Content uploaded by Md. Shahinoor Rahman
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Md. Shahinoor Rahman on Oct 12, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Content uploaded by Md. Shahinoor Rahman
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Md. Shahinoor Rahman on Feb 22, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://dx.doi.org/10.7592/EJHR2022.10.3.597
The European Journal of
Humour Research 10 (3) 78–87
www.europeanjournalofhumour.org
The effect of laughter yoga on working memory:
A pilot study
Md. Shahinoor Rahman
University of Chittagong, Bangladesh
shahinpsy@cu.ac.bd
Farida Binte Wali
Government City College, Chittagong, Bangladesh
bintewali36@gmail.com
Abstract
A growing body of evidence suggests that there is a link between laughter and memory.
However, no research has been done to show a link between simulated laughter (laughter
yoga) and the enhancement of working memory. Because laughter has numerous benefits, we
examined whether simulated laughter can improve healthy adults’ working memory (WM). A
total of 30 participants (15 experimental and 15 control) were enrolled in this study. The
research design was experimental and pretest-posttest with a control group. Participants in
the laughter yoga intervention group had eight sessions twice a week for four weeks, whereas
the control group received no intervention. We assessed all participants before and after
laughter activity with the WM measures (Corsi Block Test and Digit Span). The laughter
intervention programme focused on simulated laughter (laughter yoga) without relying on
humour, jokes, or comedy. The results revealed a significant improvement in the memory of
both visual and verbal WM performances in the experimental group after the intervention
programme. In contrast, the study found no significant differences in the control group.
Simulated laughter intervention is the easiest, practical, and cost-efficient method that seems
to affect WM positively.
Keywords: laughter, working memory, visual-spatial working memory, simulated laughter.
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 79
1. Introduction
Laughter is a spontaneous psycho-physiological response to humour or any other favourable
external or internal stimuli (Mora-Ripoll, 2013) that has remarkable physiological and
psychological benefits (Yim, 2016; Yoshikawa et al., 2019). Simulated laughter is not the
same as spontaneous laughter, which was used in this study. Spontaneous laughter is triggered
by various (external) stimuli and positive emotion that is unrelated to one’s own free will.
Simulated laughter, on the other hand, is elicited at will (self-induced) for no specific reason
(purposeful, unconditional), and thus unaffected by humour, fun, or other stimuli or positive
emotions (Mora-Ripoll, 2011, 2013). Laughter yoga is a well-known form of simulated
laughter that was used in this study to enhance working memory performance (Bressington et
al., 2018).
Working memory (WM) is the ability to maintain and manipulate information (Baddeley
& Hitch, 1974; Alloway & Copello, 2013) which is essential for language comprehension,
reasoning, facilitates planning, solving problems, and learning. For academic success,
working memory becomes a more powerful predictor than IQ (Alloway & Alloway, 2010).
Thus, the improvement of working memory is essential. Alone with physiological and
psychological enhancement, laughter may improve cognitive function, specifically memory
(Mora-Ripoll, 2011). Thus, in this study, we assumed that laughter would improve working
memory.
Several studies have explored the enhancing effect of humour on cognitive performance,
in particular on memory. For example, Schmidt (1994) performed a series of experiments
using various humorous sentences in incidental and intentional learning and tested memory
with free and cued recall. The researcher found that humorous content has a memory benefit:
humorous sentences enhance sentence memory. In another study, Carlson (2011) investigated
the impact of humour on memory using humorous and inspirational materials (photographs,
keywords, phrases). The researcher found participants recall humorous photographs,
keywords, and phrases more than the inspirational group. Similarly, Badli and Dzulkifli
(2013) investigated the impact of humour on memory recall by showing participants funny
video clips with words. They found that humour improved memory recalls in both neutral and
dysphoric moods. The above findings indicated that humorous materials such as a word,
phrase, sentence, photograph, and video improve memory performance.
A large body of work showed that positive mood affects cognitive processes such as
memory. For example, Esmaeili et al. (2011) investigated the impact of a positive emotional
state (through a positive film) on undergraduate students’ working memory. They used a self-
report Working Memory Scale with two subscales: number-letter sequence and spatial span.
They found that positive emotion dramatically improved working memory performance
compared to the participants who watched a neutral movie. So, positively aroused films
improve working memory. Later, Storbeck and Maswood (2015) identified how mood affects
working memory using 5 minutes video clips (comedy, sad, neutral) and operation span tasks.
