Content uploaded by Zuzanna Brunarska
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Zuzanna Brunarska on Jan 10, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
ARTICLE
Does Origin Matter? Ethnic Group Position and Attitudes
Toward Immigrants: The Case of Russia
Zuzanna Brunarska
1,2
and Wiktor Soral
2,3
1
Centre of Migration Research, University of Warsaw, Poland,
2
The Robert B. Zajonc Institute for Social Studies, University of
Warsaw, Poland and
3
Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Poland
Corresponding author: Zuzanna Brunarska, email: zuzanna.brunarska@uw.edu.pl
Abstract
This article analyzes the relationship between the relative position of an ethnic group, as measured by its
majority/minority status at a subnational level, and attitudes of its members toward immigrants of different
origins. Based on the Russian case, it addresses the question whether the effects of in-group majority status
within a region on attitudes toward the general category of immigrants hold regardless of out-group origin
and, if not, what may drive this variation. Using data from the Russia Longitudinal Monitoring Survey of the
Higher School of Economics and Bayesian hierarchical structural equation modeling, the study demon-
strates that the relative position of an ethnic in-group is of varying importance as a predictor of attitudes
toward migrant groups of European versus non-European origin in Russia. A group’s majority status within
a region proved to play a role in predicting attitudes toward migrants originating from the “south”
(encompassing North and South Caucasus; Central Asia; and China, Vietnam, and Korea) but not toward
migrants coming from the “west”(Ukraine and Moldova). We draw on arguments related to the source and
the level of threat induced by the out-groups, ethnic hierarchies, and group cues to explain this pattern of
results.
Keywords: attitudes toward immigrants; group position; majority status; origin; Russia
Given the prevalence of migration processes and integration challenges faced by various receiving
societies in today’s world, attitudes toward newcomers have become an increasingly topical issue.
Public opinion polls often measure attitudes toward a broad category of immigrants. Meanwhile, it
has been shown that mental images of immigrants vary across individuals, and people usually do
not generalize their answers across different subcategories—for example, representing different
origins—but rather tend to concentrate on specific subcategories (Blinder 2015). Even when a
survey asks about various subcategories of immigrants, attitudes toward immigrant targets have
often been analyzed jointly in academic research (Ford 2011; Ruedin 2020). The implicit assump-
tion underlying such an approach is that the same theoretical mechanisms work in the cases of all
immigrant groups. This might not necessarily be true, as shown by the recently growing body of
research that acknowledges the existence of differences in the attitudes toward different immigrant
groups and in the mechanisms underlying them (Brader, Valentino, and Suhay 2008; Ford 2011;
Ford and Mellon 2020; Gorodzeisky and Semyonov 2019; Hainmueller and Hopkins 2015; Harell
et al. 2012; Konitzer et al. 2019; Raijman, Hochman, Davidov 2021; Ruedin 2020; Tartakovsky and
Walsh 2020; Timberlake et al. 2015; Turper et al. 2015; Valentino, Brader, and Jardina 2013). We
contribute to this line of research by testing the assumption of common theoretical mechanisms for
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Association for the Study of Nationalities. This is an Open Access
article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits
unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Nationalities Papers (2022), 1–18
doi:10.1017/nps.2021.71
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
a model explaining anti-immigrant attitudes with the relative position of an ethnic in-group for
different immigrant origins in Russia.
The argument that perceived in-group position is important in explaining negative attitudes
toward out-groups was raised over 60 years ago (Blumer 1958). In his original group position
model, Blumer named four inherent elements underlying race prejudice: a sense of superiority
regarding one’s own group, a belief in its distinctiveness from other groups, a feeling of entitlement
to certain advantages and institutionalized privileges, and perceived competitive threat. The
perception of an in-group position may stem from an in-group’s majority or minority status in
terms of its relative size compared to other ethnic groups inhabiting a particular territory.
However, in multiethnic societies, several complexities may arise, which justify extending
Blumer’s original model. In such societies, ethnic composition of the population may differ across
regions, and it is possible that an ethnic group constituting a majority at a country level does not
possess a majority status within some of the country’s subdivisions. In this vein, the Russian
Federation, with its multiethnic society, offers an interesting, yet still largely understudied, setting
for analyzing attitudes toward immigrants. It is a suitable site for examining the role of relative
group position at a subnational level in shaping attitudes toward immigrants, since it contains
several regions in which ethnic groups other than the country-level dominant group (ethnic
Russians) hold a majority status. Consequently, it allows examination of the potential consequences
of positionality that may be overlooked in a typical nation-state context.
Previous research suggested that the attitudes toward immigrants among majority ethnic group
and among members of minority ethnic groups in Russia are driven by different mechanisms with
regard to the position of an in-group. At the national level, they to a varying degree rely on
perceptions of collective interests and vulnerabilities, with attitudes of ethnic Russians to a greater
extent dependent on the group position-related factors (Gorodzeisky and Glikman 2018). At the
subnational level, in turn, they proved to be differently related to the in-group position. While a
majority status at a regional level held by ethnic Russians seems to promote anti-immigrant
attitudes (Brunarska 2019; cf. Hraba et al. 1997), in the case of members of titular ethnic groups,
majority status at a regional level seems to inhibit exclusionist attitudes toward immigrants
(Brunarska 2019; cf. Alexseev 2010; Minescu and Poppe 2011 for interethnic relations). Brunarska
(2019) explains this pattern of results with reference to two theoretical perspectives: social
dominance theory (Pratto et al. 1994) and the group position model (Bobo and Hutchings
1996). The former posits that members of high-status groups show greater out-group prejudice
than members of low-status groups due to higher levels of social dominance orientation, which may
explain the operation of group position in the case of ethnic Russians. The latter posits that groups
in the subordinate position experience a feeling of racial alienation, which makes them more likely
to perceive out-group members as competitors for scarce resources. It is argued that the feeling of
alienation experienced by titulars, due to their subordinate position at the national level, is alleviated
when they hold a majority status within a region. These relationships have not been, however, tested
across different migrant groups. We assumed that accounting for the heterogeneity of immigrants
in terms of their origin would allow for a more informed consideration of the mechanisms shaping
attitudes toward them.
Perceived competitive threat constitutes a core mechanism explaining exclusionary attitudes
toward out-groups in the literature on attitudes toward immigrants (Raijman, Hochman, and
Davidov 2021; Ruedin 2020; Schoon and Anderson 2017). Given that it is one of the four inherent
elements underlying prejudice according to Blumer’s group position model, we also make it a key
element of our theoretical framework and, as a result, of our considerations. Since immigrants
coming from different origins challenge various interests of receiving communities, and different
migrant groups may consequently activate different types (Hellwig and Sinno 2017; Meuleman et al.
2018) and levels of threat (cf. Bloom, Arikan, and Lahav 2015), the question arises as to whether the
relationship between in-group’s majority status within a region and attitudes toward the general
category of immigrants holds regardless of out-group origin. In broader terms, we ask whether the
2 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
effects of the relative position of an ethnic in-group on attitudes toward immigrants vary between
origins and, if so, what may drive this variation. In our approach, we thus follow Timberlake et al.
(2015), who sought to determine whether the link between immigrant stereotypes and perceived
impact of immigration depends on the immigrant group under consideration. We presume that a
group position may be differentially important as a predictor of attitudes toward different migrant
groups. We formulate more specific research hypotheses further in the paper.
