Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Eect of timing of school
enrollment on physical tness
in third graders
Thea Fühner *, Urs Granacher, Kathleen Golle & Reinhold Kliegl
Timing of initial school enrollment may vary considerably for various reasons such as early or delayed
enrollment, skipped or repeated school classes. Accordingly, the age range within school grades
includes older-(OTK) and younger-than-keyage (YTK) children. Hardly any information is available on
the impact of timing of school enrollment on physical tness. There is evidence from a related research
topic showing large dierences in academic performance between OTK and YTK children versus
keyage children. Thus, the aim of this study was to compare physical tness of OTK (N = 26,540) and
YTK (N = 2586) children versus keyage children (N = 108,295) in a representative sample of German
third graders. Physical tness tests comprised cardiorespiratory endurance, coordination, speed,
lower, and upper limbs muscle power. Predictions of physical tness performance for YTK and OTK
children were estimated using data from keyage children by taking age, sex, school, and assessment
year into account. Data were annually recorded between 2011 and 2019. The dierence between
observed and predicted z-scores yielded a delta z-score that was used as a dependent variable in the
linear mixed models. Findings indicate that OTK children showed poorer performance compared to
keyage children, especially in coordination, and that YTK children outperformed keyage children,
especially in coordination. Teachers should be aware that OTK children show poorer physical tness
performance compared to keyage children.
e importance of physical tness for children’s health is undisputed1. According to Caspersen etal.2, physical
tness can be categorized as health- (e.g., cardiorespiratory endurance, muscular endurance, muscular strength,
body composition, and exibility) or skill-related tness (e.g., agility, balance, coordination, speed, [muscle]
power, and reaction time). ere is evidence from original research3, systematic reviews, and meta-analyses4,5
that cardiorespiratory endurance and muscular strength are positively associated with markers of physical health
(e.g., body mass index, waist circumference, skinfold thickness, cardiovascular disease risk score) in youth.
Accordingly, it is important to regularly monitor and evaluate children’s physical tness to identify potential
decits in physical tness as early as possible. Recent studies on global secular trends in youth physical tness
indicated physical tness declines particularly for measures of cardiorespiratory endurance. is trend addition-
ally emphasizes the relevance of physical tness testing6,7.
Physical tness tests represent an easy-to-administer, reliable, and valid means to assess and evaluate chil-
dren’s physical tness in large scale studies conducted in sport clubs or schools8. Several studies from around the
globe8–17 showed developmental increases in physical tness from childhood to adolescence8,10–12,15,17. Irrespective
of age, boys outperform girls in most components of physical tness9–17, except for balance10,17 and exibility9–15,17.
e available studies on physical tness development have been conducted in youth aged 5–18years. In these
studies, children and adolescents were matched into 1-year age groups. is age grouping system is also evident
in many settings of children’s everyday life. For instance, children are matched in 1-year age teams within sport
clubs or in grades within schools. However, this age grouping system is not without limitations because of dif-
ferences in relative age depending on the specic cut-o date under consideration. For schools in general, the
cut-o date of initial school enrollment is specic to the country under investigation. For instance, in the Federal
State of Brandenburg, Germany the ocial and initial school enrollment date is September 30th. Accordingly,
children are enrolled to school (i.e., rst grade) if they are aged between 6years and 0months and 6years and
11months on September 30th of the respective year (i.e., keyage children in rst grade). us, children who are
born on September 30th or slightly later are at the extreme end, i.e., almost 1 year older than their classmates
who are born in August. ese dierences in the birthdate may have an impact on anthropometrics (e.g., body
height, body mass) and physical tness (e.g., muscular strength, power, cardiorespiratory endurance, or speed)18
OPEN
Division of Training and Movement Sciences, Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Potsdam, Am Neuen Palais
10, Building 12, 14469 Potsdam, Germany. *email: fuehner@uni-potsdam.de
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
because physical tness performance increases with age from childhood to adolescence8,10–12,15,17. us, within
a 1-year age-group, the relatively older children (i.e., born near the cut-o date) may outperform their relative
younger classmates (i.e., born later to the cut-o date) because of their relatively older age18,19. In fact, a previous
study conducted with keyage third graders (i.e., children aged 8years and 0months to 8years and 11months) has
shown that physical tness increased linearly with chronological age20. Furthermore, even within the single ninth
year of life, the relatively older children (i.e., aged 8years and 6months to 8years and 11months) signicantly
outperformed the younger children (i.e., aged 8years and 0months to 8years and 5months) in physical tness20.
Within one school-grade, there are keyage children as well as younger- (YTK) or older-than-keyage (OTK)
children. is is due to early or late school enrollment, skipping or repetition of a school year. With reference to
our data, age ranged from 5years and 11months to 14years and 5months for our study sample that included
YTK and OTK children. Given that there are already large dierences in physical tness within the group of key-
age children20, the question arises as to physical tness performance of YTK and OTK children. To the authors’
knowledge there is hardly any information available in the literature on dierences in physical tness of YTK and
OTK children versus keyage children. A major goal of physical education is to create a learning setting for each
child according to his/her individual needs to ensure a holistic development. us, ndings on physical tness
performance of YTK and OTK children provide valuable information to promote physical tness according to
the child´s individual needs. For instance, children who show delayed physical tness development should receive
additional health and tness programs to compensate their decits in physical tness. Furthermore, given that
grading systems are only available for keyage children, ndings of this study can be used to individually grade
physical tness according to age, sex, and timing of school enrollment.
Information from a related research topic shows large dierences in academic performance between OTK
and YTK children versus keyage children21–23. For instance, in a study including 1144 German primary school
children, Urschitz etal.23 reported that especially OTK children aged > 9years compared with keyage children
showed poor academic performance in terms of grades in mathematics, science, reading, spelling, and hand-
writing. In a study including 3,684 Australian high school students aged 14years, Martin22 reported that YTK
children scored signicantly better in academic performance (i.e., performance in literacy and numeracy) than
keyage children. However, as already mentioned this has not yet been examined for physical tness. erefore,
the aim of this cross-sectional study was to compare physical tness of OTK and YTK children versus keyage chil-
dren in a sample of German primary school children taking age, sex, school, and assessment year into account.
