ArticlePDF Available

Moderating effects of brooding on the link between functional impairment and interpersonal needs in survivors of serious trauma

Authors:
  • Portland Psychotherapy--Clinic, Research, and Training Center

Abstract

Objectives: Trauma and resulting functional limitations demonstrate associations with perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, factors contributing to elevated risk for suicidal ideation. However, survivors display differential risk in response to impairment, highlighting the need for research on exacerbating factors. The current study examined the impact of brooding on the association of functional impairment with burdensomeness and belongingness among trauma-exposed undergraduates (N = 262). Method: Trauma was assessed via clinical interview with questionnaires for study variables. Regression models examined the unique and interactive effects of physical impairment, emotional impairment, and brooding on burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness. Results: An interaction of brooding and impairment due to emotional difficulties was observed for burdensomeness with impairment linked to elevated burdensomeness at high (β = -0.46; p < 0.001), but not low (β = -0.07; p = 0.476) brooding. Impairment due to emotional difficulties (β = -0.38; p < 0.001) and brooding (β = 0.25; p < 0.001) were associated with belongingness. Conclusions: Findings identify brooding as a potential target for assessment and intervention in trauma-exposed individuals.
Running Head: FUNCTIONAL IMPAIRMENT AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 1
Moderating Effects of Brooding on the Link Between Functional Impairment and
Interpersonal Needs in Survivors of Serious Trauma
Kozina, R. M.*, Lear, M. K., Stacy, S. E., Kern, S. M., Ripley, A. J., Clapp, J. D.
University of Wyoming
Department of Psychology
1000 E. University Ave.
Laramie, WY 82071
United States
Correspondence regarding this manuscript should be directed to Ryan M. Kozina at:
Department of Psychology
University of Wyoming
1000 E. University Avenue
Laramie, WY 82070
Phone: (916) 529-9942
Fax: 307.766.2926
email: rkozina@uwyo.edu
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 2
Abstract
Objectives: Trauma exposure is associated functional impairment in response to both physical
and emotional difficulties. Limitations in these domains have been subsequently shown to hold
relations with perceptions of burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, factors contributing
to further negative outcomes including suicidal ideation. However, not all survivors demonstrate
equal levels of risk in the face of common impairment, highlighting the need for research on
possible exacerbating factors. The current study examined the potential impact of brooding on
the association of physical and emotional impairment with burdensomeness and thwarted
belongingness among trauma-exposed undergraduates (N = 262).
Method: Trauma exposure was assessed via clinical interview with questionnaire measures
completed for study variables. Regression models examined the unique and interactive effects of
physical impairment, emotional impairment, and brooding on burdensomeness and thwarted
belongingness.
Results: An interaction of brooding and impairment due to emotional difficulties was observed
for burdensomeness with impairment linked to elevated burdensomeness at high (β = -.46; p
<.001), but not low (β = -.07; p = .476) levels of brooding. Impairment due to emotional
difficulties (β = -.38; p <.001) and brooding (β = .25; p <.001) were associated with thwarted
belongingness.
Conclusions: Findings identify brooding as a potential target for assessment and intervention in
trauma-exposed individuals.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 3
Moderating Effects of Brooding on the Link Between Functional Impairment and
Interpersonal Needs in Survivors of Serious Trauma
Individuals exposed to traumatic events are at increased risk for a range of negative
outcomes including psychopathology, physical injury, chronic health issues, and elevated rates of
self-harm (e.g., Atwoli et al., 2016; Fullerton & Ursano, 2005; Goldney et al., 2000). Survivors
may also experience considerable levels of functional impairment due to corresponding
emotional and physical concerns (e.g., Clapp et al., 2010; Momartin et al., 2003). Current
research indicates that impairment related to physical and emotional difficulties may increase
survivors’ perceptions of social isolation and being a burden to others, outcomes known to
increase risk of suicidal ideation (Calandre et al., 2021; Khazem et al., 2015; Wiblin et al., 2021).
However, few studies have examined specific factors that may influence the magnitude of these
effects. Rumination, an emotion regulation strategy associated with elevated mental health
difficulties, may heighten perceptions of burdensomeness and isolation following trauma. The
present study builds on existing research by examining rumination as a potential exacerbating
factor in the relation of physical and emotional impairment with burdensomeness and thwarted
belongingness.
Exposure to traumatic events involving actual or threatened death, serious injury, and/or
sexual violence (APA, 2013) is linked to deleterious effects on both physical and emotional
health (Wen et al., 2012; Fullerton & Ursano, 2005). In addition to difficulties resulting from
acute physical injury (e.g., Galarneau et al., 2006), data suggests that individuals exposed to
significant trauma are more likely than non-exposed individuals to develop chronic physical
conditions contributing to long-term functional impairment (e.g., Atwoli et al., 2016; Schnurr &
Green, 2004; Wen et al., 2012). For example, survivors of military sexual trauma have been
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 4
found to report worse overall health outcomes and more frequent somatic complaints than
personnel who were not victimized (Millegan et al., 2016). Similarly, research exploring the
long-term health effects of earthquake survivors suggests that those most impacted by the quake
report more physical conditions and resulting impairment than those living outside the disaster
area (Wen et al., 2012).
In addition to increased risk for medical and health-related sequelae, trauma exposure
holds well-established associations with compromised emotional functioning (Kilpatrick et al.,
2003; Fullerton & Ursano, 2005). In a nationally representative sample of adolescents, exposure
to potentially traumatic events was linked to symptoms of depression and posttraumatic stress
disorder (PTSD), conditions commonly associated with functional impairment (Kilpatrick et al.,
2003). Increased rates of emotional difficulties including anxiety, depression, PTSD, and other
forms of psychopathology have been noted across survivors of motor vehicle trauma (e.g.,
Medetsky & Parnes, 1993), sexual assault (e.g., Millegan et al., 2016), combat (e.g., Dedert et
al., 2009), and intimate partner violence (e.g., Ogunsiji & Clisdell, 2017).
