA; hgazelle@fsu.edu;
Tel.: +1-(850)-644-5722
Abstract:
This commentary features a review of two recently reformulated models of the development
of child and adolescent: (1) social withdrawal by Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano 2021, and (2) social
anxiety by Spence and Rapee 2016. The articles that present these reformulated models now cover
advances made during the prior 12 to 18 years of research, including increased knowledge of genetic
vulnerability to anxiety and longitudinal patterns of development, and acknowledgement of multiple
pathways towards and away from the development of social withdrawal or social anxiety (i.e.,
equifinality, multifinality). However, these reformulated models also contain several blind spots.
The model of social withdrawal development would be improved by explicitly referring to peer
treatment (not only attitudinal peer rejection), especially peer exclusion; and incorporating the
potential development of clinically significant anxiety in childhood (not only adolescence) and delays
in developmental milestones in adulthood. The model of social anxiety development would be
improved by featuring social withdrawal as a proximal affective-behavioral profile (rather than a
temperament) and drawing upon the literature on social withdrawal and its links to peer relations.
Overall, there is a continuing lack of integration between developmental and clinical research and
models of the development of social withdrawal and social anxiety.
Keywords:
social anxiety; social withdrawal; shyness; development; peer relations; parenting;
childhood; adolescence
1. Introduction
Conceptual models of the development of social withdrawal [
1
] and etiology of social
anxiety [
2
] in childhood and adolescence have recently been updated by their authors to
reflect the past 12 to 18 years of research. These updated models [
3
,
4
] reflect progress in the
understanding of the development of social withdrawal and social anxiety, but also reveal
blind spots in conceptualization. Furthermore, these models reveal a continuing lack of
integration of developmental and clinical approaches to research on social withdrawal and
social anxiety, despite earlier efforts towards integrating these research traditions [5,6].
Integrating developmental and clinical models and research on social withdrawal and
social anxiety is important because it would foster a greater understanding of the devel-
opmental processes (e.g., parenting, peer relations, emotion regulation, social cognitive,
and self-development processes) that lead to divergent psychosocial adjustment among
vulnerable children and youth over time. Vulnerable children and youth can move toward
(equifinality) or away from (multifinality [
7
]) the development of the clinical disorder
and accompanying impairment, and encounter roadblocks to full participation in society
(e.g., as manifested in delayed or unsuccessful attempts at adult developmental milestones
for education, employment, romantic partnership, procreation, etc.). Likewise, clinical
research on social cognitive patterns related to the development and maintenance of social
anxiety [8] should inform investigations of the development of social withdrawal.
Children 2022,9, 734. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050734 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/children
Children 2022,9, 734 2 of 6
Definitions
Social withdrawal is an umbrella term which identifies children and youths who remain
alone at high rates relative to their age mates in social contexts (e.g., school recess) in which
peers are available as interaction partners [
3
]. Social anxiety/shyness is the most common
motivation for social withdrawal, although some children and youths demonstrate social
withdrawal for other reasons (e.g., unsociability or lack of interest in peer interaction) [
9
11
].
Anxious solitary/withdrawn children and youths are conceptualized as wanting to interact
with peers (possessing normative social approach motivation), but being held back by social
anxiety (or elevated social avoidance motivation) that occurs even with familiar peers such
as classmates [
12
]. Social withdrawal, which refers to a child’s solitary behavior, should
not be confused with terms that refer to peer treatment of a child, such as peer exclusion or
isolation by the peer group [
12
]. Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano’s [
3
] model posits that social
withdrawal in childhood may forecast social anxiety in early adolescence and beyond.
The diagnosis of social anxiety disorder (SAD, previously known as “social phobia”)
requires that an individual experiences fear or anxiety in social situations, avoids or en-
dures social situations with anxiety, experiences these symptoms for at least six months,
and that these symptoms are accompanied by distress or life impairment (e.g., interfere
with attending childcare, school, or work) [
4
,
13
,
14
]. Importantly, in children and adoles-
cents, anxiety must occur in interactions with peers (not just adults). Spence and Rapee’s
model [
4
] of the etiology of social anxiety places social withdrawal in a category labelled
“genes/temperament”.
2. Strengths and Blind Spots of Reformulated Models
2.1. Development of Social Withdrawal: Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano, 2021
A strength of Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano’s [
3
] model of the development of social
withdrawal is its detailed description of the role of children’s and youths’ key interpersonal
relations with parents and peers in contributing to the development of children and youths’
social withdrawal by developmental period, including infancy, toddlerhood, preschool,
early elementary school, late elementary school, and early adolescence. The model also
features additional influences on the development of social withdrawal in childhood
and adolescence that are connected to children’s and youths’ relations with their parents
and peers, including behaviorally inhibited toddler temperament, influences on parents,
parenting beliefs, and sociological setting conditions.
Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano [
3
] deliver an insightful critique of the current state of the
literature that informs their model of the development of social withdrawal. They note that
evidence is modest or lacking for (1) linking behaviorally inhibited temperament among
unfamiliar peers to social withdrawal among familiar peers, (2) sub-cultural differences in the
prevalence and outcomes of social withdrawal, and (3) the development and consequences
of social withdrawal in late adolescence and young adulthood. Additionally, they note that
although the study of social motivations associated with social withdrawal has become
popular [
10
], there is little evidence that these social motivations relate to observable
behavior [9].
Although the description of the role of peer difficulties in maintaining and exacerbating
anxious withdrawal in childhood and adolescence is a strength of Rubin and Chronis-
Tuscano’s [
3
] model, in an unfortunate blind spot, the model refers to peer difficulties as
“peer rejection.” Peer rejection is a construct that carries a specific meaning for peer relations
researchers: peer rejection is an attitudinal variable that indicates that a child or youth is
disliked by many of his or her peers [
15
]. Although peer attitudes and peer treatment of
children and youths are often related, they are not one and the same. Children and youths
are often directly affected by the way they are overtly treated by peers, rather than by
covert peer attitudes. For instance, peer exclusion, or being left out of peer interaction,
predicts not only the maintenance of anxious solitude/withdrawal over the course of
middle childhood after controlling for peer rejection [
12
], but also the emergence of anxious
solitude/withdrawal in early adolescence in youth with no previous history of anxious
Children 2022,9, 734 3 of 6
solitude/ withdrawal [
16
]. Ironically, explicit mention was made of peer exclusion in the
earlier iteration of the model [
1
], but it is missing from the updated model [
3
]. Nonetheless,
Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano draw upon some evidence of peer mistreatment in support
of their model, but mislabeling this as evidence of peer rejection is not likely to accurately
communicate these findings to the community of researchers who have a different shared
understanding of the meaning of this term. Importantly, featuring peer rejection versus peer
exclusion as a key interpersonal process in a model of the development of social withdrawal
is not just a matter of semantics, but rather of accuracy in specifying the interpersonal
processes (i.e., overt peer treatment) known to contribute to the development of social
withdrawal in the current evidence base [12,1619].
Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano’s model [
3
] is ultimately aimed at explaining the develop-
ment of internalizing problems in socially withdrawn children and youth, which makes
it comparable to Spence and Rapee’s model of the etiology of social anxiety [
4
]. Rubin
and Chronis-Tuscano’s model [
3
] accurately suggests that childhood social withdrawal
may lead to the development of anxiety, but in another blind spot (or perhaps understate-
ment), proposes that this does not occur until the early adolescent period (12 years of
age and beyond). However, even preschool age children can be diagnosed with anxiety
disorders [
20
,
21
]. Moreover, of children identified as anxious solitary/withdrawn in fourth
grade (at about 9 years of age), a third were also diagnosed with SAD [
6
]. A large study
of prevalence [
22
] indicated that the age of onset for SAD occurred by 13 years of age
for 50 percent of individuals who developed the disorder (the median), by eight years
of age for 25 percent of individuals, and by 5 years of age for 5 percent of individuals.
Importantly, these joint age of onset and prevalence rates for child and adolescent SAD
were obtained retrospectively from individuals aged 18 and up in this large-scale preva-
lence study [
22
], and prospective studies of children have produced evidence of similar
rates in unselected samples [
21
,
23
], and substantially higher rates in children at risk due to
social withdrawal [
6
] or behavioral inhibition [
24
]. Thus, models of the development of
social withdrawal should acknowledge the potential for clinically significant social anxiety
to develop in middle childhood, and probably also in the preschool period. Research is
needed on processes involved in the development of clinically significant social anxiety in
withdrawn children and adolescents. Peer adversity may be a key factor contributing to the
development of clinically significant social anxiety and other internalizing problems [12].
To address another blind spot, in addition to internalizing problems, the equally
important potential adult outcomes of childhood social withdrawal—delays in achieving
adult developmental milestones [
25
] (e.g., moving out of the parental home, obtaining a
higher education, initiating a career, initiating a romantic partnership, cohabitation with a
romantic partner, childbearing, childrearing, income, homeownership)—should be added
to the model of the development of social withdrawal. Delays in adult developmental
milestones are likely to have profound impacts on wellbeing. For instance, the absence
of a romantic partner and children contributes to loneliness, which can compromise life
satisfaction and even forecast premature death [26].
