ArticlePDF Available

Linkages between Psychedelics and Meditation in a Population-Based Sample in the United States

Authors:

Abstract

There are neurophysiological and phenomenological overlaps between psychedelic and meditative states, but there is little evidence on how exposure to psychedelics might be associated with meditation-related variables. We assessed lifetime classic psychedelic use, ego dissolution during one’s most intense experience using a classic psychedelic, and exposure to meditation in a representative sample (n = 953) of American adults. Those who reported experience with meditation were invited to complete a follow-up survey (n = 536, 92.1% response rate) measuring meditation-related variables. Models controlled for a range of potential confounds. Exposure to meditation was associated with lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution in covariate-adjusted models. In addition, among meditators, greater ego dissolution was associated with more frequent meditation practice. Both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution were associated with enlightenment as motivation to practice meditation as well as lower likelihood of overall perceived barriers to meditation practice. Ego dissolution was positively associated with finding meditation more effective. Neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolution was associated with greater likelihood of meditation-related adverse effects. Taken together, results support potential synergy between psychedelics and meditation, but randomized controlled trials are necessary to establish safety and evaluate potential causal relationships.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ujpd20
Journal of Psychoactive Drugs
ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ujpd20
Linkages between Psychedelics and Meditation in
a Population-Based Sample in the United States
Otto Simonsson & Simon B. Goldberg
To cite this article: Otto Simonsson & Simon B. Goldberg (2022): Linkages between Psychedelics
and Meditation in a Population-Based Sample in the United States, Journal of Psychoactive Drugs,
DOI: 10.1080/02791072.2021.2022816
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2021.2022816
© 2022 The Author(s). Published with
license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
View supplementary material
Published online: 09 Jan 2022.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Linkages between Psychedelics and Meditation in a Population-Based Sample in
the United States
Otto Simonsson
a,b,c
and Simon B. Goldberg
a,d
a
Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI, USA;
b
Center for Psychiatry Research, Department of Clinical
Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Sweden;
c
Department of Sociology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK;
d
Department of Counseling
Psychology, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI, USA
ABSTRACT
There are neurophysiological and phenomenological overlaps between psychedelic and meditative
states, but there is little evidence on how exposure to psychedelics might be associated with
meditation-related variables. We assessed lifetime classic psychedelic use, ego dissolution during
one’s most intense experience using a classic psychedelic, and exposure to meditation in
a representative sample (n = 953) of American adults. Those who reported experience with
meditation were invited to complete a follow-up survey (n = 536, 92.1% response rate) measuring
meditation-related variables. Models controlled for a range of potential confounds. Exposure to
meditation was associated with lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution in covariate-
adjusted models. In addition, among meditators, greater ego dissolution was associated with more
frequent meditation practice. Both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution were
associated with enlightenment as motivation to practice meditation as well as lower likelihood of
overall perceived barriers to meditation practice. Ego dissolution was positively associated with
nding meditation more eective. Neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolution was
associated with greater likelihood of meditation-related adverse eects. Taken together, results
support potential synergy between psychedelics and meditation, but randomized controlled trials
are necessary to establish safety and evaluate potential causal relationships.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 27 May 2021
Revised 1 October 2021
Accepted 18 October 2021
KEYWORDS
Psychedelics; Psilocybin; LSD;
ego dissolution; meditation;
mindfulness
Introduction
There has been a dramatic reemergence of research
into the therapeutic effects of psychedelics over the
past decades. Although experimental studies remain
few in number and limited in sample size, the avail-
able evidence suggests psychedelics may hold thera-
peutic potential, particularly in the context of
psychiatric disorders (Goldberg et al. 2020; Romeo
et al. 2020; Sessa 2018). This research has mainly
focused on classic psychedelics, which commonly
refers to psychoactive substances known to act as
agonists principally at serotonin 2A receptors (Nutt
and Carhart-Harris 2021). Most notably, classic psy-
chedelics include N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT),
the DMT-containing brew ayahuasca, psilocybin,
lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), mescaline, and the
mescaline-containing cactus peyote (Sexton, Nichols,
and Hendricks 2019). The evidence to date suggests
that classic psychedelics have a good safety and toler-
ability profile and can be effective in the treatment of
internalizing disorders (Davis et al. 2021; Luoma
et al. 2020; Nutt, King, and Phillips 2010).
One aspect of the psychedelic experience that
appears to be particularly important for producing
therapeutic effects is a phenomenon known as ego
dissolution (Kettner et al. 2019; Lebedev et al. 2016;
Nour, Evans, and Carhart-Harris 2017; Uthaug et al.
2018), which broadly refers to a loss of subjective
self-identity (Nour et al. 2016). There are intriguing
overlaps in phenomenology and neurophysiology
between psychedelic-induced ego dissolution and
experiences that have been reported in the context
of meditation practice (Millière et al. 2018). And as
with classic psychedelics, alterations in one’s experi-
ence of the self has been proposed as a causal
mechanism underlying potential benefits of medita-
tion practice (Dahl, Lutz, and Davidson 2015).
Despite this overlap, limited research has examined
potential associations between use of classic psyche-
delics and meditation practice.
The available research suggests there may be potential
linkages and perhaps synergy between classic psychede-
lics and meditation practice. In two randomized con-
trolled trials, the use of classic psychedelics was shown to
CONTACT Otto Simonsson simonsson@wisc.edu Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publisher’s website.
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS
https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2021.2022816
© 2022 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-
nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built
upon in any way.
increase both frequency and depth of meditation prac-
tice (Griffiths et al. 2018; Smigielski et al. 2019).
Associations from cross-sectional surveys also suggest
a relationship between psychedelic use and subsequent
interest in Buddhism (Osto 2016). Taken together, the
limited available evidence suggests that classic psyche-
delics may augment interest in and depth of meditation
practice. However, important questions remain at the
intersection of the contemplative and psychedelic
sciences. Based on the possibility that classic psychede-
lics may impact one’s engagement with meditation, it
would be valuable to know whether individuals with
lifetime exposure to classic psychedelics are more likely
to have lifetime exposure to meditation and whether
meditators with and without lifetime classic psychedelic
use show different patterns of engagement with medita-
tion (e.g., reasons for practice, amount of practice, type
of practice, barriers to practice). It would also be worth
examining whether meditators with and without psy-
chedelic experience differ in their report of meditation-
related benefits (e.g., overall effectiveness of meditation,
levels of cognitive capacities linked to meditation) and
harms (i.e., adverse effects).
Using data from a nationally representative online
sample of 953 adults in the United States (US), we
sought to investigate the associations between lifetime
classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution during one’s
most intense experience using a classic psychedelic with
various meditation-related variables including (1) life-
time exposure to meditation; (2) current amount of
meditation practice; (3) current type of meditation prac-
tice; (4) current motivation for meditation practice; (5)
perceived barriers to meditation practice; (6) feeling glad
to have practiced meditation; (7) perceived efficacy of
meditation practice; (8) decentering skills; and (9) med-
itation-related adverse effects.
Methods
Participants and procedure
The data for the present study were collected as part of
a large-scale online survey (see https://osf.io/pzkmn/).
US residents over 18 years of age were recruited on
Prolific Academic (https://app.prolific.co), with the
sample (n = 953) stratified across three demographics
(age, sex and ethnicity) to reflect the demographic dis-
tribution of the US adult population. In the first part of
the study, the participants were asked about demo-
graphic characteristics, lifetime substance use, medita-
tion experience, and attitudes. Participants who failed
the attention check item (“I have been randomly select-
ing responses on this survey”) were screened out
(n = 33). The participants who reported experience
with meditation (n = 582) were invited to complete
the second part of the study (see Supplemental
Material for the study description used during recruit-
ment process), which contained a large battery of ques-
tions about meditation and health. Of those invited,
92.1% (n = 536; 227 males) completed the second part
of the study. As shown in Supplemental Table A1, the
subsample used in this study had a mean age of 44 and
55.8% had a bachelor’s degree or higher. Study proce-
dures were approved by the Institutional Review Board
at the University of Wisconsin – Madison.
