ArticlePDF Available

The (over)use of SMART goals for physical activity promotion: A narrative review and critique

Authors:
  • Southern Cross University, Coffs Harbour

Abstract

The SMART acronym (e.g., Specific, Measurable, Achievable, Realistic, Timebound) is a highly prominent strategy for setting physical activity goals. While it is intuitive, and its practical value has been recognised, the scientific underpinnings of the SMART acronym are less clear. Therefore, we aimed to narratively review and critically examine the scientific underpinnings of the SMART acronym and its application in physical activity promotion. Specifically, our review suggests that the SMART acronym: is not based on scientific theory; is not consistent with empirical evidence; does not consider what type of goal is set; is not applied consistently; is lacking detailed guidance; has redundancy in its criteria; is not being used as originally intended; and has a risk of potentially harmful effects. These issues are likely leading to sub-optimal outcomes, confusion, and inconsistency. Recommendations are provided to guide the field towards better practice and, ultimately, more effective goal setting interventions to help individuals become physically active.
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 0
The (Over)Use of SMART Goals for Physical Activity Promotion: A Narrative Review and
Critique
Christian Swann1*, Patricia C. Jackman2, Alex Lawrence1, Rebecca M. Hawkins2, Scott G.
Goddard1, Ollie Williamson1,2, Matthew J. Schweickle3, Stewart A. Vella3, Simon Rosenbaum4 &
Panteleimon Ekkekakis5
1Faculty of Health, Southern Cross University, Coffs Harbour, Australia
2 School of Sport and Exercise Science, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
3 Global Alliance for Mental Health and Sport, School of Psychology, Faculty of Social
Sciences, University of Wollongong, Australia
4School of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of New South Wales, Australia
5Department of Kinesiology, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa, U.S.A
*Corresponding author: A/Prof Christian Swann, Faculty of Health, Southern Cross University,
Coffs Harbour, NSW 2450, Australia. Email: christian.swann@scu.edu.au; phone: +61 6659
3063
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 1
Abstract
The SMART acronym (e.g., Specific, Measurable, Achievable, Realistic, Timebound) is a highly
prominent strategy for setting physical activity goals. While it is intuitive, and its practical value
has been recognised, the scientific underpinnings of the SMART acronym are less clear.
Therefore, we aimed to narratively review and critically examine the scientific underpinnings of
the SMART acronym and its application in physical activity promotion. Specifically, our review
suggests that the SMART acronym: is not based on scientific theory; is not consistent with
empirical evidence; does not consider what type of goal is set; is not applied consistently; is
lacking detailed guidance; has redundancy in its criteria; is not being used as originally intended;
and has a risk of potentially harmful effects. These issues are likely leading to sub-optimal
outcomes, confusion, and inconsistency. Recommendations are provided to guide the field
towards better practice and, ultimately, more effective goal setting interventions to help
individuals become more physically active.
Keywords: exercise; goal-setting; health; motivation; psychology.
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 2
The (Over)Use of SMART Goals for Physical Activity Promotion: A Narrative Review and
Critique
A goal is defined as what an individual is trying to accomplish – it is the aim or object of
an action (Locke et al., 1981), such as to attain a specific standard of proficiency on a task
(Locke & Latham, 2002). Goal-setting is one of the most commonly used behaviour-change
techniques in interventions designed to increase physical activity (Howlett et al., 2019)1.
Furthermore, goal-setting is an effective behaviour change technique (Epton et al., 2017) that
achieves substantive improvements in physical activity (McEwan et al., 2016). In assisting their
clients to set appropriate goals, most public health and physical activity professionals are advised
to use the "SMART" heuristic (Doran, 1981), which was first proposed as a mnemonic rule in a
965-word article published in Management Review by George Doran – a business consultant. In
its original version, the acronym stood for Specific, Measurable, Assignable ("specify who will
do it"), Realistic, and Time-related, but it is now commonly interpreted as the setting of Specific,
Measurable, Achievable, Realistic, and Time-bound goals.
The appeal of the SMART heuristic lies in its simplicity and memorability (McPherson et
al., 2015), and its broad dissemination among professionals and health organisations is testament
to its apparent practicality. For example, SMART goals are recommended to practitioners by
leading health organisations, such as: the National Health Service (e.g., NHS Health Trainer
Handbook, Michie et al., 2008) and Moving Medicine (e.g., Active Lifestyles Workbook, 2018)
in the U.K.; the American College of Sports Medicine (2017); and The Royal Australian College
of General Practitioners (2018). Indeed, some policies, such as the Clinical Framework for the
1 Researchers suggest that successful goal pursuit involves two sequential tasks: goal setting and goal
implementation (e.g., Oettingen & Gollwitzer, 2010). This review focuses on the SMART heuristic as a strategy for
goal setting. It should be acknowledged, however, that there is extensive research on strategies for goal
implementation, such as implementation intentions (e.g., Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006). While SMART goals and
implementation intentions may be complementary (see Bailey, 2019), research on implementation intentions is not
included in this review as our focus is on the task of goal setting rather than goal implementation.
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
1
2
3
4
5
6
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 3
Delivery of Health Services (Worksafe Victoria & Transport Accident Commission, 2012) in
Australia require that practitioners/clinicians set SMART goals with clients (e.g., in order to
have treatment plans approved). The implementation of SMART goals for physical activity is
also prevalent beyond professional practice. Many health organisations and government
agencies, including healthdirect in Australia (https://www.healthdirect.gov.au/goal-setting),
provide guidance to the general public to set their own SMART goals as a way of increasing
physical activity.
The practicality of the SMART heuristic is widely recognised, but its scientific
foundations in physical activity promotion are less well understood. Indeed, the SMART
heuristic was first published over 40 years ago (Doran, 1981), and since then, research in both
goal-setting and physical activity promotion has developed extensively (e.g., Locke & Latham,
2013, 2019). It is therefore important, and timely, to re-examine the scientific foundations
underpinning the SMART heuristic – especially given how widely the SMART heuristic is used
in physical activity promotion – and how prominently it influences goal-setting practice in this
field.
The purpose of this review was to critically examine the scientific underpinnings of the
SMART heuristic to: (i) address key questions relating to the use of SMART goals in physical
activity promotion; (ii) help practitioners, researchers, and organisations make more informed
decisions around whether to use the SMART heuristic to set goals, and what limitations to be
aware of if they do choose to use it; and (iii) present a starting point for discussion, and
suggestions, on how the field might move beyond its current reliance on setting SMART goals
for physical activity promotion. Ultimately, we seek to ensure that goal-setting practices are up
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 4
to date with the latest theory and empirical evidence, and are implemented in ways that maximise
their likely effectiveness in overcoming physical inactivity.
To do so, we conducted a narrative review of the use of the SMART heuristic in physical
activity promotion. Specifically, this review critically examined existing literature on goal-
setting and physical activity in terms of: (i) key questions about the use of SMART goals for
physical activity promotion; (ii) implications; and (iii) future directions for research, practice,
and education on goal-setting in physical activity promotion. A narrative review (Ferrari, 2015;
Greenhalgh et al., 2018) was considered most appropriate (e.g., compared to a systematic review
and/or meta-analysis) as the focus was primarily conceptual.
To inform this narrative review, we conducted a literature search and identified 147
relevant studies. Relevant articles were retrieved (August 6th 2020) through a combination of: (i)
checking forward searches for the original paper on SMART goals by Doran (1981) on Google
Scholar with the terms “exercise OR physical activity”; (ii) electronic database searches
(Academic Search Complete; APA PsycARTICLES; APA PsycINFO; CINAHL Complete;
MEDLINE; PubMed; Scopus; and SportsDISCUS) with the search terms “SMART goal*” AND
“exer* OR physical* activ*” (see Appendix 1 for full electronic database searches); and (iii)
checking included studies and forward citations of a meta-analysis on goal-setting in physical
activity (McEwan et al., 2016) and review papers retrieved by the electronic searches. All
records were screened for relevance by the second and fourth authors independently in three
stages: (i) title and source; (ii) abstract; and (iii) full text. Studies were considered relevant if
they: (i) were original, empirical studies or protocol papers that referred to the implementation of
SMART goals in exercise or physical activity; (ii) evaluated the use of SMART goals in physical
activity promotion; or (iii) were articles that advocated the use of, or detailed programs that used,
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 5
SMART goals in physical activity. The 147 identified studies (see Appendix 2) were used to
inform the critiques presented in the following section, using a narrative review approach
(Greenhalgh et al., 2018).
Key Questions about the use of SMART Goals for Physical Activity Promotion
To guide this review, a series of questions were identified through: literature on SMART
goals in other fields (e.g., rehabilitation – McPherson et al., 2015; organisational psychology –
Rubin, 2002; education – Day & Tosey, 2011); recent developments in goal-setting (e.g., Locke
& Latham, 2013); and common assumptions and advice provided on setting SMART goals.
Combined, these questions were intended to provide a critical examination of the scientific
foundations of SMART goals in physical activity promotion.
Is the SMART Heuristic Based on Theory?
It is often considered best practice to use theory to guide interventions in health
promotion (Glanz & Rimer, 2005; Prestwich et al., 2015). While interventions based on theory
are not guaranteed to succeed, they are much more likely to produce desired outcomes (Glanz &
Rimer, 2005). Although some researchers, practitioners, and policy makers may believe that the
SMART heuristic was based on the well-known goal-setting theory proposed by Locke and
Latham (1990), it is important to clarify that this is not the case. Doran’s (1981) article was
unaccompanied by references to any theoretical framework or supporting empirical evidence
(i.e., Doran never explicitly connected the SMART heuristic to goal-setting theory). This is in
stark contrast to goal-setting theory, which is reportedly based on over 1,000 empirical studies
across a broad range of tasks and settings (Locke & Latham, 2019). Locke and Latham have
acknowledged the existence and potential utility of the SMART acronym, when pointing out, for
example, that practitioners typically adhere to the SMART acronym when setting goals in
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 6
organisational settings (Locke & Latham, 2015), and characterising SMART as a "useful
technique designed to help you remember the characteristics of well-thought-out career goals"
(Mealiea & Latham, 1996, p. 35). However, contrary to common impression, the SMART
heuristic is not derived from goal-setting theory and has important differences compared to the
postulates of Locke and Latham’s theory.