They found that positive affect (comedy) increased verbal and spatial working memory
capacity, whereas negative affect did not influence performance. Hence, the comedy clip
induced positive emotion that improved verbal and spatial working memory. Furthermore,
Palmiero et al. (2015) found that situational contexts that cause a particular mood (e.g.,
through using music) can influence visuospatial memory and navigational working memory in
college students using the Corsi Block-Tapping Test and the Walking Corsi Test. In another
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 80
similar study in 2016, Palmiero et al. found that only the positive emotion caused by music
affected both visuospatial and navigational working memory. Then, music that elicited
positive emotions was used to improve visuospatial and navigational working memory.
Costanzi et al. (2019) investigated the impact of incidentally acquired emotional stimuli on
visuospatial working memory on undergraduate students. They measured spatial working
memory using object relocation tasks, and they used emotional pictures to induce emotion.
They found that emotions enhance spatial memory performance when neutral and emotional
stimuli compete for entry into the working memory system. So, emotional pictures enhance
visuospatial working memory. All these findings revealed that positive mood produced by
video clips (emotional, comedy), music, or picture enhances working memory (verbal,
visuospatial, and navigational) performance.
A number of studies have examined whether laughter produces positive emotions. For
example, Keltner and Bonanno (1997) found that people with a high frequency of Duchenne
laughter (that involves the contraction of the muscles surrounding the eyes) had a higher level
of enjoyment than those with a low frequency of Duchenne laughter. So, laughter that
involved the contraction of muscles around the eyes produced enjoyment. Similarly,
Bachorowski and Owren (2001) investigated the influence of laughter (presence and absence
of voicing) in emotion. They found that only voiced laughs (laughs that engage the vocal
chords and have a sing-song-like sound quality) determined the pleasantness and the degree of
positive emotion instilled in others. So, laughter that involves vocal chords with a sound
produced pleasantness. Likewise, Foley et al. (2002) used forced laughter on 17 college
students in an experiment and found that 1 minute of forced laughter significantly increased
their positive emotion, even though the participants were in a good mood before the
experiment. Hence, 1 minute of forced laughter was able to produce positive emotion. Finally,
Armony et al. (2007) investigated the influence of emotional expression on memory for
vocalisation. They used emotional stimuli, such as 12 vocalisations of happiness (laughter)
and enjoyment, as well as a memory recognition task. Armony et al. (2007) found that
positive emotion enhances voice memory recognition performance. Then, laughter produced
happiness, and this positive emotion increased voice memory. The above clearly indicated
that laughter (Duchenne laughter, voiced laughter, and forced laughter) induces positive
emotion (enjoyment, pleasantness) and this positive emotion influences memory. So, laughter
influences memory.
Recently, a few researchers reported that laughter had an effect on the cardiovascular
function. Law et al. (2018) tested the cardiovascular effects of spontaneous and simulated
laughter. In different conditions, they showed a fake laughter/humour video/documentary to
72 participants. They discovered that laughing causes an increase in heart rate and a decrease
in heart rate variability, similar to exercise’s effects. This finding was more pronounced when
simulated laughter was used. So, simulated laughter impacted cardiovascular activity in a way
similar to exercise. Besides, Hötting et al. (2016) investigated the effects of a single bout of
cardiovascular exercise on memory consolidation in young adults. They used vocabulary to
test memory, where they discovered that participants in the high-intensity cardiovascular
exercise group forgot less vocabulary than those in the relaxing group. Thus, simulated
laughter impacts cardiovascular activity, and cardiovascular activity enhances memory.
So far, we have shown some of the contributions which are related to our current topic.
