Content uploaded by Nato Lagidze
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Nato Lagidze on May 03, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Thought
psyct.swu.bg | 2193-7281 South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Research Article
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown: The
Role of Emotional Intelligence, Self-
compassion and Rumination
Khatuna Martskvishvili*a, Nato Lagidzeb
[a] Ivane Javakhisvhili Tbilisi State University, Georgia.
[b] Ivane Javakhisvhili Tbilisi State University, Georgia.
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic and its accompanying restrictions, apart from the destructive global
economic and social impact, brought negative psychological consequences to the world. The
aim of the current study is to investigate the factors that help individuals maintain psychological
well-being under the condition of self-isolation, a global restriction against the spread of the
pandemic. Specifically, the role of trait emotional intelligence, self-compassion, and rumination
was explored in order to determine how these variables relate to psychological well-
being. According to the results, trait emotional intelligence and self-compassion positively
correlate with psychological well-being, while rumination is negatively correlated. Self-
compassion mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence and well-being. The
relationship between rumination and well-being is affected by emotional intelligence and self-
compassion: Self-compassion mediates the relationship between depression related rumination
and well-being, while self-control and emotionality factors of trait emotional intelligence
moderate the relationship between reflection rumination and well-being. The findings of the
current study provide insight about self-compassion, rumination, and emotional intelligence as
different contributing factors to the individuals’ well-being in self-isolation, suggesting further
implications about the use of these strategies to help people cope with this stressful situation.
Keywords: emotional intelligence; self-compassion; rumination; psychological; well-being; self-
isolation.
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 106
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Table of Contents
Present study
Method
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
References
Psychological Thought, 2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131, https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Received: 2021-05-03. Accepted: 2021-11-07. Published (VoR): 2022-04-30.
Handling Editor: Natasha Angelova, South-West University "Neofit Rilski", Blagoevgrad, Bulgaria.
*Corresponding author at: Tbilisi State University, Georgia. E-mail: nato.laghidze8343@pes.tsu.edu.ge
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Common Attribution License
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
It has already been a year since COVID-19 became the most commonly discussed topic in the
world. The pandemic and its accompanying restrictions introduced an entirely new way of living
and created new challenges in people’s daily lives across the globe. Although there is no
specific answer as to how the quarantine, self-isolation, and other pandemic-related restrictions
have changed the quality of life, one thing is for sure - the impact is not positive.
Apart from the destructive global economic and social impact, self-isolation has brought
negative psychological results as well. For instance, according to the latest studies, COVID-19
has a significant effect on the quality of life (Panayiotou et al., 2021; Solomou & Constantinidou,
2020). Also, both centralized isolation and home isolation negatively influence the improvement
of psychological distress levels in patients (Ju et al. 2021), the pandemic-related factors
increase anxiety and depression in pregnant women (Fan et al., 2021), and have a negative
impact on subjective well-being (Paredes et al., 2021).
In line with the current challenges in today’s world, it’s interesting to study the stressful
situations caused by COVID-19 and the psychological factors that take part in this process. Self-
isolation is a type of situation like this. It’s a global restriction against the spread of the pandemic
and means the limitation of relationships with society. Distance from others can actually be an
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 107
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
essential factor in reducing the risk of spreading the virus, but it’s accompanied by feelings of
uncertainty and confusion, leading to negative psychological consequences. For example, self-
isolation can induce stress and lead to the use of coping strategies such as rumination and self-
compassion. However, the role of these strategies has not been studied in self-isolation.
Considering the current worldwide situation, scientists have already started to intensively study
the role of emotion regulation strategies in dealing with the pandemic (e.g., Brehl et al., 2021;
Fernández et al., 2020; Tyra et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2021). Generally, effective emotion
regulation strategies reduce negative emotions, facilitate work-family balance, and maintain
overall psychological well-being (Restubog et al., 2020). According to studies (e.g., Peña-
Sarrionandia, et al., 2015), individuals with effective emotion regulation skills have high
emotional intelligence and can understand and control both their own and others' emotions
(Mayer & Salovey, 1997). Besides, emotional intelligence plays a specific role in adapting to
stressful situations (Matthews & Zeidner, 2000) and significantly determines well-being (Bowen
et al., 2016; Di Fabio & Kenny, 2016; Zeidner & Olnick-Shemesh, 2010) and the quality of life
(Min, 2013). Also, emotional intelligence is positively related to positive affect and predicts low-
intensity fear, anxiety, and sadness (Kong et al., 2019; Kousha et al., 2018; Moroń & Biolik-
Moroń, 2020). So, based on previous studies, it makes sense to assume that emotional
intelligence can play an essential role in emotion regulation during a pandemic.
However, the relationship between emotional intelligence and well-being may not be direct but
mediated by various factors that lead people with different emotional intelligence to
psychological well-being. For example, people with high emotional intelligence (EI) usually have
more effective coping strategies that increase well-being (Salovey et al., 2008). On the other
hand, rumination can be considered as a destructive strategy that hinders emotion regulation
(Kircaburun et al., 2019), which is why it is likely that rumination might have a negative impact
on psychological well-being. Studies suggest that people with high emotional intelligence are
less likely to have repeating negative thoughts (Lanciano et al. 2012; Liu & Ren 2016; Petrides
et al. 2007) because adults with high emotional intelligence try to solve their everyday problems
with adaptive strategies. On the other hand, people with low EI are characterized by non-
adaptive coping strategies such as rumination, whose positive relationship to negative
emotional states has been consistently confirmed in studies (Michl et al., 2014; Newman &
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 108
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Nezlek, 2017; Yalvaç & Gaynor, 2021). However, the researchers also note that an alternative
strategy for responding to stress is self-compassion (Neff et al., 2007), a much more adaptive
strategy with a long-term effect. The reason for this effect is that accepting negative emotions
and working on them raises hope that one can effectively cope with negative events in the
future and increases positive emotions (Odou & Brinker, 2014).
Emotional Intelligence and Well-being
Trait emotional intelligence is a constellation of emotion-related self-perceptions and
dispositions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies (Petrides & Furnham, 2001).
