ArticlePDF Available

Friendship Conflict, Drinking to Cope, and Alcohol-Related Problems: A Longitudinal Actor-Partner Interdependence Model

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Drinking to cope with negative affect is a strong predictor of alcohol-related problems. We hypothesized that the association between friendship conflict and alcohol-related problems would be mediated by coping-with-depression motives in emerging adults’ close friendships. We used a 4-wave, 4-month longitudinal self-report survey design measuring friendship conflict, coping motives, and alcohol-related problems from 174 same-sex friendship dyads. Participants were recruited from Nova Scotia, Canada between September 2016 and February 2019. Participants had a mean age of 18.66 ( SD = 1.17) and were 66.1% female. Data were analyzed using multilevel structural equation modeling. Coping-with-depression motives mediated the link between conflict and alcohol-related problems at the between- and within-subject levels. Unexpectedly, coping-with-anxiety motives was an additional mediator at the within-subjects level. Interventions for emerging adults’ problem drinking should consider the influence of friendship conflict and its impact on emerging adults’ tendencies to drink to cope with both depression and anxiety. Materials/Syntax: https://osf.io/krs3v/
Content may be subject to copyright.
Substance Use
Emerging Adulthood
2022, Vol. 10(3) 595608
© 2022 Society for the
Study of Emerging Adulthood
and SAGE Publishing
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/21676968211060945
journals.sagepub.com/home/eax
Friendship Conict, Drinking to Cope, and
Alcohol-Related Problems: A Longitudinal
Actor-Partner Interdependence Model
Sean P. Mackinnon
1
, Michelle E. Tougas
1
, Ivy-Lee L. Kehayes
1
,
and Sherry H. Stewart
1
Abstract
Drinking to cope with negative affect is a strong predictor of alcohol-related problems. We hypothesized that the association
between friendship conict and alcohol-related problems would be mediated by coping-with-depression motives in emerging
adultsclose friendships. We used a 4-wave, 4-month longitudinal self-report survey design measuring friendship conict, coping
motives, and alcohol-related problems from 174 same-sex friendship dyads. Participants were recruited from Nova Scotia,
Canada between September 2016 and February 2019. Participants had a mean age of 18.66 (SD = 1.17) and were 66.1% female.
Data were analyzed using multilevel structural equation modeling. Coping-with-depression motives mediated the link between
conict and alcohol-related problems at the between- and within-subject levels. Unexpectedly, coping-with-anxiety motives
was an additional mediator at the within-subjects level. Interventions for emerging adultsproblem drinking should consider the
inuence of friendship conict and its impact on emerging adultstendencies to drink to cope with both depression and anxiety.
Materials/Syntax: https://osf.io/krs3v/
Keywords
alcohol use/abuse, coping, friendship, transitions to adulthood, peers
Emerging adults (1829 years old; Arnett et al., 2014) have
the highest prevalence of alcohol use, with 82.8% of Canadian
emerging adults drinking in the past year (Canadian Centre on
Substance Use and Addiction, 2017). Among Canadian post-
secondary students, 35% report consuming at least ve or
more drinks in one sitting in the last 2 weeks, and 49.5% report
at least one alcohol-related problem over the past 12 months
(American College Health Association, 2019). The most
common alcohol-related problems experienced by post-secondary
students are having a bad time (33.0%), noticing a change in
personality (31.7%), neglecting responsibilities (27.6%), miss-
ing a day (or part-day) of school or work (25.6%), and being
unable to do homework or study for a test (22.1%) (Neal et al.,
2006). The present study tests whether friendship conict and
drinking to cope predict alcohol-related problems in friendship
dyads using a 4-wave, 4-month longitudinal design.
Drinking Motives Theory
Young people drink alcohol for a variety of motives, some of
which are riskier than others. The motivational model of alcohol
use (Cooper, 1994;seealsoCooper et al., 2016) describes two
underlying dimensions of the consequences that young people
seek from drinking alcohol: (1) positive vs. negative
reinforcement; and (2) internal versus external motivational
sources. More specically, the desired outcomes of drinking may
involve pursuing a positive outcome (e.g., pleasurable arousal) or
avoiding a negative outcome (e.g., avoiding depression), and
achieving sought after internal (e.g., mood manipulation) or
external (social approval) consequences. Combining these un-
derlying drinking motivation dimensions results in four indi-
vidual drinking motives: social (positive reinforcement, external),
enhancement (positive reinforcement, internal), conformity
(negative reinforcement, external), and coping (negative rein-
forcement, internal) (Cooper, 1994;Cooper et al., 2016).
Each of the four drinking motives are associated with
specic alcohol-related outcomes (see Cooper et al., 2016).
Drinking to cope (i.e., drinking to alleviate negative affect) is
unique in that it is directly related to alcohol-related problems
(e.g., problems with school; Cooper et al., 1995) even after
accounting for alcohol consumption levels. Further research
1
Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
Corresponding Author:
Sean P. Mackinnon, Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Life
Sciences Centre, 1355 Oxford St., Halifax, NS B3H 4R2, Canada.
Email: mackinnon.sean@dal.ca
exploring the four-factor model of alcohol-use motives found
that subdividing coping motives into two distinct factors to
create a ve-factor model was a better t for assessing
drinking motivations in emerging adults (Grant, Stewart,
OConnor, et al., 2007). To create the ve-factor model,
coping motives were divided into coping with anxiety motives
(CAM) and coping with depression motives (CDM), which
both uniquely predicted distinct alcohol-related outcomes.
Coping with anxiety motives showed cross sectional and CDM
longitudinal associations with alcohol-related problems (Grant,
Stewart, OConnor, et al., 2007). Further exploration is needed
of factors, such as friendship conict, that may lead to each of
these coping motives and in turn to alcohol-related problems.
Friendship Conict and Alcohol
Conict within friendships may be an important trigger of both
negative emotions and of drinking in young people given that
friendships are central to the lives of emerging adults (McNamara
Barry et al., 2015). In a large cross-sectional study of 1074
emerging adult friend dyads, Boman et al. (2013) found that
friendship dyads where both friends engage in binge drinking
and cannabis use were characterized by increased conict. One
possible explanation for this link is that friendship conict may
trigger heavy drinking in both friendship dyad members. Studies
of young friendship dyads have examined whether friendship
conict leads to negative emotions. SchwartzMette et al.
(2021) found that positive friendship quality longitudinally
predicted lower depression in cross-lagged panel models for
adolescent friend dyads; however, friendship conict was
generally unrelated to depressive symptoms in this sample. In
contrast, Chow et al. (2015) found that friendship discord was
associated with increased depressive symptoms in university
student friendship dyads. One possible reason for the discrepant
results is the difference in developmental stage (adolescence vs.
emerging adulthood). The more transitory nature of friendships
during emerging adulthood (Laursen & Bukowski, 1997)and
the tendency in this developmental stage to shift focus from
friendships to romantic relationships (McNamara Barry et al.,
2009) might give rise to increased friendship conict in the
transition from adolescence to emerging adulthood (Camirand
& Poulin, 2019) and to result in negative emotions that could
trigger coping drinking. Thus, we focused our investigation on
emerging adulthood. Another difference between Chow et al.
(2015) and SchwartzMette et al. (2021) pertains to the op-
erationalization of conictthe item content of Chow et al.
(2015) focused much more on criticism, dominance, and ex-
clusion than the measure used by SchwartzMette et al. (2021).
The present study utilized measures that tap friendship conict
in a manner consistent with Chow et al. (2015).
In the present study, we conceptualized conict as a dyadic
variablefor example, mutually expressed anger, hostility, and/
or communicative disengagement that happens within a given
relationship. This contrasts with research on social negativity,
which conceptualizes anger, hostility, and criticism as an intra-
individual variable (IbarraRovillard & Kuiper, 2011). Thus, our
operationalization of conict measures it as an inherently in-
terpersonal process (i.e., relational conict between people,
rather than a measurement of intra-individual hostility), as we
believe it is dyadic conict between friends, not the act of being
hostile toward a friend per se, that predicts drinking to cope.
Replication Target
Using a 4-wave, 4-week longitudinal design with 100 romantic
dyads, Lambe et al. (2015) found that the relationship between
conict and alcohol-related problems was mediated by coping
motives. Specically, only CDM (and not CAM) motives me-
diated the conict-to-alcohol problems association at the within-
subjects level when both motives were entered as mediators in a
single model (Lambe et al., 2015). These results were found for
actor effects (how an individual inuences their own behavior),
but not for partner effects (how a partner inuences an indi-
viduals behavior). Therefore, increases in conict within a ro-
mantic relationship predicted increases in CDM drinking motives
which, in turn, predicted increases in alcohol-related problems in
the same individual.
Lambe et al. (2015) has important implications for the
treatment of drinking problems. Specically, it implies that
romantic relationship conict might be a distal predictor of
alcohol problemsthus, addressing relationship conict could
have downstream improvements for alcohol-related problems.
Nonetheless, romantic relationships represent only one facet of
an emerging adults social network and potential relationship
conicts. Thus, we wanted to see if the ndings of Lambe et al.
