- Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
- Learn more
Download available
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports
On the spatial phase distribution
of cutaneous low‑frequency
perfusion oscillations
Stefan Borik1*, Simon Lyra2, Volker Perlitz3, Micha Keller4, Steen Leonhardt2 &
Vladimir Blazek2,5
Distributed cutaneous tissue blood volume oscillations contain information on autonomic nervous
system (ANS) regulation of cardiorespiratory activity as well as dominating thermoregulation. ANS
associated with low‑frequency oscillations can be quantied in terms of frequencies, amplitudes, and
phase shifts. The relative order between these faculties may be disturbed by conditions colloquially
termed ‘stress’. Photoplethysmography imaging, an optical non‑invasive diagnostic technique
provides information on cutaneous tissue perfusion in the temporal and spatial domains. Using the
cold pressure test (CPT) in thirteen healthy volunteers as a well‑studied experimental intervention,
we present a method for evaluating phase shifts in low‑ and intermediate frequency bands in forehead
cutaneous perfusion mapping. Phase shift changes were analysed in low‑ and intermediate frequency
ranges from 0.05 Hz to 0.18 Hz. We observed that time waveforms increasingly desynchronised
in various areas of the scanned area throughout measurements. An increase of IM band phase
desynchronization observed throughout measurements was comparable in experimental and control
group, suggesting a time eect possibly due to overshooting the optimal relaxation duration. CPT
triggered an increase in the number of points phase‑shifted to the reference that was specic to the
low frequency range for phase‑shift thresholds dened as π/4, 3π/8, and π/2 rad, respectively. Phase
shifts in forehead blood oscillations may infer changes of vascular tone due to activity of various
neural systems. We present an innovative method for the phase shift analysis of cutaneous tissue
perfusion that appears promising to assess ANS change processes related to physical or psychological
stress. More comprehensive studies are needed to further investigate the reliability and physiological
signicance of ndings.
e skin is involved in multiple functions pivotal for the entire organism. Skin perfusion is thus a rich and easily
available source of information on these functions, such as the activity of the autonomic nervous system (ANS),
cardiorespiratory exercise as well as dominating thermoregulation1,2. Understandably, scientic zeal was sparked
also at the prospect of obtaining a non-invasive measure of a clinically relevant system involved in serious mental
and physical disorders. In that vein, studies of skin perfusion have a longstanding history. First published in 1937,
photoplethysmography (PPG) has been demonstrated to be an undemanding and non-invasive technology for
the study of the perfusion of supercial skin layers3. is approach has been advanced in current technologies
to non-contact instruments used in cardiovascular diagnostics4.
For long skin perfusion studies focused mostly on understanding pulsatile PPG qualities, whereas the infor-
mation on ANS processes in PPG signals remained insuciently understood. Earliest references to proprietary
oscillations in PPG signals as ‘psychomotor waves’5 had never been conrmed by objective correlates. e system-
atic analysis of frequencies and amplitudes inherent to PPG signals (or laser Doppler uxmetry, LDF) has only
as of late demonstrated that a mere 2.5% fraction of the total signal power originated in cardiovascular activity6.
is is of interest to medical elds specializing on the non-invasive and non-contact cardiovascular diagnostics4.
Scientic study of ANS inuence on cardiovascular-respiratory processes focused chiey on a low and a
high frequency band, LF and HF, resp. ose variance categories were, however, matter of scientic controversy
OPEN
1Department of Electromagnetic and Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Information
Technology, University of Zilina, Zilina, Slovakia. 2Medical Information Technology (MedIT), Helmholtz-Institute
for Biomedical Engineering, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany. 3Simplana GmbH, Aachen,
Germany. 4Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Medical School, RWTH Aachen
University, Aachen, Germany. 5The Czech Institute of Informatics, Robotics and Cybernetics (CIIRC), Czech
Technical University in Prague, Prague, Czech Republic. *email: stefan.borik@feit.uniza.sk
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
with respect to the boundaries and physiological representations of the LF-band since it is considered to contain
sympathetic as well as parasympathetic nervous trac. While the HF-band is commonly agreed to represent
vagal (parasympathetic) activity found in respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), a state of weak coupling between
cardiac and respiratory activity7, the validity of the entire approach is questionable if one of the frequency bands
evades evidence8–10. is, unfortunately, impeded its clinical use to this day.
An approach which appears to overcome the dilemma on ANS-related frequency bands was introduced
with ndings on a 0.15Hz rhythm or intermediate (IM) band. Wedged between the LF and the HF frequency
band at 0.12Hz to 0.18Hz, this IM-band bridges the frequency boundaries of upper LF and lower HF band. A
rst description of the IM-band was given for skin LDF data by Smits etal. during hyperventilation11. Further
studies showed the IM-band strongly associated with profound relaxation in skin of earlobe, forehead, and
heart12. Controlled relaxation using self-control techniques such as the autogenic training (AT) exhibited dur-
ing extended IM-band prevalence cardiorespiratory phase synchronizations (CRPS) at integer number ratios,
so-called n:m—synchronizations13. In particular, these ndings suggest a series of lower and upper harmonics of
the primary IM-band frequencies covering essential LF- and HF-frequencies. is suspends the role of distinct
frequency bands as it emphasizes instead the relevance of ratios of interacting frequencies. Comparison of human
multiple physiological time series with canine experiments showed widely identical frequency characteristics in
spite of scaling dierences which suggested identical origin of the IM-band in unspecic reticular neurons in the
common brainstem system13. is is of note since Traube speculated already in 1865 on phasical ‘irradiations’
of medullary respiratory centers on brainstem nuclei controlling heart rate14.
Low-frequency oscillations exhibit spatially variable amplitudes, which depend on the site of the PPG probe.
erefore, the photoplethysmography imaging (PPGI) method, rst described in15, is ideal for the spatial analysis
of skin perfusion dynamics, which explains increased interest of this method16. PPGI enables spatial–temporal
analysis of the distribution of perfusion oscillations using a camera system17 combined with an external light
source18, or use of ambient illumination only19.
According to20, the PPGI method can be used to dierentiate low-frequency oscillations in healthy and
damaged tissues. is research group demonstrated also that slow oscillations were not phase synchronized.
In spite of open questions concerning frequencies and amplitudes in PPG-signals, the phase-shi is an
essential yet still “under-explored” parameter, which therefore mandates systematic study19. is approach was
employed by21, using a technique published rst by22 to monitor the relative phase of perfusion changes associ-
ated with the cardiac activity in migraine patients.
is research group23 validated their ndings with a novel model of light-tissue interaction when using a
reective and non-contact PPG, i.e. PPGI technique. e authors reported that the transmural pressure of the
larger arteries apparently aected locations in cutaneous areas where HR-related blood volume changes oscillate
at opposite phase to the reference signal acquired from another location on the skin. us, depending on the
wavelength used HR-related changes in the PPG signal detected with PPGI may be induced indirectly by oscil-
lations of larger arteries located even in deeper subcutaneous tissue structures24. In contrast25, another view on
light-tissue interaction for monitoring the entire experiment is given by videocapillaroscopy.
e current communication describes a novel method for mapping the phase shi of perfusion uctuations
in cutaneous tissues. In contrast to mapping cardiac activity, we focused on -frequency oscillations in the range
between 0.05Hz and 0.18Hz. We divided this frequency band into low-frequency (LF) sections and intermedi-
ate (IM) sections with boundaries according to Pfurtscheller (_Pf)26 and Keller (_Ke)27. us, we studied four
bands: LF_Pf (0.05Hz–0.10Hz), IM_Pf (0.10Hz–0.15Hz), and LF_Ke (0.05Hz–0.12Hz), and IM_Ke
(0.12Hz–0.18Hz). To stimulate ANS responses, we used the well-studied cold pressure test in young healthy
individuals to induce changes in the spatial distribution of the phase shi relative to the frontal region’s central
reference. We chose the correlation between phase synchronization of slow perfusion rhythms and possible
changes in ANS regulation caused by stress as an outcome parameter. To the best of our knowledge, this approach
has never been probed previously.