Consideration of these recommendations in developmental research and prevention
efforts for social withdrawal in children and adolescents could yield improved cross-
fertilization in developmental and clinical knowledge bases, as well as prevention and
intervention efficacy for withdrawn children and adolescents. First, awareness of clinically
significant anxiety could be increased in families, schools, and other professionals who
serve children and adolescents to facilitate the early identification of young people suffering
with anxiety and limit the extent that this anxiety may compromise healthy development by
facilitating prevention and early treatment. Second, the overt forms of peer mistreatment
(i.e., peer exclusion) that are most often detrimental to healthy development in withdrawn
children could be targeted for prevention and intervention [
27
]. Third, patterns of cognition
characteristic of social anxiety and its maintenance [
4
] could be investigated in socially
withdrawn children and adolescents, and these cognitive patterns could also become the
targets of prevention and intervention efforts for withdrawn children and adolescents.
Children 2022,9, 734 4 of 6
Fourth, prevention and intervention efforts for withdrawn children and adolescents could
explicitly support the achievement of adult developmental milestones for participation in
society (e.g., facilitate healthy friendships and romantic relationships, transitions to higher
education, career counseling, financial counseling, and parenting skills).
2.2. Development of Social Anxiety: Spence and Rapee, 2016
Spence and Rapee’s model [
4
] posits that the interplay between “genes/temperament”
and environmental factors contributes to proximal behavioral and cognitive factors, which
in turn determine the individual’s level of anxiety. Additionally, both personal (age, gender)
and cultural factors influence whether the individual’s anxiety engenders sufficient life
impairment to warrant a diagnosis of SAD.
The literature review, which provides the basis for this updated model, has multiple
strengths. For example, it covers research on genetic vulnerability to anxiety. This literature
indicates that both genes (multiple genetic variants which individually have a small impact,
but in combination have a significant impact) and non-shared environment (environmental
influences which are not shared by family members) have a strong impact on anxiety. Major
sources of non-shared environmental influences are children’s and youths’ peer relations
and other experiences at school [
28
]. Also, Spence and Rapee [
4
] review research indicating
that behaviorally inhibited toddler temperament, or wariness to unfamiliar people and
situations/objects [
29
], conveys an increased risk of anxiety over the course of development.
Additionally, Spence and Rapee [
4
] review research on attentional “bias” towards threat,
and nuanced cognitive and social cognitive mechanisms implicated in social anxiety.
In their analysis of the extant literature, Spence and Rapee [
4
] also come to the critical
conclusion that about fifteen percent of children who do not have a history of behaviorally
inhibited temperament nonetheless develop SAD (equifinality). Although they do not
elaborate on this important observation, it may be that non-shared environmental influences
that take on the form of negative interpersonal learning processes, such as peer adversity
and other experiences at school, also contribute to the development of social anxiety in
children and youths who do not have early histories of behavioral inhibition or withdrawal.
In support of this contention, the literature on social withdrawal provides evidence that
peer exclusion predicts increasing anxious solitude in early adolescence, even among
youths without a prior history of social withdrawal in childhood [16].
However, a major blind spot of Spence and Rapee’s model [
4
] is its treatment of
social withdrawal. Social withdrawal is included under “genes/temperament” rather
than as a “proximal behavior” in the figure depicting the model, and the literature review
which supports the model does not draw upon the literature on social withdrawal. This
treatment of social withdrawal implies that it is the same phenomenon as behaviorally
inhibited temperament. This assumption is not warranted for several reasons. First, as
noted by Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano [
3
], there is little empirical evidence for the link
between behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar and social withdrawal among familiar
peers. Second, the conceptualization of the two constructs differs. Behavioral inhibition
is conceptualized as wariness in the face of the unfamiliar (people, situations, objects),
whereas withdrawal is conceptualized as elevated solitary behavior among peers due to
social evaluative concerns (fears about being poorly treated by peers and/or not behaving
competently with peers) [
12
]. Behavioral inhibition is believed to be rooted in a low
threshold for stimulation in the brain’s limbic system [
29
], whereas social withdrawal is
linked to negative interpersonal learning experiences with parents and peers [
3
]. Behavioral
inhibition may increase the risk for the development of social withdrawal among familiar
peers, but evidence suggests that social withdrawal develops in non-behaviorally inhibited
children who have negative interpersonal learning experiences [
30
,
31
]. A great strength
of the literature on the development of social withdrawal is a detailed assessment of key
interpersonal learning mechanisms with parents and peers. Therefore, in not drawing
upon this literature, models of social anxiety development fail to build upon evidence for
Children 2022,9, 734 5 of 6
the interpersonal learning processes that engender and/or exacerbate social anxiety and
social avoidance.