Measures
Psychedelic-related variables
We assessed two psychedelic-related variables: lifetime
classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution during one’s
most intense experience using a classic psychedelic.
Lifetime classic psychedelic use. A single item, adapted
from the Alcohol, Smoking, and Substance Involvement
Screening Test (ASSIST; The WHO ASSIST Working
Group 2002), assessed lifetime classic psychedelic use:
There is a group of hallucinogens called classic psyche-
delics, which include ayahuasca, DMT, LSD, mescaline,
peyote, and psilocybin (“magic mushrooms”). In your
life, which of the following classic psychedelics have you
EVER used? (select all that apply).
Response options included: ayahuasca; DMT; LSD,
also called “acid”; mescaline; peyote; psilocybin, also
called “magic mushrooms”; other (please specify);
never used a classic psychedelic.
1
All participants select-
ing other (please specify) also reported use of the options
listed above or wrote the name of one when specifying
other. Responses were coded as 1 when participants
indicated they had used a classic psychedelic (ayahuasca;
DMT; LSD, also called “acid”; mescaline; peyote; psilo-
cybin, also called “magic mushrooms”) and all other
responses were coded as 0.
Ego dissolution. For participants endorsing lifetime use
of classic psychedelics, we assessed ego dissolution dur-
ing one’s most intense experience using a classic psy-
chedelic using the highest loading item from the Ego
Dissolution Inventory (Nour et al. 2016): During your
most intense experience using a classic psychedelic, how
much would you agree with the following statement: “I
experienced a disintegration of my ‘self’ or ego.”
Participants responded on a slider scale from 0 to 100
(0 = No, not more than usually, 100 = Yes, entirely or
completely).
2O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
Meditation-related variables
We assessed several aspects of meditation practice,
including lifetime exposure to meditation; current
amount of meditation practice; current type of medita-
tion practice; current motivation for meditation prac-
tice; perceived barriers to meditation practice; feeling
glad to have practiced meditation; perceived efficacy of
meditation practice; decentering skills; and meditation-
related adverse effects.
Lifetime exposure to meditation. All participants were
assessed on lifetime exposure to meditation using
a single item: “Have you ever tried any of the following
types of meditation, even just once? (select all that
apply)” Response options included mantra meditation,
mindfulness meditation, spiritual meditation, Tibetan
meditation, yogic meditation, meditation as a part of
a movement practice (e.g., yoga), and other meditation
practices (see Supplemental Material for details on
response options).
Current amount of meditation practice. For partici-
pants who reported experience with meditation,
a single item assessed current amount of meditation
practice: “How often do you currently practice medita-
tion?” (1 = Never, 2 = Several times per year,
3 = Monthly, 4 = Weekly, 5 = Daily).
Current type of meditation practice. For participants
who reported experience with meditation, a single item
assessed current type of meditation practice: “Which of
the following is your current primary meditation prac-
tice? (select one)” The participants were presented with
a list of response options (e.g., body scan, pranayama,
centering prayer). The responses were coded as
“Buddhist meditation,” “Hindu meditation,” “Spiritual
meditation,” “Meditation as part of yoga, tai-chi, or qi
gong,” and “Other type of meditation” (see
Supplemental Material for details on response options
and how the responses were coded).
Current motivation for meditation practice. For parti-
cipants who reported experience with meditation,
a single item assessed current motivation for meditation
practice: “Why do you currently practice meditation?
(select all that apply)” The responses were coded as
“Physical health,” “Mental health,” “Part of social, cul-
tural, or religious identity,” “General spiritual,”
“Enlightenment,” and “Other reason.”
Perceived barriers to meditation practice. All partici-
pants were asked to complete the 12-item Determinants
of Meditation Practice Inventory-Revised (DMPI-R;
Hunt et al. 2020). The measure has previously shown
a four-factor structure and convergent validity with
relevant constructs (e.g., experiential avoidance, curios-
ity; Hunt et al. 2020). However, in the current sample,
internal consistency was unacceptably low for the cul-
tural barriers subscale (alpha = .40). The three remain-
ing subscales assessed perceived benefit (e.g., “I don’t
believe meditation can help me”), knowledge (e.g., “I
don’t know much about meditation”), and pragmatic
barriers (e.g., “I don’t have time”). We also calculated
a total score across all items. Higher subscale and total
score values indicate greater barriers to meditation prac-
tice. Internal consistency was adequate for all three sub-
scales and total score (alphas = .72 to .78).
Feeling glad to have practiced meditation. For partici-
pants who reported experience with meditation, a single
item assessed whether they feel glad to have practiced
meditation:
Consider the various experiences you have had through
meditation, including any challenging, difficult, or dis-
tressing experiences. How much do you agree with the
following statement: “I am glad I have practiced
meditation.”
The responses were rated on a 1- (Strongly disagree)
to 6-point (Strongly agree) Likert scale.
Perceived efficacy of meditation practice. For partici-
pants who reported experience with meditation, a single
item assessed efficacy of meditation practice: “In general,
how effective have you found meditation practice?” The
responses were rated on a 1- (Not at all effective) to
6-point (Very effective) Likert scale.
Decentering skills. For participants who reported
experience with meditation, we assessed decentering
skills using the 11-item Experiences Questionnaire –
Decentering subscale (EQ Decentering), which
included items such as “I can separate myself from my
thoughts and feelings” and “I can treat myself kindly.”
The responses were rated on a 1- (Never) to 5-point (All
the time) Likert scale. EQ – Decentering has shown good
reliability and validity in previous research (Fresco et al.
2007; Gecht et al. 2014; Naragon-Gainey et al. 2020;
Soler et al. 2014). A subscale score was calculated by
summing across all items, with higher scores indicating
greater decentering skills. Internal consistency was ade-
quate (alpha = .91).
Meditation-related adverse effects. For participants
who reported experience with meditation, we assessed
meditation-related adverse effects using the 11-item
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS 3
Meditation-Related Adverse Effects Scale
Mindfulness-Based Program (MRAES-MBP; Britton,
Lindahl, and Cooper 2018). This measure is designed
to assess meditation-related adverse effects and asks
participants “Have you ever had any of the following
occur as a result of meditation?” It includes items that
have previously been reported in qualitative interviews
with meditation practitioners (Lindahl et al. 2017) such
as “I felt anxious” and “Feeling disconnected from every-
thing around me.” Responses indicate frequency
(1 = Never, 2 = For a few days to 1 week, 3 = 1 week
to 1 month, 4, 1 month to 1 year, 5 = 1 year or longer).
Given low rates of most response options (< 5%), items
were dichotomized and a total score computed across
the 10 specific adverse effects items. Internal consistency
was adequate (Kuder-Richardson = .89).
Participants were also asked three additional single
items on meditation-related adverse effects: “I person-
ally have had challenging, difficult, or distressing experi-
ences as a result of my meditation practice” (1 = Never,
2 = Rarely, 3 = Occasionally, 4 = Regularly,
5 = Frequently; recoded as 1 = Yes, 0 = Never); “My
meditation-related challenging, difficult, or distressing
experiences impaired my ability to function” (1 = Not at
all, 2 = Somewhat, 3 = Moderately, 4 = Severely; recoded
as 1 = Yes, 0 = Not at all); and “How long did your
impairment last?” (1 = 1 day or less, 2 = For a few days to
1 week, 3 = 1 week to 1 month, 4 = 1 month to 1 year,
5 = 1 year or longer; recoded as 1 = 1+ day, 0 = 1 day or
less).
Covariates
We controlled for a number of demographic and other
covariates that may confound potential associations
between lifetime classic psychedelic use and meditation-
related variables. Given the cross-sectional nature of our
design and the possibility of spurious associations
between classic psychedelics and meditation (e.g., both
being driven by third variables), covariates were
included in all analyses. The covariates broadly mirrored
the covariates of a recent cross-sectional study to the
extent the same or similar variables were included in this
survey (Nour, Evans, and Carhart-Harris 2017).
However, given the relatively modest sample size,
many covariates were dichotomized by collapsing less
common responses into one value.
