Content uploaded by Magdalena S Svensson
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Magdalena S Svensson on Jan 10, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 202132
Lynch Alfaro, J. W., Silva, J. S. Jr. and Rylands, A. B. 2012.
How dierent are Robust and Gracile Capuchin Mon-
keys? An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus.
Am. J. Primatol. 74: 273–286.
Milano, M. Z. and Monteiro-Filho, E. L. A. 2009.Preda-
tion on small mammals by Capuchin Monkeys, Cebus
cay. Neotrop. Primates. 16(2): 78–80.
Resende, B. D., Greco, V. L. G., Ottoni, E. B. and Izar,
P. 2003. Some observations on the predation of small
mammals by Tued Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
Neotrop. Primates 11(2): 103–104.
Robinson, S. K. 1985. Coloniality In e Yellow Rumped
Cacique as a defense against nest predators. Auk. 102:
506–519.
Robinson, S. K. 1988. Foraging ecology and host relation-
ships of giant Cowbirds in southeastern Peru. Wilson
Bull. 100:224–235.
Rose, L. 1997. Vertebrate predation and food-sharing in
Cebus and Pan. Int. J. Primatol. 18: 727–765.
Sabbatini, G., Stammati, M, Tavares, M. C. H. and
Visalberghi, E. 2008. Behavioral exibility of a group
of Bearded Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in
the National Park of Brasília (Brazil): consequences of
cohabitation with visitors. Braz. J. Biol. 68(4): 685–693.
Siemers, B. M. 2000. Seasonal variation in food Resource
and forest strata use by Brown Capuchin Monkeys
(Cebus apella) in a disturbed forest fragment. Folia Pri-
matol. 71:181–184.
Torralvo, K., Rabelo, R. M., Andrade, A. and Botero-
Arias, R. 2017.Tool use by amazonian Capuchin Mon-
keys during predation on caiman nests in a high-pro-
ductivity forest. Primates 58(2): 279–283.
Watts, D. P. 2020. Meat eating by nonhuman primates: A
review and synthesis. J. Hum. Evol. 149: 102882.
PRELIMINARY INVESTIGATION INTO THE
DIFFERENCES IN VOCALISATIONS BETWEEN
WILD SAKI MONKEYS (PITHECIA SPP.)
Jen Muir
Adrian A. Barnett
Magdalena S. Svensson
Introduction
Information on primate vocalisations can be applied in
several ways, including: improving captive welfare, as a
census tool for cryptic species, or to investigate the im-
pacts of anthropogenic disturbance on species’ behaviour
(Delgado and van Shaik, 2000; Konrad and Geissman,
2006; Jacobsen et al., 2010). Vocalisations can be used
as a taxonomic tool, and structural dierences between
calls have been used to compare a wide variety of taxa,
including species of gibbon (Hylobatidae spp., Ruppell,
2010), marmosets (Callithrix spp., Mendes et al., 2009),
owls (Strigidae spp., Flint et al., 2015), wolves (Canis spp.,
Kershenbaum et al., 2016), and galagos (Galagidae spp.,
Svensson et al., 2017). Additionally, dierences in voca-
lisations across taxonomic groups can be used to help de-
termine genetic distances between species or investigate
why vocal behaviours evolved (Blumstein and Armitage,
1998; Ord and Garcia-Porta, 2012).
One primate group that recently has been revised ta-
xonomically is the genus Pithecia, the saki monkeys, in
which ve species were previously described: P. mona-
chus, P. aequatorialis, P. irrorata, P. albicans, and P. p i t he -
cia (Hershkovitz, 1987). Aer a review of morphological
data, the group was rearranged into 16 species, including
three subspecies elevated to full species and ve newly
described species (Marsh, 2014). It has been suggested
that taxonomic research should utilize several disciplines
in combination so as to decrease the risk of inaccurate
conclusions (Schlick-Steiner et al., 2010). In the case of
the sakis, the use of purely morphological features opens
the opportunity for the taxonomic conclusions to be tes-
ted by evidence based on genetic structure, behaviour,
ecology, or life history, among others.
