Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 18 January 2022
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.780319
Edited by:
Andrea Poli,
Università degli Studi di Pisa, Italy
Reviewed by:
Chao Liu,
Huaqiao University, China
Seth Davin Norrholm,
Wayne State University, UnitedStates
*Correspondence:
Anke Karl
A.Karl@exeter.ac.uk
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Psychology for Clinical Settings,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 20 September 2021
Accepted: 25 November 2021
Published: 18 January 2022
Citation:
Gerdes S, Williams H and
Karl A (2022) Psychophysiological
Responses to a Brief Self-
Compassion Exercise in Armed
Forces Veterans.
Front. Psychol. 12:780319.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.780319
Psychophysiological Responses
to a Brief Self-Compassion Exercise
in Armed Forces Veterans
SamanthaGerdes
1,2, HuwWilliams
1 and AnkeKarl
1*
1 Mood Disorder Centre, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom,
2 The Veterans’ Mental Health and Wellbeing Service, Camden and Islington NHS Trust, London, United Kingdom
Armed Forces personnel are exposed to traumatic experiences during their work; therefore,
they are at risk of developing emotional difculties such as post-traumatic stress disorder
(PTSD), following traumatic experiences. Despite evidence to suggest that self-compassion
is effective in reducing the symptoms of PTSD, and greater levels of self-compassion are
associated with enhanced resilience, self-compassion in armed forces personnel and
armed forces veterans remains under-researched. As a result, it is not known if therapeutic
approaches that use self-compassion interventions are an acceptable and effective
treatment for this population. Having previously shown that a one-off self-compassion
exercise has temporary benecial psychophysiological effects in non-clinical participants,
weconducted this proof-of concept study to investigate whether this exercise is equally
benecial in veterans who had experienced deployment to a combat zone. Additionally,
weexamined if brief a self-compassion exercise can temporarily reduce hyperarousal
symptoms and increase feelings of social connectedness. The current study also
investigated the association between PTSD symptom severity, emotion regulation, and
self-compassion in 56 veterans. All participants listened to a loving-kindness meditation
for self-compassion (LKM-S) and psychophysiological recordings were taken throughout.
Psychophysiological effects were observed including heart-rate (HR), skin conductance
(SCL), and heart-rate variability (HRV) to determine associations with PTSD and changes
in response associated with the self-compassion induction. PTSD symptom severity,
dispositional emotion regulation, and self-compassion were measured, and participants
also completed state measures of hyperarousal and social connectedness before and
after the LKM-S. The ndings partially demonstrated that self-compassion can beelicited
in a veteran population but there were considerable individual differences in
psychophysiological responses. The ndings are discussed in light of existing theories of
PTSD and self-compassion and the implications of using self-compassion based
psychological approaches with veterans.
Keywords: self-compassion, veterans, PTSD, hyperarousal, loving-kindness, heart rate variability,
skin conductance
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
INTRODUCTION
e impact of war-related trauma on soldiers is now well recognized
and exposure to traumatic events while carrying out occupational
duties can put armed forces personnel at an increased risk for
developing PTSD (Dunn etal., 2015). Reports of PTSD prevalence
rates in the armed forces population vary widely (Kok et al.,
2012) with some estimating rates of PTSD in currently serving
armed forces personnel at 4% (Hotopf et al., 2003; Fear et al.,
2010); however, rates rise exponentially when soldiers are exposed
to combat during deployments (11.9–22.5%; Kang et al., 2003)
and PTSD rates increase with greater exposure to enemy contact
and reghts (Hoge et al., 2004).
One particular characteristic of combat-related PTSD is a
pattern of hypervigilance symptomology that is dierent from
those found in civilian populations (Kimble et al., 2013).
Individuals suering from combat-related PTSD report enhanced
physiological reactivity, an overactive startle response and
emotional numbness compared with those who experience
civilian traumatic events (Prescott, 2012). For soldiers in war
zones, hypervigilance is highly adaptive such as the constant
sensory scanning and searching (e.g., listening for footsteps
and weapon sounds or looking for rising dust and shadows;
(Department of the Army, 1984). Due to the constant threat
to life or of serious physical injury endured for long periods
of time, the hypervigilance is reinforced while on deployment
(Kimble et al., 2013). As a result, it can become habitual and
triggered easily, and dicult to eradicate once back in civilian
life. Individuals are on constant “high alert” even when threat
is low (Kimble et al., 2013), thus becoming problematic in
civilian life as it can lead to disruptions in functioning such
as increased aggression and sleep problems (Germain and
Neilsen, 2003; Ta et al., 2007; Conoscenti et al., 2009).
Within the cognitive model of PTSD, elevated levels of
hypervigilance and being in a threatened state maintains PTSD
in combat veterans as it can prevent adaptive changes to the
trauma memories (Ehlers and Clark, 2000). Veterans might hold
a belief that hypervigilance was what enabled them to survive
the traumatic experience and that they therefore cannot give it
up. is could facilitate engagement in maladaptive coping
strategies and safety behaviors such as constantly being on high
alert and on the lookout for danger (Conoscenti et al., 2009).
e drive to avoid feelings of threat further reinforces hypervigilant
behaviors and prevents adaptive processing of the traumatic
events. Individuals with combat-related PTSD, therefore, nd it
more dicult to engage in psychological therapies where exposure
to traumatic memories is at the core (e.g., Ehlers and Clark, 2009).
Hypervigilance and other PTSD symptoms can leave people
feeling detached and estranged from others and having diculties
experiencing positive feelings (American Psychiatric Association,
2013) as well as a feeling of dierence, or “having changed”
since the traumatic event (Demers, 2011). is can lead to
diculties in maintaining relationships (King etal., 2006) resulting
in a lack of social support which can further contribute to a
deterioration in mental health (Freedman et al., 2015). Further,
the eect of the transition to civilian life is that the social support
network experienced during a career with the armed forces is
no longer available; hence, the combat veterans face an imminent
lack of belongingness and social connection that may have been
adaptive during their service (DeVries et al., 2003; Tick, 2005;
Wessely, 2006). e profound benecial eects of social support
(Brewin et al., 2000; Ozer et al., 2003) and perceived social
connectedness for recovery from psychological trauma and reducing
PTSD have been demonstrated (Freedman et al., 2015). For
example, post-deployment social support is negatively associated
with PTSD in combat veterans (Pietrzak et al., 2009). However,
the masculinized culture of the armed forces that promotes
emotional stoicism (Reit, 2009; McAllister et al., 2018; Neilson
et al., 2020) can prevent people from sharing emotional distress,
and this may be further compounded during civilian life where
veterans may feel alone in their experiences (Demers, 2011).
A lack of social support, diculties in social relationships,
or threats to social connection contribute to PTSD severity
(Freedman et al., 2015) and also can activate the same stress
response system as physical threats to survival, i.e., the ght/
ight response, including the sympathetic nervous system (SNS)
and the Hypothalamus-pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Eisenberger
and Cole, 2012). e combination of threats to social
connection and hypervigilance due to fragmented, emotionally
charged trauma memories can contribute to an elevated ght/
ight response and hence mental health problems in veterans
(Southwick et al., 2005). Elevated physiological arousal (Pole,
2007) and an elevated HPA response have been identied in
people with PTSD. is elevated ght/ight state (i.e.,
hyperarousal) and constant activation of threat mode
(i.e., hypervigilance), combined with a lack of social support
may maintain PTSD in combat veterans, as individuals might
be stuck in “current threat” mode (Ehlers and Clark, 2000).
erefore, therapeutic approaches that emphasize reducing
hyperarousal and the stress response as well as building social
connectedness could reduce PTSD symptoms in veterans. e
new concept of self-compassion has shown to be a promising
approach for alleviation of PTSD symptoms in both civilian
and veteran populations (e.g., Lee, 2009; Steen et al., 2021).
