ArticlePDF Available

Linkages between Psychedelics and Meditation in a Population-Based Sample in the United States

Authors:

Abstract

There are neurophysiological and phenomenological overlaps between psychedelic and meditative states, but there is little evidence on how exposure to psychedelics might be associated with meditation-related variables. We assessed lifetime classic psychedelic use, ego dissolution during one’s most intense experience using a classic psychedelic, and exposure to meditation in a representative sample (n = 953) of American adults. Those who reported experience with meditation were invited to complete a follow-up survey (n = 536, 92.1% response rate) measuring meditation-related variables. Models controlled for a range of potential confounds. Exposure to meditation was associated with lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution in covariate-adjusted models. In addition, among meditators, greater ego dissolution was associated with more frequent meditation practice. Both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution were associated with enlightenment as motivation to practice meditation as well as lower likelihood of overall perceived barriers to meditation practice. Ego dissolution was positively associated with finding meditation more effective. Neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolution was associated with greater likelihood of meditation-related adverse effects. Taken together, results support potential synergy between psychedelics and meditation, but randomized controlled trials are necessary to establish safety and evaluate potential causal relationships.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ujpd20
Journal of Psychoactive Drugs
ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ujpd20
Linkages between Psychedelics and Meditation in
a Population-Based Sample in the United States
Otto Simonsson & Simon B. Goldberg
To cite this article: Otto Simonsson & Simon B. Goldberg (2022): Linkages between Psychedelics
and Meditation in a Population-Based Sample in the United States, Journal of Psychoactive Drugs,
DOI: 10.1080/02791072.2021.2022816
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2021.2022816
© 2022 The Author(s). Published with
license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
View supplementary material
Published online: 09 Jan 2022.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Linkages between Psychedelics and Meditation in a Population-Based Sample in
the United States
Otto Simonsson
a,b,c
and Simon B. Goldberg
a,d
a
Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI, USA;
b
Center for Psychiatry Research, Department of Clinical
Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Sweden;
c
Department of Sociology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK;
d
Department of Counseling
Psychology, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI, USA
ABSTRACT
There are neurophysiological and phenomenological overlaps between psychedelic and meditative
states, but there is little evidence on how exposure to psychedelics might be associated with
meditation-related variables. We assessed lifetime classic psychedelic use, ego dissolution during
one’s most intense experience using a classic psychedelic, and exposure to meditation in
a representative sample (n = 953) of American adults. Those who reported experience with
meditation were invited to complete a follow-up survey (n = 536, 92.1% response rate) measuring
meditation-related variables. Models controlled for a range of potential confounds. Exposure to
meditation was associated with lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution in covariate-
adjusted models. In addition, among meditators, greater ego dissolution was associated with more
frequent meditation practice. Both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution were
associated with enlightenment as motivation to practice meditation as well as lower likelihood of
overall perceived barriers to meditation practice. Ego dissolution was positively associated with
nding meditation more eective. Neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolution was
associated with greater likelihood of meditation-related adverse eects. Taken together, results
support potential synergy between psychedelics and meditation, but randomized controlled trials
are necessary to establish safety and evaluate potential causal relationships.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 27 May 2021
Revised 1 October 2021
Accepted 18 October 2021
KEYWORDS
Psychedelics; Psilocybin; LSD;
ego dissolution; meditation;
mindfulness
Introduction
There has been a dramatic reemergence of research
into the therapeutic effects of psychedelics over the
past decades. Although experimental studies remain
few in number and limited in sample size, the avail-
able evidence suggests psychedelics may hold thera-
peutic potential, particularly in the context of
psychiatric disorders (Goldberg et al. 2020; Romeo
et al. 2020; Sessa 2018). This research has mainly
focused on classic psychedelics, which commonly
refers to psychoactive substances known to act as
agonists principally at serotonin 2A receptors (Nutt
and Carhart-Harris 2021). Most notably, classic psy-
chedelics include N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT),
the DMT-containing brew ayahuasca, psilocybin,
lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), mescaline, and the
mescaline-containing cactus peyote (Sexton, Nichols,
and Hendricks 2019). The evidence to date suggests
that classic psychedelics have a good safety and toler-
ability profile and can be effective in the treatment of
internalizing disorders (Davis et al. 2021; Luoma
et al. 2020; Nutt, King, and Phillips 2010).
One aspect of the psychedelic experience that
appears to be particularly important for producing
therapeutic effects is a phenomenon known as ego
dissolution (Kettner et al. 2019; Lebedev et al. 2016;
Nour, Evans, and Carhart-Harris 2017; Uthaug et al.
2018), which broadly refers to a loss of subjective
self-identity (Nour et al. 2016). There are intriguing
overlaps in phenomenology and neurophysiology
between psychedelic-induced ego dissolution and
experiences that have been reported in the context
of meditation practice (Millière et al. 2018). And as
with classic psychedelics, alterations in one’s experi-
ence of the self has been proposed as a causal
mechanism underlying potential benefits of medita-
tion practice (Dahl, Lutz, and Davidson 2015).
Despite this overlap, limited research has examined
potential associations between use of classic psyche-
delics and meditation practice.
The available research suggests there may be potential
linkages and perhaps synergy between classic psychede-
lics and meditation practice. In two randomized con-
trolled trials, the use of classic psychedelics was shown to
CONTACT Otto Simonsson simonsson@wisc.edu Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publisher’s website.
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS
https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2021.2022816
© 2022 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-
nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built
upon in any way.
increase both frequency and depth of meditation prac-
tice (Griffiths et al. 2018; Smigielski et al. 2019).
Associations from cross-sectional surveys also suggest
a relationship between psychedelic use and subsequent
interest in Buddhism (Osto 2016). Taken together, the
limited available evidence suggests that classic psyche-
delics may augment interest in and depth of meditation
practice. However, important questions remain at the
intersection of the contemplative and psychedelic
sciences. Based on the possibility that classic psychede-
lics may impact one’s engagement with meditation, it
would be valuable to know whether individuals with
lifetime exposure to classic psychedelics are more likely
to have lifetime exposure to meditation and whether
meditators with and without lifetime classic psychedelic
use show different patterns of engagement with medita-
tion (e.g., reasons for practice, amount of practice, type
of practice, barriers to practice). It would also be worth
examining whether meditators with and without psy-
chedelic experience differ in their report of meditation-
related benefits (e.g., overall effectiveness of meditation,
levels of cognitive capacities linked to meditation) and
harms (i.e., adverse effects).
Using data from a nationally representative online
sample of 953 adults in the United States (US), we
sought to investigate the associations between lifetime
classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution during one’s
most intense experience using a classic psychedelic with
various meditation-related variables including (1) life-
time exposure to meditation; (2) current amount of
meditation practice; (3) current type of meditation prac-
tice; (4) current motivation for meditation practice; (5)
perceived barriers to meditation practice; (6) feeling glad
to have practiced meditation; (7) perceived efficacy of
meditation practice; (8) decentering skills; and (9) med-
itation-related adverse effects.
Methods
Participants and procedure
The data for the present study were collected as part of
a large-scale online survey (see https://osf.io/pzkmn/).
US residents over 18 years of age were recruited on
Prolific Academic (https://app.prolific.co), with the
sample (n = 953) stratified across three demographics
(age, sex and ethnicity) to reflect the demographic dis-
tribution of the US adult population. In the first part of
the study, the participants were asked about demo-
graphic characteristics, lifetime substance use, medita-
tion experience, and attitudes. Participants who failed
the attention check item (“I have been randomly select-
ing responses on this survey”) were screened out
(n = 33). The participants who reported experience
with meditation (n = 582) were invited to complete
the second part of the study (see Supplemental
Material for the study description used during recruit-
ment process), which contained a large battery of ques-
tions about meditation and health. Of those invited,
92.1% (n = 536; 227 males) completed the second part
of the study. As shown in Supplemental Table A1, the
subsample used in this study had a mean age of 44 and
55.8% had a bachelor’s degree or higher. Study proce-
dures were approved by the Institutional Review Board
at the University of Wisconsin – Madison.