One important difference is that the SMART heuristic focuses on achievable/realistic
goals, whereas goal-setting theory encourages the setting of challenging/difficult goals. Goal-
setting theory advocates, and distinguishes between, performance and learning goals – both of
which should be specific and challenging to produce optimal results (e.g., Locke & Latham,
2013). Specific, challenging performance goals focus on the achievement of specific tasks
through certain standards of proficiency (Tasa et al., 2013), with examples including to ‘aim for
10,000 steps per day’ or to ‘increase last week’s average by 1500 steps’. Specific, challenging
performance goals are proposed to be appropriate when four moderators are present
(commitment, feedback, ability, and resources) and work through four mechanisms (choice,
effort, persistence, and cueing of existing strategies; Locke & Latham, 2013). In a conceptual
review and update of the application of goal-setting theory to physical activity promotion, Swann
et al. (2021) suggested that specific, challenging performance goals appear to be most
theoretically appropriate for those who are already active (i.e., those for whom the four
moderators of commitment, ability, knowledge, and resources are already present). Instead,
according to recent developments in goal-setting theory, it now appears that specific, challenging
learning goals may be most theoretically appropriate for inactive individuals (see Swann et al.,
2021 for discussion).
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 7
Specific, challenging learning goals are defined as “a desired number of strategies,
processes, or procedures to be developed in order to master a task” (Locke & Latham, 2015, p.
116), with examples such as to ‘identify three effective strategies to increase your daily step
count’ (Swann et al., 2021). In terms of moderators, learning goals are appropriate when: the
individual is new to a complex task or when an individual lacks the ability to perform the task
(Locke & Latham, 2015; Seijts et al., 2013); learning rather than just performance motivation is
required (Latham & Locke, 2006; Williams, 2013); individuals have lower cognitive ability
(Latham et al., 2008); and negative feedback is given on the performance of a task that is
experienced as highly complex (Locke & Latham, 2019). In terms of mediators, learning goals
focus more on searching, planning, monitoring, evaluating, and generating ideas or strategies
(see Swann et al., 2021). Doran (1981) mentioned neither moderators nor mechanisms when
proposing SMART goals.
Put simply, the SMART heuristic is not a theory-based strategy. Therefore, it is not
underpinned by a framework to generate research questions and test hypotheses, or aid
explanation, prediction and control (Doherty, 2013; Prestwich et al., 2015). Furthermore, the
SMART acronym does not explain how such goals are proposed to work and does not consider
moderators that denote when a SMART goal is (and is not) appropriate. The SMART heuristic,
therefore, should be considered less likely to be effective than interventions based on formal
goal-setting theory.
Is the SMART Heuristic Consistent with Empirical Evidence?
It is generally presumed that all elements of the SMART acronym are unambiguously
supported by empirical evidence, especially in the context of physical activity. In actuality, the
simplicity of SMART belies a far more complex pattern of empirical findings. The following
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 8
section discusses two key issues on which the SMART acronym is not consistent with empirical
evidence.
Goals do not need to be specific to be effective. A cornerstone of SMART goals is that
goals must be specific: all studies found in our review that detailed the meaning of S (k = 101)
stated that it referred to Specific. However, a meta-analysis of goal-setting interventions in the
context of physical activity found no significant difference between specific (k = 31, d = 0.589,
95% C.I. 0.43-0.75) and vague or unclear (k = 21, d = 0.511, 95% C.I. 0.33-0.70) goals
(McEwan et al., 2016). That is, goals do not need to be specific to be effective at increasing
physical activity. McEwan et al. (2016) explained that although this finding may be surprising
given the common assumption that specific goals are superior to vague goals, it is actually in line
with goal setting theory. For example, Latham and Locke (1991) noted that “trying for specific,
challenging goals may actually hurt performance in certain circumstances” such as “in the early
stages of learning a new, complex task” (p. 229). The majority of the samples included in the
McEwan et al. (2016) meta-analysis comprised participants who were insufficiently active at
baseline, meaning it is possible that vague goals were advantageous for these participants as they
were in the early stages of learning to be physically active.
Furthermore, emerging research also indicates that there can be benefits to setting non-
specific goals in physical activity. Non-specific goals were found to lead to at least as much
(Swann et al., 2020a, 2020b), if not more (Hawkins et al., 2020) physical activity for
insufficiently active adults. Collectively, this indicates that there is empirical evidence and a
theoretical rationale to suggest that goals do not need to be specific to be effective at increasing
physical activity, thus contesting the appropriateness of the common use of ‘Specific’ within
SMART goals.
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 9
Goals should be ‘challenging’ rather than ‘achievable’ or ‘realistic’. According to
common interpretation of the SMART acronym, goals should be Achievable or Realistic (e.g.,
51% and 72% of studies reporting the meaning of A [k = 101] and R [k = 100] in our search
reported these interpretations, respectively). However, extensive research suggests that goals
should be challenging to produce optimal outcomes (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2013). These
recommendations are based on empirical evidence collated over many decades and through
various research designs and samples (Locke & Latham, 2013), including research in behaviour
change (e.g., Epton et al., 2017) and physical activity. For example, Anson and Madras (2016)
found that participants with high/challenging goals walked more than participants with low
goals, even if they did not meet the assigned goal; Chevance et al. (2021) reported that the
difficulty of performance goals was positively and significantly associated with physical activity
– that is, more challenging performance goals led to larger increases in physical activity; and
Gao and Podlog (2012) found that children's physical activity levels increased significantly more
in a difficult-goal group than those in an easy-goal group. In short, Achievable/Realistic goals
are not in line with the best available evidence and the SMART acronym is missing
‘challenging’ as a core criterion. While users of the SMART acronym may assume or interpret
that goals should be challenging yet achievable/realistic, it remains the case that ‘challenging’ is
not included as a SMART criterion, despite being recognised as highly important in goal-setting
research. Therefore, the SMART acronym is not consistent with extensive research on goal
specificity or difficulty (i.e., the Achievable, Realistic criteria), suggesting that SMART goals
are unlikely to lead to optimal physical activity outcomes.
Does the SMART Heuristic Consider What Type of Goal is Set?
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 10
The SMART acronym ignores one of the most fundamental considerations in goal-setting
research, namely that there are many types of goals, each of which can lead to different
performance and psychological outcomes. There are over 20 types of goal, each of which can
impact differently on the individual’s performance and experience (Grant, 2012). Prominent
examples of goal distinctions include performance vs. learning goals in goal-setting theory
(Locke & Latham, 2013) and behavioural vs. outcome goals as behaviour change techniques
(e.g., Carey et al., 2019). Therefore, all goals are not equal – and each type of goal can result in
different performance and psychological outcomes for an individual. However, the SMART
acronym does not consider which type of goal is set, and contrasting goal types could still be
‘SMART’.
There are also a variety of other considerations essential to goal-setting that are not
accounted for in the SMART acronym. For example, the SMART acronym does not address
whether goals should be: positively or negatively valenced; set with normative (i.e., comparison
against others) or intrapersonal (i.e., self-referenced) framing (Duda & Balageur, 2014); or short-
term (e.g., today or this week) vs. long-term (e.g., this year, or on an ongoing basis) goals. These
issues suggest that the SMART acronym is likely causing those setting goals to overlook one of
the most fundamental considerations in goal-setting, leading to inconsistency in the goal types
set. Table 1 illustrates how two goals could both meet the SMART criteria, yet vary substantially
based on these important considerations.
Is the SMART Heuristic Applied Consistently?
Over the years, and given the absence of a link to a specific underlying theoretical
framework, different authors have taken the liberty to modify what the acronym SMART stands
for, to emphasise elements that each considered more relevant or important for a particular
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 11
Table 1: Examples of how SMART goals are missing important additional considerations
Example SMART
goal
Evidence of SMART criteria Additional
considerations
For the week of the
1st of April, I aim to
increase my daily
average step count
compared to the
previous week
Specific: average weekly step count Positively
valenced;
approach;
intrapersonal,
short-term
Measurable: average daily step count in relation to
previous week
Achievable: increase compared to previous week
Realistic: increase compared to previous week
Timebound: week of the 1st of April
By the end of the year,
I aim to avoid
finishing last place in
my walking group for
daily average step
counts
Specific: placing in walking group Negatively
valenced;
avoidance;
normative;
long-term
Measurable: average daily step count; placing in
walking group
Achievable: avoid recording lowest step count
Realistic: avoid recording lowest step count
Timebound: by end of year
context. At present, there is no single, agreed upon, consistently applied approach to SMART
goals (Levack et al., 2015), with Wade (2009) identifying over 55 possible terms that could be
used as SMART criteria in rehabilitation. In the 147 studies that we identified, 34 terms were
identified (see Table 2). Of equal concern is the absence of a clear definition of what SMART
stands for. Only two-thirds (65.97%; k = 97) of the 147 studies included in our review specified
their interpretation of all five letters of the SMART acronym, with six studies only specifying
what 2-4 letters stood for, and the remainder (k = 44) failing to provide any details on the
interpretation employed. Furthermore, two papers provided multiple interpretations of the same
letters.
These definitional issues are also evident in practice, where different variations exist in
how the SMART heuristic is phrased between health organisations and policies. For example,
The Royal Australian College of General Practitioners guidelines for the implementation of
prevention in general-practice settings (2018) outlines that A refers to Assignable; whereas the
Australian Clinical Framework for the Delivery of Health Services states that A refers to
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 12
Table 2: SMART goal interpretations reported in studies included in the review.