Elevations in the prevalence of suicidal ideation and self-harm are also noted in trauma-
exposed populations (e.g., LeBouthillier et al., 2015; Bryan et al., 2010). Joiner’s (2005)
Interpersonal Psychological Theory of Suicide (IPTS), an influential model developed to account
for specific relations between suicidal thoughts and subsequent behavior, identifies unmet
interpersonal needs as a primary factor in the development of suicidal ideation. Specific
interpersonal needs in Joiner’s model include perceptions of burdensomeness and thwarted
belongingness. Burdensomeness refers to the belief that one is an encumbrance on support
members and/or loved ones, resulting in self-hatred, perceptions that the individual is a liability
to others, and beliefs that friends and family would be “better off” without them. Thwarted
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 5
belongingness - a sense of alienation from friends, family, and other important social groups -
involves feelings of loneliness and the belief that the individual is left without resources for
support (Van Orden et al., 2010). Research has indicated that perceived burdensomeness and
thwarted belongingness are individually sufficient for passive suicidal ideation (Van Orden et al.,
2010), with recent meta-analyses providing evidence for robust associations with thoughts of
suicide (Chu et al., 2017). Results suggest that the evaluation of factors contributing to unmet
interpersonal needs is relevant for clinical risk assessment and treatment as well as for clarifying
the theoretical underpinnings of suicide more generally.
Within the context of trauma exposure, individuals experiencing functional impairment
as a result of chronic physical and/or emotional difficulties may experience greater perceptions
of burdensomeness due to an increased reliance on others for emotional, functional, and/or
financial support (e.g., disability due to chronic pain or travel avoidance following a serious
motor vehicle accident; Blanchard & Hickling, 1997). Survivors may also develop feelings of
isolation if participation in social activities is restricted as a consequence of emotional and/or
health-related impairments (e.g., changes in social relations as a result of stigma or emotional
lability following sexual assault; Ullman, 1999). Consistent with these hypotheses, data from the
extant literature provide evidence of elevated burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness
across a number of clinical populations. Wiblin and colleagues (2021) found that functional
impairment as a result of mental health difficulties were associated with greater perceptions of
unlovability among male and female survivors of military sexual assault. Similarly, Khazem et
al. (2015) observed elevations in burdensomeness among undergraduates reporting histories of
physical disability as compared to those without. Examination of impairment as a result of
fibromyalgia (Calandre et al., 2021), chronic pain (Hirsch et al., 2016), and general mental health
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 6
difficulties (Bryan, 2011) provide additional evidence for associations with both perceived
burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness.
Despite evidence for associations with these outcomes, the majority of trauma survivors
who develop limitations due to physical and/or emotional difficulties will show resilience to
ideation and self-harm, suggesting that an assessment of factors that impact these relations is
warranted. Cognitive processes including rumination are known to exacerbate risk for a range of
negative outcomes in the general population. (e.g., Kirkegaard Thomsen, 2006, Smith & Alloy,
2009). Brooding, a distinct component of rumination, is conceptualized as an inclination to
reflect judgmentally on one’s situation, feelings, or past actions with an emphasis on barriers to
solving problems (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Treynor et al., 2003). Brooding has been shown
to impact problem solving ability (Haigh et al., 2018) and is believed to contribute to difficulties
related to depression, anxiety, and substance use (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Xavier et al.,
2016). It is also possible that brooding may play a role in strengthening the relation between
functional impairment and the development and/or exacerbation of thwarted interpersonal needs
in individuals exposed to significant trauma. Specifically, survivors with a predisposition for
brooding appraisals are likely to focus on the consequences of trauma as well as current physical
and emotional limitations in ways that do not facilitate growth or practical, value-driven
solutions. This approach to processing the experience may, in turn, strengthen associations
between increasing functional impairment and perceptions of burdensomeness and thwarted
belonging. Research evaluating the impact of coping strategies on interpersonal difficulties has
noted associations between rumination and reductions in relationship satisfaction (Pearson et al.,
2010), intimacy, and sociability (Lam et al., 2003). However, no studies to date have tested the
specific interrelations between brooding, physical and emotional limitations, and interpersonal
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 7
needs in trauma survivors as indexed by measures of perceived burdensomeness and thwarted
belongingness.
Aims of the current study were to examine the extent to which brooding may strengthen
the associations of physical and emotional impairment with indices of burdensomeness and
thwarted belongingness in individuals exposed to significant trauma. Participants included 262
university students involved in an ongoing study of functional outcomes following Criterion-A
exposure (APA, 2013). Regression models were used to evaluate the direct and interactive
effects of brooding and functional impairment due to physical and emotional difficulties on
perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness. Based on existing theoretical models
(Joiner, 2005; Van Orden et al., 2010) and evidence of rumination as a potential exacerbating
factor for negative behavioral outcomes (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Smith & Alloy, 2009),
indices of functional impairment were expected to demonstrate stronger relations with perceived
burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness in survivors reporting high versus low levels of
brooding.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Participants included university students enrolled in an ongoing project evaluating
functional outcomes following significant trauma. Students endorsing exposure to one or more
potentially traumatic events on a checklist administered in an initial mass testing protocol were
invited to participate in the larger study (reference omitted for anonymous review). Following the
provision of informed consent, participants completed a series of structured and semi-structured
interviews with a graduate- or doctoral-level clinician. Interviews were used to determine the
frequency and severity of Criterion-A exposure as well as the presence of trauma-related
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 8
psychopathology and potential comorbid disorders. Following interviews, participants completed
a series of online questionnaires targeting domains of psychosocial functioning. Participants
received course credit for their involvement in the project. All study procedures received
Institutional Review Board approval.
A total of 271 individuals completed the initial clinical interview. Two participants failed
to report an event consistent with Criterion-A trauma as outlined in DSM-5 and were excluded
from subsequent analyses. Four individuals were excluded due to missing data. Three additional
cases were identified as multivariate outliers during tests of underlying model assumptions.
Subsequent screening failed to identify further violations of normality, linearity,
multicollinearity, or homoscedasticity. Characteristics of the final sample (N = 262) are provided
in Table 1.
