ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Objectives We investigated the relationships between fine motor skills, fitness, anthropometrics, gender and perceived motor performance in school beginners. The aim of our study was to delineate whether and to what extent fine motor control would show meaningful synchrony with other motor variables in the age of onset of handwriting in school. Methods A sample of N = 239 of 6-to-8-year-old children were tested with an array of tasks measuring fine motor (i.e., dexterity and speed) and grapho-motor performance (tracing on a tablet screen), anthropometric indexes, and fitness (shuttle run) measures. A subset of 95 children was also tested for perceived motor competence. Results In spite of an overall poor anthropometric condition, our participants were relatively fit. As expected, older children performed better in both, fine motor tasks and the shuttle test. The girls were better in fine motor skills, and an original speed-quality trade-off in the drawing was found. However, the magnitude of difference by grade was greater for boys’ fine motor skills than those of girls’. A network analysis revealed three specific clusters, (1) perceived competencies, (2) fitness, and (3) fine motor skills. Conclusions Given the relative independence of these areas of physical performance, we suggest focusing on these three clusters as distinct areas of physical education. Fine motor skills deserve further consideration, especially at an early school age. We have demonstrated that network analysis and technology devices used to evaluate motor development are useful and meaningful tools.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Fine motor skills and motor control networking in
developmental age
Danilo Bondi
1
| Claudio Robazza
2
| Christiane Lange-Küttner
3,4
|
Tiziana Pietrangelo
1
1
Department of Neuroscience, Imaging
and Clinical Sciences, University G.
d'Annunzioof Chieti-Pescara, Chieti,
Italy
2
Department of Medicine and Aging
Sciences, University G. d'Annunzioof
Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
3
Department of Psychology, University of
Greifswald, Germany
4
Department of Psychology, University of
Bremen, Bremen, Land Bremen, Germany
Correspondence
Danilo Bondi, Department of
Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical
Sciences, University G. d'Annunzioof
Chieti-Pescara, Via dei Vestini, 31, Chieti,
66100, Italy.
Email: danilo.bondi@unich.it
Funding information
Italian Ministry of Education, University
and Research - Departments of Excellence
2018-2022; University G. d'Annunzioof
Chieti - Pescara grant
Abstract
Objectives: We investigated the relationships between fine motor skills, fit-
ness, anthropometrics, gender and perceived motor performance in school
beginners. The aim of our study was to delineate whether and to what extent
fine motor control would show meaningful synchrony with other motor vari-
ables in the age of onset of handwriting in school.
Methods: A sample of N=239 of 6-to-8-year-old children were tested with an
array of tasks measuring fine motor (i.e., dexterity and speed) and grapho-
motor performance (tracing on a tablet screen), anthropometric indexes, and
fitness (shuttle run) measures. A subset of 95 children was also tested for per-
ceived motor competence.
Results: In spite of an overall poor anthropometric condition, our participants
were relatively fit. As expected, older children performed better in both, fine
motor tasks and the shuttle test. The girls were better in fine motor skills, and
an original speed-quality trade-off in the drawing was found. However, the
magnitude of difference by grade was greater for boys' fine motor skills than
those of girls'. A network analysis revealed three specific clusters, (1) perceived
competencies, (2) fitness, and (3) fine motor skills.
Conclusions: Given the relative independence of these areas of physical per-
formance, we suggest focusing on these three clusters as distinct areas of physi-
cal education. Fine motor skills deserve further consideration, especially at an
early school age. We have demonstrated that network analysis and technology
devices used to evaluate motor development are useful and meaningful tools.
1|INTRODUCTION
We have considerable knowledge about how gross motor
skills develop in children (Ghassabian et al., 2016;
Smith & Thelen, 2003) and how to assess them (Griffiths
et al., 2018). Thus, such results in gross motor skills
become included in educational settings. In contrast,
although some effort has been devoted to research on
fine motor control development and interventions in
babies and pre-school children (Strooband et al., 2020),
less is known about fine motor skills in school age chil-
dren. The aim of our study was to delineate whether and
Received: 13 October 2021 Revised: 30 April 2022 Accepted: 4 May 2022
DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23758
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
© 2022 The Authors. American Journal of Human Biology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC.
Am J Hum Biol. 2022;e23758. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ajhb 1of15
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23758
to what extent fine motor control would show meaning-
ful synchrony with other motor variables because of the
onset of handwriting in school.
The astonishing use of hands is a fundamental
achievement of humankind: a wide-ranging sort of hand-
based interaction with the environment led to a vital loop
increasing our manual skills. In particular, the morphol-
ogy and function of the thumb in manipulation and com-
plex grasping is considered a cornerstone in functional
evolutionary development (Marzke, 1992). Phenotypical
features in postural and functional kinematics lead to the
genesis of grapho-motor gestures, and the pattern of
those gestures may reflect the developmental trajectories,
allowing to assess grapho-motor levels (Vaivre-Douret
et al., 2021). Increasing complexity and meaning in the
use of tools was instrumental in the evolution of human
intelligence (Lockman & Kahrs, 2017). Gender differ-
ences exist in performing grapho-motor tasks during
developmental ages, with girls having an early advantage
in performing fine motor tasks (Reikerås et al., 2017) and
learning novel tasks (Flatters et al., 2014), while boys out-
perform girls in tasks requiring object control (such as
throwing, Escolano-Pérez et al., 2021). The reasons lie
both in biological factorssuch as the differential
involvement of visuo-motor integration (Giammarco
et al., 2016)and environmental factors, such as gender-
differentiated expectations made by parents about suit-
able and proper motor tasks (Dinkel & Snyder, 2020).
Developing object manipulation skills in childhood
promotes future physical activity (Barnett et al., 2009).
Especially the advanced use of graphic tools has been
playing a fundamental role in society: the complex use of
such devices in drawing and handwriting was conducive
to nearly every culture in this world (Milbrath &
Council, 2003). This argument has been extended to digi-
tal technologies as they are integrated into school settings
and online learning from home as innovative (and
socially distanced) learning tools during the COVID pan-
demic (see also Scaradozzi et al., 2021).
The current study investigates how motor skills are
related in children who have just begun school. This cru-
cial transition point in child development when they are
learning to write changes drawing from basic shapes and
minimal detail to visually realistic drawing that captures
modulated figure contours (Lange-Küttner et al., 2002;
Toomela, 2002).
1.1 |Motor networks in development
From a kinesiological point of view, we can consider
grapho-motor skills as special tool-related fine motor
skills. Drawing, tracing, and writing result from an
integrated input of visual and motor modalities (eye-hand
coordination) during precise, small-scale movements.
Defining fine motor skills, beyond grapho-motricity,
other clusters involved are dexterity, manipulation of
small objects, and speed-dominated skills in simple and
very likely repetitive movements (Martzog et al., 2019).
Fine motor skills in school children consist of an
interplay of spatio-temporal processing, visuo-motor
integration, strength control, perceptual, and cognitive
skills (Feder & Majnemer, 2007; Lange-Küttner, 1998;
Tabatabaey-Mashadi et al., 2015). These related skills are
a great example of networked neuromotor development.
Different grapho-motor tasks such as writing and draw-
ing lie on both common and distinct underlying neural
circuits in the motor and posterior parietal. Drawing acti-
vates a ventral pathway responsible for overseeing and
orienting in spatial fields, while writing activates a dorsal
pathway that controls letter features (Raimo et al., 2021;
Yuan & Brown, 2015). Moreover, delays and disturbances
in the fine motor network are present in children with
special needs such as ADHD and autism (Lange-
Küttner & Kochhar, 2020) and are greatly correlated with
low IQ, math and reading (Mayes et al., 2018; see also
Suggate et al., 2018). Gaul and Issartel (2016) claimed
that children's fine motor skill proficiency was not pro-
gressing at the expected rate given by normative data,
over early school ages.
Thus, researchers have been using digital tablets and
pen to acquire and evaluate the parameters pressure,
velocity, and time in children's drawing and writing
(Lange-Küttner, 1998; Tabatabaey-Mashadi et al., 2015).
There is an emerging field of Artificial Intelligence in
Education (AIED) for motor skills learning (Santos, 2016)
that needs a clear characterization of the motor tasks in
school children to set interactive technologies and foster
advanced learning experiences. The multidisciplinary
field of graphonomicsneeds to be linked within a
broad spectrum of other motor skills and behavior (Van
Gemmert & Contreras-Vidal, 2015). Recently, it has been
suggested that grapho-motor performances can provide a
paradigm to investigate motor skills that may deviate
from the expected developmental trajectories (Bondi, Di
Sano, Verratti, et al., 2020). While grapho-motor tasks, as
other fine motor tasks, share common underlying path-
ways with gross-motor tasks rely on conscious and inten-
tional motor strategy and control development, they
develop differently in the way of how these shared roots
emerge. Various factors such as the set of task con-
straints, goal-oriented decisions, motor synergies, optimal
feedback, previous learning experience, and the adapta-
tion processes influence general motor control
(Todorov & Jordan, 2002), feeding into an aggregate per-
formance in motor coordination tests. Scordella
2of15 BONDI ET AL.
et al. (2015) showed a functional common basis of gross-
motor and grapho-motor skills based on visualspatial
processes.
Such performances may be also differently influenced
by the fitness and anthropometric status as relevance can
be assumed in all coordinative motor skills (Augustijn
et al., 2018). Overweight children perform poorly on
motor tests and report greater body dissatisfaction and
lower self-efficacy than their nonoverweight peers
(Colella et al., 2009). Matarma et al. (2018) cogently dem-
onstrated that, in young children, gross-motor coordina-
tion was related to anthropometric status, while fine
motor coordination was not influenced by body weight.
However, D'Hondt et al. (2008) reported childhood obe-
sity to have a negative impact on fine motor perfor-
mances, both with or without a simultaneous mechanical
postural demand, suggesting a likely suffering from
underlying perceptual-motor coordination difficulties in
obese preschool children.
Among several determinant factors, Zaqout
et al. (2016) highlighted BMI, age and sex as the strongest
determinants of children's physical fitness, independent
of physical activity. Boys often overestimate and girls
underestimate their own competence regarding sports
practice; this mismatch between objective competence
and perceived competence may negatively influence
health outcomes and motor skills in particular (Pesce
et al., 2018). The perceptual judgment of sports rules is
related to actual play experience, and gender difference
influence the amount of sports experience (Lange-
Küttner & Bosco, 2016).
1.2 |The current study
Because of the onset of handwriting, fine motor skills
gained at preschool age are expected to change during
the first scholastic years, ensuring a new proactive con-
trol of manual tasks (Meulenbroek & Van Galen, 1988).
Thus, we explored the grapho-motor performance of
early school-age children.
Given that the expression of fine motor skills and
their development occur in different contexts and are
thus task-dependent, our analysis included fine and gross
motor tests as well as ancillary variables to assess the
different underlying motor pathways. We analyzed the
ability clusters of school children's graphonomics perfor-
mance in relation to fitness and anthropometric status, as
well as perceived competence. We also considered gender
and age in early school-age children from 6 to 8 years of
age. We hypothesized that the different domains of fine
motor skills, including those related to graphonomics,
were differently related to each other across age and
gender, as the extent and time course of fine motor skills
may have different development for boys and girls. We
hypothesized a link between physical characteristics and
performance with fine motor skills.