Generalized Prejudice versus Differentiated Threat Approach
According to the generalized prejudice approach (Bergh and Akrami 2016), sometimes also
referred to in the literature as group-focused enmity (Zick et al. 2008), attitudes toward different
sorts of out-groups share a common core. In other words, this approach expects that some people
score high in prejudice against multiple target groups. However, by generalizing attitudes across
target groups, the perspective of generalized prejudice disregards the social context of specific
interethnic relations, in particular the out-group-specific contexts. The differentiated threat
approach (Meuleman et al. 2018), in turn, accounts for ethnic contexts in which intergroup
relations are embedded. As Meuleman et al. state, this approach concentrates on “how the
positionality of the different others to the ingroup may invoke differentiated othering mechanisms”
(2018, 239). According to this theoretical perspective, specific prejudices depend on the source of
threat a particular out-group triggers.
1
Sources of out-group threat are usually divided in the
literature into two main types—realistic and symbolic (according to the revised integrated threat
theory; Stephan, Ybarra, and Morrison 2009), sometimes also framed as material versus cultural
threats (Bloom, Arikan, and Lahav 2015). The former relates to political and economic power,
resources, general welfare, and physical and material well-being, that is, “the very existence of the
ingroup”(Stephan et al. 2009, 43). The latter relates to the in-group’s cultural or religious values,
beliefs, morality, and worldview, that is, the in-group’s“way of life.”In line with the differentiated
threat approach, migrants coming from different places of origin “challenge diverse realistic and
symbolic interests”(Meuleman et al. 2018, 222) and therefore they are not perceived uniformly by
the receiving society.
We expect that not only may different groups trigger different types and levels of threat,
which influences attitudes toward them, but also that different theory-based mechanisms,
assuming threat as an underlying factor, may or may not work for different groups. Regarding
the role of group position, in line with the generalized prejudice approach, one would assume
that the relative position of an ethnic in-group should have a similar effect on attitudes
regardless of the target group. In contrast, the differentiated threat perspective would entail
that group position may have a divergent effect depending on the object. Consequently, in line
with both approaches combined, we hypothesize that one may note divergent effects of group
position for the clusters combining immigrant target groups associated with different types of
threat. A stronger claim may be derived from the study by Minescu and Poppe (2011), who
noted that the effect of in-group size on perceived interethnic conflict is amplified by higher
levels of perceived cultural threat and out-group encroachment (growing out-group influence).
As they argued, perceived threat and out-group encroachment act as “levers or switches that
conditioned the effects of group entitlement in fueling conflict perceptions”(185). This leads us
to expect the effect of relative in-group size, as expressed by majority status, to be smaller in the
case of migrant out-groups whose presence is less often associated with symbolic threat and
encroachment. Unfortunately, due to the limitations of our data, which do not allow operatio-
nalization of perceived threat triggered by separate origins, we will not be able to provide direct
evidence for the operation of the threat mechanism. We thus, similarly to Timberlake et al.
(2015), explain why we believe this mechanism is plausible and discuss potential alternative
explanations for the patterns observed in our data.
Nationalities Papers 3
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
The Russian Context
Russia offers a well-suited setting for testing the relevance of in-group position at a regional level
across different migrant origins as both a multiethnic society, in which some national minority
ethnic groups hold a majority status in the regions they inhabit, and one of the world’s largest
migrant-receiving states that attracts migrants of different origin. According to the official statistics,
immigrants from the former Soviet republics amount to around 90% of all incoming migrants in
Russia. Other important origins include China, India, Vietnam, and North Korea. Previous studies
showed that despite the official Soviet ideology promoting the existence of a common Soviet
identity, post-Soviet migrants are not perceived homogeneously by the Russian society (Bessudnov
and Shcherbak 2020; Gudkov 2005; Levada 2018; Pipiia 2017; Popova and Osipova 2013; see also
Gerber and Zavisca 2020). It has also been shown that in-group members tend to arrange ethnic
groups in a consensual ethnic hierarchy of social distance, based on the evaluation of cultural rather
than socioeconomic differences or economic competition (Bessudnov 2016; Hagendoorn 1995;
Hagendoorn et al. 1998). This hierarchy reflects the political dominance of highly positioned
Russians and other Slavic peoples (Ukrainians, Belarusians), while the Caucasian and Central Asian
minority groups have belonged to systematically rejected out-groups (Bessudnov 2016; Hagen-
doorn et al. 1998; Popova and Osipova 2013). Importantly, internal migrants representing the
titular nations of the North Caucasus republics, despite being Russian citizens, become targets of
hostile attitudes throughout other Russian regions (Laruelle 2010; Bessudnov 2016; Grigoryan
2016; Markowitz and Peshkova 2016). This is because a decisive factor shaping xenophobia in
Russia is ethnic belonging, not citizenship or country of origin (Popova and Osipova 2013).
Warmer feelings toward newcomers of Slavic ethnicity suggest that devaluation of North Caucasian
and Central Asian immigrant groups is based on a symbolic (cultural) rather than on a realistic
(socioeconomic) threat. Interestingly, immigrants from countries of the South Caucasus, despite
their cultural heterogeneity, are usually placed in one group with the North Caucasian and Central
Asian migrants (Grigor’ev 2017; Pipiia 2017; Butkovich Kraus and Herrera 2010; Gerber 2014),
which may be because religion is not a decisive factor shaping ethnic hierarchies in Russia either
(Bessudnov and Shcherbak 2020).
Despite the results of survey research showing that attitudes toward different ethnic or migrant
groups vary in Russia, previous theory-driven studies have often limited their considerations to
attitudes toward a general category of immigrants, not distinguishing between different target
groups (Alexseev 2010,2011; Bahry 2016; Brunarska 2019; Gorodzeisky, Glikman, and Maskiley-
son 2015; Grigoryan 2016). A notable exception is the study by Bessudnov (2016), who noted that
theory-driven associations between attitudes toward immigrants and their predictors (immigrant
concentration in his case) are present only for immigrants from the Caucasus and Central, East, and
Southeast Asia, while they do not prove significant for migrants of Ukrainian and Moldovan origin.
Although he focused on a different research problem than the one handled in this paper, his
observation provides evidence that attitudes toward the two clusters of migrant groups are
qualitatively different. Another noteworthy exception is the study by Gorodzeisky (2019), who,
based on European Social Survey data, differentiated between exclusionary attitudes based on
national membership (directed at all foreigners regardless of their origin) and exclusionary attitudes
based on race or ethnicity (directed solely at racially or ethnically different foreigners). She studied,
among other things, the role of perceived collective (state) vulnerability in predicting exclusionary
attitudes and concluded that it prompts exclusionary attitudes toward all foreigners. While
differentiation between target groups is not common in theory-driven studies that focus on
attitudes toward migrants in Russia, it is more common in theory-driven empirical research
focused on attitudes toward members of ethnic out-groups, without an emphasis on their migration
background (Butkovich Kraus and Herrera 2010; Gerber 2014; Hagendoorn et al. 1998; Herrera and
Butkovich Kraus 2016; Hraba et al. 1997). Although the out-groups considered differed across
studies, which led researchers to adopt various names to label their groupings, taken together,
4 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
previous research pointed to the existence of a fundamental divide between how Russian society
views groups of European, western origin as opposed to non-European, southern origin. For
instance, Bessudnov and Shcherbak (2020) differentiated between ethnic groups of European
(Germans, Jews, Latvians, Lithuanians, ethnic Russians, and Ukrainians) and non-European
(Armenians, Azerbaijanis, Chechens, Georgians, Tajiks, Uzbeks, and Tatars) origin. Gerber
(2014) distinguished views toward western (Americans, Jews, Swedes) and southern groups
(Azerbaijanis, Chechens, Gypsies, Muslims, Tajiks). Butkovich Kraus and Herrera (2010) made a
distinction between a Western (Jews, Americans, Italians, Swedes, and Catholics) and an Eastern/
Southern (Chechens, Azeris, Roma, and Muslims) factor within xenophobic attitudes. While we
acknowledge that there might be further distinctions within these broad categories (Gerber and
Zavisca 2020), we hypothesize that this divide between the two clusters of migrant groups is also
relevant when the focus is on the role of group position in determining attitudes toward newcomers.
