Content uploaded by Pieter Meyns
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Pieter Meyns on Feb 09, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
Available online 3 February 2022
0167-9457/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Full Length Article
Effects of age and surface instability on the control of the center
of mass
Maud van den Bogaart
a
,
b
,
*
, Sjoerd M. Bruijn
b
,
c
, Joke Spildooren
a
,
Jaap H. van Die¨
en
b
, Pieter Meyns
a
a
Rehabilitation Research Center (REVAL), Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Hasselt University, Diepenbeek 3590, Belgium
b
Amsterdam Movement Sciences, Department of Human Movement Sciences, Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit
Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1081 BT, the Netherlands
c
Institute Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Postural control
Ageing
CoP mechanism
Counter-rotation mechanism
Balance board
Center of mass acceleration
ABSTRACT
During standing, posture can be controlled by accelerating the Center of Mass (CoM) through
shifting the center of pressure (CoP) within the base of support by applying ankle moments (“CoP
mechanism”), or through the “counter-rotation mechanism”, i.e., changing the angular momentum
of segments around the CoM to change the direction of the ground reaction force. Postural control
develops over the lifespan; at both the beginning and the end of the lifespan adequate postural
control appears more challenging. In this study, we aimed to assess mediolateral balance perfor-
mance and the related use of the postural control mechanisms in children, older adults and younger
adults when standing on different (unstable) surfaces. Sixteen pre-pubertal children (6-9y), 17
younger adults (18-24y) and eight older adults (65-80y) performed bipedal upright standing trials
of 16 s on a rigid surface and on three balance boards that could freely move in the frontal plane,
varying in height (15–19 cm) of the surface of the board above the point of contact with the oor.
Full body kinematics (16 segments, 48 markers, using SIMI 3D-motion analysis system (GmbH) and
DeepLabCut and Anipose) were retrieved. Performance related outcome measures, i.e., the number
of trials with balance loss and the Root Mean Square (RMS) of the time series of the CoM accel-
eration, the contributions of the CoP mechanism and the counter-rotation mechanism to the CoM
acceleration in the frontal plane and selected kinematic measures, i.e. the orientation of the board
and the head and the Mean Power Frequency (MPF) of the balance board orientation and the CoM
acceleration were determined. Balance loss only occurred when standing on the highest balance
board, twice in one older adult once in one younger adult. In children and older adults, the RMS of
the CoM accelerations were larger, corresponding to poorer balance performance. Across age
groups and conditions, the contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was
much larger than that of the counter-rotation mechanisms, ranging from 94% to 113% vs 23% to
38% (with totals higher than 100% indicating opposite effects of both mechanisms). Deviations in
head orientation were small compared to deviations in balance board orientation. We suggest that
the CoP mechanism is dominant, since the counter-rotation mechanism would conict with sta-
bilizing the orientation of the head in space.
* Corresponding author at: Rehabilitation Research Center (REVAL), Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Hasselt University, Diepenbeek 3590,
Belgium.
E-mail address: maud.vandenbogaart@uhasselt.be (M. van den Bogaart).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Human Movement Science
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/humov
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2022.102930
Received 10 September 2021; Received in revised form 17 January 2022; Accepted 19 January 2022
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
2
1. Introduction
Postural control is the ability to align body segments such that the body center of mass (CoM) stays within safe boundaries above
the base of support (BoS) or to bring it back above the BoS after perturbations (Horak, 1987). This is essential for activities of daily
living. Postural control depends on the central nervous system to continually integrate and reweigh information from visual, vestibular
and proprioceptive systems and elicit coordinated muscular responses (Peterka, 2002).
During standing, the base of support is xed and posture can be controlled by accelerating the CoM by shifting the center of
pressure (CoP) within the BoS (Hof, 2007). If CoP shifts are used, the body rotates more or less as a single segment around the ankle(s),
often modeled as a single inverted pendulum. Consequently, it has been coined the “CoP mechanism” (Hof, 2007; Horak & Nashner,
1986). Another mechanism to accelerate the CoM, is the “counter-rotation mechanism”, i.e., changing the angular momentum of
segments around the CoM to change the direction of the ground reaction force (Hof, 2007). In this mechanism, body segments are
rotated with respect to the CoM (Otten, 1999). The hip mechanism, dened by its kinematic characteristics, i.e. the rotation of the
trunk and legs around the hip, is one example of the counter-rotation mechanism (Hof, 2007; Otten, 1999). Rotations of other body
segments, such as the arms or head can be used in the same way. Therefore, the umbrella term “counter-rotation mechanism” will be
used here. Especially when trunk rotations are included, the counter-rotation mechanism may result in head rotations and conse-
quently affect visual and vestibular input (Alizadehsaravi, Bruijn, & van Die¨
en, 2021). Thus, people may prefer to keep their head
orientation stable in space, rather than using rotations involving the head to change angular momentum to control the CoM (Fino,
Raffegeau, Parrington, Peterka, & King, 2020).
The use of these postural control mechanisms has been suggested to be direction-specic (Winter, Patla, Prince, Ishac, & Gielo-
Perczak, 1998; Winter, Prince, Frank, Powell, & Zabjek, 1996). For anteroposterior control, the CoP can be shifted along the length
of the feet by modulating plantar and dorsiexor muscle activity, while the counter-rotation mechanism in anteroposterior direction
could involve hip and trunk exor/extensor muscle modulation to rotate the trunk and consequently accelerate the CoM or the arms.