SMART letter
(variations) Label (number of citing studies)1
S (1) Specific (k = 101)
M (3) Measurable (k = 99), meaningful (k = 1); manageable (k = 1)
A (8) Achievable (k = 52), attainable (k = 31), acceptable (k = 6), action-oriented
(k = 5), assignable (k = 3), action-based (k = 2), agreed (k = 1), attractive (k
= 1)
R (4) Realistic (k = 72), relevant (k = 27), results-focused (k = 1), recorded (k = 1)
T (18) Time-bound (k = 25), timely (k = 25), time-based (k = 11), timed (k = 10),
time-specific (k = 6), time-framed (k = 5), time-related (k = 5), time time-
targeted (k = 3), time (k = 2), time-limited (k = 2), timeline (k = 1), with a
timeline (k = 1), have a target date (k = 1), time-oriented (k = 1), -sensitive (k
= 1), with a given time frame (k = 1), with time frames (k = 1)
Notes: (1) 103 studies in our search reported information on at least two letters of the SMART
acronym, but six of these studies did not report all five letters; (2) two studies reported two
interpretations for some letters (Armburst et al., 2015; Rieder et al., 2018)
Achievable. Similarly, the government healthdirect website
(https://www.healthdirect.gov.au/goal-setting) states that the “R” refers to “Realistic”, whereas
the Clinical Framework for Delivery of Health Services that the “R” refers to “Relevant”. This
inconsistency is likely to create confusion.
Authors have also introduced extensions of the SMART acronym, such as SMARTS and
SMARTER. As a case in point, the American College of Sports Medicine (2017), without citing
a source or supporting evidence, refers to the "SMARTS principle," with the added "S" referring
to "Self-determined" ("Goals should be developed primarily by the client/patient"). Indeed, the
additional letters also acquire various definitions, such as Ethical, Enjoyable, or Evaluated as the
added “E” in SMARTER. Rubin (2002) aptly characterised this phenomenon as "acronym drift",
whereby common interpretations have strayed away from the original intentions or principles
upon which the acronym was based. Indeed, this indicates that researchers believe that the
SMART acronym, as it stands, is insufficient or unacceptable, and needs to be modified or
extended.
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 13
Is there Sufficient Detail in the SMART Criteria?
A further critique is that there is insufficient detail in certain criteria, such as effective
ways of setting “timebound” SMART goals. A meta-analysis (McEwan et al., 2016) found that
daily goals, such as to aim for 10,000 steps per day (k = 28, d = 0.600, 95% CI [0.44, 0.76]), and
daily-plus-weekly goals, such as to aim for 30 minutes of physical activity per day and at least
150 minutes per week (k = 2, d = 0.947, 95% CI [0.45, 1.45]), were effective for increasing
physical activity; however, weekly goals such as 150 minutes of moderate-intensity physical
activity per week (k = 7, d = 0.152, 95% CI [-0.14, 0.45]) were not. This missing detail in the
SMART heuristic could determine whether the goal is efficacious; that is, two SMART goals
could be phrased almost identically, but one could be effective (based on daily or daily-plus-
weekly goals), whereas the other may not (based on weekly goals only). Furthermore, there is
little evidence to suggest whether ‘timebound’ goals should be set as a one-off (e.g., “I will aim
to reach 10,000 steps today”) or on an ongoing basis (e.g., “I will aim to reach 10,000 steps
every day”), which could also affect the efficacy of the goal. Similarly, it is difficult to pinpoint
the meaning of ‘specific’. Doran’s (1981) guidance referred to “target a specific area for
improvement” (p. 36), but this could be interpreted in many ways in the context of physical
activity, such as: increasing one’s step count; going to the gym; finishing in a certain position on
a leaderboard; or parking farther away from the workplace. Therefore, important aspects of the
SMART heuristic are lacking crucial details, which could negatively affect the efficacy of the
goals subsequently set.
Are all SMART Criteria Necessary?
Common interpretation of the SMART acronym is based on Specific (reported by 100%
of studies we found), Measurable (98% of studies), Achievable (51%), Realistic (70%), and
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 14
Timebound (100% of studies reported a variation relating to time) criteria. However, there is
often redundancy between criteria in the SMART acronym. For example, ‘measurable’ goals are
already ‘specific’, so there is little need for both terms (Levack, 2018). Similarly, the terms
‘realistic’ and ‘achievable’ arguably refer to redundant ideas. On this basis, two of the five
criteria identified in common interpretation of the SMART acronym are not necessary and could
create confusion for those seeking to set goals that uniquely address all criteria.
Is the SMART Heuristic Being Used as Originally Intended?
Common interpretation and application of the SMART acronym in physical activity
promotion are different to that originally intended by Doran (1981). For Doran, the “A” referred
to “Assignable”, which differs from the interpretation of “Achievable” commonly found in
physical activity promotion, with only three of the 147 studies we identified specifying A as
“Assignable”. Importantly, Doran (1981) originally noted that "the suggested acronym doesn't
mean that every objective written will have all five criteria" (p. 36); that is, goals can be set by
only selecting certain criteria depending on what is relevant. Indeed, Doran (1981) emphasised
that “in some situations it is not realistic to attempt quantification” (p. 36). Thus, according to the
original conceptualisation, not all criteria in the SMART heuristic need to be used, and it has
even been recognised that SMART goals may not be appropriate in certain circumstances at all.
These considerations appear to have been forgotten or overlooked in physical activity promotion,
where it appears to be almost universally assumed that all criteria must be employed, and there is
often no process to check whether the SMART acronym is appropriate before use. As an
example, the Clinical Framework for the Delivery of Health Services in Australia states that
“Goals should be SMART: specific, measurable, achievable, relevant and timed” (emphasis
added; p. 13), and that “All healthcare professionals providing services…are expected to adopt
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 15
these principles” (p. 2). That is, practitioners are required to set SMART goals in all cases, and
are expected to adhere to all of the SMART criteria, in contrast to Doran’s original guidance.
Are There Risks in Setting SMART Goals?
McPherson et al. (2015) raised the question of whether SMART goals could be harmful,
and there is increasing awareness in goal-setting literature that inappropriately set goals can have
harmful or detrimental effects. These effects include: stress, anxiety, pressure, and threat
appraisals; inhibition of learning; unethical behaviour; and perceptions of failure (Ordonez et al.,
2009; Drach-Zahavy & Erez, 2002; Locke & Latham, 2006). There are examples of such
detrimental effects within physical activity studies. Swann et al. (2020a) found that
individualised SMART goals elicited significantly higher pressure/tension than open goals for
healthy adults undertaking a walking task. An example of unethical behaviour was reported in
China, where health insurance companies offer discounted premiums for individuals who
consistently achieve certain daily step count targets. This has led to the development of
electronic cradles designed to rock smartphones and ‘cheat the step counter’ (Morris, 2019).
There is also evidence of high rates of failure to achieve physical activity goals. For example,
even ‘easy’ physical activity goals - such as a 10% increase in one’s average step count - can
lead to high rates of failure (31%) in brief interventions (Moon et al., 2016). Similarly, in a three
week intervention, participants who self-set a challenging yet realistic step count goal only
achieved it around 50% of the time (Sidman et al., 2004).
Furthrmore, emerging evidence suggests that there could be risks and adverse effects to
pursuing SMART goals within physical activity. Hawkins et al. (2020) found that insufficiently
active participants (relative to recommended physical activity guidelines) walked significantly
further when given an open goal (to ‘see how far you can walk in six minutes’) compared to an
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 16
individualised SMART goal (e.g., to ‘walk 450 metres in six minutes’). That is, insufficiently
active participants completed significantly less physical activity (i.e., distance walked) when
pursuing SMART goals. Insufficiently active participants also reported significantly lower
enjoyment, pleasure, perceptions of performance, and motivation when pursuing SMART goals
compared to open goals (Hawkins et al., 2020). The finding that SMART goals led to
significantly less pleasure during physical activity compared to open goals (Hawkins et al., 2020)
is particularly important as, for example, a systematic review found that affect during exercise
predicts future physical activity (Rhodes & Kates, 2015). As such, this initial evidence suggests
that pursuing SMART goals could be creating less pleasurable experiences during physical
activity for insufficiently active participants, which could ultimately lead to harmful
consequences such as dropout. Overall, there is evidence to suggest that inappropriately set
goals, including SMART goals, may have risks of inferior physical activity outcomes, as well as
harmful effects on important psychological predictors of long-term engagement in physical
activity.
Summary
Despite widespread adoption, it is evident that there are many concerns and
misconceptions related to the use of SMART goals for physical activity promotion. Specifically,
the SMART acronym: (i) is not based on scientific theory; (ii) is not consistent with empirical
evidence; (iii) does not consider what type of goal is set; (iv) is not applied consistently; (v) is
lacking detailed guidance; (vi) has redundancy in its criteria; (vii) is not being used as originally
intended; and (viii) has a risk of potentially harmful effects. Table 3 provides a summary and
explanation of these issues. It is important for practitioners, researchers, and organisations to be
aware of these issues in order to make informed decisions around whether to use the SMART
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 17
heuristic in their goal-setting practice and, if it is used, what the potential risks and pitfalls may
be. The following section discusses the implications of these issues for goal-setting in physical
activity promotion.
Implications
There are several important consequences and implications of the issues identified above.
First, there is a risk of sub-optimal outcomes. McPherson et al. (2015) noted that “Very little
evidence…exists to suggest SMART goals are the most effective strategy to enhance the goal-
setting process or [improve] goal-related outcomes” (p. 107). Core properties of the SMART
acronym are not in line with the best available evidence (e.g., ‘achievable’ and ‘realistic’ goals
are set rather than ‘challenging/difficult’ goals) or have not been supported strongly by evidence
in physical activity (e.g., specific goals were found to be no more effective than vague goals such
as to ‘be more active’ – McEwan et al., 2016). Therefore, interventions based on SMART goals
do not appear to provide insufficiently active individuals with best-practice goal setting support,
which risks sub-optimal outcomes.