As studies show (Görgens-Ekermans & Brand, 2012; Marks et al., 2016), emotional intelligence
is a useful construct related to real-life outcomes. For instance, emotional intelligence is
associated with well-being (Extremera et al., 2011), which is defined as optimal human
functioning (Diener et al., 2009) and is related to concepts, such as having meaning and
purpose in life, valuable relationships, feelings of competence, and self-acceptance (Diener et
al., 2009).
People with high EI better acknowledge their emotions and, therefore, are able to regulate them
more effectively, experiencing lower levels of distress and higher levels of well-being (Zeidner &
Olnick-Shemesh, 2010). Many studies prove the significant contribution of emotional intelligence
to increasing psychological well-being (e.g., Di Fabio & Kenny, 2016). In general, good
emotional skills can lead to emotional growth and well-being (Extremera et al., 2011).
Furthermore, a higher level of emotional intelligence is related to positive mood and self-
esteem, which in turn leads to psychological well-being (Schutte et al., 2010).
Rumination and Well-being
Rumination is defined as "intrusive thoughts or images about past mistakes or failures that
cause negative feelings when they occur” (McLaughlin et al., 2007, p. 27). Rumination is often
referred to as a stress response strategy (Lanciano et al., 2012), although in Nolen-Hoeksema’s
Response Styles Theory (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991), rumination is also associated with distress
and is defined as “behaviors and thoughts that focus one's attention on one's depressive
symptoms and the implications of these symptoms” (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991, p. 569). People
who turn to rumination stay fixated on their problem and the feelings associated with it without
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 109
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
taking any action, and this is why rumination is referred to as a maladaptive strategy of emotion
regulation that contributes to negative affect (Kircaburun et al., 2019; Marcus, et al., 2008).
Considering this, it’s not surprising that the relationship between rumination and well-being is
negative. Particularly, rumination is associated with reduced well-being (Newman & Nezlek,
2017). Therefore, an understanding of rumination as a maladaptive coping strategy is shared in
the study in order to discuss its maladaptive role concerning emotional intelligence and well-
being.
Self-compassion and Well-being
Self-compassion is a healthy and adaptive attitude towards oneself, occurring when one is
having a difficult time and includes openness, caring, and kindness; understanding non-
judgmental attitude towards negative traits, and being aware that negative experiences are
natural and similar to others’ experiences (Neff, 2003). According to this definition, this construct
includes three distinct elements: Self-Kindness, Common Humanity, and Mindfulness, which
create a continuum with Self-Judgment, Isolation, and Over-Identification.
Self-compassion positively correlates to factors such as life satisfaction (Neff, 2003), happiness
(Neff & Vonk, 2009), optimism (Neff et al., 2007), positive affect (Neff et al., 2007), emotional
intelligence (Castilho et al., 2017; Di Fabio & Saklofske, 2021; Heffernan et al., 2010; Neff,
2003) and coping skills (Neff et al., 2005). Besides, unlike rumination, self-compassion improves
mood by reducing negative affect (Gilbert & Procter, 2006). This is because accepting negative
emotions and expressing self-compassion increases positive feelings, leading to well-being
(Bluth & Blanton, 2014). Consequently, self-compassion is positively associated with
psychological well-being (Neff et al., 2007). Therefore, one can assume that self-compassion
mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence and well-being.
Present study
Research consistently shows that emotion related personality traits play a crucial role in dealing
with stress. There are two ways to respond to stress within the framework of Response Styles
Theory (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991): rumination and distraction. Both strategies are considered
maladaptive in terms of long-term outcomes and correlate with negative emotional states.
However, at the same time, studies (Odou & Brinker, 2014) pointed out that self-compassion is
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 110
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
an alternative way to respond to stress and it leads individuals to well-being. Thus, the aim of
the study is to investigate the role of emotional intelligence, self-compassion, and rumination in
maintaining well-being in a stressful situation on the sample of individuals living in a lockdown
and to answer the following questions: Does emotional intelligence help individuals to maintain
well-being in stressful situations? Do self-compassion and emotional intelligence facilitate the
relationship between rumination and well-being? It was expected that emotional intelligence
would be positively related to self-compassion as well as to well-being and negatively - to
rumination; Emotional intelligence will lead individuals to high self-compassion and
consequently, to high well-being; and finally, it was assumed that emotional intelligence would
facilitate the relationship between rumination and well-being.
Method
Participants and Procedure
The sample was composed of 120 self-isolated individuals between the ages of 18 and 59
(convenience snowball sample; M = 24.67; SD = 9.06). Among them, 94 were women (77.7%),
and 26 - men (21.5%). 86.8% of the participants reported having higher education or being
students, while the rest had secondary (7.4%) or vocational (5%) education. The majority of the
participants (38.8%) were psychology faculty students. The remaining professions included law
(9.9%), economics (5%), sociology (6.6%), business (6.6%), medicine (5.8%), information
technology (4.1%), etc. As for the marital status, 83.5% of participants are single, while the rest
are either married (12.4%) or divorced (3.3%).
The participants had been in self-isolation as a result of the two-month state of emergency
(21.03.2020 - 22.05.2020) for the first time in the country of Georgia due to the pandemic. They
were restricted from leaving the home. Authorities announced a nighttime curfew. Educational
institutions and jobs were closed. All recreational and entertainment activities were stopped.
Public transport was banned in the country and people were restricted to move. As a
consequence, the main activities of participants switched to online.
Since moving between cities and within the city was limited, participants spent their time at
home, surrounded only by family members, having restricted social contact with others. The fact
that they didn’t have clear information about when self-isolation would finish and when they
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 111
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
would be able to return to normal activities made this stressful situation even more severe.
Besides, COVID-19 was an entirely new and unexplored virus by this time. Consequently, the
research participants did not have enough information about the virus, its aspects, and coping
methods. This uncertainty further increased the stress during self-isolation.