Measures
Psychedelic-related variables
We assessed two psychedelic-related variables: lifetime
classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution during one’s
most intense experience using a classic psychedelic.
Lifetime classic psychedelic use. A single item, adapted
from the Alcohol, Smoking, and Substance Involvement
Screening Test (ASSIST; The WHO ASSIST Working
Group 2002), assessed lifetime classic psychedelic use:
There is a group of hallucinogens called classic psyche-
delics, which include ayahuasca, DMT, LSD, mescaline,
peyote, and psilocybin (“magic mushrooms”). In your
life, which of the following classic psychedelics have you
EVER used? (select all that apply).
Response options included: ayahuasca; DMT; LSD,
also called “acid”; mescaline; peyote; psilocybin, also
called “magic mushrooms”; other (please specify);
never used a classic psychedelic.
1
All participants select-
ing other (please specify) also reported use of the options
listed above or wrote the name of one when specifying
other. Responses were coded as 1 when participants
indicated they had used a classic psychedelic (ayahuasca;
DMT; LSD, also called “acid”; mescaline; peyote; psilo-
cybin, also called “magic mushrooms”) and all other
responses were coded as 0.
Ego dissolution. For participants endorsing lifetime use
of classic psychedelics, we assessed ego dissolution dur-
ing one’s most intense experience using a classic psy-
chedelic using the highest loading item from the Ego
Dissolution Inventory (Nour et al. 2016): During your
most intense experience using a classic psychedelic, how
much would you agree with the following statement: “I
experienced a disintegration of my ‘self’ or ego.”
Participants responded on a slider scale from 0 to 100
(0 = No, not more than usually, 100 = Yes, entirely or
completely).
2O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
Meditation-related variables
We assessed several aspects of meditation practice,
including lifetime exposure to meditation; current
amount of meditation practice; current type of medita-
tion practice; current motivation for meditation prac-
tice; perceived barriers to meditation practice; feeling
glad to have practiced meditation; perceived efficacy of
meditation practice; decentering skills; and meditation-
related adverse effects.
Lifetime exposure to meditation. All participants were
assessed on lifetime exposure to meditation using
a single item: “Have you ever tried any of the following
types of meditation, even just once? (select all that
apply)” Response options included mantra meditation,
mindfulness meditation, spiritual meditation, Tibetan
meditation, yogic meditation, meditation as a part of
a movement practice (e.g., yoga), and other meditation
practices (see Supplemental Material for details on
response options).
Current amount of meditation practice. For partici-
pants who reported experience with meditation,
a single item assessed current amount of meditation
practice: “How often do you currently practice medita-
tion?” (1 = Never, 2 = Several times per year,
3 = Monthly, 4 = Weekly, 5 = Daily).
Current type of meditation practice. For participants
who reported experience with meditation, a single item
assessed current type of meditation practice: “Which of
the following is your current primary meditation prac-
tice? (select one)” The participants were presented with
a list of response options (e.g., body scan, pranayama,
centering prayer). The responses were coded as
“Buddhist meditation,” “Hindu meditation,” “Spiritual
meditation,” “Meditation as part of yoga, tai-chi, or qi
gong,” and “Other type of meditation” (see
Supplemental Material for details on response options
and how the responses were coded).
Current motivation for meditation practice. For parti-
cipants who reported experience with meditation,
a single item assessed current motivation for meditation
practice: “Why do you currently practice meditation?
(select all that apply)” The responses were coded as
“Physical health,” “Mental health,” “Part of social, cul-
tural, or religious identity,” “General spiritual,”
“Enlightenment,” and “Other reason.”
Perceived barriers to meditation practice. All partici-
pants were asked to complete the 12-item Determinants
of Meditation Practice Inventory-Revised (DMPI-R;
Hunt et al. 2020). The measure has previously shown
a four-factor structure and convergent validity with
relevant constructs (e.g., experiential avoidance, curios-
ity; Hunt et al. 2020). However, in the current sample,
internal consistency was unacceptably low for the cul-
tural barriers subscale (alpha = .40). The three remain-
ing subscales assessed perceived benefit (e.g., “I don’t
believe meditation can help me”), knowledge (e.g., “I
don’t know much about meditation”), and pragmatic
barriers (e.g., “I don’t have time”). We also calculated
a total score across all items. Higher subscale and total
score values indicate greater barriers to meditation prac-
tice. Internal consistency was adequate for all three sub-
scales and total score (alphas = .72 to .78).
Feeling glad to have practiced meditation. For partici-
pants who reported experience with meditation, a single
item assessed whether they feel glad to have practiced
meditation:
Consider the various experiences you have had through
meditation, including any challenging, difficult, or dis-
tressing experiences. How much do you agree with the
following statement: “I am glad I have practiced
meditation.”
The responses were rated on a 1- (Strongly disagree)
to 6-point (Strongly agree) Likert scale.
Perceived efficacy of meditation practice. For partici-
pants who reported experience with meditation, a single
item assessed efficacy of meditation practice: “In general,
how effective have you found meditation practice?” The
responses were rated on a 1- (Not at all effective) to
6-point (Very effective) Likert scale.
Decentering skills. For participants who reported
experience with meditation, we assessed decentering
skills using the 11-item Experiences Questionnaire –
Decentering subscale (EQ Decentering), which
included items such as “I can separate myself from my
thoughts and feelings” and “I can treat myself kindly.”
The responses were rated on a 1- (Never) to 5-point (All
the time) Likert scale. EQ – Decentering has shown good
reliability and validity in previous research (Fresco et al.
2007; Gecht et al. 2014; Naragon-Gainey et al. 2020;
Soler et al. 2014). A subscale score was calculated by
summing across all items, with higher scores indicating
greater decentering skills. Internal consistency was ade-
quate (alpha = .91).
Meditation-related adverse effects. For participants
who reported experience with meditation, we assessed
meditation-related adverse effects using the 11-item
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS 3
Meditation-Related Adverse Effects Scale
Mindfulness-Based Program (MRAES-MBP; Britton,
Lindahl, and Cooper 2018). This measure is designed
to assess meditation-related adverse effects and asks
participants “Have you ever had any of the following
occur as a result of meditation?” It includes items that
have previously been reported in qualitative interviews
with meditation practitioners (Lindahl et al. 2017) such
as “I felt anxious” and “Feeling disconnected from every-
thing around me.” Responses indicate frequency
(1 = Never, 2 = For a few days to 1 week, 3 = 1 week
to 1 month, 4, 1 month to 1 year, 5 = 1 year or longer).
Given low rates of most response options (< 5%), items
were dichotomized and a total score computed across
the 10 specific adverse effects items. Internal consistency
was adequate (Kuder-Richardson = .89).
Participants were also asked three additional single
items on meditation-related adverse effects: “I person-
ally have had challenging, difficult, or distressing experi-
ences as a result of my meditation practice” (1 = Never,
2 = Rarely, 3 = Occasionally, 4 = Regularly,
5 = Frequently; recoded as 1 = Yes, 0 = Never); “My
meditation-related challenging, difficult, or distressing
experiences impaired my ability to function” (1 = Not at
all, 2 = Somewhat, 3 = Moderately, 4 = Severely; recoded
as 1 = Yes, 0 = Not at all); and “How long did your
impairment last?” (1 = 1 day or less, 2 = For a few days to
1 week, 3 = 1 week to 1 month, 4 = 1 month to 1 year,
5 = 1 year or longer; recoded as 1 = 1+ day, 0 = 1 day or
less).
Covariates
We controlled for a number of demographic and other
covariates that may confound potential associations
between lifetime classic psychedelic use and meditation-
related variables. Given the cross-sectional nature of our
design and the possibility of spurious associations
between classic psychedelics and meditation (e.g., both
being driven by third variables), covariates were
included in all analyses. The covariates broadly mirrored
the covariates of a recent cross-sectional study to the
extent the same or similar variables were included in this
survey (Nour, Evans, and Carhart-Harris 2017).
However, given the relatively modest sample size,
many covariates were dichotomized by collapsing less
common responses into one value.
