ArticlePublisher preview available

Characterizing ontogeny of quantity discrimination in zebrafish

The Royal Society
Proceedings of the Royal Society B
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

A sense of non-symbolic numerical magnitudes is widespread in the animal kingdom and has been documented in adult zebrafish. Here, we investigated the ontogeny of this ability using a group size preference (GSP) task in juvenile zebrafish. Fish showed GSP from 21 days post-fertilization and reliably chose the larger group when presented with discriminations of between 1 versus 3, 2 versus 5 and 2 versus 3 conspecifics but not 2 versus 4 conspecifics. When the ratio between the number of conspecifics in each group was maintained at 1 : 2, fish could discriminate between 1 versus 2 individuals and 3 versus 6, but again, not when given a choice between 2 versus 4 individuals. These findings are in agreement with studies in other species, suggesting the systems involved in quantity representation do not operate separately from other cognitive mechanisms. Rather they suggest quantity processing in fishes may be the result of an interplay between attentional, cognitive and memory-related mechanisms as in humans and other animals. Our results emphasize the potential of the use of zebrafish to explore the genetic and neural processes underlying the ontogeny and function of number cognition.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rspb
Research
Cite this article: Sheardown E, Torres-Perez
JV, Anagianni S, Fraser SE, Vallortigara G,
Butterworth B, Miletto-Petrazzini ME, Brennan
CH. 2022 Characterizing ontogeny of quantity
discrimination in zebrafish. Proc. R. Soc. B 289:
20212544.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2544
Received: 22 November 2021
Accepted: 7 January 2022
Subject Category:
Behaviour
Subject Areas:
behaviour, cognition
Keywords:
zebrafish, numerical cognition, ontogeny,
quantity discrimination
Authors for correspondence:
Maria Elena Miletto-Petrazzini
e-mail: mariaelena.milettopetrazzini@gmail.
com
Caroline H. Brennan
e-mail: c.h.brennan@qmul.ac.uk
Characterizing ontogeny of quantity
discrimination in zebrafish
Eva Sheardown
1,3
, Jose Vicente Torres-Perez
2,3
, Sofia Anagianni
3
,
Scott E. Fraser
4
, Giorgio Vallortigara
5
, Brian Butterworth
6
,
Maria Elena Miletto-Petrazzini
3,7
and Caroline H. Brennan
3
1
Centre for Developmental Neurobiology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, Kings College
London, New Hunts House, Guys Campus, London SE1 1UL, UK
2
Departament de Biologia Cellular, Biologia Funcional i Antropologia física, Fac. de CC. Biològiques, Universitat
de València, C/ Dr. Moliner 50, 46100 Burjassot (València), Spain
3
School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London,
E1 4NS, UK
4
Michelson Center for Convergent Bioscience, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA
5
Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
6
UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AZ, UK
7
Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy
ES, 0000-0002-1801-7025; JVT-P, 0000-0001-6390-9690; SA, 0000-0001-5784-582X;
SEF, 0000-0002-5377-0223; GV, 0000-0001-8192-9062; BB, 0000-0001-8201-3347;
MEM-P, 0000-0002-5204-5863; CHB, 0000-0002-4169-4083
A sense of non-symbolic numerical magnitudes is widespread in the animal
kingdom and has been documented in adult zebrafish. Here, we investi-
gated the ontogeny of this ability using a group size preference (GSP) task
in juvenile zebrafish. Fish showed GSP from 21 days post-fertilization and
reliably chose the larger group when presented with discriminations of
between 1 versus 3, 2 versus 5 and 2 versus 3 conspecifics but not 2
versus 4 conspecifics. When the ratio between the number of conspecifics
in each group was maintained at 1 : 2, fish could discriminate between 1
versus 2 individuals and 3 versus 6, but again, not when given a choice
between 2 versus 4 individuals. These findings are in agreement with studies
in other species, suggesting the systems involved in quantity representation
do not operate separately from other cognitive mechanisms. Rather they
suggest quantity processing in fishes may be the result of an interplay
between attentional, cognitive and memory-related mechanisms as in
humans and other animals. Our results emphasize the potential of the use
of zebrafish to explore the genetic and neural processes underlying the onto-
geny and function of number cognition.