2|METHODS
2.1 |Participants
The original sample consisted of 239 children of the first
(all children of this grade were 6-to-7 years old) and sec-
ond (all children of this grade were 7-to-8 years old)
grade of First-Level School, living in the Abruzzo region,
Italy. No child was years ahead or behind. All children
had a similar preschool experience at local public
schools. All schools were following similar curricula, that
is, teaching of handwriting during the first and second
grades. A total of 13 school classes were involved,
according to school availability. All children were
enrolled in the classrooms with their age peers chrono-
logically matched. All children were invited to take part,
with neither inclusion nor exclusion criteria. The
number of boys and girls did not differ significantly,
χ
2
(1) =0.235, p=.628 (Table 1).
Participants' median weight and height were 25.9 kg
and 123 cm, respectively (24.9 kg and 121 cm in first
grade, 26.9 kg and 126 cm in second grade). Manual
dominance was evaluated as a self-report by asking chil-
dren to write their names. Splitting by manual domi-
nance, 88.1% of children were right-handed (RH) and
11.9% left-handed (LH). In particular, the ratio of LH
children was 14.4% in first grade and 8.9% in second
grade, 10.6% in girls and 13.1% in boys. Because we
focused on healthy motor behavior, the results of the six
children with a certificate of disability were excluded
from the data analysis. In this way, we were considering
the setting of an inclusive experience as they were
allowed to be part of the testing experience. In the
remaining sample there were no children with graphic or
writing problems.
We randomly selected a subgroup of seven school
classes to administer the Pictorial Scale of Perceived
TABLE 1 Boys and girls distribution across school grades
Grade
Gender First Second Total
Girls 60 52 112
Boys 72 55 127
Total 132 107 239
BONDI ET AL.3of15
Movement Skill Competence-2 (PMSC-2; Barnett,
Robinson, et al., 2015). This was due to time and logistic
constraints, Thus, only a subsample of n=95 children
(42% of the first grade and 58% of the second grade, 49%
boys and 51% girls, 88% RH and 12% LH) completed
this test.
2.2 |Materials and apparatus
The grapho-motor test was performed on a tablet PC, car-
ried out following an established procedure (Giammarco
et al., 2016) which involved a blank page with a square
and a diamond represented in four gray segments shown
on a tablet screen (Samsung Galaxy note, 10.1 in.,
1280 800). The software was developed by the authors
of the cited study (Giammarco et al., 2016) and got by
them, who also positively tested the retest reliability with
Primary school children. The size of the square and dia-
mond were large enough to cover the entire screen of the
tablet (see the original article for a deeper description
and images). Children were supposed to redraw the
shapes with a digital pen.
The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Movement Skill
Competence-2 (PMSC-2; Barnett, Robinson, et al., 2015)
was administered on printed A4 sheets. It is a widely
used pictorial scale used to assess perceived competence
on object controlhand competence skills (C), locomo-
tordynamic leg competence skills (L), and other skills
involved in free time, active play activities (A). Anthropo-
metric testing required a measuring tape and a portable
scale. Reliability of this scale was demonstrated for the
age group of the present study (internal consistency of
0.73 and testretest reliability of 0.83 for children aged 5
7 years; Barnett, Ridgers, et al., 2015; internal consistency
of 0.72 and testretest reliability of 0.73 for children aged
58 years: Barnett, Robinson, et al., 2015).
Fine motor coordination was assessed with two tests:
a transitive (tool-related) action test (Floppy) and an
intransitive (non-tool related) action test (Thumb). These
tests required a box of floppy disks and a stopwatch; they
have been already used for testing children of similar
ages (Bondi, Robazza, & Pietrangelo, 2020; Bondi,
Robazza, Russo, et al., 2020). For procedural details, see
the following sections.
2.3 |Procedure
The cross-sectional design encompassed physiological,
anthropometric, and motor measurements. The time
frame of our study consisted of 46 months after the
onset of the scholastic season. The study ethics was
approved by the Italian Olympic Committee (CONI) and
by the regional Board of Education of the Italian Ministry
of Education, University and Research (MIUR), Abruzzo
region, Italy. Permission was obtained by the school
administration and informed consent was signed by the
parents of the children. The study conformed to the Dec-
laration of Helsinki.
Children's motor skills were tested individually. Chil-
dren were assessed at their school in the presence of the
teachers, during regular physical education time. The
assessment of the subsample of N=95 children who
completed the PMSC-2 assessment procedure took
around 1015 min per child.
In the first data collection session, after the PMSC-2
was administered, we collected anthropometric mea-
sures and conducted the fitness test (Shuttle test, see
below). In the following data collection session, the
grapho-motor tablet task and the fine motor tasks were
given in random order. The testretest reliability of
Floppy and Thumb tests was assessed by the same
observer in three consecutive trials during this data col-
lection session: the Intraclass Correlation Coefficients
(ICCs) were good-to-excellent (Koo & Li, 2016), see
Table 2. Therefore, the data of first repetition were used
for further analyses. The total testing time of the second
data collection session was about 15 min per child, plus
5 min for retest.
Of the entire sample, we collected height, weight,
and waist circumference as anthropometric data, then
we calculated body mass index (BMI) and waist-to-
height ratio (WtHR; Mamen & Fredriksen, 2018). We
assessed speed and agility through the 4 10 m shuttle
run test (Shuttle; Ruiz et al., 2011), that is a running
and turning test at greatest speed: briefly, two parallel
lines are drawn on the floor 10 m apart; the child runs
from the starting to the other line and returns with a
sponge; the sponge is changed by another sponge, then
the child goes back running to the opposite line and
changes the sponge by another one and finally runs
back to the start. The time to complete the task was
the outcome variable.
The grapho-motor test started with a familiarization
phase and thereafter consisted of two trials: the first trial
TABLE 2 Reliability coefficients of floppy and thumb
tests (N=233)
Test Grade
First Second Overall
Floppy 0.95 0.76 0.88
Thumb 0.96 0.90 0.93
4of15 BONDI ET AL.
required to trace as accurate as possible (Best); the second
trial required to trace as accurate as possible with a
higher velocity (Fast). All lines drawn with the pen on
the tablet screen were stored as strokes. The pressure and
the speed exerted on the pen were calculated as scores of
the average over the length of lines. For both trials, we
acquired the measures of the number of Strokes, the Pres-
sure, the Speed as well as the mean oscillation (quadratic
deviation of the points) from the graphic line (Quality).
Recording data were processed whenever the digital pen
exerted a pressure on the screen. Using this tool
permitted us to simulate the effective functional parame-
ters of drawing and to measure the required objective
parameters.
The Floppy test was used to assess manual dexterity
(Martzog et al., 2019). It required participants to insert
12 floppy disks one at a time in the floppy box, as fast as
possible, holding the box while inserting the disks
(to provide a different role for each hand). The rationale
for using this test was based on manual dexterity assess-
ment and, with respect to similar testslike the coins
task of M-ABC 2 batterythe present test was not
affected by motor experience of common tasks (Zoia
et al., 2018). This test required a strategy to better carry
out the task, with increased difficulty during execution
(as the disks are placed into the box, the empty space is
reduced).
The Thumb testwasusedtoassessupperlimb
coordination, as well as speed-dominated skills
(Martzog et al., 2019). Participants were required to
touch each finger of one hand with the thumb of the
same hand in an alternating pattern (5th, 4th, 3rd, and
then 2nd finger, and reverse), as fast as possible. The
rationale for using this test was based on the visual-
motor control assessment of the BOT-2 battery
(Bruininks & Bruininks, 2005) which is a common task
in clinical assessment (fingerthumb test) and motor
rehabilitation. As for the Floppy test, the outcome vari-
able was time of completing as measured with a
stopwatch.
2.4 |Data generation
We conducted an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) on
four tablet test parameters, see Table 3, with a Maximum
Likelihood rotation. We obtained only one fine motor skills
factor, based on an eigenvalue >1 and on the scree plot.
We then conducted a Confirmatory Structural
Equation Modeling (SEM), considering the expected cor-
relations, and putting into the model the factor from the
EFA in Best condition, the same factor in Fast condition,
the hypothesized relationships between Best and Fast
conditions. We tested age and gender in the model, eval-
uating fit measures to choose the best model. Such mea-
sures suggested including gender and the correlation
between Speed and Quality in Best condition. With more
than two predictors, it is possible to obtain values larger
than +1, as the value of the path between TPB and
Speed-B. Error calculation method: Robust; estimator:
Diagonally Weighted Least Squares (DWLS). Fit mea-
sures: RMSEA =.042, SRMR =.050, TLI =.987,
CFI =.995, GFI =.989; χ
2
(9, 239) =11.56, p=.239.
TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet Performance
Fast; B: Best; F: Fast. Four equations were produced:
Girls TPB:Quality +(38.28 Speed)(11.62
Strokes)
Girls TPF:Quality +(65.10 Speed)(2.76
Strokes)
Boys TPB:Quality +(55.66 Speed)(22.78
Strokes)
Boys TPF:Quality +(70.04 Speed)(2.22
Strokes)
We evaluated the fit measures for different models,
concluding that the best model was the one shown in
Figure 1.
We applied the assumption of normality and multi-
variate normality, and subsequently a nonparanormal
transformation (Liu et al., 2009) before conducting corre-
lations. The coefficients in Figure 1were used to build
unweighted undirected graphs. The analysis revealed that
gender had to be taken into account when defining the
structural model of tracing performance. Finally, the four
equations were used to calculate TPB and TPF, separately
for both boys and girls.
2.5 |Data analysis
Statistical analyses were carried out using GraphPad
Prism Software, version 9 (GraphPad Software, La Jolla,
USA), R-based open-source software Jamovi (https://
www.jamovi.org) and Jasp (https://jasp-stats.org). Data
TABLE 3 Exploratory factor analysis for four tablet test
parameters (N=233)
Factor
1
Strokes 0.749
Pressure /
Speed 0.997
Quality 0.572
Note: Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) =.102; Tucker-
Lewis index (TLI) =.947; χ
2
(2, 239) =5.97, p=.050.
BONDI ET AL.5of15
distributions were analyzed using ShapiroWilk and
D'Agostino-Pearson methods. Analysis of outliers was
conducted with the ROUT method, implemented in
Prism (Motulsky & Brown, 2006). An alpha value of .05
was considered as statistically significant.
Considering the assumption check, robust ANOVA
and robust t-test were chosen to run the comparison
tests. In particular, a series of robust ANOVAs
(median method) were carried out to test the gender
age effect on TPB,TPF,Shuttle,Floppy,Thumb,
BMI,WtHR,Control,Locomotor,Active play.Incase
of a significant interaction effect, post-hoc analyses
with Tukey correction for multiple comparisons were
conducted. Robust t-tests (Yuen's test with 0.1
trimmed proportion and median method), in addition
to the nonparametric MannWhitney Utest, were car-
ried out for the left versus right-side manual domi-
nance effect on TPB,TPF,Shuttle,Floppy,Thumb,
BMI,andWtHR.
The links between graphonomic outcomes, other fine
motor skills, fitness, and anthropometric status, as well
as perceived competence, were addressed with network
models, using correlations as estimators. Further network
analyses were conducted with TPB,TPF,Shuttle,Floppy,
Thumb,BMI and WtHR, splitting by age and gender.
PMSC-2 parameters were excluded from the split
networks because of the smaller sample sizes of the sub-
groups. For the details of network analysis, refer to the
figure legends.