Demographic covariates included age in years (con-
tinuous), sex (male, other), and educational attainment
(Bachelor’s degree or higher, other). We also controlled
for lifetime use of cocaine (yes, no) and alcohol (yes, no)
with an item adapted from ASSIST (The WHO ASSIST
Working Group 2002).
Statistical analyses
We used linear and logistic regression models to evalu-
ate associations between meditation-related variables
with lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution
during one’s most intense experience using a classic
psychedelic. In these models, lifetime classic psychedelic
use or ego dissolution (z-scored) were entered as the
independent variables along with the covariates
described above while meditation-related variables
were entered as the dependent variables. Bivariate asso-
ciations between study variables were also examined
using the Pearson correlation coefficient, the point biser-
ial correlation coefficient, and the Phi coefficient.
Results
Table 1 presents the associations between psychedelic-
related and meditation-related variables when a range of
covariates are controlled for. Sample descriptive statistics
for all independent and dependent variables are reported in
Supplemental Table A1. Zero-order associations between
study variables are reported Supplemental Table A2.
Across the full sample, lifetime classic psychedelic
use was associated with lifetime exposure to medita-
tion (aOR = 2.04, p = .002), with 28% of those
exposed to meditation reporting lifetime classic psy-
chedelic use compared to 17% of those not exposed
to meditation. Ego dissolution was also associated
with lifetime exposure to meditation (aOR = 1.39,
p = .047).
Among meditators, ego dissolution but not lifetime
classic psychedelic use was associated with greater
amount of current meditation practice (β = .19 and
.03; p = .032 and .554, respectively) as well as current
use of Buddhist meditation practice (aOR = 1.58 and
0.97; p = .019 and .890, respectively). However, neither
lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolution were
associated with current use of Hindu meditation prac-
tice, spiritual meditation practice, meditation as part of
movement practice, or other type of meditation practice.
For current motivation for meditation practice,
both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolu-
tion were associated with enlightenment as motiva-
tion (aOR = 2.38 and 1.64; p = .009 and .043,
respectively). Ego dissolution but not lifetime classic
psychedelic use was associated with mental health as
motivation (aOR = 1.69 and 0.97; p = .008 and .905,
respectively). However, neither lifetime classic psy-
chedelic use nor ego dissolution were associated
with other motivations for practice (physical health;
part of social, cultural, or religious identity; general
spiritual; or other reason).
4O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
Both lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolu-
tion were associated with lower likelihood of overall
perceived barriers to meditation practice (i.e., total
score; β = −.16 and −.23; p = < .001 and .001, respec-
tively) as well as with lower likelihood of perceived bar-
riers related to benefit (β = −.13 and −.23; p = .001 and
.001, respectively) and pragmatism (β = −.14 and −.14;
p = .001 and .045, respectively). Lifetime classic psyche-
delic use but not ego dissolution was associated with
lower likelihood of perceived barriers related to knowl-
edge (β = −.12 and −.13; p = .004 and .062, respectively).
Ego dissolution but not lifetime classic psychedelic
use was associated with perceived efficacy of meditation
practice (β = .20 and .06; p = .025 and .259, respectively).
Neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego dissolu-
tion were associated with likelihood of feeling glad to
have practiced meditation (β = .06 and .12; p = .250 and
.160, respectively) or decentering skills (β = −.05 and .11;
p = .369 and .213, respectively).
Contrary to the possibility that classic psychedelic use
is associated with higher likelihood of meditation-
related adverse effects, neither lifetime classic psychede-
lic use nor ego dissolution were associated with total
score of meditation-related adverse effects (β = −.01 and
.05; p = .850 and .560, respectively), lifetime occurrence
of adverse effects (aOR = 1.36 and 1.31, p = .256 and
.179, respectively), functional impairment (aOR = 0.94
and 1.01, p = .876 and .987, respectively), or impaired
function lasting longer than a day (aOR = 1.56 and 1.25,
p = .380 and .577, respectively).
Discussion
Against a backdrop of clinical, phenomenological, and
neurophysiological overlap, the current study sought to
examine associations between classic psychedelic use
and meditation practice in a population-based sample.
Several associations were noted between lifetime classic
psychedelic use and a key element of the acute psyche-
delic experience (i.e., ego dissolution) with variables
related to meditation practice. Lifetime exposure to
meditation was associated with lifetime classic psyche-
delic use and ego dissolution during one’s most intense
experience using a classic psychedelic. Although causal-
ity cannot be inferred in the current design, this finding
suggests that merely being exposed to classic
Table 1. Associations between psychedelic-related and meditation-related variables.
Lifetime Classic Psychedelic Use Ego Dissolution
β aOR (CI 95%) pβ aOR (CI 95%) p
Past and current use of meditation
Lifetime exposure to meditation 2.04 (1.31–3.18) .002 1.39 (1.01–1.91)
a
.047
Current amount of meditation practice .03 .554 .19 .032
Current type of meditation practice
Buddhist meditation 0.97 (0.59–1.58) .890 1.58 (1.08–2.32) .019
Hindu meditation 2.03 (0.48–8.63)
a
.340 0.71 (0.26–1.91)
a
.497
Spiritual meditation 0.44 (0.15–1.28) .132 0.38 (0.15–1.02)
a
.054
Meditation as part of yoga, tai-chi, or qi gong 0.92 (0.41–2.05) .833 0.89 (0.46–1.71) .727
Other type of meditation 4.66 (0.93–23.21)
a
.061 1.00 (0.34–2.96)
a
.995
Current motivation for meditation practice
Physical health 0.95 (0.54–1.68) .859 1.21 (0.80–1.83)
a
.358
Mental health 0.97 (0.57–1.64) .905 1.69 (1.15–2.48)
a
.008
Part of social, cultural, or religious identity 0.99 (0.37–2.70) .991 1.07 (0.49–2.32)
a
.868
General spiritual 1.13 (0.65–1.96) .659 1.48 (0.99–2.23)
a
.059
Enlightenment 2.38 (1.24–4.57) .009 1.64 (1.02–2.64)
a
.043
Other reason 1.63 (0.36–7.24) .524 0.94 (0.37–2.37)
a
.890
Perceived barriers to meditation practice
Benefit −.13 .001 −0.23 .001
Knowledge −.12 .004 −.13 .062
Pragmatic barriers −.14 .001 −.14 .045
Total −.16 <.001 −.23 .001
Perception of meditation practice
Feeling glad to have practiced meditation .06 .250 .12 .160
Perceived efficacy of meditation practice .06 .259 .20 .025
Experiences Questionnaire
Decentering Skills −.05 .369 .11 .213
Adverse Effects
Meditation-Related Adverse Effects −.01 .850 .05 .560
Adverse Effects – Ever 1.36 (0.80–2.31) .256 1.31 (0.88–1.94) .179
Impaired Function – Any 0.94 (0.42–2.08) .876 1.01 (0.54–1.86)
a
.987
Impaired Function – 1+ Day 1.56 (0.58–4.18) .380 1.25 (0.57–2.76) .577
β = standardized coefficients; aOR = adjusted Odds Ratios; β and aOR are adjusted for age, sex, educational attainment, lifetime cocaine use, and lifetime alcohol
use; results in linear regression models are reported with β and results in logistic regression models are reported with aOR. Note: a = lifetime alcohol use
omitted from model due to collinearity reported by Stata. For ease of understanding, Supplemental Table A3 shows full specification of first regression model
(lifetime classic psychedelic use and lifetime meditation exposure).
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS 5
psychedelics as well as greater ego dissolution during
one’s most intense psychedelic experience increases the
likelihood that one later practices meditation (or vice
versa, that exposure to meditation increases the likeli-
hood that one later uses classic psychedelics or experi-
ences greater ego dissolution). In addition, greater ego
dissolution but not lifetime classic psychedelic use was
associated with greater current meditation practice as
well as current use of Buddhist meditation specifically.