Considerations of these recommendations in clinical prevention and early treatment
for SAD in children and adolescents could yield many improvements in selection for
intervention as well as intervention efficacy. First, at-risk children and adolescents could
be targeted for intervention based on social withdrawal or the combination of social
withdrawal and peer difficulties, not only behavioral inhibition. This may address the
psychosocial needs and mental health of at-risk children and adolescents that are currently
overlooked. Second, these children and adolescents’ peer relations (e.g., peer exclusion)
could become a target of intervention [
27
], alongside efforts to ameliorate individual
social and emotional competence. Third, interventions could be timed just prior to the
beginning of the new school year to take advantage of the natural rhythms of this ecological
transition to maximize children’s and adolescents’ chances for a fresh start when they
have encountered peer difficulties [
16
]. This opens the possibility that naturally occurring
interactions and relationships with peers could serve a therapeutic purpose.
3. Conclusions
Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano and Spence and Rapee’s updated models [
3
,
4
] reflect
progress in the understanding of the development of social withdrawal and social anxiety
during the last 12–18 years, but also reveal blind spots in conceptualization and evidence, as
well as continuing lack of integration of developmental and clinical research and conceptual
models of social withdrawal and social anxiety, despite earlier efforts towards integrating
these research traditions [
5
,
6
]. Research at the intersection of the development of social
withdrawal and social anxiety has the potential to provide a more integrated explanation
and evidence base for both phenomena by borrowing from the strengths of both traditions
and highlighting cross-disciplinary gaps in evidence and intervention efforts.
Funding:
This research was funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development (NICHD), grant number 1 R03 HD104881-01A1.
Institutional Review Board Statement:
Ethical review and approval were not needed for this review,
because no data were presented.
Informed Consent Statement: Not applicable.
Acknowledgments:
Thanks for support from Florida State University and from the students in my
graduate research seminar who have shown such enthusiasm for our discussion of models of social
withdrawal and social anxiety: Jessie Shafer, Jacob Williams, and Karina Sandoval Jalapa.
Conflicts of Interest:
The author declares no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the
writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.
References
1.
Rubin, K.H.; Coplan, R.J.; Bowker, J.C. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu. Rev. Psychol.
2009
,60, 141–171. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
2.
Rapee, R.M.; Spence, S.H. The etiology of social phobia: Empirical evidence and an initial model. Clin. Psychol. Rev.
2004
,24,
737–767. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
3.
Rubin, K.H.; Chronis-Tuscano, A. Perspectives on social withdrawal in childhood: Past, present, and prospects. Child Dev.
Perspect. 2021,15, 160–167. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4.
Spence, S.H.; Rapee, R.M. The etiology of social anxiety disorder: An evidence-based model. Behav. Res. Ther.
2016
,86, 50–67.
[CrossRef]
5.
Gazelle, H.; Rubin, K.H. Social Anxiety in Childhood: Bridging Developmental and Clinical Perspectives. In Social Anxiety in
Childhood: Bridging Developmental and Clinical Perspectives: New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development; Gazelle, H., Rubin,
K.H., Eds.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2010; pp. 1–16.
6.
Gazelle, H.; Workman, J.O.; Allan, W. Anxious solitude and clinical disorder in middle childhood: Bridging developmental and
clinical approaches to childhood social anxiety. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2010,38, 1–17. [CrossRef]
7.
Cicchetti, D.; Rogosch, F.A. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol.
1996
,8, 597–600.
[CrossRef]
Children 2022,9, 734 6 of 6
8.
Miers, A.C.; Blöte, A.W.; de Rooij, M.; Bokhorst, C.L.; Westenberg, P.M. Trajectories of Social Anxiety during Adolescence and
Relations with Cognition, Social Competence, and Temperament. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2013,41, 97–110. [CrossRef]
9.
Spangler, T.; Gazelle, H. Anxious solitude, unsociability, and peer exclusion in middle childhood: A multitrait-multimethod
matrix. Soc. Dev. 2009,18, 833–856. [CrossRef]
10.
Coplan, R.J.; Rose-Krasnor, L.; Weeks, M.; Kingsbury, A.; Kingsbury, M.; Bullock, A. Alone is a crowd: Social motivations, social
withdrawal, and socioemotional functioning in later childhood. Dev. Psych. 2013,49, 861. [CrossRef]
11. Asendorpf, J.B. Beyond social withdrawal: Shyness, unsociability, & peer avoidance. Human Dev. 1990,33, 250–259.
12.
Gazelle, H.; Ladd, G.W. Anxious solitude and peer exclusion: A diathesis-stress model of internalizing trajectories in childhood.