Self-compassion can be described as “an intimate awareness
of the suering of oneself and others with the wish to alleviate
it” (Germer and Ne, 2013). Dispositional self-compassion is
negatively related to psychopathology (Barnard and Curry, 2011;
MacBeth and Gumley, 2012) including PTSD and has been
shown to predict recovery from PTSD (ompson and Waltz,
2008; Meyer et al., 2019). Additionally, self-compassion can
be cultivated in therapeutic settings and is therefore gaining
popularity to treat a number of mental health diculties (e.g.,
Germer and Ne, 2013, 2015) including PTSD and shame-
based ashbacks (Lee, 2009; Daneshvar et al., 2020) as well
as other shame-based diculties (Leaviss and Uttley, 2015).
Additionally, studies have shown that self-compassion could
oer a protective process in preventing suicide in veterans, in
times of distress as higher levels of self-compassion are associated
with lower levels of psychopathology and suicidality (Kelley
et al., 2019; Rabon et al., 2019).
Self-compassion could be benecial for the treatment of
PTSD in several ways: rstly, increasing self-compassion can
reduce the “threat” emotion regulation system (Gilbert, 2009a)
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
that is reected by the excessive hyperarousal previously used
as a survival mechanism and the negative self-appraisals that
prevent adaptive processing and integration of the traumatic
experience into the individual’s autobiographical memory
(Ehlers and Clark, 2000). Both hyperarousal and negative
self-appraisals are associated with activation of the sympathetic
division of the autonomic nervous system as indicated by
increased heart rate (HR) and skin conductance level (SCL;
Pole, 2007) as part of the ght/ight response. In contrast,
facilitating self-compassion secondly activates the “soothing
and contentment” system (Gilbert, 2009a) characterized by
a calm and content positive state and increased parasympathetic
activation (as indicated by increased heart rate variability;
HRV, Kirschner et al., 2019). is allows the individual not
only to activate self-soothing and kindness but also to feel
safe and socially connected (Gilbert, 2010). Increasing feelings
of social connectedness via the activation of compassion
represents the second possible mechanism via which it could
reduce PTSD symptomology in veterans (e.g., Pearlman and
Curtois, 2005; Freedman et al., 2015).
Facilitating self-compassion in a one-o meditation in
civilian populations has been shown to increase perceived
interpersonal connectedness (Hutcherson et al., 2008) and
state secure attachment (Kirschner et al., 2019). However,
the use of self-compassion with veterans with PTSD is in
its infancy, though initial studies have found that self-
compassion is negatively associated with PTSD (Dahm etal.,
2015), that dispositional self-compassion levels are predictive
of PTSD symptom severity (Hiraoka etal., 2015), and self-
compassion is negatively related to maladaptive coping
strategies such as impulsivity in military recruits (Mantzios,
2014). A 12-week course of loving kindness meditation (LKM)
in veterans with PTSD led to an increase in self-compassion
while symptoms of PTSD decreased (Kearney et al., 2013).
Although self-compassion has demonstrated eectiveness for
shame-based diculties in PTSD (Lee, 2009), the mechanisms
via which self-compassion interventions facilitate PTSD
symptom reduction in veterans are not well understood
to date.
It has not previously been studied whether facilitating self-
compassion in veterans can reduce hypervigilance/hyperarousal
as assessed by self-report and physiological measurements. In
healthy individuals, a one-o short-term Loving Kindness
Meditation for the Self (LKM-S) reduced physiological arousal
symptoms; (i.e., reduced HR, SCL) and increased parasympathetic
activation (i.e., increased HRV; Kirschner et al., 2019). In
contrast, Creaser et al. (2021) found that civilian trauma
survivors without PTSD and with subsyndromal and full PTSD
who followed the same LKM-S had a reduction in negative
self-perception and an increase in positive self-perception but
did not show the expected physiological response pattern.
Interestingly, individuals in the subsyndromal PTSD group who
presented with higher levels of hyperarousal, showed a distinct
physiological and brain response pattern, which indicated a
threat response when instructed to direct compassion to the
self. Similarly, in individuals with a history of recurrent
depression, Kirschner et al. (2021) found that the LKM-S
increased positive self-perception but this was not accompanied
by the expected physiological response pattern. However, aer
completing an 8-week MBCT course, patients with recurrent
depression showed a physiological pattern of calm and content
positive aect (reduced HR and SCL and increased HRV;
Kirschner et al., 2021).
In veterans, one prior study suggests that LKM can reduce
PTSD symptoms (e.g., (Kearney et al., 2013) although to
our knowledge there have been no further empirical studies
to support this. e aim of the current study is to investigate
the eects of a short-term one-o self-compassion meditation
(LKM-S) in veterans who have experienced deployment to
a combat zone. e pre-post changes on self-report hyperarousal
symptoms (DSM-5, PTSD Cluster E) and feelings of social
connectedness were examined. Additionally, we investigated
physiological reactions during the self-compassion meditation
to better understand the eects of the meditation on the
ght/ight response. Specically, we hypothesized that self-
compassion would be cultivated in veterans, in both those
who did and did not have PTSD as indicated by an increase
on the self-compassion questions (Hypothesis 1). Additionally,
we predicted that following the LKM-S there would be a
decrease in self-reported hyperarousal symptoms (Hypothesis
2a) and a reduction in HR and SCL as measures of physiological
arousal (Hypothesis 2b). We further predicted that there
would be an increase in self-reported social connectedness
(Hypothesis 3a) and an increase in HRV as a measure of
parasympathetic activation (Hypothesis 3b). Given that more
severe PTSD presentations can take longer to respond to
psychological interventions (Hogberg et al., 2008), we also
predicted that PTSD severity and emotion suppression would
be negatively associated with the increase of LKM-S related
self-compassion, social connectedness and HRV (Hypothesis
4a) and with the reduction in state hyperarousal and
physiological arousal as indicated by HR and SCL (Hypothesis
4b). Finally, we predicted that dispositional self-compassion
would result in a reverse association pattern and bepositively
associated with the increase of LKM-S related self-compassion,
social connectedness and HRV (Hypothesis 5a) and with
the reduction in state hyperarousal and physiological arousal
(Hypothesis 5b).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Research Design
e study used a repeated measures design to test Hypotheses
1, 2a, and 3a, using outcome score at Time 1 (pre-LKM-S)
and Time 2 (post-LKM-S) as dependent variables for self-report
state hyperarousal, social connectedness and self-compassion.
For SCL, HR, and HRV (Hypotheses 2b and 3b) the response
in relation to the baseline (prior to the LKM-S) per minute
over 11 min formed the repeated measures. In addition, a
one-sample design was used to determine signicant change
from the baseline that was set to zero. To investigate the role
of individual dierences (Hypotheses 4 and 5), a correlational
design was applied.
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
Participants
Fiy-six armed forces veterans (52 males, 4 females) took part
in the study. ey were recruited between November 2017 and
April 2018 through local veteran charities, the UK National Health
Service (NHS) and online via social media platforms. Participants
were eligible if they had experienced deployment to a combat
zone during their career in the armed forces and they experienced
combat or signicant exposure to danger during the deployment.
ey were excluded from the study if they had a severe mental
health problem such as schizophrenia or were acutely suicidal.
Participants were also excluded if they had a prior history of
cardiovascular problems including those that reported having
had heart surgery, heart attacks or being on any cardiovascular
medication. All participants gave written informed consent and
the protocol was approved by the South West – Cornwall and
Plymouth Research Ethics Committee (LREC) and the School
of Psychology Ethics Committee of the University of Exeter.