Accelerating the arms could be very benecial, as one could potentially accelerate them without having to deaccelerate them. In the
mediolateral direction, modulation of evertor and invertor muscle activity allows shifting the CoP along the width of the foot, but in
bipedal stance also loading of one, and unloading of the other leg by extensor and exor muscle respectively, causes a shift of the CoP.
The counter-rotation mechanism could involve modulation of hip abductor/adductor and trunk lateroexor muscle activity to rotate
the trunk and accelerate the CoM or swing of the arms. The current work will focus on the use of the CoP and counter-rotation
mechanisms in the mediolateral direction, as mediolateral balance impairments have been associated with an increased risk of fall-
ing (Maki, Holliday, & Topper, 1994). Falls are the most common cause of childhood injury presented at emergency departments
(Sminkey, 2008) and also in older adults, falls are a major cause of injury and even of death (Haagsma et al., 2016; Haagsma et al.,
2020). About 30% of community-dwelling Western adults over 65 years of age fall every year (Bath & Morgan, 1999; de Rekeneire
et al., 2003; Morrison, Fan, Sen, & Weisenuh, 2013; Peel, 2011; Stel, Smit, Pluijm, & Lips, 2003). To understand the causes of falling,
fundamental knowledge of how healthy ageing affects postural control is of utmost importance.
In healthy younger adults, both ankle (invertor/evertor) and hip (abductor/adductor) muscles control mediolateral CoM move-
ment during quiet stance (Day, Steiger, Thompson, & Marsden, 1993; Gatev, Thomas, Kepple, & Hallett, 1999; Winter et al., 1996).
Reliance on more proximal muscles has been shown to increase when standing on a compliant or moving support surface (Patel et al.,
2008; Riemann, Myers, & Lephart, 2003; van Dieen, van Leeuwen, & Faber, 2015). Standing on such a surface makes proprioceptive
information at the ankle less pertinent and changes the effects of ankle moments produced for postural control (Horak & Hlavacka,
2001; MacLellan & Patla, 2006). In younger adults the mean power frequency (MPF) of CoP displacements is higher when standing on
a sway-referenced support surface compared to a rigid surface (Dickin, McClain, Hubble, Doan, & Sessford, 2012). This may suggest
that the frequency of postural corrections made using the CoP mechanism is increased in such conditions. In addition, the contribution
of the counter-rotation mechanism was shown to be larger when standing on an unstable compared to a rigid surface (van Dieen et al.,
2015). As such, it is expected that younger adults would rely more on the counter-rotation mechanism with increasing surface
instability.
Postural control develops over the lifespan; at both the beginning and the end of the lifespan problems with postural control usually
occur. In children, the sensory systems are not completely developed, which diminishes postural control (Steindl, Kunz, Schrott-
Fischer, & Scholtz, 2006). Proprioceptive function seems to mature at 3–4 years of age and visual and vestibular afferent systems
reach adult levels at 15–16 years of age (Steindl et al., 2006) or even later (Hirabayashi & Iwasaki, 1995). The integration and
reweighting of sensory information do not reach adult levels until the age of 15 (Shams, Vameghi, Shamsipour Dehkordi, Allafan, &
Bayati, 2020). During quiet standing, the amplitude of mediolateral CoP displacements and velocities are larger in children between 5
and 7 years old than in older children and adults (Hsu, Kuan, & Young, 2009; Riach & Starkes, 1989). Information on the use of the
postural control mechanisms and the effects on sway amplitude and velocity at different surfaces in children is, to the best of our
knowledge, missing.
In older adults, sensory and motor impairments, as well as sensory reweighting decits lead to impaired postural control compared
to younger adults. Older adults' ability to adapt to altered sensory conditions such as visual and/or surface perturbations is reduced,
and they generally rely more on visual information for postural control than younger adults (Bugnariu & Fung, 2007; Teasdale,
Stelmach, Breunig, & Meeuwsen, 1991). Consequently, CoP displacement and velocity are larger in older adults (age >64) compared
to younger adults both when standing on a rigid surface and when standing on foam (Bergamin et al., 2014; Bugnariu & Fung, 2006).
The frequency of movements of the CoP appears similar in older adults compared to younger adults (Demura & Kitabayashi, 2006).
Older adults appeared to rely more on the counter-rotation mechanism when responding to anteroposterior perturbations than
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
3
younger adults (Wu, 1998). However, in responding to a platform rotation, older adults' initial lateral trunk and arm movements were
directed in the same direction as the platform rotation, suggesting that they anticipated breaking a potential fall, while younger adults
used arm movements to control CoM movement and recover upright posture (Allum, Carpenter, Honegger, Adkin, & Bloem, 2002).
Thus, it remains an open question whether older adults rely more, or less, on the counter-rotation mechanisms for mediolateral control
of standing on rigid and unstable surfaces.
In this study, we assessed mediolateral balance performance and the related use of the postural control mechanisms in children,
older adults and younger adults. To test if, and how, balance performance and the related use of the postural control mechanisms
changes with ageing, variations in surface instability were used. We expected poorer balance performance in children and older adults
compared to younger adults. We also expected poorer balance performance and reduced contribution of the CoP mechanism during
standing on the balance boards compared to standing on a rigid surface. Additionally, we hypothesized that the CoP mechanism is
dominant.
Understanding the use of the CoP and counter-rotation mechanisms for postural control across the lifespan and when standing on
different (unstable) surfaces may have implications for the design of therapeutic interventions that aim to decrease fall incidence.
