ArticlePDF Available

Species diversity enhances perceptions of urban coastlines at multiple scales

Authors:

Abstract

Biodiversity is increasingly understood as an important mediator of human aesthetic appreciation of scenes and landscapes, with implications for cultural services and well‐being. However, the generality of biodiversity effects across affective emotions, scales and habitats remains unclear. Urban coastal intertidal habitats on seawalls and other artificial structures are expanding worldwide. Despite growing calls to prioritise biodiversity in urban coastal planning and management, the potential co‐benefits determined by people's responses to biodiversity in these novel intertidal communities are unexplored. We investigated, using image‐based questionnaires, how several facets of biodiversity influence how people perceive urban coastal structures at both landscape and close‐up scales. Species richness strongly enhanced people's ratings of images for aesthetic appeal, interest and calming potential at both scales, but was more pronounced at the close‐up scale. Species evenness also increased ratings at the close‐up scale, while functional diversity (Rao's Q) was associated with a decline in aesthetic appeal and interest at the close‐up scale, indicating that people can disfavour scenes dominated by species with contrasting traits. Analysis of free‐text assessments showed that people strongly and positively valued scenes that were perceived to be ‘diverse’, a response that was much more common when viewing scenes with high species richness. The underlying structure type also clearly affected appraisals, with more obviously engineered structures being perceived to be less natural and thus less desirable. Our results show that biodiversity's effects on aesthetic appreciation extend to multiple affective emotions and to unfamiliar urban intertidal habitats, suggesting that managing these environments for biodiversity may simultaneously support aesthetic, educational and well‐being benefits. Nevertheless, the sensitivity of responses to the facet of biodiversity and viewing scale in our results underlines the context dependency and complexity of people's perceptions of urban environments. Read the free Plain Language Summary for this article on the Journal blog. Read the free Plain Language Summary for this article on the Journal blog.
People and Nature. 2022;00:1–18.
|
1wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/pan3
Received: 21 December 2021 
|
Accepted: 29 March 2022
DOI: 10.1002/pa n3.10 330
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Species diversity enhances perceptions of urban coastlines at
multiple scales
Tom P. Fairchild | Jasmine Weedon | John N. Griffin
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2022 The Authors. People and Nature published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Ecological Society.
Faculty of Science and Engineering,
Swansea University, Swansea, UK
Correspondence
Tom P. Fairchild
Email: t.fairchild@swansea.ac.uk
Funding information
European Regional Development Fund,
Grant/Award Number: WEFO- 80939
Ecostructure
Handling Editor: Martin Dallimer
Abstract
1. Biodiversity is increasingly understood as an important mediator of human
aesthetic appreciation of scenes and landscapes, with implications for cultural
services and well- being. However, the generality of biodiversity effects across
affective emotions, scales and habitats remains unclear.
2. Urban coastal intertidal habitats on seawalls and other artificial structures are
expanding worldwide. Despite growing calls to prioritise biodiversity in urban
coastal planning and management, the potential co- benefits determined by
people's responses to biodiversity in these novel intertidal communities are
unexplored.
3. We investigated, using image- based questionnaires, how several facets of bio-
diversity influence how people perceive urban coastal structures at both land-
scape and close- up scales.
4. Species richness strongly enhanced people's ratings of images for aesthetic ap-
peal, interest and calming potential at both scales, but was more pronounced at
the close- up scale. Species evenness also increased ratings at the close- up scale,
while functional diversity (Rao's Q) was associated with a decline in aesthetic
appeal and interest at the close- up scale, indicating that people can disfavour
scenes dominated by species with contrasting traits.
5. Analysis of free- text assessments showed that people strongly and positively
valued scenes that were perceived to be ‘diverse’, a response that was much
more common when viewing scenes with high species richness. The underlying
structure type also clearly affected appraisals, with more obviously engineered
structures being perceived to be less natural and thus less desirable.
6. Our results show that biodiversity's effects on aesthetic appreciation extend
to multiple affective emotions and to unfamiliar urban intertidal habitats, sug-
gesting that managing these environments for biodiversity may simultaneously
support aesthetic, educational and well- being benefits. Nevertheless, the sen-
sitivity of responses to the facet of biodiversity and viewing scale in our results
underlines the context dependency and complexity of people's perceptions of
urban environments.
2 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
1 |  INTRODUC TION
Humans are driving significant changes in biodiversity across eco-
logical communities globally (Chaudhary et al., 2021; Dornelas
et al., 20 14; Newbold et al., 2015), and understanding how bio-
diversity change influences ecosystem services remains a press-
ing ongoing research challenge (Balvanera et al., 2016; Gamfeldt
et al., 2013; Isbell et al., 2017). Almost three decades of research
have demonstrated that biodiversity of ecological communities
profoundly influences provisioning and regulating services such as
wild food production and carbon storage (Cardinale et al., 2012;
MEA, 2005), but growing evidence also suggests that diverse nat-
ural systems are crucial in providing valuable aesthetic and well-
being benefits (Bell et al., 2015; Ulrich, 1983; White et al., 2013).
Increasingly, researchers are beginning to investigate how biodi-
versity simultaneously influences the flow of cultural services such
as aesthetic inspiration, opportunities for tourism and recreation,
and education (Chan et al., 2012; Hoyle et al., 2017; Lindemann-
Matthies et al., 2010).
There is emerging evidence that biodiversity influences how
people perceive and respond to nature. A long history of research
in environmental psychology shows that exposure to nature can
elicit positive aspects of perception such as aesthetic pleasure
(Petrova et al., 2015; Ulrich, 1983), interest (Fairchild et al., 2018)
and a sense of calm (Dallimer et al., 2012; Fuller et al., 2007; Houlden
et al., 2018), which are key in driving inspiration, shaping positive
attitudes and enhancing psychological restoration and well- being
(Silvia, 2012; Thornhill, 2003; Ulrich, 1983). Consequently, land-
scapes that are aesthetically appealing, subjectively interesting or
providing a sense of calm are important motivators for humans to
visit and interact with natural ecosystems, while also providing sat-
isfying and engaging nature experiences (Kim et al., 2015; Pasanen
et al., 2018). Yet, rather than simply exposure to nature, it is likely
that more biodiverse and naturalistic scenes or landscapes provide
greater aesthetic appeal (Gunnarsson et al., 2017; Hoyle et al., 2017;
Palliwoda et al., 2017), and enhance well- being benefits by providing
calming spaces for relaxation and rejuvenation (Fisher et al., 2021;
Hoyle et al., 2017; Wood, Harsant, et al., 2018; Wood, Jones,
et al., 2018). However, previous studies have often taken a broad
comparative or correlational approach, with more diverse settlings
covarying with other factors, such as organism abundance, habitat
type and landscape setting, leaving the specific role of biodiversity—
and its multiple facets— poorly understood.
Indeed, it is important to consider that multiple facets of bio-
diversity may have different influences on aesthetic appeal and
other affective emotions. Most simply, increasing richness (num-
ber) of species in a community can enhance visual complexity
(Forsythe et al., 2011; Kaplan et al., 1972; Petrova et al., 2015) and
the chances that species with favourable characteristics are in-
cluded (Ulrich, 1983), intersecting strongly with empirical findings
from the psychology of aesthetics where complexity and desirable
traits can enhance aesthetic appeal (de Pinho et al., 2014; Palmer
et al., 2013; Petrova et al., 2015). Species- rich communities are also
generally perceived to be more diverse, which may lead to positive
aesthetic appraisals of communities through intrinsic existence val-
ues (Dallimer et al., 2012; Fisher et al., 2021; Schebella et al., 2019),
particularly as public awareness of the importance of biodiversity in
maintaining healthy ecosystems has increased over the past decade
(Hynes et al., 2021; Shwartz et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2020). Species
richness is, however, a coarse measure of community structure; vi-
sual complexity and diversity may be more discernible where spe-
cies abundances have higher evenness (Graves et al., 2017; Tribot
et al., 2019), or where species have more distinctive traits (char-
acteristics; e.g. colour or shape) and therefore encompass greater
functional diversity (variety of different characteristics; Fairchild
et al., 2018; Tribot et al., 2016, 2019).
Moreover, different affective emotions can be expected to vary
in responses to biodiversity. For example, increasing visual complex-
ity (Fry et al., 2009; Silvia, 2006; Sun & Firestone, 2021) and the
inclusion of a variety of species with novel, unusual traits (e.g. high
functional diversity) may elicit fascination or interest in a commu-
nity by providing unfamiliar stimuli (Fairchild et al., 2018; Kashdan &
Silvia, 2009), ultimately triggering fascination (Ballantyne et al., 2007;
Krapp, 1999; Renninger et al., 2014) and facilitating learning expe-
riences (Silvia, 2001, 2006, 2008). Conversely, restorative bene-
fits may be enhanced in communities with high species evenness
which may provide more visually coherent scenes (Fry et al., 2009;
Kang & Kim, 2019; Zhang et al., 2013), or alternatively may be in-
creased through increasing perceived health, ecological quality or
naturalness of habitats (Fisher et al., 2021; Marselle et al., 2016;
Wood, Harsant, et al., 2018; Wood, Jones, et al., 2018), which
can be driven through higher species diversity (Hoyle et al., 2019;
Purcell & Lamb, 1998; Winter, 2012). Collectively, these properties
can provide calming spaces which in turn promote self- reflection
and mental restoration (Kaplan, 2001; Pálsdóttir et al., 2018), re-
duce stress (de Kort et al., 20 06; Pálsdóttir et al., 2018; Van Den
Berg & Custers, 2011) and ultimately enhance mental and phys-
ical well- being (Garrett et al., 2019; Houlden et al., 2018; Koss &
King sley, 2010; Schwind et al., 2 017).
People also interact with natural environments at multiple scales,
from viewing a landscape while walking or travelling, to closely ex-
ploring an environment to find organisms of interest. Although
yet to be addressed, biodiversity effects on different aspects of
human– nature perceptions are likely to be highly scale dependent.
For instance, the role of abundant or conspicuous species and their
differences may be most important when viewed from afar, with
KEY WORDS
aesthetics, biodiversity, coastal structures, cultural ecosystem services, interest, seawalls,
well- being
   
|
 3
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
the total richness of organisms more discernible at close- up viewing
scales.
The role of biodiverse, naturalistic spaces for providing aes-
thetic and well- being benefits is particularly important in urbanised
areas, where opportunities to encounter nature are often limited.
Here, important nature interaction experiences for residents centre
around urban green and blue spaces, such as gardens, parks, lakes
and, increasingly, coastal spaces (Chiesura, 2004; Mills et al., 2019;
Palliwoda et al., 2017). But coastal spaces in particular, which are
increasingly becoming recognised for their potential well- being ben-
efits (Bell et al., 2015; Bell et al., 2020; Wheeler et al., 2012), are
undergoing rapid and sustained change as coastal populations con-
tinue to grow (Barragán & de Andrés, 2015; Neumann et al., 2015),
and the subsequent proliferation of urban coastal structures places
them in greater intersection with local communities and tourists
(Evans et al., 2019). This growth of fringing coastal infrastructure
and hard engineering can substantially alter the visual properties
of coastal vistas at both the landscape scale and more intimate
close- up scales (Burak et al., 2004; Morgan, 1999b) through changes
in shoreline structural complexity (Lawrence et al., 2021), with these
simplified environments typically hosting depauperate ecological
communities (Bulleri & Chapman, 2004; McKinney, 2006; Momota
& Hosokawa, 2021). This has led to increasing calls to develop
ecologically sensitive designs or management scenarios to retro-
fit coastal infrastructure, embracing varied naturalistic features to
provide suitable habitat to support natural, biodiverse ecological
communities (Evans et al., 2019; Firth, Schofield, et al., 2014; Firth
et al., 2016). Indeed, features, such as rockpools (Evans et al., 2016;
Hall et al., 2019) and surface roughness (Loke & Todd, 2016; Matias
et al., 2 010; Zawada et al., 2010) associated with older and eroded
structures— or via ecological retrofitting— may markedly increase
biodiversity along urban coastlines. In turn, the enhanced biodi-
versity may provide greater opportunities to explore and interact
with organisms at local scales (Martens, 2016; Morgan, 1999a),
together with creating more natural coastal landscape aesthetics
(Cordell et al., 2017 ). But despite the potential biodiversity benefits
provided by ecologically sensitive coastal management, there is a
lack of knowledge as to how local communities and visitors— whose
buy- in may be crucial in determining the successful implementation
of conservation or enhancement schemes— might perceive more
biodiverse urban coastal structures.