Namely, we expect to observe that attitudes toward immigrant groups of non-European, southern
immigration origin are reliably related to the in-group position, whereas attitudes toward groups of
European, western immigration origin will be unaffected by the in-group position.
Method
Data
The study utilized data derived from the 17th round of the Russia Longitudinal Monitoring Survey
of the Higher School of Economics (RLMS-HSE 2008), which included several questions concern-
ing immigrants and respondents’attitudes toward them. Although this round was conducted over
10 years ago (between October 2008 and January 2009), data related to the portion of questions on
immigrants were not released for public use until 2016 and have not been used much so far. The
survey covered 38 primary sampling units (PSUs), which included cities, towns, and rural areas that
lie within 32 federal subjects of Russia, including 19 oblasts, five republics, six krais, and two federal
cities: Moscow and St. Petersburg. The original sample size amounted to 9,874 individuals. The
current study concentrates on adults (N=8,094), excluding individuals aged under 18 from the
analysis. Importantly, in each household an attempt was made to interview all its members.
Consequently, the RLMS-HSE data have a three-level structure, with individuals nested in house-
holds and households clustered in regions (PSUs).
While the RLMS-HSE is regarded as offering data of exceptionally high quality, it has certain
limitations when the focus is on the population’s ethnic structure. This stems from the fact that it
covers only five (out of 21) ethnic republics of Russia and is not representative at the federal subject
level. The republics encompassed by the survey included the Komi Republic, Chuvashia, Tatarstan,
Kabardino-Balkaria, and Udmurtia. Consequently, representatives of non-Russian ethnic groups in
the sample are mostly composed of members of titular ethnic groups of these five regions. This is
also reflected in the structure of the two ethnically non-Russian groups we seek to compare in our
study: majority and minority titulars. The former included Tatars, Chuvashes, and Kabardins in
their ethnic republics (in three locations: Kazan, Shumerlia and Shumerlinskii raion, and Zaluko-
koazhe and Zol’skii raion). The latter included primarily Udmurts (mostly in Glazov and Glazovskii
raion in Udmurtia), Komis (resident in Syktyvkar, and Usinsk and Usinskii raion in the Komi
Republic), Balkars (living in Zalukokoazhe and Zol’skii raion in Kabardino-Balkaria), Tatars
outside of Tatarstan, Chuvashes outside of Chuvashia, and Mordvinians and Bashkirs. Comparison
of ethnic Russians in minority and majority position at a regional level, in turn, boils down to
comparing attitudes of ethnic Russians living in Kazan (Tatarstan), Shumerlia and Shumerlinskii
raion (Chuvashia), and Zalukokoazhe and Zol’skii raion (Kabardino-Balkaria) to attitudes of those
residing in the remaining 35 PSUs located in the 29 federal subjects of Russia covered by the RLMS-
HSE sample. As a result, generalizability of the study findings to other ethnic groups and localities/
regions in Russia should not be taken for granted.
Nationalities Papers 5
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Measures
Attitudes toward Immigrants
Attitudes toward immigrants were measured by a question about how a respondent would react to a
hypothetical situation in which an ordinary migrant family originating from a specific place
(country or region) had settled next door. This question was repeated for six different origins:
Ukraine; Moldova; the North Caucasus (a family of Dagestanis, Chechens, Cherkesses, and others);
South Caucasus (Azerbaijanis, Armenians, Georgians); Central Asia (Kyrgyz people, Uzbeks,
Tajiks, or others); and China, Vietnam, and Korea, forming one category.
2
The answers for each
origin were scored on a five-point Likert scale, where one represented a positive attitude and five
corresponded to a negative attitude. While such an operationalization constitutes an example of an
item typically used to measure social distance (Bogardus 1925), its focus on the question of
neighbors means that attitudes measured this way are more likely to be driven by symbolic rather
than by realistic threat (cf. Velasco González et al. 2008). To conduct the analyses regarding
differential predictive values of in-group position for the two clusters of migrant origins, we
aggregated (averaged) attitudes toward immigrants of different origins into two sets consisting of
western, European immigrant groups (from Ukraine or Moldova) and southern immigrant groups
(from the Caucasus and Central, East or Southeast Asia).
Position of an Ethnic In-Group
The relative position of an ethnic in-group was expressed in terms of a respondent’s ethnic group
status within a given federal subject—majority versus minority (where holding a majority status
means being the most numerous ethnic group in a given territorial unit, according to the 2010
Russian population census [Rosstat 2013]). Importantly, respondents were asked about their self-
declared ethnic identity and were allowed to report any identity, including a mixed one. To account
for the divergent effect of group position for ethnic Russians and members of minority ethnic
groups, we differentiated between the following five categories: majority Russians, minority
Russians, majority titulars, minority titulars, and others.
3
We separated titulars from others because
we view the two national minority subgroups as qualitatively different—we assumed that by being
in a minority position in a region, titulars, as members of an autochthonous group with a potentially
greater sense of group entitlement (Martinovic and Verkuyten 2013), may experience a stronger
feeling of alienation than members of non-titular ethnic minority groups. Moreover, the others
category, which primarily includes people of Ukrainian, Armenian, Belarusian, German,
Azerbaijani, Kazakh, Moldovan, and Georgian ethnicity, has been more heavily replenished by
immigrants, and consequently its members may show more positive attitudes toward potential
migrant neighbors than representatives of more sedentary ethnic groups (see Bessudnov 2016).
While it might be informative to disaggregate specific titular groups, we need to combine them to
increase the statistical power of our analysis. We used a five-category notation instead of a classic
2x3 interaction because the others category consists of ethnicities that do not form a majority within
any federal subject of Russia (which would otherwise lead to an empty category). While we fitted the
model to data that included the others category, we conducted hypothesis tests focused only on
ethnic Russians and titulars, as we were primarily interested in the interaction of respondent’s
ethnicity (Russian vs. titular) and group position. We estimated this interaction after fitting the
model with a linear hypothesis testing approach (by computing a contrast representing this
interaction).
Control Variables
Covariates included respondents’gender, age, education level, household’s per capita log-
transformed income, migration status, and locality type. Migration status was captured by a dummy
variable stating whether a respondent was born in Russia (or Russian SFSR) or beyond its borders.
For basic descriptive statistics of variables used in the analysis, see table A1 in the appendix.
6 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Statistical Analysis
We used Bayesian
4
hierarchical structural equation modeling (BHSEM) to examine the rela-
tionship between group position and attitudes toward immigrants. The model took the form of a
system of two regression equations in which respondents’(N=8,094) attitudes toward immi-
grants of western and southern origins were regressed on other variables. It included both fixed
and random effects. The latter involved varying intercepts both at the level of regions (PSUs,
N=38) and households (N=3,808) within regions, thus forming a three-level nested structure.
Each response variable was treated as an interval measure, and hence the respective equations
took a Gaussian form. We chose to use weakly informative prior distributions for all the
parameters (e.g., Gelman et al. 2008). Graphical posterior predictive checks (Gabry et al.
2019) indicated moderate albeit acceptable differences between predicted and observed values
of the response variables. We tested models with both correlated and noncorrelated residuals of
the response variables.
To adjust for the unequal sampling probabilities, we used sampling weights both at the level of
households and individuals. Following recommendations for fitting multilevel models with com-
plex survey data, we scaled individual level weights (using Method B [Carle 2009]). To account for
missing values (across the variables used in the analyses the proportion of missing data ranged from
0% to 6.6%), we conducted multiple imputation (with the R-package mice [van Buuren and
Groothuis-Oudshoorn 2011]) and ran the model simultaneously on five imputed datasets. The
imputation model included all the variables used in the regression models (see the suggestion by
Enders [2010]).