Materials and methods
Experimental protocol. A total of 16 subjects aged (27.0 ± 2.2) years participated in the study. e right- or
le-handed individuals were non-smokers and had no acute physical or mental illness, as was tested using the
German version of the patient health questionnaire (PHQ-D28;). To avoid falsifying measurement outcomes,
subjects were asked to refrain from consuming anything containing caeine on the day of the experiment. We
used a between-subject blocked protocol with seven stages in a supine position to allow internal and external
control of ANS stimulation. In 13 subjects, the following sequence was applied: (1) Eyes Open (EO), (2) Eyes
closed (EC), (3) EC + cold pressor/ambient water test (CPT, AWT), (4) EC, (5) EC + cold pressor/ambient water
test, (6) EC, (7) EO. Stages 1 and 7 lasted 180s, and stages 2, 4, and 6 lasted 300s. Stages 3 + 5 lasted 60s and
were applied in randomized order. Stage 3 or 5, CPT or AWT, was performed by immersion of the subjects’
non-dominant hand (up to the carpus) in water of either 4°C or water of ambient temperature (AWT) of 20°C
for 60s. In another group of 3 subjects, conditions of stage 2 (EC) were applied also in stages 3 and 5 to serve as
a time control group. Breathing was not controlled to avoid any unintended stress by external control. Further-
more, the head was not xed to minimize motion artifacts since this could also induce stress. e hand was dried
aerwards using a towel placed on the patient’s abdomen. Stage 6 (EC) and stage 7 (EO) nalized the recording.
An overview of all stages is illustrated in Fig.1A,B. e whole experiment lasted 23min in total and was well
tolerated by all participants. e study protocol was approved by the ethical committee of the University Hos-
pital at RWTH Aachen University (Ref. No. EK 219-21), informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior
to the study, and all methods were performed in accordance with the study protocol and with the Declaration
of Helsinki.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Measurement setup. e measurement setup used a high-performance complementary metal-oxide-
semiconductor (CMOS) monochrome camera of type Grasshopper 3 GS3-U3-23S6 M-C (FLIR Systems, USA),
with a xed focal length lens of type Fujinon CF12.5HA-1 (Fujilm Holdings, Japan) mounted to the camera.
e recordings were performed with a spatial resolution of 1920 × 1200 pixels (12 bit per pixel) at 30 fps. Since
ambient illumination was necessary for the measurements, four organic LED (OLED) panels of type Keuka
warmwhite 24V (OLEDWorks, Germany) were mounted next to the camera. e advantages of using OLEDs
instead of LEDs for camera-based vital sign extraction were described in29. A similar setup using one OLED to
record simulated PPGI signals from an optoelectronic skin perfusion phantom was described earlier17.
In this study, a comfortable patient bench was placed beneath the camera setup to locate the participants in a
supine position. e angle of the patient’s head to the camera was adjusted using a pillow. is ensured optimal
conditions for continuous measurement of facial skin perfusion. Two bowls, one with iced water (approx. 4°C),
another one with water at ambient temperature (approx. 20°C) were placed within the reach of the subject to
allow low-movement immersion of the hand. e measurement setup is shown in Fig.2a. Figure2b illustrates
the view of the camera and indicates the object detector and the point tracker used for automated region extrac-
tion explained below.
Data processing. As therst step, raw data were prepared to evaluate the spatial distribution of the PPGI
signal phase by determining the desired area of interest; here, we chose the subjects’ forehead. e following
precautions had to be taken prior to initiating an extended recording (23min), as was done in this case. First,
the subject’s lateral head movements have to be controlled since they can cause signal artifacts that are amplied
given the small kernel size used to extract temporal PPGI signals from the monitored image matrix. We decided
to track the selected area of interest, which has been previously used30–33. Second, since this tracking algorithm
can result in image jittering34, we used double tracking by combining the Viola-Jones algorithm35 with the point
detector method36–38. Here, the face detector automatically detected and extracted the face area as a bounding
box, which was then magnied by 10% to cover the entire face. ird, the image was cropped and inserted into
Figure1. (A) Stages of the experimental protocol in the experimental group (N = 13); with stages 3 and 5 being
applied in reversed order in 3 recordings to randomize intervention. (B) Stages of the control group (N = 3) with
all stages using identical conditions.
Figure2. (a) Measurement setup. (b) Example of the eld of view with an applied face detector and forehead
tracking.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
the auxiliary image layer (lled with zeros), ensuring the exact image size for subsequent processing and tracking
using the KLT point tracker. At this stage, a rectangle containing a region of interest (ROI)on the subject’s fore-
head was manually selected. Fourth, the KLTpoint tracker was initialised to track and extract the selected ROI
using minimal eigenvalue features36. In the case of angling the subject’s face o the horizontal plane, detected
ROIs were automatically reprojected into the image’s original horizontal plane perpendicular to its symmetry
axis. According to its upper edge, the process began by calculating the bounding box’s angle of inclination. e
image was then rotated back andinserted into another unied image layer and saved in *.pgm format. Prior to
data analysis, pre-processing of image data was nalized by resizing all images to the selected reference, the rst
image in the sequence of measured data. is compromise was due to the limitations of tracking and the fact
that various sized ROIs were detected as subjects moved. To minimize mixing of individual pixels, the nearest
extrapolation method was chosen. e modied image was then saved to the hard disk. Figure3 illustrates the
process of face and forehead ROI tracking.
Following pre-processing signals from suciently stable ROIs, the time courses of the PPGI signals were
extracted by analysing each recorded frame. Time waveform extraction was initiated by selecting a 10 × 10 px2
kernel moving without overlap in the selected forehead region (Fig.4). e spatial mean was then computed
for each kernel position of the processedframe. us, a three-dimensional matrix of time-domain signals was
obtained. Two dimensions encoded the extracted signal’s spatial location. e third dimension was a time vec-
tor at a length equal to the number of frames recorded (41,400 samples = 23min). Figure5 depicts a wavelet
scalogram of the unltered signal from a 10 × 10 px2 region close to the subject’s forehead showing perfusion
rhythms in the frequency domain. is image still contained primarily artifacts due to facial expressions and
changes in the geometry of the tracking area, which persisted despite image tracking (Fig.6). Suppression of
these artifacts was achieved using the stationary wavelet transform (SWT)39–41. Prior to SWT denoising, the
signal was downsampled to the sampling frequency of 5Hz.
is decomposed the signal using Daubechies – db5 wavelets—to the eight levels processing each detail
level separately using a moving window to compute the median and standard deviation. is helped to obtain
the envelope of a particular wavelet decompositiondetail. Subsequently, all values below this envelope were
adaptively ltered separately using so thresholding for each decomposition level. Finally, the signal was recon-
structed using inverse SWT. e purpose of this entire process was not to eliminate noise but to determine it.
e estimated noise was then subtracted from the original signal, eliminating other undesired artifacts, and
preparing the signal for further processing and analysis.