2. Methods
2.1. Subjects
Sixteen pre-pubertal children between 6 and 9 years old (10 males, age 8.2 ±1.1 years old, BMI 15.6 ±1.5 kg/m
2
), 17 healthy
younger adults between 18 and 24 years old (7 males, age 21.9 ±1.6 years old, BMI 23.5 ±3.0 kg/m
2
) and eight older adults between
65 and 80 years old (5 males, age 71.8 ±4.6 years old, BMI 26.0 ±3.4 kg/m
2
) participated in this study. Sample size was calculated for
a two-tailed unpaired sample t-test analysis using G*Power (1-β =0.8,
α
=0.05) and an effect size of 1.5 based on previous studies
(Masani et al., 2007; Oba, Sasagawa, Yamamoto, & Nakazawa, 2015). The total calculated sample size was eight per group. Potential
participants were excluded if they reported any neurological or orthopedic disorder(s), had an uncorrectable visual impairment, were
unable to maintain independent and unsupported stance for 60 s, had undergone surgery of the lower extremities during the last 2
years, or took medication that might affect postural control. Additionally, older subjects were excluded if they had experienced two or
more falls during normal daily activities in the preceding year or had a cognitive impairment (tested with Mini-Mental state exami-
nation (score <24)). Participants gave written informed consent prior to the experiment. The study protocol was in agreement with the
declaration of Helsinki and had been approved by the local ethical committee (CME2018/064, NCT04050774).
2.2. Research design
The participants performed bipedal upright standing on a rigid surface and on three balance boards varying in height of the surface
of the board above the point of contact with the oor (BB1; 15 cm, BB2; 17 cm and BB3; 19 cm). The balance board was a 48 cm by 48
cm wooden board mounted on a section of a cylinder with a 24 cm radius that could freely move in the frontal plane (Fig. 1). The four
conditions were repeated three times in random order, with each trial lasting 16 s. Trials longer than 16 s seemed not feasible as these
resulted in frequent falling or stepping off the balance board across all age groups. Furthermore, the longer the trial duration, the more
aspects like attention and motivation were tested and challenged, which could be confounding factors when assessing balance control,
Fig. 1. Illustration of the balance boards, which could freely move in the frontal plane, varying in height of the surface of the board above the point
of contact with the oor (BB1; 15 cm, BB2; 17 cm and BB3; 19 cm). The feet indicate the person's orientation on the balance board.
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
4
especially in young children. For every trial, participants were instructed to stand barefoot on two feet, placed in parallel at hip width
and arms along the body. They were asked to look at a marked spot at seven meters distance on the wall in front of them at eye level.
2.3. Materials and software
A Simi 3D motion analysis system (GmbH) with eight cameras (sample rate:100 samples/s, resolution: 1152 ×864 pixels) and 48
retro reective markers was used (A.1.1. Supplementary materials). The amount of illumination in the room at eye level was 650 Lux.
Full body 3D kinematics (16 segments) were retrieved using the open-source deep learning python toolboxes DeepLabCut (https://
github.com/AlexEMG/DeepLabCut) and Anipose (https://github.com/lambdaloop/anipose) (A.1.1. Supplementary materials)
(Mathis et al., 2018; Nath et al., 2019). This workow provided 3D data, which is similar in quality to that obtained with the Simi 3D
motion analysis software, yet requires less user interaction (A.1. Supplementary materials).
2.4. Data analysis
2.4.1. Performance
A trial was registered as a balance loss if a stepping response or an intervention by a researcher was required in order to maintain
stance. The number of balance losses per condition and per age group was recorded as a performance related outcome measure. In case
of a balance loss, the trial was excluded from the analysis. Next to the number of balance losses, the Root Mean Square (RMS) of the
time series of the CoM acceleration in the frontal plane was determined as a measure of performance.
2.4.2. Postural control mechanisms
The contributions of the CoP mechanism and counter-rotation mechanism to the CoM acceleration in the frontal plane were
calculated using Eq. (1), as described by Hof (2007).
¨
CoMML (t) = −Fz(CoPML(t) − CoMML (t) )
m∙CoMvertical (t)+
˙
Hfr(t)
m∙CoMvertical (t)(1)
in which m is body mass, CoM
ML
is the mediolateral (ML) position of the CoM, CoM
vertical
is the vertical position of the CoM, C¨
OM
ML
is
the double derivative of CoM
ML
with respect to time, t is time, F
z
is the vertical ground reaction force, CoP
ML
is the ML position of the
CoP, and
Ḣ
fr
is the change in total body angular momentum in the frontal plane. Here the rst part of the right-hand term,
−Fz(CoPML(t)− CoMML (t) )
m∙CoMvertical(t), refers to the CoP mechanism and the second part,
˙
Hfr(t)
m∙CoMvertical(t), is the ML CoM acceleration induced by the counter-
rotation mechanism.
As it was not possible to collect accurate ground reaction forces and CoP, the contribution of the CoP mechanism to the C¨
OM
ML
was
calculated by subtracting, Ḣfrfr(t)
m∙CoMvertical(t), from C¨
OM
ML
(t).
The RMS of the time series of the contribution of the CoP mechanism and the counter-rotation mechanism to the ¨
CoMML was
calculated for each trial. The relative contribution of the CoP mechanism and the counter-rotation mechanism to the ¨
CoMML (in %) was
calculated by dividing the RMS of each mechanism by ¨
CoMML, multiplied by 100.
2.4.3. Kinematics
Orientations of the board and head in the frontal plane were calculated. Head rotation was dened as the frontal plane angle
relative to the global coordinate system. Board rotation was dened as the frontal plane angle relative to the global coordinate system.