Measures
Medical Outcomes Study Short Form 36 (SF-36). The SF-36 (Ware & Sherbourne,
1992) is a self-report health status questionnaire designed to measure health-related outcomes in
the four weeks prior to administration. For the current study, role limitations due to physical
problems (Role-Physical) and role limitations due to emotional problems (Role-Emotional) were
used as indices of domain-specific impairment. Items from the SF-36 Role-Physical specifically
target difficulties with work and/or other daily activities (e.g., reducing the amount of time spent
on activities, accomplishing less than intended, difficulty performing certain tasks) as a result of
physical health symptoms. Role-Emotional items addressing similar impairment in response to
emotional concerns. Both metrics are scored on a 0 to 100 scale with higher values representing
increased functioning in physical and emotional domains. The psychometric properties of the
SF-36 are well-established in university, medical, and psychiatric samples (Burton et al., 2012;
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 9
DeBerard & Masters, 2014; McHorney et al., 1993). Internal consistencies for this sample were
excellent for Role-Physical (α = 0.80) and Role-Emotional (α = 0.82) scales.
Ruminative Response Scale—Brooding Scale (RRS-B). The RRS-B (Treynor et al.,
2003) is a self-report measure developed to assess trait-level rumination. For the current study,
the 5-item Brooding subscale was used to index respondent tendency to passively self-reflect on
circumstances in ways that augment negative mood. RRS-B items (e.g., What am I doing to
deserve this; Why do I have problems that other people don’t have) are rated on a 1 (Almost
Never) to 4 (Always) Likert scale. Responses are summed to create a total score with higher
values indicating more frequent brooding. Existing research provides strong support for the
reliability and factorial validity of RRS-B scores across diverse clinical, community, and
undergraduate samples (He et al., 2021; Schoofs et al., 2010; Thanoi & Klainin-Yobas, 2015;
Treynor et al. 2003). Internal consistency for this sample was excellent (α = 0.84).
Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire, 10-Item Version (INQ-10). The INQ-10 (Bryan et
al., 2010) is a self-report instrument designed to assess thwarted belongingness and perceived
burdensomeness consistent with Joiner’s (2005) IPTS model. Participants rate each item on a 7-
point Likert scale (1 = Not at all true for me; 7 = very true for me). Scores for 5-item
burdensomeness and belongingness subscales are calculated as item means with greater values
representing higher levels of perceived burdensomeness or thwarted belongingness, respectively.
INQ-10 subscales have demonstrated good reliability and predictive validity in both university
students and adult outpatient samples (Bryan et al., 2010; Hill et al., 2015). Internal consistencies
were excellent for both perceived burdensomeness (α = 0.91) and thwarted belongingness (α =
0.88) scales in the current sample.
Analytic Approach
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 10
Separate regression models were used to examine the unique and interactive effects of
physical role limitations, emotional role limitations, and brooding on perceived burdensomeness
and thwarted belongingness, respectively. Interactive effects and simple slopes were analyzed
consistent with procedures outlined by Aiken and West (1991). All predictors were mean
centered prior to analyses. Effect sizes for individual regression coefficients in this study are
reported as Cohen’s f2. Values of .02, .15, and .35 are consistent with small, medium, and large
effects, respectively (Cohen, 1988). Sensitivity analysis for the planned models identified the
effective sample (N = 262) as adequately powered (1 – β = .80) to detect small-magnitude effects
(f2 = .030) in these data (GPower; Faul et al., 2009).
Results
Perceived Burdensomeness. Means, standard deviations, and correlations for the
primary study variables are provided in Table 2. Results of the model for perceived
burdensomeness are provided in Table 3. Unique effects were noted for Brooding (β = .31; p
< .001; f2 = .099) and Role-Emotional (β = -.26; p < .001; f2 = .056), such that increased brooding
and lower emotional role functioning were associated with greater perceptions of
burdensomeness. These effects were qualified by a significant interaction of Brooding and Role-
Emotional (β = -.18; p = .005; f2 = .030). Simple slopes analyses indicated a reliable association
between role limitations due to emotional difficulties and perceived burdensomeness at high
levels of brooding (β = -.46; p < .001; f2 = .092). Results indicate that greater impairment was
related to elevated perceptions of burdensomeness (see Figure 1). By contrast, data failed to
support an association between Role-Emotional and burdensomeness at low levels of brooding (β
= -.07; p = .476; f2 = .003). No other direct or interactive effects were noted in the model (all p
≥ .476).
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 11
Thwarted Belongingness. Results of the analysis for thwarted belongingness are
provided in Table 3. Unique effects were noted for brooding (β = .25; p < .001; f2 = .067) and
Role-Emotional (β = -.38; p < .001; f2 = .122), such that increased brooding and functional
impairment due to emotional problems were associated with greater reported thwarted
belongingness. No direct effect of Role-Physical on thwarted belongingness was detected in the
data (β = .04; p = .585; f2 = .001). Contrary to hypotheses, no interaction effects were noted in
this model (all p ≥ .426).
Discussion
The present study builds on existing research by exploring factors that may help to
qualify the association of functional impairment with unmet interpersonal needs in those exposed
to significant trauma. Previous studies have noted specific links between limitations due to
physical and/or emotional difficulties and perceptions of burdensomeness and thwarted
belongingness in a variety of clinical populations (e.g., Calandre et al., 2021; Hirsch et al., 2016;
Wiblin et al., 2021). In line with this research, impairment related to physical and emotional
concerns reported in the current sample demonstrated expected bivariate associations with both
perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness.
Analyses also identified a moderating effect of brooding on the relation between
limitations due to emotional difficulties and perceived burdensomeness. Consistent with
hypotheses, impairment related to emotion symptoms was associated with greater perceptions of
burdensomeness at high, but not low, levels of brooding. Similar to effects noted in previous
studies (e.g., Aldao et al., 2010; Hong, 2007), results suggest that ruminative tendencies may, in
some cases, enhance the impact of affective symptoms (e.g. depression, anxiety) commonly
associated with trauma exposure. For survivors, impairment occurring within the context of
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 12
increased judgmental reflection may increase beliefs that one’s death would be beneficial to
others, a core component of perceived burdensomeness (Van Orden et al., 2010). Brooding may
also exacerbate feelings of self-hatred in response to functional impairment resulting from
psychological difficulties (e.g., reduced performance at work, less engagement with valued
activities). The current data, by contrast, provide little evidence for greater perceptions of
burdensomeness as a result of impairment due to emotional difficulties in those reporting little
judgmental reflection on current feelings and past actions. Results offer support for brooding as a
potential compounding factor in survivors experiencing functional difficulties as a result of
emotional concerns.