(2015) generalized to another important type of relationship in
the lives of emerging adults, namely friendships. We sought to
conceptually replicate and extend Lambe et al. (2015) with
three primary methodological changes. The rst change was to
recruit same-sex friends instead of romantic couples. At the
emerging adult developmental stage, young people increasingly
turn to peers for social interactions and social support, with
these friendships inuencing many areas of their lives (Buote
et al., 2007;Lewis et al., 2015). Ruptures in these friendships
may have signicant consequences on feelings of anxiety and
depression (Chow et al., 2015, which may trigger coping-
motivated drinking and in turn alcohol-related problems.
Thus, the current study investigated the mediational role of each
of the coping motives (CDM and CAM) in explaining the
relationship between friendship conict and alcohol-related
problems in both emerging adult actors and their friends.
The second methodological change was to change from a
4-wave, 4-week design to a 4-wave, 4-month design. A pri-
mary difculty in longitudinal research is to nd the right time
lag for the causal processes under study. A weakness of the
Lambe et al. (2015) study was that the 1-week measurement
occasions were short, and did not necessarily allow sufcient
time for conict, motives, and alcohol problems to change
over time. Moreover, short time lags can result in restricted
variance for comparatively rare events, such as conict and
596 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
alcohol problems. Thus, we increased the lag between mea-
surement occasions to increase the variation in our studied
constructs. The third change was a move from in-person data
collection to online data collection. This change was for ef-
ciency and pragmatics. Otherwise, the methods of the present
study were virtually identical to Lambe et al. (2015).
Objectives
Using a longitudinal, 4-month, 4-wave design, this study ex-
amined the association between friendship conict and alcohol-
related problems. This association was further explored with
CDM and CAM motives as potential mediators of the asso-
ciation between friendship conict and alcohol-related prob-
lems. Moreover, we investigated actor and partner effects.
Partner effects may be expressed directly, where a friends
drinking motive may impact an individuals own alcohol-related
problems. Partner effects may also be expressed indirectly,
wherein a friends drinking motive predicts an individualsown
alcohol-related problems, which in turn may inuence the in-
dividuals own alcohol-related problems. Given the longitudinal
nature of the study, data were analyzed using multilevel
structural equation modeling (SEM) (Preacher et al., 2010).
Structural equation modeling explores between-subject vari-
ance, the portion of variance that stays consistent across the
4 months, as well as within-subject variance, the portion of
variance that changes from month to month.
Given that this research aimed to replicate and extend the
work of Lambe et al. (2015), the hypotheses and research
questions were formulated from their ndings on the rela-
tionship between conict, coping motives, and alcohol-related
problems in romantic couples. Our hypotheses were also
informed by prior ndings linking friendship conict to de-
pression (Chow et al., 2015) and conict to alcohol outcomes
(Boman et al., 2013) in emerging adult friendship dyads. In
our conrmatory model replicating Lambe et al. (2015),we
tested the same model, and derived our predictions from the
ndings of that study. Avisual depiction of the model and our
predictions for the direction of relationships derived from
Lambe et al. (2015) can be found in Figure 1. These pre-
dictions can be summarized as follows:
1) Friendship conict would have an indirect effect on
alcohol-related problems through CDM motives,
meaning that friendship conict would lead to drinking
to cope with depression, which in turn would lead to
alcohol-related problems. Specically, we expected
these ndings to hold for actor effects at the within-
subjects level.
2) Friendship conict would lead to drinking to cope with
anxiety at both the between- and within-subject levels.
Other paths in Lambe et al. (2015) were inconclusive.
Thus, for other paths in Figure 1, we did not have a priori
predictions, and these tests were considered exploratory.
1) Would friendship conict have an indirect effect on
alcohol-related problems through CDM motives for
partner effects and/or at the between-subjects level?
2) Would friendship conict have an indirect effect on
alcohol-related problems through CAM motives for
actor and/or partner effects at either the between- and/
or within-subject levels?
Method
Participants
Participants were required to be same-sex friends who con-
sume alcohol together and had known each other for a year or
less. Participants were included in the study if they consumed
12 or more alcoholic drinks in the past year, were between the
ages of 1825 years old, and at least one of the friends was a
rst-year undergraduate student. These requirements were
listed in our recruitment materials. All participants met these
criteria when screened via email prior to arrival at the lab. All
participants were recruited from Nova Scotia, Canada. These
data were previously analyzed in three prior studies, one
examining the relationship between drinking motives and
alcohol quantity/frequency (Kehayes et al., 2021), another on
extraversion and drinking similarity (NogueiraArjona et al.,
2019), and a third on the validity of informant-reported drinking
motives (Kim et al., in press); our study made secondary use of
these data.
Participants were 348 undergraduate students from 174
same-sex friendship dyads. Individual participants had an av-
erage age of 18.66 years (SD = 1.17), were 66.1% female,
79.3% Caucasian, and 84.8% were university students. They
were recruited from Dalhousie University and the surrounding
community. Participants reported hearing about the study
through yers (37.9%), word-of-mouth (25.0%), class website
(20.7%), classroom announcements (17.0%), the Psychology
Department participant pool (10.1%), or other sources (15.8%),
with some participants reporting more than one source. At
Wave 1, the initial point of contact with the lab, the dyads re-
ported an average friendship length of 4.05 months (SD =2.21),
with an average face-to-face contact of 19.75 days/month (SD =
7.60). At Wave 1, a total of 21.0% friends reported cohabitating
together for an average of 2.88 months (SD =1.56).AtWave1,
85.1% reported experiencing at least one alcohol-related problem
in the past month (M=4.14,SD =3.60,range=018).
Procedure
Data collection for the rst wave started in September 2016
and ended February 2019
1
. Data were collected during the Fall
and Winter semesters. Participants were recruited using the
psychology subject pool, online ads, yers, and in-class an-
nouncements. Interested participants contacted the study ad-
ministrators via email and were screened for eligibility. Dyads
arrived together at the lab and were again screened for
Mackinnon et al. 597
eligibility. Participants reviewed the consent form and gave
informed consent prior to participation. During the same visit,
each participant completed the Wave 1 questionnaire battery
online. For three monthly follow-ups, participants were
emailed the same questionnaire batteries for Waves 24 at 30-
day intervals. If participants did not complete a questionnaire
the day it was mailed, they were emailed reminders daily for
7 days, with three additional reminders until the end of the 30-
day period. Reminders ceased after completion of the ques-
tionnaires. The make-up questionnaires evaluated the same
30-day period as the original to ensure that responses would be
for the same 30-day period, regardless of when completed. If
Figure 1. Figure depicting conrmatory hypotheses (solid lines) and exploratory hypotheses (dotted lines). Note. Rectangles indicate
manifest variables. Single-headed arrows indicate paths. Residual covariances are omitted on this conceptual diagram to reduce clutter.
Black lines indicate hypothesized paths based on Lambe et al. (2015) and the expected direction (+, or positive). Dotted lines represent
exploratory research questions for other paths included in the model.
598 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
participants did not complete one of the waves, their data was
counted as missing for that wave only. Skip logic was em-
ployed, where participants were not asked to complete the
drinking motives questionnaire for a given wave if they ab-
stained from alcohol during that month (i.e., an individual
cannot have a motive for drinking if they did not drink).
Participants were compensated CAD$10.00 or one credit
point for each wave that they completed on-time (within a
week of the questionnaire being sent). For questionnaires that
were completed 830 days after originally being sent, par-
ticipants were compensated with CAD$5.00 or ½ credit point.
All participants were debriefed at completion of participation.
Figure 2. Tested multilevel structural equation model including constraints. Note. Pathways that share a color and label (e.g., wa2) were
constrained to equality due to indistinguishable dyads. Paths were not constrained to equality across levels (e.g., wa2 and ba2 are not equal
to one another). Variance partitioned into between and within levels using latent mean centering. Actor effects are paths wb1, wb2, bb1, and
bb2. Partner effects are paths wp1, wp2, bp1, and bp2. Double-headed arrows are correlated residuals to account for non-independence.
Mackinnon et al. 599
Materials
Demographics and friendship. At Wave 1, each participant
completed questions about their demographic characteristics
(age, sex, and ethnicity) and information about their friendship
(friendship length, amount of face-to-face contact, and
cohabitation).
2
Conict. Friendship conict was analyzed as a composite
variable that consisted of the Social Conict Scale (Abbey &
Andrews, 1985), the Partner-Specic Rejection Behaviors Scale
(Murray et al., 2003), and the Interpersonal Qualities Scale
(Oishi & Sullivan, 2006). Each scale measured friendship
conict in the past month. The Social Conict Scale consisted
of ve items (e.g., Got on your friendsnerves) rated on a
scale ranging from 1 (not at all)to5(a great deal). The Partner-
Specic Rejection Behaviors Scale consisted of seven items
(e.g., Iinsultedmyfriend) rated on a scale ranging from 1
(strongly disagree)to9(strongly agree). The Interpersonal
Qualities Scale consisted of ve items that rated interpersonal
characteristics when in the presence of the study friend (e.g.,
moody/irritable), ranging from 1 (not at all characteristic)to
9(completely characteristic). In previous work, each individual
scale has exceeded acceptable levels of internal consistency
(alphas ranging from 0.750.84; Lambe et al., 2015). Averaged
subscale totals for each of the three scales were used for de-
scriptive statistics. For analyses, each subscale total was con-
verted to standardized Z scores, which were then summed to
create a single conict composite index score. This composite
measure has been shown to possess acceptable psychometric
properties with factor analysis showing a single factor with
loadings from each of the 17 conict items ranging from 0.47 to
0.81 (Lambe et al., 2015). This established factorial validity
indicated that combining the conict items into a single factor
was appropriate. For the current study, conict was assessed as
a dyadic variable with equal contributions from each member of
the friendship (Lambe et al., 2015;Mackinnon et al., 2012).