The criteria for inclusion in the sample were the following: Participants should have been in self-
isolation for at least three weeks and should not have left the house (unless vitally necessary
occasions); they had no physical contact with anyone (except people with whom they lived in
the household) and interacted with people through social networks and similar forms of
communication.
All participants have been informed about the goal of the study and the possibility to drop out at
any time without any explanation. Informed consent was obtained from all of them. After
receiving informed consent, the participants received an online version of the questionnaire
(Google forms) via e-mail or social platforms alongside instructions. Participants filled out the
questionnaires on a voluntary basis and did not receive a refund or any kind of reward.
Participation in the study was anonymous, and the data was interpreted confidentially.
Research Instruments
In order to assess emotional intelligence, the Georgian version (Martskvishvili et al., 2013) of
the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue; Petrides, 2009) was used. It’s a self-
report inventory consisting of 153 items and 15 distinct facets that create 4 factors: Emotionality
(α = .76; n = 4), sociability (α = .83; (n = 3), well-being (α = .86; n = 3), and self-control (α =. 81;
n = 3). These factors make up the global score of trait emotional intelligence (α = .88).
For measuring well-being, the Psychological Well-being Scale (PWB; Diener et al., 2009) was
used. This tool is an 8-item and 7-point Likert self-report scale that asseses important aspects of
human functioning, including positive relationships, a sense of competence, and meaning in life,
and provides an overall score of psychological well-being (α = .89).
Rumination was assessed with the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Nolen-Hoeksema,
1991). It’s a self-report scale consisting of depression related (α = .87; n = 10), brooding (α =
.76; n = 5), and reflection rumination (α = .79; n = 5). Respectively, the tool allows us to
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 112
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
calculate three types of rumination scores. All the scores of this 4-point and 22-item Likert scale
were summarized to determine the final score (α = .92; n = 22).
A 26-item Self-Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff, 2003) was used to measure self-compassion.
The construct of self-compassion contains 6 factors that create 3 dimensions. Therefore, the
scale has 6 sub-scales from which Self-Kindness (α = .75; n = 5) and Self-Judgment (α = .69; n
= 5) create the first dimension, Common Humanity (α = .61; n = 4) and Isolation (α = .75; n = 4)
create the second, and Mindfulness (α = .65; n = 4) and Over-Identification (α = .64; n = 4) for
the third. By reversing and summing up the scores of these subscales, a total self-compassion
score (α = .86; n = 26) was computed.
Analysis plan
To investigate the factors affecting psychological well-being, first, correlations among study
variables were analyzed. Then it was checked whether emotional intelligence, rumination, and
self-compassion predict psychological well-being using multiple regression analysis. The
moderation (Conditional Process Modeling by Hayes, 2018; Model 1) and mediation analyses
(Model 4) were used to test the effects of self-compassion on the relationship between
emotional intelligence and psychological well-being, on the one hand, and the effects of
emotional intelligence and self-compassion on the relationship between rumination and
psychological well-being, on the other.
Results
The results of the correlational analysis show that emotional personality traits positively
correlate with psychological well-being as well as with self-compassion variables, while the
correlations between emotional intelligence and rumination scales are negative, with only one
exception: reflection rumination positively correlates with the sociability factor of emotional
intelligence. Similarly, correlations between self-compassion and rumination factors are
negative (see Table 1 for the correlations and descriptives of the study variables).
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 113
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Table 1.
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of study variables
Emotional Intelligence M(Sd) 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
Emotionality (1)
4.82
(0.88)
.55** .41** .32** .76** -.21* .06 -.30** -.23* .07 .10 -.04 .20* .15 .17 .16 .47**
Sociability (2)
4.83
(0.83
.47** .31** .75** -.14 .19* -.22* -.13 .16 .05 -.07 .16 .29** .18* .19* .55**
Well-being (3) 4.79
(1.07) .53** .80** -.42** -.12 -.64** -.54** .44** .45** .17 .53** .40**
.51*
* .63** .66**
Self-control (4)
4.25
(0.85
.70** -.59** -.12 -.61** -.57** .22* .26** .07 .46** .37**
.62*
*
.50** .42**
Global EI (5) 4.65
(0.67) -.44** -.02 -.59** -.49** .27** .27** .03 .45** .37** .48*
*
.47** .71**
Rumination
Brooding (6) 13.11
(3.37) .48** .77** .87** -.21* -.42** .02 -
.51** -.07
-
.51*
*
-.45** -.16
Reflection (7)
14.86
(3.19)
.49** .69** .04 -.34** .12
-
.27**
.27** -.14 -.10 .08
Depression related
(8) 30.93
(7.93) .95** -.27** -.41** -.03 -
.52** -.17
-
.53*
*
-.50** -.37**
Rumination total
(9) 58.90
(12.67) -.22* -.45** .02 -
.53** -.06
-
.50*
*
-.46** -.25**
Self-compassion
Self kindness (10)
14.36
(3.99) .40** .44** .35** .55**
.41*
*
.79** .30**
Self judgment
(low) (11) 11.70
(3.65) -.01 .53** .07 .53*
* .66** .09
Common humanity
(12)
12.88
(3.14) .03 .43** .12 .47** .11
Isolation (low) (13) 11.60
(3.83) .21* .55*
*
.69** .20*
Mindfulness (14) 14.27
(2.89)
.30*
*
.61** .37**
Over-identification
(low) (15)
10.11
(3.49)
.74** .28**
Self-compassion
total (16) 74.90
(14.06) .33**
Psychological Well-
being (17)
5.28
(1.03)
Note: *p <.05; **p < .01
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 114
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
The multiple linear regression analysis was used to test whether emotional intelligence,
rumination and self-compassion factors predict psychological well-being (see Table 2).
First, the model for combined emotional intelligence factors and psychological well-being PWB
was tested. This explained 52% of the variance and significantly predicted PWB (R2 = .52,
adjusted R2 =.51), F(4, 115) = 31.69, p < .000) with sociability (β =.25, t = 3.07, p < .01) and
trait well-being factors (β =.46, t = 5.52, p < .001) as significant positive predictors of PWB.