Analyses also identified unique effects of limitations due to emotional difficulties and
brooding on reports of thwarted belongingness. Data from the extant literature indicate that
symptoms related to depression, anxiety, substance use, and PTSD are associated with various
forms of social impairment, which could, in turn, contribute directly to feelings of loneliness and
isolation (Girard et al., 2017; Hassel et al., 2013; King et al., 2006; McEvoy et al., 2013). Trait-
level rumination is similarly associated with a variety of interpersonal concerns (Lam et al.,
2003). In contrast to interactive effects observed for perceived burdensomeness, results of the
current study suggest that brooding and impairment as a result of emotional concerns
independently predict feelings of alienation from friends, family, and other important social
groups. It is possible that impairment due to emotional difficulties in trauma survivors may
preclude involvement in social activities (e.g., missing opportunities for socialization as a result
of depressed mood and fatigue), contributing directly to feelings of isolation regardless of
individual tendencies towards brooding. Alternatively, ruminative behavior may cause
individuals to mischaracterize those social interactions that do occur, independent of level of
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 13
functional impairment. Whereas specific effects in this study await replication, preliminary
results suggest subtle differences in the pattern of associations of brooding and impairment due
to emotional difficulties across reports of burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness.
Overall, results suggest that brooding processes may serve as an important target for
intervention, to be considered in conjunction with packages aimed at reducing condition-specific
symptoms and resulting impairment. An example of this approach is the Collaborative
Assessment and Management of Suicidality (Jobes, 2006) which highlights the need to address
“suicidal drivers”, referring to specific experiences or behaviors that contribute to suicidal crises.
Given broad acknowledgement of perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness as
drivers of risk (Joiner, 2005), strategies designed to manage ruminative self-judgement (e.g.,
mindfulness-based techniques such as those central to Dialectical Behavior Therapy and
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 2012, Linehan, 2015) could
be beneficial, either directly or in combination with empirically-supported treatments for trauma-
related concerns. A greater understanding of the ways in which functional impairment and
ruminative processes contribute to individual risk may help to guide the selection and integration
of specific therapeutic tools.
Limitations and Future Directions
Results of this study should be interpreted within the context of the relative strengths and
limitations of the data. The current project is the first to examine the impact of maladaptive
rumination on the relation between functional impairment and unmet interpersonal needs in
survivors of significant trauma. Additionally, the use of interview-based assessment permitted a
more accurate and inclusive evaluation of Criterion-A trauma relative to data based on common
self-report measures of exposure (Dohrenwend, 2006). Despite these strengths, analyses were
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 14
limited to a non-treatment-seeking sample of university students. Although, research identifies
emerging adulthood as a period of increased risk for trauma exposure, suicidal ideation, and
suicide attempts (e.g., Breslau et al., 1998; Eskin et al., 2016), it is possible that more prominent
effects could be noted in formal, help-seeking populations. Impairment due to physical and
emotional concerns among participants in this study was significantly greater that estimates for
previous unselected student (DeBerard & Masters, 2014), trauma-exposed undergraduate (Flood
et al., 2009), and young adult community samples (Ware, 1994). Specific limitations due to
emotional difficulties were similar to scores reported in help-seeking medical (Burton et al.,
2012) and comorbid medical and psychiatric samples (McHorney et al., 1993) although estimates
of impairment resulting from physical concerns is generally higher in primary care and surgical
settings. It is possible that mild to intermediate levels of physical health impairment in the
current study masked potential effects on target outcomes. Bivariate associations of limitations
due to physical difficulties with perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness
replicated those observed in the larger literature (e.g., Hirsch et al., 2016; Khazem et al., 2015).
However, the effects of SF-36 Role-Physical scores was no longer significant with the addition
of brooding and limitations due to emotional difficulties, suggesting that for emerging adults,
impairment arising from psychological concerns may exert a greater influence on unmet
interpersonal needs.
Homogeneity of the sample in terms of racial and ethnic background may also limit the
generalizability of findings. Epidemiological research suggests differential rates of exposure to
certain traumatic events across racial/ethnic groups in the U.S. with members of minority
communities less likely to receive services than White Europeans (Roberts et al., 2011). By
contrast, African American and Hispanic/Latino(a) Americans evidence lower rates of suicide as
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 15
compared to European Americans (Drapeau & McIntosh, 2018). Disparities are typically
attributed to variability in the role of family and social support in minority communities
(O’Donnell et al., 2004), suggesting that cultural differences and values of independence versus
collectivism may affect perceptions of burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness. Continued
research would help to clarify the extent to which cultural moderators affect the relation between
functional impairment and interpersonal needs.
Finally, measures utilized for the current study provided only general indices of global
emotional and physical functioning. Although role limitations due to emotional and physical
difficulties demonstrated expected associations with burdensomeness and belongingness, the
extent to which reported impairment was directly associated with index events identified by
participants unclear. It is possible impairment occurring in direct response to specific events
(e.g., chronic pain resulting from traumatic injury) may be more emotionally salient given the
acute onset of such difficulties (Clapp et al., 2010; Follick et al., 1985). While the current
approach permitted assessment of general impairment and its association with interpersonal
needs, future research should aim to examine impairment directly associated with experienced
traumatic events.
Conclusion
The present results offer support for the impact of brooding on the relation between
emotional role limitations and perceived burdensomeness in emerging adults exposed to
Criterion-A trauma. Effects are generally consistent with prior research noting the influences of
functional impairment and rumination on suicidal ideation and attempts. Findings indicate that
rumination may be an important target for suicide risk assessment and suicide specific
interventions among trauma exposed individuals.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 16
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across
psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217–237.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders: DSM-5™, 5th ed. (2013). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing,
Inc.