Child Dev. 2003,74, 257–278. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
13.
American_Psychiatric_Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; Text Rev.; APA: Washington, DC,
USA, 2022.
14.
World Health Organization. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 11th ed.; WHO Press:
Geneva, Switzerland, 2019.
15.
Ladd, G.W.; Kochenderfer-Ladd, B.; Ettekal, I.; Clifford, B.N. Manifestations of Solitude in Interpersonal Contexts and Negative
Peer Experiences. In The Handbook of Solitude; Coplan, R.J., Bowker, J.C., Nelson, L.J., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2021;
pp. 117–132.
16.
Gazelle, H.; Faldowski, R.A. Multiple trajectories in anxious solitary youths: The middle school transition as a turning point in
development. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2019,47, 1135–1152. [CrossRef]
17.
Booth-LaForce, C.; Oxford, M.L. Trajectories of social withdrawal from grades 1 to 6: Prediction from early parenting, attachment,
& temperament. Dev. Psych. 2008,44, 1298–1313.
18.
Oh, W.; Rubin, K.H.; Bowker, J.C.; Booth-LaForce, C.; Rose-Krasnor, L.; Laursen, B. Trajectories of social withdrawal from middle
childhood to early adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2008,36, 553–566. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
19.
Asendorpf, J.B. The malleability of behavioral inhibition: A study of individual developmental functions. Dev. Psych.
1994
,30,
912–919. [CrossRef]
20.
Egger, H.L.; Angold, A. The Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA): A structured parent interview for diagnosing
psychiatric disorders in preschool children. In Handbook of Infant, Toddler, & Preschool Mental Health Assessment; DelCarmen-
Wiggins, R., Carter, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 223–243.
21.
Egger, H.L.; Angold, A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and
epidemiology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2006,47, 313–337. [CrossRef]
22.
Kessler, R.C.; Berglund, P.; Demler, O.; Jin, R.; Merikangas, K.R.; Walters, E.E. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions
of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2005,62, 593–602. [CrossRef]
23.
Bufferd, S.J.; Dougherty, L.R.; Carlson, G.A.; Klein, D.N. Parent-reported mental health in preschoolers: Findings using a
diagnostic interview. Compr. Psychiatry 2011,52, 359–369. [CrossRef]
24.
Rapee, R.M.; Kennedy, S.; Ingram, M.; Edwards, S.; Sweeney, L. Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited
preschool children. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2005,73, 488–497. [CrossRef]
25.
Shafer, J.K.; Gazelle, H. Youth social withdrawal and subsequent adult psychosocial adjustment: A quantitative systematic review.
J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2022,submitted.
26.
Holt-Lunstad, J. Why social relationships are important for physical health: A systems approach to understanding and modifying
risk and protection. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018,69, 437–458. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
27.
Harrist, A.W.; Bradley, K.D. “You can’t say you can’t play”: Intervening in the process of social exclusion in the kindergarten
classroom. Early Child. Res. Q. 2003,18, 185–205. [CrossRef]
28.
Scaini, S.; Belotti, R.; Ogliari, A. Genetic and environmental contributions to social anxiety across different ages: A meta-analytic
approach to twin data. J. Anxiety Disord. 2014,28, 650–656. [CrossRef]
29.
Kagan, J.; Reznick, J.S.; Clarke, C.; Snidman, N.; Garcia Coll, C. Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Dev.
1984
,55,
2212–2225. [CrossRef]
30.
Gazelle, H.; Faldowski, R.A. Peer exclusion is linked to inhibition with familiar but not unfamiliar peers at two years of age.
Infant Child Dev. 2014,23, 220–228. [CrossRef]
31.
Asendorpf, J.B. Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for situational specificity and a two-factor model. Dev.
Psych. 1990,26, 721–730. [CrossRef]
... Moreover, Hikikomori-like social withdrawal has been significantly predicted by higher depressive levels, lower anxiety levels and by higher levels at irritable and depressive affective temperaments (as measured by TEMPS-M). Indeed, despite some studies having been carried out by exploring some child temperamental features and social isolation, there are no published studies specifically addressed to young adults [37,38]. These studies found an association between child social isolation and the presence of the so-called behavioral inhibition temperament, i.e. the tendency to react following exposure to unfamiliar stimuli by developing anxiety and avoidance behavior [37,38]. ...