Demographic covariates included age in years (con-
tinuous), sex (male, other), and educational attainment
(Bachelor’s degree or higher, other). We also controlled
for lifetime use of cocaine (yes, no) and alcohol (yes, no)
with an item adapted from ASSIST (The WHO ASSIST
Working Group 2002).
Statistical analyses
We used linear and logistic regression models to evalu-
ate associations between meditation-related variables
with lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution
during one’s most intense experience using a classic
psychedelic. In these models, lifetime classic psychedelic
use or ego dissolution (z-scored) were entered as the
independent variables along with the covariates
described above while meditation-related variables
were entered as the dependent variables. Bivariate asso-
ciations between study variables were also examined
using the Pearson correlation coefficient, the point biser-
ial correlation coefficient, and the Phi coefficient.
Results
Table 1 presents the associations between psychedelic-
related and meditation-related variables when a range of
covariates are controlled for. Sample descriptive statistics
for all independent and dependent variables are reported in
Supplemental Table A1. Zero-order associations between
study variables are reported Supplemental Table A2.
Across the full sample, lifetime classic psychedelic
use was associated with lifetime exposure to medita-
tion (aOR = 2.04, p = .002), with 28% of those
exposed to meditation reporting lifetime classic psy-
chedelic use compared to 17% of those not exposed
to meditation. Ego dissolution was also associated
with lifetime exposure to meditation (aOR = 1.39,
p = .047).
Among meditators, ego dissolution but not lifetime
classic psychedelic use was associated with greater
amount of current meditation practice (β = .19 and
.03; p = .032 and .554, respectively) as well as current
use of Buddhist meditation practice (aOR = 1.58 and
0.97; p = .019 and .890, respectively). However, neither
lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolution were
associated with current use of Hindu meditation prac-
tice, spiritual meditation practice, meditation as part of
movement practice, or other type of meditation practice.
For current motivation for meditation practice,
both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolu-
tion were associated with enlightenment as motiva-
tion (aOR = 2.38 and 1.64; p = .009 and .043,
respectively). Ego dissolution but not lifetime classic
psychedelic use was associated with mental health as
motivation (aOR = 1.69 and 0.97; p = .008 and .905,
respectively). However, neither lifetime classic psy-
chedelic use nor ego dissolution were associated
with other motivations for practice (physical health;
part of social, cultural, or religious identity; general
spiritual; or other reason).
4O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
Both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolu-
tion were associated with lower likelihood of overall
perceived barriers to meditation practice (i.e., total
score; β = −.16 and −.23; p = < .001 and .001, respec-
tively) as well as with lower likelihood of perceived bar-
riers related to benefit (β = −.13 and −.23; p = .001 and
.001, respectively) and pragmatism (β = −.14 and −.14;
p = .001 and .045, respectively). Lifetime classic psyche-
delic use but not ego dissolution was associated with
lower likelihood of perceived barriers related to knowl-
edge (β = −.12 and −.13; p = .004 and .062, respectively).
Ego dissolution but not lifetime classic psychedelic
use was associated with perceived efficacy of meditation
practice (β = .20 and .06; p = .025 and .259, respectively).
Neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolu-
tion were associated with likelihood of feeling glad to
have practiced meditation (β = .06 and .12; p = .250 and
.160, respectively) or decentering skills (β = −.05 and .11;
p = .369 and .213, respectively).
Contrary to the possibility that classic psychedelic use
is associated with higher likelihood of meditation-
related adverse effects, neither lifetime classic psychede-
lic use nor ego dissolution were associated with total
score of meditation-related adverse effects (β = −.01 and
.05; p = .850 and .560, respectively), lifetime occurrence
of adverse effects (aOR = 1.36 and 1.31, p = .256 and
.179, respectively), functional impairment (aOR = 0.94
and 1.01, p = .876 and .987, respectively), or impaired
function lasting longer than a day (aOR = 1.56 and 1.25,
p = .380 and .577, respectively).
Discussion
Against a backdrop of clinical, phenomenological, and
neurophysiological overlap, the current study sought to
examine associations between classic psychedelic use
and meditation practice in a population-based sample.
Several associations were noted between lifetime classic
psychedelic use and a key element of the acute psyche-
delic experience (i.e., ego dissolution) with variables
related to meditation practice. Lifetime exposure to
meditation was associated with lifetime classic psyche-
delic use and ego dissolution during one’s most intense
experience using a classic psychedelic. Although causal-
ity cannot be inferred in the current design, this finding
suggests that merely being exposed to classic
Table 1. Associations between psychedelic-related and meditation-related variables.
Lifetime Classic Psychedelic Use Ego Dissolution
β aOR (CI 95%) pβ aOR (CI 95%) p
Past and current use of meditation
Lifetime exposure to meditation 2.04 (1.31–3.18) .002 1.39 (1.01–1.91)
a
.047
Current amount of meditation practice .03 .554 .19 .032
Current type of meditation practice
Buddhist meditation 0.97 (0.59–1.58) .890 1.58 (1.08–2.32) .019
Hindu meditation 2.03 (0.48–8.63)
a
.340 0.71 (0.26–1.91)
a
.497
Spiritual meditation 0.44 (0.15–1.28) .132 0.38 (0.15–1.02)
a
.054
Meditation as part of yoga, tai-chi, or qi gong 0.92 (0.41–2.05) .833 0.89 (0.46–1.71) .727
Other type of meditation 4.66 (0.93–23.21)
a
.061 1.00 (0.34–2.96)
a
.995
Current motivation for meditation practice
Physical health 0.95 (0.54–1.68) .859 1.21 (0.80–1.83)
a
.358
Mental health 0.97 (0.57–1.64) .905 1.69 (1.15–2.48)
a
.008
Part of social, cultural, or religious identity 0.99 (0.37–2.70) .991 1.07 (0.49–2.32)
a
.868
General spiritual 1.13 (0.65–1.96) .659 1.48 (0.99–2.23)
a
.059
Enlightenment 2.38 (1.24–4.57) .009 1.64 (1.02–2.64)
a
.043
Other reason 1.63 (0.36–7.24) .524 0.94 (0.37–2.37)
a
.890
Perceived barriers to meditation practice
Benefit −.13 .001 −0.23 .001
Knowledge −.12 .004 −.13 .062
Pragmatic barriers −.14 .001 −.14 .045
Total −.16 <.001 −.23 .001
Perception of meditation practice
Feeling glad to have practiced meditation .06 .250 .12 .160
Perceived efficacy of meditation practice .06 .259 .20 .025
Experiences Questionnaire
Decentering Skills −.05 .369 .11 .213
Adverse Effects
Meditation-Related Adverse Effects −.01 .850 .05 .560
Adverse Effects – Ever 1.36 (0.80–2.31) .256 1.31 (0.88–1.94) .179
Impaired Function – Any 0.94 (0.42–2.08) .876 1.01 (0.54–1.86)
a
.987
Impaired Function – 1+ Day 1.56 (0.58–4.18) .380 1.25 (0.57–2.76) .577
β = standardized coefficients; aOR = adjusted Odds Ratios; β and aOR are adjusted for age, sex, educational attainment, lifetime cocaine use, and lifetime alcohol
use; results in linear regression models are reported with β and results in logistic regression models are reported with aOR. Note: a = lifetime alcohol use
omitted from model due to collinearity reported by Stata. For ease of understanding, Supplemental Table A3 shows full specification of first regression model
(lifetime classic psychedelic use and lifetime meditation exposure).
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS 5
psychedelics as well as greater ego dissolution during
one’s most intense psychedelic experience increases the
likelihood that one later practices meditation (or vice
versa, that exposure to meditation increases the likeli-
hood that one later uses classic psychedelics or experi-
ences greater ego dissolution). In addition, greater ego
dissolution but not lifetime classic psychedelic use was
associated with greater current meditation practice as
well as current use of Buddhist meditation specifically.
A similar divergence emerged in associations between
lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution for
motivations for practice, with ego dissolution but not
lifetime classic psychedelic use showing an association
with mental health motivation. However, both lifetime
classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution were asso-
ciated with enlightenment motivation (aOR = 2.38 and
1.64, respectively).