Atwoli, L., Platt, J. M., Basu, A., Williams, D. R., Stein, D. J., & Koenen, K. C. (2016).
Associations between lifetime potentially traumatic events and chronic physical
conditions in the South African Stress and Health Survey: A cross-sectional study. BMC
Psychiatry, 16.
Blanchard, E. B., & Hickling, E. J. (1997). After the crash: Assessment and treatment of motor
vehicle accident survivors. American Psychological Association.
Breslau, N., Kessler, R. C., Chilcoat, H. D., Schultz, L. R., Davis, G. C., & Andreski, P. (1998).
Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: The 1996 Detroit Area
Survey of Trauma. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(7), 626–632.
Bryan, C. J. (2011). The clinical utility of a brief measure of perceived burdensomeness and
thwarted belongingness for the detection of suicidal military personnel. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 67(10), 981-992.
Bryan, C. J., Morrow, C. E., Anestis, M. D., & Joiner, T. E. (2010). A preliminary test of the
interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidal behavior in a military sample. Personality
and Individual Differences, 48(3), 347–350.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 17
Burton, M., Walters, S. J., Saleh, M., & Brazier, J. E. (2012). An evaluation of patient-reported
outcome measures in lower limb reconstruction surgery. Quality of Life Research: An
International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care &
Rehabilitation, 21(10), 1731–1743.
Calandre, E. P., Ordoñez-Carrasco, J. L., & Rico-Villademoros, F. (2021). Marital adjustment in
patients with fibromyalgia: Its association with suicidal ideation and related factors. A
cross-sectional study. Clinical and Experimental Rheumatology, 39(Suppl.130), S89-S94.
Chu, C., Buchman-Schmitt, J. M., Stanley, I. H., Hom, M. A., Tucker, R. P., Hagan, C. R., …
Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2017). The interpersonal theory of suicide: A systematic review and
meta-analysis of a decade of cross-national research. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12),
1313–1345.
Clapp, J. D., Masci, J., Bennett, S. A., & Beck, J. G. (2010). Physical and psychosocial
functioning following motor vehicle trauma: Relationships with chronic pain,
posttraumatic stress, and medication use. European Journal of Pain, 14(4), 418–425.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
DeBerard, M. S., & Masters, K. S. (2014). Psychosocial correlates of the Short-Form-36
Multidimensional Health Survey in university students. Psychology, 5(8), 941–949.
Dedert, E. A., Green, K. T., Calhoun, P. S., Yoash-Gantz, R., Taber, K. H., Mumford, M. M., …
Beckham, J. C. (2009). Association of trauma exposure with psychiatric morbidity in
military veterans who have served since September 11, 2001. Journal of Psychiatric
Research, 43(9), 830–836.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 18
Dohrenwend, B. P. (2006). Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology:
Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological
Bulletin, 132(3), 477–495.
Drapeau, C. W. & McIntosh, J. L. (2018). U.S.S. Suicide 2017: Official final data. Retrieved
from http://suicidology.org
Eskin, M., Sun, J.-M., Abuidhail, J., Yoshimasu, K., Kujan, O., Janghorbani, M., … Voracek, M.
(2016). Suicidal behavior and psychological distress in university students: A 12-nation
study. Archives of Suicide Research, 20(3), 369–388.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*
Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research
Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160.
Flood, A. M., McDevitt-Murphy, M. E., Weathers, F. W., Eakin, D. E., & Benson, T. A. (2009).
Substance use behaviors as a mediator between posttraumatic stress disorder and physical
health in trauma-exposed college students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32(3), 234–
243.
Follick, M. J., Smith, T. W., & Ahern, D. K. (1985). The Sickness Impact Profile: A global
measure of disability in chronic low back pain. Pain, 21(1), 67–76.
Fullerton, C. S., & Ursano, R. J. (2005). Psychological and psychopathological consequences of
disasters. In J. J. López-Ibor, G. Christodoulou, M. Maj, N. Sartorius, & A. Okasha
(Eds.), Disasters and mental health. (pp. 13–36). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons
Ltd.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 19
Galarneau, M. R., Hancock, W. C., Konoske, P., Melcer, T., Vickers, R. R., Walker, G. J., &
Zouris, J. M. (2006). The Navy-Marine Corps Combat Trauma Registry. Military
Medicine, 171(8), 691–697.
Girard, J. M., Wright, A. G. C., Beeney, J. E., Lazarus, S. A., Scott, L. N., Stepp, S. D., &
Pilkonis, P. A. (2017). Interpersonal problems across levels of the psychopathology
hierarchy. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 79, 53–69.
Goldney, R. D., Wilson, D., Dal Grande, E., Fisher, L. J., & McFarlane, A. C. (2000). Suicidal
ideation in a random community sample: Attributable risk due to depression and
psychosocial and traumatic events. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 34(1), 98–106.
Haigh, E. A. P., Quiñones, V., & Moore, M. T. (2018). Structural validity of the Social Problem–
Solving Inventory-Revised Short Form: Application to brooding and reflection. Journal
of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32(4), 285–302.
Hassel, A., Nordfjærn, T., & Hagen, R. (2013). Psychological and interpersonal distress among
patients with substance use disorders: Are these factors associated with continued drug
use and do they change during treatment? Journal of Substance Use, 18(5), 363–376.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy: The
process and practice of mindful change, 2nd ed. Guilford Press.
He, J., Liu, Y., Cheng, C., Fang, S., Wang, X., & Yao, S. (2021). Psychometric properties of the
Chinese version of the 10-item Ruminative Response Scale among undergraduates and
depressive patients. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12.
Hill, R. M., Rey, Y., Marin, C. E., Sharp, C., Green, K. L., & Pettit, J. W. (2015). Evaluating the
Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire: Comparison of the reliability, factor structure, and
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 20
predictive validity across five versions. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 45(3),
302–314.
Hirsch, J. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., & Walker, K. L. (2016). Pain and suicidal behavior in primary
care patients: Mediating role of interpersonal needs: Pain, social distress and suicide in
primary care. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14(5), 820–830.
Hong, R. Y. (2007). Worry and rumination: Differential associations with anxious and
depressive symptoms and coping behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(2),
277–290.