A similar divergence emerged in associations between
lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution for
motivations for practice, with ego dissolution but not
lifetime classic psychedelic use showing an association
with mental health motivation. However, both lifetime
classic psychedelic use and ego dissolution were asso-
ciated with enlightenment motivation (aOR = 2.38 and
1.64, respectively).
One of the most robust findings was a consistent
linkage between both of the psychedelic-related vari-
ables and perceived barriers to meditation practice.
Specifically, meditators with lifetime classic psychedelic
use or greater ego dissolution tended to report lower
barriers to meditation (e.g., related to perceived benefit,
pragmatic barriers). This association could certainly be
caused by an unmeasured third variable (e.g., openness
to experience, openness to spiritual experiences specifi-
cally, psychological mindedness), but it is also possible
that resemblance between the acute psychedelic experi-
ence and meditative states decreases confusion about
how to practice meditation and reduces barriers that
inhibit engagement with meditation (Payne, Chambers,
and Liknaitzky 2021).
While neither lifetime classic psychedelic use nor ego
dissolution was associated with feeling glad to have prac-
ticed meditation, ego dissolution showed associations that
were not observed for lifetime classic psychedelic use.
Specifically, meditators reporting greater ego dissolution
during their most intense experience using a classic psy-
chedelic were also more likely to report finding meditation
more effective. It is possible that the acute psychedelic
experience could give people without meditation experi-
ence a reference point with which they can orient their
practice, thereby leading to greater efficacy. Likewise, med-
itation practice may develop the psychological capacity to
support the experience of ego dissolution (Payne,
Chambers, and Liknaitzky 2021). However, contrary to
the possibility that psychedelics increase efficacy, we did
not observe linkages between either of the psychedelic-
related variables with a mindfulness-related cognitive skill
(i.e., decentering).
A final intriguing finding was the lack of association
between either lifetime classic psychedelic use and ego
dissolution with meditation-related adverse effects. This
finding contrasts the view that psychedelics may confer
risk for meditators. An important caveat for interpreting
this finding, however, was the modest amount of medita-
tion experience within the sample. For example, 65% of the
meditators reported between 0 and 100 hours of cumula-
tive meditation experience and only 6% reported having
ever attended a meditation retreat. Given evidence that
more intensive meditation practice is linked with adverse
effects (Lindahl et al. 2017), it would be valuable to replicate
these analyses in a sample with greater lifetime exposure to
retreat. Moreover, it would be important to carefully assess
the time course of both meditation practice and classic
psychedelic use. As both meditation and classic psychede-
lics become more popular, it will be crucial to carefully
determine whether, for whom, and under what circum-
stances (e.g., practice intensity) combining meditation and
classic psychedelics confers increased risk for adverse
effects.
The present findings are novel, but there are several
limitations. First, the sample was stratified across age,
sex and ethnicity to reflect the demographic distribu-
tion of the US population, but previous research has
found Democrats to be overrepresented on online plat-
forms (Arechar and Rand 2021; Paolacci and Chandler
2014). The survey did not ask respondents to report
political identity, which meant the sample’s political
representativeness could not be determined. The per-
centage of participants who reported lifetime classic
psychedelic use in the present study’s full sample
(23%) was higher than prevalence in the US (14%)
found in recent investigations using the National
Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH;
Simonsson, Sexton, and Hendricks 2021). The
NSDUH does not ask specific questions about aya-
huasca or DMT use alone, but prior investigations
have instead derived ayahuasca and DMT use from
a write-in section, which may underestimate the per-
centage of ayahuasca and DMT users in the population.
Nonetheless, the higher prevalence of lifetime classic
psychedelic exposure in the current sample raises some
questions regarding representativeness. Second, as
noted, the meditators in the sample had limited experi-
ence with meditation practice. It is therefore possible
that the associations would be different for experienced
meditators or meditators with experience primarily in
intensive settings. Third, the survey did not ask respon-
dents to describe the set and setting of their use of
classic psychedelics. The set and setting-specific asso-
ciations (e.g., context, frequency, dose, intentions, and
psychological support) between past use of classic psy-
chedelics and meditation-related variables could there-
fore not be evaluated. Fourth, the question about
perceived efficacy of meditation practice may have
6O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
been interpreted differently across respondents (e.g.,
efficacy in improving mental health, physical health),
which makes it difficult to understand in what way
meditation practice might have been effective for the
respondents. Fifth, the relatively modest sample size
meant that a few variables had to be dichotomized
while lifetime alcohol use had to be dropped due to
collinearity in some of the regression models. Sixth,
causality could not be inferred due to the cross-
sectional design. It is, for example, possible that the
emphasis on non-self in Buddhism may have increased
the likelihood of respondents who had a Buddhist med-
itation practice to experience high degrees of ego dis-
solution during the acute psychedelic experience. The
issue of causality is further muddied by not assessing
the time course of classic psychedelic use or meditation
practice.
Notwithstanding these limitations, the current study
highlights several potentially intriguing linkages
between psychedelic-related and meditation-related
variables. Future research should use larger samples
and would ideally use randomized controlled trials to
help clarify the potential synergy between classic psy-
chedelics and meditation practice.
Note
1. Note: PCP was included as a response option but not
counted as a classic psychedelic in analyses. All partici-
pants who reported lifetime PCP use also reported life-
time classic psychedelic use.
Data availability statement
Data are available by request.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the
author(s).
Funding
OS was supported by the Sweden-America Foundation and
Osmond Foundation. SG was supported by a grant
(K23AT010879) from the National Center for
Complementary and Integrative Health.
ORCID
Otto Simonsson http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4197-7566
Simon B. Goldberg http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6888-0126
Ethical approval
All procedures performed involving human participants were
in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
standards. The study was approved by the Internal Review
Board (IRB) at UW-Madison.
Author contributions
SG conceptualized and designed the study, with input from
OS. OS analyzed the data with assistance from SG. OS wrote
the manuscript, with comments from SG. SG supervised the
study.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual partici-
pants included in the studies.
References
Arechar, A. A., and D. G. Rand. 2021. Turking in the time of
COVID. Behavior Research Methods 53 (1):1–5.
doi:10.3758/s13428-021-01588-4.
Britton, W. B., J. R. Lindahl, and D. J. Cooper. 2018.
Meditation-related adverse eects scale, mindfulness-based
program version (MRAES-MBP). Brown University.
Dahl, C. J., A. Lutz, and R. J. Davidson. 2015. Reconstructing
and deconstructing the self: Cognitive mechanisms in med-
itation practice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19 (9):515–23.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001.
Davis, A. K., F. S. Barrett, D. G. May, M. P. Cosimano,
N. D. Sepeda, M. W. Johnson, . . . R. R. Griffiths. 2021.
Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive
disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry
78 (5):481–89. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3285.
Fresco, D. M., M. T. Moore, M. H. van Dulmen, Z. V. Segal,
S. H. Ma, J. D. Teasdale, and J. M. G. Williams. 2007. Initial
psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire:
Validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior
Therapy 38 (3):234–46. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.003.
Gecht, J., R. Kessel, V. Mainz, S. Gauggel, B. Drueke,
A. Scherer, and T. Forkmann. 2014. Measuring decentering
in self-reports: Psychometric properties of the experiences
questionnaire in a German sample. Psychotherapy Research
24 (1):67–79. doi:10.1080/10503307.2013.821635.
Goldberg, S. B., B. Shechet, C. R. Nicholas, C. W. Ng, G. Deole,
Z. Chen, and C. L. Raison. 2020. Post-acute psychological
effects of classical serotonergic psychedelics: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 1–12.
doi:10.1017/S003329172000389X.
Griffiths, R. R., M. W. Johnson, W. A. Richards, B. D. Richards,
R. Jesse, K. A. MacLean, . . . M. A. Klinedinst. 2018.
Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combina-
tion with meditation and other spiritual practices produces
enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and
in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. Journal
of Psychopharmacology 32 (1):49–69. doi:10.1177/
0269881117731279.
JOURNAL OF PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS 7
Hunt, C. A., M. A. Hoffman, J. J. Mohr, and A. L. Williams.
2020. Assessing perceived barriers to meditation: The deter-
minants of meditation practice inventory-revised (DMPI-R).