One of the most robust findings was a consistent
linkage between both of the psychedelic-related vari-
ables and perceived barriers to meditation practice.
Specifically, meditators with lifetime classic psychedelic
use or greater ego dissolution tended to report lower
barriers to meditation (e.g., related to perceived benefit,
pragmatic barriers). This association could certainly be
caused by an unmeasured third variable (e.g., openness
to experience, openness to spiritual experiences specifi-
cally, psychological mindedness), but it is also possible
that resemblance between the acute psychedelic experi-
ence and meditative states decreases confusion about
how to practice meditation and reduces barriers that
inhibit engagement with meditation (Payne, Chambers,
and Liknaitzky 2021).
While neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego
dissolution was associated with feeling glad to have prac-
ticed meditation, ego dissolution showed associations that
were not observed for lifetime classic psychedelic use.
Specifically, meditators reporting greater ego dissolution
during their most intense experience using a classic psy-
chedelic were also more likely to report finding meditation
more effective. It is possible that the acute psychedelic
experience could give people without meditation experi-
ence a reference point with which they can orient their
practice, thereby leading to greater efficacy. Likewise, med-
itation practice may develop the psychological capacity to
support the experience of ego dissolution (Payne,
Chambers, and Liknaitzky 2021). However, contrary to
the possibility that psychedelics increase efficacy, we did
not observe linkages between either of the psychedelic-
related variables with a mindfulness-related cognitive skill
(i.e., decentering).
A final intriguing finding was the lack of association
between either lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego
dissolution with meditation-related adverse effects. This
finding contrasts the view that psychedelics may confer
risk for meditators. An important caveat for interpreting
this finding, however, was the modest amount of medita-
tion experience within the sample. For example, 65% of the
meditators reported between 0 and 100 hours of cumula-
tive meditation experience and only 6% reported having
ever attended a meditation retreat. Given evidence that
more intensive meditation practice is linked with adverse
effects (Lindahl et al. 2017), it would be valuable to replicate
these analyses in a sample with greater lifetime exposure to
retreat. Moreover, it would be important to carefully assess
the time course of both meditation practice and classic
psychedelic use. As both meditation and classic psychede-
lics become more popular, it will be crucial to carefully
determine whether, for whom, and under what circum-
stances (e.g., practice intensity) combining meditation and
classic psychedelics confers increased risk for adverse
effects.
The present findings are novel, but there are several
limitations. First, the sample was stratified across age,
sex and ethnicity to reflect the demographic distribu-
tion of the US population, but previous research has
found Democrats to be overrepresented on online plat-
forms (Arechar and Rand 2021; Paolacci and Chandler
2014). The survey did not ask respondents to report
political identity, which meant the sample’s political
representativeness could not be determined. The per-
centage of participants who reported lifetime classic
psychedelic use in the present study’s full sample
(23%) was higher than prevalence in the US (14%)
found in recent investigations using the National
Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH;
Simonsson, Sexton, and Hendricks 2021). The
NSDUH does not ask specific questions about aya-
huasca or DMT use alone, but prior investigations
have instead derived ayahuasca and DMT use from
a write-in section, which may underestimate the per-
centage of ayahuasca and DMT users in the population.
Nonetheless, the higher prevalence of lifetime classic
psychedelic exposure in the current sample raises some
questions regarding representativeness. Second, as
noted, the meditators in the sample had limited experi-
ence with meditation practice. It is therefore possible
that the associations would be different for experienced
meditators or meditators with experience primarily in
intensive settings. Third, the survey did not ask respon-
dents to describe the set and setting of their use of
classic psychedelics. The set and setting-specific asso-
ciations (e.g., context, frequency, dose, intentions, and
psychological support) between past use of classic psy-
chedelics and meditation-related variables could there-
fore not be evaluated. Fourth, the question about
perceived efficacy of meditation practice may have
6O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
been interpreted differently across respondents (e.g.,
efficacy in improving mental health, physical health),
which makes it difficult to understand in what way
meditation practice might have been effective for the
respondents. Fifth, the relatively modest sample size
meant that a few variables had to be dichotomized
while lifetime alcohol use had to be dropped due to
collinearity in some of the regression models. Sixth,
causality could not be inferred due to the cross-
sectional design. It is, for example, possible that the
emphasis on non-self in Buddhism may have increased
the likelihood of respondents who had a Buddhist med-
itation practice to experience high degrees of ego dis-
solution during the acute psychedelic experience. The
issue of causality is further muddied by not assessing
the time course of classic psychedelic use or meditation
practice.
Notwithstanding these limitations, the current study
highlights several potentially intriguing linkages
between psychedelic-related and meditation-related
variables. Future research should use larger samples
and would ideally use randomized controlled trials to
help clarify the potential synergy between classic psy-
chedelics and meditation practice.
Note
1. Note: PCP was included as a response option but not
counted as a classic psychedelic in analyses. All partici-
pants who reported lifetime PCP use also reported life-
time classic psychedelic use.
Data availability statement
Data are available by request.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the
author(s).
Funding
OS was supported by the Sweden-America Foundation and
Osmond Foundation. SG was supported by a grant
(K23AT010879) from the National Center for
Complementary and Integrative Health.
ORCID
Otto Simonsson http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4197-7566
Simon B. Goldberg http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6888-0126
Ethical approval
All procedures performed involving human participants were
in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
standards. The study was approved by the Internal Review
Board (IRB) at UW-Madison.
Author contributions
SG conceptualized and designed the study, with input from
OS. OS analyzed the data with assistance from SG. OS wrote
the manuscript, with comments from SG. SG supervised the
study.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual partici-
pants included in the studies.
References
Arechar, A. A., and D. G. Rand. 2021. Turking in the time of
COVID. Behavior Research Methods 53 (1):1–5.
doi:10.3758/s13428-021-01588-4.
Britton, W. B., J. R. Lindahl, and D. J. Cooper. 2018.
Meditation-related adverse eects scale, mindfulness-based
program version (MRAES-MBP). Brown University.
Dahl, C. J., A. Lutz, and R. J. Davidson. 2015. Reconstructing
and deconstructing the self: Cognitive mechanisms in med-
itation practice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19 (9):515–23.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001.
Davis, A. K., F. S. Barrett, D. G. May, M. P. Cosimano,
N. D. Sepeda, M. W. Johnson, . . . R. R. Griffiths. 2021.
Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive
disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry
78 (5):481–89. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3285.
Fresco, D. M., M. T. Moore, M. H. van Dulmen, Z. V. Segal,
S. H. Ma, J. D. Teasdale, and J. M. G. Williams. 2007. Initial
psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire:
Validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior
Therapy 38 (3):234–46. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.003.
Gecht, J., R. Kessel, V. Mainz, S. Gauggel, B. Drueke,
A. Scherer, and T. Forkmann. 2014. Measuring decentering
in self-reports: Psychometric properties of the experiences
questionnaire in a German sample. Psychotherapy Research
24 (1):67–79. doi:10.1080/10503307.2013.821635.
Goldberg, S. B., B. Shechet, C. R. Nicholas, C. W. Ng, G. Deole,
Z. Chen, and C. L. Raison. 2020. Post-acute psychological
effects of classical serotonergic psychedelics: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 1–12.
doi:10.1017/S003329172000389X.
Griffiths, R. R., M. W. Johnson, W. A. Richards, B. D. Richards,
R. Jesse, K. A. MacLean, . . . M. A. Klinedinst. 2018.
Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combina-
tion with meditation and other spiritual practices produces
enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and
in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. Journal
of Psychopharmacology 32 (1):49–69. doi:10.1177/
0269881117731279.
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS 7
Hunt, C. A., M. A. Hoffman, J. J. Mohr, and A. L. Williams.
2020. Assessing perceived barriers to meditation: The deter-
minants of meditation practice inventory-revised (DMPI-R).
Mindfulness 1–11. doi:10.1007/s12671-020-01308-7.
Kettner, H., S. Gandy, E. C. Haijen, and R. L. Carhart-Harris.