The models were fitted with the brms R package (Bürkner 2017), using 2 MCMC (Markov Chain
Monte Carlo) chains per imputed dataset, resulting in 10 chains in total. Values of the potential scale
reduction factor were lower than 1.02, indicating no problems with the chains’convergence.
Results
A look at the general descriptive statistics, showing average attitudes toward potential migrant
neighbors of different origins in Russia (see table A1), indicates that migrants of Ukrainian and
Moldovan origin are more welcome than migrants coming from all the other regions and countries
considered (i.e., the North and South Caucasus, Central Asia, and China, Vietnam, and Korea). An
average score for potential migrant neighbors coming from Ukraine and Moldova was 2.75 and
2.89, respectively, which means that they are generally perceived more positively than negatively by
the receiving society (given that three stands for a neutral attitude). Immigrants originating from
the remaining four regions are, in turn, perceived rather negatively on average (and similarly to one
another, with averages ranging from 3.52 to 3.65).
We are interested in whether the differences between the two clusters of migrant groups—that
we labelled as western and southern target out-groups—are visible when one considers the role of
in-group position in the ethnically stratified social order—namely, its majority or minority status at
a regional level. Figure 1 illustrates the role of majority status for ethnic Russians and members of
titular ethnic groups, respectively.
It shows that the role of the relative position of an ethnic in-group differs across migrant out-
groups of different origin. Both for ethnic Russians and for members of titular nations, the majority
status in a region seems to play a role in predicting attitudes toward migrant groups of Caucasian,
Central Asian, and East and Southeast Asian origin. As for the Ukrainian and Moldovan target
groups, the evidence proves to be “negative”in the case of ethnic Russians, while it might be
described as “substantial”or “very strong,”respectively, but not “decisive,”in the case of titulars
(cf. Jeffreys 1961, 432). It also demonstrates that in the case of migrants originating from the
“south,”majority status at a regional level translates into more negative attitudes among ethnic
Russians and more positive attitudes among representatives of titular ethnic groups.
Nationalities Papers 7
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
We then tested the differences in attitudes toward the two broad origins—western and southern
groups—between majority and minority in-groups, both among ethnic Russians and titulars. To
ensure that the observed differences regarding the effects of group position and ethnicity on
attitudes toward immigrants of western and southern origin are not attributed to structural
characteristics unrelated to group position, we used a hierarchical model adjusting for a number
of additional sociodemographic variables (see figure 2 for estimated marginal means).
5
Table 1
presents a summary of estimates along with highest posterior density intervals (HPDI) as a measure
of their uncertainty. The model explained around 40% of the variance of the response variables. We
Figure 1. Average Attitudes (Y-Axis) toward Different Target Groups (X-Axis) Reported by Ethnic Russians, Titulars, and Others
(See the Label on the Right Side of the Facet) Depending on Their Majority or Minority Status (See Different Shades of Grey).
Note. BF refers to Bayes Factor in support of the hypothesis that differences between majority and minority groups are not 0.
8 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
chose the model with correlated residuals because it obtained better out-of-sample predictive
accuracy measures than a model with noncorrelated residuals (WAIC =37,694.45 vs. WAIC =
40,451.65, respectively).
In the case of attitudes toward immigrants of western, European origin, the contrast describing
interaction between a respondent’s ethnicity (Russian vs. titular) and in-group status was not
credible (the interaction contrast was small and close to 0, ψ=0.10, 95% HPDI [-0.07, 0.27]).
Furthermore, there were no credible differences between majority and minority groups, neither
among ethnic Russians (difference, ψ=0.13, 95% HPDI [-0.07, 0.32], d
Cohen
=0.14) nor among
titulars (difference, ψ=-0.07, 95% HPDI [-0.28, 0.13], d
Cohen
=-0.06).
In the case of attitudes toward immigrants of southern origin, the ethnicity (Russian vs. titular)
and in-group status (majority vs. minority) of respondents formed a credible interactive pattern
(as indicated by a large magnitude of interaction contrast, ψ=0.26, 95% HPDI [0.09, 0.43]). Among
ethnic Russians, those holding a majority status in a region showed on average credibly more
negative attitudes toward immigrants than those holding minority status at a regional level
(difference, ψ=0.28, 95% HPDI [0.08, 0.46], d
Cohen
=0.30). On the contrary, among titulars,
those holding a majority status in a region had on average a credibly lower level of anti-immigrant
attitudes than titulars holding a minority status in their region of residence (difference, ψ=-0.24,
95% HPDI [-0.47, -0.03], d
Cohen
=-0.20).
6
Thus, the results show that the role of the relative position of an ethnic in-group differs across
migrant out-groups of different origin. Both for ethnic Russians and for members of titular nations,
the majority status seems to play a role in predicting attitudes toward migrant groups of southern
origin but not western origin. Moreover, in the case of migrants originating from the “south,”the
findings confirm the presence of a positive relationship between an in-group’s majority status at a
regional level and anti-immigrant attitudes in the case of ethnic Russians, while a negative
relationship is apparent in the case of members of titular nationalities, which is in line with previous
studies.
7
One should note, however, that the largest observed difference, according to a crude effect
size measure, d
Cohen
=0.30, should rather be considered as a small effect. This suggests a high degree
of variability of the effect and possible influences of other, unobserved factors.
Figure 2. Estimated Marginal Means of Attitudes toward Immigrants of Southern and Western Origin across Russians, Titulars
(Both Constituting either Majority or Minority), and Others.
Nationalities Papers 9
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
While in the case of titulars the analysis by an ethnic group might potentially give more insights
into these relationships, our data enforce an aggregated approach. This is due to the relatively small
sizes of individual non-Russian ethnic groups in the sample and the fact that most of them are to be
wholly categorized as holding either the majority or minority status. A comparison of attitudes of
members of a particular titular ethnic group depending on whether their group holds a majority or
minority status in their region of residence is plausible only for Tatars. Such an analysis shows that
negative attitudes toward southern immigrants among majority Tatars (i.e., Tatars in Tatarstan,
Table 1. Summary of Posterior Draws from the Hierarchical Structural Equation Model of Negative Attitudes towards
Western and Southern Immigrant Groups, with Correlated Residuals
western groups southern groups
Estimates HPDI (95%) Estimates HPDI (95%)
intercept 3.210 2.877 –3.543* 3.991 3.663 –4.331*
male (female=0, male=1) 0.083 0.048 –0.119* 0.002 0.033 –0.039
age 0.003 0.004 –0.002* 0.003 0.004 –0.002*
years of education 0.006 0.013 –0.001 0.000 0.007 –0.006
log income 0.021 0.051 –0.009 0.025 0.057 –0.006
born abroad (no=0, yes=1) 0.092 0.167 –0.012* 0.075 0.153 –0.005
locality (federal center=0)
(town=1) 0.030 0.251 –0.205 0.052 0.149 –0.258
(urban-type=1) 0.378 0.619 –0.132* 0.485 0.715 –0.271*
(village=1) 0.009 0.235 –0.221 0.073 0.275 –0.128
group status/ethnicity (majority Russians=0)
(majority titulars=1) 0.103 0.301 –0.077 0.351 0.553 –0.145*
(minority Russians=1) 0.126 0.322 –0.073 0.281 0.462 –0.085*
(minority titulars=1) 0.037 0.146 –0.073 0.114 0.242 –0.004
(others=1) 0.211 0.321 –0.107* 0.259 0.369 –0.156*
Random Effects
_
o
2
0.73 0.74
ρ
south/west
0.56
τ
00
0.32
region
0.28
region
0.48
region:household
0.51
region:household
ICC 0.12
region
0.09
region
0.38
region:household
0.38
region:household
observations (region/household/individual) 38/3,808/8,094
Bayes R
2
0.39 0.42
Note. * 95% highest density interval does not contain 0.