After removing these artifacts, defined frequency bands were extracted using finite response (FIR)
band-pass lters using the following parameter settings: low frequency (LF)_Pf band (cut-o frequencies
0.05Hz–0.10Hz); intermediate (IM)_Pf band (cut-o frequencies 0.10Hz–0.15Hz); LF_Ke band (cut-o
frequencies 0.05Hz–0.12Hz); IM_Pf band (cut-o frequencies 0.12Hz–0.18Hz).
is signal processing focused on observation of ANS regulation in those frequency ranges, which have
been recently demonstrated to be distinct processes27. ese frequency bands may contain respiration-induced
Figure3. Face and forehead ROI tracking.
Figure4. Process of obtaining PPG signals in time domain.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
perfusion changes42. However, there is evidence that while IM band activity and respiration are closely related
processes, they are yet distinct10,43. is is supported by other authors reporting LF changes associated with
waves formerly referred to as Mayer-Traube-Hering oscillations being only slightly aected by respiration44,45.
e eects of ltering using stationary wavelet transform denoising (SWTD) can be seen in Fig.7. e blue
curve represents the time-domain signal following tracking, while the red curve corresponds to the signal fol-
lowing denoising using the SWTD algorithm. As illustrated in this gure, ltering helped suppress artifacts
caused by the subject’s facial expressions, especially the forehead wrinkling visible in Fig.6. Furthermore, this
gure shows PPG signals from which ROIs were extracted. Along with suppressedartifacts, it is possible to see
a smoothed PPG curve and the oscillations corresponding to slow changes in cutaneous tissue perfusion. Aer
removing noise and artifacts, the signal reconstruction process included only wavelet levels whose frequency
corresponded with LF oscillations as dened above. Simultaneously, a frequency band for the heart rhythm up
to approx. 5Hz was included to observe possible higher harmonic components corresponding to the dicrotic
notch in the PPG signal. Figure7 shows a signal ltered this way, which exhibits artifact suppression even in the
frequency domain (Fig.8).
Further ltering of spectral components helped avoid inaccuracies in determining the phases of LF oscilla-
tions, which might possibly be aected by other physiological oscillators, such as cardiac activity or respiration.
e Hilbert transform was chosen to provide information on the instantaneous phase of the signal as this is a
computationally undemanding yet robust and well studied approach. e phase is determined separately for each
time waveform of the PPGI matrix. Applying the Hilbert transform yielded an analytical signal:
where xr represents the original signal and xi is its imaginary part shied by 90°.
e information on the instantaneous phase is:
(1)
x(t)=xr+jxi,
Figure5. Continuous wavelet scalogram aer tracking.
Figure6. Mimics and light change comparison (at the start—frame 1, in the middle of cold pressure test—
frame 25,989). Blue colour depicts motion estimated by optical ow.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
where ϕ(t) has the form of a sawtooth signal carrying information on changes of the phase of the signal within a
given cycle. is allowed calculation of the instantaneous phase of each time course of the PPGI matrix, creating
a three-dimensional instantaneous phase matrix carrying spatial-temporal information on phase distribution in
the forehead region and changes over time.
Selection of a suitable reference signal was essential to create a planar map of phase changes. Here, the axis
of symmetry was determined using the central vertical line of the forehead region. is reference signal was
always central element for each row of the phasematrix, which consisted here of a uniform size of 12 rows and
35 columns (Fig.9).
e algorithm used to create this map started by computing the phase dierence between the reference point
and all other points (column positions) in any given row. is allowed to estimate the relative phase shi across
all phase matrix points. us, a matrix of dimensions 12 × 35 was obtained, with the points on the symmetry
axis naturally equal to zero radians.
e phase shi between the reference and the corresponding matrix point was calculated as the absolute value
of their instantaneous phase dierences within a specied time window.
We chose a 10∙Fs window size to avoid sudden and undesirable phase changes caused, for example, by insuf-
cient artifact suppression. is window of 3000 samples (100s) moved acrosss the entire signal, calculating
the median
φmed,
r
,
c
(t)
of the dierence between the absolute phase value of the reference signal
φref ,
r
(t)
and the
signal of the corresponding point
φ
r
,
c
(t)
of the phase shi matrix:
(2)
φ(
t)=atan
x
i
x
r
,
(3)
φmed,
r
,
c(t)=|β|=
φ
ref,
r(t)−φr,c(t)
,
Figure7. Signal before and aer stationary wavelet transform denoising.
Figure8. Filtered scalogram showing heart rate (HR) and low frequency (LF) bands and experimental stages.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
where r andc represent rows and columns of the phase matrix, respectively.
An example of this process is shown in Fig.10, depicting changes in the phase synchronization between the
reference point and a selected point of the phase matrix corresponding to the forehead region of the subject
under investigation.
To create a temporal sequence of phase maps and visualize the actual phase shi changes and their spatial
distribution, a moving window of 60s was used to calculate the average phase shi value
β
for each point of
the phase matrix. Since averaging failed to correct averaging of circular quantities, a method incorporating its
vector properties was used instead:
where
βn
is the relative phase of selected points of the phase matrix, n corresponds to the frame number and W
corresponds to the moving averaging window length.
is logic was followed when selecting phase thresholds to analyse the synchronisation of spatial changes
in perfusion. First, we wanted to use intervals that would not overlap, as is usually the case, for example, when
selecting frequency bands in amplitude analysis of signals. However, instead of this approach, we used phase
thresholds and thus intervals with overlaps, i.e., from the le, each interval is bounded by a π value. is choice
is supported by the assumption that the relative temporal position between the reference and the observed signal
is tracked at a selected point in a matrix containing information about the spatial distribution of perfusion in
the low-frequency domain when working with phase shis. Furthermore, we assume that the perfusion signals
(due to their nature) are not perfectly aligned, and even in the case of resting state, some initial phase shi is
present between them (here considered as background). By gradually narrowing the interval towards the π value,
the responses to the intervention (CPT or AWT) become apparent and are dierentiable from the background.
us, to map spatiotemporal variation of phase, the values with an average phase shi less than π/4 were
marked blue, which corresponded to zero, and phase shi values in the range of π/4 to πwere colour coded
from green to red, indicating counterphase to the reference. is approach was also used for colour coding of
the 3π/8-π and π/2-π intervals. e spatiotemporal variation in reciprocal phase shis is illustrated in Fig.11,
which depicts minute-by-minute changes. e y-axis included information on the measured subjects. Another
technique of representing the relative phase shis was to encode them following their time course. is was
(4)
β
=atan2
W
n=1
sinβn,
W
n=1
cosβn
, or in complex numbers β=arg
W
n=1
ejβn
,
Figure9. Example of map creation. Reference points in the matrix are placed in the middle of each row.
Figure10. Detection of desynchronization based on the Hilbert transform. Blue and red signals represent ROIs
from dierent parts of the forehead. ere are low-frequency oscillations obtained from the signal by SWTD,
similar to initial denoising. e dashed black line is the instantaneous phase calculated in the moving window
with a length of 10s. e area marked yellow corresponds to the desynchronization threshold used for further
analysis.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
achieved by counting the number of phase matrix points with a relative phase shi to the reference exceeding
a predened threshold in a given row. Here, three dierent threshold values (thrs) π/4, 3π/8 and π/2rad were
dened. Subsequently, the ratio of phase matrix points exceeding the pre-set threshold to the total number of
phase matrix points was calculated for each frame and, thus, for the associated phase matrix. is process is
mathematically described by equation:
where N = r × c is the total number of phase matrix points and
Nφ>thrs
is the number of points exceeding the
predened threshold.