In urban coastal environments, the role of biodiversity in provid-
ing aesthetic and well- being experiences remains untested. Previous
anecdotal or implicit evidence from aesthetic appraisal studies on
artificial coastal structures— centred on how natural or congruent
they are with natural shorelines— are often contradictory (e.g. Bell
et al., 2020; Koutrakis et al., 2011; Morgan, 1999b), suggesting that
traditional positive diversity perceptions may not apply universally
across to coastal urban environments where clean architectural
lines may be sometimes preferred (Bosman et al., 2016; Koutrakis
et al., 2011). For other aspects of perception, such as interest and the
ability to provide calming spaces, experimental evidence from coastal
systems on biodiversity– benefit relationships is notably lacking, yet
is becoming particularly pressing as we begin to understand the
importance of natural blue systems for multiple cultural and well-
being functions (Bell et al., 2020; Evans et al., 2019). Furthermore,
there is limited evidence to suggest that many dominant intertidal
species, notably seaweeds, can be negatively perceived by resi-
dents and visitors at coastal locations, providing hazards to access
and egress, being associated with unfavourable aesthetics, smells or
touch sensations (Limburg et al., 2010; MacLeod et al., 2002; Merkel
et al., 2021). These potential negative perceptions of ecologically
important, characteristic species may undermine any positive bio-
diversity effects— coupled with an overall lack of understanding of
coastal biodiversity– perceptions relationships— leaving considerable
uncertainty around the aesthetics and well- being benefits created
by diversity in coastal ecosystems.
Here, we address the role of biodiversity in mediating human
perceptions of artificial coastal structures. We assessed the role
that species and functional diversity play in driving aesthetic appeal,
human interest and creating relaxing or rejuvenating environments
using an image- based questionnaire. We used a combination of
image choice, rating scales and free- text motivation tools to under-
stand the effect of biodiversity on different facets of well- being, and
to explore the underlying motivations. Furthermore, we examine
whether the scale which people view or interact with these environ-
ments— at both intimate, close- up scales associated with exploration
of nature (such as rockpooling), and at whole structure scales where
passive viewing typically occurs— may alter biodiversity– public per-
ception relationships.
2 | METHODS
2.1  |  Participant selection
Participants between the ages of 18 and 60+ were recruited
through online social media channels and promotions which linked
to the survey between 15th May and 28th September 2020. The
survey was targeted at UK and Ireland residents as part of the
Ireland- Wales ECOSTRUCTURE project, and entries submitted
from participants outside of the geographical range were disre-
garded to reduce the influence of regional variation in attitudes.
Pa rticip ant s were in for med tha t th e pu r p ose of the experim ent wa s
to ‘understanding attitudes towards coastal defence structures
and the animals and seaweed that grow on them’, but the specific
aims were not explicitly stated to prevent bias. Participants were
asked whether they consente d to being included in the study using
question- routed checkboxes (‘I consent to be part of the study’,
or ‘I DO NOT consent to be part of the study’— which routed to
the exit page) before they progressed, and informed that they
were free to withdraw, without penalty, at any time using a with-
draw (‘Exit’) button at the top of the navigation page. The survey
took approximately 15 min to complete. This study was granted
ethical approval by the Swansea University Ethics Committee
(SU- Ethics- Staff- 260,520/246).
4 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
2.2  |  Experimental design
For our study, we selected three distinctive yet complimentary
aspects of perception: aesthetic appeal, interest, and providing a
sense of calm. These aspects of perception have been identified as
important determinants of the overall motivation to visit or interact
with— and subsequent visitor satisfaction towards— natural environ-
ments (Dorwart et al., 2009; Kim et al., 2015; Pasanen et al., 2018).
To elucidate how components of biodiversity influence these dif-
ferent indicators of cultural services (aesthetic appeal, interest and
calming potential), we created an online study on Survey Monkey
(see example questionnaire section in the supplementary materials)
using images composited from photos of three different types of ar-
tificial coastal structure types and 10 species of intertidal organisms
which are common throughout the north- east Atlantic (Hawkins
et al., 2019; Table S1). The study was conducted at two focal scales:
a whole- structure scale (~c. 30 m × 5 m) and a close- up scale (~c.
1 × 1 m). Within the images, we directly manipulated species richness
(6 levels; 0, 1, 2, 4, 6, 8 species) and indirectly manipulated species
evenness (Pilou's evenness) and functional diversity (here meas-
ured as Rao's Q B o t t a - D u k á t , 2005) through varying the commu-
nity compositions within each richness level. Species richness was
hypothesised to increase appeal, interest and sense of calm as we
expected that naturalness and complexity, which can be enhanced
by having a greater number of species, may be important in driv-
ing these perceptions. Functional diversity was included as we hy-
pothesised that greater trait differences would lead to more visually
obvious differences between species, and thus increase the ability
of participants to discriminate between species. The traits used to
generate the functional diversity measure were selected to broadly
capture differences in visual properties— such as size or colourand
are presented in Table S2.
To assess aesthetic appeal, we used multiple different question
types to understand preference order and self- reported appeal
(Table S3). First, each participant was presented with 15 paired
image preference questions with two randomised images, side- by-
side, and asked to select which image they found most appealing (or
if they found each image to be equally as appealing). Paired image
preference approaches have been found to capture preference re-
liably and repeatably (Clark et al., 2018; Sankaran et al., 2021) and
are less susceptible to directionality biases or central tendencies
than other rating scales. As aesthetic appeal was not expected to
be the only cultural service indicator affected by diversity, we also
asked participants Likert- type questions on additional self- reported
perceptual components of how interesting (‘I find the structure in-
teresting to look at’) and calming (‘I find the structure relaxing and
calming to look at’) participants found each of the image, which have
been, respectively, seen to capture interest and the motivation to
learn (Fairchild et al., 2018; Moss & Esson, 2010; Turpie, 2003), as
well as providing relaxation and stress recovery (Christopher, 2019;
de Kort et al., 2006; Moran, 2019; Van Den Berg & Custers, 2 011).
Additionally, a Likert- type question on aesthetic appeal (‘I find the
structure appealing to look at’) to facilitate direct comparisons
between the different components and allow for the testing of de-
mographic effects on appeal. Participants were provided with 10
single images and rated aesthetic appeal, interest and calming using
a 5- point scale, including a central ‘neutral’ option (e.g. Strongly
Agree (5), Agree (4), Neutral (3), Disagree (2), Strongly Disagree (1);
Table S3). Due to layout restrictions in the Survey Monkey platform,
these three Likert- type questions were presented as a random order
question matrix. To assess any influence of acquiescence bias, we
validated results against a subset of participants with inversely
worded statements and ranked ELO results, finding no evidence of
systematic bias from positive question wording (see ‘Assessing ac-
quiescence and directionality biases from Likert- type scale data’ in
the Supplementary Materials for more information).
We also asked demographic questions (age, gender, educa-
tional/professional background, ocean connectedness (after Nuojua
et al., 2021, in review and Schultz, 2002) to understand whether
demographic differences modified relationships. Participants were
given the opportunity to not disclose their answer (‘prefer not to
say’) for each question. Further details are shown in Table S4.
Finally, to understand the motivations, participants were asked
to rate aesthetics using a Likert- type object and provide free- text
comments on what they liked and disliked about three randomised
images (‘Thinking about this structure, what motivated you to find
the structure unappealing or appealing? Please write, In your own
words, in the box below’). These questions were presented after the
demographic questions and were optional to complete.
2.3  |  Image composition
We created composite images using photographs of different urban
coastal structure types: stepped concrete walls, rip- rap (or ‘rock
armour’) and an old heritage stone seawall (circa 1800s), and con-
tained 10 common and ubiquitous north- east Atlantic intertidal spe-
cies, comprising of six seaweed species, and one species each from
the groups of barnacles, anemones, limpets and mussels (Hawkins
et al., 2019; Table S1). While structure and species selections were
not exhaustive and may not fully represent the diversity present
across the country, they were chosen to represent the continuum
of complexity in UK coastal artificial habitats and communities.
Composites were created for both whole structure (c. 30 m × 5 m)
and close- up (c. 1 × 1 m) focal scales and contained either 0, 1, 2, 4,
6 or 8 species. At the structure scale, we simulated randomly se-
lected species combinations, across three different structure types,
giving a total of 120 unique images. At the close- up focal scale, the
number of structure types was reduced to two, as the riprap and old
wall sections looked too similar, giving 85 unique images. Images of
artificial structures were carefully manipulated to remove any exist-
ing seaweed or animal cover. Multiple photographs for each of the
10 intertidal species were taken across the south Wales coastline
(UK) on different substrate types, and for different orientations and
presentations. Both the structures and organisms were then digitally
extracted from the images using the scissor lasso tool in GIMP 2.9.1.,
   
|
 5
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
appropriately scaled, and layered to create images which varied in
their community composition and diversity (Figure 1). Proportions
and zonation of species were selected to realistically represent
naturally occurring communities. Care was taken to ensure no large
systematic differences in community coverage were present across
the different diversity treatments or structure types, and the per-
centage of occupancy of the structure was calculated to control for
residual variation. Structures were then resized to ensure the same
total coverage in the images. Differences in the visual properties
of wider viewsheds can affect overall aesthetic appraisal of scenes
(Morgan, 1999b; Ulrich, 1983), so here, to focus on the role of dif-
ferent facets of biodiversity and to avoid biases due to differences
in the landscape setting in which the structures were located, com-
posite structures were displayed over a separate coastal background
image which was held constant across structure types.
2.4  |  Analysis
2.4.1  |  Structural and image properties
The community structure within each image was quantified by ex-
amining percentage cover on the structure for each species in each
image using the Vidana image analysis tool (Hedley, 2013). From
the species- level data, we also calculated substrate occupancy by
summing visual cover to calculate the proportion of the structure
that was occupied by seaweeds or animals. Additionally, we quan-
tified the perceptual colourfulness of the image— which has previ-
ously been observed to influence aesthetic appraisals (Fairchild
et al., 2018; Tribot et al., 2 016)— using a colourfulness algorithm de-
veloped by Hasler and Suesstrunk (2003) within the Matlab R2017A
analysis software.
2.4.2  |  Statistical analysis and visualisation
Analyses of data were performed in r statistical computing soft-
ware 3.6.3 (R Core Team, 2013). To understand how diversity
components and structural properties affected aesthetic appeal,
we created aesthetic scores from image pair win/loss/draw data
using the ELO algorithm package (Elo, 1978; Heinzen, 2017; fol-
lowing Tribot et al., 2016) to rank images. To verify that image
choices were not random, we compared the distribution of the
observed ELO aesthetic scores to randomly recompiled win/loss
dummy data using a Kolmogorov– Smirnov test. Species richness
and Pielou's evenness were calculated using the veg an package in
r (Oksanen et al., 2017) and functional diversity (Rao's Q, Botta-
Dukát, 2005) was calculated using the fd package (Laliberté &
FIGURE 1 Examples of images of structures (a– c) and close- ups (d– f) used in the survey. Images a– c include variation in the underlying
structures, from stepped walls (a) to rip- rap walls (b) and heritage stone walls (c). Images d– e show example close- up images with different
underlying structures of rip- rap walls (d and e) and stepped walls (f)
6 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
Legendre, 2010). Rao's Q was selected as the sole measure of
functional diversity in the study as it has been found to perform
well for describing average community trait dissimilarity (Botta-
Dukát, 2005), which we expected would be a crucial factor for
people to discriminate between species. Rao's Q can also be calcu-
lated for communities containing ≥2 functionally distinctive spe-
cies, and thus could be calculated for the majority of communities
contained in our survey.
To analyse the roles of biodiversity facets (species richness,
species evenness, functional diversity), structure type and over-
all image properties (colourfulness, substrate occupancy), we
used two complimentary approaches. For the paired image pref-
erence data, we used log- linear regression mixed effects models
for analysing ELO aesthetic score relationships, with the indi-
vidual respondent as a random factor to account for different
baseline appreciation or interest in coastal communities, and in-
teractions which were expected to be important (e.g. the role of
species diversity across different coastal structure types) were
explicitly included within models. Cumulative Link Mixed Models
(CLMM) with a logit- link function— via the o rdi nal package in r
(Christensen, 2018)— were used for Likert aesthetic, interest
and calming potential relationships, again with respondent as a
random factor, and interactions included between key, interact-
ing predictors. The individual- based modelling approaches with
CLMMs also included participant demographics as predictors
to understand the generality or specificality of relationships.
Although every care was taken to capture representative survey
populations, we also statistically controlled for demographic ef-
fects by including demographic data in our CLMM models, but
some residual effects or uncaptured demographic properties,
may still slightly influence ratings. Resulting model partial ef-
fects, after controlling for other variables, were then visualised
using the plotting packages visreg (Breheny & Burchett, 2016)
and ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016). We also examined whether spe-
cies identity effects altered aesthetic appraisals using log- linear
mixed effects models (using the ‘lmer’ function in lme4 r pa ckage,
Bates et al., 2015) at both focal scales— using abundances for
the 10 species— along with the covariates of species richness and
structure type as a random factor to control for diversity and
structure differences.