10 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
M=3.18, SD =0.91) were lower than among minority Tatars (Tatars outside of Tatarstan, M =
3.56, SD =1.02). The Bayes factor in favor of a difference was substantial, BF
10
=5.05. In the case of
negative attitudes toward western immigrants, the difference between majority Tatars (M =2.94,
SD =0.95) and minority Tatars (M =3.02, SD =1.08) was not supported, BF
10
=0.18 (which
indicates substantial evidence for no difference). These results are thus in line with our main
findings.
Discussion
The study suggests that the relative position of an ethnic in-group in Russia is not equally important
as a predictor of attitudes toward migrant out-groups across different origins. Group position
proved to play a role in predicting attitudes toward migrants originating from the “south”
(encompassing North and South Caucasus, Central Asia, and China, Vietnam, and Korea) but
not toward migrants coming from the “west”(Ukraine and Moldova). Namely, an ethnic in-group’s
majority status is positively (for ethnic Russians) and negatively (for titulars) related to exclusionist
attitudes toward immigrants only in the case of the southern origins. This finding suggests that the
mechanisms underlying attitudes might differ between the two clusters of migrant out-groups.
These results run contrary to the results obtained by Gorodzeisky (2019), who argued that those
who perceive their collective as vulnerable tend to show exclusionary attitudes toward all foreigners
regardless of their origin. This discrepancy may be explained by her focus on attitudes toward
immigration (admitting foreigners at all) rather than attitudes toward immigrants (who may
already be present in the country). It may also be explained by her focus on collective vulnerability
in the socioeconomic sphere, while the relative position of an in-group as expressed by the majority-
minority status may refer to competition over both material and symbolic resources. Besides, as we
argue, social distance with regard to potential migrant neighbors—by which we capture anti-
immigrant attitudes in our paper—is more likely linked to symbolic than economic considerations.
This reassures us that we should seek explanations for the differences in attitudes toward the two
clusters of origins recorded in our study in the cultural rather than the socioeconomic realm.
While we were unable to empirically test the mechanisms behind the encountered associations
due to data limitations, we can discuss possible explanations for differences between the two clusters
of target groups that may be tested in future studies. In line with the differentiated threat approach,
our result may be explained by the source of threat each out-group triggers. Ukrainian and
Moldovan migrants are usually indistinguishable from the national majority group, that is, ethnic
Russians, both in appearance and by virtue of their Russian language skills. Given the cultural
differences between ethnic groups and the “invisible character”of the Ukrainian and Moldovan
migrant minorities, we may assume that migrants of Ukrainian and Moldovan origin do not tend to
trigger a symbolic threat among members of the receiving population. Referring to Minescu and
Poppe (2011), the differing role of group position may be explained by differences in the level of
threat evoked by an out-group and whether it is perceived as encroaching on the area of in-group
privileges. Immigrants from Ukraine and Moldova are generally associated with lower levels of
symbolic threat and less often with encroachment—inter alia, given their decreasing demographic
potential and immigration trends as of 2008–2009 (the time when the survey was conducted). This
would suggest that in-group position plays a role in predicting negative attitudes toward immi-
grants only with reference to out-groups that trigger high levels of symbolic threat and are
associated with encroachment.
The interpretation referring to the source and level of threat seems plausible in the case of ethnic
Russian respondents, for whom immigrants from Ukraine and Moldova presumably indeed pose
no symbolic threat or pose a lower level of threat, contrary to newcomers from the remaining
regions of origin. It seems, however, less relevant as an explanation for members of some
non-Russian ethnic groups, given their cultural closeness to members of some other target groups.
For instance, one would not necessarily expect high levels of symbolic threat evoked by newcomers
Nationalities Papers 11
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
from Central Asia among Tatars, since both groups are predominantly Muslim. This caveat was
actually reflected in our results, since the evidence for the importance of group position in the case
of Ukrainian and Moldovan migrants was stronger (nevertheless, not decisive) for titular than for
ethnic Russian respondents (see figure 1 and figure A1).
An alternative and perhaps more universal explanation relates to the place in the ethnic
hierarchy occupied by target out-groups. Previous research demonstrated that while people show
an inclination to maintain status hierarchy between groups, natives prefer immigrant groups of
higher status to those of lower status (Kustov 2019). Bessudnov (2016), in his study on public
attitudes toward immigrants, concluded that the effect of immigrant concentration (which may be
perceived as a measure of the relative size of an out-group) on attitudes toward immigrants appears
to be stronger for groups positioned lower in the ethnic hierarchy. Our findings suggest that the
effect of relative size of an in-group is also stronger for lower-positioned ethnic out-groups. While
the compatibility of these findings may seem obvious in the context of a typical nation-state, where
the relative size of an in-group directly (inversely) translates into the relative size of a migrant out-
group, it is not as obvious in the case of a multiethnic society, where additional groups other than an
in-group and a migrant out-group are present.
Although cultural differences between an in-group and a specific out-group seem to be not
without importance, perception of immigrants may also be influenced by the group cues in
migration discourse—namely it may depend on the politicization of the presence (or inflow) of a
particular group (Valentino, Brader, and Jardina 2013). Thus, the effects of group position may be
conditional on whether a particular group is framed as threatening (or anxiety producing) by the
media, politicians, and others (which did not happen for Ukrainian and Moldovan migrants in
2008–2009 contrary to immigrants of the remaining origins). It should be stressed here, however,
that the political changes that took place in the last decade—involving the military conflict with
Ukraine and the resultant inflow of Ukrainian citizens and anti-Ukrainian propaganda—might
have influenced the way residents of Russia, in particular ethnic Russians, perceive immigrants
originating from Ukraine.
Unfortunately, the survey analyzed in this paper did not directly measure the level and type of
perceived threat associated with different migrant origins. We are also unable to discern whether
the differences between the two clusters of migrant out-groups follow from the group threat or
group status differentials (nor do we find these explanations incompatible). The inability to check
the above presumptions based on the RLMS-HSE dataset is the main limitation of the study. Future
research would benefit from deepening the existing knowledge on the source and level of threat
evoked by different migrant groups in Russia and their relation to attitudes, particularly their
potential moderating role for the association between the relative position of an ethnic in-group and
attitudes. Another important limitation is the impossibility to reliably test the effect of majority
status at the municipal (PSU) level with the RLMS-HSE data. Should the majority status be
considered at the municipal level, there will only be one group in the sample that may be classified
as majority titular (Kabardins in Zalukokoazhe and Zol’skii raion), and there will not be a sufficient
number of ethnic Russians in a minority position to allow statistical inference and warrant
generalizability of findings.
Despite these limitations, the present study advances our understanding of the relationship
between the relative position of an ethnic in-group and attitudes toward immigrants, and of
attitudes toward different immigrant targets in contemporary Russia. It extends earlier studies
that focused on the role of in-group position in determining anti-immigrant attitudes in Russia by
taking a closer look at these relations at the subnational rather than the national level and
investigating the role of a majority status in a region as a determinant of attitudes toward a number
of different migrant groups. This way it also contributes to the recently growing strand of literature
suggesting that attitudes toward different immigrant groups may not only differ in terms of their
magnitude but also be driven by different mechanisms.