To quantify relative phase changes within each group and phase shi threshold value (π/4, 3π/8 and π/2rad),
separate 2 (LF_Pf, IM_Pf and LF_Ke, IM_Ke) × 7 (Repetition) repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
of relative phase desynchronization were computed. e main experimental and random group were analysed
together aer adapting the order of AWT and CPT as well as subsequent EC stages of the random group to match
(5)
R
φ>thrs(t)=
Nφ>thrs
N,thrs =
π
4
,3π
8
,π
2,
Figure11. Spatial phase shi distribution matrices indexing desynchronization for low and intermediate
frequency (LF, and IM, resp.) bands according to boundaries used by Pfurtscheller (Pf, le column) or Keller
(Ke, right column) for individual participants (horizontal lines of squares) of the main group (n = 10). EO = eyes
open, EC = eyes closed; orange bar at top: cold water test, blue bar at top: ambient water test. From top to
bottom, colour coded points display thresholds for relative phase shi intervals from π/4rad to π rad, 3π/8rad
to π rad, and π/2rad to π rad: dark blue < blue < green < yellow. Red indexing counterphase with the reference.
Plots show only minute dierences between Pfurtscheller and Keller frequency denitions.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
9
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
the order of the main group. Despite lack of power due to a small sample size, the control group was also analysed
using a repeated measures ANOVA. A p-value of < 0.05 was considered signicant. Post-hoc paired t-tests were
computed for (1) the contrast stage 1 versus 7 to test whether our experimental conditions were followed by an
increase in phase desynchronization, and (2) contrasting AWT and CPT (stage 3 vs 5) to test dierences in phase
desynchronization. ese post-hoc tests were conducted separately for phase shis, groups as well as frequency
band denitions. Bonferroni corrections were applied for each frequency band denition pair (LF_Pf/IM_Pf
and LF_Ke/IM_Ke) and separately for each phase shi, resulting in an α = 0.05/4 = 0.0125.
Results
e matrices of spatial phase shi distribution indexing desynchronization for low and intermediate frequency
are shown for the experimental (main) group in Fig.11 and for control and random groups in Fig.12, resp.
(results based on Eqs. (3) and (4)). ese plots exhibit various patterns for phase shi threshold ranges, frequency
bands, and experimental time. Pattern dierences for Pf and Ke boundary denitions appear rather insignicant.
Furthermore, phase shi distributions were plotted for π/4, 3π/8 and π/2 thresholds, and they were more promi-
nent in either IM band as in LF bands. us, desynchronization was strongest in π/4 in the IM band. Conversely,
there was almost complete synchronization with reference for π/2 in the LF band.
To quantify individual ndings, using Eq.(5) we computed the median (red graphs) for phase shi changes
(desynchronization compared to reference) for π/4, 3π/8, and π/2rad thresholds in the main and the random
group combined, and for the time control group. is was computed separately for LF and IM frequency bands
at varying boundary denitions according to Pfurtscheller (Pf) and Keller (Ke). e increase in phase desyn-
chronization can be interpreted as an increase in the number of matrix points above the predened threshold,
with a value of 1 equal to 100% phase shied points compared to reference.
Figures13 and 14 shows the time courses of phase desynchronizations for the main and control groups for
frequency boundaries dened as IM_Pf band (0.10Hz–0.15Hz) and IM_Ke (0.12Hz–0.18Hz), respectively.
Two patterns are obvious in both groups and both frequency denitions. Firstly, the levels of phase desynchroni-
zations decreased for thresholds with π/4 > 3π/8 > π/2rad. In the IM_Pf band the smallest phase window (π/2rad)
underlines the upward trend throughout measurements pointed out by a more specic response. Secondly,
phase desynchronizations increased over time for all three thresholds and both groups. However, there were
fewer prominent oscillations in the main group during the entire experimental time course than in control. In
the main group, phase desynchronization due to CPT were of magnitudes comparable with non-experimentally
induced uctuations in all thresholds. In controls, there were prominent spontaneous oscillations. Opening the
eyes (stage 7) was followed by a transient increase in phase desynchronization in all thresholds. Here, the increase
in phase desynchronization appeared to be strongest as of the 17 min. is may have been due to the length of
the relaxation causing some discomfort for participants.
Figure15 shows the time courses of the phase desynchronizations for the main and the control groups for
frequency boundaries dened as LF_Pf frequency band (0.05Hz–0.10Hz). In the main group, there were still
few pronounced uctuations. Here, too, the level of phase desynchronization decreased with each threshold.
In all thresholds, a comparable course was found in CPT: stable conditions during CPT, then rapid increases
in phase desynchronization, then continuous decreases in phase desynchronization for 3min. Occasionally in
all 3 thresholds dierently pronounced oscillations of shorter frequency and amplitude appeared. Opening the
eyes was accompanied by hardly any changes in phase desynchronization. e control group showed very stable
conditions. ere were distinct oscillations about every 7min, most pronounced at π/4 > 3π/8 > π/2rad. ere
was no evidence of increases in phase desynchronization with time.
Figure16 shows the time courses of the phase desynchronizations for the main and the control groups for
frequency boundaries dened as LF_Ke frequency band (0.05Hz–0.12Hz). In the main group, largely identi-
cal conditions are found as in the LF_PF frequency band. e response to CPT appears overall at a comparable
course as in LF-Pf. During CPT there is a stable course of phase desynchronization in all thresholds, followed
by rapid increases, and slow decays for approx. 3min. Here, however, oscillations of shorter frequency and
amplitude are missing. In the control group, spontaneous oscillations of comparable period duration are found,
but not only of stronger amplitude, but also of somewhat dierent dynamics. Here, steep rising edges are found,
followed by slow continuous decay of this activity. is is found roughly three times: in the eighth minute, the
12th minute, and in the 17th minute.
Statistical analyses. For each phase threshold and group (main + random group, control group), 2 (LF and
IM band for each frequency denition) × 7 (Repetition) repeated measures ANOVAs were computed. Mauchly’s
test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated in the experimental group for
Rφ>π/2
in
Pfurtscheller (χ2(20) = 34.3, p = 0.03) and Keller frequency band denitions (χ2(20) = 32.7, p = 0.04) as well as for
Keller denition of
Rφ>π/4
(χ2(20) = 32.5, p = 0.047). erefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Green-
house–Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.48, ε = 0.43 and ε = 0.41). In the experimental group, the repeated
measures ANOVAs for
Rφ>π/4
,
Rφ>3π/8
, and
Rφ>π/2
, respectively, revealed signicant main eects for the fac-
tor repetition (all p < 0.05; Table1). is indicates that phase desynchronization values of all relative phase shis
showed a signicant increase throughout measurements. e main eects of the factor band as well as the inter-
action band × repetition did not yield signicant results for any frequency band denition and phase shi (all
p > 0.05; see Table1).
To further examine the signicant main eect of factor repetition in the experimental group, paired t-tests
were computed for each frequency band denition and phase threshold. e LF_Ke band showed for
Rφ>π/4
,
Rφ>3π/8
, and
Rφ>π/2
a signicant increase from stage 1 to 7 aer Bonferroni correction (p < 0.0125). e LF_
Pf band showed a signicant increase from stage 1 to 7 for
Rφ>π/4
and
Rφ>3π/8
at p < 0.05 and
Rφ>π/2
aer
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
10
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Bonferroni correction (p < 0.0125). Furthermore, the increase from stage 1 to 7 was signicant at p < 0.05 for
Rφ>3π/8
, and
Rφ>π/2
. Further paired t-tests showed a signicantly higher phase desynchronization during CPT
compared to AWT at p < 0.05 for LF_Ke at
φ>3π/8
and
φ > π/2
threshold as well as for LF_Pf at
φ > π/2
.
is indicates that CPT aected phase desynchronization particularly in the low frequency range.