1. Introduction
One aspect of quantity discrimination is the evaluation of the number of items
in a group, the numerosity. Although quantitative abilities of many species
have been studied [13], research into the ontogeny of the ability to assess
numerosity has been restricted so far to humans [4,5], fish (Poecilia reticulata)
and domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) [6,7]. Typically, in species other than
chicks, which are a precocial species, the ability to assess numerosity changes
with age until individuals reach adulthood. However, 1-day-old guppies
(P. reticulata) can distinguish between a small number of conspecifics in a
similar way to 40-day-old guppies [8].
Interestingly, guppies show no ratio effect for contrasts between numeros-
ities 1 to 4, but the familiar ratio effect, Webers Law, for larger numerosities
[9]. Similar findings have been observed in human infants [1014] suggesting
the existence of conserved mechanisms of numerosity discrimination, though
little is known about either the neural or genetic bases of these abilities (for a
recent review see Lorenzi et al. [15]).
© 2022 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.
... By expressing a nuclear-localized calcium indicator (H2B-jGCaMP7f) (Dana et al., 2019a), we were able to monitor brain-wide neuronal activity in response to non-symbolic, visual numerical stimuli. While no studies have shown any ability to discriminate different numerosities at an early age (<7 days post-fertilization (dpf)) in zebrafish, it must develop before the behavior becomes apparent (Adam et al., 2024;Lorenzi et al., 2023;Lucon-Xiccato et al., 2023;Sheardown et al., 2022a). Here, we aim to identify these number-neurons, across the brain, in real time as the animal processes a visual number stimulus, in an effort to improve our understanding of the neural basis of number sense. ...
... Consistent with studies using chick models (Kobylkov et al., 2022) and human infants (Izard et al., 2009;Xu & Spelke, 2000), we detected number-selective neurons during early post-embryonic (equivalent to post-natal) stages in zebrafish. Notably, the existence of these number-selective neurons at 3 dpf precedes any known numerically-driven behaviors such as hunting and shoaling (which start at 5 and~24 dpf, respectively) (Adam et al., 2024;Borla et al., 2002;Lucon-Xiccato et al., 2023;Sheardown et al., 2022a), underscoring the fundamental role and necessity of early numerical cognition for survival. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Number sense, the ability to discriminate the quantity of objects, is crucial for survival. To understand how neurons work together and develop to mediate number sense, we used two-photon fluorescence light sheet microscopy to capture the activity of individual neurons throughout the brain of larval zebrafish ( Danio rerio ), while displaying a visual number stimulus to the animal. We identified number-selective neurons as early as 3 days post-fertilization (dpf) and found a proportional increase of neurons tuned to larger (>2) quantities after 3 dpf. To determine if these neurons are sufficient to encode the correct quantity, we used machine learning to predict the stimulus from the neuronal activity and observed that the prediction accuracy improves with age. Given ethanol’s propensity to inhibit cognitive functions, we tested its effect on number sense and found a decrease of active number-selective neurons in the forebrain during ethanol exposure, suggesting cognitive impairment. The findings here are a significant step towards understanding neural circuits devoted to discrete magnitudes and our methodology to track single neuron activity across the whole brain is broadly applicable to other fields in neuroscience. Significance Numerically responsive neurons can be detected as early as 3 days post-fertilization (dpf) in zebrafish Number-selective neurons preferring quantities greater than one increases after 3 dpf Neurons that are highly responsive to specific numbers of objects are mainly localized to the forebrain and midbrain Number stimulus can be decoded from Ca ²⁺ activity and show increased performance with age Administration of ethanol decreases the activity of number-selective neurons in the forebrain
... The zebrafish is a widespread model in developmental biology and genetics and is one of the best available models for studying the ontogenetic development of numerical cognition [42]. Thanks to the development of neurobiological techniques and the evidence collected on the neural basis of number in this species (see Messina et al. (2022) [5]), our results may provide behavioral techniques useful for studying the neural circuitry underpinning proto-arithmetic abilities in zebrafish. ...