With reference to the relevant school-based studies on dierences in academic performance of OTK and YTK
children versus keyage children21–23, we hypothesized that OTK children show poorer and YTK children better
physical tness performance compared with keyage children.
Methods
Experimental approach. is cross-sectional study is part of the ongoing EMOTIKON research project
(www. uni- potsd am. de/ en/ emoti kon). Physical tness tests were conducted every year between September and
November starting in 2011. Physical tness tests were also administered in 2009 and 2010, but later in the
school year that is between March and April. Due to the seasonal variation in physical tness these data were
not included.
Population. Since 2009, all third graders living in the Federal State of Brandenburg, Germany were tested
annually for their physical tness. is cross-sectional study was mandated and approved by the Ministry of Edu-
cation, Youth and Sport of the Federal State of Brandenburg, Germany. e Brandenburg School Law requires
that parents are comprehensively informed prior to the start of the study. Consent is not needed given that the
tests are obligatory for both, children and schools24. None of the authors included in the author list had access
to personally identiable information on the children. e authors received the data absolutely anonymized
from the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sport of the Federal State of Brandenburg, Germany. Research was
conducted according to the latest Declaration of Helsinki25.
To compare physical tness development of YTK and OTK children with that of keyage children, we used
physical tness data recorded between 2011and 2019.
• 2586 YTK children aged 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months
• 108,296 keyage children aged 8years and 0months to 8years and 11months
• 26,540 OTK children aged 9years and 0months to 9years and 11months
Selection into keyage, OTK, and YTK groups was strictly based on children’s birthdate relative to the legal
date for school enrollment (i.e., September 30th in the Federal State of Brandenburg for all assessment years).
us, on September 30th, keyage third graders ranged between 8years and 0months to 8years and 11months.
YTK children were younger, and OTK children were older.
e selection of keyage children has been described in a previous publication of our research group20. Data
from an earlier study were used as a reference for OTK and YTK children. Initially, 30,253 OTK children were
included in the data base: 2842 were excluded due to age. e excluded third-graders ranged from 10years and
1month to 14years and 5months. Another 27 students were excluded due to adverse health events as reported
by the responsible teacher (e.g., physical disability, autism spectrum). Finally, 844 students were considered
outliers and outside + /− 3 SD of their group x sex x test cell. Finally, 26,540 OTK children were included in
the analyses (88.7%). For YTK children, initially 2654 YTK were eligible to be included in the data base. From
this initial sample, 28 were excluded due to age because they ranged from 5years and 11months to 6years and
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
11months. Moreover, 40 children were considered outliers and beyond + /− 3 SD in their group × sex × test cell.
Finally, 2586 YTK children were included in the analyses (97.4%).
Physical tness tests. Physical tness was assessed using the specic EMOTIKON test battery20. ese
tests evaluated cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e., 6-min-run test), coordination (i.e., star-run test), speed (i.e.,
20-m linear sprint test), lower (powerLOW [i.e., standing long jump test]), and upper limbs muscle power (i.e.,
powerUP [ball-push test]). e EMOTIKON test battery ocially includes six tests. In 2016, the assessment of
exibility (i.e., stand-and-reach test) was stopped and the assessment of balance (i.e., single-leg balance test with
eyes closed) was included26. Due to the much smaller number of scores and their confound with assessment year,
these two tests were not included in the analyses.
Physical tness tests were administered by qualied physical education teachers and conducted during the
regular physical education classes. All physical education teachers received standardized test instructions for the
assessment (www. uni- potsd am. de/ en/ emoti kon/ proje kt/ metho dik—for further information on the test proto-
cols). Furthermore, all physical education teachers participated in advanced training programs about standard-
ized physical tness assessment. Tests were always conducted in the morning between 8 and 12 am. Prior to
testing, all third-graders performed a standardized warm-up program consisting of dierent running exercises
(e.g., side-steps) and small games (e.g., playing tag).
Cardiorespiratory endurance. Cardiorespiratory endurance was assessed using the 6-min-run test. Participat-
ing children had to run the furthest distance during the 6 min test time around a volleyball eld (54m) at a self-
paced velocity. e test instructor provided split times every minute. Aer the 6min, maximal distance covered
in meters to the nearest nine meters was recorded and used as dependent variable. High test–retest reliability
was reported for the 6-min-run test with an intraclass correlation coecient (ICC) of 0.92 in children aged 7–11
years27.
Coordination. Coordination under time pressure was evaluated using the star-run test. During the star-run
test, the participating children had to complete a parkour with dierent running techniques (i.e., running for-
wards, running backwards, side-steps) as quickly as possible. e star shaped parkour (9m × 9m) consisted
of four spikes. Each spike and the center of the star were marked with a pylon. e participants started in the
middle of the star. First, they had to run forward to the rst pylon and backward to the middle. Next, they had
to do side-steps to the second pylon on the right side and side-steps back to the middle. en, they had to run
backward to the third pylon and forward to the middle. Finally, they had to do side-steps to the fourth pylon on
the le side and side-steps back to the middle. e participants had to touch each pylon within the parkour with
the hand. e whole covered distance was 50.912m. Time for test completion in seconds to the nearest 1/10s
was taken using a stopwatch and used as dependent variable in the analysis. e participants had two test trials of
which the best test trial in terms of time until test completion was kept for analysis. e star run test was reliable
(test–retest) for children aged 8–10years with an ICC of 0.6828.
Speed. Speed was assessed using the 20-m linear sprint test. e participating children started from a standing
position with one foot right behind the starting line. Aer an acoustic signal, they had to sprint as fast as possible
over a distance of 20m. Time for test completion in seconds to the nearest 1/10s was taken using a stopwatch
and used as dependent variable in the analysis. e participants had two test trials of which the best trial was
taken for further analysis in terms of the time until test completion. Test–retest reliability has been reported to
be high for children aged 7–11years with an ICC of 0.9027.