Second, and relatedly, there is a risk of research waste where funding is awarded to
research based on SMART goals – despite limited evidence that SMART goals are more
effective than other goal-setting approaches (McPherson et al., 2011). That is, where funding is
being invested into research incorporating SMART goals, it is important for funders to know that
this strategy lacks evidence of comparative efficacy, and they are likely investing in programs
that are not as effective, or cost-effective, as they could be.
Third, there is a risk of detrimental outcomes for individuals. Setting an inappropriate
type of goal can have detrimental effects on attempts to achieve the desired behaviour,
depending on theory-specified contextual and other moderating factors (e.g., knowledge,
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 18
Table 3: Summary of issues and misconceptions in the use of SMART goals for physical activity promotion
Problem Explanation of Problem
1 The SMART heuristic is not
based on scientific theory
The SMART heuristic does not explain how such goals are proposed to work, and does not consider
moderators that denote when a SMART goal is (and is not) appropriate. Therefore, it is less likely to
produce desired outcomes (Glanz & Rimer, 2005).
2 Aspects of the SMART
heuristic are not consistent
with empirical evidence
Against common understanding, physical activity goals do not need to be specific to be effective
(McEwan et al., 2015), while challenging/difficult goals are typically considered to be more effective
than ‘Achievable’ goals (Locke & Latham, 2013, 2015).
3 The SMART heuristic does not
consider what type of goal is
set
There are many types of goal (Grant, 2012), any of which could be stated according to the SMART
heuristic, and the wrong type of goal in the wrong context can be detrimental to the individual and their
attempts to achieve the desired behaviour (Latham & Locke, 2006; Ordonez et al., 2009).
4 The SMART heuristic is not
applied consistently
There is no single, agreed, consistently-applied approach to SMART goals (Wade, 2009; Levack et al.,
2015). Over 34 terms were identified in this review, for example, R can stand for realistic, relevant,
results-focused, or recorded, while A can stand for action-oriented, assignable, attainable, or achievable
5 There is insufficient detail in
the SMART criteria
For example, there is little guidance around optimal timeframes when setting goals. Daily, and daily plus
weekly, goals were found to be effective for increasing physical activity, but weekly goals were not
(McEwan et al., 2015).
6 There is redundancy/repetition
in the SMART criteria
Some commonly-used criteria stand for the same things, such as ‘achievable’ and ‘realistic’, and
‘specific’ and ‘measurable’ and therefore do not need to be stated twice (Levack, 2018)
7 The SMART heuristic is not
being used as originally
intended
It is typically assumed that all aspects of the SMART acronym should be incorporated into a physical
activity goal, however Doran (1981) originally stated that: “it should also be understood that the
suggested acronym doesn’t mean every objective will have all five criteria” (p.36).
8 SMART goals have potentially
harmful effects for
insufficiently active
individuals
Studies have found SMART goals can lead to inferior and potentially detrimental outcomes for
insufficiently active participants, including significantly less physical activity, enjoyment, pleasure,
motivation, and perceptions of performance, as well as significantly greater pressure/tension (Hawkins et
al., 2020; Swann et al., 2020a).
389
390
391
392
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 19
commitment, ability, or available resources – Latham & Locke, 2006). Goal-setting research has
identified that such detrimental effects can include: reduced efficacy; perceptions of failure;
stress, anxiety and threat appraisals; and unethical behaviour (Latham & Locke, 2006; Ordonez
et al., 2009). As noted above, there are examples of these detrimental effects within physical
activity, including: reduced efficacy (e.g., McEwan et al., 2016 who found that specific goals
were no more effective than vague goals such as to ‘be more active’); high rates of failure to
achieve physical activity goals (Sidman et al., 2004; Moon et al., 2016); increased
pressure/tension arising from individualised SMART goals (Swann et al., 2020a); and unethical
behaviour such as ‘cheating’ in pursuit of physical activity targets (e.g., Morris, 2019).
Fourth, the current approach is undermining the evidence base on SMART goals for
physical activity promotion because of inconsistent application, which is leading to difficulty in
synthesising outcomes. This means that it is difficult to assess the efficacy of SMART goals or
synthesise the outcomes of research to date. Without appropriate research synthesis, it would be
difficult to build confidence in the use of SMART goals.
Fifth, there is a risk that a narrow focus on SMART - by encouraging (e.g., Moving
Medicine, ACSM, RACGP) or requiring (e.g., Clinical Framework for the Delivery of Health
Services) the use of SMART goals - may constrain practitioners’ use of other goal-setting
approaches. That is, even if practitioners are equipped with other evidence-based approaches, or
question the efficacy of SMART goals (e.g., based on observing suboptimal outcomes), they
may not be able to use alternatives due to policy requirements. In combination, there may be
potential risks and implications associated with the use of SMART goals that need to be
considered carefully when deciding whether to use this heuristic. The following section
considers how the field might alleviate/avoid these risks and move beyond the current
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 20
overreliance on SMART goals for physical activity promotion. These considerations ensure that
goal-setting practices are up to date with the latest theory and empirical research, and maximise
likely effectiveness.
Moving Forward
Given the current reliance on SMART goals for physical activity promotion, and the
issues identified in this review, we suggest that more sophisticated, defensible, and evidence-
based (yet equally as practical) guidance on goal-setting is needed in physical activity promotion.
This conclusion is in line with the perspective taken by other fields, such as goal-setting in
rehabilitation, which has raised critiques of SMART goals since 2009 (Playford et al., 2009;
Wade, 2009). Researchers on goal-setting in rehabilitation reflected that:
For a time, goal-setting was, arguably, viewed as a rather simple but effective
[strategy]...Since 2000, there has been a dramatic growth in our application of some of
the complexities surrounding the application of goal-setting…[including] increasing
recognition that an atheoretical approach to goal-setting had proliferated and that current
approaches to clinical practice do not always match goal theory or have yet to be
substantiated in rehabilitation contexts (Siegert & Levack, 2014, pp. 377-378).
Similar issues are now also apparent in physical activity promotion through overreliance on the
SMART acronym. The following sections draw upon principles proposed in the rehabilitation
literature to provide suggestions on how physical activity promotion may move forward in light
of the issues identified in this review.
Prioritise Theory-Based Goal-Setting
Interventions to change health-related behaviours will have a greater chance of
effectiveness if they are grounded in appropriate theory (Carey et al., 2019; Glanz & Rimer,
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 21
2005). Indeed, theory-based interventions are proposed to be a key element in the development
of a more sophisticated approach to goal-setting (Siegert & Levack, 2014). As noted above, the
SMART acronym is not based on a particular theory. Therefore, a key recommendation moving
forward is that researchers, practitioners and organisations prioritise theory-based goal-setting
interventions2. Theories relevant to goal-setting include: goal-setting theory; achievement goal
theory; self-regulation theory; self-determination theory; and social cognitive theory (see
Pritchard-Wiart et al., 2019 for a review and comparison).
As an example, the process for setting goals in line with Locke and Latham’s (2013)
goal-setting theory should involve an initial assessment of necessary moderators: commitment,
knowledge, resources, and ability. If all of these moderators are present, a specific, challenging
performance goal should be set (e.g., to reach 10,000 steps per day), with careful checking to
ensure that the goal is perceived to be challenging/difficult by the individual. If any of these
moderators are not present, then a specific, challenging learning goal (e.g., to identify and
implement five ways to increase your daily step count) should be set; again with careful
checking to ensure that the goal is perceived to be challenging/difficult by the individual (i.e., in
terms of the number of strategies the individual needs to identify and implement). If a learning
goal is set, the moderators should be monitored, and once commitment, knowledge, resources,
and ability are developed, then specific, challenging performance goals should be set instead (see
Locke & Latham, 2013 for an overview of goal-setting theory, and Swann et al., 2021 for a
review of the application of goal-setting theory in physical activity).
By drawing upon relevant theory, goal-setting research and practice will be underpinned
by an appropriate framework to: aid explanation, prediction and control; generate research
2 Where other promising approaches are identified, such as process goals (Kingston & Hardy, 1997) and open goals
(Hawkins et al., 2020; Swann et al., 2020), efforts should be made to either develop explanatory theory or integrate
them into and/or compare them with existing theory.
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
7
8
9
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 22
questions and test hypotheses; help consider the circumstances in which particular goals are –
and are not – appropriate; and explain how those goals are proposed to work (Doherty, 2013;
Prestwich et al., 2015). This perspective is consistent with the extensive literature on behaviour
change theories and techniques of behaviour change: identifying a theoretical basis for an
intervention guides the choice of behaviour change techniques, and is likely to be more effective
(e.g., Michie & Johnson, 2012). As such, theory-based goal-setting is important moving forward,
and is likely to be more effective at increasing physical activity. Compared to the SMART
acronym, guidance grounded in theory is more likely to be informative for practitioners, and
likely to result in more effective goal-setting with better physical activity outcomes for inactive
individuals.
Embrace Diverse Approaches to Goal-Setting
It is often assumed that the SMART heuristic is the best, or only, approach to goal-
setting. However, the science of goal-setting is robust with many evidence-based approaches
(including the theories noted above). Therefore, a second important recommendation is for
organisations and practitioners to embrace a more diverse range of goal-setting perspectives
beyond just the SMART heuristic. As discussed, it is important to think carefully, and critically,
about whether to use the SMART heuristic to set physical activity goals and, if so, to be aware of
its limitations, risks and pitfalls. Otherwise, we suggest that other – ideally theory-based –
approaches should be considered instead. Given the reliance on SMART goals for physical
activity promotion at present, it is important for researchers to communicate the diverse array of
relevant theories to organisations and practitioners to help them make informed decisions about
the other options available. This step will also help organisations and practitioners to distinguish
between these approaches to determine which is best for their clients/patients (Siegert & Levack,
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 23
2014). By doing so, there will be greater opportunities to be adaptive and responsive to the needs
of those receiving the goal-setting advice.