2019. From egoism to ecoism: Psychedelics increase nature
relatedness in a state-mediated and context-dependent
manner. International Journal of Environmental Research
and Public Health 16 (24):5147. doi:10.3390/
ijerph16245147.
Lebedev, A. V., M. Kaelen, M. Lövdén, J. Nilsson, A. Feilding,
D. J. Nutt, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2016. LSD-induced
entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality
change. Human Brain Mapping 37 (9):3203–13.
doi:10.1002/hbm.23234.
Lindahl, J. R., N. E. Fisher, D. J. Cooper, R. K. Rosen, and
W. B. Britton. 2017. The varieties of contemplative experi-
ence: A mixed-methods study of meditation-related chal-
lenges in Western Buddhists. PloS One 12 (5):e0176239.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0176239.
Luoma, J. B., C. Chwyl, G. J. Bathje, A. K. Davis, and
R. Lancelotta. 2020. A meta-analysis of placebo-controlled
trials of psychedelic-assisted therapy. Journal of
Psychoactive Drugs 52 (4):289–99. doi:10.1080/
02791072.2020.1769878.
Millière, R., R. L. Carhart-Harris, L. Roseman,
F. M. Trautwein, and A. Berkovich-Ohana. 2018.
Psychedelics, meditation, and self-consciousness. Frontiers
in Psychology 9:1475. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01475.
Naragon-Gainey, K., T. P. McMahon, M. Strowger,
R. Lackner, T. H. Seah, M. T. Moore, and D. M. Fresco.
2020. A comparison of decentering across demographic
groups and meditation experience: Support for the mea-
surement invariance of the experiences questionnaire.
Psychological Assessment 32 (2):197. doi:10.1037/
pas0000767.
Nour, M. M., L. Evans, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2017.
Psychedelics, personality and political perspectives.
Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 49 (3):182–91. doi:10.1080/
02791072.2017.1312643.
Nour, M. M., L. Evans, D. Nutt, and R. L. Carhart-Harris.
2016. Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: Validation of the
ego-dissolution inventory (EDI). Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience 10:269. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269.
Nutt, D., and R. Carhart-Harris. 2021. The current status of
psychedelics in psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry 78 (2):121–22.
doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2171.
Nutt, D. J., L. A. King, and L. D. Phillips. 2010. Drug harms in
the Uk: A multicriteria decision analysis. The Lancet
376 (9752):1558–65. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61462-6.
Osto, D. 2016. Altered states: Buddhism and psychedelic spiri-
tuality in America. Columbia University Press.
Paolacci, G., and J. Chandler. 2014. Inside the Turk:
Understanding mechanical turk as a participant pool.
Current Directions in Psychological Science 23 (3):184–88.
doi:10.1177/0963721414531598.
Payne, J. E., R. Chambers, and P. Liknaitzky. 2021. Combining
psychedelic and mindfulness interventions: Synergies to
inform clinical practice. ACS Pharmacology &
Translational Science 4 (2):416–23. doi:10.1021/
acsptsci.1c00034.
Romeo, B., L. Karila, C. Martelli, and A. Benyamina. 2020.
Efficacy of psychedelic treatments on depressive symp-
toms: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychopharmacology
34 (10):1079–85. doi:10.1177/0269881120919957.
Sessa, B. 2018. The 21st century psychedelic renaissance:
Heroic steps forward on the back of an elephant.
Psychopharmacology 235 (2):551–60. doi:10.1007/s00213-
017-4713-7.
Sexton, J. D., C. D. Nichols, and P. S. Hendricks. 2019.
Population survey data informing the therapeutic potential
of classic and novel Phenethylamine, Tryptamine, and
Lysergamide Psychedelics. Frontiers in Psychiatry 10:896.
doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00896.
Simonsson, O., J. D. Sexton, and P. S. Hendricks. 2021.
Associations between lifetime classic psychedelic use and
markers of physical health. Journal of Psychopharmacology
35 (4):447–52. doi:10.1177/0269881121996863.
Smigielski, L., M. Kometer, M. Scheidegger, R. Krähenmann,
T. Huber, and F. X. Vollenweider. 2019. Characterization
and prediction of acute and sustained response to psyche-
delic psilocybin in a mindfulness group retreat. Scientific
Reports 9 (1):1–13. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-50612-3.
Soler, J., A. Franquesa, A. Feliu-Soler, A. Cebolla,
J. García-Campayo, R. Tejedor, . . . M. J. Portella. 2014.
Assessing decentering: Validation, psychometric properties,
and clinical usefulness of the experiences questionnaire in
a Spanish sample. Behavior Therapy 45 (6):863–71.
doi:10.1016/j.beth.2014.05.004.
Uthaug, M. V., K. Van Oorsouw, K. P. C. Kuypers, M. Van
Boxtel, N. J. Broers, N. L. Mason, . . . J. G. Ramaekers. 2018.
Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and
cognitive thinking style and their association with ego
dissolution. Psychopharmacology 235 (10):2979–89.
doi:10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3.
The WHO ASSIST Working Group. 2002. The alcohol, smok-
ing and substance involvement screening test (ASSIST):
Development, reliability and feasibility. Addiction
97 (9):1183–94. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00185.x.
8O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
... While the respective therapeutic evidence for meditation and psychedelics has been established as standalone interventions, recent research has started to point at their similarities (Millière et al., 2018;Scheidegger, 2021), differences (Heuschkel and Kuypers, 2020), and linkages Simonsson and Goldberg, 2023), culminating in pointers toward potential synergies in combining psychedelics and meditation practice (Eleftheriou and Thomas, 2021;Griffiths et al., 2018;Payne et al., 2021;Smigielski et al., 2019a). ...
... While our finding of a short-term increase in state mindfulness associated with a psychedelic substance is consistent with the previous literature on psilocybin-enhanced meditation (Smigielski et al., 2019a), we found a stronger placebo-related increase in state mindfulness than in the previous literature, speaking for a relevant impact of the overall meditation setting. Moreover, while a cross-sectional survey has not found any linkages between lifetime use of classic psychedelics and the mindfulness-related capability of decentering in a representative sample of American adults (Simonsson and Goldberg, 2023), previous open-label, non-placebo-controlled, ayahuasca studies have shown pre-post increases in the mindfulness-related capabilities of decentering, nonjudgment, and non-reactivity 24-h after ingesting ayahuasca (Sampedro et al., 2017;Soler et al., 2016). Interestingly, this increase in mindfulness after ayahuasca was indeed replicated in the present study with DMT-harmine as we found a significant increase in mindfulness, including its two subdimensions of curiosity and decentering, from day 1 to day 2 in the DMT-harmine group. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background In recent years, both meditation and psychedelics have attracted rapidly increasing scientific interest. While the current state of evidence suggests the promising potential of psychedelics, such as psilocybin, to enhance meditative training, it remains equivocal whether these effects are specifically bound to psilocybin or if other classical psychedelics might show synergistic effects with meditation practice. One particularly promising candidate is N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT), an active ingredient of ayahuasca. Aim This study aims to investigate the effect of the psychedelic substance DMT, combined with the monoamine oxidase inhibitor harmine ( DMT-harmine), on meditative states, compared to meditation with a placebo. Method Forty experienced meditators (18 females and 22 males) participated in a double-blind, placebo-controlled study over a 3-day meditation retreat, receiving either placebo or DMT-harmine. Participants’ levels of mindfulness, compassion, insight, and transcendence were assessed before, during, and after the meditation group retreat, using psychometric questionnaires. Results Compared to meditation with a placebo, meditators who received DMT and harmine self-attributed greater levels of mystical-type experiences, non-dual awareness, and emotional breakthrough during the acute substance effects and, when corrected for baseline differences, greater psychological insight 1 day later. Mindfulness and compassion were not significantly different in the DMT-harmine group compared to placebo. At 1-month follow-up, the meditators who received DMT and harmine rated their experience as significantly more personally meaningful, spiritually significant, and well-being-enhancing than the meditators who received placebo. Conclusion Investigating the impact of DMT-harmine on meditators in a naturalistic mindfulness group retreat, this placebo-controlled study highlights the specific effects of psychedelics during meditation. Trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT05780216.