Modied drinking motives questionnaireRevised. Coping with
depression motives and CAM were derived from the 30-day
version of the Modied Drinking Motives Questionnaire-
Revised (DMQ-R; Grant, Stewart, OConnor, et al., 2007),
amodied ve-factor version of the original four-factor
drinking motives scale (Cooper, 1994). The DMQ-R is a re-
liable and valid 28-item measure of ve drinking motives:
enhancement, social, conformity, CDM, and CAM. As the main
focus of this study was to replicate and extend the work of
Lambe et al. (2015) with friendship dyads, only CDM and
CAM were used in analyses. The CDM scale is composed of
nine items (e.g., to numb my pain), and the CAM scale is
composed of four items (e.g., to reduce my anxiety). Par-
ticipants rated how often they drank for each reason on a scale
from 1 (almost never/never)to5(almost always). The Modied
DMQ-R has been found to have good to excellent test-retest
reliability (intraclass correlation coefcients from 0.610.78),
adequate to excellent internal consistency (αsfrom0.66to
0.91), and strong factorial validity (Grant, Stewart, OConnor,
et al., 2007).
Rutgers alcohol problem index. The 30-day version of the
Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index (RAPI) was used to measure
alcohol-related problems (White & Labouvie, 1989). The
RAPI is a 23-item measure asking about the experience of
specic negative consequences (e.g., caused shame or em-
barrassment to someone) due to drinking. Participants rated
how often each problem occurred in the specied 30-day
timeframe on a scale from 0 (never)to4(4 or more times).
The RAPI has been found to have strong test-retest reliability (r
= .83), and high internal consistency (α= .92) for the total RAPI
score (Grant, Stewart, OConnor, et al., 2007). The individual
RAPI items were dichotomized (i.e., 1 = presence, 0 = absence
of alcohol-related problem). The dichotomized items were then
summed into a single value (possible range 023) for analysis
(Martens et al., 2007). The dichotomized RAPI shows good
psychometric properties including good internal consistency
with αs ranging from 0.74 to 0.83 (Martens et al., 2007).
Self-administered Timeline Follow-Back
Using a self-reported timeline follow-back procedure (Collins
et al., 2008), participants reported on their past 30 days of
alcohol consumption. Participants reported on the number of
alcoholic drinks they consumed each day, with a standard drink
dened as 12oz of beer, 5oz of wine, 3oz of fortied wine, or
1.5oz of hard liquor. Total volume of consumption was cal-
culated by summing the total number of drinks consumed over
the past 30 days. The Self-administered Timeline Follow-Back
(STLFB) provides similar results to more traditional quantity-
frequency measures of consumption (Collins et al., 2008).
Data Analytic Strategy
Data were analyzed using Mplus 7.0 software using multilevel
structural equation modeling (Preacher et al., 2010), com-
bining elements of traditional mediation models and Actor
Partner Interdependence models (Kenny & Ledermann,
2010). The tested model is depicted in Figure 2. Data were
in wide format for dyads (i.e., separate columns for partner A
and B), with dyadic non-independence handled using corre-
lated residuals. Because dyads are indistinguishable, partners
were randomly assigned the role of Partner A or B, and paths
were constrained to equality as depicted in Figure 2. Data were
in long format for the longitudinal component (i.e., one row
per timepoint), and non-independence was handled using
multilevel modeling. The multilevel model partitioned vari-
ance into between- and within-subjects levels using latent
mean centering (see Hamaker & Muth´
en, 2020). We used
random intercepts and xed slopes; this essentially assumes
compound symmetry for the longitudinal residual variances
(Streja et al., 2017, p. ii80). Missing data was handled using a
600 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
full information maximum likelihood approach. Signicance
of indirect effects was calculated using the delta method, as
bootstrapping is incompatible with TYPE = TWOLEVEL in
Mplus software. Nonnormality of residuals was accounted for
by using a robust estimator of t indices and standard errors
(MLR estimator in Mplus). Moreover, because skewness was
severe enough that the MLR estimation might still be biased,
CDM, CAM, and the RAPI were log
10
transformed
3
prior to
analysis to deal with positive skew (this also matches analytic
procedures used in Lambe et al., 2015).
Our choice to use MLR estimation and log transforms
instead of other common alternatives for dealing with violations
of the normality assumption was based on pragmatic concerns.
Bootstrapping is incompatible with multilevel models in Mplus.
Count models would require switching to the WLS estimator
which requires listwise deletion for missing data. Moreover,
count models make interpretation of mediation models difcult,
because indirect effects become conditional on values of X
(Geldof et al., 2018). Thus, we did not analyze the data using
count models. Finally, using log transforms matches the ap-
proach used in the replication target (Lambe et al., 2015).
Results
Out of four waves of surveys, individuals within dyads
completed an average of 3.49 waves (SD = 0.93), with 70.7%
of individuals completing all four waves. Across waves, 100%
of individuals completed Wave 1, 89.9% of individuals
completed Wave 2, 85.3% completed Wave 3, and 73.6%
completed Wave 4. During the study, three dyads reported
ending their friendship at Wave 3, and two dyads at Wave 4.
For these dyads, data collected before their friendships ended
were included in the analyses, while data collected after the
friendships ended were coded as missing (0.01% of wave
entries). Data from individual participants who reported ab-
staining from alcohol for any of the waves were excluded from
analyses for only the wave that they abstained from drinking
alcohol (5.7% of wave entries). In cases where one partner
abstained and the other drank alcohol, we still retained data
from the individuals who drank alcohol in analyses. No other
data were excluded from analyses. A total of 80.2% of the
surveys were completed on time, and 19.8% were completed
late (ranging from 18 days delayed). Across the four waves,
20.3% of data were missing, with covariance coverage ranging
from .76.91.
Table 1 presents the means and SDs of values across all four
waves. All values were comparable to the replication target
(i.e., within 1 SD of the mean), with the exception of the RAPI
mean for wave 2, which was still within 2 SD of the mean from
previous samples (Lambe et al., 2015).
4
Table 2 presents the
within- and between-subject level correlations, intraclass
correlations, and reliabilities. At both the between-and within-
subject levels, all variables were signicantly and positively
correlated with one another, with the magnitudes of correla-
tions being greater at the between-subjects level. Intraclass
correlations show the percentage of the variance available to
be explained at the between-subjects level; the majority of the
variance was calculated at the within-subjects level for all
variables (i.e., a state-trait with substantial state and trait
variance). All measures showed good reliability at both the
between-and within-subject levels, with the exception of
CAM at the within-subject level. When cluster sizes are small,
within-subjects level reliabilities may be underestimated, and
reliability cutoff scores are not as well-established in multi-
level models (Geldhof et al., 2014). Therefore, despite one low
reliability value, we proceeded with the planned analysis.
Nonetheless, analyses utilizing CAM have a high degree of
measurement error at the within-subjects level, and thus an
elevated potential for Type II error.
Mediation
Excellent model t was dened as follows: root-mean-square
error of approximation (RMSEA) < .06, standardized root-
mean-square residual (SRMR) < .08, and comparative t
index (CFI) and TuckerLewis index (TLI) > .95 (Hu &
Bentler, 1999;Kline, 2015). Finally, internal consistency
was examined at the between- and within-subject levels using
a multilevel adaptation of Cronbachs alpha (Geldhof et al.,
2014). The hypothesized model t well, χ
2
(20) = 32.51,
p <.05; RMSEA = .03; SRMR (within) = .07; SRMR (be-
tween) = .06, CFI = .98, TLI = .95. The unstandardized path
coefcients and the associated R
2
values are presented in
Figure 3, with indirect effects presented in Table 3.
Conrmatory analyses. At both the between- and within-subject
levels, conict signicantly predicted CDM and CAM. At the
within-subjects level, signicant actor effects were found where
CDM signicantly predicted alcohol-related problems in the
same individual; partner effects were not signicant. Tests of
indirect effects revealed that CDM signicantly mediated the
link between conict and alcohol-related problems.
Exploratory ndings. At the within-subjects level, conict sig-
nicantly predicted alcohol-related problems after controlling
for all other variables; no effects were found at the between-
subjects level. At the within-subjects level, signicant actor
effects were found where CAM signicantly predicted alcohol-
related problems in the same individual; partner effects were not
signicant. Tests of indirect effects revealed that CAM sig-
nicantly mediated the link between conict and alcohol-
related problems. No other signicant indirect effects were
found at the within-subjects level. At the between-subjects
level, signicant actor effects were found where CDM (and
not CAM) signicantly predicted alcohol-related problems in
the same individual; partner effects were not signicant. At the
between-subjects level, tests of indirect effects revealed that
CDM (but not CAM) signicantly mediated the link between
conict and alcohol-related problems. Indirect effects tests also
revealed that the actors alcohol-related problems signicantly
Mackinnon et al. 601
mediated the link between the actors CDM and the partners
alcohol-related problems (an indirect partner effect). No other
signicant indirect effects were found at the between-subjects
level.