3|RESULTS
The BMI values of participants were compared to the
normative values of providedbytheWorldHealth
Organization (WHO, 2007). Our P50 values ranged
between normative P50 and P75 matching for the
same gender and age: BMI of our participants was
therefore higher than this reference. We further classi-
fied the children according to the obesity cut-off
norms of P97 from the WHO reference (Valerio
et al., 2018). As a result, 24.76% of children in our
sample had obesity.
The WtHR values of our participants were compared
to the recent values reported by Santomauro et al. (2017)
on Italian children from Tuscany, a region in central
Italy, north of Abruzzo region. Our P50 values ranged
between their P50 and P75 matching for the same gender
and age. Therefore, WtHR values of our participants were
higher than this reference. We further characterized the
participants according to the health hazard cut-off of 0.50
(Maffeis et al., 2008). We found 14.35% of children to be
at metabolic risk.
Shuttle performances of participants were compared
to international norms for this task (Kolimechkov
et al., 2019). Median of boys in first grade were between
P30 and P40 of that reference, girls in first grade were
between P50 and P60; boys in second grade were between
P50 and P60, girls in second grade were between P60 and
P70. Shuttle performance was therefore overall higher
than this reference, except for boys in the school entrance
class.
PMSC-2 values of participants were compared to two
different samples of school children data reported by
Barnett, Robinson, et al. (2015). Australian children rated
on average 19.9 in object control subscale (C) and 20.7 in
locomotor subscale (L); American children rated 20.5 in
Cand 20.7 in L; present study, children rated 20.4 in
Cand 22.2 in L. Participants of our study therefore rated
higher on locomotor subscale of PMSC-2 than these ref-
erence values.
3.1 |Comparison by gender and grade
Group means are shown in Tables 4and 5. Results were
analyzed through a series of robust ANOVAs with the
median method. All the pvalues of statistical compari-
sons are shown in Tables S1 and S2, Supporting
FIGURE 1 Confirmatory structural equation modeling (SEM)
of tablet test parameters. P: Percentile; TPB: Tablet Performance
Best; TPF: Tablet Performance Fast; BMI: Body mass index; the
showed numbers are the standardized coefficient (standardizing
both label and observed variables)
6of15 BONDI ET AL.
Information. Box and whiskers plots of the results split
by gender and grade are shown in Figure 2
Overall, the higher the speed of the pen on the screen,
the lower the quality of drawing. Regarding the tablet
test, we found strong differences by gender in both the
TPB and TPF (Q
1,188
=23.50, p< .001, partial η
2
=.239;
Q
1,165
=59.52, p< .001, partial η
2
=.376). The gender
age interaction was not significant (p=.127 and
p=.323, respectively).
Task-specific analyses showed that older children
(Mdn =14.31 s) performed better than younger ones
(Mdn =15.78 s) in the Shuttle test which measures the
fitness domains of speed and agility (Q
1,204
=27.17, p<
.001, partial η
2
=.139). Older children (Q
1,178
=15.33,
p=.040, partial η
2
=.042) and girls (Q
1,178
=4.22,
p< .001, partial η
2
=.042) performed better in the Floppy
test which measures fine coordination on a tool-related
(namely transitive) action. Both boys and girls performed
better with age in the Thumb test (Q
1,191
=df1 =1,
df2 =191, p< .001, partial η
2
=.053) which measures
fine coordination of fingers on one hand without holding
a tool (namely intransitive action).
There was a main effect of gender on the WtHR (girls
showed a higher WtHR than boys, Q
1,200
=4.74,
p=.029, partial η
2
=.024), and a significant two-way
interaction (Q
1,198
=8.17, p=.004, partial η
2
=.037);
post-hoc analysis revealed no significant difference
between boys and girls in the first grade, whereas boys
showed higher WtHR values (Mdn =0.48) than girls
(Mdn =0.45) in the second grade (p
Tukey
=.005, Cohen's
d=0.699).
There was a difference between boys and girls by sex
in the BMI (gender age comparison: Q
1,199
=6.49,
p=.011, partial η
2
=.035); post-hoc analysis revealed no
significant difference in the first grade, but a tendency for
girls to show lower BMI values (Mdn =16.5 kg/m
2
) than
boys (Mdn =18.0 kg/m
2
) in the second grade
(p
Tukey
=.082, Cohen's d=0.496).
For what concerns the PMSC-2 test, boys perceived
their object control skills to be higher than girls
TABLE 4 Results of the comparisons by grade and gender, N=239
Graphonomics Fine motor skills Anthropometrics Fitness
Grade TPB TPF Floppy time (s) Thumb time (s) BMI (kg/m
2
) WtHR Shuttle time (s)
Q1 Girls First 979 1794 19.9 8.62 15.7 0.443 15.2
Second 832 1638 19.2 7.53 15.5 0.429 13.6
Boys First 413 906 21.1 8.58 15.9 0.447 14.5
Second 348 978 19.6 7.89 16.2 0.465 13.1
Median Girls First 1292 2424 22.8 10.00 17.2 0.468 15.8
Second 1291 2080 20.0 8.29 16.5 0.448 14.4
Boys First 649 1191 24.2 10.6 16.7 0.464 15.6
Second 848 1115 21.5 9.29 18.0 0.478 13.9
Q3 Girls First 1740 2908 25.4 11.6 19.4 0.492 16.6
Second 1643 2590 22.7 9.81 18.9 0.468 15.8
Boys First 1011 1711 27.5 12.4 17.9 0.486 16.9
Second 1066 1489 24.3 10.8 20.4 0.499 15.2
Median (whole sample) 983 1643 22.4 9.4 16.9 0.466 15.1
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; Q, quartile; TPB, Tablet Performance Best; TPF, Tablet Performance Fast; WtHR, waist-to-height ratio.
TABLE 5 Results of the comparisons in PMSC questionnaire
by grade and gender, N=95
Grade CLA
Q1 Girls First 16.0 20.5 17.0
Second 19.0 21.0 17.0
Boys First 20.0 20.0 15.0
Second 20.5 22.5 16.0
Mdn Girls First 19.0 23.0 19.0
Second 20.5 23.0 20.0
Boys First 21.0 23.0 18.5
Second 22.0 23.0 19.0
Q3 Girls First 21.5 24.0 22.0
Second 21.0 24.0 23.0
Boys First 23.0 24.0 22.8
Second 23.0 24.0 21.5
Mdn (whole sample) 21.0 23.0 19.0
Abbreviations:A, active play; C, object control; L, locomotor; Mdn, median;
Q, quartile.
BONDI ET AL.7of15
(Mdn =22 vs. 20; Q
1,93
=4.45, p=.035, partial
η
2
=.093), see Table 5, while no significant differences
were found in Locomotor or Active play subscales.
3.2 |Network analysis
To analyze the synchronous relationships between vari-
ables, we conducted a network analysis. In Figures 2
and 3, the task components that refer to one test are
depicted in the same color. Network clusters are mar-
ked by the connections between these components,
with thicker lines showing stronger connections. The
analysis of the sample of N=95 revealed three funda-
mental clusters. One is the PMSC-2 cluster, with the
three subscales strongly linked to each other with the
following weights: Locomotor and Control =.851,
Locomotor and Active play =.894, Control and Active
play =.879. The second is the Fitness cluster,withthe
anthropometric measures and the Shuttle test showing
moderate links of the Shuttle test with both BMI
(weight =.396) and WtHR (weight =.399), and a
strong link between WtHR and BMI (weight =.902).
Finally, the third fundamental cluster is the Fine Motor
Control cluster, with moderate links between the differ-
ent tablet and floppy parameters.
In particular, the following weights were observed:
Floppy and Thumb =.589, Floppy and TPB =.547,
Floppy and TPF =.368, Thumb and TPB =.522, Thumb
and TPF =.510, TPB and TPF =.433. The centrality
measures revealed Floppy to be the most central,
although weakly, node of the network (see Table 6).
FIGURE 2 Box and whiskers plots of anthropometric and motor skills results. Each of the three panels represent min-to-max lines and
median with IQR boxes. For each dataset, girls (white boxes) and boys (gray boxes) results are shown split by school grade
FIGURE 3 Network plot of the 10 parameters. The bolder the
line, the higher the weight of the correlation between nodes. Nodes
of the same colors belong to the same group, according to
clustering measures of the analysis. Nodes are positioned using the
Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm, based on the strength of
connections. TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet
Performance Fast; BMI: Body mass index; WtHR: Waist-to-height
ratio
TABLE 6 Centrality measures per variable in overall network
analysis
Closeness Strength
Active play 0.091 0.907
BMI 0.859 0.410
Control 0.180 1.031
Floppy 1.407 0.019
Locomotor 0.788 1.414
Shuttle 0.604 0.997
TPB 0.729 0.831
TPF 2.121 1.736
Thumb 0.365 0.428
WtHR 0.305 0.250
Note:Closenessrefers to the inverse of the peripherality(the sum of the
shortest paths starting from the node of interest), strengthrefers to the
centrality degree(sum of absolute weights) of the node of interest.
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; TPB, Tablet Performance Best; TPF,
Tablet Performance Fast; WtHR, waist-to-height ratio.
8of15 BONDI ET AL.
We then ran the networks split by gender and age
which revealed different models for boys and girls, see
Figure 4. Considering the size of subsamples, we run this
model without the aim to infer results, but to provide
pilot evidence to be possibly confirmed by ad hoc
designed studies.
The Shuttle, WtHR, and BMI were strongly linked
in girls of both grades, but only to some degree in the
younger group of boys. Thumb and Floppy were more
linked in second than in first grade in girls showing a
growing network of fine motor skills, but in boys,
thumb and floppy were linked in both grades. More-
over, in boys, the tablet parameters became linked with
thumbs and floppy task showing a generalization of
fine motor expertise extending to computers. In con-
trast, in the older grade of girls, fast tablet motions
were not networked anymore.
4|DISCUSSION
A structured analysis of various motor tasks provides a
better understanding of the motor tasks. In this regard,
network analyses allow going beyond the typical correla-
tion analyses, thanks to centrality measures and cluster-
ing. We examined the important transition into school
when fine motor skills become essential for learning how
to write. We addressed the structured links between
grapho-motor performances and other fine motor skills,
speed and agility, anthropometrics, and self-perception of
motor skills in early school-age children. Our result dem-
onstrated that, first, grapho-motor skills varied between
boys and girls and exhibited gender-related differences,
second, that three clusters (namely, perceived compe-
tence, fitness, and fine motor skills) emerged from the
network model, and third, that the interconnectedness of
FIGURE 4 Network plots split by gender and school grade. The drawings reflect the strength, the closeness, and the clustering of the
variables. The bolder the line, the higher the weight of the correlation between nodes. Nodes of the same colors belong to the same group,
according to clustering measures of the analysis. Nodes are positioned in space using the Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm, based on the
strength of connections. TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet Performance Fast; BMI: Body mass index; WtHR: Waist-to-height ratio
BONDI ET AL.9of15
fine motor skills with other domains may change with
age and gender during early school age.