The model for combined rumination factors explained 25% of the variance and significantly
predicted PWB (R2 =.25, adjusted R2 = .22), F(3, 116) = 12.54, p <.000) with reflection (β =.32, t
= 3.36, p <.01) as a significant positive predictor of PWB and depression related rumination (β =
-.69, t = -5.38, p<.001) as a significant negative predictor of PWB.
Combination of self-compassion variables explained 19% of the variance and significantly
predicted PWB (R2 =.19, adjusted R2 = .14), F(6, 13) = 4.30, p < .01) with only mindfulness (β
=.27, t = -2.49, p < .05) as significant predictors of PWB.
Finally, the model for combined total scores of emotional intelligence, rumination and self-
compassion was tested. This combination of factors explained 51% of the variance and
significantly predicted PWB (R2 = .51, adjusted R2 = .50), F(3, 116) = 40.77, p < .001) with only
global emotional intelligence as a significant positive predictor of PWB (β =.76, t = 9.64, p
<.001).
Table 2.
Predictors of Psychological Well-being.
Predictor
B
SE
β
95% CI
Emotional Intelligence
Emotionality
.15
.09
.13
-0.03, 0.33
Sociability
.31
.10
.25**
0.11, 0 .51
Well-being
.45
.08
.46***
0.29, 0.60
Self-control
.07
.09
.06
-0.12, 0.25
R2
.52
F
31.69***
Rumination
Brooding
.07
.04
.22
-0.01, 0.15
Reflection
.10
.03
.32**
0.04, 0.16
Depression related
-.09
.02
-.69***
-0.12, -0.06
R2
.25
F
12.54***
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 115
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Self-compassion
Self-kindness
.04
.03
.15
-0.02, 0.10
Self-judgment
-.03
.03
-.12
-0.10, 0.03
Common Humanity
-.03
.03
-.10
-0.10, 0.03
Isolation
.02
.03
.06
-0.04, 0.07
Mindfulness
.10
.04
.27*
[0.02, 0.17]
Over-identified
.05
.03
.18
[-0.01, 0.12]
R2
.19
F
4.30**
Total indices of EI, Rumination, and Self-compassion
Emotional intelligence
1.16
.12
.76***
[0.92, 1.40]
Rumination
.01
.01
.14
[-0.00, 0.02]
Self-compassion
.00
.01
.04
[-0.01,0.01]
R2
.52
F
4.77***
Note: PWB=Psychological well-being; EI=Emotional Intelligence; *p <.05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Mediation and moderation analyses were performed to get deeper insights into the direct and
indirect relationships between emotional intelligence factors, rumination, and psychological well-
being in order to underline the mechanism of the relationship between these variables and find
out whether the connection between two certain variables depends on the value of the third.
Specifically, mediation analysis was applied (Conditional Process Modeling by Hayes, 2018;
Model 4) to study whether the relationship between emotional intelligence factors and PWB or
between rumination and PWB was mediated by self-compassion. Examining the role of self-
compassion as a mediator in the relationship between emotional intelligence factors and
psychological well-being indicates that there is a significant indirect effect of the sociability factor
of emotional intelligence on psychological well-being through self-compassion (R2 = .36, F(2,
117) = 33.05, p < .005. b = .04; se = .02; BCa CI [.00, .09]. The total effect of the sociability
factor of EI on PWB is significant (b = .55, se = .10, t = 7.21, p <.005). When controlling for the
mediator, the direct effect of sociability on PWB remains significant (b =.51, se = .09, t = 6.76, p
< .005) (see Figure 1), meaning that self-compassion is a significant mediator variable in the
relationship of sociability factor of EI and well-being.
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 116
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Then, it was checked whether rumination (depression related) leads to low self-compassion and
decreases psychological well-being. Since mediation analysis offers an opportunity to find out if
the relationship between two variables can be significantly affected by the participation of the
third, mediator variable, the mediational analysis (Conditional Process Modeling by Hayes,
2018; Model 4) was used and the role of self-compassion as a mediator in between rumination
and psychological well-being was tested. The results indicate that there is a significant indirect
effect of depression related rumination on psychological well-being through self-compassion (R2
= .16, F(2, 117) = 11.55, p < .005. b = -.10; se = .04; BCa CI [-.19, -.02]. The total effect of
depression related rumination on PWB is significant (b = -.37, se = .01, t = -4.29; p < .005).
When controlling for the mediator, the direct effect of depression related rumination on PWB
remains significant (b = -.27, se = .01, t = -2.74 p = .04) (see Figure 2). The results suggest that
self-compassion significantly mediates the relationship between depression related rumination
and PWB.
Sociability
Psychological
Well-being
Self-compassion
b
= .18; se =1.53;
p
=.04
b
= .23, se = .01,
p
=.00
Total effect
b=
.55; se = .10;
p
=.000
Direct effect
b=
.51; se = .09;
p
=.000
Indirect effect
b=
.04; se = .02; 95%, CI[.00, .09]
Figure 1. The Indirect Effect of Sociability Factor (EI) on Psychological Well-being
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 117
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Afterwards, moderation analysis (Conditional Process Modeling by Hayes, 2018; Model 1) was
used to determine whether the relationship between rumination and psychological well-being
depends on the value of certain factors of emotional intelligence and whether EI has a
significant effect on the connection between those two variables. The results of moderation
show that emotionality and self-control factors of EI moderate the relationship between
reflection rumination and PWB. Specifically, the moderation model, including the emotionality
factor (EI) as a moderator in the relationship between reflection rumination and PWB, is
significant (F (3, 116) = 17.12; p < .005 R2=.22). For average scores on an emotionality factor,
there is no relationship between reflection rumination and PWB (b = .02, t = 0.87, p = .38). For
low (b = .11, t = 3.14, p = 0.002) and high scores on the emotionality factor (b = -0.07, t = -2.04,
p = .04), the relationship between reflection rumination and PWB becomes significant (see
Figure 3). Table 3 presents the regression results.