Lower limbs muscle power (PowerLOW). PowerLOW was assessed through the standing long jump test. e
participating children had to jump as far as possible from a frontal position. Arm swing prior to and during the
jump was allowed. Jump distance in centimeters between the starting line and heel of the posterior foot was
recorded to the nearest one centimeter using a measuring tape. e participants had two test trials of which
the best trial in terms of the longest jump distance was taken for further analysis. e standing long jump test
showed high test–retest reliability for children aged 6–12years with an ICC of 0.9429.
Upper limbs muscle power (PowerUP). Power up was evaluated with the ball-push test. From a standing posi-
tion, the participating children had to push a 1kg medicine ball that was held tight right in front of the chest.
e participants had to push the ball at maximal eort with both hands. e pushing distance in meters was
recorded to the nearest ten centimeters using a measuring tape. e participants had two test trials of which the
best test trial in terms of the longest pushing distance was taken for further analysis. e ball-push test was reli-
able (test–retest) for children aged 8–10years with an ICC of 0.8128.
Statistics. Pre- and post-processing of data were carried out in the R environment of statistical computing30
using the tidyverse package31. For statistical inference we relied on Linear Mixed Model analyses (LMM) with the
MixedModels package32 in the Julia programming language (v 1.7.1)33.
For measures of cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e., 6min run test), powerLOW (i.e., standing long jump test)
and powerUP (i.e., ball push test), higher scores indicate better physical tness. For measures of coordination
(i.e., star run test) and speed (i.e., 20-m linear sprint test), a Box-Cox distributional analyses indicated that a
reciprocal transformation brought scores in line with the assumption of a normal distribution34. erefore, we
converted scores from seconds to meters/seconds (i.e., pace scores; star run test = 50.912 [m]/time [s]; 20-m linear
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
sprint test = 20 [m]/time [s]). ese transformations had the advantage that a large value was indicative of good
physical tness for all ve tests. Finally, z-scores were computed in two stages. In the rst stage, we calculated
z-scores within the test (i.e., 6-min-run test, star-run test, 20-m linear sprint test, standing long jump test, ball-
push test) × sex (male, female) × group (YTK, OTK) cells and removed observations exceeding + /− 3 SDs (i.e.,
outliers). is is in accordance with a previous publication from the same research group20. In the second stage,
we used means and SDs of the ve tness tests for keyage children from a previous study20 and computed the
respective z-scores that were included in Figs.1 and 2.
To compare YTK and OTK children`s development of physical tness with that of keyage children, we pre-
dicted the physical tness performance for ages 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months and 9years and
0months to 9years and 11months using LMM parameter estimates of the 108,295 keyage children (i.e., grey
lines in Fig.1), reported in Fühner etal20. rough this predication analyses we received the information about
physical tness performance of keyage children at the ages 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months and
9years and 0months and 9years and 11months. e model parameters comprised xed eects for age, tests,
sex, and their interactions, variance components (VCs) and correlation parameters (CPs) for GM and four test
contrasts for the random factor child, VCs and CPs for GM, four test contrasts, sex, and age for the random factor
school, and VCs for test and, age for the random factor assessment year. Details about model specication for
these predictions are provided in Fühner etal.20 and in script: fggk22_lmm_pred.jl in the repository.
rough physical tness testing in EMOTIKON, we obtained the actual physical tness status of YTK children
aged 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months and OTK children aged 9years and 0months to 9years and
11months. Please note that the classication of children into YTK, keyage, or OTK groups is based solely on
children’s birthdate whereas children’s age is the dierence between the date of test and their birthdate. erefore,
some YTK children were slightly older than 8years and some OTK children were slightly younger than 9years
at the time of testing. Results did not change if non-keyage children aged between 8 and 9years were excluded.
e dierence between observed (i.e., obtained through physical tness testing) and predicted z-scores (i.e.,
predicted data from keyage children [grey lines in Fig.1]) yielded a delta z-score that was used as dependent vari-
able in the following LMMs to compare physical tness development of YTK and OTK children (i.e., obtained
scores through physical tness testing) with that of keyage children (i.e., predicted data).
We analyzed the data with separate LMMs for OTK and YTK children. e xed eects included in the start-
ing LMM were similar to the one reported by Fühner etal.20. Specically, there were four sequential-dierence
xed-eect contrasts for the ve tests: (H1) coordination versus cardiorespiratory endurance, (H2) speed versus
coordination, (H3) powerLOW versus speed, and (H4) powerUP versus powerLOW. We additionally included the
eect of age (centered at 8years and 6months) as a second-order polynomial trend, the eect of sex (boys–girls),
and all interactions between contrasts, age, and sex. We used a two-sided z-value > 2.0 as signicance criterion
for the interpretation of xed eects.
e random eect structure included VCs and CPs of the delta z-scores for the ve tests related to grouping
(random) factors of child, school, and assessment year. Tests varied within children, schools, and assessment
years; age and sex varied between children, but within schools and within assessment years. erefore, in prin-
ciple, VCs and CPs also include eects of age and sex for the factors school and assessment year.
LMM for older-than-keyage (OTK) children. e initial LMM included child (N = 26,540), school (N = 513),
and assessment year (N = 9) as three random factors; the total number of observations (i.e., max = 5 per child)
was 128,198. With three random factors, there was a need for selecting a random-eect structure that included
theoretically relevant and reliable VCs and CPs but was also still supported by the data (i.e., was not overparam-
eterized).