4.1 |Fine motor skills, cognitive
functions, and tasks
Our results showed that when the strategy to carry out
the grapho-motor task was focused on accuracy, this
reduced the speed of the graphical task which confirms
the finding by Giammarco et al. (2016) (see also Lange-
Küttner, 1998) who observed that the slower the speed,
the better is the quality. The speed accuracy trade-off has
been studied widely, with evidence also in graphic
tasks for school age children (e.g., Smits-Engelsman
et al., 2006). Looking at the underlying factors of
increased accuracy, we refer to Simpson et al. (2017),
who suggested that inhibition is the key ability that
enables children to draw recognizable objects. Inhibition
may be evoked, for instance, when stopping drawing a
line of overlapping figures (hidden line elimination)
(e.g., Chen & Holman, 1989; Lange-Küttner, 2000). This
possible inhibition requirement was here particularly evi-
dent in boys, although further research should confirm
this finding with a study designed to discriminate slow vs
fast tasks. The reason can be found in the gender-based
differences in visuo-motor integration. For instance, con-
trolling oscillations in tracing performance varies in boys
but not in girls (Giammarco et al., 2016; Tabatabaey-
Mashadi et al., 2015). Drawing development also differs
between boys and girls in the near-far conceptualization
of pictorial space as boys drew silhouettes as they would
be visible from afar, while girls focused on ornaments
that could be seen close-up (Lange-Küttner, 2011).
We could prove that the development of fine motor
skills varies in boys and girls showing gender and age-
related clusters that added further learning-related evi-
dence to the field of graphonomics and grapho-motor
performances. Beyond the strong individual motivation
in the aim of accomplishing the motor task, both
strategy and adaptation to the velocity constraint were
gender-related. Fine motor skills seemed to be already
networked in girls at the beginning of school but became
linked and generalized in boys in the second year
supported the view that sex differences; this result should
be considered to understand performance strategies and
underlying motor, cognitive, and neuronal patterns. This
showed how a task can be solved by the two genders with
different strategies and may shed new understanding on
development of competence (Lange-Küttner, 2017).
Indeed, also in the current study, strategy and perfor-
mance on fine motor control tests were task-related. The
link between the two tablet parameters and the two fine
coordinative tests were only small and changed with
regards to age and gender, as shown in the networks.
This dynamic perspective was also shown by Snapp-
Childs and colleagues who reported specific training
effects to eliminate drawing performance differences
between ages (78vs1012-year old children) and differ-
ent coordination skill levels (Snapp-Childs et al., 2015).
4.2 |Observed and perceived physical
performances
Our results of Shuttle test were comparable to findings of
the study of Kolimechkov et al. (2019). Instead, our par-
ticipants' data were slightly worse on anthropometric
indexes (Santomauro et al., 2017; WHO, 2007). In partic-
ular, the high percentage of children over the limits of
overweight and health hazard may highlight an alarming
situation for public health, with the need for better inter-
ventions. However, we found that even if our participants
performed quite well on the Shuttle test, they could nev-
ertheless have a poor anthropometric status. The moder-
ate positive weights revealed by network analysis of
Shuttle with both BMI and WtHR nevertheless support
the call for policy approaches to improve anthropometric
status as a modifiable factor to enhance physical fitness
(Zaqout et al., 2016).
Notwithstanding the anthropometric status, our par-
ticipants reported high values of perceived movement
skills competence. Fine and gross motor skills, fitness
performance and morpho-functional parameters repre-
sent necessary modules to be evaluated objectively in
order to define developmental trajectories (Bondi, Di
Sano, Verratti, et al., 2020). Assessment of perceived com-
petence is a measure that integrates such evaluations
from a subjective point of view and allows to focus physi-
cal education on the possible mismatch between objec-
tive and perceived skills. In our study, boys scored higher
than girls on the object control sub-scale; this result
agrees with the findings of Estevan et al. (2018) on Span-
ish children. However, this higher self-evaluation is not
accompanied by better objective results: ours and other
results (Flatters et al., 2014) showed that young girls per-
form better than young boys on manual skills. Further
studies should therefore increase the knowledge about
gender differences in the relationship between objective
and subjective motor skills, the developmental course of
these linkages, sports and social consequences and the
related educational interventions. A careful examination
of the alignment of the objective skills and children's
subjective ratings will help to strengthen the knowledge
on these topics. Beyond the subjective measures of
perceived skills, further studies should address also some
10 of 15 BONDI ET AL.
parameters of the biopsychosocial framework (Bortoli
et al., 2018) and some parameters of personal feeling such
as enjoyment in physical activity experiences (Carraro
et al., 2008).
4.3 |Fine motor skills in children's
development
We consider fine motor control as an emergent issue in
the understanding of motor behavior for auxological
(auxology is the science of human growth), educational
and kinesiological studies. The wide range of expressions
of motor control suggests considering all the related sub-
domains. The current study supports the argument of
addressing the several fine motor skills, in addition to
gross motor skills, for children.
Improvement of fine motor skills in children is spe-
cific to practice of fine motor skills, even in an education-
oriented household (Suggate et al., 2017). However, fine
motor coordination was demonstrated in two studies to
be of more importance for math than for literacy because
of the visualspatial imagery required in both pictorial
and mathematical space (Pitchford et al., 2016). Thus,
one could predict that stimulation of fine motor skills
may even be relevant for the improvement of higher cog-
nitive skills in the prepubertal period. More attention
should be directed toward this field, providing a wide
range of different motor experiences. Although being a
fundamental learning experience in the beginning of
school when learning to write. Fine motor skills repre-
sent a fundamental learning experience in the beginning
of school when learning to write. However, children
rarely choose to use these skills in other learning contexts
such as free play, independently of whether their fine
motor skills are well, or less well developed (Marr
et al., 2004). We should also take advantage by acquiring
longitudinal track records of fine motor coordination
data, in the same way as already reported for gross-motor
coordination (Reikerås et al., 2017; Vandorpe
et al., 2012). Thus, both higher attention on fine-
coordinative skills in early education and implementa-
tion of gross-coordinative activities and evaluation need
more articulated planning and organization of physical
education, addressing motor control and learning as a
whole.
Motor control also reflects gender differences in fine
motor coordination through the developmental process.
Reikerås et al. (2017) showed girls to have a more pro-
nounced and anticipatory fine motor coordination than
boys. Also Flatters et al. (2014) reported superior man-
ual control abilities in prepubescent girls performing
novel tasks. We contributed to the field showing that
the relationship between fine motor coordination and
other domains is different by age and gender in early
school age. Both the clustering of nodes of fine motor
control and the clustering of physical fitness with
anthropometric measures were likely influenced by gen-
der, as shown in the differential networks by the differ-
ences in both the nodes positioning and the weight of
edges. This topic is worth further investigating with
longitudinal studies and we thus suggest that the rela-
tionships between motor tasks themselves and other
physical outcomes might follow gender-based develop-
ment trajectories. Also training studies (van Abswoude
et al., 2019) may be interesting to the emerging field of
research in fine motor skills.
Cognition and motor skills are interrelated at the
level of brain structure, supported by similar cortical neu-
ral substrates (Pangelinan et al., 2011). Therefore, further
studies are needed to better characterize the relationships
of morphological and neural connections with different
coordinative domains, and the advantages of optimizing
sensorymotor integration during precisely controlled
hand movements to foster the learning of fine motor
skills (Ose Askvik et al., 2020; van der Meer & van der
Weel, 2017). Interesting findings may result from studies
focused on evaluating differences based on handedness
and asymmetries, as they have been shown to be
related to the type of manual task exerted (Bondi,
Prete, Malatesta, & Robazza, 2020; Bondi, Robazza, &
Pietrangelo, 2020).
4.4 |Limitations
The study did not come without limitations, such as the
absence of data about social background and academic
performance. One other gap in the assessment may have
been that we did not measure previous use of digital
devices that might have affected fine motor skills
(Petrigna et al., 2021). Thus, the results of the study rec-
ommend not only including fine motor skills in early
school age research, but also studying them as a special
cluster.
5|CONCLUSIONS
Our network analysis revealed three specific clusters:
perceived competence, fitness, and fine motor skills.
Given the relative independence of these physical per-
formance areas, we suggest focusing on these three
clusters as separate areas in physical education. Fine
motor skills deserve further consideration in early
school age, since these skills are important in a variety
BONDI ET AL.11 of 15
of tasks like sorting objects, handwriting, and handling
digital media. The latter has become even more urgent
during distant learning in the Covid lockdowns. Digital
devices represent fashionable and effective tools to be
used under an educational perspective; in remote
teaching times, teaching and evaluation of academic
homework inevitably require digital devices for moni-
toring and developing children's fine motor skills.
Whether learning to write and thus increased fine
motor control influences manual dominance is still an
open question and further studies should provide clear
insights.
Motor development can be interpreted in the para-
digm of Dynamic System Theory (Smith & Thelen,
2003): person, task, and environment interact dynami-
cally and critical changes in one or more subsystems can
produce improvements in the resulting performance.
From this perspective, the human movement system is a
complex network of subsystems consisting of a plethora
of interacting components (Glazier et al., 2003). The con-
straint of repetition in task-specific practice greatly
enhances related skills and has more or less potential to
be transferred to other skills. Even a slight change in
one component of the dynamic system can lead to reor-
ganization of structure and developmental change
(Smith & Thelen, 2003). Therefore, both interventions
and evaluations need to be carefully and finely tuned.
Specific evidence of several motor tests can lead to a
proposal of sentinelsfor early identification of various
neural and motor deficits. Network research provides an
effective framework for discussing fine and gross motor
skills during developmental age; network models can aid
better modeling of motor tasks and design of smart class-
rooms, while providing insights into novel interactive
technologies.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Danilo Bondi: Conceptualization (lead); formal analysis
(lead); investigation (lead); methodology (lead); resources
(supporting); visualization (lead); writing original draft
(lead); writing review and editing (equal). Claudio
Robazza: Conceptualization (supporting); methodology
(supporting); project administration (equal); resources
(lead); supervision (lead); writing original draft
(supporting); writing review and editing (equal).
Christiane Lange-Küttner: Supervision (supporting);
writing original draft (equal); writing review and
editing (lead). Tiziana Pietrangelo: Conceptualization
(equal); funding acquisition (lead); investigation
(supporting); methodology (supporting); project adminis-
tration (equal); resources (supporting); supervision (lead);
writing original draft (supporting); writing review
and editing (supporting).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank PE experts Sara Ienni
and Maristella Melfi who helped with tests and logistics.
The authors would also like to thank teachers and both
scholastic and sports administrations. Special thanks go
to all the children involved in the study. This work was
supported by a G. d'AnnunzioUniversity grant, by
2012N8YJC3_003 PRIN National Grant to Tiziana
Pietrangelo and by the Departments of Excellence
2018-2022initiative of the Italian Ministry of Education,
University and Research for the Department of Neurosci-
ence, Imaging and Clinical Sciences (DNISC) of the Uni-
versity of Chieti-Pescara. Open Access Funding provided
by Universita degli Studi Gabriele d'Annunzio Chieti
Pescara within the CRUI-CARE Agreement.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no competing interests.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are avail-
able from the corresponding author upon reasonable
request.
ORCID
Danilo Bondi https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1911-3606
REFERENCES
Augustijn, M. J. C. M., Deconinck, F. J. A., D'Hondt, E., Van
Acker, L., De Guchtenaere, A., Lenoir, M., & Caeyenberghs, K.
(2018). Reduced motor competence in children with obesity is
associated with structural differences in the cerebellar pedun-
cles. Brain Imaging and Behavior,12(4), 10001010. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11682-017-9760-5
Barnett, L. M., Ridgers, N. D., Zask, A., & Salmon, J. (2015). Face
validity and reliability of a pictorial instrument for assessing
fundamental movement skill perceived competence in young
children. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport,18(1), 98
102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2013.12.004
Barnett, L. M., Robinson, L. E., Webster, E. K., & Ridgers, N. D.