Depression related
rumination
Psychological
Well-being
Self-compassion
b
= -.50; se =.14;
p
=.000
b
= .20; se = .01;
p
=.04
Total effect
b=
-37; se = .01;
p
=.000
Direct effect
b=
.-27; se = .01;
p
=.000
Indirect effect
b=
-.08; se = .04; 95%, CI[-.16, -.003]
Figure 2. The Indirect Effect of Self-compassion on Psychological Well-being
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 118
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Table 3.
Regression Results from Moderation Analysis
b
SE
t
Constant -4.90 [-8.98, -0.83] 2.06 -2.38
Reflection
.50[0.23, -0.77]
0.13
3.75***
Emotionality
2.04 [1.22, 2.87]
0.42
4.91 ***
Emotionality x reflection
-.09 [-0.15, -0.46]
0.03
-3.68 ***
ΔR2 = .31. ***p < .001, *p < .05.
Figure 3. Simple slopes of reflection (rumination) predicting PWB for 1 SD below the mean of
emotionality factor (EI), the mean of emotionality (EI), and 1 SD above the mean of emotionality
(EI). Notes. PWB=psychological well-being; EI= emotional intelligence
Self-control, the factor of emotional intelligence, is also a significant moderator of the
relationship between reflection rumination and PWB (F (3, 116) = 11.12; p < .005 R2 = .22). The
conditional effect of reflection rumination was tested at three levels, low, average, and high
levels of self-control in order to get comprehensive information about the mechanism of
moderation. For low (b =.08, t = 2.55, p = .01] scores on the self-control factor, there is a
significant relationship between reflection rumination and PWB. While for average (b =.04, t =
1.55, p =.12) and high scores on the self-control factor (b =-.00, t (269) = -0.25, p =.80) the
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 119
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
relationship between reflection rumination and PWB is not significant (see Figure 4). Table 4
presents the regression results from the moderation analysis.
Table 4.
Regression Results from Moderation Analysis.
b
SE
t
Constant -1.12 [8.12, 10.73] 1.75 -0.63
Reflection
.27[-0.76, -0.27]
0.11
2.47*
Self-control
1.36 [1.37, 3.14]
0.40
3.40 ***
Self-control x reflection
-.05 [-0.29, -0.04]
0.03
-2.15 *
ΔR2 = .22. ***p < .001, *p < .05.
Figure 4. Simple slopes of reflection (rumination) predicting PWB for 1 SD below the mean of
self-control (EI), the mean of self-control (EI), and 1 SD above the mean of self-control (EI).
Notes. PWB=psychological well-being; EI= emotional intelligence.
Discussion
The results of correlation and regression analyses show a strong positive association between
emotional intelligence and psychological well-being, which is in line with the great part of the
scientific literature (e.g., Extremera et al., 2011; Sánchez-Álvarez et al., 2015; Zeidner & Olnick-
Shemesh, 2010). It turns out that emotional intelligence is an important contributor to
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 120
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
maintaining well-being in the case of self-isolation. This can be explained by the fact that people
with high EI are more likely to use effective coping strategies in stressful situations (Enns et al.,
2018; Hajisabbagh et al., 2019; Prentice et al., 2020).
The predictive value of certain emotional intelligence factors for well-being can be explained by
the characteristics of the stressful situation. The sociability factor, which implies feeling
comfortable in social contexts, being self-confident and open to social situations, predicts well-
being in self-isolation - in restricted social and replaced with online communication conditions.
Also, the predictive value of the trait well-being factor, which measures how one can enjoy life,
expect positive things to happen to her/him and remain confident in the face of adversity, is
completely reasonable.
As was expected according to research (Newman & Nezlek, 2017), there is a negative
correlation between well-being and rumination. These outcomes are consistent with other
studies where rumination is considered as a less effective strategy for increasing PWB (Peuters
et al., 2019). However, a study (Páez et al., 2013), where rumination is not referred to as a
maladaptive coping strategy, contradicts the results. But it can be assumed that the only
effective role that rumination can play is that of reflection rumination, which can help people
cope with stress by increasing positive affect.
One more important result is a relationship between trait EI and rumination. Emotional
intelligence negatively correlates with rumination, which is in line with the above-mentioned
hypothesis. Correlational analysis revealed a negative relationship between trait EI and the total
score of rumination. Among the key factors of emotional intelligence, rumination is most strongly
correlated with the trait well-being factor and self-control. This result is supported by reviewed
studies according to which EI is a protective factor against rumination (Abdollahi & Talib, 2015),
and people with low EI use rumination for coping (Kircaburun et al., 2019). Therefore,
rumination may play an essential role during self-isolation and reduce psychological well-being.
Furthermore, depression related rumination has a strong negative correlation with the main
factors of emotional intelligence (trait well-being and self-control). This can be explained by the
fact that depression related rumination is defined as concentrating on signs of depression and
acting according to these signs (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991) and, therefore, is an ineffective
strategy for coping. Other studies also consider rumination as a maladaptive coping strategy
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 121
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
that leads to reduced well-being (Newman & Nezlek, 2017). As for reflection rumination, it
doesn’t relate significantly to emotional intelligence either. However, it’s considered a better
coping strategy compared to other types of rumination, as it involves re-evaluating past and
present events, feelings, and behaviors.
It’s important to note that emotional intelligence moderates the relationship between rumination
and well-being. However, unlike depression related rumination, reflection rumination has a
predictive effect on self-control and emotionality, which act as moderator variables between
reflection rumination and well-being. While for low scores on the self-control factor, there is a
significant relationship between reflection rumination and well-being, the relationship between
reflection rumination and well-being is not significant for average and high scores on the self-
control factor. Self-control is defined as the effective regulation of one’s own emotions and the
ability to cope with environmental stressors easily (Petrides, 2009). Therefore, at the low level of
self-control, reflection rumination helps self-isolated individuals to cope with a stressful situation.
It can be speculated that the reason why the relationship between reflection rumination and
well-being is not significant for average and high scores on self-control could be that the
participants were more likely to avoid using this strategy because, with the help of self-control,
they perceived the situation as less stressful. Therefore, people who have high scores on the
self-control scale, cope with the conditions of self-isolation effectively and there’s no need to use
other strategies like reflection rumination.