The deviations from the mean orientations were determined by calculating the standard deviation (SD) of the orientation angles. The
MPF of the balance board orientation and ¨
CoMML were calculated using the pwelch matlab function with a window of 100 data points
(1 s), 50% overlap, and nfft of 1000. Before using the pwelch matlab function, the balance board rotation angles were high pass ltered
with a second order Butterworth lter with a cutoff frequency of 3 Hz.
2.5. Statistics
The results of the trials of each surface condition were averaged for each participant. The number of trials for each surface condition
was three unless a fall occurred, which resulted in exclusion of this trial. Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs were used to determine
the effect of Age and Surface as well as their interaction on the RMS and MPF of the ¨
CoMML, the RMS of the contribution of the CoP and
counter-rotation mechanism, the relative contribution of the CoP mechanism and the counter-rotation mechanism to ¨
CoMML, the SD
and MPF of the balance board orientation, and the SD of the head orientation. Post-hoc analyses were performed to determine dif-
ferences between the different experimental surface conditions (using a Bonferroni correction of the p-values). In addition, planned
comparisons (without correction for multiple testing) were done to compare children with younger adults and older adults with
younger adults. Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS(v25) at
α
<0.05.
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
5
3. Results
First, normality of the residuals was checked using the Q-Q plot of the unstandardized residuals and the Shapiro-Wilk test. The
residuals were not normally distributed for all variables. After log transformation of the data, the assumptions for parametric testing
were met. As the statistical results were similar between the non-transformed data and log transformed data and ANOVA is considered
robust to violations of normality (Schmider, Ziegler, Danay, Beyer, & Bühner, 2010), we used the non-transformed data.
3.1. Performance
3.1.1. Balance loss
Balance loss only occurred when standing on the highest balance board, twice in one older adult once in one younger adult. No
participants had to be excluded because at least one out of the three trials per surface condition per participant was available.
Fig. 2. Group means (dots) and individual data (transparent solid lines) of the A) Root Mean Square (RMS) of the Center of Mass (CoM) acceleration
(¨
CoM
ML), B) Mean Power Frequency (MPF) of ¨
CoMML (in Hertz (Hz)), C) the RMS of the contribution of the CoP mechanism, D) the relative
contribution of the CoP mechanism to ¨
CoMML, E) the RMS of the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism, F) the relative contribution of the
counter-rotation mechanism to ¨
CoMML, during standing on a rigid surface (RIGID) and during standing on uniaxial balance boards varying in height
(BB1; 15 cm, BB2; 17 cm and BB3; 19 cm) in children (orange), younger adults (green) and older adults (blue). # represents a signicant difference
compared to standing on a rigid surface. * represents a signicant difference compared to younger adults, with the group tested identied by the
color code. (For interpretation of the references to color in this gure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
6
3.1.2. Total CoM acceleration
There was a signicant interaction between Age and Surface on the RMS of ¨
CoMML, F (6, 114) =4.423, p <0.001. Post-hoc tests
revealed that the RMS of ¨
CoMML was signicantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on the balance
boards in all groups (Fig. 2a). Moreover, it was signicantly larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults during all
conditions (Fig. 2a), reective of a poorer balance performance. To further unravel the interaction effect, the effect of age on the
difference between the RMS of ¨
CoMML during standing on a rigid surface and on BB1, BB2 and BB3 was assessed using independent t-
tests. The RMS of ¨
CoMML increased signicantly more from standing on a rigid surface to standing on BB1, BB2 and BB3 in older adults
compared to younger adults (t (23) = − 2.697, p =0.013, t (23) = − 3.159, p =0.012 and t (23) = − 2.529, p =0.019, respectively), but
not in children compared to younger adults (t (31) =0.595, p =0.556, t (31) =0.868, p =0.392 and t (31) =1.878, p =0.072.
respectively).
3.2. Postural control mechanisms
There was a signicant interaction between Age and Surface on the RMS of the contribution of the CoP mechanism, F (6, 114) =
2.822, p =0.013. Post-hoc tests revealed that the contribution of the CoP mechanism was signicantly smaller during standing on a
rigid surface compared to standing on the balance boards in all groups (Fig. 2c). Moreover, it was signicantly larger in children and
older adults compared to younger adults during all balance board conditions. When standing on a rigid surface, the contribution of the
CoP mechanism was signicantly larger in children compared to younger adults (Fig. 2c). There was no signicant difference between
younger adults and older adults when standing on a rigid surface (p =0.088). To further unravel the interaction effect, the effect of Age
on the difference between the RMS of the contribution of the CoP mechanism during standing on a rigid surface and standing on BB1,
BB2 and BB3 was assessed using independent t-tests. The increase in the RMS of the contribution of the CoP mechanism from standing
on a rigid surface to BB3 was signicantly larger for children compared to younger adults (t (31) =2.153, p =0.039), but the increase
from standing on a rigid surface to BB1 and BB2 was not signicant different between children and younger adults (t (31) =0.78, p =
Fig. 3. Group means (dots) and individual data (transparent solid lines) of the A) standard deviation (SD) of the head orientation (in degrees), B) SD
of the balance board orientation (in degrees), C) Mean Power Frequency (MPF) of the board orientation (in Hertz (Hz)), during standing on uniaxial
balance boards varying in height (BB1; 15 cm, BB2; 17 cm and BB3; 19 cm) in children (orange), younger adults (green) and older adults (blue). *
represents a signicant difference compared to younger adults, with the group tested identied by the color code. # represents a signicant dif-
ference compared to standing on a rigid surface. (For interpretation of the references to color in this gure legend, the reader is referred to the web
version of this article.)