(2015). Reliability of the pictorial scale of perceived movement
skill competence in 2 diverse samples of Young children. Jour-
nal of Physical Activity & Health,12(8), 10451051. https://doi.
org/10.1123/jpah.2014-0141
Barnett, L. M., van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L. O., &
Beard, J. R. (2009). Childhood motor skill proficiency as a pre-
dictor of adolescent physical activity. The Journal of Adolescent
Health,44(3), 252259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.
2008.07.004
Bondi, D., Di Sano, S., Verratti, V., Neri, G., Aureli, T., &
Pietrangelo, T. (2020). New insight on motor behavior: The link
between the hopping task and the tracing performance as hint
of gross and fine motor functions. Motor Control,24(3), 349
364. https://doi.org/10.1123/mc.2019-0044
Bondi, D., Prete, G., Malatesta, G., & Robazza, C. (2020). Laterality
in children: Evidence for task-dependent lateralization of motor
12 of 15 BONDI ET AL.
functions. International Journal of Environmental Research
and Public Health,17(18), 6705. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ijerph17186705
Bondi, D., Robazza, C., & Pietrangelo, T. (2020). The assessment of
task-dependent manual laterality in second grade students.
Turkish Journal of Sports Medicine.,55, 239245. https://doi.
org/10.5152/tjsm.2020.182
Bondi, D., Robazza, C., Russo, E., Russo, P., & Pietrangelo, T.
(2020). Monitoring physical and motor traits in primary school:
A local harmful situation for older children. The Journal of
Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness.,61, 428434. https://doi.
org/10.23736/S0022-4707.20.11251-9
Bortoli, L., Vitali, F., Di Battista, R., Ruiz, M. C., & Robazza, C.
(2018). Initial validation of the Psychobiosocial states in physi-
cal education (PBS-SPE) scale. Frontiers in Psychology,9.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02446
Bruininks, R. H., & Bruininks, B. D. (2005). Bruininks-Oseretsky test
of motor proficiency (Second ed.). AGS Publishing. https://www.
pearsonclinical.com/therapy/products/100000648/bruininks-
oseretsky-test-of-motor-proficiency-second-edition-bot-2.html
Carraro, A., Young, M. C., & Robazza, C. (2008). A contribution to
the validation of the physical activity enjoyment scale in an
Italian sample. Social Behavior and Personality,36(7), 911918.
https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2008.36.7.911
Chen, M. J., & Holman, J. (1989). Emergence of drawing devices for
Total and partial occlusion: A longitudinal study. Perception,
18(4), 445455. https://doi.org/10.1068/p180445
Colella, D., Morano, M., Robazza, C., & Bortoli, L. (2009). Body
image, perceived physical ability, and motor performance in
nonoverweight and overweight Italian children. Perceptual and
Motor Skills,108(1), 209218. https://doi.org/10.2466/PMS.108.
1.209-218
D'Hondt, E., Deforche, B., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Lenoir, M.
(2008). Childhood obesity affects fine motor skill performance
under different postural constraints. Neuroscience Letters,
440(1), 7275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.05.056
Dinkel, D., & Snyder, K. (2020). Exploring gender differences in
infant motor development related to parent's promotion of play.
Infant Behavior & Development,59, 101440. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.infbeh.2020.101440
Escolano-Pérez, E., S
anchez-L
opez, C. R., & Herrero-Nivela, M. L.
(2021). Early environmental and biological influences on pre-
school motor skills: Implications for early childhood care and
education. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2021.725832
Estevan, I., Molina-García, J., Abbott, G., Bowe, S. J.,
Castillo, I., & Barnett, L. M. (2018). Evidence of reliability
and validity for the pictorial scale of perceived movement
skill competence in Spanish children. Journal of Motor
Learning and Development,6(s2), S205S222. https://doi.org/
10.1123/jmld.2016-0065
Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development,
competency, and intervention. Developmental Medicine and
Child Neurology,49(4), 312317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
8749.2007.00312.x
Flatters, I., Hill, L. J. B., Williams, J. H. G., Barber, S. E., & Mon-
Williams, M. (2014). Manual control age and sex differences in
4 to 11 year old children. PLoS One,9(2), e88692. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088692
Gaul, D., & Issartel, J. (2016). Fine motor skill proficiency in typi-
cally developing children: On or off the maturation track?
Human Movement Science,46,7885. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
humov.2015.12.011
Ghassabian, A., Sundaram, R., Bell, E., Bello, S. C., Kus, C., &
Yeung, E. (2016). Gross motor milestones and subsequent
development. Pediatrics,138(1), e20154372. https://doi.org/10.
1542/peds.2015-4372
Giammarco, E., Di Sano, S., Aureli, T., Cerratti, P., Fanò-Illic, G., &
Pietrangelo, T. (2016). Psychological and physiological pro-
cesses in figure-tracing abilities measured using a tablet com-
puter: A study with 7 and 9 years old children. Frontiers in
Psychology,7.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01528
Glazier, P. S., Davids, K., & Bartlett, R. M. (2003). Dynamical sys-
tems theory: A relevant framework for performance-oriented
sports biomechanics research. Sportscience,7.http://www.
sportsci.org/jour/03/psg.htm
Griffiths, A., Toovey, R., Morgan, P. E., & Spittle, A. J. (2018). Psy-
chometric properties of gross motor assessment tools for chil-
dren: A systematic review. BMJ Open,8(10), e021734. https://
doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-021734
Kolimechkov, S., Petrov, L., & Alexandrova, A. (2019). Alpha-fit test
battery norms for children and adolescent from 5 to 18 years of
age obtained by a linear interpolation of existing European
physical fitness references. European Journal of Physical Educa-
tion and Sport Science,5(4), 1-14. https://oapub.org/edu/index.
php/ejep/article/view/2221
Koo, T. K., & Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting
Intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Jour-
nal of Chiropractic Medicine,15(2), 155163. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012
Lange-Küttner, C. (1998). Pressure, velocity, and time in speeded
drawing of basic graphic patterns by young children. Perceptual
and Motor Skills,86(3 Pt 2), 12991310. https://doi.org/10.
2466/pms.1998.86.3c.1299
Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violation in the devel-
opment of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills,90(1), 3
24. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2000.90.1.3
Lange-Küttner, C. (2011). Sex differences in visual realism in draw-
ings of animate and inanimate objects. Perceptual and Motor
Skills,113(2), 439453. https://doi.org/10.2466/04.10.24.PMS.
113.5.439-453
Lange-Küttner, C. (2017). Sex differences as a statistical variable,
Sex differences as a statistical variable. In Gender differences in
different contexts. InTech. https://www.intechopen.com/books/
gender-differences-in-different-contexts/sex-differences-as-a-
statistical-variable
Lange-Küttner, C., & Bosco, G. (2016). On being in the wrong place:
The role of Children's conceptual understanding and ballgame
experience when judging a football player's offside position.
International Journal of Developmental Science,10(12), 7384.
https://doi.org/10.3233/DEV-160185
Lange-Küttner, C., Kerzmann, A., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). The
emergence of visually realistic contour in the drawing of the
human figure. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
20(3), 439463. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002320620415
Lange-Küttner, C., & Kochhar, R. (2020). Fine motor skills and
unsystematic spatial binding in the common region test:
Under-inclusivity in autism Spectrum disorder and
BONDI ET AL.13 of 15
over-inclusivity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Jour-
nal of Motor Learning and Development,8(3), 544568. https://
doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2019-0033
Liu, H., Lafferty, J., & Wasserman, L. (2009). The nonparanormal:
Semiparametric estimation of high dimensional undirected
graphs. The Journal of Machine Learning Research,10, 2295
2328.
Lockman, J. J., & Kahrs, B. A. (2017). New insights into the devel-
opment of human tool use. Current Directions in Psychological
Science,26(4), 330334. https://doi.org/10.1177/096372141
7692035
Maffeis, C., Banzato, C., & Talamini, G. (2008). Waist-to-height
ratio, a useful index to identify high metabolic risk in over-
weight children. The Journal of Pediatrics,152(2), 207213.e2.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.09.021
Mamen, A., & Fredriksen, P. M. (2018). Anthropometric measures
as fitness indicators in primary school children: The health ori-
ented pedagogical project (HOPP). Scandinavian Journal of
Public Health,46(21_suppl), 4853. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1403494818770129
Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S., & Henderson, A. (2004). The
relationship between fine-motor play and fine-motor skill.
NHSA Dialog,7(1), 8496. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s19309325nhsa0701_7
Martzog, P., Stoeger, H., & Suggate, S. (2019). Relations between
preschool children's fine motor skills and general cognitive
abilities. Journal of Cognition and Development,20(4), 443465.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1607862
Marzke, M. W. (1992). Evolutionary development of the human
thumb. Hand Clinics,8(1), 18.
Matarma, T., Lagström, H., Hurme, S., Tammelin, T. H.,
Kulmala, J., Barnett, L. M., & Koski, P. (2018). Motor skills in
association with physical activity, sedentary time, body fat, and
day care attendance in 5-6-year-old children-the STEPS study.
Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports,28(12),
26682676. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13264
Mayes, S. D., Frye, S. S., Breaux, R. P., & Calhoun, S. L. (2018).
Diagnostic, demographic, and neurocognitive correlates of
dysgraphia in students with ADHD, autism, learning disabil-
ities, and Neurotypical development. Journal of Developmental
and Physical Disabilities,30(4), 489507. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10882-018-9598-9
Meulenbroek, R. G., & Van Galen, G. P. (1988). Foreperiod duration
and the analysis of motor stages in a line-drawing task. Acta
Psychologica,69(1), 1934.
Milbrath, C., & Council, J. R. (2003). Book reviews: Child art in
context: A cultural and comparative perspective, the illusion of
conscious will. Imagination, Cognition and Personality,22(3),
303313. https://doi.org/10.2190/763E-RLGX-GN13-CDPV
Motulsky, H. J., & Brown, R. E. (2006). Detecting outliers when
fitting data with nonlinear regression A new method based
on robust nonlinear regression and the false discovery rate.
BMC Bioinformatics,7(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-
2105-7-123
Ose Askvik, E., van der Weel, F. R., & van der Meer, A. L. H.
(2020). The importance of cursive handwriting over typewriting
for learning in the classroom: A high-density EEG study of
12-year-old children and Young adults. Frontiers in Psychology,
11, 1810. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01810
Pangelinan, M. M., Zhang, G., VanMeter, J. W., Clark, J. E.,
Hatfield, B. D., & Haufler, A. J. (2011). Beyond age and gender:
Relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume
and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children.
NeuroImage,54(4), 30933100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2010.11.021
Pesce, C., Masci, I., Marchetti, R., Vannozzi, G., & Schmidt, M.
(2018). When children's perceived and actual motor compe-
tence mismatch: Sport participation and gender differences.
Journal of Motor Learning and Development,6(s2), S440S460.
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0081
Petrigna, L., Treigienė, M., Thomas, E., Mani, D., Pajaujiene, S.,
Drid, P., Lavanco, G., Palma, A., & Bianco, A. (2021). Time
spent on the smartphone does not relate to manual dexterity in
young adults. BMC Neuroscience,22(1), 34. https://doi.org/10.