Target sample size was determined a priori using a power
calculation applying G*Power (Faul et al., 2009). Based on a
medium eect size, it was calculated that 54 participants were
needed to determine signicant pre-to post changes in the
dependent variables for a statistical power of 0.95 and alpha = 0.05.
is sample size was deemed sucient for examining the
secondary correlational hypotheses with three predictors and
an assumed large eect size (f2 = 0.35), however if only a medium
eect size is obtained (e.g., f2 = 0.15) a larger sample size (n = 77),
for a power of 0.80 and alpha = 0.05 would be necessary.
We managed to recruit the target sample for Hypotheses 1–3.
e demographic and clinical information about the nal
sample can be seen in Tab l e 1 . e prevalence of PTSD was
n = 19in the current sample (those who had received a previous
diagnosis from a psychiatrist). Based on scores on the PCL-5,
n = 15 (26.8%) currently met criteria for PTSD, n = 5 (8.9%)
met criteria for Subsyndromal PTSD1 on the PCL-5 and n = 36
(64.3%) did not have PTSD. All apart from one of the participants
had PTSD symptoms as a result of their deployment experiences
to a war zone, n = 18 (34%). One participant had PTSD as a
result of an accident on a training operation during a non-combat
deployment. All participants had been deployed to a combat
zone which included conicts such as the Falkland Islands,
Northern Ireland, Kosovo, Iraq, and Afghanistan. Deployment
length ranged from approximately two months to three years
(including leave periods). ere was an average of M = 4.06
(SD = 2.61; Median and Mode = 3) deployments to combat zones
per participant.
ere were n = 27 (48.2%) participants who had sustained
a physical injury while on deployment. Forty participants
(71.4%) had experienced at least one Traumatic Brain Injury
(TBI) as classied by the work of Williams et al. (2010) (see
Tabl e 2 ).
Measures and Materials
Self-Report Measures
e PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5; Weathers et al., 2013)
consists of 20 items, rated on a ve-point Likert-scale, assessing
the twenty DSM-5 symptoms of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder.
1
Subsyndromal PTSD is categorized as endorsing one Cluster B symptom
(intrusion symptoms), and one of either cluster C (avoidance), D (negative
alterations in cognitions), or E (hyperarousal).
TABLE1 | Demographic information for participants.
Characteristic N = 56
Gender, no. %
Female 4 (7.1)
Male 52 (92.9)
Age 52.1 (12.90)
Marital Status, no. %
Married 38 (67.9)
Single 5 (8.9)
Divorced/separated 5 (8.9)
Cohabiting 4 (7.1)
Engaged 4 (7.1)
Religion, no. %
No religion 22 (39.3)
Church of England 24 (42.9)
Catholic 3 (5.4)
Buddhist 2 (3.6)
Methodist 1 (1.8)
Other 2 (3.6)
Not stated 2 (3.6)
Occupation, no. %
Employed FT 27 (48.2)
Employed PT 13 (23.2)
Retired 15 (26.8)
Unemployed 1 (1.8)
Nationality, no. %
British 54 (96.4%)
Dual British Nationality 2 (3.6%)
Armed Forces Branch, no. %
Army 13 (23.2%)
Royal Navy 6 (10.7%)
Royal Marines 28 (50.0%)
Royal Air Force 6 (11.0%)
Army Reserves 1 (1.8%)
Royal Marines Reserves 1 (1.8%)
Special Forces 1 (1.8%)
Rank at discharge, no. %
Colonel 1 (1.8)
Lieutenant-Colonel 4 (7.1)
Major/Lieutenant Commander 6 (10.7)
Captain/Flight Lieutenant 7 (12.5)
Sub-Lieutenant 1 (1.8)
Sergeant Major 1 (1.8)
Warrant Ofcer 1st Class 4 (7.1)
Warrant Ofcer 2nd Class 1 (1.8)
Sergeant 9 (16.1)
Corporal/Leading Hand 9 (16.1)
Lance Corporal/Junior technician 5 (8.9)
Private/Marine/Senior Aircraftman 7 (12.5)
Physical injury on deployment, no. %
Yes 27 (48.2)
No 29 (51.8)
PTSD from combat experiences, no. %
Yes 18 (32.1)
No 38 (67.9)
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
Validation studies for the PCL-5 show strong internal consistency
(a = 0.94), test–retest reliability (r = 0.82), and convergent (rs = 0.74–
0.85) and discriminant validity (rs = 0.31–0.60; Blevins etal., 2015).
e Patient Health Questionnaire for depression (PHQ-9;
Kroenke etal., 2001) consists of 9 items, rated on a four-point
Likert-scale, which is used to establish levels of depression in
primary care and other medical settings. e PHQ-9 has
excellent reliability, internal = 0.89 and test re-test = 0.84 validity
for detecting depression = 0.95 (Solomon et al., 2000).
e Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ, Gross and
John, 2003) consists of 10 items, rated on a seven-point Likert-
scale, assessing the ability to regulate emotions in terms of
cognitive reappraisal2 and expressive suppression.3 Prior research
has shown that the ERQ has high internal reliability, and
convergent and discriminant validity (Gross and John, 2003).
The Self-Compassion Scale-Short Form (SCS-SF; Raes
et al., 2011), consists of 12 items rated on a five-point
Likert-scale assessing trait level of self-compassion. The short
form is near perfectly correlated with the Self Compassion
Scale (SCS) r = 0.98 (Raes et al., 2011) and the scale has
demonstrated validity and reliability (Neff, 2003a).
Visual Analogue scales (ranging from 0 to 100) were
used to establish state levels of self-compassion, hyperarousal
and social connectedness before and after the LKM-S. State
self-compassion was used as a manipulation check to determine
self-compassion induction in participants and also engagement
with the meditation. The VAS measure is adapted from
Kirschner et al. (2019) and questions are taken from the
Self-Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff, 2003a), social connectedness
questions are based on the state adult attachment measure
(SAAM; Gillath et al., 2009) and four adapted items from
the PCL-5 have been added to measure state hyperarousal.
The VAS has been used in previous studies (Kirschner etal.,
2019, 2021) which found Cronbach’s α = 0.66 for state affiliative
affect, state self-compassion (Cronbach’s α = 0.73 in this
sample) and state self-criticism (Cronbach’s alpha in this
sample was 0.73 for the inadequate self, 0.76 for the hated
self, and 0.77 for reassure self).
2
Cognitive Reappraisal is cognitive change which can alter how we interpret
a situation and therefore changes the emotional response (Lazarus and
Alfert, 1964).
3
Expressive Suppression is the ability to inhibit the emotive-expressive behavior
triggered by an emotional response (Gross, 1998).
Loving Kindness Meditation
A self-compassion meditation (LKM-S) was used to induce
self-compassion in the current study. The LKM-S has been
developed by the ACCEPT clinic, at the University of
Exeter Mood Disorder Centre. The LKM-S audio clip was
recorded by an experienced mindfulness practitioner, and
the LKM-S has been used in prior research (e.g., Kirschner
et al., 2019). Participants are asked to direct loving/friendly
feelings toward themselves and others and the audio clip
is 11.5 min in length.
Physiological Measurements
All physiological parameters were recorded continuously using
a BIOPAC MP150 system using the AcqKnowledge 4.2 (BIOPAC
Systems; Goleta, CA) soware. HR and HRV was determined
from the electrocardiogram (ECG) using standard procedures
(Berntson et al., 1997; Berntson and Stowell, 1998). ECG was
recorded from below the participant’s right collar bone and
the participant’s le lower ribcage using a BIOPAC ECG100C
amplier at a sampling rate of 1 kHz with a low pass lter
of 0.5 Hz and a high pass lter of 35 Hz. Skin conductance
levels (SCL) were recorded using a BIOPAC SCL100C amplier
and a skin resistant transducer (TSD203) from the middle
phalanx of the rst and second ngers of the participant’s
non-dominant hand, at a sampling rate of 500 Hz with a low
pass lter of 1.0 Hz. SCL was pre-processed using recommended
procedures (Lykken et al., 1966).