Further 2 (LF and IM band for each frequency denition) × 7 (Repetition) repeated measures ANOVAs were
computed for the control group. Neither the main eects of factor repetition and band nor the interaction of
repetition × band showed a signicant eect for any threshold (all p > 0.05; see Table2). However, descriptively,
the IM band denitions displayed an increase of phase desynchronization across stages pointing at a possible
time eect (see Fig.17).
Figure12. Spatial phase shi distribution matrices indexing desynchronization for low and intermediate
frequency (LF, and IM, resp.) bands according to boundaries used by Pfurtscheller (Pf, le column) or Keller
(Ke, right column) for individual participants (horizontal lines of squares) control and random groups.
EO = eyes open, EC = eyes closed; orange bar at top: cold water test, blue bar at top: ambient water test. From
top to bottom, colour coded points display thresholds for relative phase shi intervals from π/4rad to π rad,
3π/8rad to π rad, and π/2rad to π rad: dark blue < blue < green < yellow. Red indexing counterphase with the
reference.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
11
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure13. Phase desynchronization for dierent thresholds for main and control groups in the IM_Pf band
(0.1Hz–0.15Hz) including 0.1Hz activity. ere were only few oscillations in the main group. e increases in
phase desynchronization due to CPT were of magnitudes comparable to other non-experimental oscillations. In
controls, few prominent spontaneous oscillations. In both groups, phase desynchronization increased over time.
Levels of phase desynchronization were π/4 > 3π/8 > π/2rad in both groups. Red: median curve, black vertical
lines: stages of the experiment, grey area bounds: 25th and 75th percentiles; blue dashed lines: min and max
values.
Figure14. Phase desynchronization for dierent thresholds for main and control groups in the IM_Ke band
(0.12Hz–0.18Hz) excluding 0.1Hz activity. In the main group, there were hardly any obvious responses to
CPT. is group showed also only a slight increase of phase desynchronizations over time. Of note, the control
group exhibits for all phase thresholds distinct peaks in phase desynchronizations of approx. 100s length at
minute 4 and 11. Phase desynchronizations increased distinctly at the end of the recordings in 3π/8 and π/2
thresholds. Red: median curve, black vertical lines: stages of the experiment, grey area bounds: 25th and 75th
percentiles; blue dashed lines: min and max values.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
12
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure15. Phase desynchronization for dierent thresholds for main and control groups in the LF_Pf band
(0.05Hz–0.1Hz) possibly curtailing 0.1Hz activity. In the main group, few pronounced oscillations; the level
of phase desynchronization decreased with each threshold. Comparable course of phase desynchronizations
in all thresholds for CPT: stable course during CPT, rapid increases, slow decreases for about 3min. Few
dierently pronounced oscillations of shorter frequency and amplitude in all 3 thresholds. EO (stage 7)
accompanied by hardly any changes in phase desynchronization. Stable conditions in the control group:
distinct oscillations approx. every 7min, most pronounced at π/4 > 3π/8 > π/2rad. No evidence of increases in
phase desynchronization with time. Red: median curve, black vertical lines: stages of the experiment, grey area
bounds: 25th and 75th percentiles; blue dashed lines: min and max values.
Figure16. Phase desynchronization for dierent thresholds for all groups in the LF_Ke band
(0.05Hz–0.12Hz) completely including 0.1Hz activity. Widely identical conditions in the main group as in the
LF_PF frequency band. Phase desynchronizations in all thresholds for CPT as in LF_Pf frequency band. Stable
course during CPT, rapid increases, slow decreases for about 3min. Here no oscillations of higher frequency
and amplitude. Increases of phase desynchronizations with time in all thresholds. In controls oscillations of
comparable period duration, yet of dierent dynamics: steep rising edges followed by slow continuous activity
(8min, 12min, 17 min). No increases with time in controls. Red: median curve, black vertical lines: stages of the
experiment, grey area bounds: 25th and 75th percentiles; blue dashed lines: min and max values.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Bar graph analysis underpins these ndings. In both LF bands there is a general trend of increasing phase
desynchronization over time from stage 1 to 7 in
Rφ>π/4
. For
Rφ>3π/8
and for
Rφ>π/2
this is shown also in
IM_Ke. ere is a signicant dierence between AWT and CPT for LF_PF and LF_Ke (Fig.18).
Discussion and conclusion
In this study we present an innovative method for the phase shi analysis of cutaneous tissue perfusion that
appears promising to assess ANS change processes related to physical or psychological stress. Non-contact
mapping of forehead skin perfusion was used to detect spatial phase changes in photoplethysmography imaging
(PPGI) data. irteen healthy participants were exposed to an ambient/cold pressure water test (AWT, CPT)
in randomized order with intermittent resting blocks and compared to three time-control participants who
underwent no intervention. Relative phase desynchronization was investigated using three dierent thresholds
Table 1. Repeated measures ANOVAs in experimental groups (N = 13) for dierent thresholds and frequency
band denitions.
reshold Parameter Source F P
φ > π/4
LF_Pf
IM_Pf
Repetition 3.57 0.004
Band 0.003 0.96
Repetition × Band 1.46 0.20
LF_Ke
IM_Ke
Repetition 3.66 0.03
Band 1.26 0.28
Repetition × Band 0.91 0.50
φ>3π/8
LF_Pf
IM_Pf
Repetition 4.25 0.001
Band 0.04 0.85
Repetition × Band 1.39 0.23
LF_Ke
IM_Ke
Repetition 3.94 0.002
Band 0.83 0.38
Repetition × Band 0.64 0.70
φ > π/2
LF_Pf
IM_Pf
Repetition 4.203 0.013
Band 0.025 0.878
Repetition × Band 0.81 0.57
LF_Ke
IM_Ke
Repetition 3.93 0.022
Band 0.39 0.54
Repetition × Band 0.61 0.73
Table 2. Repeated measures ANOVAs in control group (N = 3) for dierent thresholds and frequency band
denitions.
reshold Parameter Source F p
ϕ > π⁄4
LF_Pf
IM_Pf
Repetition 0.62 0.71
Band 1.38 0.36
Repetition × Band 1.04 0.45
LF_Ke
IM_Ke
Repetition 0.51 0.79
Band 0.05 0.85
Repetition × Band 2.78 0.06
ϕ > 3π⁄8
LF_Pf
IM_Pf
Repetition 0.43 0.85
Band 1.66 0.33
Repetition × Band 2.01 0.14
LF_Ke
IM_Ke
Repetition 1.09 0.42
Band 2.02 0.29
Repetition × Band 2.5 0.08
ϕ > π⁄2
LF_Pf
IM_Pf
Repetition 0.52 0.79
Band 0.61 0.52
Repetition × Band 2.59 0.08
LF_Ke
IM_Ke
Repetition 1.76 0.32
Band 1.76 0.19
Repetition × Band 1.00 0.46
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
14
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
(
φ > π/4
,
φ>3π/8
, and
φ > π/2
). PPGI data was initially ltered using pooled low frequency (LF) and inter-
mediate (IM) frequencies (data not shown). is, however, did not supply sucient sensitivity for the detection
of experimental changes. Following recent evidence showing that LF band activity is better explained by using
two subcomponents, data was ltered based on frequency boundaries dened by Pfurtscheller46 and Keller27.