Parsimonious model selection occurred in two major steps without knowledge or consideration of xed-eect
estimates35; details are provided in script: fggk22_lmm_otk.jl in the repository. e random-eect structure of
the parsimonious LMM of delta z-scores was expected to be simpler than the one for the LMM of Fühner etal.20
because the much smaller number of children and, importantly, because most of the school- and assessment-year-
related random eects as well as xed eect of age and sex were included in the predicted z-scores. We started
with a model estimating VCs and CPs between delta z-scores of the ve tests for children and VCs of delta z-scores
for the ve tests, age, and sex for school, and only varying intercept (GM) for assessment year. is LMM was
well supported by the data. Increasing the complexity of the random-eect structure by adding CPs for school
or adding VCs for assessment year did not improve the goodness of t. Moreover, the school-related VC for sex
and high-order xed-eect interactions between test, age, and sex could be removed without loss of goodness of
t. As in Fühner etal.20, we also estimated the nal model with an alternative post-hoc LMM parameterization
to test main xed eects of sex and age separately for each tness test (i.e., we specied sex and age as nested
within the ve levels of the factor test).
LMM for younger-than-keyage (YTK) children. e LMM included child (N = 2586), school (N = 437), and
assessment year (N = 9) as three random factors; the total number of observations (i.e., max = 5 per child) was
12,590. In the model selection process, we followed the model of OTK described above.
Parsimonious model selection occurred without knowledge or consideration of xed-eect estimates35; details
are provided in script: fggk22_lmm_ytk.jl in the repository. First, we applied the LMM of OTK to the data of
YTK. is model was not supported by the data (i.e., overparameterized) because of the relatively small sample
size of YTK (N = 2586) compared to OTK (N = 26,540). Indeed, the data supported only a LMM with a strongly
reduced complexity, comprising (a) xed eects on delta z-scores for the four contrasts of test, (b) VCs for the
ve delta z-scores for school and child, and (c) CPs for the ve delta z-scores of child. us, there was no statistical
support for xed or random eects of age and sex for YTK children relating to delta z-scores.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Results
Table1 summarizes descriptive statistics for the three subsamples of third-graders. Statistics about keyage chil-
dren refer to the sample reported in Fühner etal.20. Statistics about YTK and OTK children refer to the samples
of this study.
Figure1 displays the observed (points) and predicted (lines) physical tness development for YTK boys and
girls aged 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months and OTK boys and girls aged 9years and 0months
to 9years and 11months. e predicted z-scores for keyage children aged 8years and 0months to 8years and
11months are located on the predicted lines. ere is a slight overlap between groups at 8- and 9-year boundaries
due to birthdate determining the classication of children into keyage groups and age being measured as the
dierence between age at test and birthdate.
Figure2 displays the delta z-scores between observed and predicted physical tness development for YTK
boys and girls aged 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months and OTK boys and girls aged 9years and
0months to 9years and 11months. e delta z-scores for keyage children aged 8years and 0months to 8years
and 11months are represented in the horizontal zero line. e z-scores for OTK and YTK children will be
described in the next sections.
Physical tness of older-than-keyage children (OTK). Table2 displays statistics for xed eects of
age (linear and quadratic) and sex as well as their interactions with the four test contrasts for LMM of OTK
children.
e overall negative linear trend for age (z = − 6.68) and positive quadratic trend of age (z = 4.50) were signi-
cant. e positive quadratic trend of age indicates that the dierence between predicted and observed physical
tness becomes more negative initially, but plateaus with even a slight reduction of delta z-scores for the oldest
children (see Fig.2).
Furthermore, the main eect of contrast H1 was signicant (z = 2.51) indicating that the main eect was larger
for coordination than for cardiorespiratory endurance. e LMM tested the interactions of linear and quadratic
age with the four test contrasts, that is whether slopes in neighboring panels in Fig.2 (averaged across sex) were
parallel. e slope can be equated with the developmental rate. Indeed, one of four interaction was signicant
(see second and third block of Table2) the linear age developmental rate was larger for cardiorespiratory endur-
ance than coordination (H1; z = − 3.38) and the quadratic age developmental rate was larger for coordination
than cardiorespiratory endurance (H1; z = 2.73).
ree of the test contrasts interacted with sex. First, the delta z-score was more negative for boys than girls
for cardiorespiratory endurance and more negative for girls than boys for coordination (z = 3.41, see Table1).
e post-hoc LMM revealed signicantly less severe delta z-scores for girls (− 0.14) than boys (− 0.18) for cardi-
orespiratory endurance (z =− 2.30). ere was no signicant sex dierence for the delta z-score for coordination
Figure1. Observed z-scores for physical tness development for boys (closed circles) and girls (open circles)
aged 7.00–10.0years. e lines represent the predicted z-scores for physical tness development for boys (grey
line) and girls (dashed grey line). Data were z-transformed. Endurance = cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e.,
6-min-run test), Coordination = star-run test, Speed = 20-m linear sprint test, PowerLOW = lower limbs muscle
power (i.e., standing long jump test), PowerUP = upper limbs muscle power (i.e., ball-push test). Note that delta
z-scores for younger-than-keyage boys and girls were aggregated over 7.00–7.99years and that delta z-scores for
older-than-keyage boys and girls were aggregated over 9.50–9.99years. Points are binned observed child means.
Coordination and speed times were converted from seconds to meters/seconds (i.e., pace scores; star-run
test = 50.912 [m]/time [s]; 20-m linear sprint test = 20 [m]/time [s]). ese transformations have the advantage
that a large value is indicative of better physical tness and that they remove skew in the distributions.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
(z = 1.38). Second, the negative dierence between boys and girls in the delta z-score was larger for powerLOW
than speed (z = 3.38). e post-hoc LMM revealed a signicant sex dierence (favoring boys) only for power-
LOW (z = 3.90; boys: − 0.23, girls: − 0.29; see Table1). ere was no signicant sex dierence for speed (z = 1.19).
ird, the same powerLOW sex dierence was the source of the signicant interaction for the fourth contrast
(z = − 2.80). ere was no signicant sex dierence for powerUP (z = 1.12).
Table3 lists estimates of VCs for children and for school. e delta z-scores VCs were large for children
(0.88–0.94) and small for schools (0.06–0.09).