Similarly, educational providers (e.g., universities), accrediting bodies, and health
organisations should focus on providing contemporary goal-setting guidance to students and
practitioners which is grounded in scientific theory and up to date empirical evidence. Such
training should focus on teaching relevant theories and frameworks of goal-setting.
Consequently, practitioners with a robust and diverse understanding of goal-setting theories will
be better positioned to know what type(s) of physical activity goals will likely be most effective,
for whom, and in which situations. Critically, the SMART acronym should not be taught alone,
or instead of theory, and goal-setting education should highlight the limitations of the SMART
acronym in an effort to move beyond uncritical acceptance of this strategy.
More Critical Research
Research is needed to generate evidence for the most effective goal-setting approaches in
physical activity promotion. Indeed, the first priority should be developing evidence of
comparative efficacy (e.g., what is the most efficacious approach to setting goals for
insufficiently active individuals?), so that we can then focus on communicating this sophisticated
approach in a meaningful and practical way (Rubin, 2002). Based on this review, a priority is
research that compares the effects of relevant goal types to empirically establish which approach
is the most beneficial for promoting physical activity. In our search, only three (Hawkins et al.,
2020; Swann et al., 2020a, 2020b) empirical studies compared SMART goals against another
goal type. It is also important for researchers to compare the effects of goal types on both
physical activity (e.g., increased step counts) and psychological outcomes underpinning
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 24
adherence and long-term engagement (such as affect, enjoyment, self-efficacy, and intrinsic
motivation).
If the SMART heuristic continues to be used in physical activity promotion, then
researchers should aim to address issues identified in this review. In line with the experimental
medicine approach (c.f. Sheeran et al., 2017), we suggest that a series of experimental studies to
rigorously test whether or not SMART goals are superior to other goal types will be most useful.
Such experiments should focus on key issues such as: comparing the efficacy of SMART goals
versus theory-based goal-setting; examining which type(s) of goal are most beneficial for
increasing physical activity in individuals who are inactive; and examining the psychological
outcomes of various goal types and their impact on adherence and long-term engagement in
physical activity. Researchers should also examine the potential risks and detrimental effects of
SMART goals within physical activity, including outcomes such as reduced enjoyment,
increased pressure, and perceptions of failure (e.g., Hawkins et al., 2020). Furthermore,
researchers should compare the SMART heuristic in relation to theory-based goal-setting in
order to empirically test whether or not it achieves comparable outcomes. In short, after decades
of uncritically accepting and relying upon the SMART acronym, it is now time to be critical.
The Need to Focus on Generating Strong Evidence Rather than New Acronyms
Finally, it may be intuitive to seek a different acronym to replace SMART goals in the
first instance (i.e., to replace one acronym with another). However, we caution that this would
likely reinforce many of the same problematic assumptions as highlighted in this review. For
example, various alternatives and extensions to the SMART acronym have been proposed,
including RUMBA (Relevant, Understandable, Measurable, Behavioural, Achievable – Barnett,
2009), MEANING (Meaning, Engage, Anchor, Negotiate, Intention-implementation gap, New
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 25
goals, Goals as behaviour change – McPherson et al., 2015) SMART-EST (Specific,
Measurable, Attainable, Relevant, Timebound, Evidence-based, Strategic, Tailored – White et al.
2020). However, similar issues apply, such as advocating style-over-substance,
reductionism/over-simplification, and assuming one approach to goal-setting. It should be
remembered that the purpose of acronyms is to simplify and convey complex information, and
like Rubin (2002) we: “fully support tools that increase our ability to communicate complexity in
a meaningful and useful way; however, when the tool becomes the practice, and the thinking
behind it wanes, this is anything but smart” (p.27). As such, we recommend that, in the first
instance, efforts are focused on establishing strong evidence for the optimal approaches to goal-
setting for promoting physical activity, rather than on developing new heuristics/acronyms to
replace SMART.
Summary
Ultimately we suggest that physical activity promotion should move away from its
current overreliance on SMART goals. Indeed, Grant (2012) argued that “the widespread belief
that goals are synonymous with SMART action plans has done much to stifle the development of
a more sophisticated understanding and use of goal theory" (p.147). Rather than the uncritical
acceptance that has taken place previously, we call on international scientific and professional
organisations in the fields of public health and physical activity promotion to cease their
wholesale, uncritical dissemination of the SMART acronym. Instead, we encourage the use of
theory-based goal-setting, more critical research, a focus on generating strong evidence rather
than new acronyms, and embracing more diverse goal setting approaches, with more
sophisticated training for students and practitioners.
Conclusion
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 26
The purpose of acronyms/mnemonics such as SMART is to communicate extensive
research and complex information in a simple, comprehensible and memorable manner.
However, “the use of such mnemonics without a clear understanding of the deeper underpinning
knowledge may well result in ill-informed decision making, and the cultivation of inaccurate
practice doctrines and mythologies about goals and goal theory" (Grant, 2012, p. 147). As goal-
setting is one of the most-used strategies for physical activity promotion (e.g., Howlett et al.,
2019), it is imperative that goals are set with the best chance of helping individuals achieve
sustained physical activity behaviour change. Our review indicates that there are a number of
issues in the SMART heuristic which are important for practitioners, organisations and
researchers to consider in deciding whether to utilise it. Indeed, the problems identified above
(see Table 3) suggest that the SMART acronym – and the physical activity research, policy and
practice in which SMART goals are used – is based on problematic scientific foundations. Given
how commonly accepted SMART goals are in this domain and how widely they are utilised,
there is a risk that these problems may be occurring on a large scale. Therefore, 40 years after its
advent, we are calling on international scientific and professional organisations in the fields of
public health and physical activity promotion to cease the wholesale, uncritical dissemination of
the SMART acronym, in favour of more sophisticated, defensible, and evidence-based guidance
on goal-setting. By working towards better goal-setting practice, these fields may be able to
achieve greater increases in physical activity, on a large scale, at low cost and within existing
infrastructure (Swann & Rosenbaum, 2018).
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 27
References
Note: references for studies found in our literature search are presented in Appendix 1.
American College of Sports Medicine (2009). ACSM's exercise is medicine: a clinician's guide
to exercise prescription. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
American College of Sports Medicine (2017). ACSM’s guidelines for fitness testing and
exercise prescription (10th Ed). Wolters Kluwer.
Bailey, R. (2019). Goal setting and action planning for health behaviour change. American
Journal of Lifestyle Medicine, 13(6), 615-618.
Barnett, D. (1999). The rehabilitation nurse as educator. In M. Smith M (Ed). Rehabilitation in
adult nursing practice (pp. 53-76). Churchill Livingston.
Bjerke, M.B., & Renger, R. (2017). Being smart about writing SMART objectives. Evaluation
and Program Planning, 61, 125-127.
Carey, R. N., Connell, L. E., Johnston, M., Rothman, A. J., De Bruin, M., Kelly, M. P., &
Michie, S. (2019). Behavior change techniques and their mechanisms of action: a
synthesis of links described in published intervention literature. Annals of Behavioral
Medicine, 53(8), 693-707.
Conroy, M. B., Yang, K., Elci, O. U., Gabriel, K. P., Styn, M. A., Wang, J., ... & Burke, L. E.
(2011). Physical activity self-monitoring and weight loss: 6-month results of the SMART
trial. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 43(8), 1568.
Doherty, A. (2013). Investing in sport management: The value of good theory. Sport
Management Review, 16, 5-11.
Doran, G.T. (1981). There's a S.M.A.R.T. way to write management's goals and objectives.
Management Review, 70, 35-36
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 28
Day, T., & Tosey, P. (2011). Beyond SMART? A new framework for goal setting. Curriculum
Journal, 22, 515-534.
Drach-Zahavy, A., & Erez, M. (2002). Challenge versus threat effects on the goal– performance
relationship. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88(2), 667–682.
Duda, J., & Balageur, I. (2007). Coach-created motivational climate. In S. Jowette and D.
Lavallee (Eds.), Social Psychology in Sport (pp. 117–130). Human Kinetics.
Epton, T., & Armitage, C.J. (2020). Goal setting interventions. In M. Hagger, L. Cameron,
K. Hamilton, N. Hankonen & T. Lintunen (Eds). Handbook of Behavior Change (pp.
554-571). Cambridge University Press.
Epton, T., Currie, S., & Armitage, C. J. (2017). Unique effects of setting goals on behavior
change: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 85(12), 1182.
Ferrari, R. (2015). Writing narrative style literature reviews. Medical Writing, 24, 230-235.
Glanz, K., & Rimer, B.K. (2005). Theory at a glance: a guide to health promotion practice
(2nd ed.). Bethesda, MD: National Cancer Institute.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A
meta‐analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 38, 69-119.
Grant, A.M. (2012). An integrated model of goal‐focused coaching: an evidence‐based
framework for teaching and practice. International Coaching Review, 7, 146-165.
Greenhalgh, T., Thorne, S., Malterud, K. (2018). Time to challenge the spurious hierarchy of
systematic over narrative reviews? European Journal of Clinical Investigation.
https://doi.org/10.1111/eci.12931
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 29
Hawkins, R. M., Swann, C., Crust, L., & Jackman, P. C. (2020). The effects of goal types on
psychological outcomes in active and insufficiently active adults in a walking task.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 48, 101661.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101661
Howlett, N., Trivedi, D., Troop, N.A., & Chater, A.M. (2019). Are physical activity
interventions for healthy inactive adults effective in promoting behaviour change and
maintenance, and which behaviour change techniques are effective? A systematic review
and meta-analysis. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 9, 147–157.