There have been a number of studies that have investigated the effectiveness of laughter on
cognitive functions. For example, Cueva et al. (2006) investigated how and in what respects
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 81
laughter impacts learning from the learners’ viewpoint. They demonstrated the importance
and usefulness of laughter in assisting people in their learning process, as 94% of respondents
thought that laughter was essential to support adult learning. So, the majority of learners
believed that laughter aided adult learning. In support of this claim, in behavioural research,
Bains et al. (2014) studied mirthful laughter and learning in three age-matched elderly groups
(elderly, diabetic, and control). They used the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test to measure:
(1) learning, (2) recall, and (3) visual recognition ability. They found that the elderly and
diabetic elderly groups who produced mirthful laughter (by watching a humorous video),
compared to the control group, showed more significant improvement in learning, recall, and
visual recognition abilities in short-term memory function. So, mirthful laughter has an
enhancing effect on short-term memory recall and visual recognition. In another study, Berk
et al. (2016) unveiled how mirthful laughter enhances cognitive functions by investigating
gamma-wave activity in the brain using EEG. They also used a video clip (10 minutes) to
produce laughter during the EEG recording. The Gamma Band Activity of Cerebral Cortex
regions (synchrony) of their findings revealed that mirthful laughter enhanced cognitive
processing, memory, and recall. Thus, mirthful laughter activated the same region responsible
for enhancing cognitive processing (memory and recall). Recently, Morishima et al. (2019)
showed that laughter had a short-term impact on cancer patients’ cognitive functioning, as
measured through a self-report Quality of Health Questionnaire Core 30 version. They found
that a 3-7-week laughter yoga intervention significantly improves cognitive functioning in
cancer patients relative to baseline during the initial phase compared to the control group. So,
simulated laughter (laughter yoga) enhances cognitive functions. These findings are backed
with the learner’s belief that laughter (mirthful, simulated) had a strong physiological basis
for enhancing cognitive performance (learning, short-term memory, and visual recognition).
All these data led to the realisation that humorous materials (such as words, phrases,
sentences, photographs, or videos) improve memory performance. Next, it follows that
laughter (Duchenne/voiced/forced) improves positive emotion (enjoyment/pleasantness),
whereas positive emotion (induced by video clips, music, or pictures) improves working
memory (verbal/visuospatial/navigational) performance. Finally, laughter, either mirthful or
simulated, directly enhances cognitive performance, such as learning, short-term memory, and
visual recognition. However, no research has so far indicated that simulated laughter (laughter
yoga) improves working memory. Therefore, the current study aims to examine the effect of
laughter yoga on the WM of healthy adult participants. The outcomes of this research, as a
pilot study, have a potential to inform larger investigations by establishing a foundation for
understanding how simulated laughter affects working memory. Based on earlier research, we
formed the following hypotheses:
H1: Simulated laughter would result in significantly greater improvement in visual WM
test compared to the control group.
H2: Simulated laughter would result in a more significant improvement in verbal WM test
compared to the control group.
2. Methods
2.1 Participants
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 82
A total of 30 (m = 10, f = 20) participants were included in this study. We recruited 5 males for
the experimental group and 5 males for the control group from 10 males. Similarly, from 20
females, 10 were assigned to the experimental group and 10 to the control group. Their
average age was 20.57 (SD = 1.25), and they had normal vision with no uncontrolled medical
conditions, such as hypertension, chronic coughs, acute low back pain, and consumption of any
antipsychotic drugs. They also had no history of any surgery in the past three months. Participants
were randomly assigned either to the experimental group (n = 15) or the control (non-
intervention) group (n = 15).
2.2 Materials
The computerised version of the Corsi Block Test and Digit Span test were used in this
investigation. Mueller and Piper (2014) developed these instruments, which began with a
demonstration trial to familiarise participants with the procedure. The demonstration trial was
identical to the test trials.
2.2.1 Corsi Block Test
The Corsi Block Test (CBT) measures visual-spatial working memory. It assesses the ability
to recall a series of screen locations. Participants saw nine blue squares on the screen during
each CBT trial. The squares were lit up one by one in order. The participants were asked to
recall the series. When it was finished, the participants’ task was to click on each square in the
given order. The sequence began with two squares and increased by one after another as
participants correctly completed the minimum one of the two sequences.
2.2.2 Digit Span
Digit Span is a test of verbal working memory. It also assesses numerical processing ability
and short-term memory. On each trial, participants were shown a series of digits one by one
on the screen and asked to type the list of digits exactly in the order in which they saw them.
It began with a list of three items, and each length was made up of three different lists. When
participants correctly recalled two out of three lists, they were given access to the extended
list length. For this test, the correlation between the test and the retest was moderate (r=.63)
(Piper et al., 2015).
2.2.3 Simulated laughter
The experimental group was subjected to 30 minutes of simulated laughter. The study was
carried out in a group setting, and we followed the standard structure of a laughter yoga
session, which includes the repetition of three key components: clap, breathe, and laugh. After
a brief explanation of the laughter activity, the participants practised simulated laughter
(beaming all over their faces) for the first 10 minutes: laughing with eye contact, clapping
hands parallel to each other so that their palms and fingers were thoroughly clapped, and
chanting “ho, ho, ha-ha-ha”. The ‘breathe’ phase followed, in which participants focused on
relaxing and regulating their breathing. During the 15-minute ‘laugh’ phase, the researcher
guided participants through the activities designed to simulate laughter. We used the
following exercises to make people laugh: greeting laughter (salam and handshake laughter);
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 83
lion laughter; telephone laughter; airport and flight laughter; hot soup laughter; bill laughter;
and no money laughter. Although the duration of the laughter exercises and the time spent in
each phase vary from session to session, the ‘laugh, clap, breath’ structure remains consistent
across all sessions. The final 5 minutes were spent on body relaxation and deep breathing.