12 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
The theoretical implications discussed above point to the potential limitations of the model
explaining attitudes toward out-groups with a relative position of an in-group. Namely, the results
of the empirical analysis suggest that the source and the level of threat induced by out-groups, in
addition to their position in the ethnic hierarchy, may potentially contribute to the differential
operation of an in-group position between target groups. If there is no symbolic threat or there is a
low level of threat involved, or a target group occupies a high position in the ethnic hierarchy, the
mechanisms operating for the general category of immigrants—yielding a positive effect of majority
status at a regional level on attitudes of ethnic Russians and a negative effect for members of titular
ethnic groups—do not seem to work. This may be due to the fact that the conditions underlying
prejudice according to the original model of Blumer—namely, those related to the sense of
superiority regarding one’s own group, a belief in its distinctiveness from the out-group, and
perceived threat—are not fully met. One may even go a step further and state that, given high levels
of acceptance reported toward Ukrainian and Moldovan migrants, it is not a prejudice that is
explained here.
8
Hence, it should come as no surprise that the group position model is not very
useful in explaining attitudes toward them.
In the light of previous results obtained in relation to attitudes toward the general category of
immigrants (cf. Brunarska 2019), the obtained findings suggest that when respondents in Russia
answer a general question on attitudes toward immigrants, they tend to have immigrants of
southern origin in mind. This is in line with Kustov’s(2019) suggestion that people do not have
groups of higher status in mind when thinking about a general category of immigrants. This also
lends some credence to the presence of “imagined immigration”(Blinder 2015) and may indicate
that the in-group position predicts attitudes only in relation to out-groups that are featured in the
imagined immigration construct. Our findings also resonate with Ruedin’s(2020) argument that
studies based on questions on attitudes toward immigrants in the generic sense likely capture the
right correlates, but they may miss important nuances, such as variability across different origins.
When attempting to generalize the results to other contexts, such as the European one, an
important question occurs: whether the mechanism implied for national minority in-groups works
only for autochthonous (or sedentary) ethnic in-groups or whether it is also capable of explaining
the role of relative in-group size in the case of ethnic in-groups that are mostly composed of
immigrants or of a broader category of people with immigrant backgrounds (e.g., second-
generation migrants). While we are not able to answer this question in the Russian context, where
none of the non-Russian, non-titular ethnic groups hold a majority status at a regional level, the
growing diversity of Western societies may soon provide an appropriate setting to test the
usefulness of the model in relation to relatively newly arrived in-groups in various subnational
locations.
Acknowledgments. Earlier versions of the paper were presented at: the Open Seminar of the Center for Research on Prejudice
at the University of Warsaw, Poland, in June 2018; the Wisconsin Russia Project Young Scholars Conference, held at the
University of Wisconsin-Madison, US, in July 2018; and the Polish Sociological Congress, held in Wrocław, Poland, in
September 2019. We would like to express thanks to the participants of these events as well as to Saja Toruńczyk-Ruiz, Agata
Górny, Ania Janicka, Aneta Piekut and anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments.
Disclosures. None.
Notes
1By seeking the causes of prejudice in different threat profiles, differentiated threat approach
resembles the sociofunctional threat-based approach to prejudice (Cottrell and Neuberg 2005),
which posits that specific threats evoked by different groups entail different emotional reactions.
2The specific ethnic groups named in parentheses were listed in the survey questionnaire.
Inclusion of North Caucasian ethnic groups is symptomatic. It points to the extension of an
anti-immigrant frame to internal migrants in Russia (Markowitz and Peshkova 2016).
Nationalities Papers 13
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
3Both majority titulars in our sample (Tatars, Chuvashes, and Kabardins in their ethnic republics)
and minority titulars (encompassing primarily Tatars outside of Tatarstan, Udmurts, Komis,
Balkars, Mordvinians, Chuvashes outside of Chuvashia, and Bashkirs) are composed of both
Turkic and non-Turkic, and predominantly Muslim and non-Muslim ethnic groups. Hence,
cultural differences do not fully overlap with ethnic groups’demographic concentration (numer-
ical dominance).
4Bayesian methods in contrast to frequentist methods do not rely on asymptotics (e.g., Stegmueller
2013). Therefore, we found the former approach better suited to our complex study design, with a
small number of individuals within each household (N =2.21 on average). While interpretation
of results in Bayesian and frequentist approaches is similar, the Bayesian approach may be
considered more intuitive. Instead of confidence intervals, the Bayesian approach uses credible
intervals or highest posterior density intervals (HPDI). In the case of linear models, the
differences between credible intervals and HPDI are subtle, and thus the choice of one of them
is usually a matter of preference. The Bayesian intervals identify regions where finding a
parameter value is X% probable, whereas confidence interval is a range of values designed to
include the true value of the parameter X times in a series of 100 experiments. In more general
terms, while the frequentist approach treats parameters as fixed and data as random—resulting
from an infinite number of trials, the Bayesian approach treats data as given and parameters as
uncertain, and thus describes parameters with statistical distributions (rather than a single true
value).
5See also figure A1 in the appendix for the results of an analysis for the six immigrant groups
disaggregated.
6Assuming that the mechanism driving anti-immigrant attitudes may differ among nonmigrants
and people with a migration background, we additionally run the analysis for a subsample of
nonmigrants (N =7,206, instead of N =8,094). While the values of contrast parameters were
smaller, the overall pattern and thus the substantive conclusions remained the same. Among
ethnic Russians, those holding majority status had more negative attitudes toward southern
immigrants than ethnic Russians in minority position in their regions of residence, ψ=0.14, 95%
HPDI [0.02, 0.26]. Among titulars, in turn, those holding majority status within a region had on
average less negative attitudes toward southern immigrant groups than titulars in minority
position in their regions of residence, ψ=-0.13, 95% HPDI [-0.24, -0.01]. In the case of attitudes
toward western immigrants, such pattern of results was not observed. Among ethnic Russians
there was no credible difference between those holding majority vs. minority status, ψ=0.04, 95%
HPDI [-0.06, 0.14], and similarly among titulars no credible difference was observed, ψ=-0.04,
95% HPDI [-0.13, 0.06].
7To check whether it is indeed the majority and not the titular status within a region that matters
for anti-immigrant attitudes, we recalculated the model for a dominant administrative status
within a region instead of a majority status within a region. We call it a “dominant administrative
status within a region”instead of “titular status within a region”because we also test its effect for
ethnic Russians (in their case a dominant administrative status means that no ethnic group holds
a titular status in their region of residence; i.e., they reside in a territorially defined federal subject).
In the case of both targets of attitudes—southern and western immigrant groups—there was no
credible interaction of ethnic group type (Russians vs. titulars) and the dominant administrative
status within a region, for southern groups ψ=-0.03, 95% HPDI [-0.10, 0.02] and for western
groups ψ=-0.03, 95% HPDI [-0.09, 0.03]. Furthermore, in the case of both target groups, there
were also no main effects of dominant administrative status within a region, for southern
immigrants ψ=0.08, 95% HPDI [-0.03, 0.20], and for western immigrants ψ=0.01, 95% HPDI
[-0.10, 0.12]. Hence, we conclude that it is the majority rather than the dominant administrative
status within a region that is a meaningful predictor of attitudes toward immigrants of southern
origin.
14 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
8Cf. Raijman, Hochman, and Davidov (2021), who found a negative effect of perceived threat on
opposition to Jewish immigrants in Israel, motivating it with the argument that they are seen as an
asset rather than as a threat.
References
Alexseev, Mikhail. 2010. “Majority and Minority Xenophobia in Russia: The Importance of Being Titulars.”Post-Soviet Affairs
26 (2): 89–120.
Alexseev, Mikhail. 2011. “Societal Security, the Security Dilemma, and Extreme Anti-Migrant Hostility in Russia.”Journal of
Peace Research 48 (4): 509–523.
Bahry, Donna. 2016. “Opposition to Immigration, Economic Insecurity and Individual Values: Evidence from Russia.”Europe-
Asia Studies 68 (5): 893–916.