Procedure
Participants were self-selected and were recruited from a range
of veteran organizations and charities in the South West of
England, NHS services in Devon and online social media
adverts. Ninety-two people expressed an interest in the study
and 81 people completed the telephone screening. Seventy
participants were eligible and signed up to take part in the
study, 14 participants dropped out at this stage due to reasons
such as work commitments and illness. A total of 56 participants
completed the study. Eligible participants were booked in for
a testing session at the University of Exeter following a telephone
screening call. e study procedure, which lasted approximately
1–1.5 h, included collecting demographic information, completing
psychometric measures, and listening to the compassion
meditation (LKM-S) while physiological recordings were taken.
Standardized instructions were given for the VAS questions
and LKM-S audio. e participants listened to the LKM-S in
a quiet room in the psychophysiological laboratory at the
University of Exeter. Instructions were given to the participants
and they were given the opportunity to ask questions, before
the researcher le the room and the participant listened to
the LKM-S audio. All participants received a reimbursement
for their time of £10.
Data Analysis
ere was missing psychophysiological data for one participant
therefore the analysis for physiological data is based on 55
participants. No other missing data were detected in the data
TABLE2 | Traumatic Brain Injury Assessment (Williams etal., 2010).
Classication N = 56 (%)
0 = No history 15 (26.8)
1 = Feeling dazed and confused but no LOC, minor concussion 1 (1.8)
2 = LOC < 10 min, mild TBI 24 (42.9)
2a = LOC but no concussion symptoms 14 (25.0)
3 = LOC 10 to 30 min, complicated mild TBI 1 (1.8)
4 = LOC 30 to 60 min, moderate/severe TBI 1 (1.8)
5 = LOC > 60 min, very severe TBI 0
LOC = Loss of consciousness. TBI was assessed over the participant’s lifetime rather
than restricted to just their armed forces career.
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
set. Outliers were detected aer examining boxplots, however
inspection of their values did not reveal them to be extreme,
so they were kept in the analysis in order to include all
participants in the analysis. e outlying data points were
changed to the next closest value that was under the cut o,
which is a technique for dealing with outliers, while maintaining
the shape of the sample distribution but the outliers do not
distort the data (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007). Assumptions
of normality were not violated. For the regression analysis,
assumptions of independence of observations, linearity,
homoscedasticity, normality, and multicollinearity were
all fullled.
Data pre-processing and further statistical analyses of the
psychophysiological data followed established procedures; i.e.,
determining the size of the response in relation to a
pre-induction baseline as per previous studies (Kirschner
etal., 2019). To test Hypotheses 1a, 2a, and 3a, paired sample
t-tests were used to examine pre and post scores. Repeated
measures ANOVAs over the 11 timepoints were conducted
to investigate if there is a signicant main eect of time.
One sample t-tests were used for the psychophysiological
data to determine whether scores diered from zero (i.e.,
HRV; as index of parasympathetic activation and HR, SCL
as measure of sympathetic arousal; Hypotheses 2b and 3b).
Correlations and regression analysis were used to examine
the associations between responses to the LKM-S and individual
dierences in PTSD severity, emotion regulation, and
dispositional self-compassion (Hypotheses 4 and 5). For this,
residualized gains scores (RGS; Mintz etal., 1979; Williams
et al., 1984) were calculated for state hyperarousal, self-
compassion, and social connectedness, which were used as
outcome variables.
RESULTS
Change in State Self-Compassion
(Hypothesis 1)
In line with Hypothesis 1, there was a signicant increase in
state self-compassion from pre to post LKM-S, F (1, 55) = 13.62,
p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.199 (Figure 1).
Change in State Hyperarousal
(Hypothesis 2a)
In line with our hypothesis, there was a signicant reduction
in state arousal pre to post the LKM-S, F (1, 55) = 17.59,
p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.242 (Figure 2).
Physiological Arousal Response
(Hypothesis 2b)
Heart Rate Response
Overall, there was a signicant eect of time on HR response,
F (10, 45) = 3.57, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.442 (Figure 3) suggesting a
rise of HR toward the end of the LKM-S. Although the one-sample
t-test revealed that mean HR response (M = 0.52, SD = 2.71) did
not signicantly dier from zero, t(54) = 1.68, p = 0.09, Cohen’s
d = 0.27, it is of interest that, the one-sample t-test revealed that
HR response to directing compassion towards the self (6–11 min;
M = 0.87, SD = 3.36) revealed a signicantly increased HR,
t(54) = 2.01, p = 0.04, Cohen’s d = 0.27. is indicates that a one-o
LKM-S did not signicantly reduce physiological arousal as indicated
by HR, on the contrary, there is indication of an increase in
physiological arousal when individuals direct compassion
toward themselves.
Skin Conductance Level Response
Overall, there was a signicant eect of time, F(10, 45) = 6.00,
p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.571 (Figure4) suggesting a reduction of SCL.
A one-sample t-test revealed that the mean SCL response
(M = −0.04, SD = 0.07) was signicantly lower than zero,
t(54) = −4.33, p < 0.001, C ohen’s d = −0.58. is indicates that
there was a reduction in sympathetic arousal as indicated by
FIGURE1 | Change in state self-compassion (pre to post).
FIGURE2 | Change in state hyperarousal (pre to post).
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
SCL, during the one-o LKM-S, and a medium eect size
was observed.
Change in Social Connectedness
(Hypothesis 3a)
Contrary to our hypothesis, there was no signicant increase
in social connectedness from pre to post the LKM-S, F
(1, 55) = 2.36, p = 0.130, ηp2 = 0.041 (Figure 5).
Parasympathetic Response
(Hypothesis 3b)
Heart Rate Variability Response
ere was a signicant eect of time, F (10, 45) = 2.57, p = 0.015,
ηp2 = 0.364 (Figure 6). A one-sample t-test revealed that mean
HRV response (M = −0.05, SD = 1.06) did not signicantly dier
from zero, t(54) = −0.61, p = 0.54, Cohen’s d = −0.08. is indicates
that a one-o LKM-S did not signicantly increase
parasympathetic activation as indicated by HRV.
Role of Individual Differences (Hypotheses
4 and 5)
Effects of Individual Differences on Change in
Self-Compassion
A multiple regression was used to predict the change in
self-compassion, as measured by the VAS, pre–post the loving
kindness meditation (LKM-S). Overall, the regression model
was signicant, F(1,53) = 4.08, p = 0.048; R2 = 0.07, and explained
7% of variance. Overall, levels of state self-compassion at
both pre and post time points were associated with PTSD
(r = −0.496, p < 0.001), trait self-compassion (r = 0.498, p < 0.001)
and emotion suppression (r = −0.296, p = 0.027). However,
PTSD severity, trait self-compassion, and emotion suppression
were not signicantly associated with LKM-S-induced state
self-compassion change. Instead, the greater increase in state
self-compassion was associated with reduced skin conductance
to compassion for others (rst 5 min of the LKM-S), β = −0.267,
t = −2.02, p = 0.048.
FIGURE3 | Heart rate (HR) response over time.
FIGURE4 | Skin conductance (SCL) response over time.
FIGURE5 | Change in social connectedness (pre to post).
FIGURE6 | HR variability over time.
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
Effects of Individual Differences on Change in
Social Connectedness
A multiple regression was used to predict the change in social
connectedness pre to post the LKM-S. Overall, the regression
model was signicant, F(1,53) = 4.26, p = 0.044; R2 = 0.07, and
explained 7% of variance. ough the levels of social
connectedness at both time points were negatively associated
with PTSD (r = −0.649, p < 0.001), self-compassion (r = 0.594,
p < 0.001) and emotion suppression (r = −0.433, p = 0.001), PTSD
severity, trait self-compassion, and emotion suppression were
not signicantly associated with LKM-S-induced change in social
connectedness. Instead, greater increase in social connectedness
associated with higher baseline HRV, β = 0.273, t = 2.06, p = 0.044.