... In addition, for each ERP component, we constructed a model incorporating the fixed effect of the data collection site (Warsaw or Cracow). The recruited groups were not matched in terms of meditation practice, which can have similar effects on emotionality as the use of psychedelics (Millière et al., 2018;Orłowski et al., 2022;Simonsson and Goldberg, 2023;Tang et al., 2016), we utilized weights in our statistical models to mitigate the influence of individuals with a history of extensive meditation practice following the methodological guidelines of the statistical tool employed (Bates et al., 2015). Weights were generated as follows: (1) the number of lifetime meditation hours is transformed using the Yeo-Johnson method (Weisberg, 2001) to decrease dispersion (range 0-2000); (2) values are centered around five to obtain positive values only; (3) the inverse of the values is computed to down weight participants with higher numbers of lifetime mediation hours. ...
... Subjective experiences induced by psychedelics appear to share similarities with those associated with meditation (Millière et al., 2018;Simonsson and Goldberg, 2023). Specifically, both practices are associated with improved emotional regulation (Orłowski et al., 2022;Tang et al., 2016); also, both have therapeutic potential for mood disorders (Marchand, 2012;Pascoe et al., 2021;Vollenweider and Kometer, 2010) and have similar effects on neuronal activity, such as reduction of activity in the default mode network (Gattuso et al., 2023;Jerath et al., 2012;Taylor et al., 2013). ...
Article
Background Previous research has suggested that controlled administration of psychedelic substances can modulate emotional reactivity, enhancing positive and diminishing negative emotions. However, it is unclear whether similar effects are associated with using psychedelics in less-controlled naturalistic environments. Aims This cross-sectional study investigated the neural markers associated with the perception of emotional stimuli in individuals with extensive experience of naturalistic psychedelic use (15 or more lifetime experiences), comparing them to non-users. Methods Electroencephalography (EEG) signals were recorded from two groups: experienced psychedelics users ( N = 56) and non-users ( N = 55). Participants were presented with facial images depicting neutral or emotional expressions (anger, sadness, and happiness). Event-related potential (ERP) components were analyzed as indices of emotional reactivity. Results Psychedelic users were characterized by significantly lower amplitudes of the N200 component in response to fearful faces, in comparison to non-users. In addition, interaction effects between Group and Emotional expression were observed on N170 and N200 amplitudes, indicating group differences in the processing of fearful faces. However, no significant between-group differences emerged in the analysis of later ERP components associated with attention and cognitive processes (P200 and P300). Conclusions The results suggest that naturalistic use of psychedelics may be linked to reduced reactivity to emotionally negative stimuli at the early and automatic processing stages. Our study contributes to a better understanding of the effects related to using psychedelics in naturalistic contexts.
... describes broad domains with existing coverage by established AE assessment standards and presents several novel domains for AEs particularly relevant to psychedelics. Based on AEs described in psychedelics, psychotherapies, and related interventions that may have overlapping AEs (e.g., meditation, Britton et al., 2021;Simonsson and Goldberg, 2023), Supplemental Document 1 provides a list of proposed PATrelated AEs with definitions, organized according to the theorized higher-order categories presented in Table 1. These criteria and their definitions are intended to be distinct from assessments of their severity and attributions of their relatedness to the intervention, which is based on the judgment of the AE assessor (who is ideally an independent arbiter: van Elk and Fried, 2023), taking into account the impact, timing, and potential causal processes for the AE. ...
Article
Objective: Despite considerable research examining the efficacy of psychedelic-assisted therapies (PATs) for treating psychiatric disorders, assessment of adverse events (AEs) in PAT research has lagged. Current AE reporting standards in PAT trials are poorly calibrated to features of PAT that distinguish it from other treatments, leaving many potential AEs unassessed. Methods: A multidisciplinary working group of experts involved in PAT pooled formally and informally documented AEs observed through research experience and published literature. This information was integrated with (a) current standards and practices for AE reporting in pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy trials and (b) published findings documenting post-acute dosing impacts of psychedelics on subjective states, meaning, and psychosocial health variables, to produce a set of AE constructs important to evaluate in PAT as well as recommended methods and time frames for their assessment and monitoring. Correspondence between identified potential AEs and current standards for AE assessment was examined, including the extent of coverage of identified AE constructs by 25 existing measures used in relevant research. Results: Fifty-four potential AE terms warranting systematized assessment in PAT were identified, defined, and categorized. Existing measures demonstrated substantial gaps in their coverage of identified AE constructs. Recommendations were developed for how to assess PAT AEs (including patient, clinician, and informant reports), and when to assess over preparation, dosing session, integration, and follow-up. Application of this framework is demonstrated in a preliminary assessment protocol (available in the supplement). Conclusions: This assessment framework addresses the need to capture post-acute dosing AEs in PAT, accounting for its pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy components, as well as documented impacts of psychedelics on worldviews and spirituality.
... [ Table 1 Here] Based on AEs described in psychedelics, psychotherapies, and in related interventions that may have overlapping AEs (e.g., meditation, Britton et al., 2021;Simonsson and Goldberg, 2023), Table 2 provides a list of proposed PAT-related AEs with definitions, organized according to the theorized higher-order categories presented in Table 1. These criteria and their definitions are intended to be distinct from assessments of their severity and attributions of their relatedness to the intervention, which are based on the judgment of the AE assessor (who is ideally an independent arbiter: 62), taking into account the impact, timing, and potential causal processes for the AE. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Objective: Despite considerable research examining the efficacy of psychedelic-assisted therapies (PATs) for treating psychiatric disorders, assessment of adverse events (AEs) in PAT research has lagged. Current AE reporting standards in PAT trials are poorly calibrated to features of PAT that distinguish it from other treatments, leaving many potential AEs unassessed. Methods: A multidisciplinary working group of experts involved in PAT pooled formally and informally documented AEs observed through research experience and in published literature. This information was integrated with (a) current standards and practices for AE reporting in pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy trials, and (b) published findings documenting post-acute dosing impacts of psychedelics on subjective states, meaning, and psychosocial health variables, to produce a set of AE constructs important to evaluate in PAT as well as recommended methods and time frames for their assessment and monitoring. Correspondence between identified potential AEs and current standards for AE assessment was examined, including the extent of coverage of identified AE constructs by 22 existing measures used in relevant research. Results: 53 potential AE terms warranting systematized assessment in PAT were identified, defined, and categorized. Existing measures demonstrated substantial gaps in their coverage of identified AE constructs. Recommendations were developed for how to assess PAT AEs (including patient, clinician, and informant reports), and when to assess over preparation, dosing session, integration, and follow-up. Conclusions: This assessment framework addresses the need to capture post-acute dosing AEs in PAT, accounting for its pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy components, as well as documented impacts of psychedelics on worldviews and spirituality.
... Frequency of meditation practice predicted meaningfulness of psychedelic experiences, which is consistent with Simonsson et al. and Simonsson and Goldberg [65,66]. Meditative practice is, therefore, a potential variable of interest Table 5. Motivations for use of psychedelics as predictors of personality, well-being and psychopathology variables (significant effects in bold and trends toward significance without bolding). ...
Article
Free E-Print available here: https://www.tandfonline.com/eprint/CYSUD5IQWXNZUVQAEYIC/full?target=10.1080/17512433.2023.2295997 Background: In psychedelic therapy, the importance of set and setting is a fundamental but under-researched assumption. The aim of this study is to correlate variables of set (psychedelic use motivation) and setting (psychedelic use location and type of companion) with psychopathology, wellbeing and personality variables. Research design and methods: A sample of 1022 participants of the Spanish-speaking population was collected through an online survey. A novel instrument, the Psychedelic Use Scale (PUS), was developed to measure substance use variables of LSD, mescaline, psilocybin, DMT, 5-Meo-DMT, ketamine, Salvia divinorum, ibogaine and MDMA. Various personality, well-being and psychopathology instruments were implemented to measure outcome variables. Results: Growth motivations, natural settings and presence of significant others predicted less psychopathology, greater wellbeing and meaningfulness of psychedelic experiences, whereas problematic motivations predicted greater psychopathology, lower wellbeing and did not predict meaningfulness of psychedelic experiences. Conclusions: Based on these results, we suggest experimental hypotheses for future clinical trials and longitudinal studies with potential clinical implications.