Physical tness of younger than keyage (YTK) children. Table4 displays estimates and test statis-
tics for xed eects of the four test contrasts. Figure2 displays the delta z-scores between observed and pre-
dicted physical tness development for YTK boys and girls aggregated over 7years and 0months to 7years and
11months.
e grand mean was signicant (z = 5.09). Furthermore, three of the four main eects of contrasts were
signicant: the main eect was larger for coordination than cardiorespiratory endurance (H1; z = 3.05), larger
for coordination than speed (H2; z = − 2.49) and larger for powerUP than powerLOW (H4; z = 2.47), which can
also be seen in Fig.2.
Table5 lists estimates of VCs between delta z-scores for children and for school. e delta z-scores VCs were
large for children (0.83–0.89) and small for schools (0.09–0.12).
Discussion
e aim of this cross-sectional study was to examine physical tness of YTK and OTK children versus keyage
children in a representative sample of German primary school children. Our ndings indicate that (1) OTK chil-
dren showed poorer performance compared to keyage children, especially for coordination, (2) OTK girls outper-
formed OTK boys, and (3) YTK children showed better results than keyage children, especially for coordination.
Several studies conrmed a linear increase in physical tness performance with chronological age9–11,15. For
instance, in a study with 424,328 Greek children and adolescents aged 6–18years, Tambalis etal.15 reported a
linear increase in physical tness performance with age for cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e., 20-m shuttle run
test), lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test), exibility (i.e., sit-and-reach test), muscular
strength (i.e., sit-ups test), and agility (i.e., 10 × 5m shuttle run test). e development of physical tness of
keyage children (see predicted gray lines in Fig.1) is in accordance with the above reported results. For keyage
children, physical tness performance increased linearly with age. However, the development of physical t-
ness for OTK children is dierent. Poor performance was found in OTK children aged 9years and 0months
to 9years and 11months compared with age-matched keyage children for all components of physical tness,
especially for coordination. is could be due to the fact that third graders aged 9years and 0months to 9years
Figure2. Delta z-score between observed and predicted physical tness development for boys (closed
circles) and girls (open circles) aged 7.00–10.0years. Data were z-transformed. Endurance = cardiorespiratory
endurance (i.e., 6-min-run test), Coordination = star-run test, Speed = 20-m linear sprint test,
PowerLOW = lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test), PowerUP = upper limbs muscle power
(i.e., ball-push test). Note that delta z-scores for younger-than-keyage boys and girls were aggregated over 7.00–
7.99years and that delta z-scores for older-than-keyage boys and girls were aggregated over 9.50–9.99years.
Points are binned delta child means. Coordination and speed times were converted from seconds to meters/
seconds (i.e., pace scores; star-run test = 50.912 [m]/time [s]; 20-m linear sprint test = 20 [m]/time [s]). ese
transformations have the advantage that a large value is indicative of better physical tness and that they remove
skew in the distributions.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
and 11months (i.e., OTK children) are not representative for the “average” age-matched keyage child which is
why we observed a deviation from the typically reported tness development with age in this cohort9–11,15. We
do not know the exact circumstances which lead to the delayed enrollment into rst grade or to the repetition
of a school year. According to our results, we can only speculate that maybe a delay in cognitive development
might be the reason why children are late enrolled into rst grade or must repeat a school class. ese results
are in line with a study of Urschitz etal.23 who examined dierences in academic performance. ese authors
observed that poor academic performance signicantly increased with age for mathematics, science, reading,
spelling, and handwriting in a sample of 1144 German third graders. Of note, children who repeated a school
class were more prone to poor academic performance. ese results were conrmed by other studies for academic
performance21,22. Interestingly, in our study OTK girls showed better performance compared to OTK boys which
is in accordance with Urschitz etal.23. ese authors reported that except for mathematics, boys showed a larger
prevalence for poor academic performance compared with girls23. As girls mature approximately two years earlier
than boys, the better performance of girls compared to boys might be inuenced by biological maturation. Girls
enter the adolescent growth spurt at approximately ten years of age and peak height velocity at 12years, whereas
boys enter the growth spurt on average at age 12 and peak height velocity at 1436.
In contrast, YTK children outperformed keyage children especially in tests requiring motor coordination.
Again, we do not know the exact circumstances which resulted in early enrollment into rst grade or reasons
for skipping a school year. According to our results, we speculate that accelerated cognitive development could
be a reason why early enrolled children skip a school year. is is supported by the fact that in this study, YTK
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for younger-than-keyage, keyage, older-than-keyage children. N = sample
size, SD = standard deviation, delta = dierence between observed (i.e., obtained through physical
tness testing) and predicted z-scores (i.e., predicted data from keyage children [grey lines in Fig.1]),
Endurance = cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e., 6-min-run test), Coordination = star-run test, Speed = 20-m
linear sprint test, PowerLOW = lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test), PowerUP = upper
limbs muscle power (i.e., ball-push test), OTK = older-than-keyage children (i.e., aggregated over 9years and
0months to 9years and 11months), YTK = younger-than-keyage children (i.e., aggregated over 7years and
0months to 7years and 11months). Coordination and speed times were converted from seconds to meters/
seconds (i.e., pace scores; star-run test = 50.912 [m]/time [s]; 20-m linear sprint test = 20 [m]/time [s]). ese
transformations have the advantage that a large value is indicative of better physical tness.