... Smaller ratios from 0.25 to 0.33 were quite easy for them to distinguish while ratios of 0.50 and higher were unequally harder or not possible to distinguish for these larvae (Fig. 5a). This coincides with the findings of Sheardown et al. (2022) that juvenile zebrafish at the age of 21-33 dpf reliably choose the larger group of different numerosities within ratios of 0.25 and 0.33 and that more difficult comparisons (ratio 0.67) are only feasible for older zebrafish starting from 27 dpf. Once this ability is reached, it seems to stay consistent over time since adult zebrafish have also been shown to be able to discriminate small numerosities with a ratio of up to 0.67, but failed to discriminate between numerosities with a ratio of 0.75 (Potrich et al. 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite their young age, zebrafish larvae have a well-developed visual system and can distinguish between different visual stimuli. First, we investigated if the first visual surroundings the larvae experience during the first days after hatching shape their habitat preference. Indeed, these animals seem to “imprint” on the first surroundings they see and select visual stimuli accordingly at 7 days post fertilization (dpf). In particular, if zebrafish larvae experience a bar background just after hatching, they later on prefer bars over white stimuli, and vice versa. We then used this acquired preference for bars to investigate innate numerical abilities. We wanted to specifically test if the zebrafish larvae show real numerical abilities or if they rely on a lower-level mechanism—i.e. spatial frequency—to discriminate between two different numerosities. When we matched the spatial frequency in stimuli with different numbers of bars, the larvae reliably selected the higher numerosity. A previous study has ruled out that 7 dpf zebrafish larvae use convex hull, cumulative surface area and density to choose between two numerosities. Therefore, our results indicate that zebrafish larvae rely on real numerical abilities rather than other cues, including spatial frequency, when spontaneously comparing two sets with different numbers of bars. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10071-024-01888-0.
... The study with guppies showed an innate capacity to discriminate between social groups differing by one individual up to three vs four units (0.75 ratio), the same numerical acuity shown by adults tested in similar conditions (Bisazza et al. 2010). Zebrafish tested between age 21 and 33 dpf and showed an age effect in the most difficult comparison, two versus three (0.67 ratio), which was discriminated only by the oldest larvae (Sheardown et al. 2022). Although in this case, the cognitive ability under investigation (i.e. ...
Article
Full-text available
Most studies on developmental variation in cognition have suggested that individuals are born with reduced or absent cognitive abilities, and thereafter, cognitive performance increases with age during early development. However, these studies have been mainly performed in altricial species, such as humans, in which offspring are extremely immature at birth. In this work, we tested the hypothesis that species with other developmental modes might show different patterns of cognitive development. To this end, we analysed inhibitory control performance in two teleost species with different developmental modes, the zebrafish Danio rerio and the guppy Poecilia reticulata , exploiting a simple paradigm based on spontaneous behaviour and therefore applicable to subjects of different ages. Zebrafish hatch as larvae 3 days after fertilisation, and have an immature nervous system, a situation that mirrors extreme altriciality. We found that at the early stages of development, zebrafish displayed no evidence of inhibitory control, which only begun to emerge after one month of life. Conversely, guppies, which are born after approximately one month of gestation as fully developed and independent individuals, solved the inhibitory control task since their first days of life, although performance increased with sexual maturation. Our study suggests that the typical progression described during early ontogeny in humans and other species might not be the only developmental trend for animals’ cognition and that a species’ developmental mode might determine variation in cognition across subjects of different age.
... This finding indicates that the ability of number sense in amphibians can be underestimated or misinterpreted. Plasticity in this aspect of number sense is already known in other taxa (e.g., in fish, Bisazza et al. 2010;Sheardown et al. 2022). When measuring the number competence of frogs and toads, it will be crucial to consider ontogeny and plasticity. ...
Article
Full-text available
The ability to perceive group size and discriminate the ontogeny of conspecifics would play a crucial role in the grouping behavior of animals. However, the relative importance of numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in shaping grouping rules remains poorly understood. In this study, I examined the responses of Miyako toad (Bufo gargarizans miyakonis) tadpoles to number quantity and size discrimination by choice tests at different ontogenetic stages (small, medium, and large). The results revealed that small-sized tadpoles in early developmental stages significantly preferred larger numbers (4) compared to smaller ones (1). However, this preference was not observed in later developmental stages (medium and large). And interestingly, when there was no quantity bias, size discrimination was not observed in tadpoles, irrespective of their ontogeny. These findings suggest that Miyako toad tadpoles discern quantity, i.e., the number of conspecifics, but exhibit ontogeny-dependent utilization of this ability. Understanding the interplay between numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in grouping behavior will provide esteemed insights into the adaptive value of number sense in vertebrates and shed light on evolutionary processes.