2.3. Procedure
This study primarily enlisted the participation of 50 undergraduate students. However, only 30
people met the inclusion criteria and were therefore included in this study. Healthy vision, no
uncontrolled medical conditions (e.g., hypertension, chronic coughs, acute low back pain, and
antipsychotic drug use), and no history of surgery in the previous three months were all inclusion
criteria. The participants were then randomly assigned to one of two groups: experimental or
control (non-intervention). After completing consent forms and a demographic information form,
participants were asked to attend one of their weekly sessions. The study’s purpose was explained
to them, and their anonymity was ensured. The measurements were taken the day before the first
laughter session and the day after the last session. The participants went through a quick practice
session prior to the measurements. The experimental group received simulated laughter (laughter
yoga) training twice a week for four weeks, whereas the control group remained passive, that is,
they received no intervention. Each session lasted 30 minutes and was delivered in a total of eight
sessions.
3. Results
To examine the potential effects of the simulated laughter programme, we used Paired
Samples t-test. Participants who participated in the simulated laughter activity obtained higher
scores on the CBT from pre to post than controls. The experimental group’s t-test results
show a significant improvement in visual WM for the post-test (M = 5.43, SD = 0.59) over
pre-test (M = 4.83, SD = 0.56) measure t (14) = -4.05, p < .01. The size of the effect was
determined to be significant. The control group, on the other hand, showed no significant
difference for the post-test (M=5.40, SD=.73) over pre-test (M=4.93, SD=.90;) measure t (14)
= -1.74, p >.05.
Similarly, individuals in the simulated laughter activity had higher scores in the digit span
test from pre to post than controls. The results also showed a substantial improvement in
verbal WM for the post-test (M= 7.60, SD= 1.59) compared to the pre-test (M= 6.33, SD=
1.54), t(14)= -3.02, p < .01. The size of the effect was determined to be significant. The
control group, on the other hand, demonstrated no significant difference in verbal WM for the
post-test (M = 6.93, SD = 1.53) over pre-test (M = 6.87, SD = 1.77) measures t(14) = -0.16, p
>.05.
Table 1. Pre-test and post-test measures of Corsi Block Test (CBT) and Digit Span test score
for the experimental and control groups.
Variable
Group
Pre-intervention
Post-intervention
t
Effect
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 84
M (SD)
M (SD)
size
(d)
CBT
Experimental
4.83 (0.56)
5.43 (0.59)
-4.05*
-1.04
Control
4.93 (0.90)
5.40(0.73)
-1.74
Digit Span
Experimental
6.33 (1.54)
7.60 (1.59)
-3.02*
-.81
Control
6.87 (1.77)
6.93(1.53)
-.16
*p < .01
4. Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine the effectiveness of simulated laughter intervention
on both visual and verbal WM in healthy adults. The broader goal was to advance our
understanding of the potential benefits of laughter practise among adults. We expected that
simulated laughter would significantly improve visual WM and verbal WM compared to the
control group. To our knowledge, this is the first study that identifies the incremental effect of
simulated laughter on working memory.
In our first hypothesis, we expected that simulated laughter would significantly improve
visual WM. Our results revealed a significant increase in visual WM for participants in the
laughter group, whereas those in the control group did not reveal significant changes. These
findings supported our first hypothesis and suggested that simulated laughter effectively
improves visual working memory. This outcome is consistent with the findings of Bains et al.
(2014) who showed that mirthful laughter increases visual recognition but did not show an
effect on visual working memory. The possible explanation of our findings is compatible with
the results of Foley et al. (2002) and Palmiero et al. (2015) that simulated laughter increases
positive emotion. Positive emotion plays a role in enhancing visuospatial working memory.