At the within-subjects level, the correlated error terms
between the actors and partners alcohol-related problems
were not signicant (B= 0.003, p= .47). Although the cor-
related error terms between the actors and partners CDM (B
< 0.001, p= .88) and CAM (B= 0.001, p= .30) were also not
signicant, CDM and CAM were strongly related within the
same individual (B= 0.005, p< .001). The correlated error
terms between alcohol-related problems were signicant at the
between-subjects level (B= 0.018, p= .001). Although the
correlated error terms between partners CDM (B< 0.001, p=
.55) and CAM (B< .0001, p= .96) remained unrelated, CDM
and CAM maintained their relationship within the same in-
dividual (B= 0.011, p< .001). In general, effect sizes at the
within-subjects level were small with about 13% of the
variance explained in both coping motives, and 9% of the
variance explained in alcohol-related problems. At the
between-subjects level, for both coping motives, effect sizes
were small with 38% of variance explained in both coping
motives. For alcohol-related problems, however, a large
amount of the variance was explained by conict and coping
motives combined (3541%).
Supplementary Analyses
Upon reviewer request, we included a few additional un-
planned analyses. We tested the same model depicted in
Figure 2, replacing alcohol problems with total volume of
consumption using the STLFB measure. As in prior analyses,
alcohol consumption was log
10
transformed prior to analysis.
The hypothesized model t well, χ
2
(20) = 37.24, p = .01;
RMSEA = .04; SRMR (within) = .06; SRMR (between) = .06,
CFI = .97, TLI = .94. Broadly speaking, variables were weaker
predictors of alcohol consumption (R
2
= .12.15) than of
alcohol problems (R
2
= .35.41). In terms of hypothesis tests,
the following differences from the analyses with alcohol
problems as the outcome were noted: (a) Actor effects from
CDM to alcohol consumption were non-signicant at both the
between- and within-subjects levels; (b) Actor effects from
CAM to alcohol consumption became statistically signicant
and positive at the between-subjects level; and (c) The cor-
related error term for alcohol consumption in each partner
became statistically signicant and positive. All other paths
showed the same pattern of statistical signicance and di-
rection of effects as the model with the alcohol problems
measure as the outcome. All unstandardized path coefcients
and the associated R
2
values are presented in Supplementary
Figure 1, with indirect effects presented in Supplementary
Table 1.
5
We also ran a supplementary analysis where we entered sex
as a between-subjects covariate (i.e., a predictor of all vari-
ables in Figure 3). Note that sex cannot predict within-subjects
variables, due to how the variance was partitioned for the
multilevel model (i.e., it is a between-subjects variable that
Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations.
Variable
Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4
Actual Range Possible RangeMSDMSDMSDMSD
RAPI 4.14 3.60 4.04 4.14 3.58 4.08 2.79 3.77 023 023
CDM 1.42 0.69 1.37 0.59 1.33 0.60 1.38 0.77 1515
CAM 1.85 0.83 1.79 0.70 1.73 0.72 1.75 0.84 1515
Social conict 1.63 0.65 1.59 0.65 1.60 0.71 1.50 0.64 14.80 19
Rejecting behaviors 1.64 0.97 1.66 0.96 1.83 1.16 1.65 1.18 1919
Interpersonal qualities 3.08 1.61 2.98 1.60 3.06 1.74 3.07 1.85 1919
STLFB 42.89 40.26 31.43 32.34 28.19 29.25 23.98 27.91 0376 Open-ended numerical
Note. Ns vary by variable and wave and range from 197 to 348. Ms and SDs are shown for the averaged total of the summed dichotomized RAPI items, as well as
for the averaged subscale totals for the CDM and CAM motives, and the three conict variables prior to log
10
transformation. RAPI = Rutgers Alcohol Problem
Index; CDM = coping with depression motives; CAM = coping with anxiety motives. STLFB = Volume of alcohol consumption on the self-reported timeline
follow-back measure. Actual range is the minimum and maximum values across all 4 waves.
Table 2. Correlation Matrix, Intraclass Correlations, and
Reliabilities for Analyses at Between- and Within-Subject Levels.
Variable 12345
1. Conict .18*** .15*** .21*** .03
2. CDM .43*** .64*** .38*** .17***
3. CAM .26* .79*** .35*** .24***
4. RAPI .33*** .54*** .53*** .32***
5. STLFB .06 .26** .34 .72
ICC .42 .33 .29 .36 .51
Alpha reliability (within) .85 .92 .66 .80
Alpha reliability (between) .95 .98 .86 .95
Note. Between-subject correlations are below the diagonal; within-subject
correlations are above the diagonal. CDM, CAM and RAPI were log
10
transformed. CDM = coping with depression motives; CAM = coping with
anxiety motives; RAPI = Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index; STLFB = Volume of
alcohol consumption on the self-reported timeline follow-back measure; ICC
= intraclass correlation.
*p< .05. **p< .01. ***p< .005.
602 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
does not vary over time). In this model, sex was not signif-
icantly related to any other variable (all ps > .05). Moreover,
all the coefcients in Figure 3 retained the same pattern of
statistical signicance and direction of effects when sex was
included as a covariate. The raw output of this supplementary
analysis can be found on our OSF page: https://osf.io/krs3v/
Discussion
In both Lambe et al. (2015) and the present paper, relationship
conict positively predicted both CAM and CDM. That is,
conict in close relationships predicts drinking to cope at the
between-person and within-person levels. Thus, the ndings
Figure 3. Results of the multilevel structural equation model predicting alcohol-related problems. Note. Solid lines indicate signicant paths,
dashed lines indicate nonsignicant paths. Rectangles indicate manifest variables. Single-headed arrows indicate paths. Double-headed
arrows indicate covariances. Coefcients are unstandardized and paths were constrained to equality across both partners. R
2
values are
indicated in the upper right-hand corner of endogenous variables. See https://osf.io/krs3v/for full output.
Mackinnon et al. 603
for the link of conict to both types of coping drinking motives
were successfully replicated. Where the results diverge is at
the coping motives to alcohol problems stage. In Lambe et al.
(2015), there was an actor effect only for CDM predicting
alcohol problems, whereas the present paper nds actor effects
for both CDM and CAM, at the within-subjects level. At the
between-subjects level, there was substantial collinearity
that is, CAM and CDM were more strongly correlated with
each other. This is not surprising, as depression and anxiety
have been well-known to be comorbid for decades (Stavrakaki
& Vargo, 1986). Thus, in the original Lambe et al. (2015)
paper, CDM and CAM both predicted alcohol problems, but
when entered together, the results became inconclusive. The
present paper had a larger sample size, and was better able to
handle the collinearity issue, and thus we observed CDM
emerging as statistically signicant predictor at the between-
subjects level, even when controlling for CAM. Thus, the
actor effects from CDM to alcohol problems were successfully
replicated from Lambe et al. (2015). Additionally, supple-
mentary analyses suggest that these ndings persist even when
controlling sex, and that coping motives and conict are
stronger predictors of alcohol-related problems than of vol-
ume of alcohol consumption. However, in the present paper
relative to Lambe et al., we saw a stronger relationship be-
tween CAM and alcohol problems at the within-subjects level,
which bears further consideration.
When conict is present, friends may be turning to mal-
adaptive coping mechanisms (i.e., coping drinking) for dealing
with feelings of anxiety over time. There is some evidence to
suggest that anxiety and depression are each associated with
distinct patterns of alcohol use (Grant, Stewart, OConnor,
et al., 2007). Given that the mediation occurred in the cur-
rent study with friendship pairs, and not when romantic couples
were assessed, the type of relationship may explain the dif-
ference in CAM as a mediator between conict and alcohol-
related problems. Sex differences are reported in response to
conict, where men respond to conict with both depression
and anxiety, whereas women respond with only anxiety (El-
Sheikh et al., 2013). Considering that the romantic couples
study involved similar numbers of men and women, and this
study was comprised of more women (66.1%), perhaps these
differences are attributable to the greater proportion of women
in the present study. Given that depression and anxiety may be
associated with different alcohol-use patterns (Grant, Stewart,
OConnor, et al., 2007), the mechanisms and contextual factors
underlying depression-related drinking may be distinct from
those underlying anxiety-related drinking (Grant & Stewart,
2007;Grant, Stewart, & Birch, 2007;Grant et al., 2009). Of
interest, supplementary analyses suggested that CAM (but not
CDM) were positively associated with volume of drinking when
both motives were entered as simultaneous mediators. This
makes some sense, given the well-known anxiolytic properties
of alcohol (Stewart et al., 2016).