1186/s12868-021-00639-y
Pitchford, N. J., Papini, C., Outhwaite, L. A., & Gulliford, A. (2016).
Fine motor skills predict Maths ability better than they predict
Reading ability in the early primary school years. Frontiers in
Psychology,7.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00783
Raimo, S., Santangelo, G., & Trojano, L. (2021). The neural bases of
drawing. A meta-analysis and a systematic literature review of
Neurofunctional studies in healthy individuals. Neuropsychol-
ogy Review,31, 689702. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-021-
09494-4
Reikerås, E., Moser, T., & Tønnessen, F. E. (2017). Mathematical
skills and motor life skills in toddlers: Do differences in mathe-
matical skills reflect differences in motor skills? European Early
Childhood Education Research Journal,25(1), 7288. https://
doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1062664
Ruiz, J. R., España, V. R., Castro, J. P., Artero, E. G., Ortega, F. B.,
Cuenca, M. G., Jiménez, D. P., Chill
on, P., Girela, M. R.,
Mora, J., Gutiérrez, A., Suni, J., Sjöstrom, M., & Castillo, M. J.
(2011). ALPHA-fitness test battery: Health-related field-based
fitness tests assessment in children and adolescents. Nutrici
on
Hospitalaria,26(6), 12101214. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0212-
16112011000600003
Santomauro, F., Lorini, C., Pieralli, F., Niccolai, G., Picciolli, P.,
Vezzosi, S., & Bonaccorsi, G. (2017). Waist-to-height ratio and
its associations with body mass index in a sample of Tuscan
children in primary school. Italian Journal of Pediatrics,43(1),
53. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-017-0372-x
Santos, O. C. (2016). Training the body: The potential of AIED to
support personalized motor skills learning. International Jour-
nal of Artificial Intelligence in Education,26(2), 730755.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40593-016-0103-2
Scaradozzi, D., Guasti, L., Di Stasio, M., Miotti, B., Monteriù, A., &
Blikstein, P. (2021). Makers at school, educational robotics and
innovative learning environments: Research and experiences from
FabLearn Italy 2019, in the Italian schools and beyond (Vol.
240). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.
1007/978-3-030-77040-2
Scordella, A., Di Sano, S., Aureli, T., Cerratti, P., Verratti, V., Fanò-
Illic, G., & Pietrangelo, T. (2015). The role of general dynamic
coordination in the handwriting skills of children. Frontiers in
Psychology,6.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00580
Simpson, A., Al Ruwaili, R., Jolley, R., Leonard, H., Geeraert, N., &
Riggs, K. J. (2017). Fine motor control underlies the association
between response inhibition and drawing skill in early
14 of 15 BONDI ET AL.
development. Child Development,90, 911923. https://doi.org/
10.1111/cdev.12949
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic sys-
tem. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,7(8), 343348. https://doi.org/
10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00156-6
Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Sugden, D., & Duysens, J. (2006). Devel-
opmental trends in speed accuracy trade-off in 610-year-old
children performing rapid reciprocal and discrete aiming move-
ments. Human Movement Science,25(1), 3749. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.humov.2005.12.002
Snapp-Childs, W., Fath, A. J., Watson, C. A., Flatters, I., Mon-
Williams, M., & Bingham, G. P. (2015). Training to improve
manual control in 78 and 1012 year old children: Training
eliminates performance differences between ages. Human
Movement Science,43,9099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.
2015.07.006
Strooband, K. F. B., De Rosnay, M., Okely, A. D., &
Veldman, S. L. C. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analyses:
Motor skill interventions to improve fine motor development in
children aged birth to 6 years. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics,41(4), 319331. https://doi.org/10.1097/
DBP.0000000000000779
Suggate, S., Pufke, E., & Stoeger, H. (2018). Do fine motor skills
contribute to early reading development? Journal of Research in
Reading,41(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12081
Suggate, S., Stoeger, H., & Pufke, E. (2017). Relations between
playing activities and fine motor development. Early Child
Development and Care,187(8), 12971310. https://doi.org/10.
1080/03004430.2016.1167047
Tabatabaey-Mashadi, N., Sudirman, R., Khalid, P. I., & Lange-
Küttner, C. (2015). Automated syntax analyses of drawing two
tangent patterns in children with low and average handwriting
ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills,120(3), 865894. https://
doi.org/10.2466/24.10.PMS.120v15x1
Todorov, E., & Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a
theory of motor coordination. Nature Neuroscience,5(11),
12261235. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963
Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: A
study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuospatial
skills and drawing in children. International Journal of Behav-
ioral Development,26(3), 234247. https://doi.org/10.1080/
01650250143000021
Vaivre-Douret, L., Lopez, C., Dutruel, A., & Vaivre, S. (2021).
Phenotyping features in the genesis of pre-scriptural gestures in
children to assess handwriting developmental levels. Scientific
Reports,11(1), 731. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-79315-w
Valerio, G., Maffeis, C., Saggese, G., Ambruzzi, M. A., Balsamo, A.,
Bellone, S., Bergamini, M., Bernasconi, S., Bona, G.,
Calcaterra, V., Canali, T., Caroli, M., Chiarelli, F., Corciulo, N.,
Crinò, A., Di Bonito, P., Di Pietrantonio, V., Di Pietro, M., Di
Sessa, A., Zito, E. (2018). Diagnosis, treatment and preven-
tion of pediatric obesity: Consensus position statement of the
Italian Society for Pediatric Endocrinology and Diabetology
and the Italian Society of Pediatrics. Italian Journal of Pediat-
rics,44(1), 88. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-018-0525-6
van Abswoude, F., van der Kamp, J., & Steenbergen, B. (2019). The
roles of declarative knowledge and working memory in explicit
motor learning and practice among children with low motor
abilities. Motor Control,23(1), 3451. https://doi.org/10.1123/
mc.2017-0060
van der Meer, A. L. H., & van der Weel, F. R. (2017). Only three fin-
gers write, but the whole brain works: A high-density EEG
study showing advantages of drawing over typing for learning.
Frontiers in Psychology,8, 706. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.
2017.00706
Van Gemmert, A. W. A., & Contreras-Vidal, J. L. (2015).
Graphonomics and its contribution to the field of motor behav-
ior: A position statement. Human Movement Science,43, 165
168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.017
Vandorpe, B., Vandendriessche, J., Vaeyens, R., Pion, J.,
Matthys, S., Lefevre, J., Philippaerts, R., & Lenoir, M. (2012).
Relationship between sports participation and the level of
motor coordination in childhood: A longitudinal approach.
Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport,15(3), 220225.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2011.09.006
WHO. (2007). Growth reference 5-19 years. https://www.who.int/
growthref/who2007_bmi_for_age/en/
Yuan, Y., & Brown, S. (2015). Drawing and writing: An ALE meta-
analysis of sensorimotor activations. Brain and Cognition,98,
1526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.05.004
Zaqout, M., Vyncke, K., Moreno, L. A., De Miguel-Etayo, P.,
Lauria, F., Molnar, D., Lissner, L., Hunsberger, M.,
Veidebaum, T., Tornaritis, M., Reisch, L. A., Bammann, K.,
Sprengeler, O., Ahrens, W., & Michels, N. (2016). Determinant
factors of physical fitness in European children. International
Journal of Public Health,61(5), 573582. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s00038-016-0811-2
Zoia, S., Biancotto, M., Guicciardi, M., Lecis, R., Lucidi, F.,
Pelamatti, G. M., Carrozzi, M., Skabar, A., Sugden, D. A.,
Barnett, A. L., & Henderson, S. E. (2018). An evaluation of the
movement ABC-2 test for use in Italy: A comparison of data
from Italy and the UK. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
84,4356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.04.013
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found in the
online version of the article at the publisher's website.
How to cite this article: Bondi, D., Robazza, C.,
Lange-Küttner, C., & Pietrangelo, T. (2022). Fine
motor skills and motor control networking in
developmental age. American Journal of Human
Biology, e23758. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23758
BONDI ET AL.15 of 15
... Kegiatan outbound berbasis permainan tradisional seperti "Karapan Pesapean Challenge" telah terbukti menjadi metode yang inovatif dan relevan dalam meningkatkan kompetensi calon guru PJOK, terutama pada aspek keterampilan motorik (Bondi et al., 2022;Kotevski, 2024;Mnif et al., 2024), kerja sama, dan kepemimpinan. Hasil penelitian menunjukkan bahwa pendekatan berbasis budaya lokal ini memiliki dampak signifikan dalam memberi pengalaman belajar yang kontekstual dan holistik. ...
Article
Kegiatan outbound merupakan salah satu metode inovatif untuk meningkatkan kompetensi calon guru Pendidikan Jasmani, Olahraga, dan Kesehatan (PJOK), khususnya dalam aspek keterampilan motorik, kerja sama, dan kepemimpinan. Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk menganalisis landscape kompetensi 100 calon guru PJOK di STKIP PGRI Sumenep melalui pelaksanaan kegiatan outbound di Bumi Perkemahan Bedengan, Batu. Kegiatan dilaksanakan dalam kondisi lingkungan yang dinamis, termasuk hujan yang menambah tantangan fisik dan mental peserta. Outbound ini dirancang menggunakan permainan berbasis budaya lokal, yaitu "Karapan Pasapean Challenge," untuk mengintegrasikan nilai budaya dalam pembelajaran. Metode penelitian melibatkan observasi, wawancara, dan evaluasi langsung terhadap pelaksanaan kegiatan. Hasil penelitian menunjukkan bahwa kegiatan outbound secara signifikan meningkatkan keterampilan motorik, komunikasi efektif, dan kemampuan kerja sama peserta. Selain itu, integrasi elemen budaya lokal memberikan pengalaman pembelajaran yang kontekstual dan relevan dengan kebutuhan calon guru, serta mendorong kreativitas mereka dalam mengembangkan modul ajar berbasis permainan tradisional. Penelitian ini merekomendasikan kegiatan outbound berbasis budaya sebagai bagian integral dari kurikulum untuk membekali calon guru PJOK dengan kompetensi yang holistik, relevan, dan berbasis nilai-nilai lokal. Implementasi kegiatan serupa diharapkan dapat memperkuat karakter dan keterampilan guru masa depan yang sesuai dengan kebutuhan generasi Z
... Various screening tools, such as parent-completed questionnaires and standardized assessments, are combined with the clinical judgment of healthcare professionals to evaluate developmental delays in children [10]. For the assessment for motor development specifically, including gross motor and fine motor skills, a number of screening tools such as Gross Motor Function Measure-88 (GMFM-88) and Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (MABC-2) are available [11,12]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Screen time, defined as the amount of time spent engaging with electronic screens, has become inevitable in modern life. The rise in screen time among children under 5 years old has raised concerns about its association with motor development including gross and fine motor skills. Conflicting evidence on the association of screen time requires more investigation and the planning of targeted interventions. This systematic review aims to explore the relationship between screen time and motor development in children aged 0–7 years, considering various influencing factors like screen type, exposure duration, and context. Following preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses (PRISMA) and assessing the methodological quality of systematic reviews (AMSTAR) guidelines, a literature search was conducted in May 2024 in databases including PubMed, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), and ScienceDirect. Eligible studies were observational or experimental, involved children aged 0–7 years, and assessed the outcomes between screen time and motor development. The quality of the included studies was assessed through Joanna Briggs Institute's (JBI) critical appraisal checklist. Out of 1490 records initially identified, 24 studies met the inclusion criteria for this review. Among these, 17 studies reported a significant negative correlation between screen time and motor development in children, while 5 studies found no statistically significant association. Two studies presented mixed findings, indicating both negative and positive associations between screen time and motor development. Excessive screen time in early childhood is mainly linked to negative effects on motor development. The association varies with screen content and environment, highlighting the need for balanced screen time and early interventions.