... This nding indicates that numerical abilities in amphibians can be underestimated or misinterpreted. Plasticity in this aspect of number sense is already known in other taxa (e.g., in sh, Bisazza et al., 2010;Sheardown et al., 2022). When measuring the number competence of frogs and toads, it will be crucial to consider ontogeny and plasticity. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The ability to perceive group size and discriminate the ontogeny of conspecifics would play a crucial role in the grouping behavior of animals. However, the relative importance of numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in shaping grouping rules remains poorly understood. In this study, I examined the responses of Miyako toad ( Bufo gargarizans miyakonis ) tadpoles to number quantity and size discrimination by choice tests at different ontogenetic stages (small, medium, and large). The results revealed that small-sized tadpoles in early developmental stages significantly preferred larger numbers (4) compared to smaller ones (1). However, this preference was not observed in later developmental stages (medium and large). And interestingly, when there was no group size bias, size discrimination was not observed in tadpoles, irrespective of their ontogeny. These findings suggest that Miyako toad tadpoles possess innate numerical competence but demonstrate ontogeny-dependent utilization of this ability. Understanding the interplay between numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in grouping behavior will provide esteemed insights into the adaptive value of number sense in vertebrates and sheds light on evolutionary processes.
... However, if the numerical information is excluded and the two stimuli differ only in their continuous property (cumulative area) guppies no longer discriminate between them (Miletto Petrazzini et al. 2014;Piffer et al. 2013). Findings of a recent study of young zebra fish (Danio rerio) are consistent with these results, at least in terms of number discrimination (Sheardown et al. 2022). In contrast with this, tadpoles of the two anuran species tested (Bufotes balearicus and Pelophylax esculentus) seem to have the capacity to discriminate between only small numbers (Balestrieri et al. 2019), suggesting that they rely more on the OFS, whereas fishes might operate on both. ...
Article
Full-text available
Quantitative abilities are well described in many species and in diverse life situations, including in the adult domestic cat. However, such abilities have been much less studied during ontogeny. In the present study we examined spontaneous quantity discrimination by pre-weaning age kittens in two-way food choice experiments. In Experiment 1, 26 kittens performed 12 trials with different ratios between the number of same-size food items. In Experiment 2, 24 other kittens performed eight trials with different ratios between the size of two food items. We found, in general, that the kittens discriminated between the different amounts of food and spontaneously chose the larger one, but that their choice was influenced by the ratio of difference. The kittens in Experiment 1 chose the larger number of same-size food items if the ratio was smaller than 0.4 and in Experiment 2 they chose the larger pieces of food if the ratio between the items was smaller than 0.5. Because the kittens’ choice was not influenced by the absolute number of food items or the numerical difference between them in Experiment 1, it suggests that their cognitive performance relied on an analog magnitude system rather than on an object file system during the quantity discrimination tasks. We discuss our results considering the ecological and social background of cats and compare it with the performance of previously studied species.
... In the aforementioned situations, pure numerical abilities provide reliable information for adaptive decisions. Therefore, it is not difficult to explain their presence in animals that are requested to perform such decisions, including adult and juvenile zebrafish 58,59 . What is puzzling, is the presence of numerical abilities in zebrafish at an early developmental stage when they seem to have reasonably no functional role. ...
Article
Full-text available
An intriguing hypothesis to explain the ubiquity of numerical abilities is that all vertebrates are born with hardwired neuronal networks for processing numbers. To date, only studies on human foetuses have clearly supported this hypothesis. Zebrafish hatch 48–72 h after fertilisation with an embryonic nervous system, providing a unique opportunity for investigating this hypothesis. Here, we demonstrated that zebrafish larvae exposed to vertical bars at birth acquired an attraction for bar stimuli and we developed a numerical discrimination task based on this preference. When tested with a series of discriminations of increasing difficulty (1vs.4, 1vs.3, 1vs.2, and 2vs.4 bars), zebrafish larvae reliably selected the greater numerosity. The preference was significant when stimuli were matched for surface area, luminance, density, and convex hull, thereby suggesting a true capacity to process numerical information. Converging results from two phylogenetically distant species suggests that numerical abilities might be a hallmark feature of vertebrates’ brains.
... However, we see evidence for the preference for the larger shoal both in the case of 4 versus 2 and 6 versus 3 dichotomous trials through trajectory density analysis, where the more dense regions were near the larger shoal. Previous studies on shoaling preference report that zebrafish can discriminate between shoal sizes of 6 versus 3 in dichotomous choices, although many studies indicate a failure to discriminate 4 versus 2 shoal sizes 39,51 .However, both those studies also use a different rectangular three-tank shoal choice set up from ours, and one 39 used a perforated divider through which shoaling decisions were based on olfactory and not just visual cues from the display fish. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Several organisms, from slime molds to humans, are known to violate normative principles of economic rationality in decision making. In animals, the neural circuitry underlying behaviors that violate or conform to normative rationality is relatively poorly understood. We investigated whether zebrafish, a model organism with a strong suite of functional neuroimaging and genetic manipulation tools, showed rational behavior with respect to the principle of the Independence of Irrelevant Alternatives (IIA). We examined IIA in social decision making by measuring revealed preferences from spatial trajectories of freely swimming individual zebrafish in an arena where they could view and perform shoaling behavior near conspecific zebrafish in adjacent display tanks. IIA was tested in terms of the invariance of shoaling choices between binary and ternary sets of various display fish group sizes. We provide the first report of evidence for violation of IIA in zebrafish, both in terms of the constant ratio rule and in terms of the principle of regularity. This also is a rare example of violation of rationality in a single attribute dimension, and it opens up a range of possibilities to study the neural basis of context dependent decision making.