Pfurtscheller etal. suggested to separate LF bands as LFa: 0.05Hz–0.1Hz (referred to by us as LF_Pf), and LFb:
0.1Hz–0.15Hz, whereas Keller etal., used LF_Ke (0.05Hz–0.12Hz) as well as and IM_Ke (0.12Hz–0.18Hz)
bands. us, Keller LF boundaries included all 0.1Hz activity reported to exhibit individual variation between
0.075Hz and 0.12 Hz26, which therefore LF_Pf might fall short to reect. In the same vein, IM_Pf may confound
IM activity by including 0.1Hz activity, which is excluded when starting IM boundaries at 0.12Hz. Overall, we
observed an increase of phase desynchronization throughout the measurement for both LF and IM frequency
band denitions in the experimental group and for all phase shi thresholds. A non-signicant increase in IM
bands of the control group suggests this increase be related to time eects in this frequency range.
CPT compared to AWT signicantly enhanced phase desynchronization in both LF bands, though to a larger
extent for LF_Ke. Spontaneous increases at seemingly regular intervals unrelated to intervention observed in the
control group may indeed indicate that phase desynchronizations were specic for the LF_Pf and LF_Ke bands
during CPT. is nding is also of relevance since it supports the notion that the LF_Ke band includes 0.1Hz
activity to a larger extent. Yet, there is ample evidence that LF frequencies reect activity of dierent physiological
processes. At profound rest, it was shown to contain subharmonics of primary IM band activity13. It has been
much disputed that conventional LF represents also sympathetic activity47. erefore, measures such as the ratio
between LF and HF supposedly indexing autonomic balance have been challenged long8,48 and investigating
subcomponents of the LF band seems crucial for understanding autonomic dynamics.
Malpas already pointed out that “one must use care in relating changes in the strength of an oscillation in
blood pressure and heart rate as denitively due to a change in autonomic control”. But even for ANS con-
trol, there are still not all questions suciently resolved. Slow oscillations at 0.1Hz may represent primarily
Figure17. Bar graphs of phase desynchronization for LF and IM frequency band denitions based on
Pfurtscheller (Pf) and Keller (Ke), and dierent thresholds in the main group (N = 13). (A) Ratio
Rφ>π/4
shows
a general increasing trend of phase desynchronization and a signicant increase from stage 1 to 7 for LF_Pf and
LF_Ke. (B) Ratio
Rφ>
3
π/
8 showing general increasing trend throughout measurement and signicant increase
from stage 1 to 7 for IM_Ke, LF_Pf and LF_Ke exhibit signicantly higher phase desynchronization during CPT.
(C) Ratio
Rφ>π/2
showing general increasing trend and signicant increase from stage 1 to 7 for IM_Ke, LF_Pf
and LF_Ke. ere is a signicant dierence between AWT and CPT for LF_PF and LF_Ke. Asterisks indicate (*)
p-value < .05 or (**) p-value signicant aer Bonferroni correction at p < .0125. Error bars show standard error of
the mean.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
15
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
baroreceptor-mediated blood pressure regulation via sympathetic nerve bres47. However, other authors con-
vincingly demonstrated 0.1Hz oscillations in HRV might rather reect activity of the unmyelinated vagus nerve
originating in the dorsal lateral vagal complex49, which was supported by ndings that those oscillations were
abolished by vagal blockade but only little unaected by sympathetic blockade50. Following other authors who
presented a model of frequency-power distribution in skin blood ow6, we herewith seem to be able to provide
a promising approach to the methodological toolbox needed to improve capturing dynamic ANS regulation on
the level of the skin.
According to von Holst51, relative coordination is the rule in biological systems, while absolute coordination is
the exception. Further, frequency and amplitude responses to various stressors have long been known to exhibit
great variation51. is has been conrmed more recently in multiple skin locations for IM band frequencies
showing great variation with respect to presence of IM band52. is has led to the description of these opera-
tions as the meshing of cogs and the observed frequencies as clanking of the cogs53. erefore, a more recent
study suggested to include ideally a minimum of 2 biological systems in parallel when studying cardiovascular
system oscillations54.
In general, the coordination of blood ow and intravascular blood volume mandatory for intravascular blood
pressure may alter with changes in windkessel properties or with local redistribution of blood ow. is may
result in delays reected as phase dierences detected e.g., in cutaneous blood ow. In hard vessels this phase
dierence may be very small or vanish altogether, whereas so (dilatated) vessels may exhibit ’large’ phase dif-
ference. Shiing phases may therefore allow to infer changes in vascular tone. In PPG signals, this may index
changes in the tone of pre- and postcapillary sphincters, arterioles or venioles. Given the prominent function of
skin temperature regulation, cold pressure test may have had a signicant impact.
When combined with established high-resolution algorithms for the analysis of frequencies (e.g., continu-
ous wavelet scalograms), our novel methodology presented here on high resolution of dynamic phase changes
should greatly expand and therefore improve the study of rhythmic coordination processes. e improvement
comes with transforming the time-domain PPGI signal into an instantaneous phase waveform using the Hilbert
transform. Furthermore, the advantage of the non-contact and camera-based approach allows for high spatial
resolution of blood volume changes in cutaneous tissues. e matrix of PPGI signals transformed into a matrix of
instantaneous phases contained colour coded information on phases relative to the reference point. Determining
Figure18. Phase desynchronization for LF and IM frequency band denitions based on Pfurtscheller (Pf) and
Keller (Ke), and dierent thresholds in the control group (N = 3). R atio
Rφ>π/
4 , (B)
Rφ>
3
π/
8 and (C)
Rφ>π/
2
shows a general increasing trend of phase desynchronization, however, no signicant eects. Error bars show
standard error of the mean.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
16
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
the number of points of phase shis on the forehead region exceeding a predened threshold, allowed evaluation
of ANS responses found to be sensitive and capable of determining the state of the subject’s autonomic system.
is is supported by our control group which, however, due to its small sample size should be viewed with
caution. e control group showed a clear increase for IM band phase desynchronizations by the end of the
experimental session which may be related to the duration of relaxation experienced as tedious since the optimal
duration of relaxation should be limited to 7 min55. is is reason to assume the present methodology ideally
sensitive for the analysis of cutaneous responses qualifying it as a complementary tool for non-invasive diagnosis
compared to other evaluation methods, e.g., in the spectral amplitude domain56,57, or time-domain analysis58.
Our current results rest on analysis of primary cutaneous blood ow rhythmicity. More recently, PPG signals
frequently served as an undemanding source for the study of rhythms related to cardiac and respiratory activ-
ity (see59). On the other hand, methods such as laser Doppler devices proved valuable in physiological studies
investigating frequency response characteristics of sympathetically mediated vasomotor activity60. Analysis of
phase responses are of sound physiological relevance since changes observed in the frequency domain have
been burdened by various shortcomings. Among the most important ones are inherent frequency variations
or instabilities in biological systems. In systems with low complexity such as medulla sh, inherent frequency
variations may amount up to 35% of variation in either direction51, and up to 20% in humans reported in osteo-
pathic medicine61.
e current ndings present a novel technological approach of investigating phase changes in PPGI data.
However, reliability of ndings should further be investigated in larger samples to dierentiate e.g. the long-
disputed 0.1Hz activity in the contexts of LF and IM bands. Furthermore, a variety of physiological factors
are known to modulate cardiovascular oscillations. ese include the hormonal circuits (HPA axis) regulating
blood volume, blood pressure, and vascular tone (renin—angiotensin system; epinephrine and norepinephrine;
etc.). Some of these factors can be partly inuenced by experimental paradigms, e.g., orthostasis stress (tilt table
experiments), physical activity, mental activity (arithmetic tests). Other factors require testing specic patient
populations to investigate deviations from physiologic controls. Furthermore, in the present study, we used PPG
of the center of the forehead as a reference. In further studies, however, e.g., respiration should be included,
since blood ow to the skin and respiration are known to interact intensively6. Lastly, physiological changes
should be investigated only in a psychometrically validated manner. Essentially, the results of Keller etal. on
peripheral 0.15Hz activity demonstrate the urgent need to interpret physiological measurements with respect
to interoceptive processes. is has also been demonstrated by recent results of Pfurtscheller etal. who showed
that dierent levels of fear processing occur at LFa and LFb26. Questionnaires such as the Multidimensional
Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness62 or the state and trait anxiety questionnaire63 are important tools for
psychophysiological interpretation of phase analyses.