2.4.3  |  Textual analysis
As well as the numerical collection of data, optional free- text ques-
tions were included to explore the motivations of participants to
find structures appealing or unappealing. As free- text responses
can feature different words or phrases by participants to describe
the same thing, we identified motivational themes, that is, words
corresponding to broad themes within the free- text answers. For
instance, participants interchangeably used the words or phrases
‘unnatural’, ‘not natural’ and ‘artificial’ to describe the aesthetics
of the structures, which were subsequently collapsed to the com-
mon theme of ‘Artificial’, as the use of the words has the same broad
meaning. These themes were assessed by manually reading through
responses and categorising individual observations by participants,
adding new themes where observations did not fit into an existing
category. These identified themes were then visualised using a word
cloud, with the size of text scaled from the natural logarithm of the
number of responses in each category using Adobe Illustrator (CC
2019).
3 | RESULTS
3.1  |  Ecological communities
A to t al of 937 people fro m the UK and Ire lan d complet ed the im age
preference sections of the sur vey (for ELO analysis), and 754 com-
pleted the image preference including demographics (CLMM anal-
ysis). Participants responded strongly to species richness at both
whole structure and close- up viewing scales, finding more spe-
cies rich and even communities on the presented seawalls to be
more app ealing (Figure 2a,b), but also surprising ly found that mo re
functionally diverse communities were less appealing (Figure 2c,f,
Tables 1 and 2, Tables S5 and S6), interesting (Tables S7 and S8),
and calming (Tables S9 and S10). The strength of these relation-
ships was stronger in close- up images than at the whole structure
scale. We found that the colourfulness of scenes positively influ-
enced aesthetic appeal, and calming potential at close- up viewing
scale s (Tables S6 and S10) but had no apparent effect at the whole
structure scale (Tables S5 and S9). We also found limited evidence
suggesting that human preferences were driven by the compo-
sition of species, finding only a weak negative effect of mussels
(Mytilus) and anemones (Actinia) at the structure- scale (Table S11),
and weak positive effect of a red alga (Palmaria) at closer viewing
distances (Table S12). As well as the effects of the ecological com-
munity there were strong preferences for the underlying structure
type. The less regular old stone wall and riprap structures were
far more appealing than the more regular concrete stepped wall
(Table 1; Figure S1).
Participants also generally found diverse structures more in-
teresting and calming than those with fewer, and less even, species
(Figure 3; Table 3). Interest was particularly strongly related to spe-
cies richness, and positive effects on interest accumulated faster
than either aesthetic appeal or sense of calm. Structure type had
a strong influence, with the more obviously artificial stepped con-
crete walls being found as less appealing, interesting and calming
compared to the old wall and riprap structures (Table 3). As with
aesthetic appeal, the positive relationships between diversity and
interest/calming were more apparent at closer viewing distances
than at the whole structure scale (Figure 3d,e,f). At the close- up
scale, we also observed negative effects of functional diversity for
interest (Table S8).
   
|
 7
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
3.2  |  Participant motivations
The previous analysis showed that participants generally preferred
biologically diverse communities with less regular underlying struc-
tures. We next conducted an analysis of free- text data to better
understand what drove people's preferences for images of the pre-
sented artificial structures. In all, 500 people answered optional
free- text questions, and generated 1333 valid text responses. We
found 24 positive and 22 negative motivational themes for finding
structures aesthetically appealing or unappealing. These included
the positive attributes of ‘natural’, ‘diverse’, ‘colourful’, ‘exploration’
and ‘interesting structure’, as well as negative aspects such as ‘in-
accessibility’, seaweed- related associations (‘slimy’, ‘dirty’ and the
presence of ‘green’ algae), perceptually ‘hard’ structures, as well as
the lack of diversity in ‘depauperate’ (low diversity) communities.
Of these motivations, naturalness and diversity were the two most
mentioned positive and negative drivers of appeal by far (Figure 4;
Table S13), and were strongly related to reported aesthetic appeal
FIGURE 2 Species diversity, but not functional diversity, effects drive aesthetic preferences on artificial structures, and strengthen
with decreasing viewing scale. The effect of species richness (left), evenness (Centre) and functional diversity (right) on aesthetic appeal
scores at both the structure (a– c) and close- up scales (d– f). Centre lines on panel a and d represent the median, inner boxes represent 25th–
75th percentile and whiskers represent the maximum and minimum values. Points and lines on panels b, c, e and f represent model partial
residuals and model fits, respectively. Asterisks represent significance at α = 0.05 (*), 0.01 (**) and <0.001 (***), and ‘ns’ represents non-
significance
Estimate SE p- value
Intercept 6.350 0.563 <0.001***
Species richness 0.068 0.013 <0.001***
Species evenness 0.036 0.022 0.102
Functional diversity (Rao Q)−0.305 0.170 0.076
rip- rap structure −0.037 0.044 0.393
Stepped concrete structure −0.084 0.033 0.012*
Occupancy 0.029 0.082 0.728
Species richness: rip- rap structure 0.014 0.017 0.385
Species richness: stepped concrete
structure
−0.010 0.016 0.547
TAB LE 1  Species richness and
structure type influence aesthetic appeal
at the whole structure scale. Regression
model summary table examining the
role of biodiversity, structure and image
properties in driving aesthetic appeal
scores (ELO). Asterisks for p- values
represent significant results at α = <0.05
(*) and <0.001 (***)
8 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
scores (Figure 5a,b). These motivations appeared to largely explain
people's preferences of structure type and species richness. We
found strong effects of structure type on mentions of naturalness
vs. artificialness, with greater mentions of naturalness associated
with the old wall and riprap shores, particularly as these shore types
were perceived as appealing or strongly appealing. Species rich-
ness was only very weakly associated with mentions of naturalness
(Figure 5c). On the other hand, positive mentions of diversity were
associated with positive appeal rating irrespective of structure type
(Figure 5b), while species richness was clearly associated with in-
creased mentions of diversity. Thus, the influence of structure type
appeared to be largely determined by a perception of naturalness,
while the influence of species richness was determined by a positive
perception of diversity.
3.3  |  Demographic effects
Of the 937 people that answered the image questions in the sur-
vey, 754 people completed the demographics section. The analysis
of demographic effects is presented above for the whole structure
scale (Table 3), and full results table are provided in Tables S5, S7,
S9 and S6, S8, S10 for structure and close up scales, respectively.
Participants identifying as ‘female’ were overrepresented in the
survey (71%; Table 4), but other demographic groups were well
represented. While every care was taken to recruit a wide cross-
section of the public, people working in biological, environmental
or maritime roles were proportionally overrepresented compared
to the UK and Irish general population (35% of sample). Within
our sample population, we observed that demographic differences
Estimate SE p- value
Intercept 7.1 0 9 0.121 <0.001***
Species richness 0.068 0.019 <0.001***
Species evenness 0.079 0.034 0.023*
Functional diversity (Rao Q)−1.113 0.385 0.076
Stepped concrete structure −0.154 0.034 0.393
Occupancy 0.213 0 .157 0.728
Species richness: stepped concrete
structure
0.047 0.020 0.385
TAB LE 2  Different facets of
biodiversity and structure type influence
aesthetic appeal at close- up viewing
scales. Regression model summary
table examining the role of biodiversity,
structure and image properties in driving
aesthetic appeal scores. Asterisks for p-
values represent significant results at α =
<0.05 (*) and <0.001 (***)
FIGURE 3 Species richness positively influences aesthetic appeal (left), interest (Centre) and sense of calm (right) at the structure scale
(a– c), and increases in strength at close- up (d– f) viewing scales. The percentage of respondents from 5- Likert agreement categories (strongly
disagree– strongly agree)
   
|
 9
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
between participants led to slightly different participant aesthetic
appeal (Tables S5, S7 and S9). Participant age had a slightly negative
effect on aesthetic, interest and sense of calm appraisals, with older
participants more likely to award lower scores, whereas increases
in self- reported ‘ocean connectedness’ led to participants awarding
slightly higher aesthetic appeal scores, but did not influence interest
and calming scores. For aesthetic appeal, participants that reported
high levels of ocean connectedness also responded more strongly
and positively to species richness of communities than those repor t-
ing lower ocean connectedness. However, participants working in
environmental or biology jobs were slightly more likely to award
lower aesthetic appeal and sense of calm scores than those who
did not work in an environmental industry. At the close- up scale,
however, we found demographic effects did not influence any of the
perception metrics (Tables S6, S8 and S10), and gender identity had
no effect at either scale.
4 | DISCUSSION
Despite growing interest in how biodiversity affects cultural ser-
vices, there have been few experimental tests of the role biodi-
versity's facets play in determining people's affective emotional
responses to ecological communities. Furthermore, whether
biodiversity elicits positive perceptions in more peripheral or
unfamiliar habitats remains uncertain. Our experiment test, set
across globally proliferating coastal urban environments, shows
that biodiversity affects multiple affective emotions, but that
these influences depend on both the biodiversity facet and view-
ing scale. Specifically, species richness and evenness increased
aesthetic, interest and calming qualities, while functional diver-
sity reduced these responses, and all influences of biodiversity
were stronger at the small— close- up— scale. Alongside biodiver-
sity, less uniform and more complex structures were preferred.
Estimate SE p- value
Aesthetic appeal (n = 3765)
Species richness 0.893685 0.185244 <0.001***
Stepped concrete wall (structure) −1. 3 4 3 21 0.286337 <0.001***
Participant age 0.35316 0.12 2385 <0.001***
Works in industry −0. 24605 0.102839 0.0163*
Ocean connectedness 1.38 458 0.694684 0.0462*
Species richness: ocean connectedness −1.12187 0.435947 0.0100**
Interest (n = 3770)
Species richness 0.716235 0.195321 <0.001***
Stepped concrete wall (structure) −2.306 54 0.300249 <0.001***
Participant age 0.35187 0.138959 0.0113*
Species richness: stepped concrete wall 0.325097 0.159688 0.0417*
Sense of calm (n = 3770)
Species richness 0.490491 0.182809 0.0070**
Stepped concrete wall (structure) −1.61119 0.290972 <0.001***
Participant age −0.2915 0.134378 0.0300*
Works in industry −0.3003 0.11293 0.0078**
TABLE 3 Species richness enhances
aesthetic appeal, interest and sense
of calm provided by coastal scenes at
the structure scale, whereas artificial
stepped concrete structures reduced
benefits. Model summaries for significant
predictors of appeal, interest and
calm from the Likert CLMM models,
including demographic effects from 754
participants. Full model summary tables
can be found in Tables S5– S10). Asterisks
for p- values represent significant results
at α = <0.05 (*), <0.01 (**) and <0.001
(***)
FIGURE 4 Perceived naturalness and diversity were the most important drivers of aesthetic appeal of artificial structures. The word
clouds visualise the number of mentions of different motivations for finding a structure appealing (panel a, blue hues) or unappealing (panel
b, red hues). Letter size indicates the number of mentions. Underlying data are presented in Table S12
10 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
Finally, our text analysis suggests that species diversity has
positive effects by increasing perceived ‘diversity’, while irregu-
lar underlying structures illicit positive responses by appearing
more ‘natural’. Collectively, these results imply that managing
and designing urban coastal environments to encourage more
biologically diverse and naturalistic environments, particularly
with irregular structures that support more species, will serve to
enhance the flow of cultural services.
How biodiversity of coastal structures is perceived by peo-
ple was unknown and we suspected that the presence of dis-
liked or unfamiliar species might generate weaker or even
negative relationships between diversity and aesthetic appeal,
FIGURE 5 Positive mentions of the important motivators of ‘naturalness’ and ‘diversity’ increase in line with appeal, while positive
mentions of ‘diversity’ increased with the species richness of communities. The top two panels represent the proportion of respondents
mentioning (a) naturalness/artificialness or (b) diversity/lack of diversity as their motivations for finding scenes aesthetically appealing or
unappealing. The bottom two panels represent the role of species richness in driving (c) perceived naturalness and (d) perceived diversity
reported by participants
   
|
  11
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
if not interest. However, despite negative associations with sea-
weed species by participants— and previous evidence of negative
seaweed associations (Limburg et al., 2010; MacLeod et al., 2002;
Morgan, 1999b)— we found a strong positive effect of species
diversity on the aesthetic appeal of common coastal structures,
which persisted regardless of demographic background. The pres-
ence of this diversity– aesthetic relationship suggests that diver-
sity effects can override potentially negative identity effects. The
diversity– aesthetic relationship also appeared to be driven by
speciose structures being accurately perceived as more diverse—
even at the structure scale where species differences were hard
to discern— with perceived diversity found to be a key driver of
aesthetic appeal from our textual analysis. These findings are in
line with previous work in terrestrial systems (Dallimer et al., 2012;
Hoyle et al., 2017; Southon et al., 2018), where perceived diversity
and naturalness have been seen to drive appeal and engagement
(Dodds et al., 2010; Turpie, 2003).
The positive effects of richness and evenness also extended to
other affective emotions of interest and calming potential, which
can mediate educational (Kashdan & Silvia, 2009; Krapp, 1999;
Renninger et al., 2014) and well- being (Cracknell et al., 2 016; Fisher
et al., 2021; White et al., 2 017) cultural ecosystem service benefits.