Kingston, K. M., & Hardy, L. (1997). Effects of different types of goals on processes that
support performance. The Sport Psychologist, 11(3), 277-293.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (1991). Self-regulation through goal setting. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 212-247.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (2006). Enhancing the benefits and overcoming the pitfalls of
goal setting. Organizational Dynamics, 35(4), 332-340.
Levack, W.M. (2018). Goal setting in rehabilitation. In S. Lennon, G. Ramdharry & G.
Verheyden (Eds). Physical Management for Neurological Conditions (4th edition,
pp. 91-109). Elsevier.
Levack, W.M., Weatherall, M., Hay‐Smith, E.J.C., Dean, S.G., McPherson, K., & Siegert, R.J.
(2015). Goal setting and strategies to enhance goal pursuit for adults with acquired
disability participating in rehabilitation. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews.
https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD009727.pub2
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 30
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and
task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57(9), 705-717.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2013). New developments in goal setting and task performance.
New York, NY: Routledge.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2015). Breaking the rules: a historical overview of goal-setting
theory. In Advances in motivation science (Vol. 2, pp. 99-126). Elsevier.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2019). The development of goal setting theory: A half century
retrospective. Motivation Science, 5, 93-105.
Locke, E. A., Shaw, K. N., Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P. (1981). Goal setting and task
performance: 1969–1980. Psychological Bulletin, 90(1), 125-152.
McEwan, D., Harden, S. M., Zumbo, B. D., Sylvester, B. D., Kaulius, M., Ruissen, G. R., ...
& Beauchamp, M. R. (2016). The effectiveness of multi-component goal setting
interventions for changing physical activity behaviour: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. Health Psychology Review, 10(1), 67-88.
McPherson, K.M., Kayes, N.M., & Kersten, P. (2014). MEANING as a smarter approach to
goals in rehabilitation. In R. Siegert and W. Levack (Eds.). Rehabilitation goal setting:
Theory, practice and evidence (pp. 105-19). CRC Press.
Mealiea, L., & Latham, G.P. (1996). Skills for managerial success: Theory, experience, and
practice. Irwin Professional Publishing.
Michie, S., & Johnston, M. (2012). Theories and techniques of behaviour change: Developing a
cumulative science of behaviour change. Health Psychology Review, 6, 1-6.
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 31
Michie, S., Rumsey, N., Fussell, A., Hardeman, W., Johnston, M., Newman, S., & Yardley, L.
(2008). Improving health: changing behaviour. NHS health trainer handbook.
Department of Health.
Moon, D. H., Yun, J., & McNamee, J. (2016). The effects of goal variation on adult physical
activity behaviour. Journal of Sports Sciences, 34(19), 1816-1821.
Morris, N. (2019). Fitness app users are ‘trying to cheat step counters’ with this simple hack.
Retrieved from https://metro.co.uk/2019/05/16/fitness-app users-trying-cheat-step-
counters-simple-hack-9570167/ .
Moving Medicine (2018). Workbook for an active lifestyle,
https://movingmedicine.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2018/09/Patient-workbook.pdf
Accessed 4th December, 2020.
Oettingen, G. & Gollwitzer, P. (2010). Strategies of setting and implementing goals. In J.E.
Maddux & J.P. Tangney (Eds.), Social Psychological Foundations of Clinical
Psychology. Guildford Press.
Ordóñez, L. D., Schweitzer, M. E., Galinsky, A. D., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). Goals gone
wild: The systematic side effects of overprescribing goal setting. Academy of
Management Perspectives, 23(1), 6-16.
Playford, E. D., Siegert, R., Levack, W., & Freeman, J. (2009). Areas of consensus and
controversy about goal setting in rehabilitation: a conference report. Clinical
Rehabilitation, 23(4), 334-344.
Prestwich, A., Webb, T. L., & Conner, M. (2015). Using theory to develop and test
interventions to promote changes in health behaviour: evidence, issues, and
recommendations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 5, 1-5.
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 32
Pritchard-Wiart, L., Thompson-Hodgetts, S., & McKillop, A.B. (2019). A review of goal setting
theories relevant to goal setting in paediatric rehabilitation. Clinical Rehabilitation, 33,
1515-1526.
Rosewilliam, S., Roskell, C.A., & Pandyan, A.D. (2011). A systematic review and synthesis of
the quantitative and qualitative evidence behind patient-centred goal setting in stroke
rehabilitation. Clinical Rehabilitation, 25, 501-514.
Rubin, R.S. (2002). Will the real SMART goals please stand up? The Industrial Organisational
Psychologist, 39, 26-27.
Schwarzer, R., Lippke, S., & Luszczynska, A. (2011). Mechanisms of health behaviour change
in persons with chronic illness or disability: The health action process approach
(HAPA). Rehabilitation Psychology, 56, 161-170.
Scobbie, L., Dixon, D., & Wyke, S. (2011). Goal setting and action planning in the rehabilitation
setting: development of a theoretically informed practice framework. Clinical
Rehabilitation, 25, 468-482.
Seijts, G. H., Latham, G. P., & Woodwark, M. (2013). Learning goals: A qualitative and
quantitative review. In E. A. Locke, & G. P. Latham (Eds.), New developments in goal
setting and task performance (pp. 195–212). Routledge.
Sheeran, P., Klein, W., & Rothman, A. (2017). Health behavior change: Moving from
observation to intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 573-600.
Sidman, C. L., Corbin, C. B., & Masurier, G. L. (2004). Promoting physical activity among
sedentary women using pedometers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 75(2),
122-129.
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 33
Siegert, R.J., & Levack, W.M. (2014). Rehabilitation goal setting: Theory, practice and
evidence. CRC press.
Siegert, R.J., & Taylor, W.J. (2004). Theoretical aspects of goal-setting and motivation in
rehabilitation. Disability Rehabilitation, 26, 1-8.
Sugavanam,T., Mead, G., Bulley, C., Donaghy, M., & van Wijck, F. The effects and experiences
of goal setting in stroke rehabilitation–a systematic review. Disability
Rehabilitation, 35, 177-190.
Swann, C., Hooper, A., Schweickle, M.J., Peoples, G., Mullan, J., Hutto, D., Allen, M., & Vella
S.A. (2020a). Comparing the effects of goal types in a walking session with
healthy adults: Preliminary evidence for open goals in physical activity. Psychology of
Sport and Exercise, 47, 101475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.01.003
Swann, C., & Rosenbaum, S. (2018). Do we need to reconsider best practice in goal setting
for physical activity promotion? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 52(8), 485-486.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2017-098186
Swann, C., Rosenbaum, S., Lawrence, A., Vella, S. A., McEwan, D., & Ekkekakis, P. (2021).
Updating goal-setting theory in physical activity promotion: a critical conceptual
review. Health Psychology Review, 15, 34-50.
Swann, C., Schweickle, M. J., Peoples, G. E., Goddard, S. G., Stevens, C., & Vella, S. A.
(2020b). The potential benefits of nonspecific goals in physical activity promotion:
Comparing open, do-your-best, and as-well-as-possible goals in a walking task. Journal
of Applied Sport Psychology. Advance publication online.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200.2020.1815100
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
SMART GOALS FOR PHYSICAL ACTIVITY 34
Tasa, K., Whyte, G., & Leonardelli, G. (2013). Goals and negotiation. In E. A. Locke, & G. P.
Latham (Eds.), New developments in goal setting and task performance (pp. 397–
414). Routledge.
The Royal Australian College of General Practitioners (2018). Putting prevention into practice:
Guidelines for the implementation of prevention in the general practice setting (3rd
edition). East Melbourne, Vic: RACGP.
Wade, D.T. (2009). Goal setting in rehabilitation: an overview of what, why and how. Clinical
Rehabilitation, 23, 291-295.
White, N., Bautista, V., Lenz, T., & Cosimano, A. (2020). Using the SMART-EST goals in
lifestyle medicine prescription. Pharmacy Review, 14(3), 271-273.
Williams, K. (2013). Goal setting in sports. In E. A. Locke, & G. P. Latham (Eds.), New
developments in goal setting and task performance (pp. 375–398). Routledge.
Worksafe Victoria & Transport Accident Commission. (2012). Clinical framework for the
delivery of health services. Melbourne: WorkSafe Victoria.
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
... Potential moderating factors should also be considered. For example, evidence suggests less specific goals may be more beneficial for people starting to be physically active from inactivity (Swann et al., 2023), so it is important the impact of action planning consistency is tested in a variety of populations. Evidence also suggests that when dyads make plans together, it can enhance their likelihood of success , so how and with whom plans are made are important to consider as well. ...
... Whether people engaged in the physical activity in the contexts specified by their action plans has been shown to be important for habit formation and long-term behaviour change maintenance and should be a focus in future intervention approaches and assessments . Importantly, the focus of this study was action planning but there are many other related forms of self-regulation such as goals, coping plans and a more specific form of action planning-implementation intentions, that may also have relevancy for habit formation and long-term behavioural maintenance (Carraro & Gaudreau, 2013;Gollwitzer, 1999;Swann et al., 2023). Given their prominence as intervention approaches, it is essential research continues to help establish how to utilise goals and plans in the most effective ways for the betterment of people's health and wellbeing. ...
Article
Objective: Action planning is a common approach used in physical activity interventions. The aim of this study was to assess the association of frequency, consistency and content of action planning with physical activity behaviour, intention strength and habit strength. Methods and measures: Within a 3-month web-based, computer-tailored physical activity intervention, participants (N = 115; 68.7% female, M age =43.9; range = 22-73 years) could create 6 rounds of action plans for 4 activities each (24 total). Results: Consistency of action planning during the intervention was associated with change in physical activity at 9-months, and intention and habit strength at 3-months and 9-months. Frequency of action planning was negatively associated with intention at 3-months and 9-months. The effect of action planning consistency on physical activity behaviour was no longer significant when accounting for change in intention and habit strength. Conclusion: Consistency of how, where, when and with whom people plan their physical activity may translate into stronger physical activity habits. Interventions should avoid encouraging making many distinct action plans, but rather encourage stable contexts through consistent action planning.