In the current study, vocalisations produced by wild
golden-faced sakis, Pithecia chrysocephala, white-faced
sakis, P. pithecia, and Equatorial sakis, P. aequatorialis,
were compared to study how calls might dier between
closely related species (P. pithecia and P. chrysocephala),
and whether any features were conserved across more
evolutionarily separated species (P. chrysocephala and P.
pithecia compared to P. aequatorialis). Before the reclas-
sication of Pithecia by Marsh (2014), P. chrysocephala
was considered a subspecies of P. pithecia, and so these
taxa were expected to display similar vocalisations.
Methods
Study Species
Pithecia chrysocephala, P. pithecia, and P. aequatorialis all
occupy forest habitats, including Amazonian várzea, iga-
pó, and terra rme forests. However, the species occupy
dierent geographical areas (Fig. 1), with P. chrysocepha-
la occurring within the Brazilian states of Amazonas,
Pará, and Roraima, and P. pithecia found in the states of
Pará and Amapá, as well as in the countries of Venezuela,
Guyana, French Guiana and Suriname (Marsh, 2014). Pi-
thecia aequatorialis is present only in central Peru (Marsh
and Heymann, 2018).
Data Organisation and Analysis
We collected vocalisation data for Pithecia chryso-
cephala in June-August 2018 in Manaus, Brazil (-3.083,
59.983) (for methodology and vocal repertoire see Muir
et al., 2019). We then gathered vocalisation data on two
other Pithecia species from previous published studies,
which we re-analysed and compared across species. For
P. pithecia, data from Henline’s (2007) study conducted
in Isla Redonda, Venezuela (7.766, – 62.883) was used,
and P. aequatorialis data came from Keiren’s (2012)
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 2021 33
study in the Tahuayo River Amazon Research Center,
Peru (-4.383, – 73.25). While Henline’s (2007) study was
conducted before the reclassication of Pithecia species,
it is clear that this study did not include vocalisation
from P. chrysocephala as the in-situ part of the study was
conducted in Venezuela, far from the natural range of
this species, but within that P. pithecia. Our study and
the two previous studies each report between 5 and 12
distinct vocalisations per species (Table 1). One limita-
tion of these studies is that individual sakis could not be
distinguished during recordings and so pseudo-replica-
tion is a potential issue as it is unknown how much any
given individual contributed to the sample.
Table 1. e complete adult vocal repertoires of P. chrysocephala,
P. pithecia, and P. aequatorialis as recorded by Muir et al. (2019),
Henline (2007), and Keiran (2012). P. aequatorialis calls were not
put into groups within their study and so have been grouped ac-
cording to their similarity to the others based on their description
and/or behavioural context.
Group Pithecia
chrysocephala
Pithecia
pithecia
Pithecia aequatorialis
Whistles Whistle
(n = 778)
Pee (n = 20) Bird (n = 23)
See (n = 6) Chits (n = 61)
Chew (n=8) Chits/XX (n = 10)
Chucks Chuck
(n = 212)
Chuck
(n = 16)
Bark (n = 45)
Chits with Bark
(n = 6)
Churk
(n = 1)
Croak (n = 2)
Yip (n = 1)
Trills Trill (n = 253) Cheeyeep
(n = 29)
Trill (n = 5)
Seeyeep
(n = 5)
Peeyeep
(n = 2)
Warbled
Trill (n = 8)
Purrs Soft Growl
(n = 4)
Soft Growl
(n = 3)
Growl (n = 17)
Moans and
Alarms
roat Rattle
(n = 96)
roat
Rattle
(n = 9)
Scream (n = 3)
Z-trill
(n = 3)
Unknown Grunt (n = 1)
Results
From spectrograms, Pithecia chrysocephala and P. p i t h e -
cia display similar chuck calls, while P. chrysocephala and
P. aequatorialis show similar throat rattle/scream calls, as
well as juvenile cry/juvenile yip calls (Fig. 2, Table 2). Ad-
ditionally, these calls are similarly used in alarm, antago-
nistic, and parent-ospring contact, respectively.