Self- reported sense of calm increased similarly to aesthetic appeal in
our study despite a exhibiting a slightly lower baseline calming po-
tential than was observed for appeal. Previous studies have indicated
that restorative calming properties of scenes or landscapes may be
strongly linked to aesthetic appeal, as the same properties such as
naturalness and ecological quality are thought to be important in re-
ducing stress induced by the psycho- evolutionary appraisal of habi-
tat suitability, favouring perceptually ‘healthy’ ecosystems (Bratman
et al., 2019; Kaplan, 1987; Orians & Heerwagen, 1992). Importantly,
species diversity can substantially contribute to human appraisals
of the perceptual naturalness and ecological health of landscapes
(Barker & Fisher, 2019; Rapport et al., 1998; Tribot et al., 2018) and
as such may promote a sense of calm and relaxation in people, ulti-
mately reducing stress (de Kort et al., 2006; Fritz et al., 2 010; Van
Den Berg & Custers, 2011) and providing mental and physical well-
being benefits (Gu et al., 2015; Pálsdóttir et al., 2018).
Interest, in particular, increased strongly with species richness
and accumulated more quickly with diversity than aesthetic appeal
or sense of calm. This rapid accumulation of interest with increasing
diversity was likely driven through a combination of increasing visual
diversity, complexity and increasing probability of including novel
species— mechanisms which have been previously demonstrated to
drive interest (Kashdan & Silvia, 2009; Krapp, 1999; Silvia, 20 08)—
and an increase in participant reported desire to explore the ‘un-
seen’ diversity potential which can be offered by seaweed canopies
(Martens, 2016). These diversity– interest relationships are ex-
pected to provide strong motivations for exploration and facilitate
learning experiences (Ballantyne et al., 2007; Fairchild et al., 2018;
Simmons, 1998), ultimately providing educational opportuni-
ties (Kashdan & Silvia, 2009; Krapp, 1999; Renninger et al., 2014).
Furthermore, this strong diversity– interest relationship, and the ap-
parent decoupling of the magnitude of diversity– interest relation-
ships from calming potential and the better understood aspect of
aesthetic appeal (Cupchik & Gebotys, 1990; Kashdan & Silvia, 2009;
Turner & Silvia, 2006), suggests that diversity independently drives
multiple aspects of perception— and therefore supporting cultural
and well- being services— through different mechanisms.
How biodiversity influences the functions and values of ecosys-
tems has been a highly active area of research in ecology (Balvanera
et al., 2016; Gamfeldt & Roger, 2017; Lefcheck et al., 2015), with
theory suggesting these relationships can be strongly scale de-
pendent (Isbell et al., 2017; Pasari et al., 2013). Our work shows
that scale dependence also extends to diversity– aesthetics and
diversity– interest relationships. Indeed, we found that viewing scale
determined the impact of several facets of biodiversity on aesthetic,
and interest benefits. Previous studies have tended to focus on
TAB LE 4  Demographic background summaries for the
participants for the online study who completed the demographic
questions
Category
Number of
participants
Gender identity
Female 537
Male 214
Other gender identity 3
Age
18– 30 166
31– 45 278
46– 60 221
61+89
Frequency of coastal visits
I have never been to the coast 1
I rarely visit the coast 153
I visit the coast at least once a month 201
I visit the coast at least once a week 129
I live on the coast 270
Work in environmental related job
No 457
Yes, for a port authority or in port operations 8
Yes, in a regulatory environmental role 50
Yes, in biological sciences 214
Yes, in civil engineering 25
Colour- blindness
Yes 10
No 744
Ocean connectedness (see Table S3)
No connection 58
Some connection 295
Strong connection 290
‘As one’ 106
12 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
broader, feature or landscape scale, effects of biodiversity (Dallimer
et al., 2012; Lindemann- Matthies et al., 2010; Tribot et al., 2019),
but close- up scales are also relevant because coastal structures are
frequently used intimately, providing a setting for exploration and
learning opportunities through close examination of the rocks and
ecological communities which can inhabit them (Martin et al., 2006).
Our study found a marked strengthening of species richness and
evenness effects at the close- up scale, which was likely explained
through differences between species and communities (e.g. identity,
colour, texture and shape, dominance)— and the desirable property
of ‘diversity’ underlined in the textual analysis— being more percep-
tible at close- up viewing scales.
While species diversity strongly contributed to multiple as-
pects of perception at both the landscape scale and close- up
scale, we observed more complex relationships with functional
diversity. We found no evidence that functional diversity (mea-
sured as Rao's Q) substantially affected aesthetic appraisal, in-
terest or calming potential at the whole structure level, but we
observed stronger, negative effects at close- up viewing scales.
This suggests that the inclusion of organisms with the extreme
trait differences may be undesirable at close viewing scales where
species (and trait) differences can be more readily resolved, ei-
ther through species with contrasting extreme traits being disliked
(Ulrich, 1983), or by exceeding some threshold of visual complex-
ity, which may lead to poorer aesthetic appraisals of structures
(Forsythe et al., 2011). Combined with recent findings from coral
reef fish assemblages that functional diversity metrics can have
weak, or hump- shaped, relationships with aesthetic preference
and be outweighed in importance by species richness (Tribot
et al., 2019), this result suggests that— unlike ecosystem func-
tions such as primary productivity, and related provisioning and
regulating services, which are often strongly explained by func-
tional diversity (Cadotte et al., 2011; Gross et al., 2017; Mensah
et al., 2020)— functional diversity and cultural services related to
aesthetics may be somewhat decoupled or even negatively asso-
ciated and may trade- off against different functions and services.
Overall, our results underline that different facets of biodiversity
can have different impacts on people's preferences and at differ-
ent focal scales, although more work is required to fully untangle
the causes and generality of these differences, including across
other systems and habitats.
Furthermore, the application of these scale- dependent rela-
tionships to other systems will depend on the spatial structure of
communities. In our model communities, species were well- mixed
spatially, such that close- up images contained a large proportion of
the overall species pool. However, in some systems and contexts,
systems can be patchy with different patches dominated by differ-
ent species such that small- scale (close- up) diversity can be much
lower than wider— landscape— diversity. In such cases, aesthetic ap-
preciation and even interest may be greater at the larger landscape
scale. Future work should explore the interaction between spatial
community organisation and scales of diversity (i.e. alpha, beta and
gamma) and human perceptions and well- being.
While more diverse structures were seen to be more natural, di-
verse and subsequently appealing, the structural characteristics of
underlying coastal structures were highly important for aesthetic
appraisals of appeal. More obviously engineered structures, such as
the concrete stepped walls, were far less preferred than those which
were perceived to be more natural. The less defined edges and facets
of the riprap and heritage stone walls led to greater perceived natu-
ralness and perceptions of diversity than for similar communities on
the hard- edged, regular stepped concrete wall structure, with many
participants no ting the app are nt ‘hardness’ of the st ructure as an un-
desirable trait which influenced their overall aesthetic appraisal. This
desire for more natural or ‘organic’ shapes is a well- understood aes-
thetic driver of building architecture (Coburn et al., 2019; Vartanian
et al., 2013), shifting baseline aesthetic appreciation, interest and
calming potential of structures— underlining the multiple ecological
and anthropogenic motivators for aesthetic appraisal, and the par-
ticular importance of structural properties which needs to be better
integrated into future natural aesthetics studies.
The strong relationships between species diversity and aesthet-
ics, interest and calming potential demonstrate the importance of
human– nature interactions that healthy, diverse ecosystems can
provide. This further strengthens growing arguments for the need
to better integrate cultural ecosystem services into the discourse
around the value of nature— which has tended to be neglected (Chan
et al., 2012; Daniel et al., 2012; Martin et al., 2016)— into coastal
management and planning activities. At the broad scale, our findings
suggest that managing coastal structures to promote diverse com-
munities is likely to positively contribute towards well- being com-
mitments set out in international and national goals and legislation,
such as the U.N. Sustainable Development Goals (United Nations
General Assembly, 2015; Wood, Harsant, et al., 2018; Wood, Jones,
et al., 2018) or the Wellbeing of Future Generations Act 2015
(Wales), as well as directly benefiting local communities. Given the
importance of aesthetics in driving attitudes and generating local
support for changes to— or creation of— artificial coastal structures,
we found strong evidence that images of biodiverse seawalls are
more desirable to communities than highly engineered, low diversity
depauperate structures, and found that these biodiversity benefits
were realised regardless of demographic background. However, it
is important to note that other aspects of nature experiences may
not be captured by visual senses alone (e.g. sound, smell or touch),
with growing evidence suggesting that aesthetic experiences and
well- being benefits can be further enhanced through non- visual
stimuli driven by increased biodiversity (Austen et al., 2021; Fisher
et al., 2021; Franco et al., 2017). This suggests that while the posi-
tive biodiversity– perceptions relationships presented here are likely
to be good indicators of human benefits, our value estimates from
image- based elicitation surveys associated with diverse coastal
communities are likely to be conservative (Fisher et al., 2021; Franco
et al., 2017; Schebella et al., 2019). Future studies, including con-
trolled scenes as well as in- situ surveys, are important to address this
uncertainty. Our results also suggest that increasing engagement
with coastal areas may also generate greater biodiversity— well- being
   
|
  13
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
benefits, with the feeling of connectedness to the sea enhancing
biodiversity– appeal relationships. Along with evidence that bio-
diverse communities can support a wide range of supporting and
regulating services which sustain resilient and healthy ecosystems
(Balvanera et al., 2016; Barbier et al., 2011; Cardinale et al., 2012),
our findings support calls to explicitly include ecologically sensitive
design into coastal defence projects, either through retrofitting
existing infrastructure to improve biodiversity outcomes (Evans
et al., 2016; O'Shaughnessy et al., 2020), or through including
biodiversity- enhancing features on new or replacement structures
(Firth, Thompson, et al., 2014; Perkins et al., 2015) to enhance well-
being ecosystem service provisions.
5 | CONCLUSION
In this study, we provide experimental evidence that multiple as-
pects of perception— which are important mediators of a wealth
of educational, aesthetic and well- being ecosystem services— are
generally enhanced by specific facets of biodiversity in urbanised
coastal settings. While limited evidence of biodiversity enhance-
ment of individual aesthetic and well- being benefits exists for ma-
rine systems, we show how despite negative preconceptions of
intertidal species, more diverse artificial coastal habitats can still
provide attractive, interesting and restorative spaces for people.
This simultaneous facilitation of the perceptions of interest, aes-
thetic appeal and sense of calm in this study, coupled with pre-
vious evidence of provisioning and regulating services, suggests
that biodiversity plays a crucial role in providing multi- functional
coastal urban spaces which maximise human benefits at multiple
scales of use. As such, management interventions that enhance
diversity on urban coastal structures may help offset some of
the negative ecological and cultural costs associated with coastal
urbanisation. However, the unexpected effects of functional di-
versity and evidence of scale dependence of biodiversity– benefit
relationships highlight those mechanisms driving often- neglected
cultural and well- being benefits need to be explored more fully to
maximise the impact of sustainable biodiversity management and
enhancement efforts.
5.1  |  Study limitations
A further discussion on the limitations of the methodologies
and study system is found in the ‘Limitations’ section in the
Supplementary Materials.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors would like to thank the Ecostructure project staff for
providing feedback on the survey design. This research was funded
as part of the Ecostructure project (part- funded by the European
Regional Development Fund (ERDF) through the Ireland- Wales
Cooperation Programme 2014- 2015).
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
AUTHORS' CONTRIBUTIONS
T.P.F. and J.N.G. conceptually conceived and developed the study
and contributed equally to the writing of the manuscript; T.P.F. and
J.W. created the composite images, online survey templates and led
participant recruitment; T.P.F. led the analysis of survey results and
was supported by J.N.G. and J.W.
DATA AVA ILAB ILITY STATE MEN T
Anonymised summary data used in the analysis of this experi-
ment are archived on Figshare at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figsh
are.195510 37
ORCID
Tom P. Fairchild https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7133-8824
John N. Griffin https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3295-6480
REFERENCES
Austen, G. E., Dallimer, M., Irvine, K. N., Maund, P. R., Fish, R. D., &
Davies, Z. G. (2021). Exploring shared public perspectives on bio-
diversity attributes. People and Nature, 3(4), 901913. https://d o i .
org/10.1002/pan3.10237
Ballantyne, R., Packer, J., Hughes, K., & Dierking, L. (2007).
Conservation learning in wildlife tourism settings: Lessons
from research in zoos and aquariums. Environmental Education
Research, 13(3), 367– 383.
