ArticlePublisher preview available
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

This article discusses the role of gestures in enhancing inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility as the three components of executive functions during the processing of mathematical concepts that are metaphorically described in terms of motion events. Gestures can contribute to the process of inhibition by highlighting the relevant information and keeping the irrelevant information out of focus of attention. Gestures contribute to working memory in two ways during mathematical processing. They increase activity in the motor areas of the brain. Therefore, they may facilitate the process of understanding those mathematical concepts that are described in terms of motion event, as the motor system could play a role in the grounding and the processing of these concepts. Also, gestures can function as an external working memory and keep the visual representation of some parts of information for a short period of time in order to manipulate that information in later stages of processing. Gestures enhance cognitive flexibility by allowing us to have a spatial representation of that concept or idea for a period of time. During this time, we can shift our perspective and process that concept or idea from a variety of perspectives.
REGULAR ARTICLE
1 3
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-022-09694-4
Abstract
This article discusses the role of gestures in enhancing inhibition, working memory,
and cognitive exibility as the three components of executive functions during the
processing of mathematical concepts that are metaphorically described in terms of
motion events. Gestures can contribute to the process of inhibition by highlighting
the relevant information and keeping the irrelevant information out of focus of
attention. Gestures contribute to working memory in two ways during mathemati-
cal processing. They increase activity in the motor areas of the brain. Therefore,
they may facilitate the process of understanding those mathematical concepts that
are described in terms of motion event, as the motor system could play a role in
the grounding and the processing of these concepts. Also, gestures can function as
an external working memory and keep the visual representation of some parts of
information for a short period of time in order to manipulate that information in
later stages of processing. Gestures enhance cognitive exibility by allowing us to
have a spatial representation of that concept or idea for a period of time. During this
time, we can shift our perspective and process that concept or idea from a variety
of perspectives.
Keywords Gestures · Executive functions · Mathematical concepts · Embodied
cognition
Introduction
The role of gesture in the processing of concepts has been the subject of a large body
of research (e.g., Johnson-Glenberg & Megowan-Romanowicz, 2017; Macedonia,
2019; Yeo et al., 2017; Radford, 2003). Some works have supported the idea that
gestures play a causal role in the process of learning new material and the develop-
Accepted: 9 April 2022
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature
2022
Gestures Enhance Executive Functions for the Understating
of Mathematical Concepts
OmidKhatin-Zadeh1· ZahraEskandari2· FernandoMarmolejo-Ramos3
Extended author information available on the last page of the article
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... They add that motor components may play a key role in mental imagery. The role of the motor system in the understanding of abstract concepts has been emphasized in a large number of works (e.g., Glenberg and Kaschak, 2002;Nathan and Walkington, 2017;Khatin-Zadeh et al., 2019, 2022aMacedonia, 2019;Harpaintner et al., 2020). The following section discusses several examples in which mathematical entities are described in terms of visual representations and motion domains. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article discusses the cognitive process of transforming one representation of mathematical entities into another representation. This process, which has been called mathematical metaphor, allows us to understand and embody a difficult-to-understand mathematical entity in terms of an easy-to-understand entity. When one representation of a mathematical entity is transformed into another representation, more cognitive resources such as the visual and motor systems can come into play to understand the target entity. Because of their nature, some curves, which are one group of visual representations, may have a great motor strength. It is suggested that directedness, straightness, length, and thinness are some possible features that determine degree of motor strength of a curve. Another possible factor that can determine motor strength of a curve is the strength of association between shape of the curve and past experiences of the observer (and her/his prior knowledge). If an individual has had the repetitive experience of observing objects moving along a certain curve, the shape of the curve may have a great motor strength for her/him. In fact, it can be said that some kind of metonymic relationship may be formed between the shapes of some curves and movement experiences.