... Enterprises and physical fitness service providers can also provide other types of goals (learning, open, do your best goals) rather than specific goals. There is a growing body of evidence that other goals can be equally if not more effective than specific goals (Hawkins et al., 2020;Swann et al., 2023). For those individuals with specific goals, self-quantification conditions should be avoided in activity participation as much as possible. ...
Article
Full-text available
As individuals are involved in self-quantification, increasing fields are applying self-quantification with technical support, and a clear understanding of the influencing mechanism and effect boundary of self-quantification is indispensable. Focusing on the field of physical fitness, under different goal settings, the influencing mechanisms of self-quantification on an individual’s participation performance (outcome, experience, persistence intention) and behavioral decision-making (category selection, novelty-seeking) are analyzed and confirmed. The results show that when there is no goal, self-quantification will enhance outcome salience, thus enhancing the individual’s participation outcome and selection for highly healthy categories; self-quantification will reduce perceived enjoyment, thus damaging participation experience and novelty-seeking, but does not affect the persistence intention. When there is a goal, self-quantification will enhance perceived certainty, thus reducing the individual’s participation outcome and selection for highly healthy categories; self-quantification will enhance perceived enjoyment, thus improving the participation experience, persistence intention, and novelty-seeking. Compared to descriptive analysis of phenomena, through laboratory and field experiments, this research confirms the influencing mechanism and effect boundary of self-quantification on an individual’s activity participation performance in real situations. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding the targeted behavioral decision-making patterns of individuals in different types of self-quantification activities.
... Motor Training based on goals that are specific and difficult is likely to produce better outcomes than training without a goal or with a nonspecific easily attained goal [33]. The SMART acronym is often used to facilitate the goal-setting process, prompting therapists to set goals with patients that are Specific, Measurable, Attainable, Realistic and Timebound (there are several variations of the words associated with each letter) [44,45]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Study design: Descriptive. Objectives: The primary objective is to describe the intervention that will be provided in a large multi-centre randomised controlled trial titled: Early and Intensive Motor Training for people with Spinal Cord Injuries (the SCI-MT Trial). The secondary objective is to describe the strategies that will be used to operationalise and standardise the Motor Training provided to participants while keeping the intervention person-centred. Methods: The paper focuses on the rationale and principles of Motor Training for people with spinal cord injuries (SCI). The description of the intervention is based on the Template for Intervention Description and Replication (TIDieR) checklist. Specifically, it addresses the following 6 criteria of the TIDieR checklist: why the effectiveness of Motor Training is being examined; what, how, where and when the Motor Training will be administered; and how much Motor Training will be provided. Results: A detailed intervention manual has been developed to help standardise the delivery of the intervention. Conclusions: This paper describes the details of a complex intervention administered as part of a large randomised controlled trial. It will facilitate the subsequent interpretation of the trial results and enable the intervention to be reproduced in clinical practice and future trials.
... If this information is not clear, goal framing and goal flexibility may result in negative outcomes. For example, setting a challenging short-term goal on a day when the person does not have necessary time to achieve it, may result in stress and fail the long-term goal, leading to negative associations with more challenging goals [90]. Contrastingly, always choosing easier short-term goals may delay long-term goal achievement. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Research has extensively explored how personal informatics tools can support people's health goal setting practices. To understand the current state and reflect on the future of goal setting in personal informatics, we report the results of a scoping review of 51 papers that use and provide design implications for implementing goal setting. Our review highlights six implications for using goal setting in personal informatics tools (clarity, transparency, flexibility, framing and reframing, personalization, and reflection). We find that goal setting is becoming increasingly complex as the number of goals and their characteristics increase. We discuss these insights and point towards the importance of supporting self-efficacy during goal setting, showing adaptive goal evolution over time, reducing burden during goal setting, and framing goals to understand the complexity of health goals and support a holistic view on goal setting.
Article
Full-text available
In new era digital now, internet is the important think for various activities including in the process to improve business promotion. One way to increase promotion through online is to improve digital marketing strategies using Search Engine Optimization (SEO) techniques. The aim of this research is to implementation SEO to improve digital marketing in F&B industry. Developing method that used are On Page and Off Page SEO to optimize the search engine. The Researcher using literature analysis and deep interview to get the data from SMEs in F&B Industry. To create the implementation of SEO, the researcher undertakes case studies from SMEs in F&B Industry who already run the business and need to improve the Digital Marketing through SEO. However, it is important to note that SEO optimization is not a one-time task but rather an ongoing process. It requires constant attention and effort to maintain high search engine rankings and ensure the continued success of online business. Beside website, to improve the business promotion the researcher also use tools in Google Business and E-Commerce to optimize the SEO.The finding suggest that the implementation of Search Engine Optimization can make impact for improvement in visibility search engines, attract more visitors, and ultimately drive business growth in the area Google Business, E-Commerce and website owned by SMEs F&B Industry. It is important to remain vigilant in implementing SEO techniques to maintain high search engine rankings and ensure the continued success of SMEs online business.
Article
The prevailing wisdom in neurological rehabilitation, and particularly for stroke, is that physical therapies are the key to improvements in function. Despite accepting the importance of ‘the motivated patient’, the lack of simple, proven ways to improve intrinsic motivation has hindered efforts to combine physical therapies with motivation. Now there is available a simple, free, well-validated approach to encourage intrinsic motivation (‘Take Charge’). The benefits for people who had a stroke are well-established but this could be applied to people with a range of neurological and other disorders. We provide the evidential support for this approach and suggest ways of incorporating it into daily practice.
Article
There is a growing focus on developing person-adaptive strategies to support sustained exercise behavior, necessitating conceptual models to guide future research and applications. This paper introduces Flexible nonlinear periodization (FNLP) - a proposed, but underdeveloped person-adaptive model originating in sport-specific conditioning - that, pending empirical refinement and evaluation, may be applied in health promotion and disease prevention settings. To initiate such efforts, the procedures of FNLP (i.e., acutely and dynamically matching exercise demand to individual assessments of mental and physical readiness) are integrated with contemporary health behavior evidence and theory to propose a modified FNLP model and to show hypothesized pathways by which FNLP may support exercise adherence (e.g., flexible goal setting, management of affective responses, and provision of autonomy/variety-support). Considerations for future research are also provided to guide iterative, evidence-based efforts for further development, acceptability, implementation, and evaluation.
Chapter
Mental skills training is an essential component of sport performance. Mental skills training with youth athletes has great potential, as sport often serves as a place where they formulate facilitative—or alternatively, debilitative—views of success and failure. Athletes who engage in mental skills training learn concrete strategies to better navigate the mental landscape of performance. This chapter seeks to explore various mental skills applications (e.g., stress management, imagery, self-talk, and goal setting) through the use of a case study. Evidence-based research supports the mental skills consultant’s applied approach, allowing for creativity and flexibility to best support the athlete’s needs.KeywordsSport psychologyMindsetYouth sportSport performanceStress managementImagerySelf-talkGoal setting
Article
Full-text available
A goal-setting technique could be considered as one of the modern tools of client-centered social work. Such techniques are designed to motivate and direct the client to achieve a specific goal and result. The paper systematizes academic approaches to understanding the theory of goal setting, defines the specifics of its practical use in social work, and presents specific goal setting techniques that have been tested as part of structured interventions for clients who are in difficult life circumstances and are motivated to change them.The elaboration of social work interventions must be based on the understanding that goals, by their very nature, are more attainable and accessible than wishes, intentions or dreams. Their formulation by social work clients who are in difficult life circumstances and may have a reduced social perspective cannot be considered a simple process, since it requires the use of specific techniques such as setting goals and tasks («Wheel of life balance», various visualizations and discussions; «Map achievement of goals», «Suitcase of goals»), as well as the implementation of steps to achieve them (diaries, weekly goal achievement maps, skill trackers, coaching, etc.).As evidenced by the literature review, the authors’ own experience, as well as the results of other applied studies, the goal-setting theory contains ideas and concepts that are consistent with the basic principles of professional social work. It suggests that the client and the social worker work together to find differences in the client's current state of life and the outcome he or she is striving for. The paper reveals that goal setting stimulates activity, initiative and motivation of clients, encourages choice and forms a sense of responsibility for this choice, and thus strengthens the client-centeredness of social work.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Despite evidence that goal setting is valuable for physical activity promotion, recent studies highlighted a potential oversimplification in the application of this behavior change technique. While more difficult performance goals might trigger higher physical activity levels, higher performance goals might concurrently be more difficult to achieve, which could reduce long-term motivation. This study examined (a) the association between performance goal difficulty and physical activity and (b) the association between performance goal difficulty and goal achievement. Method: This study used data from an e-Health intervention among inactive overweight adults (n = 20). The study duration included a 2-week baseline period and an intervention phase of 80 days. During the intervention, participants received a daily step goal experimentally manipulated by taking participants' baseline physical activity median (i.e., number of steps) multiplied by a pseudorandom factor ranging from 1 to 2.6. A continuous measure of goal achievement was inferred for each day by dividing the daily number of steps by the goal prescribed that day. Linear and generalized additive models were fit for each participant. Results: The results confirm that, for a majority of the participants involved in the study, performance goal difficulty was positively and significantly associated with physical activity (n = 14), but, concurrently, negatively and significantly associated with goal achievement (n = 19). These associations were mainly linear. Conclusion: At the daily level, setting a higher physical activity goal leads to engaging in higher physical activity levels, but concurrently lower goal achievement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Recent evidence suggests non-specific goals may be beneficial for physical activity adoption, however, it is currently unclear which variation of non-specific goal is most beneficial. Therefore, this study compared open, do-your-best, as-well-as-possible, SMART goals and no instruction (control) on distance walked during a series of six-minute walk tests, as well as ratings of perceived exertion (RPE), mental effort, autonomy, and interest in further exercise. In total, 82 healthy adults ranging from high to low levels of physical activity (59 women; 23 men; Mage = 48.10) took part. Participants were randomly assigned to either: open goals; do-your-best goals; as-well-as-possible goals; SMART goals; or control. Participants in the goal groups completed a baseline and then two manipulated attempts of the six-minute walk test, while the control group followed the baseline instructions for all three attempts. There was no significant difference in the distance walked by participants pursuing open, do-your-best, as-well-as-possible and SMART goals; all of whom walked significantly further than participants in the control. Open, do-your-best and SMART goals resulted in significantly higher RPE than control. Do-your-best goals resulted in significantly greater mental effort compared to control. Open goals resulted in significantly higher interest in repeating the session, and significantly higher interest in pursuing a program, compared to control. This study provides further evidence of the potential benefits of non-specific goals for physical activity. Findings suggest each version of non-specific goals leads to different psychological outcomes, and that open goals may be the most beneficial form of non-specific goals for physical activity.