Balvanera, P., Quijas, S., Martin- Lopez, B., Barrios, E., Dee, L., Isbell, F.,
Durance, I., White, P., Blanchard, R., & Groot, R. S. d. (2016). The
links between biodiversity and ecosystem services. In Routledge
handbook of ecosystem services (1 ed., pp. 45– 61). Routledge.
h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 4 3 2 4 / 9 7 8 1 3 1 5 7 7 5 3 0 2 - 5
Barbier, E. B., Hacker, S. D., Kennedy, C., Koch, E. W., Stier, A. C., &
Silliman, B. R. (2011). The value of estuarine and coastal ecosys-
tem services. Ecological Monographs, 81(2), 169193. ht t p s : //d o i .
org/10.1890/10- 1510.1
Barker, T., & Fisher, J. (2019). Ecosystem health as the basis for human
health. In Water and sanitation- related diseases and the changing en-
vironment: Challenges, interventions, and preventive measures (2nd
ed., pp. 244– 254). John Wiley & Sons.
Barragán, J. M., & de Andrés, M. (2015). Analysis and trends of the
world's coastal cities and agglomerations. Ocean & Coastal
Management, 114, 11– 20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oceco
aman.2015.06.004
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear
mixed- effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Sof tware,
67(1), 1– 48. https://doi.org/10.18637/ jss.v067.i01
Bell, S. L., Phoenix, C., Lovell, R., & Wheeler, B. W. (2015). Seeking ev-
eryday wellbeing: The coast as a therapeutic landscape. Social
Science & Medicine, 142 , 56– 67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socsc
imed.2015.08.011
Bell, S., Mishra, H. S., Elliott, L. R., Shellock, R., Vassiljev, P., Porter,
M., Sydenham, Z., & White, M. P. (2020). Urban blue acupunc-
ture: A protocol for evaluating a complex landscape design in-
tervention to improve health and wellbeing in a coastal commu-
nity. Sustainability, 12(10), 4084. https://doi.org/10.3390/su121
04084
Bosman, C., Dedekorkut- Howes, A., & Leach, A. (2016). Off the plan: The
urbanisation of the G old Coas t. Csiro Publishing.
14 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
Botta- Dukát, Z. (2005). Rao's quadratic entropy as a measure of func-
tional diversity based on multiple traits. Journal o f Vegetation Scienc e,
16(5), 533– 540. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1654- 1103. 2005.tb023
93.x
Bratman, G. N., Anderson, C. B., Berman, M. G., Cochran, B., de Vries, S.,
Flanders, J., Folke, C., Frumkin, H., Gross, J. J., Hartig, T., Kahn, P.
H., Kuo, M., Lawler, J. J., Levin, P. S., Lindahl, T., Meyer- Lindenberg,
A., Mitchell, R., Ouyang, Z., Roe, J., Daily, G. C. (2019). Nature
and mental health: An ecosystem service perspective. Science
Advances, 5(7), eaax0903. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aax0903
Breheny, P., & Burchett, W. (2016). visreg: Visualization of regression mod-
els. Retrieved from h t t p s : / / C R A N . R - p r o j e c t . o r g / p a c k a g e =visreg
Bulleri, F., & Chapman, M. G. (2004). Intertidal assemblages on artificial
and natural habitats in marinas on the north- west coast of Italy.
Marine Biology, 145 (2), 381– 391. https://doi.org/10.1007/s0022
7 - 0 0 4 - 1 3 1 6 - 8
Burak, S., Dogˇan, E., & Gaziogˇlu, C. (2004). Impact of urbanization and
tourism on coastal environment. Ocean & Coastal Management,
47(9), 515– 527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oceco aman.2004.07.007
Cadotte, M. W., Carscadden, K., & Mirotchnick, N. (2011). Beyond spe-
cies: Functional diversity and the maintenance of ecological pro-
cesses and services. Journal of Applied Ecology, 48(5), 1079– 1087.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365- 2664.2011.02048.x
Cardinale, B. J., Emmett Duffy, J., Gonzalez, A., Hooper, D. U., Perrings,
C., Venail, P., Narwani, A., Mace, G. M., Tilman, D., Wardle, D. A.,
Kinzig, A. P., Daily, G. C., Loreau, M., Grace, J. B., Larigauderie, A.,
Srivastava, D. S., & Naeem, S. (2012). Biodiversity loss and its im-
pact on humanity. Nature, 486(7401), 59– 67.
Chan, K. M. A., Satterfield, T., & Goldstein, J. (2012). Rethinking eco-
system services to better address and navigate cultural values.
Ecological Economics, 74 , 8– 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecole
con .2 011.11.0 11
Chaudhary, C., Richardson, A. J., Schoeman, D. S., & Costello, M. J.
(2021). Global warming is causing a more pronounced dip in marine
species richness around the equator. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United State s of America, 118(15). h t t p s : //
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.20150 94118
Chiesura, A. (2004). The role of urban parks for the sustainable city.
Landscape and Urban Planning, 68(1), 129– 138. h t t p s : //d o i .
org/10.1016/j.landu rbplan.2003.08.003
Christensen, R. H. B. (2018). Cumulative link models for ordinal regres-
sion with the R package ordinal. Journal of Statistical Software.
Submitted. Retrieved from http://cran.uni- muens ter.de/web/packa
ges/ordin al/vigne ttes/clm_artic le.pdf
Christopher, K. (2019). Personalised Prediction of self- reported emotion re-
sponses to music stimuli. Retrieved from http://resea rchar chive.vuw.
ac.nz/handl e/10063/ 8091
Clark, A. P., Howard, K. L., Woods, A. T., Penton- Voak, I. S., & Neumann, C.
(2018). Why rate when you could compare? Using the “EloChoice”
package to assess pairwise comparisons of perceived physical
strength. PLoS One, 13(1), e0190393. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journ al.pone.0190393
Coburn, A., Kardan, O., Kotabe, H., Steinberg, J., Hout, M. C., Robbins,
A., MacDonald, J., Hayn- Leichsenring, G., & Berman, M. G. (2019).
Psychological responses to natural patterns in architecture.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 62, 133– 145. htt p s://d o i .
org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.02.007
Cordell, J. R., Toft, J. D., Munsch, S. H., & Goff, M. (2017). Benches,
beaches, and bumps: How habitat monitoring and experimental
science can inform urban seawall design. In Living shorelines (pp.
421– 438). CRC Press. https://doi.org/10.1201/97813 15151465
Cracknell, D., White, M. P., Pahl, S., Nichols, W. J., & Depledge, M. H.
(2016). Marine biota and psychological well- being: A preliminary
examination of dose– response effects in an aquarium setting.
Environment and Behavior, 48(10), 1242– 1269.
Cupchik, G. C., & Gebotys, R. J. (1990). Interest and pleasure as dimen-
sions of aesthetic response. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 8(1), 1– 14.
h t t p s : // d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 2 1 9 0 / L 7 8 9 - T P P Y - B D 2 Q - T 7 T W
Dallimer, M., Irvine, K. N., Skinner, A. M., Davies, Z. G., Rouquette, J. R.,
Maltby, L. L., & Gaston, K. J. (2012). Biodiversity and the feel- good
factor: Understanding associations between self- reported human
well- being and species richness. Bioscience, 62(1), 47– 55.
Daniel, T. C., Muhar, A., Arnberger, A., Aznar, O., Boyd, J. W., Chan, K. M.,
& Grêt- Regamey, A. (2012). Contributions of cultural services to the
ecosystem services agenda. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America, 109 (23), 8812– 8819.
de Kort, Y. A. W., Meijnders, A. L., Sponselee, A. A. G., & IJsselsteijn, W.
A. (2006). What's wrong with virtual trees? Restoring from stress in
a mediated environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 26(4),
3 0 9 – 3 2 0 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.09.001
de Pinho, J. R., Grilo, C., Boone, R. B., Galvin, K. A., & Snodgrass, J. G.
(2014). Influence of aesthetic appreciation of wildlife species on at-
titudes towards their conservation in Kenyan agropastoralist com-
munities. PLoS One, 9, 2.
Dodds, R., Graci, S. R., & Holmes, M. (2010). Does the tourist care? A
comparison of tourists in Koh phi phi, Thailand and Gili Trawangan,
Indonesia. Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 18(2), 207222. ht t p s : //
doi.org/10.1080/09669 58090 3215162
Dornelas, M., Gotelli, N. J., McGill, B., Shimadzu, H., Moyes, F., Sievers,
C., & Magurran, A. E. (2014). Assemblage time series reveal bio-
diversity change but not systematic loss. Science, 344(6181), 296–
299. https://doi.org/10.1126/scien ce.1248484
Dorwart, C. E., Moore, R. L., & Leung, Y.- F. (20 09). Visitors' perceptions
of a trail environment and effects on experiences: A model for
nature- based recreation experiences. Leisure Sciences, 32(1), 33–
54. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490 40090 3430863
Elo, A. E. (1978). The rating of Chessplayers. Past and Present.
Evans, A. J., Firth, L. B., Hawkins, S. J., Hall, A. E., Ironside, J. E., Thompson,
R. C., & Moore, P. J. (2019). From ocean sprawl to blue- green in-
frastructure– a UK perspective on an issue of global significance.
Environmental Science & Policy, 91, 60– 69.
Evans, A. J., Firth, L. B., Hawkins, S. J., Morris, E. S., Goudge, H., & Moore,
P. J. (2016). Drill- cored tide pools: An effective method of ecolog-
ical enhancement on artificial structures. Marine and Freshwater
Research, 67(1), 123– 130.
Fairchild, T. P., Fowler, M. S., Pahl, S., & Griffin, J. N. (2018). Multiple
dimensions of biodiversity drive human interest in tide pool com-
munities. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 15234. https://doi.org/10.1038/
s 4 1 5 9 8 - 0 1 8 - 3 3 1 5 5 - x
Firth, L. B., Browne, K. A., Knights, A. M., Hawkins, S. J., & Nash, R.
(2016). Eco- engineered rock pools: A concrete solution to biodiver-
sity loss and urban sprawl in the marine environment. Environmental
Research Letters, 11(9), 094015. https://doi.org/10.108 8/174
8- 9326/11/9/094015
Firth, L. B., Schofield, M., White, F. J., Skov, M. W., & Hawkins, S. J.
(2014). Biodiversity in intertidal tide pools: Informing engineering
criteria for artificial habitat enhancement in the built environment.
Marine Environmental Research, 102, 122– 130.
Firth, L. B., Thompson, R. C., Bohn, K., Abbiati, M., Airoldi, L., Bouma,
T. J., Bozzeda, F., Ceccherelli, V. U., Colangelo, M. A., Evans, A.,
Ferrario, F., Hanley, M. E., Hinz, H., Hoggart, S. P. G., Jackson, J. E.,
Moore, P., Morgan, E. H., Perkol- Finkel, S., Skov, M. W., … Hawkins,
S. J. (2014). Between a rock and a hard place: Environmental and
engineering considerations when designing coastal defence struc-
tures. Coastal Engineering, 87, 122– 135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
coast aleng.2013.10.015
Fisher, J. C., Irvine, K. N., Bicknell, J. E., Hayes, W. M., Fernandes, D.,
Mis try, J. , & Davies , Z. G. (2021). Perceived biodiversity, sound, nat-
uralness and safety enhance the restorative quality and wellbeing
benefits of green and blue space in a neotropical city. Science of
   
|
  15
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
the Total Environment, 755, 143095. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scito
tenv.2020.143095
Forsythe, A., Nadal, M., Sheehy, N., Cela- Conde, C. J., & Sawey, M.
(2011). Predicting beauty: Fractal dimension and visual complex-
ity in art. British Journal of Psychology, 102 (1), 49– 70. ht t p s ://d o i .
org/10.1348/00071 2610X 498958
Franco, L. S., Shanahan, D. F., & Fuller, R. A. (2017). A review of the ben-
efits of nature experiences: More than meets the eye. International
Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14 (8), 864.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerp h1408 0864
Fritz, C., Sonnentag, S., Spector, P. E., & McInroe, J. A. (2010). The week-
end matters: Relationships between stress recovery and affective
experiences. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31(8), 1137– 1162.
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.672
Fry, G., Tveit, M. S., Ode, Å., & Velarde, M. D. (2009). The ecology of vi-
sual landscapes: Exploring the conceptual common ground of visual
and ecological landscape indicators. Ecological Indicators, 9(5), 933–
947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoli nd.2008.11.008
Fuller, R. A., Irvine, K. N., Devine- Wright, P., Warren, P. H., & Gaston, K.
J. (2007). Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with bio-
diversity. Biology Letters, 3(4), 390– 394. https://doi.org /10.1098/
rsbl.2007.0149
Gamfeldt, L., & Roger, F. (2017). Revisiting the biodiversity- ecosystem
multifunctionality relationship. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(7),
168. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 3 8 / s 4 1 5 5 9 - 0 1 7 - 0 1 6 8
Gamfeldt, L., Snäll, T., Bagchi, R., Jonsson, M., Gustafsson, L., Kjellander,
P., Ruiz- Jaen, M. C., Fröberg, M., Stendahl, J., Philipson, C. D.,
Mikusiński, G., Andersson, E., Westerlund, B., Andrén, H., Moberg,
F., Moen, J., & Bengtsson, J. (2013). Higher levels of multiple eco-
system services are found in forests with more tree species. Nature
Communications, 4, 1340. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomm s2328
Garrett, J. K., Clitherow, T. J., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., & Fleming,
L. E. (2019). Coastal proximity and mental health among urban
adults in England: The moderating effect of household income.