Sample Physical tness component Sex N schools N child Mean age [years] SD age [years] Mean s core SD score Mean delta SD delta
Keyage Endurance [m] Boys 513 51,116 8.56 0.28 1041.38 154.03 0 0.4
Keyage Endurance [m] Girls 511 52,821 8.55 0.28 967.72 132.50 0 0.3
Keyage Coordination [m/s] Boys 512 51,023 8.56 0.28 2.08 0.30 0 0.4
Keyage Coordination [m/s] Girls 510 52,886 8.55 0.28 2.01 0.27 0 0.3
Keyage Speed [m/s] Boys 513 51,700 8.56 0.28 4.58 0.42 0 0.4
Keyage Speed [m/s] Girls 512 53,259 8.55 0.28 4.45 0.39 0 0.4
Keyage PowerLOW [cm] Boys 513 52,141 8.56 0.28 129.41 19.53 0 0.4
Keyage PowerLOW [cm] Girls 509 53,856 8.55 0.28 122.00 18.44 0 0.4
Keyage PowerUP [m] Boys 514 52,254 8.56 0.28 3.99 0.70 0 0.4
Keyage PowerUP [m] Girls 512 54,070 8.55 0.28 3.50 0.63 0 0.3
OTK Endurance [m] Boys 511 14,870 9.35 0.25 1017.86 166.33 − 0.18 1.1
OTK Endurance [m] Girls 499 10,519 9.35 0.26 950.97 140.53 − 0.14 1.0
OTK Coordination [m/s] Boys 509 14,808 9.35 0.25 2.06 0.31 − 0.28 1.1
OTK Coordination [m/s] Girls 502 10,542 9.35 0.26 1.99 0.29 − 0.30 1.1
OTK Speed [m/s] Boys 511 15,010 9.36 0.25 4.58 0.44 − 0.17 1.1
OTK Speed [m/s] Girls 503 10,644 9.35 0.26 4.44 0.41 − 0.19 1.1
OTK PowerLOW [cm] Boys 511 15,137 9.35 0.26 127.83 21.08 − 0.23 1.2
OTK PowerLOW [cm] Girls 502 10,699 9.35 0.26 119.42 19.45 − 0.29 1.1
OTK PowerUP [m] Boys 511 15,236 9.36 0.25 4.13 0.75 − 0.22 1.1
OTK PowerUP [m] Girls 503 10,733 9.35 0.26 3.62 0.67 − 0.23 1.0
YTK Endurance [m] Boys 350 1087 7.85 0.19 1042.92 149.54 0.036 1.0
YTK Endurance [m] Girls 384 1408 7.88 0.18 973.11 132.88 0.035 1.0
YTK Coordination [m/s] Boys 350 1091 7.85 0.19 2.05 0.29 0.10 1.1
YTK Coordination [m/s] Girls 382 1397 7.87 0.18 1.99 0.26 0.10 1.0
YTK Speed [m/s] Boys 349 1097 7.85 0.19 4.51 0.40 0.035 1.1
YTK Speed [m/s] Girls 385 1423 7.87 0.18 4.42 0.39 0.070 1.0
YTK PowerLOW [cm] Boys 350 1112 7.85 0.19 128.54 18.48 0.082 1.1
YTK PowerLOW [cm] Girls 384 1433 7.87 0.18 121.77 18.06 0.078 1.0
YTK PowerUP [m] Boys 348 1111 7.85 0.19 3.79 0.70 0.12 1.0
YTK PowerUP [m] Girls 384 1431 7.88 0.18 3.33 0.61 0.14 0.9
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
children showed the best performance in the coordination test which has an inherent large cognitive demand.
Moreover, ndings from Martin22 point in a similar direction by showing that in a cohort of 3684 Australian
high school students, YTK children outperformed keyage children in academic performance.
Our study is not without limitations. First, anthropometric factors such as body mass, body height, and
sitting height were not assessed in this study so that associations between anthropometric factors, biological
maturation, and physical tness could not be calculated. ese factors would have provided additional insight
as there is strong evidence that children’s physical tness is associated with anthropometric characteristics37–39
and biological maturation36. One explanation of the deviation of YKT and OKT children might be a dierence
between chronological and biological age. It appears plausible to argue that YKT children may be more mature
and that OKT children are biologically somewhat younger than indicated by their chronological age. us, in a
hypothetical plot of performance over biological age, the linear trend may well hold for all children. Second, we
predicted the performance of the YTK and OTK children based on a linear extrapolation recently reported by
Fühner etal.20. However, we do not know if this linear extrapolation exactly ts to the data of keyage children
aged 7years and 0months to 7years and 11months/9years and 0months to 9years and 11months as we do
not have such longitudinal data. ird, we cannot parse out the exact number of OTK children that were late
enrolled or repeated a school class.
To sum up, this study is the rst study that examined dierences in physical tness development of YTK and
OTK children compared to keyage children. Our study ndings complement results reported in the literature
on the development of academic performance in youth21–23. Politicians and decision makers, schools, (physical
education) teachers, and parents should be aware that OTK versus keyage children showed poorer physical tness
performance. is is a novel and somehow unexpected result. erefore, OTK children should be specically
promoted through additional health and tness programs to compensate their decits in physical tness to enable
a holistic development. Furthermore, the assessment of physical tness should be performed regularly to tailor
the contents of physical education classes based on the results of physical tness assessments (e.g., data driven
physical education classes). More specically, the physical tness status of OTK children should be monitored
regularly over time to evaluate whether e.g., additional health and tness programs already helped to compensate
the observed decits in physical tness.
Given that reference values for the grading of physical tness is only available for keyage children, raw data
from this study can be used to calculate age-, sex-, and timing of school enrollment-specic percentile values.
Table 2. Fixed-eect estimates of linear mixed model for older-than-keyage (OTK) children. H1–
H4 = hypothesis 1–4, endurance = cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e., 6min run test), coordination = star run
test, speed = 20-m linear sprint test, powerLOW = lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test),
powerUP = upper limbs muscle power (i.e., ball push test), * = z-value > 2.0, linear mixed model random
factors: assessment years (9), schools (513), children (26,540), observations = 128,198 (missing = 3.4%). For
estimates of variance components and correlation parameters see Table3.