Outlook. ANS responses at the full scale should be studied further using spatial phase changes as a highly
promising method. It will undoubtedly be worthwhile to further investigate these phenomena through a series
of additional, comprehensive experiments that could help develop a highly eective tool for evaluating ANS
responses. is will eventually help our understanding of the still enigmatic concert of physiological rhythms.
Received: 13 September 2021; Accepted: 24 March 2022
References
1. Haken, H. & Koepchen, H. P. Physiology of rhythms and control systems: An integrative approach. In Rhythms in Physiological
Systems Vol. 55 (eds Haken, H. & Koepchen, H. P.) 3–20 (Springer, 1991). https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ 978-3- 642- 76877-4_1.
2. Schmid-Schönbein, H., Ziege, S., Blazek, V & Perlitz, V. New paradigm. Objective quantication of temporal patterns in skin
perfusion. In Computer-Aided Noninvasive Vascular Diagnostics. Vol. 1: Proceedings of 9th International Symposium CNVD 2000
(eds Blazek, V., Schultz-Ehrenburg, U., Stvrtinova, V. 75–98 (2000).
3. Hertzman, A. B. Photoelectric plethysmography of the ngers and toes in man. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 37(3), 529–534 (1937).
4. Zaunseder, S., Trumpp, A., Wedekind, D. & Malberg, H. Cardiovascular assessment by imaging photoplethysmography—A review.
Biomed. Eng. Biomedizinische Technik 63(5), 617–634 (2018).
5. Deutsch, F. Some psychodynamic considerations of psychosomatic skin disorders: Plethysmographic and psychoanalytic observa-
tions. Psychosom. Med. 14(4), 287–294. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1097/ 00006 842- 19520 7000- 00007 (1952).
6. Häfner, H.-M. et al. Wavelet analysis of skin perfusion in healthy volunteers. Microcirculation 14(2), 137–144. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1080/ 10739 68060 11312 34 (2007).
7. Rassler, B., Schwerdtfeger, A., Aigner, C. S. & Pfurtscheller, G. ‘Switch-o’ of respiratory sinus arrhythmia can occur in a minority
of subjects during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Front. Physiol. 9, 1688 (2018).
8. Kettunen, J. & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. Intraindividual analysis of instantaneous heart rate variability. Psychophysiology 38(4),
659–668. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ 1469- 8986. 38406 59 (2001).
9. Lambertz, M. & Langhorst, P. Simultaneous changes of rhythmic organization in brainstem neurons, respiration, cardiovascular
system and EEG between 0.05 Hz and 0.5 Hz. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 68(1–2), 58–77. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0165- 1838(97) 00126-4
(1998).
10. Perlitz, V. et al. Cardiovascular rhythms in the 0.15-Hz band: Common origin of identical phenomena in man and dog in the
reticular formation of the brain stem?. Pugers Arch. Eur. J. Physiol. 448(6), 579–591. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s00424- 004- 1291-4
(2004).
11. Smits, T., Aarnoudse, J., Geerdink, J. & Zijlstra, W. Hyperventilation-induced changes in periodic oscillations in forehead skin
blood ow measured by laser Doppler owmetry. Int. J. Microcirc. Clin. Exp. 6(2), 149–159 (1987).
12. Ziege, S. Optoelektronische Analyse von aktiven und passiven Hautperfusionsrhythmen und deren Bedeutung hinsichtlich der zentralen
vegetativen regulation (1992).
13. Perlitz, V. et al. Coordination dynamics of circulatory and respiratory rhythms during psychomotor drive reduction. Auton.
Neurosci. 115(1–2), 82–93 (2004).
14. Traube, L. Ueber periodische ätigkeits-Aeusserungen des vasomotorischen und Hemmungs-Nervencentrum. Centralblatt
Medicin. Wissenschaen Berlin 3, 881–885 (1865).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
17
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
15. Wu, T., Blazek, V. & Schmitt, H. J. “Photoplethysmography imaging: A new noninvasive and noncontact method for mapping of
the dermal perfusion changes. Int. Soc. Opt. Photon 4163, 62–70 (2000).
16. Leonhardt, S. Concluding remarks and new horizons in skin perfusion studies. In Studies in Skin Perfusion Dynamics 223–232
(Springer, 2021).
17. Borik, S. et al. Photoplethysmography imaging: Camera performance evaluation by means of an optoelectronic skin perfusion
phantom. Physiol. Meas. 41(5), 054001 (2020).
18. McDu, D., Estepp, J., Piasecki, A., & Blackford, E. A survey of remote optical photoplethysmographic imaging methods. In 2015
37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) 6398–6404 (2015).
19. Verkruysse, W., Svaasand, L. O. & Nelson, J. S. Remote plethysmographic imaging using ambient light. Opt. Express 16(26),
21434–21445 (2008).
20. Blanik, N., Abbas, A. K., Venema, B., Blazek, V. & Leonhardt, S. Hybrid optical imaging technology for long-term remote monitor-
ing of skin perfusion and temperature behavior. J. Biomed. Opt. 19(1), 016012 (2014).
21. Zaproudina, N. et al. Asynchronicity of facial blood perfusion in migraine. PLoS ONE 8(12), e80189 (2013).
22. Kamshilin, A. A., Miridonov, S., Teplov, V., Saarenheimo, R. & Nippolainen, E. Photoplethysmographic imaging of high spatial
resolution. Biomed. Opt. Express 2(4), 996–1006 (2011).
23. Kamshilin, A. A., Teplov, V., Nippolainen, E., Miridonov, S. & Giniatullin, R. Variability of microcirculation detected by blood
pulsation imaging. PLoS ONE 8(2), e57117 (2013).
24. Kamshilin, A. A. et al. A new look at the essence of the imaging photoplethysmography. Sci. Rep. 5(1), 1–9 (2015).
25. Moço, A. V., Stuijk, S. & de Haan, G. New insights into the origin of remote PPG signals in visible light and infrared. Sci. Rep. 8(1),
8501. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s41598- 018- 26068-2 (2018).
26. Schwerdtfeger, A. R. et al. Heart rate variability (HRV): From brain death to resonance breathing at 6 breaths per minute. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 131(3), 676–693. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. clinph. 2019. 11. 013 (2020).
27. Keller, M. et al. Neural correlates of uctuations in the intermediate band for heart rate and respiration are related to interoceptive
perception. Psychophysiology 57, 9. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ psyp. 13594 (2020).
28. Gräfe, K., Zipfel, S., Herzog, W. & Löwe, B. Screening psychischer Störungen mit dem “Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten
(PHQ-D)”. Diagnostica 50(4), 171–181 (2004).
29. Lyra, S. & Paul, M. Organic LED panels for pulse rate measurement using photoplethysmography imaging. In 23rd International
Student Conference on Electrical Engineering - POSTER 2019, Prague 1–4 (2019).
30. Kumar, M., Veeraraghavan, A. & Sabharwal, A. DistancePPG: Robust non-contact vital signs monitoring using a camera. Biomed.
Opt. Express 6(5), 1565–1588 (2015).