Health & Place, 59, 102200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healt
hplace.2019.102200
Graves, R. A., Pearson, S. M., & Turner, M. G. (2017). Species richness
alone does not predict cultural ecosystem service value. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
114(14), 3774– 3779. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.17013 70114
Gross, N., Bagousse- Pinguet, Y. L., Liancourt, P., Berdugo, M., Gotelli, N.
J., & Maestre, F. T. (2017). Functional trait diversity maximizes eco-
system multifunctionality. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(5), s41559-
017- 0132– 017. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 3 8 / s 4 1 5 5 9 - 0 1 7 - 0 1 3 2
Gu, J., Strauss, C., Bond, R., & Cavanagh, K. (2015). How do mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy and mindfulness- based stress reduction
improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and
meta- analysis of mediation studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 37,
1– 12 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006
Gunnarsson, B., Knez, I., Hedblom, M., & Sang, A. A. O. (2017). Effects
of biodiversity and environment- related attitude on perception of
urban green space. Urban Ecosystem, 20(1), 37– 49.
Hall, A. E., Herbert, R. J. H., Britton, J. R., Boyd, I. M., & George, N. C.
(2019). Shelving the coast with Vertipools: Retrofitting artificial
rock pools on coastal structures as mitigation for coastal squeeze.
Frontiers in Marine Science, 6, 1– 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fm ars. 2 019.0 0 456
Hasler, D., & Suesstrunk, S. E. (2003). Measuring colorfulness in natural
images, 50 07, 8795. https://doi.org/10.1117/12.47 7378
Hawkins, S J., Pack, K. E., Firth, L. B., Mieszkowska, N., Evans, A. J.,
Martins, G. M., Åberg, P., Adams, L. C., Arenas, F., Boaventura, D.
M., Bohn, K., Borges, C. D. G., Castro, J. J., Coleman, R. A., Crowe, T.
P., Cruz, T., Davies, M. S., Epstein, G., Faria, J., … Jenkins, S. R. (2019).
The intertidal zone of the north- East Atlantic region: Pattern and
process. In G. A. Williams, K. Bohn, L. B. Firth, & S. J. Hawkins (Eds.),
Interactions in the marine benthos: Global patterns and processes (pp.
7– 46). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/97811
08235 792.003
Hedley, J. (2013). VidAna sof tware for cover analysis from video footage or
still images. (1.0) [computer software]. Marine Spatial Ecology Lab,
School of Biological Sciences, University Of Queensland. Retrieved
from www.marin espat ialec ology lab.org/resou rces/vidan a/
Heinzen, E. (2017). elo: Elo Ratings. (1.0.1) [R; R Statistical Computing].
Retrieved from h t t p s : / / C R A N . R - p r o j e c t . o r g / p a c k a g e =elo
Houlden, V., Weich, S., Albuquerque, J. P. de, Jarvis, S., & Rees, K. (2018).
The relationship between greenspace and the mental wellbeing of
adults: A systematic review. PLoS One, 13(9), e0203000. ht t p s : //
doi.org/10.1371/journ al.pone.0203000
Hoyle, H., Hitchmough, J., & Jorgensen, A. (2017). All about the ‘wow
factor’? The relationships between aesthetics, restorative effect
and perceived biodiversity in designed urban planting. Landscape
and Urban Planning, 164, 109– 123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landu
rbplan.2017.03.011
Hoyle, H., Jorgensen, A., & Hitchmough, J. D. (2019). What deter-
mines how we see nature? Perceptions of naturalness in designed
urban green spaces. People and Nature, 1(2), 167– 180. ht t p s ://d o i .
org/10.1002/pan3.19
Hynes, S., Ankamah- Yeboah, I., O'Neill, S., Needham, K., Xuan, B. B.,
& Armstrong, C. (2021). The impact of nature documentaries on
public environmental preferences and willingness to pay: Entropy
balancing and the blue planet II effect. Journal of Environmental
Planning and Management, 64(8), 1428– 1456. h t t p s : //d o i .
org/10.1080/09640 568.2020.1828840
Isbell, F., Gonzalez, A ., Loreau, M., Cowles, J., Díaz, S., Hector, A., Mace,
G. M., Wardle, D. A., O'Connor, M. I., Duffy, J. E., Turnbull, L. A.,
Thompson, P. L., & Larigauderie, A. (2017). Linking the influence
and dependence of people on biodiversity across scales. Nature,
546(7656), 65– 72. https://doi.org/10.1038/natur e22899
Kang, Y., & Kim, E. J. (2019). Differences of restorative effects while
viewing urban landscapes and green landscapes. Sustainability,
11(7), 2129. https://doi.org/10.3390/su110 72129
Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home: Psychological
benefits. Environment and Behavior, 33(4), 507– 542. ht tps: //doi.
o r g / 1 0 . 1 1 7 7 / 0 0 1 3 9 1 6 0 1 2 1 9 7 3 1 1 5
Kaplan, S. (1987). Aesthetics, affect, and cognition environmental pref-
erence from an evolutionary perspective. Environment and Behavior,
19(1), 3– 32.
Kaplan, S., Kaplan, R., & Wendt, J. S. (1972). Rated preference and com-
plexity for natural and urban visual material. Attention, Perception,
& Psychophysics, 12(4), 354– 356.
Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The bene-
fits of thriving on novelty and challenge. In The Oxford hand-
book of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 367– 374). Oxford
University Press. Retrieved from http://www.oxfordhandbooks.
com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.001.0001/
o x f o r d h b - 9 7 8 0 1 9 5 1 8 7 2 4 3 - e - 0 3 4
Kim, H., Lee, S., Uysal, M., Kim, J., & Ahn, K. (2015). Nature- based tourism:
Motivation and subjective well- being. Journal of Travel & Tourism
Marketing, 32(sup1), S76– S96. https://doi.org/10.10 80/10548
408.2014.997958
Koss, R. S., & Kingsley, J. ‘Yotti.’ (2010). Volunteer health and emotional
wellbeing in marine protected areas. Ocean & Coastal Mana ge-
ment, 53(8), 447453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oceco aman. 2010.
06.002
Koutrakis, E., Sapounidis, A., Marzetti, S., Marin, V., Roussel, S., Martino,
S., Fabiano, M., Paoli, C., Rey- Valette, H., Povh, D., & Malvárez, C.
G. (2011). ICZM and coastal defence perception by beach users:
Lessons from the Mediterranean coastal area. Ocean & Coastal
Management, 54(11), 821– 830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oceco
aman.2011.09.004
16 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
Krapp, A. (1999). Interest, motivation and learning: An educational-
psychological perspective. European Journal of Psycholog y of
Education, 14(1), 23– 40.
Laliberté, E., & Legendre, P. (2010). A distance- based framework for
measuring functional diversity from multiple traits. Ecology, 91(1),
299– 305. https://doi.org/10.1890/08- 2244.1
Lawrence, P. J., Evans, A. J., Jackson- B ué, T., Brooks, P. R. , Crowe, T. P.,
Dozier, A. E., Jenkins, S. R., Moore, P. J., Williams, G. J., & Davies,
A. J. (2021). Artificial shorelines lack natural structural complex-
ity across scales. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences, 288 (1951), 20210329. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.
2021.0329
Lefcheck, J. S., Byrnes, J. E. K., Isbell, F., Gamfeldt, L., Griffin, J. N.,
Eisenhauer, N., Hensel, M. J. S., Hector, A., Cardinale, B. J., & Duffy,
J. E. (2015). Biodiversity enhances ecosystem multifunctionality
across trophic levels and habitats. Nature Communications, 6, 6936.
https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomm s7936
Limburg, K. E., Luzadis, V. A., Ramsey, M., Schulz, K. L., & Mayer, C. M.
(2010). The good, the bad, and the algae: Perceiving ecosystem
services and disservices generated by zebra and quagga mus-
sels. Journal of Great Lakes Research, 36(1), 86– 92. ht tps://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jglr.2009.11.007
Lindemann- Matthies, P., Junge, X., & Matthies, D. (2010). The influ-
ence of plant diversity on people's perception and aesthetic
appreciation of grassland vegetation. Biological Conservation,
143 (1), 195– 202.
Loke, L. H. L., & Todd, P. A. (2016). Structural complexity and component
type increase intertidal biodiversity independently of area. Ecology,
97(2), 383– 393. https://doi.org/10.1890/15- 0257.1
MacLeod, M., Silva, C. P. d., & Cooper, J. A. G. (2002). A comparative
study of the perception and value of beaches in rural Ireland and
Portugal: Implications for coastal zone management. Journal of
Coastal Research, 18(1), 14– 24.
Marselle, M. R., Irvine, K. N., Lorenzo- Arribas, A., & Warber, S. L. (2016).
Does perceived restorativeness mediate the effects of perceived
biodiversity and perceived naturalness on emotional well- being fol-
lowing group walks in nature? Journal of Environmental Psychology,
46, 217– 232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.04.008
Martens, L. (2016). From intergenerational transmission to intra- active
ethical- generational becoming: Children, parents, crabs and rock-
pooling. Families, Relationships and Societies, 5(3), 447– 462. h t t p s : //
d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 3 3 2 / 2 0 4 6 7 4 3 1 6 X 1 4 7 5 8 4 9 8 3 7 4 7 4 6
Martin, C. L., Momtaz, S., Gaston, T., & Moltschaniwskyj, N. A. (2016). A
systematic quantitative review of coastal and marine cultural eco-
system services: Current status and future research. Marine Policy,
74, 25– 32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marpol.2016.09.004
Martin, S., Thouzeau, G., Chauvaud, L., Jean, F., Guérin, L., & Clavier, J.
(2006). Respiration, calcification, and excretion of the invasive slip-
per limpet, Crepidula fornicata L.: Implications for carbon, carbonate,
and nitrogen fluxes in affected areas. Limnology and Oceanography,
51(5), 1996– 2007. https://doi.org/10.4319/lo.2006.51.5.1996
Matias, M. G., Underwood, A. J., Hochuli, D. F., & Coleman, R. A. (2010).
Independent effects of patch size and structural complexity on di-
versity of benthic macroinvertebrates. Ecology, 91(7), 1908– 1915.
McKinney, M. L. (2006). Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homog-
enization. Biological Conservation, 127(3), 247– 260. h t t p s : //d o i .
org/10.1016/j.biocon.2005.09.005
MEA. (2005). Ecosystems and human well- being: Synthesis. Island Press.
http://chapt er.ser.org/europ e/files/ 2012/08/Harris.pdf
Mensah, S., Salako, K. V., Assogbadjo, A., Glèlè Kakaï, R., Sinsin, B., &
Seif ert, T. (20 20). Fun ctiona l tra it dive rsi ty is a stro nger pr edic tor of
multifunctionality than dominance: Evidence from an Afromontane
forest in South Africa. Ecological Indicators, 115, 106415. h t tps://
doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoli nd.2020.106415
Merkel, A., Säwe, F., & Fredriksson, C. (2021). The seaweed experi-
ence: Exploring the potential and value of a marine resource.
Scandinavian Journal of Hospitality and Tourism, 21(4), 1– 16. h t tps: //
doi.org/10.1080/15022 250.2021.1879671
Mills, J. G., Brookes, J. D., Gellie, N. J. C., Liddicoat, C., Lowe, A. J.,
Sydnor, H. R., Thomas, T., Weinstein, P., Weyrich, L. S., & Breed,
M. F. (2019). Relating urban biodiversity to human health with the
‘Holobiont’ concept. Frontiers in Microbiology, 10, 18. https://d o i .
org/10.3389/fmicb.2019.00550
Momota, K., & Hosokawa, S. (2021). Potential impacts of marine urban-
ization on benthic macrofaunal diversity. Scientific Reports, 11(1),
4028. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 3 8 / s 4 1 5 9 8 - 0 2 1 - 8 3 5 9 7 - z
Moran, D. (2019). Back to nature? Attention restoration theor y and the
restorative effects of nature contact in prison. Health & Place, 57,
35– 43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healt hplace.2019.03.005
Morgan, R. (1999a). Preferences and priorities of Recreational Beach
users in Wales, UK. Journal of Coastal Research, 15(3), 653– 667.
Morgan, R. (1999b). Some factors affecting coastal landscape aesthetic
quality assessment. Landscape Research, 24(2), 167– 184. ht tps://
doi.org/10.1080/01426 39990 8706557
Moss, A., & Esson, M. (2010). Visitor interest in zoo animals and the im-
plications for collection planning and zoo education programmes.
Zoo Biology, 29(6), 715– 731.
Neumann, B., Vafeidis, A. T., Zimmermann, J., & Nicholls, R. J. (2015).