Effects of Individual Differences on Change in
State-Hyperarousal
A multiple regression was used to predict the change in state-
hyperarousal, as measured by the VAS, pre–post the loving
kindness meditation (LKM-S). Overall the regression model
was signicant, F(2,52) = 9.22, p < 0.001; R2 = 0.26, and explained
26% of variance. Although state arousal at both time points
was associated with PTSD (r = 0.768, p < 0.001), self-compassion
(r = −0.453, p < 0.001) and emotion suppression (r = 0.318,
p = 0.017), PTSD severity, trait self-compassion and emotion
suppression were not signicantly associated with LKM-S-
induced arousal change. Instead, greater state arousal reduction
was associated with state change in social connectedness,
β = −0.403, t = −3.37, p = 0.001, and baseline heart rate, β = 0.347,
t = 2.90, p = 0.005.
Effects of Individual Differences on Heart Rate
Response
A regression model was used to predict HR response in compassion
to others, which was signicant: F(1,53) = 4.51, p = 0.038; R2 = 0.08
(8% of variance explained). However, only emotion suppression
was signicantly associated with change in HR when directing
compassion to others, β = 0.280, t = 2.12, p = 0.038.
Effects of Individual Differences on Skin
Conductance Level Response
PTSD severity, trait self-compassion, and emotion suppression
were not signicantly associated with LKM-S-induced SCL response.
Effects of Individual Differences on Heart Rate
Variability Response
A regression model was used to predict the HRV response when
directing compassion to the self, which was signicant, F(1,53) = 5.54,
p = 0.022; R2 = 0.10, and explained 10% of variance. However only
trait self-compassion was signicantly associated with change in
HRV when directing compassion to self, β = 0.308, t = 2.35, p = 0.022.
Exploratory Analyses: Traumatic Brain
Injury
No signicant associations were found between TBI severity
and any of the variables.
DISCUSSION
is study aimed to investigate the eects of a brief, one-o
loving kindness meditation for the self (LKM-S) on state self-
compassion, hyperarousal, social connectedness, and physiological
responses, in armed forces veterans who had experienced
deployment to a combat zone. Following one’s listening to the
LKM-S, participants had a signicant increase in state self-
compassion and a signicant reduction in self-reported
hyperarousal. Additionally, there was a decrease in the SCL
response following the LKM-S. However, there was no increase
in state social connectedness and there was not the expected
increase in HRV and decrease in HR. Interestingly, the
physiological responses were partially associated with individual
dierences in trait self-compassion and emotion regulation but
not with PTSD severity as we predicted.
Overall, the ndings are partially in line with previous
research by Kearney etal. (2013) who found that veterans who
took part in a 1.5 h weekly loving-kindness meditation course
over 12 weeks, had increased levels of state self-compassion
and reduced PTSD symptoms, including levels of state arousal,
immediately aer the treatment and at a 3-month follow-up.
Additionally, changes in self-compassion mediated the changes
in PTSD symptoms pre to post treatment. However, in the
current study the changes in the physiological responses were
not in line with our predictions except for SCL. We had
hypothesized this in line with previous ndings from Kirschner
et al. (2019) who found that the LKM-S induced a pattern of
reduced physiological arousal (HR and SCL reductions) and
increased parasympathetic activity (HRV increase).
ere are several possible explanations for the physiological
ndings of this study. First, there could be a “dose–response”
eect, whereby a one-o administration of a short (11.5 min)
LKM-S is not enough to have an impact at a physiological level
in a clinical sample. In line with PTSD theories (Ehlers and
Clark, 2000) and the tripartite model of emotion regulation
(Gilbert, 2009a), it may bemore challenging for trauma survivors,
in particular those with higher PTSD symptoms, to switch from
“threat and self-protection” system to the “soothing and
contentment” emotion system. In contrast to the ndings of the
current study, a longer loving-kindness meditation course (Kearney
etal., 2013) where veterans attended weekly sessions over 12-weeks,
led to signicant reductions in PTSD symptoms and also an
increase in self-compassion. Additionally, the course was run by
experienced mindfulness teachers who guided participants through
loving-kindness meditations and also encouraged discussion around
integrating loving-kindness meditation into everyday life.
Participants were also provided with a book and CD to encourage
practice between the weekly sessions. Comparing our and Kearney’s
ndings suggests that a longer and more in-depth loving-kindness
meditation practice may be needed to establish changes on a
physiological level in individuals with an overactivated threat
system such as those with PTSD.
Second, and in support of the notion that an overactivated
threat system may aect a person’s ability to engage eectively
with a one-o LKM-S, the absence of the expected physiological
eect is more in line with previous research in a sample of
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
individuals with a history of recurrent depression, some of
which had reported early childhood adversity (Kirschner etal.,
2021). Prior to an 8-week course of mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy (MBCT), the participants did not have a reduction
in physiological arousal and an increase in parasympathetic
activation aer listening to the LKM-S, despite self-reported
increases in state self-compassion, which is similar to our
ndings in a veteran population. On the contrary, participants
in the current study showed an increase in physiological arousal
through an elevated heart-rate, and in Kirschner et al. (2021)
showed an indication of increased arousal and reduced HRV,
when directing compassion to themselves, thus supporting the
theoretical accounts cited above (Ehlers and Clark, 2000; Gilbert,
2009a). Interestingly, the group of recurrent depressed individuals
in Kirschner et al. (2021) who completed the MBCT, which
has been shown to increase dispositional self-compassion despite
not having a direct compassion component (Kuyken et al.,
2010), showed the predicted reduction in HR and SCL and
increase in HRV post intervention, whereas the untreated
control group showed a more elevated SCL response at the
second exposure to the LKM-S. Taken together, our and the
previous studies (Kirschner, 2021) suggest that a “one-o”
self-compassion meditation in individuals with an overactivated
threat system may result in a subjective eect for participants
as recorded by self-report measures, but does not on an
automatic, habitual level, which would have been reected in
participants’ physiological responses.
In contrast to our hypothesis, we also did not nd that
the one-o LKM-S increased a feeling of social connectedness
as had been previously reported by Hutcherson et al. (2008)
in civilian populations. Given that self-compassion approaches
activate the “soothing and contentment” system, which is
underpinned by the parasympathetic nervous system and enables
feelings of social safety and connectedness (e.g., Gilbert, 2010),
wehypothesized that participants would experience an increase
in feelings of social connectedness aer listening to the LKM-S.
It might be that similarly to physiological responses, there is
a “dose–response” eect, and participants need longer than a
short meditation to experience changes in their felt experience
of social connectedness. In addition, this may be due to an
inherent lack of social support given that veterans have oen
needed to adapt to a dierent social system in civilian life
aer leaving the armed forces. is can mean that veterans
are without the social networks and social support that were
so adaptive during service in the armed forces (Wessely, 2006)
and therefore might explain why we did not see the expected
changes following the LKM-S in the current study.
We hypothesized that PTSD severity and emotion suppression
would benegatively associated with the increase of LKM-S related
self-compassion, social connectedness and HRV (Hypothesis 4a),
given that it is well-established that severity of PTSD aects
participants response to and ability to engage in psychological
interventions (Hogberg et al., 2008). However, we did not nd
support for a role of PTSD symptoms. is was unexpected
but could bedue to the sample largely consisting of participants
not fullling criteria for full or subsyndromal PTSD resulting
in overall low levels of PTSD severity in our participants. Instead,
wefound that there was partial support for role of dispositional
self-compassion and emotion suppression. ose with lower levels
of self-compassion and those who use strategies such as emotion
suppression, found it more dicult to engage with the LKM-S
exercise, which is in line with the work by Gilbert (2010) around
fears and blocks to compassion in those with low levels of
dispositional self-compassion, i.e., people who have low levels
of self-compassion can nd it threatening to engage in compassion
which is seen in an elevated threat response (Gilbert, 2010).