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
7
0.438 and t (31) =0.871, p =0.391). The increase in the RMS of the contribution of the CoP mechanism from standing on a rigid
surface to BB2 and BB3 was signicantly larger in older adults compared to younger adults (t (23) = − 3.105, p =0.005 and t (23) =
−2.128, p =0.044 respectively), but the increase from standing on a rigid surface to BB1 was not signicantly different between older
adults and younger adults (t (23) = − 2.027, p =0.054.
The relative contribution of the CoP mechanism to ¨
CoMML ranged from 94% to 113%. (Fig. 2d). The average relative contribution
increased from standing on a rigid surface to standing on the balance boards (effect of Surface, F (2.010, 76.370) =32.621, p ≤0.001,
Fig. 2d). There was no signicant interaction between Age and Surface and the effect of Age was not signicant.
The RMS of the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was signicantly smaller when standing on a rigid surface than
when standing on the balance boards (effect of Surface, F (3, 114) =53.863, p <0.001). Moreover, the contribution of the counter-
rotation mechanism was signicantly larger in children compared to younger adults (effect of Age, F (2, 38) =4.076, p =0.025,
Fig. 2e). There was no signicant interaction between Age and Surface.
The relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism to ¨
CoMML ranged from 23% to 38%. (Fig. 2f). The relative contribution
of the counter-rotation mechanism increased from standing on a rigid surface to standing on the balance boards (effect of Surface, F
(1.993, 75.727) =10.813, p ≤0.001, Fig. 2f). There was no signicant interaction between Age and Surface and the effect of Age was
not signicant.
3.3. Kinematics
3.3.1. Balance board orientation
The SD of balance board orientation was larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults (effect of Age, F (2, 38) =
8.256, p =0.001, Fig. 3b), implying larger deviations from the mean board orientation. There was no signicant interaction between
Age and Surface and the effect of Surface was not signicant.
3.3.2. Head orientation
The SD of head orientation was signicantly smaller when standing on a rigid surface than when standing on the balance boards
(effect of Surface, F (2.521, 95.805) =10.524, p <0.001, Fig. 3a). Moreover, the SD of head orientation was signicantly larger in
children compared to younger adults, while there was no difference between older and younger adults (effect of Age, F (2, 38) =7.329,
p =0.002, Fig. 3a). There was no signicant interaction between Age and Surface.
3.3.3. Frequency of CoM accelerations and balance board rotations
Age and Surface had a signicant interaction effect on the MPF of ¨
CoMML, F (4.417, 83.927) =7.316, p <0.001. In older adults, the
MPF of ¨
CoMML was signicantly lower during standing on the rigid surface compared to standing on BB1 and BB2 (Fig. 2b). The MPF of
¨
CoMML was signicantly higher in older adults compared to younger adults during all balance board conditions (Fig. 2b). There was no
difference between children and younger adults.
The MPF of balance board orientation was larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults (effect of Age, F (2,38) =
5.081, p =0.011, Fig. 3c), implying increased frequency of the balance board rotations. There was no signicant interaction between
Age and Surface and the effect of Surface was not signicant.
4. Discussion
The aim of this study was to obtain insight in differences in balance performance and the related use of postural control mechanisms
between children, older adults and younger adults during standing on a rigid surface and on balance boards which could freely move in
the frontal plane. As hypothesized, we found a larger RMS of CoM accelerations both in children and older adults compared to younger
adults, implying worse balance performance. Additionally, across age groups and conditions, the contribution of the CoP mechanism to
the total CoM acceleration was much larger than that of the counter-rotation mechanism, ranging from 94% to 113% vs 23% to 38%.
The CoP mechanism was dominant, which is in accordance with our hypothesis.
4.1. Effects of surface instability
Balance loss only occurred when standing on the highest balance board. The RMS of CoM accelerations was larger during standing
on a balance board than on the rigid surface. These performance outcome measures, as well as increased RMS-values and contribution
of the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism when standing on the balance boards reect that standing on the balance board was indeed
more challenging than standing on the oor (van Dieen et al., 2015). This can be explained by the decrease of pertinent proprioceptive
information from ankle muscles and a reduction of the effectiveness of ankle moments for postural control when standing on a balance
board (Horak & Hlavacka, 2001). Furthermore, participants rotated their head more when standing on the balance boards compared to
standing on a rigid surface, probably due to the moving support surface. However, the standard deviation of the head rotation was
limited to only 2.2 degrees.
Surprisingly, the increasing height of the balance boards did not cause a further deterioration of balance performance, change in
use of postural control mechanisms or differences in kinematics. This was unexpected, as the stability of a person on a balance board
and therefore the difculty of the task is affected by the height of the surface of the board above the point of contact with the oor (B.1.
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
8
Supplementary materials, Eq. (B.1.)). Most likely, the differences between the height of the balance boards were not big enough to
cause differences. Overall, the standard deviation of the board orientation ranged between 1.9 and 3.6 degrees, suggesting that the
challenge imposed was limited for all participants.
4.2. Age effects
4.2.1. Performance and kinematics
One older adult lost balance twice and one younger adult once when standing on the highest balance board. No child lost balance,
indicating better performance in children. However, the limited number of balance losses when standing on the highest balance board
and the absence of balance losses on the other balance boards suggests that all participants had good capacity to keep standing on the
balance boards. In children and older adults, sensorimotor control is worse compared to younger adults, hence an increased RMS of
CoM accelerations, corresponding to a deterioration of balance performance, and larger deviations from the mean board orientation
compared to younger adults were expected (Bugnariu & Fung, 2006; Hirabayashi & Iwasaki, 1995; Shams et al., 2020; Steindl et al.,
2006; Teasdale et al., 1991). In older adults, the increase in RMS of CoM accelerations from standing on a rigid surface to standing on a
balance board was signicantly larger compared to younger adults. Thus, the effect of ageing on balance performance was amplied by
the task difculty, suggesting that a decrease of pertinent proprioceptive information and/or a reduction of effectiveness of ankle
moments for postural control poses more of a challenge for the older adults. The differences between older and younger adults during
standing on a rigid surface or a balance board are in line with previous studies (Bergamin et al., 2014; Sturnieks, Arnold, & Lord, 2011).