Bergh, Robin, and Nazar Akrami. 2016. “Generalized Prejudice: Old Wisdom and New Perspectives.”In The Cambridge
Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice, edited by Chris Sibley and Fiona Barlow, 438–460. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Bessudnov, Alexey. 2016. “Ethnic Hierarchy and Public Attitudes towards Immigrants in Russia.”European Sociological Review
32 (5): 1–14.
Bessudnov, Alexey, and Andrey Shcherbak. 2020. “Ethnic Discrimination in Multi-Ethnic Societies: Evidence from Russia.”
European Sociological Review 36 (1): 104–120.
Blinder, Scott. 2015. “Imagined Immigration: The Impact of Different Meanings of ‘Immigrants’in Public Opinion and Policy
Debates in Britain.”Political Studies 63 (1): 80–100.
Bloom, Pazit Ben-Nun, Gizem Arikan, and Gallya Lahav. 2015. “The Effect of Perceived Cultural and Material Threats on
Ethnic Preferences in Immigration Attitudes.”Ethnic and Racial Studies 38 (10): 1760–1778.
Blumer, Herbert. 1958. “Race Prejudice as a Sense of Group Position.”Pacific Sociological Review 1 (1): 3–7.
Bobo, Lawrence, and Vincent Hutchings. 1996. “Perceptions of Racial Group Competition: Extending Blumer’s Theory of
Group Position to a Multiracial Social Context.”American Sociological Review 61 (6): 951–972.
Bogardus, Emory. 1925. “Measuring Social Distance.”Journal of Applied Sociology 9:299–308.
Brader, Ted, Nicholas Valentino, and Elizabeth Suhay. 2008. “What Triggers Public Opposition to Immigration? Anxiety,
Group Cues, and Immigration Threat.”American Journal of Political Science 52 (4): 959–978.
Brunarska, Zuzanna. 2019. “Anti-Immigrant Attitudes in Russia: The Group Position Model Reconsidered.”Europe-Asia
Studies 71 (9): 1508–1531.
Bürkner, Paul-Christian. 2017. “Brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan.”Journal of Statistical Software
80 (1): 1–28.
Butkovich Kraus,Nicole, and Yoshiko Herrera. 2010. “Xenophobia and Nationalism in Russia: A Multilevel Analysis of
Xenophobia and Its Relationship to Nationalism.”Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science
Association, Washington, DC, September 2–5.
Carle, Adam. 2009. “Fitting Multilevel Models in Complex Survey Data with Design Weights: Recommendations.”BMC
Medical Research Methodology 9 (49): 1–13.
Cottrell, Catherine, and Steven Neuberg. 2005. “Different Emotional Reactions to Different Groups: A Sociofunctional Threat-
Based Approach to ‘Prejudice.’” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88 (5): 770–789.
Enders, Craig. 2010. Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
Ford, Robert. 2011. “Acceptable and Unacceptable Immigrants: How Opposition to Immigration in Britain Is Affected by
Migrants’Region of Origin.”Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 37 (7): 1017–1037.
Ford, Robert, and Jonathan Mellon. 2020. “The Skills Premium and the Ethnic Premium: A Cross-National Experiment on
European Attitudes to Immigrants.”Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 46 (3): 512–532.
Gabry, Jonah, Daniel Simpson, Aki Vehtari, Michael Betancourt, and Andrew Gelman. 2019. “Visualization in Bayesian
Workflow.”Journal of the Royal Statistical Society A 182 (2): 389–402.
Gelman, Andrew, Aleks Jakulin, Maria Grazia Pittau, and Yu-Sung Su. 2008. “A Weakly Informative Default Prior Distribution
for Logistic and Other Regression Models.”Annals of Applied Statistics 2 (4): 1360–1383.
Gerber, Theodore P. 2014. “Beyond Putin? Nationalism and Xenophobia in Russian Public Opinion.”The Washington
Quarterly 37 (3): 113–134.
Gerber, Theodore P., and Jane Zavisca. 2020. “Experiences in Russia of Kyrgyz and Ukrainian Labor Migrants: Ethnic
Hierarchies, Geopolitical Remittances, and the Relevance of Migration Theory.”Post-Soviet Affairs 36 (1): 61–82.
Gorodzeisky, Anastasia. 2019. “Opposition to Immigration in Contemporary Russia.”Post-Soviet Affairs 35 (3): 205–222.
Gorodzeisky, Anastasia, and Anya Glikman. 2018. “Two Peoples –Two Stories: Anti-Immigrant Attitudes in Post-Socialist
Russia.”Social Problems 65 (4): 543–563.
Gorodzeisky, Anastasia, Anya Glikman, and Dina Maskileyson. 2015. “The Nature of Anti-Immigrant Sentiment in Post-
Socialist Russia.”Post-Soviet Affairs 31 (2): 115–135.
Gorodzeisky, Anastasia, and Moshe Semyonov. 2019. “Unwelcome Immigrants: Sources of Opposition to Different Immigrant
Groups among Europeans.”Frontiers in Sociology 4 (24): 1–10.
Nationalities Papers 15
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Grigor’ev, Dmitrii. 2017. “Diskriminatsiia migrantov v sotsioekonomicheskoi sfere: Rol’mezhgruppovykh ustanovok prini-
maiushchego naseleniia.”Sotsial’naia psikhologiia i obshchestvo 8 (3): 63–84.
Grigoryan, Lusine. 2016. “National Identity and Anti-Immigrant Attitudes: The Case of Russia.”In Dynamics of National
Identity: Media and Societal Factors of What We Are, edited by Jürgen Grimm, Leonie Huddy, Peter Schmidt, and Josef
Seethaler, 206–228. New York: Routledge.
Gudkov, Lev. 2005. “Smeshchennaia agressiia: Otnoshenie Rossiian k migrantam.”Vestnik obshchestvennogo mneniia 6 (80):
60–77.
Hagendoorn, Louk. 1995. “Intergroup Biases in Multiple Group Systems: The Perception of Ethnic Hierarchies.”European
Review of Social Psychology 6 (1): 199–228.
Hagendoorn, Louk, Rian Drogendijk, Sergey Tumanov, and Joseph Hraba. 1998. “Inter-Ethnic Preferences and Ethnic
Hierarchies in the Former Soviet Union.”International Journal of Intercultural Relations 22 (4): 483–503.
Hainmueller, Jens, and Daniel Hopkins. 2015. “The Hidden American Immigration Consensus: A Conjoint Analysis of
Attitudes toward Immigrants.”American Journal of Political Science 59 (3): 529–548.
Harell, Allison, Stuart Soroka, Shanto Iyengar, and Nicholas Valentino. 2012. “The Impact of Economic and Cultural Cues on
Support for Immigration in Canada and the United States.”Canadian Journal of Political Science / Revue canadienne de
science politique 45 (3): 499–530.
Hellwig, Timothy, and Abdulkader Sinno. 2017. “Different Groups, Different Threats: Public Attitudes towards Immigrants.”
Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 43 (3): 339–358.
Herrera, Yoshiko, and Nicole Butkovich Kraus. 2016. “Pride versus Prejudice Ethnicity, National Identity, and Xenophobia in
Russia.”Comparative Politics 48 (3): 293–315.
Hraba, Joseph, Carolyn S. Dunham, Sergey Tumanov, and Louk Hagendoorn. 1997. “Prejudice in the Former Soviet Union.”
Ethnic and Racial Studies 20 (3): 613–627.
Jeffreys, Harold. 1961. The Theory of Probability. 3rd ed. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Konitzer, Tobias, Shanto Iyengar, Nicholas Valentino, Stuart Soroka, and Raymond Duch. 2019. “Ethnocentrism versus Group-
Specific Stereotyping in Immigration Opinion: Cross-National Evidence on the Distinctiveness of Immigrant Groups.”
Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 45 (7): 1051–1074.