... Within the fields related to social and cultural aspects of language, metaphors are investigated as tools for social communication (e.g., Martynyuk & Meleshchenki, 2022;Mohnke et al., 2022). Some researchers have looked at metaphors as a process for mathematical cognition (e.g., Khatin-Zadeh, Eskandari et al., 2022;Lakoff & Núñez, 2001;Núñez & Lakoff, 1998). In poetry and literature, metaphor is studied as a figurative tool to beatify language. ...
Article
Full-text available
Results of the studies on the way language and body coordinate in communication (through language and gesture) have significant implications for language, education, and cognitive studies. However, there is a lack of convenient research in this area analyzing perceptual changes within individuals. To fill this gap, we investigated the embodied realizations of literal sentences representing perceptual changes and metaphorical sentences describing the phenomena in terms of perceptual changes. We classified sentences that referred to a visual, auditory, haptic, gustatory, or olfactory change. Participants listened to four narratives that contained five literal sentences describing a real perceptual change and five metaphorical sentences that described a phenomenon in terms of a perceptual change. We analyzed the recorded videos of the participants. The total number of literal sentences that described a real perceptual change was obtained for each modality (visual, auditory, haptic, gustatory, olfactory) separately. Also, the total number of gestures used with each category of these literal statements was obtained. Only iconic and non-iconic gestures that described such perceptual changes as the movement of an object in the space were counted. The results revealed that gestures accompanied at least 53% of literal and 56% of metaphorical sentences. These results suggest that literal and metaphorical perceptual change sentences could be understood as movements at a conceptual level. The similarity between embodied realizations of literal and metaphorical sentences supports one of the main assumptions of the strong version of embodied cognition.
... They play an important role in learning and problem-solving situations (Goldin-Meadow et al., 2012; see also Schwartz & Black, 1996). Gestures improve inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility during the processing of mathematical concepts (Khatin-Zadeh et al., 2022) and are an important resource in the acquisition of a second language (Gullberg et al., 2010). Kita et al. (2017) proposed the gesture-for-conceptualization hypothesis, where self-directed representational gestures have a cognitive function. ...
Article
The position I take in this paper is that investigation of the construction and development of executive function (EF) and cognitive self-regulation must give children the opportunity to behave with initiative, as the agents they are. It must be based on children’s goal-directed behaviours and the challenges they face to achieve their own goals from the last third of the first year of life. It must consider children’s interests, the ecological validity of the situations, and the social context. Investigation into the origin and early development of cognitive self-regulation must be firmly anchored within a developmental framework. I will suggest that such developmental framework is provided by the functional turn developed in the School of Geneva in the 1970s and its influence in the Pragmatics of the Object perspective. According to this pragmatic approach actions and (private) gestures are central in the emergence and construction of EF.
... This could explain why body movements are so effective in enhancing students' learning in mathematics. In recent two decades, a large body of works on embodied cognition has emphasized the effective role of body, motion, and gesture in enhancing science and mathematics learning (e.g., Abrahamson et al., 2020;Khatin-Zadeh et al., 2022;Schaefer, 2019;Shvarts et al., 2021;Zona et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Motion-based metaphors help explain a single, often a static, concept (e.g., number, mathematical function, limit of function, continuity of function) in terms of a human motion. In mathematics, many mathematical concepts, such as function and continuity, are described in terms of graphical representations. Although these graphical representations are static, they can be transformed into motion events and understood as motions by motion-based metaphors. This can be done by either using a hand gesture to depict the graphical representation of the target concept or by mentally simulating hand movements that depict the graphical representation. By employing these mechanisms, the motor system becomes engaged in the process of aiding the learner to understand static mathematical concepts (concepts that are defined in terms of non-moving mathematical objects), acting as a cognitive resource to ground and understand non-motion mathematical concepts. In this paper, we theorize that visual representations of mathematical concepts have varying degrees of what we term to be “motor strength” whereby, for example, curves of functions may be either strongly or weakly motoric depending on the degree to which they aid in the development of associated deep mathematical thinking.