Article
Full-text available
It is widely acknowledged that vertebrates can discriminate non-symbolic numerosity using an evolutionarily conserved system dubbed Approximate Number System (ANS). Two main approaches have been used to assess behaviourally numerosity in fish: spontaneous choice tests and operant training procedures. In the first, animals spontaneously choose between sets of biologically-relevant stimuli (e.g., conspecifics, food) differing in quantities (smaller or larger). In the second, animals are trained to associate a numerosity with a reward. Although the ability of fish to discriminate numerosity has been widely documented with these methods, the molecular bases of quantities estimation and ANS are largely unknown. Recently, we combined behavioral tasks with molecular biology assays (e.g c-fos and egr1 and other early genes expression) showing that the thalamus and the caudal region of dorso-central part of the telencephalon seem to be activated upon change in numerousness in visual stimuli. In contrast, the retina and the optic tectum mainly responded to changes in continuous magnitude such as stimulus size. We here provide a review and synthesis of these findings.
Article
Full-text available
We found a region of the zebrafish pallium that shows selective activation upon change in the numerosity of visual stimuli. Zebrafish were habituated to sets of small dots that changed in individual size, position, and density, while maintaining their numerousness and overall surface. During dishabituation tests, zebrafish faced a change in number (with the same overall surface), in shape (with the same overall surface and number), or in size (with the same shape and number) of the dots, whereas, in a control group, zebrafish faced the same stimuli as during the habituation. Modulation of the expression of the immediate early genes c-fos and egr-1 and in situ hybridization revealed a selective activation of the caudal part of the dorso-central division of the zebrafish pallium upon change in numerosity. These findings support the existence of an evolutionarily conserved mechanism for approximate magnitude and provide an avenue for understanding its underlying molecular correlates.
Article
Full-text available
The ability to represent, discriminate, and perform arithmetic operations on discrete quantities (numerosities) has been documented in a variety of species of different taxonomic groups, both vertebrates and invertebrates. We do not know, however, to what extent similarity in behavioral data corresponds to basic similarity in underlying neural mechanisms. Here, we review evidence for magnitude representation, both discrete (countable) and continuous, following the sensory input path from primary sensory systems to associative pallial territories in the vertebrate brains. We also speculate on possible underlying mechanisms in invertebrate brains and on the role played by modeling with artificial neural networks. This may provide a general overview on the nervous system involvement in approximating quantity in different animal species, and a general theoretical framework to future comparative studies on the neurobiology of number cognition.
Article
Full-text available
Consciousness shared Humans have tended to believe that we are the only species to possess certain traits, behaviors, or abilities, especially with regard to cognition. Occasionally, we extend such traits to primates or other mammals—species with which we share fundamental brain similarities. Over time, more and more of these supposed pillars of human exceptionalism have fallen. Nieder et al. now argue that the relationship between consciousness and a standard cerebral cortex is another fallen pillar (see the Perspective by Herculano-Houzel). Specifically, carrion crows show a neuronal response in the palliative end brain during the performance of a task that correlates with their perception of a stimulus. Such activity might be a broad marker for consciousness. Science , this issue p. 1626 ; see also p. 1567
Article
Full-text available
There is considerable evidence that animals are able to discriminate between quantities. Despite the fact that quantitative skills have been extensively studied in adult individuals, research on their development in early life is restricted to a limited number of species. We, therefore, investigated whether 2-month-old puppies could spontaneously discriminate between different quantities of food items. We used a simultaneous two-choice task in which puppies were presented with three numerical combinations of pieces of food (1 vs. 8, 1 vs. 6 and 1 vs. 4), and they were allowed to select only one option. The subjects chose the larger of the two quantities in the 1 vs. 8 and the 1 vs. 6 combinations but not in the 1 vs. 4 combination. Furthermore, the last quantity the puppies looked at before making their choice and the time spent looking at the larger/smaller amounts of food were predictive of the choices they made. Since adult dogs are capable of discriminating between more difficult numerical contrasts when tested with similar tasks, our findings suggest that the capacity to discriminate between quantities is already present at an early age, but that it is limited to very easy discriminations.