Mindfulness 1–11. doi:10.1007/s12671-020-01308-7.
Kettner, H., S. Gandy, E. C. Haijen, and R. L. Carhart-Harris.
2019. From egoism to ecoism: Psychedelics increase nature
relatedness in a state-mediated and context-dependent
manner. International Journal of Environmental Research
and Public Health 16 (24):5147. doi:10.3390/
ijerph16245147.
Lebedev, A. V., M. Kaelen, M. Lövdén, J. Nilsson, A. Feilding,
D. J. Nutt, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2016. LSD-induced
entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality
change. Human Brain Mapping 37 (9):3203–13.
doi:10.1002/hbm.23234.
Lindahl, J. R., N. E. Fisher, D. J. Cooper, R. K. Rosen, and
W. B. Britton. 2017. The varieties of contemplative experi-
ence: A mixed-methods study of meditation-related chal-
lenges in Western Buddhists. PloS One 12 (5):e0176239.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0176239.
Luoma, J. B., C. Chwyl, G. J. Bathje, A. K. Davis, and
R. Lancelotta. 2020. A meta-analysis of placebo-controlled
trials of psychedelic-assisted therapy. Journal of
Psychoactive Drugs 52 (4):289–99. doi:10.1080/
02791072.2020.1769878.
Millière, R., R. L. Carhart-Harris, L. Roseman,
F. M. Trautwein, and A. Berkovich-Ohana. 2018.
Psychedelics, meditation, and self-consciousness. Frontiers
in Psychology 9:1475. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01475.
Naragon-Gainey, K., T. P. McMahon, M. Strowger,
R. Lackner, T. H. Seah, M. T. Moore, and D. M. Fresco.
2020. A comparison of decentering across demographic
groups and meditation experience: Support for the mea-
surement invariance of the experiences questionnaire.
Psychological Assessment 32 (2):197. doi:10.1037/
pas0000767.
Nour, M. M., L. Evans, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2017.
Psychedelics, personality and political perspectives.
Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 49 (3):182–91. doi:10.1080/
02791072.2017.1312643.
Nour, M. M., L. Evans, D. Nutt, and R. L. Carhart-Harris.
2016. Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: Validation of the
ego-dissolution inventory (EDI). Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience 10:269. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269.
Nutt, D., and R. Carhart-Harris. 2021. The current status of
psychedelics in psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry 78 (2):121–22.
doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2171.
Nutt, D. J., L. A. King, and L. D. Phillips. 2010. Drug harms in
the Uk: A multicriteria decision analysis. The Lancet
376 (9752):1558–65. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61462-6.
Osto, D. 2016. Altered states: Buddhism and psychedelic spiri-
tuality in America. Columbia University Press.
Paolacci, G., and J. Chandler. 2014. Inside the Turk:
Understanding mechanical turk as a participant pool.
Current Directions in Psychological Science 23 (3):184–88.
doi:10.1177/0963721414531598.
Payne, J. E., R. Chambers, and P. Liknaitzky. 2021. Combining
psychedelic and mindfulness interventions: Synergies to
inform clinical practice. ACS Pharmacology &
Translational Science 4 (2):416–23. doi:10.1021/
acsptsci.1c00034.
Romeo, B., L. Karila, C. Martelli, and A. Benyamina. 2020.
Efficacy of psychedelic treatments on depressive symp-
toms: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychopharmacology
34 (10):1079–85. doi:10.1177/0269881120919957.
Sessa, B. 2018. The 21st century psychedelic renaissance:
Heroic steps forward on the back of an elephant.
Psychopharmacology 235 (2):551–60. doi:10.1007/s00213-
017-4713-7.
Sexton, J. D., C. D. Nichols, and P. S. Hendricks. 2019.
Population survey data informing the therapeutic potential
of classic and novel Phenethylamine, Tryptamine, and
Lysergamide Psychedelics. Frontiers in Psychiatry 10:896.
doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00896.
Simonsson, O., J. D. Sexton, and P. S. Hendricks. 2021.
Associations between lifetime classic psychedelic use and
markers of physical health. Journal of Psychopharmacology
35 (4):447–52. doi:10.1177/0269881121996863.
Smigielski, L., M. Kometer, M. Scheidegger, R. Krähenmann,
T. Huber, and F. X. Vollenweider. 2019. Characterization
and prediction of acute and sustained response to psyche-
delic psilocybin in a mindfulness group retreat. Scientific
Reports 9 (1):1–13. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-50612-3.
Soler, J., A. Franquesa, A. Feliu-Soler, A. Cebolla,
J. García-Campayo, R. Tejedor, . . . M. J. Portella. 2014.
Assessing decentering: Validation, psychometric properties,
and clinical usefulness of the experiences questionnaire in
a Spanish sample. Behavior Therapy 45 (6):863–71.
doi:10.1016/j.beth.2014.05.004.
Uthaug, M. V., K. Van Oorsouw, K. P. C. Kuypers, M. Van
Boxtel, N. J. Broers, N. L. Mason, . . . J. G. Ramaekers. 2018.
Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and
cognitive thinking style and their association with ego
dissolution. Psychopharmacology 235 (10):2979–89.
doi:10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3.
The WHO ASSIST Working Group. 2002. The alcohol, smok-
ing and substance involvement screening test (ASSIST):
Development, reliability and feasibility. Addiction
97 (9):1183–94. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00185.x.
8O. SIMONSSON AND S. B. GOLDBERG
... First, the sample was stratified across sex, age, and ethnicity to reflect the US adult population, but it does not appear to have been representative on other variables related to health behavior and physical health (most notably the socioeconomic indicators educational attainment and annual household income). The percentage of participants who reported lifetime classic psychedelic use (22%) was similar to the prevalence of lifetime classic psychedelic use (23%) in another study using Prolific Academic for study recruitment, 25 but it was still higher than the prevalence in the United States (14%) in previous research using the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). 2 This may be because the current sample was of higher socioeconomic status than the NSDUH sample 26 and also because the previous research using the NSDUH likely underestimates ayahuasca and DMT use. 25 Second, the questionnaire did not include items related to set and setting of the most insightful classic psychedelic experience, which could have been used to identify context-dependent variables related to psychological insight, health behavior, and physical health. Since previous research has found set and setting to be an important aspect of the classic psychedelic experience, 27,28 it would, therefore, be interesting to explore the contribution of these variables in future research. ...
... The percentage of participants who reported lifetime classic psychedelic use (22%) was similar to the prevalence of lifetime classic psychedelic use (23%) in another study using Prolific Academic for study recruitment, 25 but it was still higher than the prevalence in the United States (14%) in previous research using the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). 2 This may be because the current sample was of higher socioeconomic status than the NSDUH sample 26 and also because the previous research using the NSDUH likely underestimates ayahuasca and DMT use. 25 Second, the questionnaire did not include items related to set and setting of the most insightful classic psychedelic experience, which could have been used to identify context-dependent variables related to psychological insight, health behavior, and physical health. Since previous research has found set and setting to be an important aspect of the classic psychedelic experience, 27,28 it would, therefore, be interesting to explore the contribution of these variables in future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Preliminary evidence suggests that classic psychedelics may be effective in the treatment of some psychiatric disorders, yet little remains known about their effects on health behavior and physical health. Objectives The purpose of this study was to investigate associations of lifetime classic psychedelic use and psychological insight during one’s most insightful classic psychedelic experience with health behavior and physical health. Methods Using data representative of the US population with regard to sex, age, and ethnicity ( N = 2822), this study examined associations of lifetime classic psychedelic use and psychological insight with health behavior and physical health. Results Lifetime classic psychedelic use was associated with more healthy tobacco-related and diet-related behavior ( β = 0.05 and 0.09, respectively). Among lifetime classic psychedelic users ( n = 613), greater Psychological Insight Questionnaire (PIQ) total scale, PIQ Avoidance and Maladaptive Patterns (AMP) subscale, and PIQ Goals and Adaptive Patterns (GAP) subscale scores were each associated with higher odds of more healthy exercise-related behavior [adjusted odds ratios (aOR) (95% confidence interval, CI = 1.38 (1.13–1.68), 1.38 (1.13–1.68), and 1.32 (1.10–1.60), respectively] and higher odds of having a healthy body mass index (BMI) [aOR (95% CI) = 1.32 (1.07–1.63), 1.36 (1.10–1.69), and 1.23 (1.01–1.50), respectively], and greater GAP subscale scores were associated with more healthy diet-related behavior ( β = 0.10). All PIQ scales were positively associated with some health behavior improvements (overall, diet, exercise) attributed to respondents’ most insightful classic psychedelic experience ( β = 0.42, 0.18, and 0.17; β = 0.40, 0.19, and 0.17; and β = 0.40, 0.15, and 0.15, respectively), but only PIQ total scale and AMP subscale scores were positively associated with alcohol-related health behavior improvements ( β = 0.13 and 0.16, respectively). Conclusion Although these results cannot demonstrate causality, they suggest that psychological insight during a classic psychedelic experience may lead to positive health behavior change and better physical health in some domains, in particular in those related to weight management.