... Indeed, despite some studies having been carried out by exploring some child temperamental features and social isolation, there are no published studies specifically addressed to young adults [37,38]. These studies found an association between child social isolation and the presence of the so-called behavioral inhibition temperament, i.e. the tendency to react following exposure to unfamiliar stimuli by developing anxiety and avoidance behavior [37,38]. Akiskal already identified a possible association between social isolation and specific affective temperaments, such as cyclothymic (particularly in transient social isolation episodes), and depressive affective temperaments (more associated with the tendency to develop a social withdrawal) [39,40]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Hikikomori (HK) is characterized by self-isolation and social refusal, being more likely also associated with affective disorders, including depression. This case–control study primarily aimed at identifying (if any) predominant affective temperaments are associated with HK in depressed versus not-depressed individuals. Secondary objectives comprise assessing which other psychopathological dimensions (e.g., boredom, anxiety) are associated with the HK specifier in depressed individuals. Methods From the larger SWATCH study, 687 Italian young people were screened for depression, as measured by 9 items-Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) and HK-like social withdrawal, through the Hikikomori Questionnaire-25 (HQ-25). All subjects were administered a brief-Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris and San Diego (TEMPS-M), the 7 items-Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD-7) and the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale (MSBS). Results Males reported significantly higher scores at HQ-25 total score than females (p = 0.026). In the total sample, HK social withdrawal is positively predicted by MSBS low arousal, disengagement, depressive levels, depressive and irritable affective temperaments, while negatively by anxiety (F(6, 680) = 82.336, p < 0.001, R² = 0.421). By selecting only depressed sample, HQ-25 is positively predicted by MSBS total score, low arousal and depressive affective temperament, while negatively by MSBS high arousal (F(4, 383) = 48.544, p < 0.001, R² = 0.336). The logistic regression model found that the likelihood of developing depression with the HK specifier is significantly predicted by depressive and cyclothymic affective temperaments. Conclusions These preliminary findings could help in clinically characterizing the relationship between specific affective temperamental profiles among individuals with depression with/without HK specifier, in order to provide a more tailored and personalized therapeutic approach. Our Italian study should be extensively replicated in larger, longitudinal and multicentric pan-European studies, by specifically assessing the impact of these findings on depression clinical course, prognosis and treatment outcomes.
... Reactive and non-self-determined motivations to be away from others or having too much solitude that is unwanted (e.g., social exclusion), has been associated with socio-emotional maladjustment and self-harm (Borg & Willoughby, 2022;Coplan et al., 2021). Studies also show that when one is alone, they may experience negative feelings such as loneliness or peer exclusion, as well as feelings of depression and social anxiety (Gazelle, 2022;Pearcey et al., 2020;Rubin et al., 2021). Many studies with youth also show that personality traits reflecting emotional stability were linked with an aversion of aloneness (Lay et al., 2019;Uziel, 2016;2021;Wilson et al.2014). ...
... These findings support past research that shows adolescent girls are more likely to experience anxiety and negative feelings around others (Gazelle, 2022;Pearcey et al., 2020). Such results may be due to differences in how girls and boys experience solitude and social experiences, as genderrole stereotypes which may encourage girls to spend more time socializing instead of by themselves . ...
Article
This paper explores adolescents’ solitude activities, their reasons for choosing to be alone, and how they think and feel when they are away from others. Gender and age differences were also explored.ApproachWe looked at the links between solitary activities and emotional well-being and personal happiness. Sixty-one adolescents (68% female, Mage = 16.14 ± .50) completed self-report on-line measures assessing time alone, solitary activities, and indices of adjustment.FindingsMajority of participants reported that they were alone by choice, and that they were more often with others than alone. Age and gender differences emerged in time alone, feelings of self-worth, and emotional well-being.Research Limitations The demographics of participants was limited to English-speakers in Eastern Canada, and data collection was affected by the COVID-19 pandemic.Practical Implications School programs could be developed to better equip adolescents with the skills necessary to be productive in, and feel positive about, time spent alone.
... In addition, future research could investigate the effects of more types of social feedback (e.g., personal feedback and non-personal feedback) on socially avoidant individuals [71]. Moreover, children and adolescents have a high prevalence of clinical social avoidance, indicating a significant developmental risk prior to emerging adulthood [72]. In the area of education, future research could explore social avoidance among children and adolescents to provide a developmental perspective and develop corresponding intervention programs (e.g., exercise intervention, mindfulness intervention) in families or school to promote their social development [73,74]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social avoidance refers to active non-participation in social activities, which is detrimental to healthy interpersonal interaction for emerging adults. Social rejection is a kind of negative social evaluation from others making people feel social pain. However, how socially avoidant emerging adults process social feedback information after experiencing social rejection has received less attention. The current study aimed to explore the differences in social interaction feedback processing after social rejection between a socially avoidant group (n = 16) and a comparison group (n = 16) in a human-to-human interaction context. Computer game tasks with two types of interaction (cooperation and competition) were used to record the event-related potentials when receiving social interaction feedback in two conditions (social rejection and control condition). The results showed that (1) the socially avoidant group had lower reward positivity amplitudes than the comparison group when receiving social feedback; (2) the socially avoidant group presented larger P300 amplitudes in the social rejection condition than in the control condition, but the comparison group did not; and (3) social rejection evoked more negative N1 amplitudes in the socially avoidant and comparison groups. The findings suggest that socially avoidant emerging adults may have flaws in reward sensitivity during interpersonal interaction, and they might also exert more attentional and emotional resources to social feedback after social rejection.