31. Iozzia, L., Cerina, L. & Mainardi, L. Relationships between heart-rate variability and pulse-rate variability obtained from video-
PPG signal using ZCA. Physiol. Meas. 37(11), 1934 (2016).
32. Gambi, E. et al. Heart rate detection using microso kinect: Validation and comparison to wearable devices. Sensors 17(8), 1776
(2017).
33. Tarbox, E. A. et al. Motion correction for improved estimation of heart rate using a visual spectrum camera. In Smart Biomedical
and Physiological Sensor Technology XIV, California, United States, Vol. 10216, p. 1021607 (2017).
34. Butler, M., Crowe, J. A., Hayes-Gill, B. R. & Rodmell, P. I. Motion limitations of non-contact photoplethysmography due to the
optical and topological properties of skin. Physiol. Meas. 37(5), N27 (2016).
35. Viola, P. & Jones, M. Rapid object detection using a boosted cascade of simple features. In Proceedings of the 2001 IEEE Computer
Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition. CVPR 2001, Vol. 1, I–I (2001).
36. Shi, J. & Tomasi. Good features to track. In 1994 Proceedings of IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition
593–600. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1109/ CVPR. 1994. 323794 (1994).
37. Lucas, B. D. & Kanade, T. An iterative image registration technique with an application to stereo vision. In Proceedings of the 7th
International Joint Conference on Articial Intelligence, Vancouver, Canada 674–679 (1981).
38. Tomasi, C. & Kanade, T. Detection and tracking of point features (School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon Univ., 1991).
39. Nason, G. P. & Silverman, B. W. e stationary wavelet transform and some statistical applications. In Wavelets and Statistics
281–299 (Springer, 1995).
40. Coifman, R. R. & Donoho, D. L. Translation-invariant de-noising. In Wavelets and statistics 125–150 (Springer, 1995).
41. Pesquet, J.-C., Krim, H. & Carfantan, H. Time-invariant orthonormal wavelet representations. IEEE Trans. Signal Process. 44(8),
1964–1970 (1996).
42. Liu, H. et al. Comparison of dierent modulations of photoplethysmography in extracting respiratory rate: From a physiological
perspective. Physiol. Meas. 41(9), 094001 (2020).
43. L anghorst, P., Schulz, G. & Lambertz, M. Oscillating neuronal network of the “Common Brainstem System". Mech. Blood Pressure
Waves 257–275 (1984).
44. Meredith, D. J. et al. Photoplethysmographic derivation of respiratory rate: A review of relevant physiology. J. Med. Eng. Technol.
36(1), 1–7 (2012).
45. Hernando, A., Peláez-Coca, M. D., Lozano, M., Lázaro, J. & Gil, E. Finger and forehead PPG signal comparison for respiratory
rate estimation. Physiol Meas 40(9), 095007 (2019).
46. Pfurtscheller, G. et al. Distinction between neural and vascular BOLD oscillations and intertwined heart rate oscillations at 0.1
Hz in the resting state and during movement. PLoS ONE 12(1), e0168097. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 01680 97 (2017).
47. Friedman, B. H. An autonomic exibility–neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biol. Psychol. 74(2),
185–199. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ycho. 2005. 08. 009 (2007).
48. Billman, G. E. e LF/HF ratio does not accurately measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Front. Physiol. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3389/ fphys. 2013. 00026 (2013).
49. Porges, S. W. e polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74(2), 116–143. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ycho. 2006. 06. 009 (2007).
50. Kromenacker, B. W., Sanova, A. A., Marcus, F. I., Allen, J. J. B. & Lane, R. D. Vagal mediation of low-frequency heart rate variability
during slow yogic breathing. Psychosom. Med. 80(6), 581–587. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1097/ PSY. 00000 00000 000603 (2018).
51. Holst, E. Die relative Koordination: als Phänomen und als Methode zentralnervöser Funktionsanalyse. Ergebnisse der Physiologie
und exper. Pharmakologie 42(1), 228–306. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ BF023 22567 (1939).
52. Ernst, J.-P. Dynamik des 0.15-Hz-Rhythmusbandes in der Hautdurchblutung bei Musikexposition (Doctoral Dissertation), Pub-
lisher: Hochschulbibliothek der Rheinisch-Westfälischen Technischen Hochschule Aachen (2012).
53. Malpas, S. C. Neural inuences on cardiovascular variability: possibilities and pitfalls. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circulat. Physiol. 282(1),
H6–H20. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1152/ ajphe art. 2002. 282.1. H6 (2002).
54. von Bonin, D. et al. Adaption of cardio-respiratory balance during day-rest compared to deep sleep—An indicator for quality of
life?. Psychiatry Res. 219(3), 638–644. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psych res. 2014. 06. 004 (2014).
55. Vaitl, D. & Petermann, F. Handbuch der Entspannungsverfahren Vol II, Anwendungen (Beltz-Psychologie Verlagsunion, 1993).
56. McDu, D. et al. Non-contact imaging of peripheral hemodynamics during cognitive and psychological stressors. Sci. Rep. 10(1),
1–13 (2020).
57. Pilz, C. S., Ben Makhlouf, I., Habel, U. & Leonhardt, S. Predicting Brainwaves from Face Videos 282–283 (2020).
58. Rasche, S. et al. Association of remote imaging photoplethysmography and cutaneous perfusion in volunteers. Sci. Rep. 10(1), 1–9
(2020).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
18
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2022) 12:5997 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09762-0
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
59. Schäfer, A. & Vagedes, J. How accurate is pulse rate variability as an estimate of heart rate variability?. Int. J. Cardiol. 166(1), 15–29.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. ijcard. 2012. 03. 119 (2013).
60. Stauss, H. M., Anderson, E. A., Haynes, W. G. & Kregel, K. C. Frequency response characteristics of sympathetically mediated
vasomotor waves in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circulat. Physiol. 274(4), H1277–H1283. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1152/ ajph e art. 1998.
274.4. H1277 (1998).
61. Nelson, K. E., Sergueef, N. & Glonek, T. Recording the rate of the cranial rhythmic impulse. J. Am. Osteopath. Assoc. 106(6),
337–341 (2006).
62. Mehling, W. E., Acree, M., Stewart, A., Silas, J. & Jones, A. e multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version
2 (MAIA-2). PLoS ONE 13(12), e0208034. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 02080 34 (2018).
63. Spielberger, C. D. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults (American Psychological Association, 2012). https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/
t06496- 000.
Acknowledgements
is work was supported by the National Scholarship Programme of the Slovak Republic (Application No. 29417).
Dr. Volker Perlitz and Micha Keller received nancial support from the HEAD Genuit Foundation, Herzogen-
rath/Germany, grant number: HGS-03S-18072016. e authors are indebted to Prof. Dr. Dipl.-Ing. Reinhard
Grebe, Babeuf/France for his valuable assistance in the writing of the reviewed manuscript.
Author contributions
Conceptualization—S.B., S.L., V.P., V.B.; Data curation—S.B., S.L.; Formal analysis—S.B., M.K.; Investigation—
S.B., S.L.; Methodology—S.B., V.P., V.B.; Resources—St.L., V.B.; Soware—S.B., S.L.; Supervision—V.B.; Vali-
dation—S.L., V.P., M.K., St.L., V.B.; Visualization—S.B., S.L.; Writing—original dra—S.B., S.L., V.P.; Writing—
review and editing—S.B., S.L., V.P., M.K., St.L., V.B.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to S.B.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
© e Author(s) 2022
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Content uploaded by Micha Keller
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Micha Keller on Apr 11, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.