Future coastal population growth and exposure to sea- level rise and
coastal flooding— A global assessment. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0118571.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journ al.pone.0118571
Newbold, T., Hudson, L. N., Hill, S. L. L., Contu, S., Lysenko, I., Senior,
R. A ., Börger, L., Bennett, D. J., Choimes, A., Collen, B., Day, J., De
Palma, A., Díaz, S., Echeverria- Londoño, S., Edgar, M. J., Feldman,
A., Garon, M., Harrison, M. L. K., Alhusseini, T., … Purvis, A. (2015).
Global effects of land use on local terrestrial biodiversity. Nature,
520(7545), 45– 50. https://doi.org/10.1038/natur e14324
Nuojua, S., Pahl, S. & Thompson, R. (2021). Ocean connectedness and
consumer responses to single- use packaging. Unpublished Data.
Oksanen, J., Blanchet, F. G., Friendly, M., Kindt, R., Legendre, P., McGlinn,
D., Minchin, P. R., O'Hara, R. B., Simpson, G. L., Solymos, P., Stevens,
M. H. H., Szoecs, E., & Wagner, H. (2017). vegan: Communit y ecol-
ogy package (2.4) [Computer software]. Retrieved from htt ps://
C R A N . R - p r o j e c t . o r g / p a c k a g e =vegan
Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to land-
scapes. In The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the genera-
tion of culture (pp. 555– 579). Oxford University Press.
O'Shaughnessy, K. A., Hawkins, S. J., Evans, A. J., Hanley, M. E., Lunt,
P., Thompson, R. C., Francis, R. A., Hoggart, S. P. G., Moore, P. J.,
Iglesias, G., Simmonds, D., Ducker, J., & Firth, L. B. (2020). Design
catalogue for eco- engineering of coastal artificial structures: A
multifunctional approach for stakeholders and end- users. Urban
Ecosystem, 23(2), 431– 443. ht tps://doi.org/10.1007/s1125 2- 019-
0 0 9 2 4 - z
Palliwoda, J., Kowarik, I., & von der Lippe, M. (2017). Human- biodiversity
interactions in urban parks: The species level matters. Landscape
and Urban Planning, 157, 394– 406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landu
rbplan.2016.09.003
Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., & Sammartino, J. (2013). Visual aesthetics
and human preference. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 77– 107.
h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 1 4 6 / a n n u r e v - p s y c h - 1 2 0 7 1 0 - 1 0 0 5 0 4
Pálsdóttir, A. M. K., Stigsdotter, U., Persson, D., Thorpert, P., & Grahn,
P. (2018). The qualities of natural environments that support
the rehabilitation process of individuals with stress- related
mental disorder in nature- based rehabilitation. Urban Forestry
and Urban Greening, 29, 312– 321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ufug.2017.11.016
Pasanen, T. P., Neuvonen, M., & Korpela, K. M. (2018). The psychology
of recent nature visits: (how) are motives and Attentional focus re-
lated to post- visit restorative experiences, creativity, and emotional
well- being? Environment and Behavior, 50(8), 913– 944. https://d o i .
org /10.1177/00139 16517 720261
   
|
  17
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
Pasari, J. R., Levi, T., Zavaleta, E. S., & Tilman, D. (2013). Several scales
of biodiversity affect ecosystem multifunctionality. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
110(25), 10219– 10222.
Perkins, M. J., Ng, T. P. T., Dudgeon, D., Bonebrake, T. C., & Leung, K.
M. Y. (2015). Conserving intertidal habitats: What is the potential
of ecological engineering to mitigate impacts of coastal structures?
Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science, 167, 504– 515. h t t p s : //doi .
org/10.1016/j.ecss.2015.10.033
Petrova, E. G., Mironov, Y. V., Aoki, Y., Matsushima, H., Ebine, S., Furuya,
K., Petrova, A., Takayama, N., & Ueda, H. (2015). Comparing the
visual perception and aesthetic evaluation of natural landscapes in
Russia and Japan: Cultural and environmental factors. Progress in
Earth and Planetary Science, 2(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/s4064
5 - 0 1 5 - 0 0 3 3 - x
Purcell, A. T., & Lamb, R. J. (1998). Preference and naturalness: An eco-
logical approach. Landscape and Urban Planning, 42(1), 5766.
R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical comput-
ing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from ht t p : //
w w w . R - p r o j e c t . o r g /
Rap po rt, D. J., Costanza, R., & McMichael, A . J. (1998). Assessing ecosys-
tem health. Trends in Ecolog y & Evolution, 13(10), 397– 402. ht t p s : //
d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / S 0 1 6 9 - 5 3 4 7 ( 9 8 ) 0 1 4 4 9 - 9
Renninger, A., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (2014). The role of interest in learning
and development. Psychology Press.
Sankaran, S., Derechin, J., & Christakis, N. A. (2021). CurmElo: The theor y
and practice of a forced- choice approach to producing preference
rankings. PLoS ONE, 16 (5), e0252145. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journ al.pone.0252145
Schebella, M. F., Weber, D., Schultz, L., & Weinstein, P. (2019). The well-
being benefits associated with perceived and measured biodiver-
sity in Australian urban green spaces. Sustainability, 11(3), 802.
https://doi.org/10.3390/su110 30802
Schultz, P. W. (20 02). Inclusion with nature: The psychology of human–
nature relations. In psychology of sustainable development (pp. 61–
78). Springer. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 0 7 / 9 7 8 - 1 - 4 6 1 5 - 0 9 9 5 - 0 _ 4
Schwind, J. K., McCay, E., Beanlands, H., Schindel Martin, L., Martin, J.,
& Binder, M. (2017). Mindfulness practice as a teaching- learning
strategy in higher education: A qualitative explorator y pilot study.
Nurse Education Today, 50, 92– 96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
nedt.2016.12.017
Shwartz, A., Turbé, A., Simon, L., & Julliard, R. (2014). Enhancing urban
biodiversity and its influence on city- dwellers: An experiment.
Biological Conservation, 171 , 82– 90. https://doi.org /10.1016/j.
biocon.2014.01.009
Silvia, P. J. (2001). Interest and interests: The psychology of constructive
capriciousness. Review of General Psycholog y, 5(3), 270– 290.
Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. Oxford University
Press.
Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest— The curious emotion. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 17(1), 57– 60. ht tps://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1467- 8721. 2008.00548.x
Silvia, P. J. (2012). Human emotions and aesthetic experience (pp. 250–
275). Connecting Minds, Brain and Experience.
Simmons, D. (1998). Using natural settings for environmental educa-
tion: Perceived benefits and barriers. The Journal of Environmental
Education, 29(3), 23– 31. https://doi.org/10.1080/00958 96980
9599115
Southon, G. E., Jorgensen, A., Dunnett, N., Hoyle, H., & Evans, K. L.
(2018). Perceived species- richness in urban green spaces: Cues, ac-
curacy and well- being impacts. Landscape and Urban Planning, 172,
1– 10 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landu rbplan.2017.12.002
Sun, Z., & Firestone, C. (2021). Curious objects: How visual complexity
guides attention and engagement. Cognitive Science, 45(4), e12933.
https://doi.or g/10.1111/cogs.12933
Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics informs traditional aesthetics.
In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 9– 35).
Springer. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 0 7 / 9 7 8 - 3 - 6 6 2 - 0 7 1 4 2 - 7 _ 2
Tribot, A.- S., Deter, J., Claverie, T., Guillhaumon, F., Villéger, S., &
Mouquet, N. (2019). Species diversity and composition drive the
aesthetic value of coral reef fish assemblages. Biology Letters,
15(11), 20190703. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2019.0703
Tribot, A.- S., Deter, J., & Mouquet, N. (2018). Integrating the aesthetic
value of landscapes and biological diversity. Proceedings of th e Royal
Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1886), 20180971. ht tps://d oi.
org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0971
Tribot, A.- S., Mouquet, N., Villéger, S., Raymond, M., Hoff, F., Boissery,
P., & Deter, J. (2016). Taxonomic and functional diversity increase
the aesthetic value of coralligenous reefs. Scientific Repo rts, 6, 1 – 1 2 .
Turner, S. A., & Silvia, P. J. (2006). Must interesting things be pleasant?
A test of competing appraisal structures. Emotion, 6(4), 670– 674.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1528- 3542.6.4.670
Turpie, J. K. (2003). The existence value of biodiversity in South Africa:
How interest, experience, knowledge, income and perceived level
of threat influence local willingness to pay. Ecological Economics,
46(2), 199– 216. h t t p s : / /d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / S 0 9 2 1 - 8 0 0 9 ( 0 3 ) 0 0 1 2 2 - 8
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural en-
vironment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Behavior and
the natural environment (pp. 85– 125). Springer US. ht t p s : //doi.
o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 0 7 / 9 7 8 - 1 - 4 6 1 3 - 3 5 3 9 - 9 _ 4
United Nations General Assembly. (2015). Transforming our world: The
2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development (A/RES/70/1). Retrieved
from https://sdgs.un.org/2030a genda
Van Den Berg, A. E., & Custers, M. H. G. (2011). Gardening promotes
neuroendocrine and affective restoration from stress. Journal of
Health Psychology, 16(1), 3– 11. https://doi.org/10.1177/13591
05310 365577
Vartanian, O., Navarrete, G., Chatterjee, A., Fich, L. B., Leder, H.,
Modroño, C., Nadal, M., Rostrup, N., & Skov, M. (2013). Impact of
contour on aesthetic judgments and approach- avoidance decisions
in architecture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America, 110 (Suppl 2), 10446– 10453. https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.13012 27110
Wheeler, B. W., White, M., Stahl- Timmins, W., & Depledge, M. H.
(2012). Does living by the coast improve health and wellbeing?
Health & Place, 18(5), 1198– 1201. https://doi.org /10.1016/j.healt
hplace.2012.06.015
White, M. P., Pahl, S., Ashbullby, K., Herbert, S., & Depledge, M. H.
(2013). Feelings of restoration from recent nature visits. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 35, 40– 51.
White, M. P., Weeks, A., Hooper, T., Bleakley, L., Cracknell, D., Lovell, R.,
& Jefferson, R. L. (2017). Marine wildlife as an important compo-
nent of coastal visits: The role of perceived biodiversity and species
behaviour. Marine Policy, 78, 80– 89.
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer.
Winter, S. (2012). Forest naturalness assessment as a component of bio-
diversity monitoring and conservation management. Forestr y: An
International Journal of Forest Research, 85(2), 293– 304. h t t p s : //doi.
org/10.1093/fores try/cps004
Wood, E., Harsant, A., Dallimer, M., Cronin de Chavez, A., McEachan,
R. R. C., & Hassall, C. (2018). Not all green space is created equal:
Biodiversity predicts psychological restorative benefits from
urban green space. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1– 13. ht t p s : //d o i .
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02320
Wood, S. L. R., Jones, S. K., Johnson, J. A., Brauman, K. A., Chaplin-
Kramer, R., Fremier, A., Girvetz, E., Gordon, L. J., Kappel, C. V.,
Mandle, L., Mulligan, M., O'Farrell, P., Smith, W. K., Willemen, L.,
Zhang, W., & DeClerck, F. A. (2018). Distilling the role of ecosystem
services in the sustainable development goals. Ecosystem Services,
29, 70– 82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2017.10.010
18 
|
   
People and Nature
FAIRCHILD et al.
Wu, Y., Xie, L., Yuan, Z., Jiang, S., Liu, W., & Sheng, H. (2020). Investigating
public biodiversity conservation awareness based on the propaga-
tion of wildlife- related incidents on the Sina Weibo social media
platform. Environmental Research Letters, 15(9), 094082. ht tps: //
doi.org/10 .1088/1748- 9326/ab9ed1
Zawada, D. G., Piniak, G. A., & Hearn, C. J. (2010). Topographic com-
plexity and roughness of a tropical benthic seascape. Geophysical
Research Letters, 37(14), L14604. https://doi.org/10.1029/2010G
L043789
Zhang, H., Chen, B., Sun, Z., & Bao, Z. (2013). Landscape perception
and recreation needs in urban green space in Fuyang, Hangzhou,
China. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 12(1), 44– 52. ht t p s ://d o i .
org/10.1016/j.ufug.2012.11.001
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found in the online
version of the article at the publisher’s website.
How to cite this article: Fairchild, T. P., Weedon, J. & Griffin,
J. N. (2022). Species diversity enhances perceptions of urban
coastlines at multiple scales. People and Nature, 00, 1–18.
https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10330
... When attempting to determine if a species or assemblage offers a net bioprotective gain or biodeteriorative loss, acknowledgement of the additional ecosystem services offered by it should be considered (Fig. 3). For example, carbon dioxide sequestration and water filtration (Layman et al., 2014), habitat and food provision (Vaughn 2018), contribution to 'natural' aesthetic (Fairchild et al., 2022), and cultural and socio-economic benefits (education, tourism, recreation, fisheries industry) may be provided (Naylor et al. 2014 and could tip the scales in favour of encouraging or retaining existing colonisation . Ultimately, a bioprotective assemblage is not guaranteed as colonisation of a substrate is dependent on spatiotemporally variable factors, such as latitude, climate, orientation, aspect, substrate material, larval supply, predator-prey dynamics, competition, and habitat and food availability. ...