Further, the two studies used different measurement time-
frames; whereas Lambe et al. (2015) followed romantic couples
over four 1-week study waves, the current study followed
friendship pairs over four 1-month study waves. The longer
intervals between waves inthe current study may have provided
greater opportunity for CAM to exert effects on alcohol-related
problems over time (i.e., at the within-subjects level). Con-
sistent with our nding of CAM as a predictor of alcohol-related
problems at the within-subjects level (over time), Grant,
Stewart, OConnor, et al. (2007) identied that only CAM
were predictive of alcohol-related problems prospectively over
a mean follow-up interval of 94.8 days, when usual alcohol use
was controlled. Thus, CAM may be more relevant for alcohol
problems over longer time frames.
Similar to the ndings of Lambe et al. (2015), no direct
partner effects were found, suggesting that coping motives in
response to interpersonal conict may inuence alcohol
problems at only the individual level. An indirect partner effect
observed by Lambe et al. (2015) was replicated, with the re-
lationship between an individuals CDM and their partners
alcohol-related problems being mediated by the individualsown
alcohol-related problems at the between-subjects level. There-
fore, although no direct inuence of a friendsCDMonthe
individuals alcohol-related problems was found, the presence of
these motives in the friend indirectly led to alcohol-related
Table 3. Tests of Indirect Effects for the Multiple Mediation Model with CDM and CAM.
Predictor (X) Mediator (M) Outcome (Y) CI (within-subjects) CI (between-subjects)
Conict Actors CDM Actors RAPI
a
[.000, .004]*
a
[.002, .021]*
Conict Actors CDM Partners RAPI [.001, .002] [.009, .003]
Actors CDM Actors RAPI Partners RAPI [.002, .004] [.004, .031]*
Actors CDM Partners RAPI Actors RAPI [.001, .001] [.013, .005]
Conict Actors CAM Actors RAPI [.000, .005]* [.001, .007]
Conict Actors CAM Partners RAPI [.002, .001] [.002, .007]
Actors CAM Actors RAPI Partners RAPI [.002, .005] [.002, .015]
Actors CAM Partners RAPI Actors RAPI [.002, .001] [.003, .014]
Note. Indirect effects were derived using unstandardized coefcients. CI = Condence Interval (95% level of condence); RAPI = Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index;
CDM = coping with depression motives; CAM = coping with anxiety motives.
a
Conrmatory analyses; Bold CIs with * identify signicant indirect effects whose 95% CIs do not cross zero.
604 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
problems for the individual by way of rst inuencing the
friends alcohol-related problems which in turn inuenced the
individuals alcohol-related problems (potentially via modeling
of maladaptive drinking; see Muyingo et al., 2020).
At both the within- and between-subject levels, conict
was a signicant predictor of CDM and CAM, and an indirect
predictor of alcohol-related problems. At the within-subjects
level, both CDM and CAM subsequently predicted alcohol-
related problems. These ndings are broadly consistent with
previous research with romantic couples (Lambe et al., 2015),
and support predictions of drinking motives theory (Cooper
et al., 2016). These ndings suggest that emerging adults may
use alcohol to cope with feelings of depression and anxiety
following conict with their friends, a maladaptive behavior
that leads to alcohol-related problems. Further, over time,
alcohol-related problems may occur following conict with
friends. Coping with both feelings of depression and anxiety
appear to be important mechanisms through which friendship
conict leads to alcohol-related problems over time, whereas
only CDM appears to be an important mechanism through
which conict leads to alcohol-related problems at the
between-subjects level.
In adolescents, it has been found that peer drinking motives
inuence individual drinking motives (Kuntsche & Stewart,
2009). It was therefore unexpected in the present study that
coping motives were unrelated between friends over time.
However, this nding replicated the work of Lambe et al.
(2015) that also found no partner effects for coping motives
within romantic couples. The lack of partner effects found in
both studies may be related to contextual factors for coping
drinking, in that individuals drinking to cope with negative
affect are more likely to do so alone at home than with a friend
or partner (Cooper, 1994). Therefore, drinking to cope with
feelings of anxiety or depression resulting from interpersonal
conict may be a relatively solitary process that does not
inuence a friends coping motives. This is supported by our
supplementary analyses, which demonstrate a positive cor-
related error term for volume of consumption, but not for
alcohol problemsthat is, partners inuence each others
drinking habits, but may not be similar in their levels of
alcohol-related problems.
Clinical Implications
These results extend the Lambe et al. (2015) ndings with
emerging adultsromantic partners to the role of coping
drinking motives mediating the relationship between friend-
ship conict and both alcohol-related problems and alcohol
volume in emerging adults. Interventions for emerging adults
heavy and problem drinking should consider the inuence of
conict within close friendships and its impact on tendencies
to drink to cope with both depression and anxiety. Further,
interventions may include strategies for reducing CDM and
CAM to reduce associated heavy drinking and alcohol-related
problems. Considering that our results showed that alcohol-
related problems and alcohol volume changed systematically
in the same direction between friends, dyadic-level inter-
ventions may be useful to address both friendsalcohol-related
problems and associated inuences, like conict.
Limitations and Future Directions
This study is not without its limitations. The sample was pri-
marily White university students who were less than 21 years
old, which limits generalizability. Additionally, there may have
been some self-selection of dyads in the participation pool (i.e.,
people who agree to participate in research studies may be
different from those who do not). Despite rejecting the null
hypothesis, the magnitudes of relationships were often small.
Through supplementary analyses, Lambe et al. (2015) identi-
ed sex differences where the mediation of CDM between
conict and alcohol-related problems was signicant only for
women. Due to the relatively small sample size of men in the
current study, sex differences were not explored, although our
results did hold when sex was controlled in supplementary
analyses. Future research with a larger sample size and more
male participants might examine sex differences in friendship
dyads to determine if thesesex differences extend to other forms
of interpersonal conict. Statistical power for the within-
subjects path from dyadic conict to CAM was low; thus,
non-signicant results here may reect a Type II error. Data
were collected retrospectively at the end of every 30-day period.
Future studies may examine these variables daily via ecological
momentary assessment for more accurate in-time measurement,
as drinking to cope may occur relatively quickly following
conict. This study focused on the relationship between conict
and alcohol-related problems. It may be interesting to explore
the role of conict resolution, and whether this has any in-
uence on associated drinking motives (Rodriguez et al., 2019).
The study sample consisted of emerging adult undergraduate
friendship pairs, with 66.1% of the sample being women. The
ndings may not be generalizable to other samples (e.g., ad-
olescents, older friends, and opposite-sex friendships). At least
one member of the dyad was always a rst-year undergraduate
student; these students may be particularly vulnerable to
friendship conict due to the social difculties of transitioning
to university so these results may not generalize to all types of
friendships. Nonetheless, it is interesting that these ndings
occur in comparatively new friendships, which may be a more
socially tumultuous time as friends test new boundaries and
limits in their friendship. Finally, although existing theory
suggests that other drinking motives should be unrelated to
conict, future research may explore these other motives to
establish the specicity of the present ndings to the coping
motives.
Our analytic strategy has two notable limitations. First, our
use of a xed slopes, random intercepts model assumes
compound symmetry for the longitudinal correlated residuals,
which may be overly simplistic. Though more complex, other
analysts are developing approaches to incorporate AR(1)
Mackinnon et al. 605
correlated residual structures, which might t the data better
(Gistelinck & Loeys, 2019). Second, our use of log trans-
formations trades interpretability for robustness. That is, by
transforming the raw data, our residuals look closer to a
normal distribution and thus the estimates for p-values will
likely be more trustworthy. However, because coefcients are
difcult to interpret in the log scale, it is more difcult to
assess the magnitude of effects.
Conclusions
This study replicated and extended the work of Lambe et al.
(2015), conrming that in undergraduate friendships, the
relationship between friendship conict and alcohol-related
problems is mediated by CDM at the within-subjects level.
Further, the replication extended to CDM mediating friend-
ship conict and alcohol-related problems at the between-
subject level. Exploratory analyses also revealed indirect
partner effects where CDM in the friend lead to alcohol-related
problems in the individual by way of rst inuencing the
friends alcohol-related problems. Unique to our results was
the nding that in friendships, the relationship between
conict and alcohol-related problems is also mediated by
CAM at the within-subjects level. The ndings from this work
are important in contributing understanding of conict and
coping drinking motives to intervention efforts for preventing
and intervening with alcohol-related problems in emerging
adults.
Acknowledgments
Trevor Shannon, Brett Hopkins, Lauren Shenkar, Kyra Farrelly, Nacera
Hanzal, Jocelyn Brown, Kaitlin Coker, Sarah Wells, Pam Collins, and
Jennifer Swansburg are thanked for their research assistance.
Author Contributions
Mackinnon, S., contributed to conception and design, contributed to
analysis and interpretation, drafted manuscript, critically revised man-
uscript, gave nal approval, agrees to be accountable for all aspects of
work ensuring integrity and accuracy. Tougas, M., contributed to
conception, contributed to analysis and interpretation, drafted manu-
script, gave nal approval, agrees to be accountable for all aspects of
work ensuring integrity and accuracy. Kehayes, I-L., contributed to
conception and design, contributed to acquisition and interpretation,
critically revised manuscript, gave nal approval, agrees to be ac-
countable for all aspects of work ensuring integrity and accuracy.