In our second hypothesis, we anticipated that simulated laughter would improve verbal
WM significantly. When compared to the control group, participants in the laughter group
showed a significant increase in verbal WM. As a result, our second hypothesis was also
supported by our findings, indicating that simulated laughter can successfully increase verbal
WM. Our finding is in line with the findings of Berk et al. (2016) who showed that mirthful
laughter increases memory. The reason behind this increment of verbal memory may be
emotion. We know that voice laughter or simulated laughter produces positive emotion (Foley
et al., 2002), and this positive emotion (Armony et al., 2007) may lead to increased verbal
working memory in our study.
Both findings of this study were consistent with Morishima et al. (2019) who found that
simulated laughter (i.e., laughter yoga) enhances cognitive performance. WM is one of the
components of cognitive processing according to the information processing model.
Moreover, our findings also are explained by the information processing theory, according to
which, when we selectively attend to certain information, it is kept in our working memory
and processed for later use. This process may be the cause of the increase in working
memory. Another possible explanation for our findings is that simulated laughter (laughter
yoga) increases cardiovascular activity, causing a rise in heart rate and a decrease in heart rate
variability (Law et al., 2018), and this cardiovascular activity improves memory (Hötting et
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 85
al., 2016). In this way, laughter yoga may help to improve working memory.
5. Conclusion
We may train our bodies and minds to laugh at will. In this preliminary study, eight weeks of
simulated laughter (laughter yoga) successfully boosted participants’ visual and verbal
working memory. According to these findings, young individuals can benefit from merely
laughing in their daily lives. More precisely, laughter yoga may aid tertiary-level students
who are performing poorly due to poor working memory. Furthermore, laughter yoga could
be a novel and simple method for working memory training. It makes no difference if our
laughter is artificial or genuine; laughing helps us improve our visual and verbal working
memory.
5.1. Limitations and future directions
There were a few limitations in this preliminary study. First of all, the small number of
participants at the outset is one of our limitations. In the future, a larger number of
participants may be included to replicate and strengthen the findings. Secondly, one of the
drawbacks was that we used a passive control group rather than an active control group in this
experiment. Next, the fact that we did not include a follow-up condition was another
limitation. In the future, a follow-up study could be considered to investigate how long the
positive effect of simulated laughter on WM lasts. Finally, the participants were all young
adults in their twenties. In the future, one could investigate the laughter yoga effect on WM
considering different age groups (i.e., children and senior citizens).
References
Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). 'Investigating the predictive roles of working
memory and IQ in academic attainment'. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
106 (1), pp. 20-29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003
Alloway, T. P. & Copello, E. (2013). 'Working memory: The what, the why, and the how'.
The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist 30 (2), pp. 105–118. doi:
10.1017/edp.2013.13
Armony, J. L., Chochol, C., Fecteau, S. & Belin, P. (2007). 'Laugh (or cry) and you will be
remembered: influence of emotional expression on memory for vocalizations'.
Psychological Science 18 (12), pp. 1027-1029. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2007.02019.x.
Bachorowski, J.-A. & Owren, M. J. (2001). 'Not all laughs are alike: voiced but not unvoiced
laughter readily elicits positive affect'. Psychological Science 12 (3), pp. 252-257. doi:
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00346
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. (1974). 'Working memory', in G.H. Bower (ed.), The
Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. New
York: Academic Press, pp. 47-89.
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 86
Badli, T. S.-Z. T. & Dzulkifli, M. A. (2013). 'The effect of humour and mood on memory
recall'. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 97, pp. 252-257. doi:
10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.10.230
Bains, G. S., Berk, L. S., Daher, N., Lohman, E., Schwab, E., Petrofsky, J. & Deshpande, P.
(2014). 'The effect of humor on short-term memory in older adults: a new component
for whole-person wellness;. Adv Mind Body Med 28 (2), pp. 16-24.
Berk, L., Lee, J., Mali, D., Lohman, E., Bains, G., Daher, N., . . . Shah, P. (2016). 'Humor
associated mirthful laughter increases the intensity of power spectral density (μV2)
EEG gamma wave band frequency (31–40Hz) which is associated with neuronal
synchronization, memory, recall, enhanced cognitive processing and other brain health
benefits when compared to distress'. The FASEB Journal 30 (S1), 1284.9-1284.9. doi:
https://doi.org/10.1096/fasebj.30.1_supplement.1284.9
Bressington, D., Yu, C., Wong, W., Ng, T. C., & Chien, W. T. (2018). 'The effects of group-
based Laughter Yoga interventions on mental health in adults: A systematic review'.
Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 25 (8), pp. 517-527. doi:
10.1111/jpm.12491
Carlson, K. A. (2011). 'The impact of humor on memory: Is the humor effect about humor?'