Article
Full-text available
The deleterious effects (biodeterioration) and the protective benefits (bioprotection) of biological colonisation on manmade structures have long been debated. Lichens, biofilms, algae, bivalves and gastropods contribute both directly and indirectly to damaging substrata in the coastal zone which can enhance abiotic erosive forces that exploit biologically induced superficial damage. There is mounting evidence that these same species may also provide protective benefits. This debate often impacts approaches to managing fouling on concrete assets in the coastal environment. The net benefit or detriment a species or assemblage has on a structure is spatially and temporally dynamic and subject to the influence of various abiotic and biotic factors at different scales. However, the net outcome may be more pronounced under different contexts, particularly under warming and ocean acidifying climate change scenarios which is where further research should focus. Additionally, as bioprotection represents a potentially valuable ecosystem service, it supports the argument for increasing and improving habitat availability and biodiversity on artificial coastal structures via ecological enhancement. Quantifying bioprotection in useful metrics, such as monetary value or time added to serviceable life, would help demonstrate the benefits of bioprotective species in a meaningful way. Outline:
Thesis
Full-text available
In this thesis I develop a robust system and method for predicting individuals’ emotional responses to musical stimuli. Music has a powerful effect on human emotion, however the factors that create this emotional experience are poorly understood. Some of these factors are characteristics of the music itself, for example musical tempo, mode, harmony, and timbre are known to affect people's emotional responses. However, the same piece of music can produce different emotional responses in different people, so the ability to use music to induce emotion also depends on predicting the effect of individual differences. These individual differences might include factors such as people's moods, personalities, culture, and musical background amongst others. While many of the factors that contribute to emotional experience have been examined, it is understood that the research in this domain is far from both a) identifying and understanding the many factors that affect an individual’s emotional response to music, and b) using this understanding of factors to inform the selection of stimuli for emotion induction. This unfortunately results in wide variance in emotion induction results, inability to replicate emotional studies, and the inability to control for variables in research. The approach of this thesis is to therefore model the latent variable contributions to an individual’s emotional experience of music through the application of deep learning and modern recommender system techniques. With each study in this work, I iteratively develop a more reliable and effective system for predicting personalised emotion responses to music, while simultaneously adopting and developing strong and standardised methodology for stimulus selection. The work sees the introduction and validation of a) electronic and loop-based music as reliable stimuli for inducing emotional responses, b) modern recommender systems and deep learning as methods of more reliably predicting individuals' emotion responses, and c) novel understandings of how musical features map to individuals' emotional responses. The culmination of this research is the development of a personalised emotion prediction system that can better predict individuals emotional responses to music, and can select musical stimuli that are better catered to individual difference. This will allow researchers and practitioners to both more reliably and effectively a) select music stimuli for emotion induction, and b) induce and manipulate target emotional responses in individuals.
Article
Full-text available
1. Researchers, practitioners and policymakers have widely documented the multifarious ways that nature influences human well-being. However, we still have only a limited understanding of how the public interact with, respond to and talk about attributes of biodiversity. 2. We used image-based Q methodology to explore the shared and contrasting perspectives people hold for biodiversity. This approach is a powerful way of allowing people to articulate what is, or is not, important to them, free from constraints associated with statement-based stimuli. 3. We used British woodlands as a study system, as they are accessible and well-visited by the public. The elements of biodiversity incorporated in the Q methodology represented vertebrates, invertebrates, trees and understorey plants and fungi. 4. The shared public perspectives varied, and the reasons underpinning those perspectives were rich and diverse. People articulated reasons related to an array of biodiversity attributes (e.g. functions, behaviours, colours, smells, shapes). Many of the perspectives transcended specific species or taxonomic groups. 5. Although woodlands were used as a study system, people referenced perceptions and experiences external to this habitat (e.g. within their gardens) and associated with their everyday lives. Cultural influences and memories linked to particular people and places were also prominent. 6. Few of the shared perspectives map onto the objective measures and dimensions that researchers use to describe and categorise biodiversity (e.g. rarity, ecosystem service provision). K E Y W O R D S Britain, culture, ecosystem services, human wellbeing, human-nature interactions, Q methodology, sensory, woodland
Article
Full-text available
We introduce CurmElo, a forced-choice approach to producing a preference ranking of an arbitrary set of objects that combines the Elo algorithm with novel techniques for detecting and correcting for (1) preference heterogeneity induced polarization in preferences among raters, and (2) intransitivity in preference rankings. We detail the application of CurmElo to the problem of generating approximately preference-neutral identifiers, in this case four-letter and five-letter nonsense words patterned on the phonological conventions of the English language, using a population of Amazon Mechanical Turk workers. We find evidence that human raters have significant non-uniform preferences over these nonsense words, and we detail the consequences of this finding for social science work that utilizes identifiers without accounting for the bias this can induce. In addition, we describe how CurmElo can be used to produce rankings of arbitrary features or dimensions of preference of a set of objects relative to a population of raters.
Article
Full-text available
Some things look more complex than others. For example, a crenulate and richly organized leaf may seem more complex than a plain stone. What is the nature of this experience-and why do we have it in the first place? Here, we explore how object complexity serves as an efficiently extracted visual signal that the object merits further exploration. We algorithmically generated a library of geometric shapes and determined their complexity by computing the cumulative sur-prisal of their internal skeletons-essentially quantifying the "amount of information" within each shape-and then used this approach to ask new questions about the perception of complexity. Experiments 1-3 asked what kind of mental process extracts visual complexity: a slow, deliberate, reflective process (as when we decide that an object is expensive or popular) or a fast, effortless, and automatic process (as when we see that an object is big or blue)? We placed simple and complex objects in visual search arrays and discovered that complex objects were easier to find among simple distractors than simple objects are among complex distractors-a classic search asymmetry indicating that complexity is prioritized in visual processing. Next, we explored the function of complexity: Why do we represent object complexity in the first place? Experiments 4-5 asked subjects to study serially presented objects in a self-paced manner (for a later memory test); subjects dwelled longer on complex objects than simple objects-even when object shape was completely task-irrelevant-suggesting a connection between visual complexity and exploratory engagement. Finally, Experiment 6 connected these implicit measures of complexity to explicit judgments. Collectively, these findings suggest that visual complexity is extracted efficiently and automatically, and even arouses a kind of "perceptual curiosity" about objects that encourages subsequent attentional engagement.
Article
Full-text available
Urbanization and associated human activities have caused numerous changes to natural environments, including the loss of natural habitats and replacement with artificial structures. How these changes impact coastal marine biodiversity and ecosystem functioning is not well known. In this study, we examined the potential impacts of habitat changes by comparing species commonality and community structure (i.e., species richness, abundance, and functional composition) among artificial (a breakwater wall) and natural habitats (eelgrass bed, intertidal flat, and subtidal bottom) within a semi-enclosed coastal sea impacted by marine urbanization. We found considerable species overlap (i.e., high species sharing) among the eelgrass bed, intertidal flat, and subtidal bottom habitats. By contrast, the breakwater wall was a distinctive habitat with little overlap in species and functional groups with the other habitats, and was therefore a poor substitute for natural habitats. Our study suggests that marine urbanization degrades redundancy and inhibits the maintenance of biodiversity in coastal marine zones.
Article
Full-text available
The article addresses the broader relationships between seaweed and algae as a marine resource, destination development, and sustainability. In many European countries, an industry around seaweed has emerged, ranging from high-end restaurants that provide their customers with local, seasonal and sustainable ingredients, to entrepreneurs offering “harvest your own seaweed”-tours. In this explorative study, we focus on different ways of experiencing this marine resource in a Swedish context, to better understand its economic and social potential. Taking a consumer perspective, we investigate through an online questionnaire and reviews from an online consumer-to-consumer travel-planning portal how seaweed and algae are validated. By exploring people's notions, the aim is to understand the potential and value of this marine resource better. Where is seaweed encountered and enjoyed? How is it used? What is experienced to be valuable about this resource? We identify possibilities for utilizing algae and seaweed in order to foster future business opportunities for local communities, as well as to integrate the resource more in our society. We show how algae and seaweed are experienced in diverse settings and dimensions, such as at home, while being a tourist, as part of everyday life, as a special treat, within nature, and as food.
Article
Full-text available
Urban land cover expansion and human population growth are accelerating worldwide. This is resulting in the loss and degradation of green and blue spaces (e.g. parks, waterways, lakes) in cities, which provide resources to sustain biodiversity and improve human wellbeing. The specific characteristics of these spaces (e.g. sounds, species, safety) that enhance or detract from wellbeing are underexplored, yet this knowledge is needed to inform urban planning, management and policies that will ultimately benefit both people and biodiversity. Research of this kind is rarely conducted in the Global South, where rapid urbanisation threatens biodiversity-rich ecosystems of worldwide significance. Here, we examine how perceptions of green, waterway, and dense urban spaces relate to wellbeing in Georgetown, Guyana. Specifically, we use mediation models to test how perceptions of sound, bird species richness, naturalness, and safety concerns contribute to sites being perceived as restorative which, subsequently, influences wellbeing. We assess the accuracy of these site perceptions with objective measures of sound (using a bioacoustic sound index), bird species richness, and percent coverage of vegetation, water, and impervious surfaces. Results showed that if sites were perceived as species rich, containing natural sounds like birdsong, natural rather than artificial, and safe, they were perceived as more restorative, resulting in improved wellbeing. In general, people's perceptions were consistent with objective measures. Green, compared with waterway and dense urban sites, contained more biophonic sounds, higher species richness, greater vegetation and water coverage. Although waterways were biodiverse, they were dominated by anthrophonic sounds, so were perceived as artificial and non-restorative. We shed light on how city planners might augment specific characteristics to improve the wellbeing of urban dwellers, with implications for biodiversity conservation. Our findings provide a scientific evidence base for urban design and management plans that could deliver multiple co-benefits, particularly in biodiversity-rich cities in neotropical regions.
Article
Single-use packaging items constitute a large proportion of the plastic litter found in the marine environment. Consumer decisions contribute to the accumulation of this pollution in the environment. Here we undertook two studies to assess consumer responses to different types of single-use packaging. Moreover, we introduce a new measure of ocean connectedness adapted from nature connectedness measures and investigate its association with consumer response. In Study 1, 60 UK undergraduate students completed a packaging rating task and a survey on ocean connectedness in a laboratory environment. In the rating task pictures of bottled drinks with unique combinations of packaging recyclability (recyclable or non-recyclable) and type of material (plastic, glass, aluminium or carton) were rated in terms of willingness to buy, anticipated affective response and attractiveness. Study 2 used the same experimental approach online, with responses gathered from a broader UK public sample (n = 512). The data were analysed using linear mixed models. Both studies demonstrated a strong preference for recyclable over non-recyclable packaging and found interaction effects between recyclability and ocean connectedness: We found larger differences between ratings for recyclable and non-recyclable packaging in consumers high in ocean connectedness than in respondents low in ocean connectedness. Interactions between packaging material and consumer ratings showed that plastic packaging in general was viewed as less benign by those high in ocean connectedness. Ocean connectedness has the potential to encourage sustainable purchasing and help minimise plastic waste. Study limitations and wider implications are discussed.
Article
From microbes to humans, habitat structural complexity plays a direct role in the provision of physical living space, and increased complexity supports higher biodiversity and ecosystem functioning across biomes. Coastal development and the construction of artificial shorelines are altering natural landscapes as humans seek socio-economic benefits and protection from coastal storms, flooding and erosion. In this study, we evaluate how much structural complexity is missing on artificial coastal structures compared to natural rocky shorelines, across a range of spatial scales from 1 mm to 10 s of m, using three remote sensing platforms (handheld camera, terrestrial laser scanner and uncrewed aerial vehicles). Natural shorelines were typically more structurally complex than artificial ones and offered greater variation between locations. However, our results varied depending on the type of artificial structure and the scale at which complexity was measured. Seawalls were deficient at all scales (approx. 20-40% less complex than natural shores), whereas rock armour was deficient at the smallest and largest scales (approx. 20-50%). Our findings reinforce concerns that hardening shorelines with artificial structures simplifies coastlines at organism-relevant scales. Furthermore, we offer much-needed insight into how structures might be modified to more closely capture the complexity of natural rocky shores that support biodiversity.
Article
Significance We use distribution data on 48,661 species to show that marine biodiversity has been responding to climate warming at a global scale. We show that marine species richness levels off or declines in latitudinal bands with average annual sea surface temperatures exceeding 20 °C. This results in a dip in species richness around the equator that has become more pronounced as the climate has warmed, especially for pelagic species. Previous studies have either only predicted such effects or have provided data at regional scales or for limited numbers of taxa.