Stewart, S., contributed to conception and design, contributed to ac-
quisition and interpretation, critically revised manuscript, gave nal
approval, agrees to be accountable for all aspects of work ensuring
integrity and accuracy.
Declaration of Conicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conicts of interest with respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following nancial support for
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This re-
search was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research
Council Award (4352015-1798).
ORCID iD
Michelle Tougas https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0015-6705
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material for this article is available online.
Open Practices.
The analysis code and materials used in this manuscript are openly
available (https://osf.io/krs3v/) The raw data contained in this
manuscript are not openly available due to privacy restrictions set
forth by the institutional research ethics board but can be obtained
from the corresponding author following the completion of a privacy
and fair use agreement. No aspects of the study were pre-registered.
Notes.
1. Data collection in this study continued until the timeline for our
funding ran out. However, we conducted a power simulation prior
to our analyses. Broadly, this analysis suggested that the sample size
was adequate to detect similarly-sized effect sizes to Lambe et al.
(2015). See the online supplementary materials for more details.
2. The full questionnaires for the materials listed here can be found at
https://osf.io/krs3v/. Other variables measured in the study are
available upon request.
3. When the log transformations are not used, the pattern of statistical
signicance and direction of effects noted in Figure 3 do not
change. However, the overall model t indices are considerably
poorer when the log transformations are not used: χ
2
(20) = 52.85,
p < .001; RMSEA = .05; SRMR (within) = .11; SRMR (between)
= .09, CFI = .94, TLI = .88. Simulation work by Gao et al. (2020)
suggests that, in the presence of non-normality, robust t indices
tend to erroneously reject models as tting poorly. Thus, we
believe the log transform is useful in the present case. The raw
output of this analysis available at https://osf.io/krs3v/.
4. In the current study, the RAPI assessed alcohol-related problems
over the past 30 days. In past studies (e.g., Lambe et al., 2015), the
RAPI has been used to assess a 1-week time frame. Due to these
differences in time frame, the means are not directly comparable,
and it was expected that the 30-day RAPI scores would be higher.
5. Readers should note that this analysis overlaps somewhat with
another paper that analyzes the same dataset (Kehayes et al.,
2021). Specically, they tested the bivariate relationships between
drinking motives and alcohol quantity and frequency separately.
Though these analyses differ slightly (i.e., using volume instead of
separate quantity/frequency indices; adding conict and both
motives as simultaneous mediators), readers interested in this topic
are encouraged to also refer to Kehayes et al. (2021) for further
analyses on alcohol consumption using this dataset.
606 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
References
Abbey, A., & Andrews, F. M. (1985). Modeling the psychological
determinants of life quality. Social Indicators Research,16(1),
134. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00317657
American College Health Association (2019). American College Health
association-National College Health assessment II: Reference
group executive summary spring 2019.https://www.acha.org/
documents/ncha/NCHAII_SPRING_2019_US_REFERENCE_
GROUP_EXECUTIVE_SUMMARY.pdf.
Arnett, J. J,
ˇ
Zukauskien˙
e, R., & Sugimura, K. (2014). The new life
stage of emerging adulthood at ages 1829 years: Implications
for mental health. The Lancet Psychiatry,1(7), 569576. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00080-7
Boman, J. H., Stogner, J., & Lee Miller, B (2013). Binge drinking,
marijuana use, and friendships: The relationship between similar
and dissimilar usage and friendship quality. Journal of Psy-
choactive Drugs,45(3), 218226. https://doi.org/10.1080/
02791072.2013.803646
Buote, V., Pancer, S., Pratt, M., Adams, G., Birnie-Lefcovitch, S.,
Polivy, J., & Wintre, M. (2007). The importance of friends:
Friendship and adjustment among 1st-year university students.
Journal of Adolescent Research,22(6), 665689. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0743558407306344
Camirand, E., & Poulin, F. (2019). Changes in best friend quality
between adolescence ad emerging adulthood: Considering the
role of romantic involvement. International Journal of Be-
havioral Development,43(3), 231237. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0165025418824995
Canadian Centre on Substance Use and Addiction (2017). Ca-
nadian drug summary: Alcohol.https://www.ccsa.ca/sites/
default/les/2019-04/CCSA-Canadian-Drug-Summary-Alcohol-
2017-en.pdf.
Chow, C. M., Tan, C. C., & Ruhl, H. (2015). Misery loves company: A
dyadic approach to examining the effects of depressive symptoms
on friendship discord. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
34(9), 774787. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.9.774
Collins, R. L., Kashdan, T. B., Koutsky, J. R., Morsheimer, E. T., &
Vetter, C. J. (2008). A self-administered Timeline Followback to
measure variations in underage drinkersalcohol intake and
binge drinking. Addictive Behaviors,33(1), 196200. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.07.001
Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adoles-
cents: Development and validation of a four-factor model.
Psychological Assessment,6(2), 117128. https://doi.org/10.
1037/1040-3590.6.2.117
Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P (1995).
Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A moti-
vational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,69(5), 9901005. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-
3514.69.5.990
Cooper, M. L., Kuntsche, E., Levitt, A., Barber, L., & Wolf, S (2016).
Motivational models of substance use: A review of theory and
research on motives for using alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco.
In K. Sher (Ed.), Oxford handbook of substance use disorders
(pp. 375421). Oxford Handbooks Online.
El-Sheikh, M., Keiley, M., Erath, S., & Dyer, W. J. (2013).
Marital conict and growth in childrens internalizing
symptoms: The role of autonomic nervous system activity.
Developmental Psychology,49(1), 92108. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0027703
Gao, C., Shi, D., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2020). Estimating the
maximum likelihood root mean square error of approximation
(RMSEA) with non-normal data: A Monte-Carlo study. Struc-
tural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal,27(2),
192201. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2019.1637741
Geldhof, G. J., Anthony, K. P., Selig, J. P., & Mendez-Luck, C. A.
(2018). Accommodating binary and count variables in media-
tion: A case for conditional indirect effects. International
Journal of Behavioral Development,42(2), 300308. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0165025417727876
Geldhof, G. J., Preacher, K. J., & Zyphur, M. J. (2014). Reliability
estimation in a multilevel conrmatory factor analysis frame-
work. Psychological Methods,19(1), 7291. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0032138
Gistelinck, F., & Loeys, T. (2019). The actorpartner interdepen-
dence model for longitudinal dyadic data: An implementation in
the SEM framework. Structural Equation Modeling,26(3),
329347. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2018.1527223
Grant, V. V., & Stewart, S. H (2007). Impact of experimentally in-
duced positive and anxious mood on alcohol expectancy strength
in internally motivated drinkers. Cognitive Behavior Therapy,
36(2), 102111. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506070701223289
Grant,V.V.,Stewart,S.H.,&Birch,C.D(2007).Impactofpositive
and anxious mood on implicit alcohol-related cognitions in
internally motivated undergraduate drinkers. Addictive Be-
haviors,32(10), 22262237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.
2007.02.012
Grant, V. V., Stewart, S. H., & Mohr, C. D. (2009). Coping-anxiety
and coping-depression motives predict different daily mood-
drinking relationships. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,
23(2), 226237. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015006
Grant, V. V., Stewart, S. H., OConnor, R. M., Blackwell, E., &
Conrod, P. J. (2007). Psychometric evaluation of the ve-factor
Modied Drinking Motives QuestionnaireRevised in under-
graduates. Addictive Behaviors,32(11), 26112632. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.07.004
Hamaker, E. L., & Muth´
en, B. (2020). The xed versus random
effects debate and how it relates to centering in multilevel
modeling. Psychological Methods,25(3), 365379. https://doi.
org/10.1037/met0000239
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for t indexes in
covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new
alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary
Journal,6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Ibarra-Rovillard, M. S., & Kuiper, N. A. (2011). Social support and
social negativity ndings in depression: Perceived responsive-
ness to basic psychological needs. Clinical Psychology Review,
31(3), 342352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.01.005
Kehayes, I. L., Mackinnon, S. P., Sherry, S. B., Leonard, K. E., &
Stewart, S. H. (2021). The inuence of drinking buddies: A
Mackinnon et al. 607
longitudinal investigation of drinking motivations and drinking
behaviors in emerging adults. Substance Use and Misuse,56(2),
286296. https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2020.1861631
Kenny, D. A., & Ledermann, T. (2010). Detecting, measuring, and
testing dyadic patterns in the actor-partner interdependence
model. Journal of Family Psychology,24(3), 359366. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0019651
Kim, A. J., Sherry, S. B., Shannon, R., Kehayes, I-L., & Stewart, S. H
(in press). A matter of perspective: The convergent and in-
cremental validity of informant-reported drinking motives.
Drug and Alcohol Review. Online ahead of print.
Kline, R. B (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation
modeling (4th ed.). The Guilford Press.
Kuntsche, E., & Stewart, S. H (2009). Why my classmates drink:
Drinking motives of classroom peers as predictors of individual
drinking motives and alcohol use in adolescence a mediational
model. Journal of Health Psychology,14(4), 536546. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1359105309103573
Lambe, L., Mackinnon, S. P., & Stewart, S. H. (2015). Dyadic
conict, drinking to cope, and alcohol-related problems: A
psychometric study and longitudinal actor-partner interdepen-
dence model. Journal of Family Psychology,29(5), 697707.
https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000098
Laursen, B., & Bukowski, W. M. (1997). A developmental guide to
the organisation of close relationships. International Journal of
Behavioral Development,21(4), 747770. https://doi.org/10.