One-sample t-tests suggested that Pithecia chrysocephala
and P. pithecia dier signicantly in the duration of their
chuck calls, t(217) = 2.11, p =.036, and in fundamental fre-
quency, t(217) = 15.191, p < .001. is was also suggested
for the duration, t(95) = 21.575, p < .001, and fundamen-
tal frequency, t(95) = 3.445, p = .001, of the throat rattle
call. Comparisons between the P. chrysocephala throat
rattle and P. aequatorialis scream were also conducted,
Figure 1. Location of study site and distributions of the studied
Pithecia species. Map created using spatial data obtained from
IUCN (Marsh et al. 2018a; Marsh et al. 2018b; Marsh and Hey-
mann 2018).
In our study calls were initially compared visually by
spectrograms from each paper, with similar calls then
being statistically compared in terms of acoustic mea-
surements. Similarity in terms of the behavioural con-
text of the calls was also considered. Call measurements
of duration and fundamental frequency were taken us-
ing the speech analysis program Praat (Boersma, 2001).
One sample t-tests were performed using SPSS v.25 to
compare call features between species as the raw data
was not available for P. pithecia and P. aequatorialis.
When making comparisons, the two calls within Hen-
line’s (2007) chucks group were averaged to allow for
a comparison with the graded chuck call found in P.
hrysocephala (Table 1). e whistle and trill groups were
not compared in this manner as the calls within them
appeared to dier from those of P. chrysocephala when
shown as spectrograms. e so growl group was also
not compared, due to a small sample size for both P.
pithecia and P. chrysocephala. From Keiran’s (2012) study
of P. aequatorialis, only the scream and juvenile yip calls
were similar enough on spectrograms to be compared
(to the P. chrysocephala throat rattle and juvenile cry,
respectively).
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 202134
with results suggesting that they dier signicantly in
fundamental frequency, t(95) = 55.401, p < .001, but not
in duration, t(95) = .671, p = .504. e P. chrysocephala
juvenile cry and P. aequatorialis juvenile yip call diered
signicantly in duration, t(107) = 15.619, p < .001and
fundamental frequency, t(107) = 35.503, p < .001.
Figure 2. Spectrograms of the vocal repertoires of Pithecia chrysocephala, P. pithecia, and P.
aequatorialis, with visually similar calls. Calls include: P. pithecia chuck (a), P. chrysocephala
chuck (b), P. chrysocephala throat rattle (c), P. aequatorialis scream (d), P. chrysocephala juvenile
cry (e) and P. aequatorialis juvenile yip (f).
Table 2. Structural properties of structurally and contextually similar calls between Pithecia chry-
socephala, P. pithecia, and P. aequatorialis.
Species Compared Calls Compared Mean Duration in
Seconds
Mean Fundamental
Frequency in Hz
P. chrysocephala
P. pithecia
Chuck
Chuck
0.352
0.335
6307.10
6507.15
P. chrysocephala
P. aequatorialis
roat rattle
Scream
1.537
1.554
3029.84
784.38
P. chrysocephala
P. aequatorialis
Juvenile cry
Juvenile yip
0.419
0.158
6486.16
766.73
Discussion
At a glance, structural properties between Pithecia pithe-
cia calls in Henline (2007) and those of the current study
of P. chrysocephala are very similar, with chucks being the
shortest calls, throat rattles the longest, chucks and trills
at the highest frequencies, and so growls at the lowest.
However, few calls between these species were sucient-
ly similar on spectrograms to be compared statistically.
All visually similar calls among P. chrysocephala, P. pi t he -
cia and P. aequatorialis were found to have dierences in
their structure in terms of their fundamental frequency.
ese preliminary ndings add to the morphological evi-
dence presented by Marsh (2014) on the distinctness of
Pithecia taxa, supporting the classication of P. chryso-
cephala as a separate species from P. pithecia, rather than
a subspecies. However, as only one population of each
species was studied, the possibility of variations within
populations of a single evolutionary unit must be consi-
dered before any more substantial claims can be made.
e calls of Pithecia aequatorialis appeared notably less
similar to P. chrysocephala than P. pithecia, potentially
reecting the greater evolutionary distance between
these species. Phylogenetic dierences in call struc-
ture across a genus have been previously observed in
a number of primate taxa, including sportive lemurs,
Lepilemur spp., in which species that were less closely
related to each other had more distinctly dierent calls
(Mendez-Cárdenas et al., 2008). Similar results have
been obtained with titi monkeys, Callicebinae sp., (Ar-
det et al., 2018). It is also possible that these calls are
conserved across the genus, similar to how the startle
call is conserved across mouse lemurs, Microcebus spp.