Article
Full-text available
In this study, we examined the types of gestures that occurred with motion-based, static space-based, static object-based, and static event-based metaphors. A group of participants listened to three short stories and then retold them in front of a camera. The camera could record participants’ gestures. Each story contained two metaphors of each type. Our aim was to calculate the frequency of occurrence of metaphoric gestures with these metaphors when participants were retelling the stories. We used a contingency table analysis to make a comparison between the four types of gestures in terms of the number of metaphoric gestures that occurred with them. The results of this analysis showed that the number of metaphoric gestures that occurred with motion-based and static space-based metaphors was significantly higher than the number of metaphoric gestures that occurred with static object-based and static event-based metaphors.
Article
Full-text available
Bu çalışma depremi yaşayan 6-10 yaş arası çocuklara okunan metaforik hikayenin, çocukların yaptıkları resimlerin temaları ile incelenmesi amacıyla yapılmıştır. Çalışma nitel araştırma yöntemlerinden Durum Çalışması tekniğinde tasarlanmıştır. Araştırmanın çalışma grubunu, 20 Şubat Kahraman Maraş merkezli deprem felaketinden sonra, Konya iline gelerek, yurtlara yerleştirilen ve yurtlar içinde açılan okullara devam eden temel eğitim düzeyindeki çocuklar oluşturmaktadır. Verilerin toplanması için serbest resim çizim tekniği kullanılmıştır. Ön test verileri olarak toplanan bu resimlerin ardından çocuklara, Kahraman Maraş merkezli depremden etkilenen çocuklar için yazılmış olan metaforik hikaye okunmuştur. Bir gün sonra çocuklardan tekrar resim yapmaları istenmiştir. Hikâye sonrası istenen resimler son test verileri olarak kullanılmıştır. Çocuklardan alınan resimlerin içeriği iki uzman araştırmacı tarafından incelenmiş ve resim içerisindeki tüm temalar not edilmiştir. NVivo 14 paket programı kullanılarak temalar, ortak özelliklerine göre kodlara ayrılmış ve 29 kod elde edilmiş ve aynı program ile karşılaştırmalar yapılmıştır. Çalışmanın sonuçlarına göre çocukların resim temaları incelendiğinde, metaforik hikayenin tüm yaş gruplarında(6-10 yaş) olumlu değişimlere yol açtığı görülmüştür.
Article
Purpose: Real-world communication is situated in rich multimodal contexts, containing speech and gesture. Speakers often convey unique information in gesture that is not present in the speech signal (e.g., saying "He searched for a new recipe" while making a typing gesture). We examine the narrative retellings of participants with and without moderate-severe traumatic brain injury across three timepoints over two online Zoom sessions to investigate whether people with TBI can integrate information from co-occurring speech and gesture and if information from gesture persists across delays. Methods: 60 participants with TBI and 60 non-injured peers watched videos of a narrator telling four short stories. On key details, the narrator produced complementary gestures that conveyed unique information. Participants retold the stories at three timepoints: immediately after, 20-min later, and one-week later. We examined the words participants used when retelling these key details, coding them as a Speech Match (e.g., "He searched for a new recipe"), a Gesture Match (e.g., "He searched for a new recipe online), or Other ("He looked for a new recipe"). We also examined whether participants produced representative gestures themselves when retelling these details. Results: Despite recalling fewer story details, participants with TBI were as likely as non-injured peers to report information from gesture in their narrative retellings. All participants were more likely to report information from gesture and produce representative gestures themselves one-week later compared to immediately after hearing the story. Conclusion: We demonstrated that speech-gesture integration is intact after TBI in narrative retellings. This finding has exciting implications for the utility of gesture to support comprehension and memory after TBI and expands our understanding of naturalistic multimodal language processing in this population.