Humor - International Journal of Humor Research 24 (1), pp. 21-41. doi:
10.1515/humr.2011.002.
Costanzi, M., Cianfanelli, B., Saraulli, D., Lasaponara, S., Doricchi, F., Cestari, V. & Rossi-
Arnaud, C. (2019). 'The effect of emotional valence and arousal on visuo-spatial
working memory: incidental emotional learning and memory for object-location'.
Frontiers in Psychology 10 (2587). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02587
.Cueva, M., Kuhnley, R., Lanier, A. & Dignan, M. (2006). 'Healing hearts: laughter and
learning'. Journal of Cancer Education 21 (2), pp. 104-107. doi:
10.1207/s15430154jce2102_14.
Esmaeili, M. T., Karimi, M., Tabatabaie, K. R., Moradi, A. & Farahini, N. (2011). 'The effect
of positive arousal on working memory'. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences
30, pp. 1457-1460. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.282.
Foley, E., Matheis, R. & Schaefer, C. (2002). 'Effect of forced laughter on mood'. Psychol
Rep, 90 (1), p. 184. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2002.90.1.184.
Hötting, K., Schickert, N., Kaiser, J., Röder, B. & Schmidt-Kassow, M. (2016). 'The effects of
acute physical exercise on memory. Peripheral BDNF, and cortisol in young adults'.
Neural Plasticity 2016:6860573. doi: 10.1155/2016/6860573.
Keltner, D. & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). 'A study of laughter and dissociation: distinct correlates
of laughter and smiling during bereavement'. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 73 (4), pp. 687-702. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.687.
Law, M. M., Broadbent, E. A. & Sollers, J. J. (2018). 'A comparison of the cardiovascular
effects of simulated and spontaneous laughter'. Complementary Therapies in Medicine
37, pp. 103-109. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2018.02.005
Mora-Ripoll, R. (2011). 'Potential health benefits of simulated laughter: a narrative review of
the literature and recommendations for future research'. Complementary Therapies in
Medicine 19 (3), pp. 170-177. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2011.05.003
Mora-Ripoll, R. (2013). 'Laughter techniques for therapeutic use in medicine'. OA Alternative
Medicine 1 (3), p. 25.
The European Journal of Humour Research 10 (3)
Open-access journal | www.europeanjournalofhumour.org 87
Morishima, T., Miyashiro, I., Inoue, N., Kitasaka, M., Akazawa, T., Higeno, A., . . .
Matsuura, N. (2019). 'Effects of laughter therapy on quality of life in patients with
cancer: an open-label, randomized controlled trial'. PLoS One 14 (6), e0219065. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0219065
Mueller, S. T. & Piper, B. J. (2014). 'The psychology experiment building language (PEBL)
and PEBL Test Battery'. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 222, pp. 250-259.
Palmiero, M., Nori, R., Rogolino, C., D'Amico, S. & Piccardi, L. (2015). 'Situated
navigational working memory: the role of positive mood'. Cogn Process 16 (Suppl 1),
S327-S330. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0670-4
Palmiero, M., Nori, R., Rogolino, C., D'Amico, S. & Piccardi, L. (2016). 'Sex differences in
visuospatial and navigational working memory: the role of mood induced by
background music'. Exp Brain Res 234 (8), pp. 2381-2389. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-
4643-3
Piper, B. J., Mueller, S. T., Geerken, A. R., Dixon, K. L., Kroliczak, G., Olsen, R. H. J. &
Miller, J. K. (2015). 'Reliability and validity of neurobehavioral function on the
Psychology Experimental Building Language test battery in young adults'. PeerJ 3,
e1460. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1460
Schmidt, S. R. (1994). 'Effects of humor on sentence memory'. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20 (4), pp. 953-967. doi:
doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.953
Storbeck, J. & Maswood, R. (2015). 'Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory
capacity where sadness does not: emotion, working memory and executive control'.
Cognition & Emotion 30, pp. 1–14. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1034091
Yim, J. (2016). 'Therapeutic benefits of laughter in mental health: a theoretical review'.
Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine 239 (3), pp. 243-249. doi:
10.1620/tjem.239.243.
Yoshikawa, Y., Ohmaki, E., Kawahata, H., Maekawa, Y., Ogihara, T., Morishita, R. & Aoki,
M. (2019). 'Beneficial effect of laughter therapy on physiological and psychological
function in elders'. Nursing Open 6 (1), pp. 93–99. doi: 10.1002/nop2.190