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives This study aimed to extend recent work on the effects of goal types in physical activity (PA; Swann, Hooper et al., 2020) by comparing the effects of SMART, open, and do-your-best (DYB) goals on performance and psychological responses in active and insufficiently active adults in a walking task. Design 4 (goal condition) x 3 (attempt) x 2 (group) mixed design. Methods Active (n = 18) and insufficiently active (n = 18) participants completed baseline and two experimental attempts of a 6-minute walking test in four conditions: SMART goal; open goal; DYB goal; and control. A range of measures were taken during and following each attempt, and after each session. A series of mixed ANOVA’s were conducted for all measures assessed between groups. Results Insufficiently active participants achieved greater distances in the open condition compared to the SMART condition (p < .001), whereas active participants achieved greater distances in the SMART condition compared to the open condition (p < .001). Additionally, exploratory analyses revealed that insufficiently active participants reported greater pleasure and enjoyment (p < .05) in the open condition compared to active participants, who conversely reported more pleasure and enjoyment (p < .05) in the SMART condition than insufficiently active participants. Conclusions Findings provide initial evidence that PA and psychological responses differ between active and insufficiently active individuals depending on goal type. This work has potential implications for goal setting strategies in PA promotion and raises further questions about current practices of setting SMART goals for insufficiently active participants.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Goal-setting is one of the most common strategies used to increase physical activity. Current practice is often based on specific, measurable, achievable, realistic, and time-bound (SMART) goals. However, theory and research suggests that this approach may be problematic. Open goals (e.g., “see how well you can do”) have emerged as a possible alternative, but are yet to be tested experimentally in physically active tasks. In a walking-based session, this study aimed to experimentally compare the effects of open, SMART and do-your-best goals with a control condition on distance walked and psychological variables related to engagement. Design Repeated measures design (mixed model). Method Participants (N = 78; Mage = 55.88) were randomly assigned to one of four goal conditions: an open, SMART, or do-your-best goal, or a control condition (“walk at your normal pace”), before completing a baseline and two manipulated attempts of a 6-min walking test. Results Open, SMART, and do-your-best goals achieved greater distance walked, and higher ratings of perceived exertion, than the control across both experimental attempts. Open and SMART goals led to greater enjoyment of the session. However, SMART goals led to higher pressure/tension, while open goals led to higher perceptions of performance and higher interest in repeating the session. Conclusions These findings provide preliminary evidence for the efficacy of setting open goals in physical activity, and suggest that they may be more psychologically adaptive to pursue than SMART or do-your-best goals. Implications are discussed, and recommendations are made for future goal-setting research in physical activity.
Article
Full-text available
Effective obesity prevention and treatment interventions are lacking in the United States, especially for impoverished minority youths at risk for health disparities, and especially in accessible community-based settings. We describe the launch and pilot implementation evaluation of the first year of the B’N Fit POWER initiative as a middle school-based comprehensive wellness program that integrates weight management programming into existing onsite preventive and clinical services. Consistent with the existing implementation science literature, we focused on both the organizational structures that facilitate communication and the development of trust among stakeholders, students, and families and the development of realistic and timely goals to implement and integrate all aspects of the program. New implementation and programming strategies were developed and tested to increase the proportion of students screened, support the linkage of students to care, and streamline the integration of program clinical and afterschool components into routine services already offered at the school. We report on our initial implementation activities using the Standards for Reporting Implementation Studies (StaRI) framework using hybrid outcomes combining the Reach element from the RE-AIM framework with a newly conceptualized Wellness Cascade.
Article
There is a considerable body of literature on goals and goal setting in the psychological literature, but little of this has found its way into the scholarly coaching literature. This article draws on the goal-setting literature from the behavioural sciences. It discusses a range of approaches to understanding the goal construct, presents a definition of goals relevant to coaching, and details a new and comprehensive model of goal-focused coaching that can be helpful both in teaching and in applied coaching practice. It also outlines new empirical research that highlights the vital role that coaches’ goal-focused coaching skills play in determining successful coaching outcomes. This preliminary research suggests that the use of a goal-focused coaching style is more effective than a ‘common factors’ or person-centred coaching style in facilitating goal attainment in coaching. It is argued that, by understanding the different types of goals and their relationship to the process of change, professional coaches can work more efficiently with their clients, helping them to achieve insight and behavioural change that enhances their workplace performance, their professional working lives and, most importantly, their personal well-being and sense of self.
Article
Goal-setting is a widely used and accepted strategy for promoting physical activity. Locke and Latham’s goal-setting theory is the primary theoretical framework for setting goals in psychology and plays a prominent role in physical activity promotion. Recently, however, there have been calls to reconsider current goal-setting practice in this field. Therefore, we aimed to critically review and update the application of goal-setting theory in physical activity promotion, by examining core developments in this theory since 1990. Current practice relies on setting specific ‘performance’ goals as a means of increasing physical activity (e.g., 10,000 steps; national physical activity guidelines). This approach was initially consistent with key tenets of goal-setting theory. However, since 1990 this theory has evolved to differentiate between performance and learning goals. Both goal types are context-dependent and it is now recognised that, in some cases, performance goals can even be detrimental to the achievement of desired outcomes. Consequently, current practice may be theoretically appropriate for physically active individuals but a different approach (e.g., learning goals) may be preferable for inactive individuals who are new to physical activity (i.e., most of the population). We conclude by discussing implications for policy, research, and practice in goal-setting for physical activity promotion.
Article
Background: Goal setting in paediatric rehabilitation is influenced by shifting parent, sibling, caregiver, and child roles over time and evolving child developmental capacity for participation in the process. A theoretical and evidence-informed approach to goal setting, specific to paediatrics, would provide a framework for goal setting in practice and facilitate systematic evaluation of the effects of goal-setting processes on child and family outcomes. Objective: To provide an overview of relevant goal-setting theories and their implications for paediatric rehabilitation. Methods: Prevalent theories were identified from relevant rehabilitation, motivation, behaviour change, and goal-setting literature. Implications for goal setting in paediatrics are summarized according to goal-setting and action-planning phases: (1) preparation, (2) formulation of goals, (3) formulation of action plan, (4) coping planning, and (5) follow up. Results: Social cognitive theory, self-determination theory, Health Action Process Approach, Mastery Motivation, and goal-setting theory are reviewed. Examples of implications for goal setting include, sharing information with families about the purpose of goal setting; identifying goals that are specific, proximal, challenging, and important to the child; and addressing self-efficacy. Conclusion: The theories reviewed have clear implications for paediatric rehabilitation research and practice. They address considerations not typically discussed in adult rehabilitation such as observing children to obtain information about meaningful goals when they are unable to communicate them directly and the importance of establishing flexible processes that will accommodate changing family roles over time. Research is needed to evaluate the effects of goal-setting processes and strategies on outcomes in paediatric rehabilitation.
Article
This chapter summarizes the authors’ joint development of the goal setting theory. The basic concept was based on more than 50 years of research and the formal theory has endured for 28 years (Locke & Latham, 1990). The theory was not developed through overgeneralization from only a few studies or by deduction but rather by induction. The inductions involved the integration of hundreds of studies involving thousands of participants. The theory initially focused solely on consciously set goals. To date, the goal setting theory has shown generality across participants, tasks, nationality, goal source, settings, experimental designs, outcome variables, levels of analysis (individual, group, division, and organizational), and time spans. The theory identifies both mediators and moderators of goal effects. Numerous subsequent studies since 1990 have supported the main tenets of the theory. New findings have enlarged our knowledge of the relevant mediators and moderators as well as showing new applications (Locke & Latham, 2013). Among these discoveries are when to set learning rather than performance goals, the effect of goals primed in the subconscious on job performance, and that goal effects are enhanced by having people write at length about them.
Chapter
There is growing acknowledgement that scientific progress in rehabilitation requires development of theories in addition to empirical research (Dunn & Elliot, 2008; Siegert, McPherson, & Dean, 2005; Siegert & Taylor, 2004). While a grand unified theory of rehabilitation seems highly unlikely to emerge, there is increasing acceptance that sophisticated theoretical accounts of important phenomena in rehabilitation are necessary to understand complex processes and predict outcomes (Scobbie, Wyke, & Dixon, 2009; Siegert, McPherson, & Taylor, 2004; Whyte, 2007). This awareness of the important role of theory building and theory testing in a scientific rehabilitation is a recent development. Rehabilitation theory of goal setting still lags well behind goal-setting research in fields such as industrial-organizational psychology, education and sport psychology. This imbalance between theory development and empirical research limits the scientific credibility of rehabilitation (Siegert et al., 2005). More importantly, it does our clients or patients a disservice. It assumes that all we need is evidence about which interventions are effective - without a comprehensive understanding of how an intervention works for an individual client with a particular set of problems in a unique psychosocial context.