Article
Full-text available
Metaphors that describe an abstract concept in terms of a motion concept are widely used to enhance our understanding of abstract concepts. These metaphors are used not only in our daily language but also in learning mathematics. As an example, in the process of understanding the abstract representation of a mathematical concept, a graphical representation may play the role of a mediatory domain. This graphical representation could have a high degree of perceptual and action effector strength. This is particularly the case when a gestures (as a motion) is used to depict the graphical representation. After looking at this example, we discuss perceptual and action effector strength of the base domains of several mathematical metaphors that describe mathematical concepts in terms of spatial and motion concepts. Then, based on the data in the Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms, it is suggested that high degrees of perceptual and action effector strength of the base domains of these metaphors play an important role in the grounding of abstract mathematical concepts in the physical environment.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we draw on Charles Sanders Pierce's typology of semiotic signs and suggest that the three types of signs (icon, index, and symbol) enhance working memory and inhibition through three different mechanisms: icons support the process of embodiment; indexes strengthen focus of attention; symbols enhance the process of generalization. Mathematical icons enhance the process of embodying mathematical entities. They help the individual hold the information associated with a mathematical entity in the visual working memory and suppress information that is irrelevant to the task. Indexes strengthen focus of attention by directly referring to the place of mathematical entities and disregarding contextually-irrelevant stimuli. Symbols enhance the process of generalization by suppressing low-level information and reducing the load on working memory. Since cognitive flexibility as the third component of executive functions builds on working memory and inhibition, it can be hypothesized that mathematical signs contribute to this component as well.
Article
Full-text available
The role of gestural schematization in enhancing thinking processes has been the subject of a large body of works. In this process, contextually unimportant or irrelevant information related to a concept (or a system of concepts) is deleted or ignored, while relevant spatial information is maintained. This process is a special type of inhibition, which is one of the key components of executive functions. In this short paper, it is suggested that gestural schematization is a special type of symbolic schematization, a much more general process through which irrelevant information related to features of a concept (or a system of concepts) is suppressed, while relevant information (spatial and non-spatial) is maintained. Through symbolic schematization, abstract structural similarity between two concepts or between two systems of concepts can be discovered. In this way, an individual’s knowledge about the first situation can be generalized to the second situation. Symbolic schematization is the basis of abstraction, knowledge generalization, and knowledge development. This is particularly the case with abstract mathematical thinking. This proposal offers a picture of cognitive mechanisms through which knowledge of abstract mathematical concepts is created and developed in the mind.
Article
Full-text available
Representational transformation is a technique for deepening our understanding of mathematical concepts and facilitating the process of solving mathematical problems. This article suggests that representational transformation can contribute to thought processes in four ways. Firstly, representational transformation helps people employ a wider range of cognitive resources. When one representation of a concept or a problem is transformed into another representation, the sensory-motor resources that are involved in the processing of the base representation are employed to process the target representation. Secondly, representational transformation helps in the process of organizing scattered information into suitable units. This gives structure to various parts of information and makes them much easier to handle. Thirdly, representational transformation helps in the process of suppressing irrelevant information and unnecessary elements of problems. This is a key step in organizing and structuring information. Fourthly, representational transformation helps in the process of discovering general rules and turning them into explicit knowledge.
Article
Full-text available
The two first years of life are critical in the development of Executive Functions (EF). However, very little is known about their early manifestations, how they develop, how they relate to other psychological constructions or the status of other people’s influence in this early development. The study of EFs has been carried out through standardised tasks, but some authors question their ecological validity and suggest an approach involving everyday situations and the challenges that children set for themselves. In this article we first review these issues in relation to the first manifestations of EFs. We secondly present a longitudinal case study at nursery school of a child between the ages of 8 and 17 months, considering the challenges and the means he employed in order to resolve them. We found that, from 8 months of age, the child gave himself challenges in relation to the functional uses of objects and instruments. He regulated his own behavior both through object and instrument uses and private gestures. He also involved the teacher at 17 months. This finding suggests that (1) the material world is particularly important in these early manifestations of EF, (2) teachers’ interventions are essential. Implications of the findings for early years education are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Many Western industrialized nations have high levels of ethnic diversity but to date there are very few studies which investigate prelinguistic and early language development in infants from ethnic minority backgrounds. This study tracked the development of infant communicative gestures from 10 to 12 months (n = 59) in three culturally distinct groups in the United Kingdom and measured their relationship, along with maternal utterance frequency and responsiveness, to vocabulary development at 12 and 18 months. No significant differences were found in infant gesture development and maternal responsiveness across the groups, but relationships were identified between gesture, maternal responsiveness, and vocabulary development.