(Zimmerman, 2012).
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 2021 35
Future studies could usefully investigate dierences
among Pithecia populations and examine the inuence
of habitat dierences on vocalisations. It would also be
interesting to investigate Pithecia vocal behaviour in the
context of all pitheciids, as several similarities are evi-
dent throughout this group (Bezerra et al., 2017). For
example, the tcho calls, whistles, and loud screams of gol-
den-backed uacaris, Cacajao ouakar y, are similar to the
P. chrysocephala chucks, whistles, and throat rattles res-
pectively (Bezerra et al., 2010). Likewise, juvenile B-calls
(an alarm call) of black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus
nigrifrons, are similar to juvenile P. chrysocephala peeps,
as are the juvenile purrs of red-bellied titi monkeys, Plec-
turocebus moloch, to juvenile golden faced saki trews
(Moynihan, 1966; Berthet et al., 2018). Additionally, the
chucks and intergroup call/throat rattle of Plecturocebus
moloch and Pithecia chrysocephala resemble each other
(Moynihan, 1966; Caselli et al., 2014). More wide-sca-
le comparisons across all pitheciid species could also be
conducted to examine the social and ecological eects
on the evolution of their communication, and why such
calls appear to be similar. However, a full phylogenetic
comparison of Pithecia vocalisations is currently limited
by a lack of data, and so further studies of Pithecia and
pitheciid species are recommended. e development of
a pitheciid vocalisation database as suggested by Bezerra
et al. (2017) would be ideal for this purpose.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Makiko Take and Tianara So-
broza for their assistance in Brazil. We would also like to
thank Instituto National de Pesquisas da Amazonia for
providing the opportunity to carry out this project, and
for Santander for providing a Student Project Grant.
Jen Muir, School of Social Sciences, Oxford Brookes
University, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK, E-mail: <jenmuir93@
gmail.com>, Adrian A. Barnett, Amazon Mammal Re-
search Group, Biodiversity Studies, Instituto National de
Pesquisas da Amazonia, Manaus, Brazil, and Magdalena
S. Svensson, School of Social Sciences, Oxford Brookes
University, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK.
References
Adret, P., Dingess, K. A., Caselli, C. B., Vermeer, J., Mar-
tínez, J., Luna Amancio, J. C., ... & Di Fiore, A. 2018.
Duetting patterns of titi monkeys (Primates, Pithe-
ciidae: Callicebinae) and relationships with phylo-
geny.Animals 8: 178.
Berthet, M., Neumann, C., Mesbahi, G., Cäsar, C., and Zu-
berbühler, K. 2018. Contextual encoding in titi monkey
alarm call sequences. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72: 8.
Bezerra, B. M., Souto, A. S., and Jones, G. 2010. Vocal
repertoire of golden-backed uakaris (Cacajao melano-
cephalus): call structure and context. Int. J. Primatol. 31:
759–778.
Bezerra, B., Casar, C., Jerusalinsky, L., Barnett, A., Bas-
tos, M., Souto, A., and Jones, G. 2017. Pitheciid vocal
communication: what can we say about what they are
saying? Ethnobiol. Cons. 6: 1–23.
Blumstein, D. T., & Armitage, K. B. 1998. Why do yellow-
-bellied marmots call? Anim. Behav. 56: 1053–1055.
Boersma, P. 2001. Praat, a system for doing phonetics by
computer. Glot Int. 5: 341–345.
Caselli, C. B., Mennill, D. J., Bicca‐Marques, J. C., and
Setz, E. Z. 2014. Vocal behaviour of black‐fronted titi
monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons): Acoustic properties
and behavioral contexts of loud calls. Am. J. Primatol.
76: 788–800.