Jobes, D. A. (2006). Managing suicidal risk: A collaborative approach. Guilford Press.
Joiner, T. (2005). Why people die by suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Khazem, L. R., Jahn, D. R., Cukrowicz, K. C., & Anestis, M. D. (2015). Physical disability and
the interpersonal theory of suicide. Death Studies, 39(10), 641–646.
Kilpatrick, D. G., Ruggiero, K. J., Acierno, R., Saunders, B. E., Resnick, H. S., & Best, C. L.
(2003). Violence and risk of PTSD, major depression, substance abuse/dependence, and
comorbidity: Results from the National Survey of Adolescents. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 71(4), 692–700.
King, D. W., Taft, C., King, L. A., Hammond, C., & Stone, E. R. (2006). Directionality of the
association between social support and posttraumatic stress disorder: A longitudinal
investigation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36(12), 2980–2992.
Kirkegaard Thomsen, D. (2006). The association between rumination and negative affect: A
review. Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1216-1235.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 21
Lam, D., Schuck, N., Smith, N., Farmer, A., & Checkley, S. (2003). Response style,
interpersonal difficulties and social functioning in major depressive disorder. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 75(3), 279–283.
LeBouthillier, D. M., McMillan, K. A., Thibodeau, M. A., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2015). Types
and number of traumas associated with suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in PTSD:
Findings from a US nationally representative sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28(3),
183–190.
Linehan, M. M. (2015). Dialectical Behavior Therapy Skills Training Manual (2nd.). New York:
The Guilford Press.
McEvoy, P. M., Burgess, M. M., Page, A. C., Nathan, P., & Fursland, A. (2013). Interpersonal
problems across anxiety, depression, and eating disorders: A transdiagnostic
examination. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52(2), 129–147.
McHorney, C. A., Ware, J. E., & Raczek, A. E. (1993). The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health
Survey (SF-36): II Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and
mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31(3), 247–263.
Medetsky, H. A., & Parnes, L. (1993). The psychological examination in motor vehicle
accidents. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 11(3), 47–60.
Millegan, J., Wang, L., LeardMann, C. A., Miletich, D., & Street, A. E. (2016). Sexual trauma
and adverse health and occupational outcomes among men serving in the US
Military. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 29(2), 132–140.
Momartin, S., Silove, D., Manicavasagar, V., & Steel, Z. (2003). Dimensions of trauma
associated with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) caseness, severity and functional
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 22
impairment: A study of Bosnian refugees resettled in Australia. Social Science &
Medicine, 57(5), 775–781.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.
O’Donnell, L., O’Donnell, C., Wardlaw, D. M., & Stueve, A. (2004). Risk and Resiliency
Factors Influencing Suicidality Among Urban African American and Latino
Youth. American Journal of Community Psychology, 33(1–2), 37–49.
Ogunsiji, O., & Clisdell, E. (2017). Intimate partner violence prevention and reduction: A review
of literature. Health Care for Women International, 38(5), 439–462.
Pearson, K. A., Watkins, E. R., Kuyken, W., & Mullan, E. G. (2010). The psychosocial context
of depressive rumination: Ruminative brooding predicts diminished relationship
satisfaction in individuals with a history of past major depression. British Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 49(2), 275–280.
Roberts, A. L., Gilman, S. E., Breslau, J., Breslau, N., & Koenen, K. C. (2011). Race/ethnic
differences in exposure to traumatic events, development of post-traumatic stress
disorder, and treatment-seeking for post-traumatic stress disorder in the United
States. Psychological Medicine, 41(1), 71–83.
Schnurr, P. P., & Green, B. L. (2004). Understanding Relationships Among Trauma, Post-
Tramatic Stress Disorder, and Health Outcomes. Advances in Mind-Body
Medicine, 20(1), 18–29.
Schoofs, H., Hermans, D., & Raes, F. (2010). Brooding and reflection as subtypes of rumination:
Evidence from confirmatory factor analysis in nonclinical samples using the Dutch
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 23
Ruminative Response Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 32(4), 609-617.
Smith, J. M., & Alloy, L. B. (2009). A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition,
assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct. Clinical Psychology
Review, 29(2), 116-128.
Thanoi, W., & Klainin-Yobas, P. (2015). Assessing rumination response style among
undergraduate nursing students: A construct validation study. Nurse Education
Today, 35(5), 641-646.
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A
psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.
Ullman, S. E. (1999). Social support and recovery from sexual assault: A review. Aggression
and Violent Behavior, 4(3), 343-358.
Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T.
E., Jr. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review, 117(2), 575–
600.
Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I
Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.
Ware, J. E. (1994). SF-36 Physical and Mental Health Summary Scales: A User’s Manual.
Boston, MA: The Health Institute, New England Medical Center Hospitals.
Wen, J., Shi, Y., Li, Y., Yuan, P., & Wang, F. (2012). Quality of life, physical diseases, and
psychological impairment among survivors 3 years after Wenchuan earthquake: A
population based survey. PLoS ONE, 7(8).
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 24
Wiblin, J., Holder, N., Holliday, R., & Surís, A. (2021). Predictors of unbearability, unlovability,
and unsolvability in veterans with military-sexual-trauma-related posttraumatic stress
disorder. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(7-8), 3814-3830.
Xavier, A., Cunha, M., & Pinto-Gouveia, J. (2016). Rumination in adolescence: The distinctive
impact of brooding and reflection on psychopathology. The Spanish Journal of
Psychology, 19.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 25
Table 1
Sample Characteristics (N =262)
Age 20.55 (4.77)
Sex (% female) 198 75.9%
Race/Ethnicity
European American
Hispanic
Other
206
24
32
78.6%
9.2%
12.2%
Index Trauma
Sexual Assault
Physical Assault
Motor Vehicle Accident
Combat
Witnessed Suicide
Threatened Death/Injury
Other
98
29
66
6
14
10
39
37.4%
11.1%
25.2%
2.3%
5.3%
3.8%
14.9%
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 26
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations between Primary Study Variables
Measure 12345
1: INQ-10 Burden [.51, .67] [-.33, .06] [-.51, -.30] [.33, .54]
2: INQ-10 Belonging .59** [-.35, -.09] [-.57, -.38] [.32, .52]
3: Role-Physical -.20** -.23** [.42, .60] [-.43, -.17]
4: Role-Emotional -.41** -.48** .52** [-.58, -.39]
5: Brooding .44** .42** -.31** -.49**
M1.59 3.04 77.48 57.38 10.17
SD 0.90 1.46 32.22 42.22 3.64
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01
Note: INQ-10 Burden = Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire - 10 Perceived Burdensomeness; INQ-10 Belonging = Interpersonal Needs
Questionnaire - 10 Thwarted Belongingness; Role-Physical = SF-36 Role Limitations due to Physical Problems; Role-Emotional =
SF-36 Role Limitations due to Emotional Problems; Brooding = Ruminative Response Scale—Brooding Subscale.