1080/016502597384659
Lewis, M. A., Sheng, E., Geisner, I. M., Rhew, I. C., Patrick, M. E., &
Lee, C. M. (2015). Friend or foe: Personal use and friendsuse
of protective behavioral strategies and spring break drinking.
Addictive Behaviors,50,96101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
addbeh.2015.06.029.
Mackinnon, S. P., Sherry, S. B., Antony, M. M., Stewart, S. H.,
Sherry, D. L., & Hartling, N. (2012). Caught in a bad romance:
Perfectionism, conict, and depression in romantic relationships.
Journal of Family Psychology,26(2), 215225. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0027402
Martens, M. P., Neighbors, C., Dams-OConnor, K., Lee, C. M., &
Larimer, M. E. (2007). The factor structure of a dichotomously
scored Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index. Journal of Studies on
Alcohol and Drugs,68(4), 597606. https://doi.org/10.15288/
jsad.2007.68.597
McNamara Barry, C., Madsen, S. D., & DeGrace, A (2015). Growing
up with a little help from their friends in emerging adulthood. In
J. J. Arnett (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Emerging Adulthood.
Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/
9780199795574.013.008
McNamaraBarry,C.,Madsen,S.D.,Nelson,L.J.,Carroll,J.S.,&
Badger, S. (2009). Friendship and romantic relationship qualities in
emerging adulthood: Differential associations with identity de-
velopment and achieved adulthood. Journal of Adult Development,
16(4), 209222. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-009-9067-x
Murray, S. L., Grifn, D. W., Rose, P., & Bellavia, G. M. (2003).
Calibrating the sociometer: The relational contingencies of
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
85(1), 6384. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.63
Muyingo, L., Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., McEachern, E., Leonard,
K. E., & Stewart, S. H. (2020). Relationships on the rocks: A
meta-analysis of romantic partner effects on alcohol use. Psy-
chology of Addictive Behaviors,34(6), 629640. https://doi.org/
10.1037/adb0000578
Neal, D. J., Corbin, W. R., & Fromme, K (2006). Measurement of
alcohol-related consequences among high school and college
students: Application of item response models to the Rutgers
Alcohol Problem Index. Psychological Assessment,18(4),
402414. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.402
Nogueira-Arjona, R., Shannon, T., Kehayes, I. L., Sherry, S. B.,
Keough, M. T., & Stewart, S. H (2019). Drinking to keep pace:
A study of the moderating inuence of extraversion on alcohol
consumption similarity in drinking buddy dyads. Addictive Behav-
iors,92,6975. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.12.023
Oishi, S., & Sullivan, H. (2006). The predictive value of daily vs.
retrospective well-being judgments in relationship stability.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,42(4), 460470.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.07.001
Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z (2010). A general
multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation.
Psychological Methods,15(3), 209233. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0020141
Rodriguez, L. M., Dell, J. B., Lee, K. D. M., & Onufrak, J. (2019).
Effects of a brief cognitive reappraisal intervention on reduc-
tions in alcohol consumption and related problems. Psychology
of Addictive Behaviors,33(7), 637643. https://doi.org/10.
1037/adb0000509
Schwartz-Mette,R.A.,Lawrence,H.R.,&Harrington,R.V.
(2021). Transactional associations among adolescents
depressive symptoms and self-and friend-reported friend-
ship experiences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
chology,74, 101266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.
2021.101266.
Stavrakaki, C., & Vargo, B. (1986). The relationship of anxiety
and depression: A review of the literature. The British
JournalofPsychiatry,149(1), 716. https://doi.org/10.1192/
bjp.149.1.7
Stewart, S. H., Grant, V. V., Mackie, C. J., & Conrod, P. J (2016).
Comorbidity of anxiety and depression with substance use
disorders. In K J. Sher (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Sub-
stance use and substance use disorders, Volume 2 (pp.
149186). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/0.1093/
oxfordhb/9780199381708.013.19
Streja, E., Goldstein, L., Soohoo, M., Obi, Y., Kalantar-Zadeh, K., &
Rhee, C. M (2017). Modeling longitudinal data and its impact on
survival in observational nephrology studies: Tools and consid-
erations. Nephrology Dialysis Transplantation,32(suppl_2),
ii77ii83. https://doi.org/10.1093/ndt/gfx015
White, H. R., & Labouvie, E. W (1989). Towards the assessment of
adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcoholism,
50(1), 3037. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30
608 Emerging Adulthood 10(3)
... McDonald's ω = 0.64, and Cronbach's α = 0.69 (95% CI = 0.66-0.72), McDonald's ω = 0.71 at T2.The 7-item Partner-Specific Rejecting Behaviors Scale (60) was adapted to friendship by Mackinnon et al.(62). Examples of items are: "I get angry or irritated with my friends", "I am impulsive or selfish with my friends". ...
... Participants were asked to think about their closest friends and to rate items on a 5-point scale (from 1 = not at all, to 5 = completely true). This measure has been used in previous studies with adolescents and has demonstrated good psychometric qualities [e.g.,(62,63)]. In this sample, the scale confirmed an adequate longitudinal factorial structure (CFA: χ 2 = 507.397, ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Digital stress, resulting from expectations of online availability, can increase the risk of conflicts with friends. However, friendship conflict remains an underexplored indicator, particularly in association with stressful online experiences. This study aims to examine the association between digital stress and conflict levels overtime, considering the role of social media expectations. Method: 1185 adolescents (59.3% f, Mage = 15.97 years, SD = 1.43) completed self-report measures at two timepoints, six months apart. A Structural Equation Model was employed to examine the longitudinal associations of social media expectations (i.e., friends’ social media norms, friends’ social media use, and perceived social media features) on friendship conflict, focusing on the mediating role of digital stress (i.e., entrapment and disappointment). Gender differences were explored. Results: Perceived norms about social media use and unique features of social media (i.e., visualness) contributed to explain digital stress and, in turn, friendship conflict. Specifically, emotional responses to unmet expectations of availability on social media (i.e., disappointment) emerged as particularly relevant in explaining conflictual interactions, compared to the perceived pressure to be responsive to friends (i.e., entrapment). Males perceiving high availability of social media experience lower levels of entrapment, compared to females. Discussion: Results from this study support the importance of considering social media as a context where to study friendship dynamics, as this knowledge can have several implications for promoting positive online experiences and preventing conflicts with friends.
... According to this model, people higher in socially prescribed perfectionism perpetuate conflict and erode closeness in their relationships, thereby contributing to depressive symptoms (see Smith et al., 2020 for a review). An extension of this model includes maladaptive coping strategies and posits that when people higher in socially prescribed perfectionism experience various negative emotions, such as depressive affect, they engage in higher rates of self-defeating behaviors such as problematic drinking (Mackinnon et al., 2019(Mackinnon et al., , 2022. ...
... Data were drawn from a 4-wave, 4-month longitudinal dyadic study on close friendship dyads published elsewhere (Kehayes et al., 2021;Kim et al., 2022;Mackinnon et al., 2022;Nogueira-Arjona et al., 2019). Eligible participants were same-sex, non-romantic friendship dyads. ...
Article
Full-text available
Friendships are important for the mental well-being of emerging adults. Socially prescribed perfectionism, where individuals feel pressured to be perfect by others, can be destructive, leading to conflict with others, depressive symptoms, and problematic drinking. However, its impact on friendships is not well-explored. This study examined 174 emerging adult friendship dyads using a 4-wave, 4-month dyadic design. Data were analyzed using longitudinal actor-partner interdependence models. Using a novel friend-specific measure of socially prescribed perfectionism, we found that an individual's perceived expectation to be perfect from a friend was positively associated with increased conflict between friends, as well as with higher levels of depressive symptoms and problematic drinking in the individual. Findings lend credence to longstanding theoretical accounts and case histories suggesting socially prescribed perfectionism leads to harmful individual and relational outcomes and extends them to the specific context of friendships.
Article
Full-text available
The partner influence hypothesis postulates one partner's alcohol use influences the other partner’s alcohol use over time. While several studies have examined the partner influence hypothesis, the magnitude and gender-specific nature of partner influences on alcohol use are unclear and have yet to be examined meta-analytically. We addressed this by conducting a traditional bivariate meta-analysis and two-stage meta-analytic structural equation modelling (TS-MASEM) across 17 studies (N = 10,553 couples). Studies that assessed both romantic partners’ alcohol use at a minimum of two time-points were selected. Results suggest romantic partners do influence one another’s drinking, to a small but meaningful degree, with women (β = .19) exerting a statistically stronger (p < .05) influence than men (β = .12). Results also suggest time lag between assessment, alcohol indicator, married, and year of publication may moderate partner influence. Thus, social influences on individual alcohol use include important partner influences. These influences can serve either risk or protective functions. Given the economic, social, and health consequences associated with alcohol misuse, advancing knowledge of social risk factors for alcohol misuse is essential. Therefore, assessment and treatment of alcohol misuse should extend beyond the person to the social context. We encourage clinicians to consider involving romantic partners when assessing and treating alcohol misuse.