Delgado Jr., R. A., and Van Schaik, C. P. 2000. e beha-
vioural ecology and conservation of the orangutan
(Pongo pygmaeus): a tale of two islands. Evo. Anthropol.
9: 201–218.
Flint, P. Whaley, D., Kirwan, G. M., Charalambides, M.,
Schweizer, M. and Wink, M. 2015. Reprising the taxo-
nomy of Cyprus scops owl Otus (scops) cyprius, a ne-
glected island endemic. Zootaxa 4040: 301–316.
Henline, W. T. 2007. Vocal repertoire of white-faced sakis
(Pithecia pithecia). Master’s thesis, Eastern Kentucky
University, USA.
Hershkovitz, P. 1987. e taxonomy of South American
sakis, genus Pithecia (Cebidae, Platyrrhini): a prelimi-
nary report and critical review with the description of
a new species and a new subspecies. Am. J. Primatol.
12: 387–468.
Jacobsen, K. R., Mikkelsen, L. F., and Hau, J. 2010. e
eect of environmental enrichment on the behaviour
of captive tued capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Lab
Anim. 39: 269.
Kershenbaum, A., Root-Gutteridge, H., Habib, B., Koler-
-Matznick, J., Mitchell, B., Palacios, V., and Waller, S.
2016. Disentangling canid howls across multiple spe-
cies and subspecies: Structure in a complex communi-
cation channel. Behav. Processes. 124: 149–157.
Kieran, T. J. 2012. Identity and vocal repertoire of a Pe-
ruvian saki monkey (Pithecia aequatorialis). Master’s
thesis, Winthrop University, USA.
Konrad, R., and Geissmann, T. 2006. Vocal diversity and
taxonomy of Nomascus in Cambodia. Int. J. Primatol.
27: 713.
Marsh, L. K. 2014. A taxonomic revision of the saki
monkeys, Pithecia (Desmarest, 1804). Neotrop. Prima-
tes. 21: 1–165.
Marsh, L. K., Boubli, J., Mittermeier, R. A., Rohe, F.,
Urbani, B. & de Azevedo, R. 2018a. Pithecia pithecia.
e IUCN Red List of reatened Species 2018: e.
T43942A17991915. Website: http://dx.doi.org/10.2305/
IUCN.UK.2018-2.RLTS.T43942A17991915.en. Acces-
sed 20 January 2021.
Marsh, L. K., Mittermeier, R.A., Rohe, F. & de Azevedo,
R.2018b.Pithecia chrysocephala.e IUCN Red List of
reatened Species 2018: e.T43943A17991938.Websi-
te: http://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2018-2.RLTS.
T43943A17991938.en.Accessed 20 January 2021.
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 202136
Marsh, L. K. and Heymann, E. W. 2018. Pithecia aequato-
rialis. e IUCN Red List of reatened Species 2018:
e.T17402A17971831. Website: https://www.iucnre-
dlist.org/species/17402/17971831. Accessed 21 January
2021.
Mendes, S. L., Vielliard, J. M. E., & De Marco, P. 2009. e
vocal identity of the Callithrix species (Primates, Calli-
trichidae). In:e Smallest Anthropoids, S. M. Ford, L.
M. Porter and L. C. Davis (eds.),pp. 63–84. Springer,
Boston, MA.
Méndez‐Cárdenas, M., Randrianambinina, B., Rabesan-
dratana, A., Rasoloharijaona, S., and Zimmermann,
E. 2008. Geographic variation in loud calls of sportive
lemurs (Lepilemur ssp.) and their implications for con-
servation. Am. J. Primatol. 70: 828-838.
Moynihan, M. 1966. Communication in the titi monkey,
Callicebus. J. Zool. 150: 77–127.
Muir, J., Barnett, A., & Svensson, M. S. 2019. e vocal
repertoire of golden-faced sakis, Pithecia chrysocepha-
la, and the relationship between context and call struc-
ture. Int. J. Primatol, 40(6): 721–743.
Ord, T. J., and Garcia-Porta, J. 2012. Is sociality requi-
red for the evolution of communicative complexity?
Evidence weighed against alternative hypotheses in di-
verse taxonomic groups. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 367:
1811–1828.