Oen, this is due to having experienced past trauma and especially
those who have experienced interpersonal trauma, such as that
in combat, are particularly susceptible to this aversive response.
In addition, people who use emotion suppression as a strategy
to manage emotions, nd it more dicult to engage with and
experience positive emotional states when they arise, such as
social safety and connectedness associated with the “soothing
and contentment” system (Gilbert, 2010). Although suppressing
emotions can be adaptive for soldiers in combat situations and
is likely to be reinforced as a habitual response (Neilson et al.,
2020), it can cause problems once veterans are re-immersed in
civilian life as it is dicult to establish social safety and feel
connected to others (e.g., Prescott, 2012). Similarly to altering
physiological responses, it is more dicult to change emotion
suppression in veterans when it has been conditioned as part
of their occupational role combat trauma (Prescott, 2012). Given
the high percentage of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) in
our sample, which could aect emotion regulation abilities and
contribute to posttraumatic stress symptoms (Belanger et al.,
2009; Mounce etal., 2013), weadditionally explored the associations
of mTBIs and response to LKM-S and our symptom and
dispositional measures. No signicant associations were revealed,
which suggests that mTBIs were not associated with individuals’
ability to engage in a one-o self-compassion meditation.
LIMITATIONS
Overall, the interpretation of the ndings of the current study
needs to take into account several limitations.
Firstly, there was not a control group of participants who
were not exposed to the LKM-S. is means we were not
able to determine whether the changes noted were due to
other factors, such as becoming more comfortable in the
surroundings. It would have been interesting to determine
whether there were baseline dierences in physiological responses
in veterans who have experienced deployment to a combat
zone vs. a civilian group of trauma survivors who have not
held a role in the armed forces, as studies have shown that
combat trauma results in higher levels of hyperarousal than
does other types of trauma (Prescott, 2012). Additionally, there
was no follow up aer the study, so we are not aware for
how long the changes in self-compassion were maintained.
e VAS scale was used in the study as a self-report measure,
and it is not known whether individual dierences in emotional
expression and motor control aected the responses across
participants. In terms of the sample, there was an unequal
proportion of men and women in the study, which is a common
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
recruitment bias in veteran populations and is representative
of the armed forces population. ere was also a large dierence
in the severity of PTSD in participants – some had no symptoms
at all, whereas others had very severe levels of PTSD. Additionally,
the measurement of PTSD in the current study relied on self-
report measures rather than clinician-administered interviews,
so it might be that some participants felt unable to disclose
their distress in the study setting. Another limitation of the
sample is that although the screening process excluded
participants who had not experienced combat or exposure to
life threatening danger during their deployment to combat
zones, the length of and number of deployments was not
controlled for. Lastly, although the target sample size for the
current study was calculated a priori, the sample size recruited
meant that for the regression analyses, we were only able to
detect medium-to-large eect sizes.
CONCLUSION
Overall, the results demonstrate that there are temporary benets
of a one-o compassion meditation on self-report measures of
self-compassion and hyperarousal but not on physiological responses.
Our results suggest that a self-compassion-based approach appeared
acceptable for veterans who are experiencing emotional distress
including PTSD. Ours and previous ndings considered together
(Kearney etal., 2013; Kirschner etal., 2021) suggest that compassion-
based approaches are potentially benecial for clinical samples
and could extend or complement existing psychological treatments
for veterans with PTSD, given the well-established role of social
support for recovery from trauma and PTSD, (Brewin etal., 2000;
Ozer et al., 2003). e ndings also highlight the importance of
individual dierences and the need for a longer-term intervention
in such populations. Given the need for treatments for PTSD to
create lasting changes that are on a psychological as well as
physiological level, increasing HRV could bean important treatment
target for development of better self-soothing/emotion regulation
in trauma survivors (e.g., Arch etal., 2013). Additionally, reducing
hyperarousal and increasing social connectedness are areas that
can be targeted in treatment for PTSD.
FUTURE RESEARCH
Future studies should investigate the individual dierence factors
identied in the current study, such as the eect on the
participant’s ability to engage with self-compassion approaches,
including cultivating feelings of compassion toward oneself and
others (e.g., Gilbert, 2010). For example, Creaser et al. (2021)
found that people who had high levels of PTSD hyperarousal
symptoms (also common in veterans) had a large threat response
as indicated by physiological measures, when instructed to
direct compassion toward oneself. Gaining further understanding
of the eects of individual dierences on ability to engage
with self-compassion, could be key in understanding why
psychological therapies for combat trauma are less eective
compared with other types of trauma (Bradley et al., 2005);
therefore, it is important that researchers continue to develop
an understanding in this area. Additionally, it might be that
there are dierent PTSD symptom patterns and a link between
numbing/emotion suppression and threat response indicators,
such as HR (Gutner et al., 2010), which also warrant further
investigation. Activating self-compassion in veterans could
provide a helpful avenue to address the issues of shame and
moral injury (Litz etal., 2009; Saraiya and Lopex-Castor, 2016)
that also appear to maintain hyperarousal symptoms in PTSD
(Feiring and Taska, 2005) and can lead to social withdrawal
and lack of social connectedness (Litz et al., 2009).
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
e raw data cannot be shared because the participants did
not agree that their data can be shared at the consent stage.
Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Anke
Karl at A.Karl@exeter.ac.uk.
ETHICS STATEMENT
e studies involving human participants were reviewed and
approved by the South West – Cornwall and Plymouth Research
Ethics Committee (LREC) and the School of Psychology Ethics
Committee of the University of Exeter. e patients/participants
provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
SG, HW, and AK contributed to the conception and design
of the study. SG and AK organized the database and performed
the statistical analysis. SG wrote the rst dra of the manuscript.
AK wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed
to the article and approved the submitted version.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders. 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Arch, J. J., Ayers, C. R., Baker, A., Almklov, E., Dean, D. J., and Craske, M. G.
(2013). Randomized clinical trial of adapted mindfulness-based stress
reduction versus group cognitive behavioural therapy for heterogeneous
anxiety disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 185–196. doi: 10.1016/j.
brat.2013.01.003
Barnard, L. K., and Curry, J. F. (2011). Self-compassion: conceptualisation, correlates,
and interventions. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 15, 289–303. doi: 10.1037/a0025754
Belanger, H. G., Kretzmer, T., Vanderploeg, R. D., and French, L. M. (2009).
Symptom complaints following combat-related traumatic brain injury:
relationship to traumatic brain injury severity and posttraumatic stress
disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 194–199. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990841
Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M.,
et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats.
Psychophysiology 34, 623–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 11 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
Berntson, G. G., and Stowell, J. R. (1998). ECG artifacts and heart period variability:
don’t miss a beat! Psychophysiology 35, 127–132. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3510127
Blevins, C. A., Weathers, F. W., Davis, M. T., Witte, T. K., and Domino, J. L.
(2015). e posttraumatic stress disorder checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5):
development and initial psychometric evaluation. J. Trauma. Stress 28, 489–498.
doi: 10.1002/jts.22059
Bradley, R., Greene, J., Russ, E., Dutra, L., and Westen, D. (2005). A
multidimensional meta-analysis of psychotherapy for PTSD. Am. J. Psychiatry
162, 214–227. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.214
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., and Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-analysis of risk
factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. J. Consult.
Clin. Psychol. 68, 748–766. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.748
Conoscenti, L. M., Vine, V., Papa, A., and Litz, B. T. (2009). “Scanning for
danger: readjustment to the noncombat environment,” in Living and Surviving
in harm’s Way: A Psychological Treatment Handbook for Pre and Post-
Deployment of Military Personnel. eds. S. M. Freeman, B. A. Moore and
A. Freeman (New York, NY: Routledge), 126–145.
Creaser, J., Storr, J., and Karl, A. (2021). Brain responses to a self-compassion
induction in trauma survivors with and without PTSD. Frontiers in Psychology
(in press).