Article
Full-text available
Backgrounds Social skills are frequently impaired in neurodevelopmental disorders and genetic conditions, including 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS) and autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Although often assessed with questionnaires, direct assessment provides a more valid estimate of the constructs. Role-plays (i.e., simulates situational settings) therefore appear to be an appropriate indicator of social skills in daily life. Methods This co-registered study involved 53 individuals with 22q11DS, 34 individuals with ASD, and 64 typically developing (TD) peers aged 12–30 years. All participants were assessed with role-plays as well as parent-reported questionnaires and clinical interviews focusing on social skills, functioning and anxiety. Results Both clinical groups showed impaired social skills compared to TD, but distinct social profiles emerged between the groups. Individuals with 22q11DS displayed higher social appropriateness and clarity of speech but weaker general argumentation and negotiation skills, with the opposite pattern observed in participants with ASD. No association was found between social skills measured by direct observation and caregiver reports. Social anxiety, although higher in clinical groups than in TD, was not associated with role-plays. Conclusions This study highlights the need to train social skills through tailored interventions to target the specific difficulties of each clinical population. It also highlights the importance of combining measures as they do not necessarily provide the same outcome.
Article
Full-text available
The closure of schools during the COVID-19 pandemic affected adolescents' social withdrawal and social anxiety. Yet, self-esteem may have acted as a protective factor during this period. This study aimed to compare the trajectories of social withdrawal and social anxiety before (Year 1), during (Year 2 and 3), and after (Year 4) the closure of schools imposed by the COVID-19-related lockdowns, and to investigate the association of self-esteem with these trajectories. Participants were 844 (50.6% boys) Portuguese adolescents (mean age 12.70 years, SD = 1.14). The Social and Emotional Competencies Evaluation Questionnaire (QACSE) was used to assess social withdrawal and social anxiety, while The Global Self-Esteem scale of the Self-Description Questionnaire II was used to measure self-esteem. Growth curve analysis showed that social withdrawal and social anxiety had more negative trajectories during the year in which the school closures occurred. In addition, adolescents reported higher social withdrawal after the lockdowns than before the pandemic. Higher self-esteem was associated with a more positive trajectory in social withdrawal. Therefore, the results showed the negative of impact of the closure of schools on adolescents' social anxiety and social withdrawal, and that self-esteem was a protective factor during these challenging and adverse events.
Article
This chapter describes the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA), a structured diagnostic parent-administered psychiatric interview about children ages 2–5 (24 months through 72 months) that assesses parent-reported psychiatric symptoms, disorders, and impairment in preschool children. The PAPA also assesses school or day-care functioning, family structure and functioning, parenting behaviors, adverse life events, and a host of demographic variables including socioeconomic status (Egger, Ascher, & Angold, 1999). Also described is the ePAPA, a web-based tool for administering the PAPA electronically. The PAPA is derived from the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment, an interview for children ages 9–18 years old. The PAPA, the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment, and now the Young Adult Psychiatric Assessment comprise a suite of interviews that employ a consistent approach to the assessment of psychopathology in childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. The chapter reviews the history of the PAPA’s development and describes the structure, content, reliability, and uses of the PAPA in research. Implications for clinical practice are also discussed.
Article
The purpose of this article is to: (1) Provide definitional clarity for the construct of social withdrawal as it was originally construed; (2) review the original theoretical and conceptual bases that led to the first program of research dedicated to the developmental study of social withdrawal; (3) describe correlates, precursors, and consequences of social withdrawal; (4) provide a conceptual, working model that connects the dots pertaining to precursors, concomitants, and consequences of social withdrawal; and (5) describe how data derived from the study of social withdrawal and related constructs have led to the development of a novel intervention which targets risk factors known to predict social withdrawal and its negative psychological consequences.