Ruppell, J. C. 2010. Vocal diversity and taxonomy of No-
mascus in central Vietnam and southern Laos. Int. J.
Primatol. 31: 73–94.
Schlick-Steiner, B. C., Steiner, F. M., Seifert, B., Stauer,
C., Christian, E., and Crozier, R. H. 2010. Integrative
taxonomy: a multisource approach to exploring biodi-
versity. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 55: 421–438.
Svensson, M. S., Bersacola, E., Mills, M. S., Munds, R.
A., Nijman, V., Perkin, A., ... and Bearder, S. K. 2017. A
giant among dwarfs: a new species of galago (Primates:
Galagidae) from Angola. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 163:
30–43.
Zimmermann, E. 2012. Primate serenades: call variation,
species diversity, and adaptation in nocturnal strep-
sirhines. In: Leaping Ahead: Advances in Prosimian
Biology, J. C. Masters, M. Gamba, and F. Genin, (eds.),
pp.287–295. Springer, New York.
FIRST RECORD OF BROWN HOWLER MONKEYS
IN SÃO PEDRO DA ALDEIA, RIO DE JANEIRO,
BRAZIL
Fabio Mostacato Bastos
Bruno Coutinho Kurtz
Luís Fernando Faulstich Neves
Leonardo de Carvalho Oliveira
Introduction
e discovery of a threatened species in a new locality
provides important information to reassess its extent of
occurrence, area of occupancy, and conservation status
(IUCN, 2019). In times where human-induced defau-
nation in forest fragments is of major scientic concern
(Canale et al., 2012; Dirzo et al., 2014; Galleti et al.,
2016) and outbreaks of yellow fever virus are severely
impacting populations of wild non-human primates in
Brazil’s Atlantic Forest (Holzmann et al., 2010; Almeida
et al., 2012; Bicca-Marques et al., 2017), the discovery of
a group of threatened primate species in a small fragment
of Atlantic Forest should be celebrated.
We report here the rst record of a group of brown howler
monkeys, Alouatta guariba clamitans, in São Pedro da
Aldeia, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. A. guariba is the primate
species with most records and with the largest distribu-
tion in the Atlantic Forest (Culot et al., 2019). In Rio
de Janeiro state brown howlers inhabit the coastal and
the northern regions (Gregorin, 2006), with populations
occurring in at least 23 protected areas (Bicca-Marques
et al., 2018). e deforestation and fragmentation of the
southern and southeastern forests have played an impor-
tant role in decreasing its current distribution compared
to its historical occurrence (Bicca-Marques et al., 2018).
It is currently listed as a Vulnerable species on the IUCN
Red List of reatened Species, with ongoing population
decline (Jerusalinsky et al., 2020).
Study site
Our study site (22°43'20.02"S, 42°07'25.37"W) is a small
patch of lowland seasonal semideciduous forest with 36
ha. It has an elliptical shape, with altitude varying from 7
to 35 m above sea level. Locally known as Ilha dos Maca-
cos (Monkeys’ Island), it is connected to other fragments
of Atlantic Forest totaling about 418 ha (Fig. 1). e site
lies in a swampy plain bordered by two small rivers that
drain northward up to the basin’s main river, Rio Una.
Grassy elds characterize the landscape. e rainy sea-
son (December-May) turns most of the plain into wet-
lands. e forest fragment studied lies in a higher terrain
so that waters reach only part of its border. Cattle ran-
ching, eucalyptus forestry and agriculture characterize
local land use around the fragment (Bastos, 2020). e
local climate is an interface between two Köppen-Geiger
climate classes, Aw and BSh (Barbiéri, 1984, 1997), and
the precipitation is between 900-1,000 mm per year (Pin-
to et al., 2011).
Results and discussion
During our survey focused on oristic and phytosocio-
logical data collection (Bastos, 2020), we unexpectedly
heard howls at the study site. erefore, we decided to
collect ad libitum data (Altmann, 1974) on all monkey
observations during every visit. We registered howler
vocalizations in 10 of 31 visits. e records encompas-
sed two years, from May 2018 to February 2020. e
only visualization was in January 2020. We observed an