Similar to this, a low level of emotionality leads to a positive association between rumination and
well-being, and in this case, the participants used rumination to cope with stress. But reflection
rumination had a negative impact on well-being in the case of high scores on the emotionality
scale. This result can be explained by the fact that emotionality can be defined as the
perception and expression of emotions and the understanding of other people's viewpoints and
taking their feelings into account (Petrides, 2009). Conscious thinking about their own and
others’ emotional states during stressful situations may prevent people high on emotionality to
remain calm and consequently, it results in a low level of well-being. This interpretation is
supported by the idea that high EI can be detrimental to its possessor and there might also be
an intrapersonal dark side of EI (Austin, 2018). In fact, EI and specifically, emotion perception, is
associated with an increased stress reaction (Bechtoldt & Schneider, 2016). Therefore, emotion
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 122
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
perception and even empathy towards others can be damaging for high EI individuals in
stressful situations.
Another interesting outcome is that while reflection rumination is a positive predictor of well-
being, depression related rumination is negative, as already noted. However, this result can be
interpreted by self-compassion which acts as a mediator variable between depression related
rumination and PWB. Depression related rumination probably reduces self-compassion, and
since self-compassion is an effective strategy for well-being (Bluth & Blanton, 2014), reduced
self-compassion leads to lower PWB. Therefore, the relationship between depression related
rumination and psychological well-being and the effect of self-compassion as a mediator is very
interesting to study further in the future.
In line with the research hypothesis, self-compassion plays an important role in the process of
maintaining well-being in a stressful situation. Particularly, self-compassion positively predicts
psychological well-being, which is consistent with previous studies (Neff et al., 2007; Odou &
Brinker, 2014). Among the variables of self-compassion, mindfulness is the only significant
predictor of well-being. Besides, the scales and factors of emotional intelligence relate positively
to self-compassion. This result is consistent with previous studies (e.g., Neff, 2003). The results
also showed that emotional intelligence positively relates to each scale of self-compassion.
Considering this, it could be concluded that self-compassion can act as an effective coping
strategy in stressful situations like self-isolation.
The role of self-compassion as a mediator between emotional intelligence and well-being is also
very interesting. The results showed that self-compassion and emotional intelligence affect the
relationship between rumination and well-being. Probably, people with high emotional
intelligence who use the self-compassion strategy, cope with self-isolation better, and as a
result, the negative effect of rumination is reduced. Also, the results showed that the sociability
factor has a significant indirect effect on well-being through self-compassion. This means that
high emotional intelligence leads self-isolated people to high self-compassion and,
consequently, to psychological well-being. Even though the direct relationship between
sociability and well-being is also statistically significant, it’s likely that sociability helps people
who use the self-compassion strategy to cope with self-isolation and maintain a high level of
well-being.
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 123
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Conclusion
To sum up, the study shows the positive effect of emotional intelligence and self-compassion on
the one hand and the negative effect of rumination on the other in maintaining psychological
well-being during self-isolation. It turns out that both trait emotional intelligence and
psychological well-being are negatively correlated with rumination, meaning that rumination can
play a significant role in reducing well-being and it’s a less effective coping strategy for self-
isolated people, while self-compassion positively predicts well-being. However, the relationship
between depression related rumination and well-being can be moderated by emotional
intelligence factors. Trait EI factors moderate the relationship between reflection rumination and
PWB, but this type of rumination positively predicts well-being. Consequently, reflection
rumination can be a positive coping strategy for self-isolated people, unlike depression related
rumination, which reduces self-compassion and leads to lower PWB as a result.
Considering these results, it can be concluded that self-isolated people with higher EI are more
likely to use self-compassion for coping in this stressful situation and have better psychological
well-being, while the use of rumination complicates coping with self-isolation.
Since the pandemic goes on and the future situation of the related restrictions can’t be
predicted, these results could be used to help self-isolated people effectively cope with stressful
situations like self-isolation and avoid using maladaptive strategies such as rumination which
can significantly reduce psychological well-being. Instead, attention could be focused on self-
compassion and study its positive role in maintaining well-being in comparison with rumination.
Limitations and Future Directions of the Study
The study has some limitations. First, the sample size is not big enough and a large proportion
of it consists of psychology students. However, the main criterion of the sample was to select
participants whose lifestyle was dramatically changed by the potentially stressful event, as they
had to stay in self-isolation for at least three weeks and they had to have restricted interaction
with other people. Nevertheless, in the future, it’s more optimal to use a bigger and more
diverse sample. Besides, the cross-sectional design that is used in the study doesn’t allow us to
make conclusions about causality. It’s difficult to determine whether psychological well-being is
a direct outcome of self-compassion and emotional intelligence or exposure. But future research
should be focused on getting the longitudinal data about the causal connection between the
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 124
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
variables. Another limitation is related to using solely the positive indices for studying emotional
state. Although the main focus of the study was to examine the factors that help people maintain
psychological well-being in stressful situations, still, it would be better to evaluate the symptoms
of psychological distress too. Notwithstanding these restrictions, the current study sheds light on
the mechanisms of how emotional intelligence might lead to well-being. It enhances existing
knowledge about the factors helping individuals to maintain psychological well-being in a
stressful situation and uncertainty.
Funding/Financial Support
The authors have no funding to report
Other Support/Acknowledgement
The authors have no support to report.
Competing Interests
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
References
Abdollahi, A., & Talib, M. A. (2015). Emotional intelligence as a mediator between rumination and suicidal
ideation among depressed inpatients: The moderating role of suicidal history. Psychiatry Research,
228(3), 591–597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.046
Austin, E. J (2018). Trait and Ability Conceptualizations of Emotional Intelligence. In L.D. Pool & P.
Qualter (eds.), An Introduction to Emotional Intelligence (pp. 18-31). John Wiley & Sons.