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 27
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 28
Table 3
Coefficients, Confidence Intervals, and Effect Sizes for Moderation of Perceived Burdensomeness and Thwarted Belongingness
R2β b β (SE) β CI95% p f2
INQ-10 Burden .269
Brooding .312 .077 .062 [.190, .434] < .001 .099
Role-Physical .046 .001 .064 [-.081, .173] .476 .003
Role-Emotional -.263 -.006 .069 [-.398, -.128] < .001 .056
BroodxRPa.047 .000 .059 [-.075, .159] .479 .002
BroodxREa-.179 -.001 .068 [-.326, -.057] .005 .030
INQ-10 Belonging .262
Brooding .253 .102 .062 [.132, .375] < .001 .067
Role-Physical .035 .002 .064 [-.091, .161] .585 .001
Role-Emotional -.376 -.013 068 [-.511, -.242] < .001 .122
Brooding x RPa.000 .000 .059 [-.117, .116] .995 .000
Brooding x REa.051 .001 .068 [-.079, .188] .426 .003
Note: Brood = Ruminative Response Scale—Brooding Subscale; Role-Physical = SF-26 Role Limitations due to Physical Problems;
Role-Emotional = SF-36 Role Limitations due to Emotional Problems.
Interpretive benchmarks for f2: small =.020, medium = .150, large = .350 (Cohen, 1988).
a Standardized coefficients for interaction terms were calculated as the product of standardized main effects, consistent with
procedures outlined by Aiken and West (1991, pg. 43).
FUNCTIONAL LIMITATIONS AND INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 29
Low Role-Emoonal (-1SD) High Role-Emoonal (+1SD)
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
Burdensomeness
Figure 1. Simple slopes analysis for the association of emotional role limitations and perceived burdensomeness for high and low
levels of brooding
High Brooding: b = -.010, p < .001, f2 = .092
Low Brooding: b = -.002, p = .476, f2 = .003
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Rumination is considered as a key process in the mechanism of depression. Assessing rumination is important for both research and clinical practice. The Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) is a widely-used instrument to measure rumination. This study aimed to examine the psychometric properties of the Chinese 10-item Ruminative Response Scale (RRS-10) in a large sample of Chinese undergraduates and depressive patients. Methods: A total of 1,773 university students and 286 clinical patients with major depressive disorder finished the Chinese version of the RRS10, State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to examine the two-factor structure (reflection and brooding) of the RRS-10. The correlations among RRS-10, STAI, and BDI were explored in two samples. In addition, the measurement invariance of the RRS-10 across gender, time, and groups with and without depressive symptoms were further investigated. The internal consistency and test-retest reliability were also evaluated. Results: Confirmatory Factor Analysis revealed that the two-factor structure of RRS-10 fitted reasonably both in undergraduates (CFI = 0.933, TLI = 0.905, RMSEA = 0.071, SRMR = 0.035) and depressive patients (CFI = 0.941, TLI = 0.910, RMSEA = 0.077, SRMR = 0.057). The results of the multi-group confirmatory factor analysis supported the full strict invariance across genders and across groups (undergraduates and depressive patients). The full strong invariance over time was also supported by MGCFA. Besides, the RRS-10 showed acceptable internal consistency and good stability. Conclusions: The RRS-10 has good reliability and validity in different samples and over time, which demonstrated that RRS-10 is a valid measurement instrument to assess rumination.
Article
Full-text available
Research on social problem-solving ability or the cognitive-behavioral processes used to find successful solutions to everyday problems has been advanced by the availability of easy-toadminister self-report measures. The goal of the current study is twofold: (a) validate the factor structure of the Social Problem-Solving Inventory-Revised: Short Form (SPSI-R:S) and (b) examine the relationship between depressive rumination subtypes and dimensions of social problem solving. An initial confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) in Sample 1 revealed poor model fit. An exploratory factor analysis revealed that several items cross-loaded on more than one factor. These items were removed and a subsequent CFA demonstrated adequate model. The revised model was validated in a second sample and convergent and divergent validity were examined. Brooding was related to more maladaptive problem-solving ability while reflection was generally associated with a more adaptive problem-solving style. No gender differences were found.
Article
Full-text available
Military sexual trauma (MST) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) both increase risk for suicidal self-directed violence (SDV). Suicide cognitions (i.e., unbearability, unlovability, and unsolvability) are strong predictors of future suicidal SDV. The present study investigated potential predictors of unbearability, unlovability, and unsolvability in veterans with MST-related PTSD. Suicide cognitions, depression, PTSD, quality of life, trauma-related negative cognitions, physical health functioning, mental health functioning, and childhood sexual assault were assessed in 12 male and 103 female veterans with MST-related PTSD. Higher depression scores, greater trauma-related negative cognitions about self, and poorer physical health functioning predicted increased unbearability scores. Greater trauma-related negative cognitions about self and self-blame, higher level of education, and higher depression scores predicted increased unlovability scores. Higher depression scores and greater trauma-related negative cognitions about self predicted increased unsolvability scores. In veterans with MST-related PTSD who express unbearability, unlovability, and unsolvability, assessing and addressing depression, trauma-related negative cognitions about self and self-blame, and physical health functioning may be an important step in reducing SDV.