Moreover, the differences in postural control between children and younger adults were in line with previous literature using foam as
an unstable surface (Hsu et al., 2009; Riach & Starkes, 1989).
The difference in RMS of CoM accelerations between children and younger adults could also be explained by signicantly more
head rotation in children in addition to the reduction in sensorimotor control. More head rotation could be self-generated and form a
sign of less attention in children, which is common in children compared to younger adults (Huang-Pollock, Carr, & Nigg, 2002;
Wickens, 1974). Self-generated head rotation may be disadvantageous, as it leads to changing visual and vestibular inputs, requiring
more processing to discern between body motion and self-imposed head motion (Khan & Chang, 2013). However, the difference in
head rotation between children and younger adults was less than 1 degree.
The standard deviation and the MPF of the board orientation were signicantly higher in children compared to younger adults. This
indicates more frequent movements of the board over larger angles. This could be due to faster feedback, due to shorter neural
pathways in children. However, when standing on the balance boards, the MPF of the CoM accelerations was not signicantly larger in
children compared to younger adults. Thus, the increased frequency of balance board rotations, potentially reecting an increased
frequency of control actions due to the CoP mechanism, did not result in an increased frequency of CoM corrections, or improved
balance performance. Increased frequency of balance board rotations did not automatically result in an increased frequency of CoM
corrections as the frequency of CoM corrections depends on the control actions of both the CoP mechanism and the counter-rotation
mechanism. In older adults, the RMS of CoM accelerations and standard deviation of the board orientation were higher compared to
younger adults, as were the MPF of the CoM accelerations and of the board orientation. This implies more frequent movements of the
balance board over larger angles, decreased balance performance and an increased frequency of CoM corrections in older adults
compared to younger adults. It should be kept in mind that board rotations can reect corrective actions as well as perturbations due to
neuromuscular noise. Overall, our results suggest that both children and older adults had more difculty in controlling the board as
compared to younger adults.
4.2.2. Postural control mechanisms
The relative contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was around 100% (ranging from 94% to 113%) and
the relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was around 30% (ranging from 23% to 38%). Overall, the contribution of
the counter-rotation mechanism was limited and not always in the same direction as the contribution of the CoP mechanism, as the
RMS values of the contribution of the CoP mechanism were often larger than the RMS values of the total mediolateral CoM acceleration
and the relative contributions of the CoP mechanism were around 100%. However, the desired direction is unclear. It could be that
segmental rotations were used to achieve a proper orientation of segments such as regulating the orientation of the head in space,
rather than controlling the CoM acceleration/position (Alizadehsaravi et al., 2021). Overall, the participants, even the children, kept
their head quite stable, in comparison to the board. This suggests that people prefer to keep the head stable, to maintain a constant
visual and vestibular input, rather than using upper body rotations as a counter-rotation mechanism to control the CoM. Larger
contributions of counter-rotation were found in unipedal stance on a balance board, but this was to a large part generated by the free
leg (van Dieen et al., 2015). A limited use of the counter-rotation mechanism to control the CoM was also found in our previous study,
in which we found that using the counter-rotation mechanism would actually have interfered with the gait pattern (van den Bogaart,
Bruijn, van Dieen, & Meyns, 2020). Combined, these results suggest that the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism to control
the CoM is limited. This may be because rotational accelerations of body parts may interfere with other task constraints and because
they need to be reversed leading to the opposite effect on the acceleration of the body center of mass.
4.3. Limitations
Trials leading to balance loss were excluded, which implies that we somewhat overestimated the performance in one older adult
and one younger adult. However, balance losses occurred only three times, and no participants had to be excluded, because at least one
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
9
out of the three trials per condition per participant was available. Additionally, it should be considered that differences in body mass
and height of the center of mass inuence the difculty of standing on the balance boards (B.1.1. Supplementary materials). These
could be considered confounding variables, especially when comparing the children to the younger adults, but on the other hand this is
part and parcel of the age difference. Finally, it was not possible to collect accurate ground reaction forces and CoP when standing on a
balance board. However, the contribution of the CoP mechanism could be calculated based on the other terms in Eq. (1).
5. Conclusion
In children and older adults, the RMS of CoM accelerations and deviations from the mean balance board orientation were larger,
corresponding to poorer balance performance, probably related to less optimal sensorimotor control compared to younger adults. The
contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was around 100%, regardless of age and condition, while the
contribution of the counter-rotation was only around 30%. In addition, across groups and conditions, deviations in head orientation
were smaller than deviations in balance board orientation. Our results suggest that the CoP mechanism is dominant, likely because the
counter-rotation mechanism would conict with stabilizing the orientation of the head in space.
Author contributions
MvdB: study design, data collection, data analysis, interpretation of data, manuscript preparation and editing.
SMB: study design, data analysis, interpretation of data and manuscript revision.
JS: interpretation of data and manuscript revision.
JHvD: study design, interpretation of data and manuscript revision.
PM: study design, interpretation of data and manuscript revision.