Article
This study aimed to identify divergent patterns of individual continuity and change in anxious solitude (AS) in the last half of elementary school (3rd – 5th grade) and the first two years of middle school (6th – 7th grade), and test predictors and outcomes of these pathways. Participants were 688 youths (girls n = 354, 51.5%; M age at outset = 8.66 years, SD = 0.50). Latent class growth analyses identified two AS trajectory classes in elementary school (moderate-decreasing, high-increasing) and three in middle school (low-stable, low-increasing, high-decreasing). The elementary school moderate-decreasing class was two-and-a-half times more likely than others to end in the middle school low-stable class. In contrast, the elementary school high-increasing class was twice as likely as others to end in the middle school low-increasing class, and four times as likely to end in the middle school high-decreasing class. Peer exclusion predicted membership in increasing AS trajectory classes in both elementary and middle school, whereas the middle school high-decreasing AS trajectory class demonstrated decreasing peer exclusion during middle school. Likewise, inability to defend oneself predicted membership in increasing AS trajectory classes in both elementary and middle school, whereas membership in the middle school high-decreasing AS trajectory class was predicted by inability to defend oneself in elementary but not middle school. High-decreasing AS youths’ improved ability to defend themselves in middle school appeared to be related to a cascade of improvements in related domains. In contrast, membership in increasing AS classes in elementary and middle school predicted symptoms of social anxiety and depression.
Article
Social relationships are adaptive and crucial for survival. This review presents existing evidence that our social connections to others have powerful influences on health and longevity and that lacking social connection qualifies as a risk factor for premature mortality. A systems perspective is presented as a framework by which to move social connection into the realm of public health. Individuals, and health-relevant biological processes, exist within larger social contexts including the family, neighborhood and community, and society and culture. Applying the social ecological model, this review highlights the interrelationships of individuals within groups in terms of understanding both the causal mechanisms by which social connection influences physical health and the ways in which this influence can inform potential intervention strategies. A systems approach also helps identify gaps in our current understanding that may guide future research. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology Volume 69 is January 4, 2018. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Article
The current paper presents an update to the model of social anxiety disorder (social phobia) published by Rapee and Spence (2004). It evaluates the research over the intervening 11 years and advances the original model in response to the empirical evidence. We review the recent literature regarding the impact of genetic and biological influences, temperament, cognitive factors, peer relationships, parenting, adverse life events and cultural variables upon the development of SAD. The paper draws together recent literature demonstrating the complex interplay between these variables, and highlights the many etiological pathways. While acknowledging the considerable progress in the empirical literature, the significant gaps in knowledge are noted, particularly the need for further longitudinal research to clarify causal pathways, and moderating and mediating effects. The resulting model will be valuable in informing the design of more effective treatment and preventive interventions for SAD and will provide a useful platform to guide future research directions.
Article
Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) and Social Anxiety Symptoms (SAS) have been largely studied both epidemiologically and genetically, however, estimates of genetic and environmental influences for these phenotypes widely vary across reports. Based upon available literature, 13 cohorts (42585 subjects) were included in 3 meta-analytic estimates of the standardized variance components of aetiological influences on SAD/SAS, on the effect of age and of phenotype (symptoms vs diagnosis). The proportions of variance accounted for by genetic and environmental factors were calculated by averaging estimates among studies, and pondered by the number of individuals in each sample. Meta-analytic estimations showed that genetic and non-shared environmental factors explain most of individual differences for SAD/SAS. In adults, the genetic contribution was half than that in younger patients, with higher contribution of non-shared environmental influences. In contrast, the shared environmental factors seem to be less relevant.
Article
This study examined the extent that inhibition among familiar peers was related to inhibition among unfamiliar peers versus exclusion by familiar peers at 2 years of age. Peer inhibition at 2 years of age was assessed by both mothers and teachers on versions of the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire and the Preschool Play Behavior Scale (N= 141 children, 53% girls) that were adapted such that each item was posed separately regarding familiar versus unfamiliar peers. A subset of participants (n = 82) were also observed during free-play with familiar childcare classmates at 30 months using the Peer Interaction Observation System – Early Childhood. There was no significant relation between children exhibiting extreme levels of inhibition with familiar versus unfamiliar peers across informants. Substantial numbers of children demonstrated inhibition with familiar but not unfamiliar peers (18%), with unfamiliar but not familiar peers (10%), or with neither familiar nor familiar peers (69%). In contrast, few children exhibited inhibition with both familiar and unfamiliar peers (3%). Observed peer exclusion among familiar childcare peers was systematically positively correlated with the display of inhibition among familiar childcare peers but not unfamiliar peers across informants. These findings suggest that proximal relational learning processes may have more impact than inhibited temperament on inhibition among familiar peers at 2 years of age. However, the concurrent correlational nature of this study does not provide evidence about the direction of effect. Additionally, in the future, it will be important to conduct observations of children’s interactions with both familiar and unfamiliar peers.