... To investigate the networking between mental functions, a widely adopted strategy is to examine the pattern of cross-variable associations using network analysis [14]. For example, the pattern of networking between motor and cognitive factors was associated with the mental health of children [15], cognitive impairment [16], and the age-related difference in fine motor movement [17]. A network analysis may reveal the pattern of networking between mental functions, including visual attention, stereoacuity, hand steadiness, finger and tweezer dexterity, and finger force generation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Manual dexterity is key to clinical skill training. The networking between sensorimotor and visual-attention functions may be associated with the expertise of dental students. The study aims to investigate the pattern of networking between sensorimotor and visual-attention functions and related resting-state (rs) functional connectivity (FC). Methods Twenty-six dental and 19 non-dental students completed visual-attention tests and psychomotor tests for manual dexterity and steadiness. Thirty participants also received structural and rs-functional magnetic resonance imaging. Results Compared to non-dental students, dental students showed increased networking in performance between visual-attention tests and manual dexterity, increased rsFC between bilateral sensorimotor network (SMN) and the left middle frontal gyrus, and decreased rsFC between bilateral SMN and the left posterior insula. Better performance of manual dexterity was associated with increased rsFC between the left SMN and the right supramarginal gyrus. Conclusions Dental skill training is associated with the networking of sensorimotor and visual-attention functions and coupled with increased rsFC of the SMN. In dental students, better manual dexterity is associated with the functional connectivity of the SMN. Clinical trial number Not applicable.
... изменение качества мелкой моторики на фоне утомления [14]; ...
Article
Full-text available
Мелкая моторика имеет огромное значение в профессиях, где от слаженной работы пальцев рук зависит профессиональный результат. В статье выявлены особенности совершенствования и оценки мелкой моторики при подготовке специалистов в различных профессиях. Мелкая моторика рассматривается как неотъемлемая часть профессиональной подготовки во множестве профессий. Наиболее часто эту сторону профессиональной подготовки исследуют у медиков (стоматологов и хирургов), музыкантов (пианистов и скрипачей), авиакосмических специалистов (парашютистов и космонавтов), лиц рабочих специальностей (сварщиков). Все исследования можно разделить на не-сколько категорий: I-изучение физиологических детерминантов мелкой моторики; II-изучение взаимосвязи мелкой моторики с психологическими процессами; III-развитие мелкой моторики как неотъемлемая часть профессионально-прикладного физического воспитания; IV-разработка более совершенных способов оценки мелкой моторики; V-изучение влияния мелкой моторики на трудовые процессы; VI-изучение внешних факторов, влияющих на уровень проявления мелкой моторики. Поиск литературы по теме проводился в наукометрических базах, электронных библиотеках и поисковых системах. Всего в обзоре рассмотрено 46 научных публикаций за период с 1976 по 2024 год. Ключевые слова: мелкая моторика рук, профессиональная подготовка, методы оценки мелкой моторики, ловкость рук Fine motor skills are important in professions where results depend on the coordinated work of the fingers. The article identifies the features of improving and evaluating fine motor skills in the training of specialists in various professions. Fine motor skills are considered an integral part of vocational training in a wide range of occupations. This aspect of professional training is most often studied in relation to the following occupations: doctors (dentists and surgeons), musicians (pianists and violinists), aerospace specialists (parachutists and cosmonauts) and workers (welders). All the studies can be divided into several categories: I-study of the physiological determinants of fine motor skills; II-study of the relationship of fine motor skills to psychological processes; III-development of fine motor skills as an integral part of vocational physical education; IV-development of more advanced methods of assessing fine motor skills; V-study of the influence of fine motor skills on work processes; VI-study of external factors affecting the level of manifestation of fine motor skills. The literature on the subject was searched in scientometric databases, electronic libraries and search engines. All in all 46 scientific publications from 1976 to 2024 were reviewed. Key words: fine motor skills of the hand, vocational training, assessment methods of fine motor skills, manual dexterity Бахтиарова Т.В., Померанцев А.А., Фаренбрух А.С. Мелкая моторика как важный компонент профессионального образования: литературный обзор // Гуманитарные исследования Центральной России. – 2025. – №1 (34). – С. 81-90.
... As the foundation of fine motor skills, upper-limb function is of great importance in daily human life. This function encompasses actions such as brushing teeth, eating, combing hair, and fastening buttons, all of which significantly influence children's independence and development during the preschool years [12]. In this study, sport biomechanics analysis was utilized to examine postural adjustment characteristics during reaching tasks carried out by children aged 3-6. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reaching is a fundamental motor skill essential for daily living, with over 50% of daily activities involving reaching movements. Understanding the development of postural adjustments made during reaching in early childhood is crucial for motor development. This study investigated the developmental characteristics of postural adjustments made by children aged 3–6 years during reaching tasks. A total of 135 typically developing children from Chinese kindergartens participated in this study. Kinematic and electromyographic (EMG) data were collected using an infrared motion capture system and surface electromyography, respectively. A two-way ANCOVA was performed to examine the effects of age and gender on kinematic and electromyographic parameters. Post hoc analyses revealed that completion time and shoulder angle showed a linear decreasing trend (p < 0.05). The variable wrist joint had an increasing trend in the high-touch task, while the elbow joint showed a nonlinear decreasing trend (p < 0.05). EMG results regarding Extensor Carpi Ulnaris (ECU) showed a decreasing trend at all phases (p < 0.05). The developmental patterns observed suggest that children progressively utilize more distal (wrist) and fewer proximal (elbow and shoulder) joints during reaching, indicating the maturation of motor patterns. However, the mechanisms of anticipation and compensation for children aged 3–6 are not yet fully understood.
Article
Full-text available
In human infants, exploratory object manipulations (henceforth called “EOM”) stimulate cognitive development and affect cognitive performance in later life. Zoo-housed great apes are frequently used to study the evolution of human cognition, however, it is unknown how the zoo environment affects their daily expression of EOM. We investigated how wild and zoo-housed Sumatran orangutans differ in their daily EOM throughout life. We collected ~ 12′000 EOM events by 51 wild and zoo-housed individuals of all ages. Zoo-housed orangutans showed significantly higher EOM rates than wild orangutans. Exploratory actions were more diverse in zoos than in the wild, even with objects available in both settings. Zoo-housed orangutans also showed a larger repertoire of exploratory actions and a higher probability of multi-object exploration, including tool use. There was no difference between settings at which age individuals first showed specific exploratory actions. Our results show that the zoo environment significantly affects EOM in orangutans and that the species’ exploratory potential exceeds its natural expression. This may have important implications for cognitive performance, as zoo-housed individuals are likely to have a broader range of affordances to draw from when confronted with novel problems. These results highlight the potential of captive-wild comparisons to study cognitive development and evolution.
Preprint
Full-text available
In human infants, exploratory object manipulations (henceforth called “EOM”) stimulate cognitive development and affect cognitive performance in later life. Zoo-housed great apes are frequently used to study the evolution of human cognition, however, it is unknown how the zoo environment affects their daily expression of EOM. We investigated how wild and zoo-housed Sumatran orangutans differ in their daily EOM throughout life. We collected ~ 12’000 EOM events by 51 wild and zoo-housed individuals of all ages. Zoo-housed orangutans showed significantly higher EOM rates than wild orangutans. Exploratory actions were more diverse in zoos than in the wild, even with objects available in both settings. Zoo-housed orangutans also showed a larger repertoire of exploratory actions and a higher probability of multi-object exploration, including tool use. There was no difference between settings at which age individuals first showed specific exploratory actions. Our results show that the zoo environment significantly affects EOM in orangutans and that the species’ exploratory potential exceeds its natural expression. This may have important implications for cognitive performance, as zoo-housed individuals are likely to have a broader range of affordances to draw from when confronted with novel problems. These results highlight the potential of captive-wild comparisons to study cognitive development and evolution.
Article
Full-text available
Children with cerebral palsy (CP) typically exhibit lower fine motor skills compared to their typically developing peers. Various interventions, including constructive LEGO play, have been explored to support fine motor skill development. This study examines the effectiveness of constructive LEGO play in enhancing fine motor skills in children with CP at SDLB Putra Jaya Malang. A Single-Subject Design (SSD) with an A-B-A format was employed. Data were collected through structured observations and assessments of fine motor skills during baseline, intervention, and post-intervention phases. Findings indicate a notable improvement in fine motor skills from the first baseline (A1) to the intervention phase (B), with sustained progress in the second baseline (A2) after the intervention was withdrawn. This suggests that constructive LEGO play positively influences fine motor skill development in children with CP. The study highlights the potential of LEGO-based activities as an engaging and effective tool for improving fine motor coordination in children with CP. The observed progress reinforces the need for structured and repetitive fine motor activities in special education settings. Constructive LEGO play proves to be a beneficial intervention for enhancing fine motor skills in children with CP. Its integration into therapy and special education programs could provide long-term benefits. Further research is recommended to explore its scalability and long-term impact.
Article
State-of-the-art Virtual Reality (VR) Head-Mounted Displays (HMDs) enable users to interact with virtual objects using their hands via built-in camera systems. However, the accuracy of the hand movement detection algorithm is often affected by limitations in both camera hardware and software, including image processing & machine learning algorithms used for hand skeleton detection. In this work, we investigated a visual feedback mechanism to create an early warning system that detects hand skeleton recognition failures in VR HMDs and warns users in advance. We conducted two user studies to evaluate the system's effectiveness. The first study involved a cup stacking task, where participants stacked virtual cups. In the second study, participants performed a ball sorting task, picking and placing colored balls into corresponding baskets. During both of the studies, we monitored the built-in hand tracking confidence of the VR HMD system and provided visual feedback to the user to warn them when the tracking confidence is ‘low’. The results showed that warning users before the hand tracking algorithm fails improved the system's usability while reducing frustration. The impact of our results extends beyond VR HMDs, any system that uses hand tracking, such as robotics, can benefit from this approach.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Fine motor skill (FMS) development during childhood is essential to many learning processes, especially in school. FMS impairment can have a major impact on children’s quality of life. Developing effective and engaging rehabilitation solutions to train FMS that engage children in the abundant practice required for motor learning can be challenging. Virtual reality (VR) is a promising intervention option offering engaging FMS training tasks and environments that align with evidence-based motor learning principles. Other potential advantages of VR for rehabilitation include accessibility for home-based use and adaptability to individual needs. The objective of this scoping review is to map the extent, range and nature of VR applications focused on FMS training in paediatric rehabilitation, including hardware, software and interventional parameters. Methods and analysis We are following methodological guidelines for scoping review conduct and reporting from the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) Manual for Evidence Synthesis and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for scoping reviews recommendations. We will search four databases (Pubmed, Web of Science, PsycInfo and Scopus) for articles that meet inclusion criteria defined by the Population, Concept, Context method; specifically studies focused on development or evaluation of immersive or non-immersive VR applications to deliver FMS training in paediatric rehabilitation. Different populations of children with FMS impairments will be included (such as children with cerebral palsy, children with developmental coordination disorder or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder). The first search took place in December 2023, and a second is planned for February 2025. One reviewer will complete title, abstract and full paper screening, with consultation by a second reviewer in case of uncertainty. A data extraction framework will be tested by two reviewers on five randomly selected studies to ensure inter-rater reliability, and one reviewer will complete data extraction. Quantitative and qualitative extraction will follow JBI guideline recommendations. Results will be presented in a descriptive and tabular format, including a narrative summary. Results will enhance understanding of the potential of FMS training in VR and inform subsequent directions for research and clinical practice. Ethics and dissemination Data for this review will be collected from the published literature. Ethical approval is not required. We will present our findings at scientific conferences and submit this review to a peer-reviewed journal for publication.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated whether sensitivity to object violations in perception as well as in action would vary with age. Five-, 6-, and 11-yr.-old children and adults solved tasks which involved perception only, motoric indication of parts, actual assembly of parts, and drawing of a violated figure. In perception, object violation was the only factor showing change across age groups, with violations being increasingly noticed. In composition tasks involving motor components, object violation was just one factor besides quantity of parts and type of segmentation contributing to task difficulty and showing increase in performance across age groups. Analysis of object violations in visual structure required abilities similar to those needed when analysing shape interference. Improved visual detection and graphic construction of object violation seemed not to occur because segmentation increased quantitatively but more likely because fast perceptual processes came under scrutiny.