Article
Full-text available
Evidence has shown that a variety of vertebrates, including fish, can discriminate collections of visual items on the basis of their numerousness using an evolutionarily conserved system for approximating numerical magnitude (the so-called Approximate Number System, ANS). Here we combine a habituation/dishabituation behavioural task with molecular biology assays to start investigating the neural bases of the ANS in zebrafish. Separate groups of zebrafish underwent a habituation phase with a set of 3 or 9 small red dots, associated with a food reward. The dots changed in size, position and density from trial to trial but maintained their numerousness, and the overall areas of the stimuli was kept constant. During the subsequent dishabituation test, zebrafish faced a change (i) in number (from 3 to 9 or vice versa with the same overall surface), or (ii) in shape (with the same overall surface and number), or (iii) in size (with the same shape and number). A control group of zebrafish was shown the same stimuli as during the habituation. RT-qPCR revealed that the telencephalon and thalamus were characterized by the most consistent modulation of the expression of the immediate early genes c-fos and egr-1 upon change in numerousness; in contrast, the retina and optic tectum responded mainly to changes in stimulus size.
Article
Full-text available
A large body of literature is available on wound healing in humans. Nonetheless, a standardized ex vivo wound model without disruption of the dermal compartment has not been put forward with compelling justification. Here, we present a novel wound model based on application of negative pressure and its effects for epidermal regeneration and immune cell behaviour. Importantly, the basement membrane remained intact after blister roof removal and keratinocytes were absent in the wounded area. Upon six days of culture, the wound was covered with one to three-cell thick K14+Ki67+ keratinocyte layers, indicating that proliferation and migration were involved in wound closure. After eight to twelve days, a multi-layered epidermis was formed expressing epidermal differentiation markers (K10, filaggrin, DSG-1, CDSN). Investigations about immune cell-specific manners revealed more T cells in the blister roof epidermis compared to normal epidermis. We identified several cell populations in blister roof epidermis and suction blister fluid that are absent in normal epidermis which correlated with their decrease in the dermis, indicating a dermal efflux upon negative pressure. Together, our model recapitulates the main features of epithelial wound regeneration, and can be applied for testing wound healing therapies and investigating underlying mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
To make appropriate decisions, animals need to accumulate sensory evidence. Simple integrator models can explain many aspects of such behavior, but how the underlying computations are mechanistically implemented in the brain remains poorly understood. Here we approach this problem by adapting the random-dot motion discrimination paradigm, classically used in primate studies, to larval zebrafish. Using their innate optomotor response as a measure of decision making, we find that larval zebrafish accumulate and remember motion evidence over many seconds and that the behavior is in close agreement with a bounded leaky integrator model. Through the use of brain-wide functional imaging, we identify three neuronal clusters in the anterior hindbrain that are well suited to execute the underlying computations. By relating the dynamics within these structures to individual behavioral choices, we propose a biophysically plausible circuit arrangement in which an evidence integrator competes against a dynamic decision threshold to activate a downstream motor command.
Article
Zebrafish is an emerging model in the study of brain function; however, knowledge about its behaviour and cognition is incomplete. Previous studies suggest this species’ limited ability in visual learning tasks compared to other teleosts. In this study, we systematically examined zebrafish’s ability to learn to discriminate colour, shape, size, and orientation of figures using an appetitive conditioning paradigm. Contrary to earlier reports, the zebrafish successfully completed all tasks. Not all discriminations were learned with the same speed and accuracy. Subjects discriminated the size of objects better than their shape or colour. In all three tasks, they were faster and more accurate when required to discriminate between outlined figures than between filled figures. With stimuli consisting of outlines, the learning performance of zebrafish was comparable to that observed in higher vertebrates. Zebrafish easily learned a horizontal–vertical discrimination task, but like many other vertebrates, they had great difficulty discriminating a figure from its mirror image. Performance was more accurate for subjects reinforced on one stimulus (green over red, triangle over circle, large over small). Unexpectedly, these stimulus biases occurred only when zebrafish were tested with filled figures, suggesting some causal relationship between stimulus preference, learning bias and performance.