Article
Full-text available
Despite all methodological efforts made in the last three decades, Western instruction grounds on traditional principles. Most educational programs follow theories that are mentalistic, i.e., they separate the mind from the body. At school, learners sit, watch, listen, and write. The aim of this paper is to present embodied learning as an alternative to mentalistic education. Similarly, this paper wants to describe embodied learning from a neuroscientific perspective. After a brief historical overview, I will review studies highlighting the behavioral effectiveness of embodied instruction in second language learning, mathematics and spatial thinking. On this base, I will discuss some of the brain mechanisms driving embodied learning and describe its advantages, clearly pleading in favor of instructional practice that reunites body and mind.
Article
Full-text available
Studies on cardinality date back many years, and it remains a current research issue today. Indeed, despite the many findings on the topic, there is still controversy surrounding when and how children understand cardinality. The main studies on this understanding employs the “how many” question in situations involving the quantification of objects, analyzing the relationship between counting and cardinality. In the present study, we argue that it is essential to consider how cardinality is used in the context in which it arises, including the in- depth consideration of semiotic, social, and pragmatic dimensions, in order to fully comprehend the topic. We analyze in microgenesis the interaction between a two-year-old girl and her mother when they are playing a game requiring the understanding and use of cardinality through five sessions conducted over the course of a year. Our findings suggest that, in the proposed situation, cardinal understanding develops slowly and gradually requires an integrated body of resources (such as gestures and the use of objects). In highlighting the role of semiotics and social interactions in the development of cardinal understanding, this research underscores the fundamental role that early education should play in its development.
Article
Full-text available
During mathematics instruction, teachers often make links between different representations of mathematical information, and they sometimes use gestures to refer to the representations that they link. In this research, we investigated the role of such gestures in students’ learning from lessons about links between linear equations and corresponding graphs. Eighty-two middle-school students completed a pretest, viewed a video lesson, and then completed a posttest comparable to the pretest. In all of the video lessons, the teacher explained the links between equations and graphs in speech. The lessons varied in whether the teacher referred to the equations in gesture and in whether she referred to the graphs in gesture, yielding four conditions: neither equations nor graphs, equations only, graphs only, and both equations and graphs. In all conditions, the gestures were redundant with speech, in the sense that the referents of the gestures were also mentioned in speech (e.g., pointing to “2” while saying “2”). Students showed substantial learning in all conditions. However, students learned less when the teacher referred to the equations in gesture than when she did not. This was not the case for gesture to graphs. These findings are discussed in terms of the processing implications of redundancy between gesture and speech, and the possibility of “trade-offs” in attention to the visual representations. The findings underscore the need for a more nuanced view of the role of teachers’ gestures in students’ comprehension and learning. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s41235-017-0077-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
The motor system is one of cognitive resources that can be employed to acquire an embodied understanding of abstract mathematical concepts. This work discusses the ways through which fictive motions and accompanying gestures can help us to process mathematical concepts. It is suggested that the visual representation of any concept (the metaphorical description of a concept in terms of an image) can be understood at least partly through a fictive motion. When we create a mental image of something, we scan various parts of the image in our mind. This scanning is similar to the motion of a point on that image. The trace of the movement is similar to the fictive motion of an object along the contour of that image.
Article
We review major developmental evidence on the continuity from action to gesture to word and sign in human children, highlighting the important role of caregivers in the development of multimodal communication. In particular, the basic issues considered here and contributing to the current debate on the origins and development of the language-ready brain are: (1) links between early actions, gestures and words and similarities in representational strategies; (2) importance of multimodal communication and the interplay between gestures and spoken words; (3) interconnections between early actions, gestures and signs. The innovation of this report is in connecting these themes together to relevant findings from studies on children between 6 and 36 months of age and highlighting interesting parallels in studies on ape communicative behavior.