Dahm, K., Meyer, E. C., Ne, K., Kimbrel, N. A., Gulliver, S. B., and Morissette, S. B.
(2015). Mindfulness, self-compassion, posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms,
and functional disability in U.S. Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans. J. Trauma.
Stress 28, 460–464. doi: 10.1002/jts.22045
Daneshvar, S., Shaei, M., and Basharpoor, S. (2020). Group-based compassion-
focused therapy on experiential avoidance, meaning-in-life, and sense of coherence
in female survivors of intimate partner violence with PTSD: a randomized
controlled trial. J. Interpers. Violence. doi: 10.1177/0886260520958660 [Epub
ahead of print]
Demers, A. (2011). When veterans return: the role of community in reintegration.
J. Loss Trauma 16, 160–179. doi: 10.1080/15325024.2010.519281
Department of the Army (1984). Field Manual (FM 21-75): Combat Skills of
the Soldier. Washington DC: Headquarters Department of the Army.
DeVries, C. A., Glasper, E. R., and Detillion, C. E. (2003). Social modulation
of stress responses. Physiol. Behav. 79, 399–407. doi: 10.1016/
s0031-9384(03)00152-5
Dunn, R., Brooks, S., Rubin, J., and Greenberg, N. (2015). Psychological impact
of traumatic events. Occup. Health Work 12, 17–21.
Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress
disorder. Behav. Res. er. 38, 319–345. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967
(99)00123-0
Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M. (2009). Post-traumatic stress disorder: the development
of eective psychological treatments. Nord. J. Psychiatry 62, 11–18. doi:
10.1080/08039480802315608
Eisenberger, N. I., and Cole, S. W. (2012). Social neuroscience and health:
neurophysiological mechanisms linking social ties with physical health. Nat.
Neurosci. 15, 669–674. doi: 10.1038/nn.3086
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Bucher, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power
analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses.
Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Fear, N. T., Jones, M., Murphy, D., Hull, L., Iversen, A. C., Coker, B., et al.
(2010). What are the consequences of deployment to Iraq and Afghanistan
on the mental health of the UK armed forces? A cohort study. Lancet 375,
1783–1797. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60672-1
Feiring, C., and Taska, L. S. (2005). e persistence of shame following sexual
abuse: a longitudinal look at risk and recovery. Child Maltreat. 10, 337–349.
doi: 10.1177/1077559505276686
Freedman, S. A., Gilad, M., Ankri, Y., Roziner, I., and Shalev, A. Y. (2015).
Social relationship satisfaction and PTSD: which is the chicken and which
is the egg? Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 6:28864. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.28864
Germain, A., and Neilsen, T. (2003). Sleep pathophysiology in posttraumatic
stress disorder and idiopathic nightmare suerers. Biol. Psychiatry 54,
1092–1098. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00071-4
Germer, C. K., and Ne, K. D. (2013). Self-compassion in clinical practice.
J. Clin. Psychol. 69, 856–867. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22021
Germer, C. K., and Ne, K. D. (2015). “Cultivating self-compassion in trauma
survivors,” in Mindfulness-Oriented Interventions for Trauma: Integrating
Contemplative Practices. eds. V. M. Follette, J. Briere, D. Rozelle, J. W. Hopper
and D. I. Rome (New York, NY, US: Guilford Press), 43–58.
Gilbert, P. (2009a). e Compassionate Mind. London: Constable Robinson.
Gilbert, P. (2010). An introduction to compassion focused therapy in cognitive
behaviour therapy. J. Cogn. Psychother. 3, 97–112. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2010.3.2.97
Gillath, O., Nole, E. E., and Stockdale, G. D. (2009). Development and validation
of a State Adult Attachment Measure (SAAM). J. Res. Pers. 43, 362–373.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.009
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent
consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
74, 224–237. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.224
Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual dierences in two emotion
regulation processes: implications for aect, relationships, and well-being.
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Gutner, C. A., Pineles, S. L., Grin, M. G., Bauer, M. R., Weierich, M. R.,
and Resick, P. A. (2010). Physiological predictors of posttraumatic stress
disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 23, 775–784. doi: 10.1002/jts.20582
Hiraoka, R., Meyer, E. C., Kimbrel, N. A., DeBeer, B. B., Gulliver, S. B., and
Morissette, S. B. (2015). Self-compassion as a prospective predictor of PTSD
symptom severity among trauma-exposed U.S. Iraq and Afghanistan war
veterans. J. Trauma. Stress 28, 127–133. doi: 10.1002/jts.21995
Hogberg, G., Pagani, M., Sundin, O., Soares, J., Aberg-Wistedt, A., Tarnell, B.,
et al. (2008). Treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder with eye movement
desensitization and reprocessing: outcome is stable in 35-month follow-up.
Psychiatry Res. 159, 101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.10.019
Hoge, C. W., Castro, C. A., Messer, S. C., McGurk, D., Cotting, D. I., and
Koman, R. L. (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health
problems and barriers to care. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 13–22. doi: 10.1056/
NEJMoa040603
Hotopf, M., Hull, L., Fear, N. T., Browne, T., Horn, O., Iversen, A., et al. (2003).
e health of UK military personnel who deployed to the 2003 Iraq war: a
cohort study. Lancet 367, 1731–1741. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68662-5
Hutcherson, C. A., Seppala, E. M., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Loving-kindness
meditation increases social connectedness. Emotion 8, 720–724. doi: 10.1037/
a0013237
Kang, H. K., Natelson, B. H., Mahan, C. M., Lee, K. Y., and Murphy, F. M.
(2003). Post-traumatic stress disorder and chronic fatigue syndrome-like
illness in Gulf War veterans: A population-based survey of 30,000 veterans.
Am. J. Epidemiol. 157, 141–148. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf187
Kearney, D. J., Malte, C. A., McManus, C., Martinez, M. E., Felleman, B.,
and Simpson, T. L. (2013). Loving-kindness meditation for posttraumatic
stress disorder: a pilot study. J. Trauma. Stress 26, 426–434. doi: 10.1002/
jts.21832
Kelley, M. L., Bravo, A. J., Davies, R. L., Hamrick, H. C., Vinci, C., and
Redman, J. C. (2019). Moral injury and suicidality among combat-wounded
veterans: e moderating eects of social connectedness and self-compassion.
Psychol. Trauma eory Res. Pract. Policy 11, 621–629. doi: 10.1037/tra0000447
Kimble, M. O., Fleming, K., and Bennion, K. A. (2013). Contributors to
hypervigilance in a military and civilian sample. J. Interpers. Violence 28,
1672–1692. doi: 10.1177/0886260512468319
King, D. W., Ta, C., King, L. A., Hammond, C., and Stone, E. R. (2006).
Directionality of the association between social support and posttraumatic
stress disorder: a longitudinal investigation. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 36, 2980–2992.
doi: 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00138.x
Kirschner, H., Kuyken, W., and Karl, A. (2021). A biobehavioural approach to
understand how mindfulness-based cognitive therapy reduces dispositional negative
self-bias in recurrent depression. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/53trs [Epub ahead of print]
Kirschner, H., Kuyken, W., Wright, K., Roberts, H., Brejcha, C., and Karl, A.
(2019). Soothing your heart and feeling connected: A new experimental
paradigm to study the benets of self-compassion. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7,
545–565. doi: 10.1177/2167702618812438
Kok, B. C., Herrell, R. K., omas, J. L., and Hoge, C. W. (2012). Posttraumatic
stress disorder associate with combat service in Iraq or Afghanistan: reconciling
prevalence dierences between studies. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 200, 444–450.
doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182532312
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., and Williams, J. B. (2001). e PHQ-9: validity
of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 16, 606–613.
doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, R. S., Byford, S., et al.