Funding
Sjoerd M. Bruijn was supported by grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientic Research (NWO #451-12-041 and 016.
Vidi.178.014).
Declarations of interest
None. The authors declare that they have no known competing nancial interests or personal relationships that could have
appeared to inuence the work reported in this paper.
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge The Expertise centre for Digital Media (EDM) for technical support and Kimmy Daenen and
Charlotte Uwents for their help during the experiments.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2022.102930.
References
Alizadehsaravi, L., Bruijn, S. M., & van Die¨
en, J. H. (2021). Balance training improves feedback control of perturbed balance in older adults. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/
10.1101/2021.03.31.437824, 2021.2003.2031.437824.
Allum, J. H. J., Carpenter, M. G., Honegger, F., Adkin, A. L., & Bloem, B. R. (2002). Age-dependent variations in the directional sensitivity of balance corrections and
compensatory arm movements in man. Journal of Physiology (London), 542(2), 643–663. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2001.015644
Bath, P. A., & Morgan, K. (1999). Differential risk factor proles for indoor and outdoor falls in older people living at home in Nottingham, UK. European Journal of
Epidemiology, 15(1), 65–73. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1007531101765
Bergamin, M., Gobbo, S., Zanotto, T., Sieverdes, J. C., Alberton, C. L., Zaccaria, M., & Ermolao, A. (2014). Inuence of age on postural sway during different dual-task
conditions. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, 271. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00271
van den Bogaart, M., Bruijn, S. M., van Dieen, J. H., & Meyns, P. (2020). The effect of anteroposterior perturbations on the control of the center of mass during
treadmill walking. Journal of Biomechanics, 103, Article 109660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2020.109660
Bugnariu, N., & Fung, J. (2006). Aging and selective sensorimotor strategies in the regulation of upright balance. In 2006 International workshop on virtual rehabilitation
(pp. 187-+).
Bugnariu, N., & Fung, J. (2007). Aging and selective sensorimotor strategies in the regulation of upright balance. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation, 4, 19.
https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-4-19
Day, B. L., Steiger, M. J., Thompson, P. D., & Marsden, C. D. (1993). Effect of vision and stance width on human body motion when standing: Implications for afferent
control of lateral sway. The Journal of Physiology, 469, 479–499. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019824
Demura, S., & Kitabayashi, T. (2006). Comparison of power spectrum characteristics of body sway during a static upright standing posture in healthy elderly people
and young adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102(2), 467–476. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.102.2.467-476
M. van den Bogaart et al.
Human Movement Science 82 (2022) 102930
10
Dickin, D. C., McClain, M. A., Hubble, R. P., Doan, J. B., & Sessford, D. (2012). Changes in postural sway frequency and complexity in altered sensory environments
following whole body vibrations. Human Movement Science, 31(5), 1238–1246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2011.12.007
van Dieen, J. H., van Leeuwen, M., & Faber, G. S. (2015). Learning to balance on one leg: Motor strategy and sensory weighting. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(5),
2967–2982. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00434.2015
Fino, P. C., Raffegeau, T. E., Parrington, L., Peterka, R. J., & King, L. A. (2020). Head stabilization during standing in people with persisting symptoms after mild
traumatic brain injury. Journal of Biomechanics, 112, Article 110045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2020.110045
Gatev, P., Thomas, S., Kepple, T., & Hallett, M. (1999). Feedforward ankle strategy of balance during quiet stance in adults. The Journal of Physiology, 514(Pt 3),
915–928. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.915ad.x
Haagsma, J. A., Graetz, N., Bolliger, I., Naghavi, M., Higashi, H., Mullany, E. C., … Vos, T. (2016). The global burden of injury: Incidence, mortality, disability-
adjusted life years and time trends from the Global Burden of Disease study 2013. Injury Prevention, 22(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1136/injuryprev-2015-
041616
Haagsma, J. A., Olij, B. F., Majdan, M., van Beeck, E. F., Vos, T., Castle, C. D., … Polinder, S. (2020). Falls in older aged adults in 22 European countries: Incidence,
mortality and burden of disease from 1990 to 2017. Injury Prevention. https://doi.org/10.1136/injuryprev-2019-043347
Hirabayashi, S., & Iwasaki, Y. (1995). Developmental perspective of sensory organization on postural control. Brain Development, 17(2), 111–113. https://doi.org/
10.1016/0387-7604(95)00009-z
Hof, A. L. (2007). The equations of motion for a standing human reveal three mechanisms for balance. Journal of Biomechanics, 40(2), 451–457. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jbiomech.2005.12.016
Horak, F. B. (1987). Clinical measurement of postural control in adults. Physical Therapy, 67(12), 1881–1885. https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/67.12.1881
Horak, F. B., & Hlavacka, F. (2001). Somatosensory loss increases vestibulospinal sensitivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86(2), 575–585.
Horak, F. B., & Nashner, L. M. (1986). Central programming of postural movements: Adaptation to altered support-surface congurations. Journal of Neurophysiology,
55(6), 1369–1381. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1986.55.6.1369
Hsu, Y. S., Kuan, C. C., & Young, Y. H. (2009). Assessing the development of balance function in children using stabilometry. International Journal of Pediatric
Otorhinolaryngology, 73(5), 737–740. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2009.01.016
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Carr, T. H., & Nigg, J. T. (2002). Development of selective attention: Perceptual load inuences early versus late attentional selection in children
and adults. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 363–375.