Article
Full-text available
This narrative review explores the utilization of psychedelic states in therapeutic contexts, deliberately shifting the focus from psychedelic substances back to the experiential phenomena which they induce, in alignment with the original meaning of the term “mind-manifesting”. This review provides an overview of various psychedelic substances used in modern therapeutic settings and ritualistic indigenous contexts, as well as non-pharmacological methods that can arguably induce psychedelic states, including breathwork, meditation, and sensory deprivation. While the occurrence of mystical experiences in psychedelic states seems to be the strongest predictor of positive outcomes, the literature of this field yields several other psychological processes, such as awe, perspective shifts, insight, emotional breakthrough, acceptance, the re-experiencing of memories, and certain aspects of challenging experiences, that are significantly associated with positive change. Additionally, we discuss in detail mystical experience-related changes in metaphysical as well as self-related beliefs and their respective contributions to observed outcomes. We conclude that a purely medical and neurobiological perspective on psychological health is reductive and should not overshadow the significance of phenomenological experiences in understanding and treating psychological issues that manifest in the subjective realities of human individuals.
Preprint
Full-text available
This narrative review explores the utilization of psychedelic states in therapeutic contexts, deliberately shifting the focus from psychedelic substances back to the experiential phenomena they induce, in alignment with the original meaning of the term "mind-manifesting." The review provides an overview of various psychedelic substances used in modern therapeutic settings and ritualistic indigenous contexts, as well as non-pharmacological methods that can arguably induce psychedelic states, including breathwork, meditation, and sensory deprivation. While the occurrence of mystical experiences in psychedelic states seems to be the strongest predictor of positive outcomes, the literature of the field yields several other psychological processes, such as awe, perspective shifts, insight, emotional breakthrough, acceptance, re-experiencing of memories, and certain aspects of challenging experiences, that are significantly associated with positive change. We additionally discuss in detail mystical experience related changes in metaphysical as well as self-related beliefs and their respective contributions to observed outcomes. We conclude that a purely medical and neurobiological perspective on psychological health is reductive and should not overshadow the significance of phenomenological experiences in understanding and treating psychological issues that manifest in subjective realities of human individuals.
Article
Full-text available
In this study, for the first time, we explored a dataset of functional magnetic resonance images collected during focused attention and open monitoring meditation before and after a five-day psilocybin-assisted meditation retreat using a recently established approach, based on the Mapper algorithm from topological data analysis. After generating subject-specific maps for two groups (psilocybin vs. placebo, 18 subjects/group) of experienced meditators, organizational principles were uncovered using graph topological tools, including the optimal transport (OT) distance, a geometrically rich measure of similarity between brain activity patterns. This revealed characteristics of the topology (i.e. shape) in space (i.e. abstract space of voxels) and time dimension of whole-brain activity patterns during different styles of meditation and psilocybin-induced alterations. Most interestingly, we found that (psilocybin-induced) positive derealization, which fosters insightfulness specifically when accompanied by enhanced open-monitoring meditation, was linked to the OT distance between open-monitoring and resting state. Our findings suggest that enhanced meta-awareness through meditation practice in experienced meditators combined with potential psilocybin-induced positive alterations in perception mediate insightfulness. Together, these findings provide a novel perspective on meditation and psychedelics that may reveal potential novel brain markers for positive synergistic effects between mindfulness practices and psilocybin.
Article
Full-text available
Background Previous research has proposed that there may be potential synergies between psychedelic and meditation interventions, but there are still knowledge gaps that merit further investigation. Methods Using a longitudinal observational research design with samples representative of the US and UK adult population with regard to sex, age, and ethnicity ( N = 9732), we investigated potential associations between self-reported psychedelic use and meditation practice. Results The follow-up survey was completed by 7667 respondents (79% retention rate), with 100 respondents reporting psychedelic use during the 2-month study period (1.3% of follow-up respondents). In covariate-adjusted regression models, psychedelic use during the study period was associated with greater increases in the number of days of mindfulness meditation practice in the past week ( B = 0.40, p = 0.004). Among those who reported psychedelic use during the study period, covariate-adjusted regression models revealed that the subjective experience of insight during respondents' most intense psychedelic experience in that period was also associated with greater increases in the number of days of mindfulness and loving-kindness or compassion meditation practice in the past week ( B = 0.42, p = 0.021; B = 0.38, p = 0.017). Notably, more days of loving-kindness or compassion meditation practice in the past week at baseline was associated with less severe subjective feelings of death or dying during respondents' most intense psychedelic experience in the study period ( B = −0.29, p = 0.037). Conclusions Psychedelic use might lead to greater engagement with meditation practices such as mindfulness meditation, while meditation practices such as loving-kindness or compassion medication might buffer against certain challenging experiences associated with psychedelic use.
Article
Full-text available
Background In recent years, there has been significant research on the mental health effects of classic psychedelic use, but there is very little evidence on how classic psychedelics might influence physical health. Aims The purpose of the present study was to investigate the associations between lifetime classic psychedelic use and markers of physical health. Methods Using data from the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (2015-2018) with 171,766 (unweighted) adults aged 18 or above in the United States, the current study examined the associations between lifetime classic psychedelic use and three markers of physical health (self-reported overall health, body mass index, and heart condition and/or cancer in the past 12 months) while controlling for a range of covariates. Results Respondents who reported having tried a classic psychedelic at least once in their lifetime had significantly higher odds of greater self-reported overall health and significantly lower odds of being overweight or obese versus having a normal weight. The association between lifetime classic psychedelic use and having a heart condition and/or cancer in the past 12 months approached conventional levels of significance, with lower odds of having a heart condition and/or cancer in the past 12 months for respondents who had tried a classic psychedelic at least once. Conclusion The results of the present study suggest that classic psychedelics may be beneficial to physical health. Future research should investigate the causal effects of classic psychedelics on physical health and evaluate possible mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
Importance: Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a substantial public health burden, but current treatments have limited effectiveness and adherence. Recent evidence suggests that 1 or 2 administrations of psilocybin with psychological support produces antidepressant effects in patients with cancer and in those with treatment-resistant depression. Objective: To investigate the effect of psilocybin therapy in patients with MDD. Design, Setting, and Participants: This randomized, waiting list–controlled clinical trial was conducted at the Center for Psychedelic and Consciousness Research at Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center in Baltimore, Maryland. Adults aged 21 to 75 years with an MDD diagnosis, not currently using antidepressant medications, and without histories of psychotic disorder, serious suicide attempt, or hospitalization were eligible to participate. Enrollment occurred between August 2017 and April 2019, and the 4-week primary outcome assessments were completed in July 2019. A total of 27 participants were randomized to an immediate treatment condition group (n = 15) or delayed treatment condition group (waiting list control condition; n = 12). Data analysis was conducted from July 1, 2019, to July 31, 2020, and included participants who completed the intervention (evaluable population). Interventions: Two psilocybin sessions (session 1: 20 mg/70 kg; session 2: 30 mg/70 kg) were given (administered in opaque gelatin capsules with approximately 100 mL of water) in the context of supportive psychotherapy (approximately 11 hours). Participants were randomized to begin treatment immediately or after an 8-week delay. Main Outcomes and Measures: The primary outcome, depression severity was assessed with the GRID-Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (GRID-HAMD) scores at baseline (score of ≥17 required for enrollment) and weeks 5 and 8 after enrollment for the delayed treatment group, which corresponded to weeks 1 and 4 after the intervention for the immediate treatment group. Secondary outcomes included the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology-Self