Article
OBJECTIVES: Fibromyalgia has been associated with suicidal ideation, suicide attempts and completed suicide. Non-married status is a risk factor for suicidal behaviours but the quality of the marital relationship has been scarcely investigated. The objectives of the present study were to evaluate, in patients with fibromyalgia, the relationship between marital adjustment suicidal ideation and potentially related variables: depression severity, sleep disturbance, perceived burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, fibromyalgia severity and pain intensity. METHODS: A survey was done in patients with fibromyalgia which collected sociodemographic data and included the following questionnaires: the Locke and Wallace Marital Adjustment Test, the Beck’s Depression Inventory II, the Plutchik Suicide Risk Scale, a 10-item version of the Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire, the Insomnia Severity Index, and the Revised Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire. Suicidal ideation was assessed with item 9 of the Beck’s Depression Inventory, and pain intensity was measured with the VAS scale of the Revised Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire. RESULTS: Of 257 participants 71 were single, 107 had a good marital adjustment and 79 a poor marital adjustment. Suicidal ideation was most frequent among patients with poor marital adjustment than among those with good marital adjustment or patients without partner. The poor marital adjustment group showed significantly worse scores in most of the remaining variables with the only exceptions of the FIQR and pain scores. CONCLUSIONS: In patients with fibromyalgia the degree of marital adjustment seems to be a relevant factor for suicidal ideation and related variables.
Article
The reliability and validity of reported exposure to significant trauma is critical to research evaluating outcomes following serious and distressing life events. The current study examined the reliability of reported exposure to disaster, fire, transportation accidents, physical assault, and sexual assault across 5- (N = 251), 12- (N = 223), and 24-month (N = 109) intervals in undergraduates completing a screening measure of probable trauma (N = 3,045). Concordance with later responses to an alternate checklist and events assessed via clinical interview was examined in a subset of participants (N = 274). Five-month reliabilities ranged from good to fair (κ = .40 -.71) and were similar to 1-2 week retest estimates in the extant literature. Reliabilities for fire, accidents, and sexual assault remained stable over 12- and 24-month intervals. Coefficients for disaster and physical assault decreased over time. Agreement with the alternate checklist was fair to excellent in those completing the follow-up assessment (κ = .51-.87). Concordance with interview-based trauma was acceptable for accidents (κ = .52) and sexual violence (κ = .82) but poor for disaster, fire, and physical assault (κ = .34 -.38). Specificity, negative predictive power, and negative likelihood ratios suggest checklists may hold utility in ruling out previous trauma. Sensitivities indicate that screening instruments may broadly capture individuals experiencing traumatic life events although positive predictive power was limited except in the prediction of traffic accidents and sexual assault. Variability across domains suggests that the properties of checklist measures could be better conceptualized at the level of individual exposure events.
Article
Over the past decade, the interpersonal theory of suicide has contributed to substantial advances in the scientific and clinical understanding of suicide and related conditions. The interpersonal theory of suicide posits that suicidal desire emerges when individuals experience intractable feelings of perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness and near-lethal or lethal suicidal behavior occurs in the presence of suicidal desire and capability for suicide. A growing number of studies have tested these posited pathways in various samples; however, these findings have yet to be evaluated meta-analytically. This paper aimed to (a) conduct a systematic review of the unpublished and published, peer-reviewed literature examining the relationship between interpersonal theory constructs and suicidal thoughts and behaviors, (b) conduct meta-analyses testing the interpersonal theory hypotheses, and (c) evaluate the influence of various moderators on these relationships. Four electronic bibliographic databases were searched through the end of March, 2016: PubMed, Medline, PsycINFO, and Web of Science. Hypothesis-driven meta-analyses using random effects models were conducted using 122 distinct unpublished and published samples. Findings supported the interpersonal theory: the interaction between thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness was significantly associated with suicidal ideation; and the interaction between thwarted belongingness, perceived burdensomeness, and capability for suicide was significantly related to a greater number of prior suicide attempts. However, effect sizes for these interactions were modest. Alternative configurations of theory variables were similarly useful for predicting suicide risk as theory-consistent pathways. We conclude with limitations and recommendations for the interpersonal theory as a framework for understanding the suicidal spectrum.
Article
We examined the relationship between psychopathology and interpersonal problems in a sample of 825 clinical and community participants. Sixteen psychiatric diagnoses and five transdiagnostic dimensions were examined in relation to self-reported interpersonal problems. The structural summary method was used with the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems Circumplex Scales to examine interpersonal problem profiles for each diagnosis and dimension. We built a structural model of mental disorders including factors corresponding to detachment (avoidant personality, social phobia, major depression), internalizing (dependent personality, borderline personality, panic disorder, posttraumatic stress, major depression), disinhibition (antisocial personality, drug dependence, alcohol dependence, borderline personality), dominance (histrionic personality, narcissistic personality, paranoid personality), and compulsivity (obsessive-compulsive personality). All dimensions showed good interpersonal prototypicality (e.g., detachment was defined by a socially avoidant/nonassertive interpersonal profile) except for internalizing, which was diffusely associated with elevated interpersonal distress. The findings for individual disorders were largely consistent with the dimension that each disorder loaded on, with the exception of the internalizing and dominance disorders, which were interpersonally heterogeneous. These results replicate previous findings and provide novel insights into social dysfunction in psychopathology by wedding the power of hierarchical (i.e., dimensional) modeling and interpersonal circumplex assessment.
Book
Since the original publication of this seminal work, acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) has come into its own as a widely practiced approach to helping people change. This book provides the definitive statement of ACT—from conceptual and empirical foundations to clinical techniques—written by its originators. ACT is based on the idea that psychological rigidity is a root cause of a wide range of clinical problems. The authors describe effective, innovative ways to cultivate psychological flexibility by detecting and targeting six key processes: defusion, acceptance, attention to the present moment, self-awareness, values, and committed action. Sample therapeutic exercises and patient–therapist dialogues are integrated throughout. New to This Edition *Reflects tremendous advances in ACT clinical applications, theory building, and research. *Psychological flexibility is now the central organizing focus. *Expanded coverage of mindfulness, the therapeutic relationship, relational learning, and case formulation. *Restructured to be more clinician friendly and accessible; focuses on the moment-by-moment process of therapy.