Khan, S., & Chang, R. (2013). Anatomy of the vestibular system: A review. NeuroRehabilitation, 32(3), 437–443. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-130866
MacLellan, M. J., & Patla, A. E. (2006). Adaptations of walking pattern on a compliant surface to regulate dynamic stability. Experimental Brain Research, 173(3),
521–530. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0399-5
Maki, B. E., Holliday, P. J., & Topper, A. K. (1994). A prospective study of postural balance and risk of falling in an ambulatory and independent elderly population.
Journal of Gerontology, 49(2), M72–M84. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/49.2.m72
Masani, K., Vette, A. H., Kouzaki, M., Kanehisa, H., Fukunaga, T., & Popovic, M. R. (2007). Larger center of pressure minus center of gravity in the elderly induces
larger body acceleration during quiet standing. Neuroscience Letters, 422(3), 202–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.06.019
Mathis, A., Mamidanna, P., Cury, K. M., Abe, T., Murthy, V. N., Mathis, M. W., & Bethge, M. (2018). DeepLabCut: Markerless pose estimation of user-dened body
parts with deep learning. Nature Neuroscience, 21(9), 1281–1289. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y
Morrison, A., Fan, T., Sen, S. S., & Weisenuh, L. (2013). Epidemiology of falls and osteoporotic fractures: A systematic review. Clinicoecon Outcomes Res, 5, 9–18.
https://doi.org/10.2147/CEOR.S38721
Nath, T., Mathis, A., Chen, A. C., Patel, A., Bethge, M., & Mathis, M. W. (2019). Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors.
Nature Protocols, 14(7), 2152–2176. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-019-0176-0
Oba, N., Sasagawa, S., Yamamoto, A., & Nakazawa, K. (2015). Difference in postural control during quiet standing between young children and adults: Assessment
with Center of Mass Acceleration. PLoS One, 10(10), Article e0140235. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140235
Otten, E. (1999). Balancing on a narrow ridge: Biomechanics and control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 354
(1385), 869–875. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1999.0439
Patel, M., Fransson, P. A., Lush, D., Petersen, H., Magnusson, M., Johansson, R., & Gomez, S. (2008). The effects of foam surface properties on standing body
movement. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 128(9), 952–960. https://doi.org/10.1080/00016480701827517
Peel, N. M. (2011). Epidemiology of falls in older age. Canadian Journal on Aging, 30(1), 7–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S071498081000070X
Peterka, R. J. (2002). Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88(3), 1097–1118. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097
de Rekeneire, N., Visser, M., Peila, R., Nevitt, M. C., Cauley, J. A., Tylavsky, F. A., … Harris, T. B. (2003). Is a fall just a fall: Correlates of falling in healthy older
persons. The health, aging and body composition study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 51(6), 841–846. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-
2389.2003.51267.x
Riach, C., & Starkes, J. (1989). Visual xation and postural sway in children. Journal of Motor Behavior, 21, 265–276. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00222895.1989.10735481
Riemann, B. L., Myers, J. B., & Lephart, S. M. (2003). Comparison of the ankle, knee, hip, and trunk corrective action shown during single-leg stance on rm, foam,
and multiaxial surfaces. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(1), 90–95. https://doi.org/10.1053/apmr.2003.50004
Schmider, E., Ziegler, M., Danay, E., Beyer, L., & Bühner, M. (2010). Is it really robust? Reinvestigating the robustness of ANOVA against violations of the normal
distribution assumption. Methodology European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social Sciences, 6(4), 147–151. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-
2241/a000016
Shams, A., Vameghi, R., Shamsipour Dehkordi, P., Allafan, N., & Bayati, M. (2020). The development of postural control among children: Repeatability and normative
data for computerized dynamic posturography system. Gait & Posture, 78, 40–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.03.002
Sminkey, L. (2008). World report on child injury prevention. Injury Prevention, 14(1), 69. https://doi.org/10.1136/ip.2007.018143
Steindl, R., Kunz, K., Schrott-Fischer, A., & Scholtz, A. W. (2006). Effect of age and sex on maturation of sensory systems and balance control. Developmental Medicine
and Child Neurology, 48(6), 477–482. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162206001022
Stel, V. S., Smit, J. H., Pluijm, S. M., & Lips, P. (2003). Balance and mobility performance as treatable risk factors for recurrent falling in older persons. Journal of
Clinical Epidemiology, 56(7), 659–668. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0895-4356(03)00082-9
Sturnieks, D. L., Arnold, R., & Lord, S. R. (2011). Validity and reliability of the Swaymeter device for measuring postural sway. BMC Geriatrics, 11, 63. https://doi.org/
10.1186/1471-2318-11-63
Teasdale, N., Stelmach, G. E., Breunig, A., & Meeuwsen, H. J. (1991). Age differences in visual sensory integration. Experimental Brain Research, 85(3), 691–696.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00231755
Wickens, C. D. (1974). Temporal limits of human information processing: A developmental study. Psychological Bulletin, 81(11), 739–755. https://doi.org/10.1037/
h0037250
Winter, D. A., Patla, A. E., Prince, F., Ishac, M., & Gielo-Perczak, K. (1998). Stiffness control of balance in quiet standing. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(3),
1211–1221. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1211
Winter, D. A., Prince, F., Frank, J. S., Powell, C., & Zabjek, K. F. (1996). Unied theory regarding A/P and M/L balance in quiet stance. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75
(6), 2334–2343. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1996.75.6.2334
Wu, G. (1998). The relation between age-related changes in neuromusculoskeletal system and dynamic postural responses to balance disturbance. The Journals of
Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 53(4), M320–M326. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/53a.4.m320
M. van den Bogaart et al.