Source of variance Fixed-eect estimates Standard error z-values
Main eects
Grand mean (intercept) 0.348 0.068 5.08*
H1: coordination versus endurance 0.230 0.091 2.51*
H2: speed versus coordination − 0.025 0.083 − 0.30
H3: powerLOW versus speed 0.029 0.075 0.39
H4: powerUP versus powerLOW − 0.022 0.094 − 0.23
Age (linear) − 1.014 0.152 − 6.68*
Age (quadratic) 0.357 0.079 4.50*
Sex 0.015 0.011 1.43
Age (linear) × Fitness component
H1: coordination versus endurance − 0.693 0.205 − 3.38*
H2: speed versus coordination26 0.180 0.187 0.96
H3: powerLOW versus speed − 0.181 0.168 − 1.08
H4: powerUP versus powerLOW 0.089 0.211 0.42
Age (quadratic) × Fitness component
H1: coordination versus endurance 0.294 0.108 2.73*
H2: speed versus coordination − 0.034 0.098 − 0.34
H3: powerLOW versus speed 0.059 0.088 0.67
H4: powerUP versus powerLOW − 0.014 0.111 − 0.13
Sex × Fitness component
H1: coordination versus endurance 0.052 0.015 3.41*
H2: speed versus coordination − 0.001 0.014 − 0.10
H3: powerLOW versus speed 0.042 0.012 3.38*
H4: powerUP versus powerLOW − 0.044 0.016 − 2.80*
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
9
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
e respective data should be useful for (physical education) teachers or researchers to individually evaluate and
grade children´s physical tness development.
e EMOTIKON test battery is easy-to-administer, cost eective, and it requires only minimal equipment
that is usually available in gyms (e.g., stopwatch, measuring tape, medicine ball, pylons). erefore, physical
education teachers, coaches, or researchers can use the EMOTIKON test battery to evaluate children’s physical
tness and use the results to promote health- and skill-related physical tness during physical education.
Data availability
e datasets generated and analyzed during the current study as well as Julia and R scripts are available in the
Open Science Framework (OSF) repository: https:// osf. io/ dmu68/? view_ only= 240bd ab8f1 be4d8 384ac f9356
ee50f 8b.
Received: 29 November 2021; Accepted: 26 April 2022
References
1. Ortega, F. B., Ruiz, J. R., Castillo, M. J. & Sjöström, M. Physical tness in childhood and adolescence: A powerful marker of health.
Int. J. Obes. 32, 1–11 (2008).
2. C aspersen, C., Powell, K. & Christenson, G. Physical activity, exercise, and physical tness: Denitions and distinctions for health-
related research CARL. Notes Queries 100(2), 125–131 (1985).
3. Hussey, J., Bell, C., Bennett, K., O’Dwyer, J. & Gormley, J. Relationship between the intensity of physical activity, inactivity, cardi-
orespiratory tness and body composition in 7–10-year-old Dublin children. Br. J. Sports Med. 41, 311–316 (2007).
4. Mintjens, S. et al. Cardiorespiratory tness in childhood and adolescence aects future cardiovascular risk factors: A systematic
review of longitudinal studies. Sport. Med. 48, 2577–2605 (2018).
5. García-Hermoso, A., Ramírez-Campillo, R. & Izquierdo, M. Is muscular tness associated with future health benets in children
and adolescents? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Sport. Med. 49, 1079–1094 (2019).
Table 3. Variance components for older-than-keyage (OTK) children. Endurance = cardiorespiratory
endurance (i.e., 6min run test), Coordination = star run test, Speed = 20-m linear sprint test,
PowerLOW = lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test), PowerUP = upper limbs muscle power
(i.e., ball push test), OTK = older-than-keyage children. VC for Residual = 0.65.
Fitness component GM Endurance Coordination Speed PowerLOW PowerUP Age
Assessment year 0.03
School 0.08 0.07 0.07 0.06 0.07 0.09
Child (OTK) 0.92 0.94 0.94 0.98 0.88
Table 4. Fixed-eect estimates of the linear mixed model for younger-than-keyage (YTK) children. H1–
H4 = hypothesis 1–4, endurance = cardiorespiratory endurance (i.e., 6min run test), coordination = star run
test, speed = 20-m linear sprint test, powerLOW = lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test),
powerUP = upper limbs muscle power (i.e., ball push test), * = z-value > 2.0, linear mixed model random
factors: schools (437), children (2586), observations = 12,590 (missing = 2.6%). For estimates of variance
components and correlation parameters see Table5.
Source of variance Fixed-eect estimates Standard error z-values
Main eects
Grand mean (intercept) 0.082 0.016 5.09*
H1: coordination versus endurance 0.074 0.024 3.05*
H2: speed versus coordination − 0.055 0.022 − 2.49*
H3: powerLOW versus speed 0.026 0.021 1.27
H4: powerUP versus powerLOW 0.060 0.024 2.47*
Table 5. Variance components for younger-than-keyage (YTK) children. Endurance = cardiorespiratory
endurance (i.e., 6min run test), Coordination = star run test, Speed = 20-m linear sprint test,
PowerLOW = lower limbs muscle power (i.e., standing long jump test), PowerUP = upper limbs muscle power
(i.e., ball push test), YTK = younger-than-keyage (YTK) children. VC for Residual = 0.62.
Fitness component Endurance Coordination Speed PowerLOW PowerUP
School 0.12 0.10 0.09 0.10 0.10
Child (YTK) 0.83 0.88 0.89 0.89 0.79
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
10
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
6. Fühner, T., Kliegl, R., Arntz, F., Kriemler, S. & Granacher, U. An update on secular trends in physical tness of children and ado-
lescents from 1972 to 2015: A systematic review. Sport. Med. 51, 303–320 (2021).
7. Tomkinson, G. R., Lang, J. J. & Tremblay, M. S. Temporal trends in the cardiorespiratory tness of children and adolescents rep-
resenting 19 high-income and upper middle-income countries between 1981 and 2014. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 478–486 (2019).
8. Niessner, C. et al. Representative percentile curves of physical tness from early childhood to early adulthood: e MoMo study.
Front. Public Heal. 8, 1–9 (2020).
9. Tomkinson, G. R. et al. European normative values for physical tness in children and adolescents aged 9–17 years: Results from
2 779 165 Eurot performances representing 30 countries. Br. J. Sports Med. 52, 1445–1456 (2018).
10. De Miguel-Etayo, P. et al. Physical tness reference standards in European children: e IDEFICS study. Int. J. Obes. 38, S57–S66
(2014).
11. Santos, R . et al. Physical tness percentiles for Portuguese children and adolescents aged 10–18 years. J. Sports Sci. 32, 1510–1518
(2014).