... It was, however, more common for respondents who had used psychedelics to report greater self-understanding and also positive changes in interpersonal attitudes and behaviors than those who had used mindfulness meditation. These identified overlaps and differences contribute to the emerging research literature on the potential complementary effects of mindfulness meditation and psychedelic use (Millière et al., 2018;Heuschkel and Kuypers, 2020;Payne et al., 2021; see also Simonsson and Goldberg, 2022;Simonsson et al., 2023). There was a higher prevalence of responses coded as positive impact among respondents who reported mindfulness meditation practice (70.9% versus 40.6% of those who reported psychedelic use) and a higher (though still low) prevalence of responses coded as negative impact among respondents who reported lifetime psychedelic use (1.4% versus 0.2% of those who reported mindfulness meditation use). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Previous research suggests that mindfulness meditation and psychedelic substances show promise as mental health interventions, but relatively little remains known about their potential impact on leadership outcomes. Aims This study aimed to investigate if and how mindfulness meditation and psychedelic use may impact leadership among respondents with a management position as their primary role at work. Methods Using samples representative of the US and UK adult populations with regard to sex, age, and ethnicity, this study used quantitative and qualitative methods to examine if and how mindfulness meditation and psychedelic use may impact leadership. Results Among respondents with a management position as their primary role at work ( n = 3,150), 1,373 reported having tried mindfulness meditation and 559 reported having tried psychedelics. In covariate-adjusted regression analyses, both lifetime number of hours of mindfulness meditation practice and greater psychological insight during respondents’ most intense psychedelic experience were associated with describing a positive impact on leadership (ORs = 2.33, 3.49; p s < 0.001), while qualitative analyses revealed nuances in the type of impacts mindfulness meditation and psychedelic use had on leadership. There were several subthemes (e.g., focus, creativity, patience, empathy, compassion) that were frequently reported with both mindfulness meditation and psychedelic use. There were also unique subthemes that were more commonly reported with mindfulness meditation (e.g., improved sleep, stress reduction, calming effects) and psychedelic use (e.g., greater self-understanding, less hierarchical attitudes toward colleagues, positive changes in interpersonal attitudes and behaviors), respectively. Conclusion Although causality cannot be inferred due to the research design, the findings in this study suggest potential complementary effects of mindfulness meditation and psychedelic use on leadership, which could inspire new approaches in leadership development.
... To maximize the wise and beneficial use of psychedelics, the contextual factors supporting self-transcendent value changes should be more closely explored. For example, the synergistic effects of psychedelic and meditative practices should be further investigated Smigielski et al., 2019a,b;Heuschkel and Kuypers, 2020;Payne et al., 2021;Simonsson and Goldberg, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Psychedelic experiences have been shown to both facilitate (re)connection to one’s values and change values, including enhancing aesthetic appreciation, promoting pro-environmental attitudes, and encouraging prosocial behavior. This article presents an empirically informed framework of philosophical psychology to understand how self-transcendence relates to psychedelic value changes. Most of the observed psychedelic value changes are toward the self-transcendent values of Schwartz’s value theory. As psychedelics also reliably cause various self-transcendent experiences (STEs), a parsimonious hypothesis is that STEs change values toward self-transcendent values. I argue that STEs indeed can lead to value changes, and discuss the morally relevant process of self-transcendence through Iris Murdoch’s concept of “unselfing”. I argue that overt egocentric concerns easily bias one’s valuations. Unselfing reduces egocentric attributions of salience and enhances non-egocentric attention to the world, widening one’s perspective and shifting evaluation toward self-transcendent modes. Values are inherently tied to various evaluative contexts, and unselfing can attune the individual to evaluative contexts and accompanying values beyond the self. Understood this way, psychedelics can provide temporarily enhanced access to self-transcendent values and function as sources of aspiration and value change. However, contextual factors can complicate whether STEs lead to long-term changes in values. The framework is supported by various research strands establishing empirical and conceptual connections between long-term differences in egocentricity, STEs, and self-transcendent values. Furthermore, the link between unselfing and value changes is supported by phenomenological and theoretical analysis of psychedelic experiences, as well as empirical findings on their long-term effects. This article furthers understanding of psychedelic value changes and contributes to discussions on whether value changes are justified, whether they result from cultural context, and whether psychedelics could function as tools of moral neuroenhancement.
... Some 77% of the spiritually motivated entheogen users reported having at least one form for spiritual practice, supporting hypothesis H2 that was based on the finding from the interview study that entheogen users tend to engage with practices such as meditation and yoga. Furthermore, this finding is congruent with recent research by Simonsson and Goldberg (2022) and with the "seeker narratives" identified by Dollar (2022). Support was also obtained for the hypothesized preference for intimate social settings (H3), as 91% of survey respondents used entheogens in solitude, with a single partner, or with a close group of friends. ...
Article
Investigations of the use of psychedelic or entheogenic drugs in spiritual contexts have focused on mystical experience. Arguing that entheogenic spirituality should be understood more broadly, this study recruited 319 individuals into an online survey. Respondents were predominantly from western countries, but reported a connection to an eclectic range of religious and spiritual traditions, with Buddhism as the largest religion. About half of the respondents reported having a meditation practice, and the most endorsed motivations for entheogen use related to personal growth and spirituality. For spiritually motivated respondents, entheogenic experiences were most commonly characterized by feelings of joy, peace, and love, by insight into oneself and one’s relations, and by improved connections with nature and with other people. Spiritually affiliated participants were more likely to report mystical experiences involving ego dissolution and contact or unity experiences and reported more positive long-term consequences from entheogen use. The study affirms the existence of a movement of spiritually motivated entheogen users that requires further investigation.