Rated (QIDS-SR). Results: Of the randomized participants, 24 of 27 (89%) completed the intervention and the week 1 and week 4 postsession assessments. This population had a mean (SD) age of 39.8 (12.2) years, was composed of 16 women (67%), and had a mean (SD) baseline GRID-HAMD score of 22.8 (3.9). The mean (SD) GRID-HAMD scores at weeks 1 and 4 (8.0 [7.1] and 8.5 [5.7]) in the immediate treatment group were statistically significantly lower than the scores at the comparable time points of weeks 5 and 8 (23.8 [5.4] and 23.5 [6.0]) in the delayed treatment group. The effect sizes were large at week 5 (Cohen d = 2.2; 95% CI, 1.4-3.0; P < .001) and week 8 (Cohen d = 2.6; 95% CI, 1.7-3.6; P < .001). The QIDS-SR documented a rapid decrease in mean (SD) depression score from baseline to day 1 after session 1 (16.7 [3.5] vs 6.3 [4.4]; Cohen d = 3.0; 95% CI, 1.9-4.0; P < .001), which remained statistically significantly reduced through the week 4 follow-up (6.0 [5.7]; Cohen d = 3.1; 95% CI, 1.9-4.2; P < .001). In the overall sample, 16 participants (67%) at week 1 and 17 (71%) at week 4 had a clinically significant response to the intervention (≥50% reduction in GRID-HAMD score), and 14 participants (58%) at week 1 and 13 participants (54%) at week 4 were in remission (≤7 GRID-HAMD score). Conclusions and Relevance: Findings suggest that psilocybin with therapy is efficacious in treating MDD, thus extending the results of previous studies of this intervention in patients with cancer and depression and of a nonrandomized study in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03181529
Article
Full-text available
After a two-decade hiatus in which research on psychedelics was essentially halted, placebo-controlled clinical trials of psychedelic-assisted therapy for mental health conditions have begun to be published. We identified nine randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trials of psychedelic-assisted therapy published since 1994. Studies examined psilocybin, LSD (lysergic acid diethylamide), ayahuasca (which contains a combination of N,N-dimethyltryptamine and harmala monoamine oxidase inhibitor alkaloids), and MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine). We compared the standardized mean difference between the experimental and placebo control group at the primary endpoint. Results indicated a significant mean between-groups effect size of 1.21 (Hedges g), which is larger than the typical effect size found in trials of psychopharmacological or psychotherapy interventions. For the three studies that maintained a placebo control through a follow-up assessment, effects were generally maintained at follow-up. Overall, analyses support the efficacy of psychedelic-assisted therapy across four mental health conditions-post-traumatic stress disorder, anxiety/depression associated with a life-threatening illness, unipolar depression, and social anxiety among autistic adults. While study quality was high, we identify several areas for improvement regarding the conduct and reporting of trials. Larger trials with more diverse samples are needed to examine possible moderators and mediators of effects, and to establish whether effects are maintained over time.
Article
On March 16, 2020, the US Government introduced strict social distancing protocols for the United States in an effort to stem the spread of the COVID-19 pandemic. This had an immediate major effect on the job market, with millions of Americans forced to find alternative ways to make a living from home. As online labor markets like Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) play a major role in social science research, concerns have been raised that the pandemic may be reducing the diversity of subjects participating in experiments. Here, we investigate this possibility empirically. Specifically, we look at 15,539 responses gathered in 23 studies run on MTurk between February and July 2020, examining the distribution of gender, age, ethnicity, political preference, and analytic cognitive style. We find notable changes on some of the measures following the imposition of nationwide social distancing: participants are more likely to be less reflective (as measured by the Cognitive Reflection Test), and somewhat less likely to be white, Democrats (traditionally over-represented on MTurk), and experienced with MTurk. Most of these differences are explained by an influx of new participants into the MTurk subject pool who are more diverse and representative – but also less attentive – than previous MTurkers.
Article
Psychedelic and mindfulness interventions have been shown to improve mental ill-health and wellbeing, with a range of clinical processes and effects in common. However, each appear to contain specific challenges in the context of mental health treatment. In this Perspective, we focus on a set of distinct affordances, “useful differences”, within psychedelic and mindfulness interventions that might address common challenges within the other intervention. Accordingly, we propose a set of applied synergies, indicating specific ways in which these two promising interventions might be combined for greater benefit. Metaphorically, on the journey toward mental health and wellbeing, we propose that psychedelic treatments may serve the role of Compass (initiating, motivating, and steering the course of mindfulness practice), with mindfulness interventions serving the role of Vehicle (integrating, deepening, generalizing, and maintaining the novel perspectives and motivation instigated by psychedelic experience). We outline a set of testable hypotheses and future research associated with the synergistic action of psychedelic and mindfulness interventions toward improved clinical outcomes.
Article
Background: Scientific interest in the therapeutic effects of classical psychedelics has increased in the past two decades. The psychological effects of these substances outside the period of acute intoxication have not been fully characterized. This study aimed to: (1) quantify the effects of psilocybin, ayahuasca, and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) on psychological outcomes in the post-acute period; (2) test moderators of these effects; and (3) evaluate adverse effects and risk of bias. Methods: We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies (single-group pre-post or randomized controlled trials) that involved administration of psilocybin, ayahuasca, or LSD to clinical or non-clinical samples and assessed psychological outcomes ⩾24 h post-administration. Effects were summarized by study design, timepoint, and outcome domain. Results: A total of 34 studies (24 unique samples, n = 549, mean longest follow-up = 55.34 weeks) were included. Classical psychedelics showed significant within-group pre-post and between-group placebo-controlled effects on a range of outcomes including targeted symptoms within psychiatric samples, negative and positive affect-related measures, social outcomes, and existential/spiritual outcomes, with large between-group effect in these domains (Hedges' gs = 0.84 to 1.08). Moderator tests suggest some effects may be larger in clinical samples. Evidence of effects on big five personality traits and mindfulness was weak. There was no evidence of post-acute adverse effects. Conclusions: High risk of bias in several domains, heterogeneity across studies, and indications of publication bias for some models highlight the need for careful, large-scale, placebo-controlled randomized trials.
Article
In the 1950s, the Swiss pharmaceutical company Sandoz, which employed the chemist Albert Hofmann, who discovered lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and the similar serotonergic psychedelic psilocybin, made these drugs available to the psychiatric research community as the products Delysid and Indocybin, respectively. By the 1960s, these drugs had caused a revolution in brain science and psychiatry because of their widespread use by researchers and clinicians in many Western countries, especially the US. Before LSD was banned, the US National Institutes of Health funded more than 130 studies exploring its clinical utility, with positive results in a range of disorders but particularly anxiety, depression, and alcoholism. However, the displacement of LSD into recreational use and eventual association with the anti-Vietnam war movement led to all psychedelics being banned in the US. This ban became ratified globally under the 1971 UN Convention on narcotics. Since then, research funding, drug production, and the study of psychedelics as clinical agents has been virtually stopped. Until very recently, no companies would manufacture medical-grade psychedelics, which made getting regulatory approval for clinical research—especially clinical trials—very difficult and in some countries (eg, Germany) impossible.
Article
Background Psychedelic drugs have shown an efficacy in some psychiatric disorders and have an original mechanism of action with a 5-HT 2A agonism. Aim The aim of this meta-analysis was to assess by a quantitative analysis the putative efficacy of psychedelic drugs on depressive symptoms and to investigate the kinetic of this efficacy. Methods We searched the MEDLINE and PsycINFO databases through April 2019, without limits on year of publication. Means and standard deviations were extracted to calculate standardized mean differences (SMD). Scores of depressive symptoms were compared with baseline scores at days 7, 14, and 21; weeks 4–5 and 6–8; and months 3 and 6. Results Eight studies were included in this meta-analysis. A significant decrease of depressive symptoms was found from day 1 ( n = 5 studies; SMD = ‒1.4, 95% confidence interval (CI): ‒2.33 to ‒0.48, p = 0.003) to 6 months ( n = 4 studies; SMD = ‒1.07, 95% CI: ‒1.44 to ‒0.7, p < 0.001) after psychedelic sessions. No serious adverse effect was reported in all included studies. A transient increase of the heart rate, blood systolic, and diastolic pressure were found after psychedelics compared with placebo. Conclusion This meta-analysis shows that psychedelic treatments were safe and could contribute to a rapid improvement of depressive symptoms.