12. Ortega, F. B. et al. Physical tness levels among European adolescents: e HELENA study. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, 20–29 (2011).
13. Catley, M. J. & Tomkinson, G. R. Normative health-related tness values for children: Analysis of 85347 test results on 9–17-year-
old Australians since 1985. Br. J. Sports Med. 47, 98–108 (2013).
14. de Roriz Oliveira, M. S., Seabra, A., Freitas, D., Eisenmann, J. C. & Maia, J. Physical tness percentile charts for children aged 6–10
from Portugal. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 54, 780–792 (2014).
15. Tambalis, K. D. et al. Physical tness normative values for 6–18-year-old Greek boys and girls, using the empirical distribution
and the lambda, mu, and sigma statistical method. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 16, 736–746 (2016).
16. Castro-Piñero, J. et al. Percentile values for running sprint eld tests in children ages 6–17 years: Inuence of weight status. Res.
Q. Exerc. Sport 81, 143–151 (2010).
17. Dobosz, J., Mayorga-Vega, D. & Viciana, J. Percentile values of physical tness levels among polish children aged 7–19 years: A
population-based study. Cent. Eur. J. Public Health 23, 340–351 (2015).
18. Cobley, S., Baker, J., Wattie, N. & McKenna, J. Annual age-grouping and athlete development: A meta-analytical review of relative
age eects in sport. Sport. Med. 39, 235–256 (2009).
19. Musch, J. & Grondin, S. Unequal competition as an impediment to personal development: A review of the relative age eect in
sport. De v. Rev. 21, 147–167 (2001).
20. Fühner, T., Granacher, U., Golle, K. & Kliegl, R. Age and sex eects in physical tness components of 108,295 third graders includ-
ing 515 primary schools and 9 cohorts. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–3 (2021).
21. Jaekel, J., Strauss, V. Y. C., Johnson, S., Gilmore, C. & Wolke, D. Delayed school entry and academic performance: A natural experi-
ment. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 57, 652–659 (2015).
22. Martin, A. J. Age appropriateness and motivation, engagement, and performance in high school: Eects of age within cohort, grade
retention, and delayed school entry. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 101–114 (2009).
23. Urschitz, M. S. et al. Snoring, intermittent hypoxia and academic performance in primary school children. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care
Med. 168, 464–468 (2003).
24. Ministerium für Bildung Jugend und Sport. Gesetz über die Schulen im Land Brandenburg. https:// bravo rs. brand enburg. de/ geset
ze/ bbgsc hulg (2018).
25. World Medical Association. Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research involving human Subjects. https://
www. wma. net/ polic ies- post/ wma- decla ration- of- helsi nki- ethic al- princ iples- for- medic al- resea rch- invol ving- human- subje cts/
(2013).
26. Nuzzo, J. L. e case for retiring exibility as a major component of physical tness. Sport. Med. 50, 853–870 (2020).
27. Bös, K. Deutscher Motorik-Test 6–18. (Czwalina, 2009).
28. Schulz, S. e Reliability of the Star Coordination Run and the 1-kg Medicine Ball Push: Physical Fitness Tests Used in the EMOTIKON-
Study (University of Potsdam, 2013).
29. Fernandez-Santos, J. R., Ruiz, J. R., Cohen, D. D., Gonzalez-Montesinos, J. L. & Castro-Pinero, J. Reliability and validity of tests to
assess lower-body muscular power in children. J. Strength Cond. Res. 29, 2277–2285 (2015).
30. Team, R. C. R a language and environment for statistical computing. https:// www.r- proje ct. org/ (2020).
31. Wickham, H. et al. Welcome to the Tidyverse. J. Open Source Sow. 4, 1686 (2019).
32. Bates, D. et al. JuliaStats/MixedModels.jl:v4.6.0. vol. 1 20071787–20071787 https:// doi. org/ 10. 5281/ zenodo. 58256 93 (2022).
33. Bezanson, J., Edelman, A., Karpinski, S. & Shah, V. B. Julia: A fresh approach to numerical computing. SIAM Rev. 59, 65–98 (2017).
34. Box, G. E. P. & Cox, D. R. An analysis of transformations. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 26, 211–243 (1964).
35. Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S. & Baayen, H. Parsimonious Mixed Models. ArXiv:1506.04967 http:// arxiv. org/ abs/ 1506. 04967
(2015).
36. Malina, R. M., Bouchard, C. & Bar-Or, O. Growth, Maturation, and Physical Activity (Human Kinetics, 2004).
37. Ceschia, A. et al. Deleterious eects of obesity on physical tness in pre-pubertal children. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 16, 271–278 (2016).
38. Sacchetti, R. et al. Physical tness of primary school children in relation to overweight prevalence and physical activity habits. J.
Sports Sci. 30, 633–640 (2012).
39. Deforche, B. et al. Physical tness and physical activity in obese and nonobese Flemish youth. Obes. Res. 11, 434–441 (2003).
Acknowledgements
e authors thank Paula Teich for helpful comments.
Author contributions
T.F., R.K., and U.G.: made substantial contributions to conception and design; K.G. and T.F.: contributed to data
collection; T.F. and R.K.: carried out data analysis; T.F., R.K., and U.G.: interpreted the data; T.F.: wrote the rst
dra of the manuscript and all authors were involved in revising it critically for important intellectual content;
all authors provide nal approval of the version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of
the work and agreed with the order of presentation of the authors.
Funding
Funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinscha (DFG, German Research Foundation) – Projektnummer
491466077.Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. e study was commissioned and
supported by the Ministry of Education, Youth, and Sport of the Federal State Brandenburg. e funders had no
role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Reinhold
Kliegl was supported by the Center for Interdisciplinary Research, Bielefeld (ZiF)/Cooperation Group “Statistical
models for psychological and linguistic data”.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
11
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:7801 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11710-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to T.F.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
© e Author(s) 2022
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Content uploaded by Thea Schlößer
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Thea Schlößer on May 12, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.