Article
Full-text available
Research has shown links between interpersonal conflict and problematic drinking behaviors as a way to cope. The present research examined the effects of a brief interpersonal conflict cognitive reappraisal intervention on short-term reductions in alcohol-related problems in a sample of college student drinkers. Undergraduates who were regular drinkers (N = 190) participated in a randomized control online study, completing self-reported measures of alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems at baseline and 2 weeks later. After completing the baseline survey, participants completed a brief writing intervention during which they were asked to reflect on a recent interpersonal conflict and write about it from 1 of 3 possible perspectives, 2 of which were targeting cognitive reappraisal (i.e., a neutral, third-party perspective and the other party's perspective), their own perspective, or to reflect on their activities that day (control). Results from negative binomial regression models supported both reappraisal conditions: Compared with control, those who thought about the conflict from a neutral third-party perspective and those who thought about the conflict from the other party's perspective reported significantly fewer drinking problems at follow-up. Results from this study suggest preliminary efficacy of a single-session writing intervention aimed at reappraising interpersonal conflict. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has provided formulae for estimating the maximum likelihood (ML) RMSEA when mean or mean and variance, corrections for non-normality are applied to the likelihood ratio test statistic. We investigate by simulation which choice of corrections provides most accurate point RMSEA estimates, confidence intervals, and p-values for a test of close fit under normality, and in the presence of non-normality. We found that, overall, any robust corrections (choices MLM, MLMV, and MLR) provide better results than ML, which assumes normality. When they err, all choices tend to suggest that the model fits more poorly than it really does. Choice MLMV (mean and variance corrections) provided the most accurate RMSEA estimates and p-values for tests of close fit results but its performance decreases as the number of variables being modeled increases.
Article
Full-text available
In many disciplines researchers use longitudinal panel data to investigate the potentially causal relationship between 2 variables. However, the conventions and concerns vary widely across disciplines. Here we focus on 2 concerns, that is: (a) the concern about random effects versus fixed effects, which is central in the (micro)econometrics/sociology literature; and (b) the concern about grand mean versus group (or person) mean centering, which is central in the multilevel literature associated with disciplines like psychology and educational sciences. We show that these 2 concerns are actually addressing the same underlying issue. We discuss diverse modeling methods based on either multilevel regression modeling with the data in long format, or structural equation modeling with the data in wide format, and compare these approaches with simulated data. We extend the multilevel model with random slopes and discuss the consequences of this. Subsequently, we provide guidelines on how to choose between the diverse modeling options. We illustrate the use of these guidelines with an empirical example based on intensive longitudinal data, in which we consider both a time-varying and a time-invariant covariate. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Changes in best friendship quality during adolescence coincide with the emergence of romantic relationships. This study aimed to examine the extent to which changes in friendship quality (intimacy, conflict, emotional support) between the ages of 16 and 22 varied according to four romantic involvement patterns (Late, Sporadic, Long-Term, Frequent) followed during this period. Gender was also included as a moderator. Participants (n ¼ 281; 61% girls) identified their romantic partners and reported on the quality of their relationship with their best friend each year. A series of multilevel growth curve models revealed that participants in the Late involvement group reported an increase in conflict with their best friend during this period; those in the Sporadic involvement and Frequent involvement groups reported increases in intimacy; and youths in the Long-Term involvement group reported no changes in friendship intimacy, conflict, or emotional support over time. Thus, among youths who reported changes in best friendship quality between adolescence and emerging adulthood, the observed changes appeared to vary according to the romantic involvement patterns followed during this period.
Article
Full-text available
In dyadic research, the actor–partner interdependence model (APIM) is widely used to model the effect of a predictor measured across dyad members on one’s own and one’s partner outcome. When such dyadic data are measured repeatedly over time, both the non-independence within couples and the non-independence over time need to be accounted for. In this paper, we present a longitudinal extension of the APIM, the L-APIM, that allows for both stable and time-varying sources of non-independence. Its implementation is readily available in multilevel software, such as proc mixed in SAS, but is lacking in the structural equation modeling (SEM) framework. We tackle the computational challenges associated with its SEM-implementation and propose a user-friendly free application for the L-APIM, which can be found at http://fgisteli.shinyapps.io/Shiny_LDD. As an illustration, we explore the actor and partner effects of positive relationship feelings on next day’s intimacy using 3-week diary data of 66 heterosexual couples.
Article
Introduction: Drinkers have social and affective reasons for using alcohol ('drinking motives'). Historically, drinking motives are self-reported. Informant-reports of drinking motives may be useful in corroborating self-report data. Thus, we investigated the correspondence between self- and informant-reports of drinking motives and the incremental validity of informant-reported motives in predicting targets' future alcohol problems. Methods: Measures were completed by 174 university-aged, same-sex drinking buddy dyads (66% women) across two waves separated by 30 days. Dyad members who contacted study organisers were treated as targets, and their buddies as informants. Targets self-reported their own drinking motives at baseline, as well as their own alcohol problems at baseline and 30 days later. Informants reported on targets' drinking motives at baseline. Results: Self- and informant-reports of targets' internal drinking motives (coping-depression and enhancement) showed significant, small positive correlations. Informants-reports of these same internal drinking motives (as well as coping-anxiety) predicted change in targets' alcohol problems over time, thereby providing additional predictive validity beyond that provided by targets' self-reports. Discussion and conclusions: We encourage incorporating informant-reported internal drinking motives when assessing risk for escalating problem drinking in emerging adult drinkers.
Article
Despite theoretical propositions regarding the transactional interplay between depressive symptoms and friendship experiences (e.g., Coyne, 1976), few studies have tested interpersonal theories of depression in adolescence. The current study used cross-lagged panel models to test longitudina, reciprocal associations of adolescents' depressive symptoms with self- and friend-reported positive friendship quality, conflict, and detachment over 3 time points spanning 6 months in a community sample (N = 186; 69.9% female; 87.6% White; Mage = 15.68 years). Gender and age differences were also considered. Results support theories positing both friendship- and symptom-driven change for associations between depressive symptoms and positive quality, whereas detachment predicted a cycle of increased depressive symptoms and later increased detachment. Moreover, only adolescents' (not friends') perceptions of friendship were linked with adolescents' depressive symptoms. Implications for future research on interpersonal theories of depression and for intervention efforts to reduce depressive symptoms and support adolescents' relationships are discussed.
Article
Background: Heavy alcohol consumption and frequent alcohol use are associated with many adverse social and physical consequences. The different motivations underlying why people drink predict different patterns of alcohol consumption. A drinking buddy (i.e. a friend with whom a person drinks alcohol) influences a person’s drinking via social learning, leading to escalations in drinking over time. Purpose: Few studies have investigated drinking motives among peers and none have studied whether the drinking motives of a drinking buddy can influence another person’s drinking behavior; we sought to fill that gap. Method: Same-sex drinking buddies (N = 174; 66.1% female) were assessed once monthly for four months using self-report questionnaires. Participants were on average 18.66 years-old (SD = 1.17). Results: Indistinguishable actor–partner interdependence models using multilevel path analysis were conducted, with each drinking motive predicting drinking frequency and quantity, respectively. There were significant actor effects for social, enhancement, conformity, and coping motives; moreover, the enhancement, social, and coping-anxiety motives of the drinking buddy influenced the individual’s drinking frequency across the four months of the study. Conversely, only the enhancement motives of the buddy predicted drinking quantity in the individual when averaged across time. Sex was not a significant moderator of these effects. Importance: When targeting risky drinking behavior in a therapeutic context, assessing and addressing a person’s reasons for drinking, as well as their drinking buddy’s reasons for drinking, may reduce the risk of escalations in either friend’s drinking frequency over time.
Article
Heavy drinking in college remains a concerning issue due to its association with both health and social risks. While modelling contributes to college students’ alcohol use, little work has identified who might be most susceptible to modelling effects. Peterson, Morey, and Higgins (2005) found males high in extraversion were more susceptible than others to matching strangers’ drinking levels in a lab-based social drinking context. We sought to replicate and extend these findings by examining the impact of extraversion on social matching of alcohol consumption levels of a drinking buddy in college students’ real lives. First, a significant relationship between buddy and target drinking levels was predicted in dyadic drinking situations. Additionally, we hypothesized that target extraversion would positively moderate this buddy- target drinking levels relationship. Data from 149 college student targets (74% F) and their same-sex drinking buddy were collected through online questionnaires examining targets’ extraversion levels, and the drinking levels and social drinking context of both dyad members through a 30-day Timeline Followback measure. Linear mixed-effects modelling confirmed the study's first social matching hypothesis, while also revealing that target extraversion positively moderated the relationship between buddy and target daily drinking levels in dyadic drinking contexts. Findings extend those of Peterson et al. (2005) to a real-world (vs. lab-based) context, modelling of a buddy's (vs. stranger's) drinking levels, and a sample including women (vs. all-male). Results provide novel information on extraversion's contributions to modelling of alcohol use that may guide useful modifications to personality-based interventions for reducing college student heavy drinking.