(2010). How does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behav. Res.
e r. 48, 1105–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.003
Gerdes et al. Self-Compassion in Armed Forces Veterans
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 12 January 2022 | Volume 12 | Article 780319
Lazarus, R. S., and Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of threat by experimentally
altering cognitive appraisal. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 69, 195–205. doi: 10.1037/
h0044635
Leaviss, J., and Uttley, L. (2015). Psychotheraputic benets of compassion-
focused therapy: an early systematic review. Psychol. Med. 45, 927–945. doi:
10.1017/S0033291714002141
Lee, D. A. (2009). “Compassion-focused cognitive therapy for shame-based
trauma memories and ashbacks in post-traumatic stress disorder,” in A
Casebook of Cognitive erapy for Traumatic Stress Reactions. ed. N. Grey
(UK: Routledge), 230–245.
Litz, B. T., Stein, N., Delaney, E., Lebowitz, L., Nash, W. P., Silva, C., et al.
(2009). Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: A preliminary model
and intervention strategy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 695–706. doi: 10.1016/j.
cpr.2009.07.003
Long, P., and Ne, K. D. (2018). Self-compassion is associated with
reduced self-presentation concerns and increased student communication
behavior. Learn. Individ. Dier. 67, 223–231. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.
2018.09.003
Lykken, D. T., Rose, R., Luther, B., and Maley, M. (1966). Correcting
psychophysiological measures for individual dierences in range. Psychol.
Bull. 66, 481–484. doi: 10.1037/h0023922
MacBeth, A., and Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: A meta-analysis
of the association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clin. Psychol.
Re v. 32, 545–552. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.06.003
Mantzios, M. (2014). Exploring the relationship between worry and impulsivity
in Military Recruits: the role of mindfulness and self-compassion as potential
mediators. Stress. Health 30, 397–404. doi: 10.1002/smi.2617
McAllister, L., Callaghan, J. E. M., and Fellin, L. C. (2018). Masculinities and
emotional expression in UK servicemen: ‘Big boys don’t cry’? J. Gend. Stud.
28, 257–270. doi: 10.1080/09589236.2018.1429898
Meyer, E. C., Szabo, Y. Z., Frankfurt, S. B., Kimbrel, N. A., DeBeer, B. B.,
and Morissette, S. B. (2019). Predictors of recovery from post-deployment
posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in war veterans: e contributions
of psychological exibility, mindfulness, and self-compassion. Behav. Res.
e r. 114, 7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.01.002
Mintz, J., Luborsky, L., and Christoph, P. (1979). Measuring the outcomes of
psychotherapy: ndings of the Penn Psychotherapy Project. J. Consult. Clin.
Psychol. 47, 319–334. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.47.2.319
Mounce, L. T. A., Jones, J. M., Jetten, J., Haslam, S. A., and Williams, W. H.
(2013). Neurogenic and psychogenic acute Postconcussion symptoms can
be identied Aer mild traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil.
28, 397–405. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e318252dd75
Ne, K. (2003a). Development and validation of a scale to measure self-
compassion. Self Identity 2, 223–250. doi: 10.1080/15298860390209035
Neilson, E. C., Singh, S. R., Harper, K. L., and Teng, E. J. (2020). Traditional
masculinity ideology, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptom severity,
and treatment in service members and veterans: a systematic review. Psychol.
Men Masculinities 21, 578–592. doi: 10.1037/men0000257
Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., and Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of
posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis.
Psychol. Bull. 129, 52–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.52
Pearlman, L. A., and Curtois, C. A. (2005). Clinical applications of the attachment
framework: relational treatment of complex trauma. J. Trauma. Stress 18,
449–459. doi: 10.1002/jts.20052
Pietrzak, R. H., Johnson, D. C., Goldstein, M. B., Malley, J. C., and Southwick, S. M.
(2009). Psychological resilience and postdeployment social support protect against
traumatic stress and depressive symptoms in soldiers returning from operations
enduring freedom and Iraqi freedom. Depression and Anxiety 26, 745–751.
doi: 10.1002/da.20558
Pole, N. (2007). e psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-
analysis. Psychol. Bull. 133, 725–746. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.725
Prescott, M. R. (2012). e dierences between war and civilian related traumatic
events and the presentation of posttraumatic stress disorder and suicidal
ideation in a sample of National Guard soldiers. Doctoral dissertation.
University of Michigan.
Rabon, J. K., Hirsch, J. K., Kaniuka, A. R., Sirois, F., Brooks, B. D., and
Ne, K. (2019). Self-compassion and suicide risk in veterans: when the
going gets tough, do the tough benet more from self-compassion? Mindfulness
10, 2544–2554. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01221-8
Raes, F., Pommier, E., Ne, K. D., and Van Gucht, D. (2011). Construction
and factorial validation of a short form of the Self-Compassion Scale. Clin.
Psychol. Psychother. 18, 250–255. doi: 10.1002/cpp.702
Reit, R. (2009). e relationship between the Military’s masculine culture and
service member’s help-seeking behaviours. Doctoral dissertation. Marquette
University.
Saraiya, T., and Lopex-Castor, T. (2016). Ashamed and afraid: a scoping review
of the role of shame in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). J. Clin. Med.
5:94. doi: 10.3390/jcm5110094
Solomon, D. A., Keller, M. B., Leon, A. C., Mueller, T. I., Lavori, P. W.,
Shea, T., et al. (2000). Multiple recurrences of major depressive disorder.
Am. J. Psychiatr. 157, 229–233. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.229
Southwick, S. M., Vythilingam, M., and Charney, D. S. (2005). e psychobiology
of depression and resilience to stress: implications for prevention and treatment.
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1, 255–291. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143948
Steen, M. P., Di Lemma, L., Finnegan, A., Wepa, D., and McGhee, S. (2021).
Self-compassion and veteran’s health: A scoping review. J. Veterans Stud. 7,
86–130. doi: 10.21061/jvs.v7i1.219
Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics (5th
Edn). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.
Ta, C., Kaloupek, D., Schumm, J., Marshall, A., Panuzio, J., King, D., et al.
(2007). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms, physiological reactivity,
alcohol problems, and aggression among military veterans. J. Abnorm. Psychol.
116, 498–507. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.498
ompson, B., and Waltz, J. (2008). Self-compassion and PTSD symptom severity.
J. Trauma. Stress. 21, 556–558. doi: 10.1002/jts.20374
Tick, E. (2005). War and the Soul: Health our nation’s Veterans from Post-
Traumatic Stress Disorder. Wheaton, IL: Quest Books.
Weathers, F. W., Litz, B. T., Keane, T. M., Palmieri, P. A., Marx, B. P., and
Schnurr, P. P. (2013). e PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5) – Standard
[Measurement instrument]. Available at: https://www.ptsd.va.gov/
Wessely, S. (2006). Twentieth-century theories on combat motivation and
breakdown. J. Contemp. Hist. 41, 268–286. doi: 10.1177/0022009406062067
Williams, W. H., Cordan, G., Mewse, A. J., Tonks, J., and Burgess, C. N. W.
(2010). Self-reported traumatic brain injury in male young oenders: a risk
factor for re-oending, poor mental health and violence? Neuropsychol.
Rehabil. 20, 801–812. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2010.519613
Williams, R. H., Zimmerman, D. W., Rich, J. M., and Steed, J. L. (1984).
Empirical estimates of the validity of four measures of change. Percept.
Mot. Skills 58, 891–896. doi: 10.2466/pms.1984.58.3.891
Conict of Interest: e authors declare that the research was conducted in
the absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could beconstrued
as a potential conict of interest.
Publisher’s Note: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent those of their aliated organizations,
or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may
be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is
not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Copyright © 2022 Gerdes, Williams and Karl. is is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). e use,
distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication
in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.