Kustov, Alexander. 2019. “Is There a Backlash Against Immigration from Richer Countries? International Hierarchy and the
Limits of Group Threat.”Political Psychology 40 (5): 973–1000.
Laruelle, Marlène. 2010. “The Ideological Shift on the Russian Radical Right.”Problems of Post-Communism 57 (6): 19–31.
Levada. 2018. Obshchestvennoe mnenie –2017. Moscow: Yuri Levada Analytical Center.
Markowitz, Lawrence P., and Vera Peshkova. 2016. “Anti-Immigrant Mobilization in Russia’s Regions: Local Movements and
Framing Processes.”Post-Soviet Affairs 32 (3): 272–298.
Martinovic, Borja, and Maykel Verkuyten. 2013. “‘We Were Here First, so We Determine the Rules of the Game’: Autochthony
and Prejudice towards out-Groups.”European Journal of Social Psychology 43 (7): 637–647.
Meuleman, Bart, Koen Abts, Koen Slootmaeckers, and Cecil Meeusen. 2018. “Differentiated Threat and the Genesis of
Prejudice: Group-Specific Antecedents of Homonegativity, Islamophobia, Anti-Semitism, and Anti-Immigrant Attitudes.”
Social Problems 66: 222–244.
Minescu, Anca, and Edwin Poppe. 2011. “Intergroup Conflict in Russia.”Social Psychology Quarterly 74 (2): 166–191.
Pipiia, Karina. 2017. “Struktura obraza migranta v rossiiskom obshchestvennom mnenii.”Vestnik obshchestvennogo mneniia
1–2 (124): 130–140.
Popova, Irina, and Valentina Osipova. 2013. “Otnoshenie prinimaiushchei storony k migrantam.”Vestnik obshchestvennogo
mneniia 1 (114): 81–88.
Pratto, Felicia, Jim Sidanius, Lisa M. Stallworth, and Bertram F. Malle. 1994. “Social Dominance Orientation: A Personality
Variable Predicting Social and Political Attitudes.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 (4): 741–763.
Raijman, Rebeca, Oshrat Hochman, and Eldad Davidov. 2021. “Ethnic Majority Attitudes toward Jewish and Non-Jewish
Migrants in Israel: The Role of Perceptions of Threat, Collective Vulnerability, and Human Values.”Journal of Immigrant &
Refugee Studies. Published online ahead of print February 22, 2021. doi: 10.1080/15562948.2021.1889107.
RLMS-HSE. 2008. Russia Longitudinal Monitoring Survey of the Higher School of Economics. National Research University
Higher School of Economics; ZAO Demoscope; Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill;
Institute of Sociology Russian Academy of Sciences. www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/rlms-hse,www.hse.ru/org/hse/rlms.
(Accessed December 1, 2016.)
Rosstat. 2013. Russian 2010 Population Census Data. www.gks.ru/free_doc/new_site/perepis2010/croc/perepis_itogi1612.htm.
(Accessed February 2, 2017.)
Ruedin, Didier. 2020. “Do We Need Multiple Questions to Capture Feeling Threatened by Immigrants?”Political Research
Exchange 2 (1).
Schoon, Eric, and Kathryn Freeman Anderson. 2017. “Rethinking the Boundaries: Competitive Threat and the Asymmetric
Salience of Race/Ethnicity in Attitudes toward Immigrants.”Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World 3:1–14.
Stegmueller, Daniel. 2013. “How Many Countries for Multilevel Modeling? A Comparison of Frequentist and Bayesian
Approaches.”American Journal of Political Science 57 (3): 748–761.
Stephan, Walter, Oscar Ybarra, and Kimberly Rios Morrison. 2009. “Intergroup Threat Theory.”In Handbook of Prejudice,
Stereotyping, and Discrimination, edited by Todd Nelson, 43–59. New York: Psychology Press.
16 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Tartakovsky, Eugene, and Sophie Walsh. 2020. “Are Some Immigrants More Equal than Others? Applying a Threat-Benefit
Model to Understanding the Appraisal of Different Immigrant Groups by the Local Population.”Journal of Ethnic and
Migration Studies 46 (19): 3955–3973.
Timberlake, Jeffrey, Junia Howell, Amy Baumann Grau, and Rhys Williams. 2015. “Who “They”Are Matters: Immigrant
Stereotypes and Assessments of the Impact of Immigration.”Sociological Quarterly 56 (2): 267–299.
Turper, Sedef, Shanto Iyengar, Kees Aarts, and Minna van Gerven. 2015. “Who Is Less Welcome? The Impact of Individuating
Cues on Attitudes towards Immigrants.”Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 41 (2): 239–259.
Valentino, Nicholas, Ted Brader, and Ashley Jardina. 2013. “Immigration Opposition Among U.S. Whites: General Ethno-
centrism or Media Priming of Attitudes About Latinos?”Political Psychology 34 (2): 149–166.
van Buuren, Stef, and Karin Groothuis-Oudshoorn. 2011. “Mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R.”Journal
of Statistical Software 45 (3): 1–67.
Velasco González, Karina, Maykel Verkuyten, Jeroen Weesie, and Edwin Poppe. 2008. “Prejudice towards Muslims in the
Netherlands: Testing Integrated Threat Theory.”British Journal of Social Psychology 47 (4): 667–685.
World Data on Education 2010/11. 2012. Geneva: United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization,
International Bureau of Education.
Zick, Andreas, Carina Wolf, Beate Küpper, Eldad Davidov, Peter Schmidt, and Wilhelm Heitmeyer. 2008. “The Syndrome of
Group-Focused Enmity: The Interrelation of Prejudices Tested with Multiple Cross-Sectional and Panel Data.”Journal of
Social Issues 64 (2): 363–383.
Appendix
Table A1. Descriptive Statistics, Weighted Data
Variable Range M (SD) or % % missing
attitudes (Ukraine) 1–5 2.75 (1.00) 3.60
attitudes (Moldova) 1–5 2.89 (0.98) 4.58
attitudes (North Caucasus) 1–5 3.54 (1.05) 4.20
attitudes (South Caucasus) 1–5 3.53 (1.05) 4.16
attitudes (Central Asia) 1–5 3.52 (1.06) 4.45
attitudes (China, Vietnam, Korea) 1–5 3.65 (1.06) 5.71
group status/ethnicity –majority Russians 0/1 84% 6.59
group status/ethnicity –majority titulars 0/1 5% 6.59
group status/ethnicity –minority Russians 0/1 3% 6.59
group status/ethnicity –minority titulars 0/1 4% 6.59
group status/ethnicity –others 0/1 4% 6.59
age 18–96 45.7 (18.3) 0
gender (female=0, male=1) 0/1 44% 0
years of education* 0–25 12.5 (3.11) 0.16
born abroad (no=0, yes=1) 0/1 8% 3.1
income 0–574,000 9,185 (12,671) 6.04
locality –town 0/1 26% 0
locality –urban type settlement 0/1 6% 0
locality –village 0/1 26% 0
*Calculated from education level referring to World Data on Education 2010/11 (2012).
Source: calculations based on RLMS-HSE (2008)
Nationalities Papers 17
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.
Cite this article: Brunarska, Z. and Soral, W. 2022. Does Origin Matter? Ethnic Group Position and Attitudes Toward
Immigrants: The Case of Russia. Nationalities Papers:1–18, doi:10.1017/nps.2021.71
Figure A1. Average Attitudes toward Immigrants of the Six Origins among Ethnic Russians, Titulars, or Other Ethnic Groups,
Qualified by Majority versus Minority Status. Estimated Means are Adjusted for Covariates: Gender, Age, Years of Education,
Income, Migration Status, Locality Type, and Clustering.
18 Zuzanna Brunarska and Wiktor Soral
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. 10 Jan 2022 at 10:26:39, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use.