Book
Full-text available
This open access book contains observations, outlines, and analyses of educational robotics methodologies and activities, and developments in the field of educational robotics emerging from the findings presented at FabLearn Italy 2019, the international conference that brought together researchers, teachers, educators and practitioners to discuss the principles of Making and educational robotics in formal, non-formal and informal education. The editors’ analysis of these extended versions of papers presented at FabLearn Italy 2019 highlight the latest findings on learning models based on Making and educational robotics. The authors investigate how innovative educational tools and methodologies can support a novel, more effective and more inclusive learner-centered approach to education. The following key topics are the focus of discussion: Makerspaces and Fab Labs in schools, a maker approach to teaching and learning; laboratory teaching and the maker approach, models, methods and instruments; curricular and non-curricular robotics in formal, non-formal and informal education; social and assistive robotics in education; the effect of innovative spaces and learning environments on the innovation of teaching, good practices and pilot projects.
Article
Full-text available
Early motor skills underpin the more complex and specialized movements required for physical activity. Therefore, the design of interventions that enhance higher levels of early motor skills may encourage subsequent participation in physical activity. To do so, it is necessary to determine the influence of certain factors (some of which appear very early) on early motor skills. The objective of this study was to examine the influence of some very early environmental variables (delivery mode, feeding type during the first 4 months of life) and some biological variables (sex and age in months) on preschool motor skills, considered both globally and specifically. The sample was composed by 43 preschool students aged 5–6 years. The participant's parents completed an ad hoc questionnaire, reporting on delivery mode, feeding type, sex, and age in months. The children's motor skills were assessed using observational methodology in the school setting, while the children participated in their regular motor skills sessions. A Nomothetic/Punctual/Multidimensional observational design was used. Results revealed that certain preschool motor skills were specifically influenced by delivery mode, feeding type, sex, and age. Children born by vaginal delivery showed higher scores than children born via C-section in throwing (p = 0.000; d = 0.63); total control of objects (p = 0.004; d = 0.97); total gross motor skills (p = 0.005; d = 0.95); and total motor skills (p = 0.002; d = 1.04). Children who were exclusively breastfed outperformed those who were formula-fed in throwing (p = 0.016; d = 0.75); visual-motor integration (p = 0.005; d = 0.94); total control of objects (p = 0.002; d = 1.02); total gross motor skills (p = 0.023; d = 0.82); and total motor skills (p = 0.042; d = 0.74). Boys outperformed girls in throwing (p = 0.041; d = 0.74) and total control of objects (p = 0.024; d = 0.63); while the opposite occurred in static balance (p = 0.000; d = 1.2); visual-motor coordination (p = 0.020; d = 0.79); and total fine motor skills (p = 0.032; d = 0.72). Older children (aged 69–74 months) obtained higher scores than younger ones (aged 63–68 months) in dynamic balance (p = 0.030; d = 0.66); visual-motor integration (p = 0.034; d = 0.63); and total balance (p = 0.013; d = 0.75). Implications for early childhood care and education are discussed since this is a critical period for motor skill development and learning.
Article
Full-text available
Background The Grooved Pegboard Test (GPT) is widely adopted to evaluate manual dexterity, it presents normative data but the test is influenced by different factors. The influence of time spent on smartphones has not been considered before, for this reason, the objective of this study was to evaluate if smartphone use influences the time to complete the GPT. A total of 38 (21 women; 17 men) young adults 20.7 (1.5) years participated in the study. The time spent on the smartphones during the last seven days was recorded through the device itself and the GPT performance was measured. A correlation analysis between the time spent on the smartphone and GPT was performed while the t -test was adopted to evaluate gender differences. Results No statistically significant differences were detected between men and women in the time to complete the GPT (p = 0.20) and in the time spent on the smartphone (p = 0.87). The GPT and the time spent using the smartphone were not correlated (r = 0.044, p = 0.78). Conclusion The time spent on the smartphone by young adults does not influence the time to complete the GPT, indicating that smartphone use does not influence measures of manual dexterity.
Article
Full-text available
Drawing is a multi-component process requiring a wide range of cognitive abilities. Several studies on patients with focal brain lesions and functional neuroimaging studies on healthy individuals demonstrated that drawing is associated with a wide brain network. However, the neural structures specifically related to drawing remain to be better comprehended. We conducted a systematic review complemented by a meta-analytic approach to identify the core neural underpinnings related to drawing in healthy individuals. In analysing the selected studies, we took into account the type of the control task employed (i.e. motor or non-motor) and the type of drawn stimulus (i.e. geometric, figurative, or nonsense). The results showed that a fronto-parietal network, particularly on the left side of the brain, was involved in drawing when compared with other motor activities. Drawing figurative images additionally activated the inferior frontal gyrus and the inferior temporal cortex, brain areas involved in selection of semantic features of objects and in visual semantic processing. Moreover, copying more than drawing from memory was associated with the activation of extrastriate cortex (BA 18, 19). The activation likelihood estimation coordinate-based meta-analysis revealed a core neural network specifically associated with drawing which included the premotor area (BA 6) and the inferior parietal lobe (BA 40) bilaterally, and the left precuneus (BA 7). These results showed that a fronto-parietal network is specifically involved in drawing and suggested that a crucial role is played by the (left) inferior parietal lobe, consistent with classical literature on constructional apraxia.
Article
Full-text available
Acquiring writing skills is a long developmental process that is conditioned by both the mastery of the gesture and the spatio-temporal arrangement of characters across the page. While the researches in the literature mainly focused on spatio-temporal and kinematics parameters of tracing letters or words using digitizing tablets, no recent research has previously studied the developmental prerequisites of the organization of handwriting useful for clinical assessment and remediation. Aims of the present study was to investigate and validate the phenotyping of the developmental genesis of pre-scriptural graphomotor gestures among school-aged children in achieving correct handwriting. The subject was examined in depth in an ecological setting similar to school, with the objective of assessing handwriting developmental levels. The pre-scriptural graphomotor task studied was to copy a line of cycloid loops on a paper sheet put on the table. This task was chosen because it reflects the execution of the hand movements from one end of the line to the other and in an anti-clockwise direction, as in handwriting. A new methodological approach was applied incorporating both the maturative evolution of postural-gestural features (video-recorded for analysis in 2D reconstruction) and spatio-temporal/kinematic measures collected with a digital pen connected to an analysis software tool to assess the developmental level and provide an understanding of the phenotypical features of the graphomotor gesture. And we also evidence the concurrent validity of the data in displacements, and the better are the spatio-temporal and kinematic measures. Consequently there are phenotypical features, both postural-gestural and spatio-temporal/kinematic in the developmental genesis of the graphomotor gesture with an easy pre-scriptural task. Typically developing school children from 1st to 5th grade, was collected from elementary schools. Five main patterns of displacement gestures were found for the production of the line of loops with a significant developmental progress from 1st to 5th grade. In addition, significant results in comparisons with spatio-temporal and kinematic age-related normative data were highlighted, associated with the quality of the coordination gesture. Lastly external validity in relation to normative values with the standardized handwriting scale BHK (French adaptation of the Concise Evaluation Scale for Children’s handwriting) showed certain significant correlations with spatio-temporal and kinematic measures and the evolution of the displacement gestures (five patterns) used to draw the loops. The better the motor control of the handwriting gesture, the less variety there is in inter-segmental and joint-scriptural task, enabling handwriting developmental levels to be assessed in screening for handwriting disorders, possibly co-occurring with other learning disabilities, and also useful in clinical decision-making processes for handwriting remediation, or simply to assist handwriting gesture acquisition in elementary school.
Article
Full-text available
The behavioral preference for the use of one side of the body starts from pre-natal life and prompt humans to develop motor asymmetries. The type of motor task completed influences those functional asymmetries. However, there is no real consensus on the occurrence of handedness during developmental ages. Therefore, we aimed to determine which motor asymmetries emerged differently during childhood. A total sample of 381 children in grades 1 to 5 (6-11 years old) of primary school were recruited and tested for two fine coordination tasks (Floppy, led by dexterity, and Thumb, led by speed-dominated skills) and handgrip strength (HS). Data about their handedness, footedness and sports participation were also collected. Children performed better with their dominant side, especially for the Floppy and HS tests. The asymmetries were more marked in right-handed children and did not differ by age, gender or type of sport. Our findings support the thesis of a functional lateralization in complex coordinative tasks and in maximal strength during developmental ages. Furthermore, our findings extend the evidence of a stronger lateralization in right-handed individuals, demonstrating it at a functional level in primary school children performing motor tasks. Fine motor skills allow a "fine" understanding of developmental trajectories of lateralized behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This study was aimed to evaluate the task-dependent manual laterality during fine coordination and grapho-motor tests in school children. Materials and Methods: We used two action tests (transitive and intransitive) to assess fine coordination skills and a tablet PC to assess number of strokes, pressure, speed and quality in a figure-tracing skill test, among 20 school children (12 girls and 8 boys) of second grade (7-8 years old). Results: Two-way RM-ANOVA (side × gender) revealed better values in the transitive task on the dominant side (p
Article
Full-text available
Introduction : The Common Region Test (CRT) is useful for predicting children’s visual memory as individual object-place binding predicted better object memory while objects-region coding predicted better place memory. Aim : To test children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with regards to spatial binding in the CRT. Methods : 19 children with ASD and 20 children with ADHD were gender-matched with 39 typically developing children by chronological age and with another 39 children by verbal mental age as control groups ( N = 117) and tested with the CRT and Bender Gestalt test. Results : Children with ASD and ADHD showed more unsystematic coding than typically developing children. This was due to lower fine motor skills, and in children with ADHD also because of reduced verbal naming. Almost all children with ASD presented the less mature under-inclusive Type I unsystematic coding which included object-place binding, while children with ADHD showed the over-inclusive Type II unsystematic coding that was overriding the Gestalt-like properties of proximity and similarity. Conclusions : It was demonstrated that the CRT is a useful screening instrument for ASD and ADHD that shows that their spatial categorization varies in their unsystematic visuo-spatial classification due to fine motor skill deficiencies.