... Prior meditation practice to a psilocybin session has been associated with greater levels of oceanic boundlessness in a psilocybin session Smigielski et al., 2019) and deep meditative states are associated with boundary dissolution, with the capacity for passive "letting go" driving the depth of the dissolution (Nave et al., 2021). Meditation practice and ego-dissolution experiences under a psychedelic also appear to have a reciprocal and reinforcing relationship, with meditation practice associated with greater occurrence of ego-dissolution experiences while under a psychedelic, while the latter experience was perceived positively in reference to meditation, motivating practice, reducing perceived barriers and associated with finding meditation more effective (Simonsson & Goldberg, 2022). Meditation practice has been associated with enhanced openness (Barner & Barner, 2011;Pokorski & Suchorzynska, 2017;van den Hurk et al., 2011) which may serve to increase propensity to mystical experiences occurring during a psychedelic session, while also helping to sustain gains in openness following it. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mystical experiences are often described as being among the most profound and meaningful events of a person’s life. Their occurrence, while a normal but uncommon phenomenon, is reliably occasioned by psychedelic substances under the appropriate conditions, although care is needed around the context of usage to help ensure safe and beneficial experiences. The occurrence of mystical experiences in psychedelic sessions is a key mediator of the sustained psychological benefits reported in both healthy and clinical populations. Certain factors including set and setting, drug dosage, trait absorption, drug type, intention and states of surrender and acceptance all predict or influence the occurrence of mystical experiences. Various additional factors may further contribute to the occurrence and intensity of mystical experiences and enhance their long-term benefits, including music, meditation and spiritual practices and nature-based settings. This review examines these factors and considers how they might be optimised to increase the chances of a mystical experience occurring, while also considering factors that are negatively associated with mystical experiences with suggestions on how these might be mitigated where applicable. Finally, potential future research avenues for furthering our knowledge of psychedelic mystical experiences and how their benefits might be enhanced is suggested. Maximising the potential for the occurrence of mystical experiences is an important aspect of the beneficial application of psychedelics.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Previous research has investigated potential synergies between classic psychedelics and meditation practice, but relatively little remains known about the relationship between classic psychedelic experiences and engagement with meditation practice. The purpose of this study was to investigate associations between classic psychedelic experiences and engagement with two popular types of meditation: mindfulness meditation and loving-kindness or compassion meditation. Method This retrospective, population-based observational study included 2822 respondents aged 18 years or older in the United States. Using covariate-adjusted regression models, this study examined associations of classic psychedelic experiences with current practice of mindfulness meditation and loving-kindness or compassion meditation. Results In covariate-adjusted regression models, lifetime classic psychedelic use was associated with a higher frequency of current mindfulness meditation practice but not current loving-kindness or compassion meditation practice. Both psychological insight and “ego dissolution” were associated with a higher frequency of current mindfulness meditation practice and current loving-kindness or compassion meditation practice. Notably, when psychological insight and “ego dissolution” were entered into the regression model simultaneously, only greater psychological insight was associated with having a higher frequency of current mindfulness meditation practice and current loving-kindness or compassion meditation practice. Conclusions Although the findings in this study cannot demonstrate causality, they suggest that classic psychedelic experiences may exert a positive effect on the cultivation and maintenance of health-related behaviors such as regular meditation practice, with psychological insight appearing to be a stronger predictor than “ego dissolution.” Preregistration This study was not preregistered.
Article
The history of psychedelia within the New Left counterculture often implies a cultural alignment between psychedelics and progressive values or the promise of radical communitarian social reform. In contrast to these potentials, this paper examines Silicon Valley’s engagement with psychedelics, a community which has demonstrated considerable financial and personal interests in these drugs despite promoting and advancing consistently neoliberal ends. This article studies Silicon Valley’s culture of psychedelic drug use through extensive analysis of published interviews by tech industrialists, news reports, and recent studies on the tech industry’s proliferation of mystical and utopian rhetoric. This work finds that psychedelics and their associated practices are given unconventional mystical meanings by some high-profile tech entrepreneurs, and that these meanings are integrated into belief systems and philosophies which are explicitly anti-democratic, individualist, and essentialist. It is argued that these mystical ideas are supported by a venture capital community which profits from the expression of disruptive utopian beliefs. These beliefs, when held by the extremely wealthy, have effects on legalization policy and the ways which psychedelics are commercialized within a legal marketplace. As Silicon Valley has put considerable resources into funding research and advocacy for psychedelics, I argue that the legalization of psychedelics will likely be operationalized to generate a near-monopoly on the market and promote further inequality in the United States that is reflective of both neoliberalism, and the essentialist beliefs of Silicon Valley functionaries.
Thesis
Full-text available
This dissertation presents and discusses a range of articles related to studies in entheogenic spirituality. As these studies have understood the matter, entheogenic spirituality is a phenomenon involving the use of entheogenic drugs – LSD, psilocybin, DMT, MDMA, and cannabis – in informal settings for spiritual purposes. It is connected to entheogenic experience, but also to the integration of experience for purposes of personal growth. The most common characteristics for entheogenic experiences were connected to insight, positive feelings, and improved connections to other people and to nature. Experiences with mystical-type characteristics such as ego dissolution and unification with transcendent forces were important to many spiritual entheogen users, but not to everybody, and rarely to spiritual cannabis users. The individual articles relate the specific findings they discuss to extant research, although most of this research has been performed by academics working in fields outside the Study of Religions. There is also a small but growing literature on entheogenic spirituality by scholars of religion, however, and the overview article discusses how my research relates to this literature. In addition, it discusses the issue of how entheogenic spirituality challenges our understanding of religion in an overall sense, and particularly from the perspective on the relationship between religion and power. As a largely non-institutionalized form of religion, entheogenic spirituality does not conform to an understanding of religion as involving institutions. Nevertheless, it can be understood in relation to discourse, practice, community, and experience as a form of institution-less religion. Since entheogens are apparently highly efficacious means of inducing experiences with mystical-type characteristics, furthermore, and since such characteristics may serve as a basis for claims to spiritual authority, entheogenic spirituality has the apparent capacity to challenge the authority and power of religious institutions. The overview article discusses how a power-centric perspective on religion may help us understand both the position of entheogenic spirituality in modern western societies and the position of studies in entheogenic spirituality in the modern academy.
Article
Full-text available
Background In recent years, there has been significant research on the mental health effects of classic psychedelic use, but there is very little evidence on how classic psychedelics might influence physical health. Aims The purpose of the present study was to investigate the associations between lifetime classic psychedelic use and markers of physical health. Methods Using data from the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (2015-2018) with 171,766 (unweighted) adults aged 18 or above in the United States, the current study examined the associations between lifetime classic psychedelic use and three markers of physical health (self-reported overall health, body mass index, and heart condition and/or cancer in the past 12 months) while controlling for a range of covariates. Results Respondents who reported having tried a classic psychedelic at least once in their lifetime had significantly higher odds of greater self-reported overall health and significantly lower odds of being overweight or obese versus having a normal weight. The association between lifetime classic psychedelic use and having a heart condition and/or cancer in the past 12 months approached conventional levels of significance, with lower odds of having a heart condition and/or cancer in the past 12 months for respondents who had tried a classic psychedelic at least once. Conclusion The results of the present study suggest that classic psychedelics may be beneficial to physical health. Future research should investigate the causal effects of classic psychedelics on physical health and evaluate possible mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