Bechtoldt, M. N., & Schneider, V. K. (2016). Predicting stress from the ability to eavesdrop on feelings:
Emotional intelligence and testosterone jointly predict cortisol reactivity. Emotion, 16(6), 815–
825. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000134
Bluth, K., & Blanton, P. W. (2014). The influence of self-compassion on emotional well being among early
and older adolescent males and females. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(3), 219–230.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.936967
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 125
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Bowen, P. W., Pilkington, A., & Rose, R. (2016). The relationship between emotional intelligence and well
being in academic employees. International Journal of Social Science Studies, 4(5).
https://doi.org/10.11114/ijsss.v4i5.1487
Brehl, A. K., Kohn, N. & Fernández, G. (2021). Maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in a vulnerable
population predict increased anxiety during the Covid-19 pandemic: A pseudo-prospective study,
Journal of Affective Disorders Reports, 4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2021.100113
Castilho, P., Carvalho, S. A., Marques, S., & Pinto-Gouveia, J. (2017). Self-compassion and emotional
intelligence in adolescence: a multigroup mediational study of the impact of shame memories on
depressive symptoms. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(3), 759–768.
Di Fabio, A., & Kenny, M. E. (2016). Promoting Well being: The Contribution of Emotional Intelligence.
Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01182
Di Fabio, A., & Saklofske, D.H. (2021). The relationship of compassion and self-compassion with
personality and emotional intelligence, Personality and Individual Differences, 169(1).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110109
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., & Oishi, S. (2009). New
Measures of Well being. Social Indicators Research Series, 39, 247–266. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-
2354-4_12
Enns, A., Eldridge, G.D., Montgomery, C., & Gonzalez, V.M. (2018). Perceived stress, coping strategies,
and emotional intelligence: A cross-sectional study of university students in helping disciplines.
Nurse Education Today. 68, 226-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2018.06.012
Extremera, N., Ruiz-Aranda, D., Pineda-Galán, C., & Salguero, J. M. (2011). Emotional intelligence and
its relation with hedonic and eudaimonic well being: A prospective study. Personality and Individual
Differences, 51(1), 11–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.029
Fan, S., Guan, J., Cao, L., Wang, M., Zhao, H., Chen, L., & Yan, L. (2021). Psychological effects caused
by COVID-19 pandemic on pregnant women: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Asian
Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 102-533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102533
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 126
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Fernández, C. M., Álvarez, R. J., Ávalos, R.I., Cuevas, L. M., de Barros, C. C., Díaz, R. F., González, C.
E, González, G. D., Hernández, F. A., Ibáñez, P. & Lizarte, S. E. (2020). Evaluation of the
Emotional and Cognitive Regulation of Young People in a Lockdown Situation Due to the Covid-19
Pandemic. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565503
Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high shame and self-
criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach. Clinical Psychology &
Psychotherapy, 13, 353–379. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.507
Görgens-Ekermans, G., & Brand, T. (2012). Emotional intelligence as a moderator in the stress-burnout
relationship: a questionnaire study on nurses. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 21(15-16), 2275–2285.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2702.2012.04171.x
Hajisabbagh, N., Fereidooni-Moghadam, M., & Etemadifar, M. (2019). Coping strategies and their
relationship with emotional intelligence in patients with epilepsy referred to Isfahan Epilepsy
Society in 2017. Epilepsy & Behavior: E&B. 92, 200-205.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.12.032
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Quantification, inference,
and interpretation. Communication Monographs, 85(1), 4–40.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.1352100
Heffernan, M., Quinn Griffin, M. T, Sister, R. M., & Fitzpatrick, J.J. (2010). Self-compassion and emotional
intelligence in nurses. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 16(4): 366-373.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-172X.2010.01853.x
Ju, Y., Chen, W., Liu, J., Yang, A., Shu, K., Zhou, Y., Wang, M., Huang, M., Liao, M., Liu, J., Liu, B., &
Zhang, Y. (2021). Effects of centralized isolation vs. home isolation on psychological distress in
patients with COVID-19. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 143, 110-365.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2021.110365
Kircaburun, K., Griffiths, M. D., & Billieux, J. (2019). Trait emotional intelligence and problematic online
behaviors among adolescents: The mediating role of mindfulness, rumination, and depression.
Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 208–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.11.024
Kong, F., Gong, X., Sajjad, S., Yang, K., & Zhao, J. (2019). How Is Emotional Intelligence Linked to Life
Satisfaction? The Mediating Role of Social Support, Positive Affect and Negative Affect. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 20 (3). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-00069-4
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 127
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Kousha, M., Bagheri, H. A., & Heydarzadeh, A. (2018). Emotional intelligence and anxiety, stress, and
depression in Iranian resident physicians. Journal of family medicine and primary care, 7(2), 420–
424. https://doi.org/10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_154_17
Lanciano, T., Curci, A., Kafetsios, K., Elia, L., & Zammuner, V. L. (2012). Attachment and dysfunctional
rumination: The mediating role of Emotional Intelligence abilities. Personality and Individual
Differences, 53(6), 753–758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.027
Liu, M., & Ren, S. (2018). Moderating Effect of Emotional Intelligence on the Relationship between
Rumination and Anxiety. Current Psychology, 37:272–279. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-
9510-7
Marcus, D. K., Hughes, K. T., & Arnau, R. C. (2008). Health anxiety, rumination, and negative affect: A
mediational analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 64(5), 495-501.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.004
Marks, A. D. G., Horrocks, K. A., & Schutte, N. S. (2016). Emotional intelligence mediates the relationship
between insecure attachment and subjective health outcomes. Personality and Individual
Differences, 98, 188–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.038
Martskvishvili, K., & Arutinov, L., & Mestvirishvili, M. (2013). A Psychometric Investigation of the Georgian
Version of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological
Assessmen, 29(2), 84-88. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000135
Matthews, G., & Zeidner, M. (2000). Emotional intelligence, adaptation to stressful encounters, and health
outcomes. In R. Bar-On & J. D. A. Parker (eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory,
development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace (p. 459–489).
Jossey-Bass.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. J. Sluyter (Eds.),
Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (p. 3–34). Basic
Books.