Importance: Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a substantial public health burden, but current treatments have limited effectiveness and adherence. Recent evidence suggests that 1 or 2 administrations of psilocybin with psychological support produces antidepressant effects in patients with cancer and in those with treatment-resistant depression. Objective: To investigate the effect of psilocybin therapy in patients with MDD. Design, Setting, and Participants: This randomized, waiting list–controlled clinical trial was conducted at the Center for Psychedelic and Consciousness Research at Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center in Baltimore, Maryland. Adults aged 21 to 75 years with an MDD diagnosis, not currently using antidepressant medications, and without histories of psychotic disorder, serious suicide attempt, or hospitalization were eligible to participate. Enrollment occurred between August 2017 and April 2019, and the 4-week primary outcome assessments were completed in July 2019. A total of 27 participants were randomized to an immediate treatment condition group (n = 15) or delayed treatment condition group (waiting list control condition; n = 12). Data analysis was conducted from July 1, 2019, to July 31, 2020, and included participants who completed the intervention (evaluable population). Interventions: Two psilocybin sessions (session 1: 20 mg/70 kg; session 2: 30 mg/70 kg) were given (administered in opaque gelatin capsules with approximately 100 mL of water) in the context of supportive psychotherapy (approximately 11 hours). Participants were randomized to begin treatment immediately or after an 8-week delay. Main Outcomes and Measures: The primary outcome, depression severity was assessed with the GRID-Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (GRID-HAMD) scores at baseline (score of ≥17 required for enrollment) and weeks 5 and 8 after enrollment for the delayed treatment group, which corresponded to weeks 1 and 4 after the intervention for the immediate treatment group. Secondary outcomes included the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology-Self Rated (QIDS-SR). Results: Of the randomized participants, 24 of 27 (89%) completed the intervention and the week 1 and week 4 postsession assessments. This population had a mean (SD) age of 39.8 (12.2) years, was composed of 16 women (67%), and had a mean (SD) baseline GRID-HAMD score of 22.8 (3.9). The mean (SD) GRID-HAMD scores at weeks 1 and 4 (8.0 [7.1] and 8.5 [5.7]) in the immediate treatment group were statistically significantly lower than the scores at the comparable time points of weeks 5 and 8 (23.8 [5.4] and 23.5 [6.0]) in the delayed treatment group. The effect sizes were large at week 5 (Cohen d = 2.2; 95% CI, 1.4-3.0; P < .001) and week 8 (Cohen d = 2.6; 95% CI, 1.7-3.6; P < .001). The QIDS-SR documented a rapid decrease in mean (SD) depression score from baseline to day 1 after session 1 (16.7 [3.5] vs 6.3 [4.4]; Cohen d = 3.0; 95% CI, 1.9-4.0; P < .001), which remained statistically significantly reduced through the week 4 follow-up (6.0 [5.7]; Cohen d = 3.1; 95% CI, 1.9-4.2; P < .001). In the overall sample, 16 participants (67%) at week 1 and 17 (71%) at week 4 had a clinically significant response to the intervention (≥50% reduction in GRID-HAMD score), and 14 participants (58%) at week 1 and 13 participants (54%) at week 4 were in remission (≤7 GRID-HAMD score). Conclusions and Relevance: Findings suggest that psilocybin with therapy is efficacious in treating MDD, thus extending the results of previous studies of this intervention in patients with cancer and depression and of a nonrandomized study in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03181529
Article
Full-text available
After a two-decade hiatus in which research on psychedelics was essentially halted, placebo-controlled clinical trials of psychedelic-assisted therapy for mental health conditions have begun to be published. We identified nine randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trials of psychedelic-assisted therapy published since 1994. Studies examined psilocybin, LSD (lysergic acid diethylamide), ayahuasca (which contains a combination of N,N-dimethyltryptamine and harmala monoamine oxidase inhibitor alkaloids), and MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine). We compared the standardized mean difference between the experimental and placebo control group at the primary endpoint. Results indicated a significant mean between-groups effect size of 1.21 (Hedges g), which is larger than the typical effect size found in trials of psychopharmacological or psychotherapy interventions. For the three studies that maintained a placebo control through a follow-up assessment, effects were generally maintained at follow-up. Overall, analyses support the efficacy of psychedelic-assisted therapy across four mental health conditions-post-traumatic stress disorder, anxiety/depression associated with a life-threatening illness, unipolar depression, and social anxiety among autistic adults. While study quality was high, we identify several areas for improvement regarding the conduct and reporting of trials. Larger trials with more diverse samples are needed to examine possible moderators and mediators of effects, and to establish whether effects are maintained over time.
Article
On March 16, 2020, the US Government introduced strict social distancing protocols for the United States in an effort to stem the spread of the COVID-19 pandemic. This had an immediate major effect on the job market, with millions of Americans forced to find alternative ways to make a living from home. As online labor markets like Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) play a major role in social science research, concerns have been raised that the pandemic may be reducing the diversity of subjects participating in experiments. Here, we investigate this possibility empirically. Specifically, we look at 15,539 responses gathered in 23 studies run on MTurk between February and July 2020, examining the distribution of gender, age, ethnicity, political preference, and analytic cognitive style. We find notable changes on some of the measures following the imposition of nationwide social distancing: participants are more likely to be less reflective (as measured by the Cognitive Reflection Test), and somewhat less likely to be white, Democrats (traditionally over-represented on MTurk), and experienced with MTurk. Most of these differences are explained by an influx of new participants into the MTurk subject pool who are more diverse and representative – but also less attentive – than previous MTurkers.
Article
Psychedelic and mindfulness interventions have been shown to improve mental ill-health and wellbeing, with a range of clinical processes and effects in common. However, each appear to contain specific challenges in the context of mental health treatment. In this Perspective, we focus on a set of distinct affordances, “useful differences”, within psychedelic and mindfulness interventions that might address common challenges within the other intervention. Accordingly, we propose a set of applied synergies, indicating specific ways in which these two promising interventions might be combined for greater benefit. Metaphorically, on the journey toward mental health and wellbeing, we propose that psychedelic treatments may serve the role of Compass (initiating, motivating, and steering the course of mindfulness practice), with mindfulness interventions serving the role of Vehicle (integrating, deepening, generalizing, and maintaining the novel perspectives and motivation instigated by psychedelic experience). We outline a set of testable hypotheses and future research associated with the synergistic action of psychedelic and mindfulness interventions toward improved clinical outcomes.
Article
Background: Scientific interest in the therapeutic effects of classical psychedelics has increased in the past two decades. The psychological effects of these substances outside the period of acute intoxication have not been fully characterized. This study aimed to: (1) quantify the effects of psilocybin, ayahuasca, and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) on psychological outcomes in the post-acute period; (2) test moderators of these effects; and (3) evaluate adverse effects and risk of bias. Methods: We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies (single-group pre-post or randomized controlled trials) that involved administration of psilocybin, ayahuasca, or LSD to clinical or non-clinical samples and assessed psychological outcomes ⩾24 h post-administration. Effects were summarized by study design, timepoint, and outcome domain. Results: A total of 34 studies (24 unique samples, n = 549, mean longest follow-up = 55.34 weeks) were included. Classical psychedelics showed significant within-group pre-post and between-group placebo-controlled effects on a range of outcomes including targeted symptoms within psychiatric samples, negative and positive affect-related measures, social outcomes, and existential/spiritual outcomes, with large between-group effect in these domains (Hedges' gs = 0.84 to 1.08). Moderator tests suggest some effects may be larger in clinical samples. Evidence of effects on big five personality traits and mindfulness was weak. There was no evidence of post-acute adverse effects. Conclusions: High risk of bias in several domains, heterogeneity across studies, and indications of publication bias for some models highlight the need for careful, large-scale, placebo-controlled randomized trials.
Article
In the 1950s, the Swiss pharmaceutical company Sandoz, which employed the chemist Albert Hofmann, who discovered lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and the similar serotonergic psychedelic psilocybin, made these drugs available to the psychiatric research community as the products Delysid and Indocybin, respectively. By the 1960s, these drugs had caused a revolution in brain science and psychiatry because of their widespread use by researchers and clinicians in many Western countries, especially the US. Before LSD was banned, the US National Institutes of Health funded more than 130 studies exploring its clinical utility, with positive results in a range of disorders but particularly anxiety, depression, and alcoholism. However, the displacement of LSD into recreational use and eventual association with the anti-Vietnam war movement led to all psychedelics being banned in the US. This ban became ratified globally under the 1971 UN Convention on narcotics. Since then, research funding, drug production, and the study of psychedelics as clinical agents has been virtually stopped. Until very recently, no companies would manufacture medical-grade psychedelics, which made getting regulatory approval for clinical research—especially clinical trials—very difficult and in some countries (eg, Germany) impossible.
Article
Background Psychedelic drugs have shown an efficacy in some psychiatric disorders and have an original mechanism of action with a 5-HT 2A agonism. Aim The aim of this meta-analysis was to assess by a quantitative analysis the putative efficacy of psychedelic drugs on depressive symptoms and to investigate the kinetic of this efficacy. Methods We searched the MEDLINE and PsycINFO databases through April 2019, without limits on year of publication. Means and standard deviations were extracted to calculate standardized mean differences (SMD). Scores of depressive symptoms were compared with baseline scores at days 7, 14, and 21; weeks 4–5 and 6–8; and months 3 and 6. Results Eight studies were included in this meta-analysis. A significant decrease of depressive symptoms was found from day 1 ( n = 5 studies; SMD = ‒1.4, 95% confidence interval (CI): ‒2.33 to ‒0.48, p = 0.003) to 6 months ( n = 4 studies; SMD = ‒1.07, 95% CI: ‒1.44 to ‒0.7, p < 0.001) after psychedelic sessions. No serious adverse effect was reported in all included studies. A transient increase of the heart rate, blood systolic, and diastolic pressure were found after psychedelics compared with placebo. Conclusion This meta-analysis shows that psychedelic treatments were safe and could contribute to a rapid improvement of depressive symptoms.