McLaughlin, K. A., Borkovec, T. D., & Sibrava, N. J. (2007). The effects of worry and rumination on affect
states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy, 38(1), 23-38.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.03.003
Michl, L. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Shepherd, K., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Rumination as a
mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: Longitudinal
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 128
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
evidence in early adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(2), 339–352.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031994
Min, J. (2013). The Relationships Between Emotional Intelligence, Job Stress, and Quality of Life Among
Tour Guides. Asia Pacific Journal of Tourism Research, 19(10), 1170–1190.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10941665.2013.839459
Moroń, M., & Biolik-Moroń, M. (2020). Trait emotional intelligence and emotional experiences during the
COVID-19 pandemic outbreak in Poland: A daily diary study. Personality and Individual
Differences, 168(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110348
Neff, K. D. (2003). The Development and Validation of a Scale to Measure Self-Compassion. Self and
Identity, 2(3), 223–250. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860309027
Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self-esteem: Two different ways of relating
to oneself. Journal of Personality, 77(1), 23–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00537.x
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y., & Dejitterat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, achievement goals, and coping with
academic failure. Self and Identity, 4(3), 263–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500444000317
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and adaptive psychological
functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 139–154.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004
Newman, D. B., & Nezlek, J. B. (2017). Private self-consciousness in daily life: Relationships between
rumination and reflection and well being, and meaning in daily life. Personality and Individual
Differences, 136, 184-189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.039
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive
episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-
843X.100.4.569
Odou, N., & Brinker, J. (2014). Self-compassion, a better alternative to rumination than distraction as a
response to negative mood. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(5), 447–457.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.967800
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 129
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Páez, D., Martínez-Sánchez, F., Mendiburo, A., Bobowik, M., & Sevillano, V. (2013). Affect regulation
strategies and perceived emotional adjustment for negative and positive affect: A study on anger,
sadness and joy. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(3), 249–262.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.786751
Panayiotou, G., Panteli, M., & Leonidou, C. (2021). Coping with the invisible enemy: The role of emotion
regulation and awareness in quality of life during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Contextual
Behavioral Science, 19(10), 17–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.11.002
Paredes, M. R., Apaolaza, V., Fernandez-Robin, C., Hartmann, P., & Yañez-Martinez, D. (2020). The
impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on subjective mental well being: The interplay of perceived
threat, future anxiety and resilience. Personality and Individual Differences, 170, 110-455.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110455
Peña-Sarrionandia, A., Mikolajczak, M., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Integrating emotion regulation and
emotional intelligence traditions: a meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(160).
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00160
Petrides, K. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue).
In C. Stough, D. H. Saklofske, & J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), The Springer series on human
exceptionality. Assessing emotional intelligence: Theory, research, and applications (p. 85–101).
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_5
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with
reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15(6), 425–448.
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.416
Petrides, K., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The Location of Trait Emotional Intelligence in Personality
Factor Space. British journal of psychology, 98(2), 273-89.
https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606X120618
Peuters, C., Kalokerinos, E. K., Pe, M. L., & Kuppens, P. (2019). Sequential effects of reappraisal and
rumination on anger during recall of an anger-provoking event. PLOS ONE, 14(1).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209029
Well-being and Pandemic Lockdown 130
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Prentice, C., Zeidan, S., & Wang, X. (2020). Personality, trait EI and coping with COVID 19 measures.
International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, 51(1):101789.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101789
Restubog, S. L. D., Ocampo, A. C. G., & Wang, L. (2020). Taking control amidst the chaos: Emotion
regulation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 119: 103440.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103440
Salovey, P., Detweiler-Bedell, B. T., Detweiler-Bedell, J. B., & Mayer, J. D. (2008). Emotional intelligence.
In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (p. 533–547). The
Guilford Press.
Sánchez-Álvarez, N., Extremera, N., & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2015). The relation between emotional
intelligence and subjective well being: A meta-analytic investigation. The Journal of Positive
Psychology, 11(3), 276–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1058968
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Simunek, M., McKenley, J., & Hollander, S. (2002). Characteristic
emotional intelligence and emotional well being. Cognition and Emotion, 16(6), 769–785.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000482
Solomou, I., & Constantinidou, F. (2020). Prevalence and Predictors of Anxiety and Depression
Symptoms during the COVID-19 Pandemic and Compliance with Precautionary Measures: Age
and Sex Matter. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(14), 49-24.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144924
Tyra, A.T., Ginty, A.T. & John-Henderson, N.A. (2021). Emotion Regulation Strategies Predict PTSS
During the COVID-19 Pandemic in an American Indian Population. International Journal of
Behavioral Medicine, 1-5. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-021-09964-2
Wang, Q. Q., Fang, Y. Y., Huang, H. L., Lv, W. J., Wang, X. X., Yang, T. T., Yuan, J. M., Gao, Y., Qian,
R. L., & Zhang, Y. H. (2021). Anxiety, depression and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in
Chinese nurses during the COVID-19 outbreak. Journal of Nursing Management.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jonm.13265
Martskvishvili & Lagidze 131
Psychological Thought South-West University “Neofit Rilski”
2022, Vol. 15(1), 105-131
https://doi.org/10.37708/psyct.v15i1.623
Yalvaç, Elif & Gaynor, Keith. (2021). Emotional Dysregulation in Adults: The Influence of Rumination and
Negative Secondary Appraisals of Emotion. Journal of Affective Disorders, 282 (9).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.194
Zeidner, M., & Olnick-Shemesh, D. (2010). Emotional intelligence and subjective well being revisited.
Personality and Individual Differences, 48(4), 431–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.011
About the authors
Khatuna Martskvishvili, Ph.D., Associate Professor, Faculty of Psychology and Educational
Sciences, Ivane Javakhishvili Tbilisi State University, Georgia
Nato Lagidze, MA student (Psychology of Influences and Communication), Faculty of
Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ivane Javakhishvili Tbilisi State University, Georgia.
Corresponding Author's Contact Address[TOP]
Email: nato.laghidze8343@pes.tsu.edu.ge