ArticlePDF Available

Preliminary investigation into the differences in vocalisations between wild saki monkeys (Pithecia spp.)

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Information on primate vocalisations can be applied in several ways, including: improving captive welfare, as a census tool for cryptic species, or to investigate the impacts of anthropogenic disturbance on species’ behaviour (Delgado and van Shaik, 2000; Konrad and Geissman, 2006; Jacobsen et al., 2010). Vocalisations can be used as a taxonomic tool, and structural differences between calls have been used to compare a wide variety of taxa, including species of gibbon (Hylobatidae spp., Ruppell, 2010), marmosets (Callithrix spp., Mendes et al., 2009), owls (Strigidae spp., Flint et al., 2015), wolves (Canis spp., Kershenbaum et al., 2016), and galagos (Galagidae spp., Svensson et al., 2017). Additionally, differences in voca- lisations across taxonomic groups can be used to help determine genetic distances between species or investigate why vocal behaviours evolved (Blumstein and Armitage, 1998; Ord and Garcia-Porta, 2012).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 202132
Lynch Alfaro, J. W., Silva, J. S. Jr. and Rylands, A. B. 2012.
How dierent are Robust and Gracile Capuchin Mon-
keys? An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus.
Am. J. Primatol. 74: 273–286.
Milano, M. Z. and Monteiro-Filho, E. L. A. 2009.Preda-
tion on small mammals by Capuchin Monkeys, Cebus
cay. Neotrop. Primates. 16(2): 78–80.
Resende, B. D., Greco, V. L. G., Ottoni, E. B. and Izar,
P. 2003. Some observations on the predation of small
mammals by Tued Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
Neotrop. Primates 11(2): 103–104.
Robinson, S. K. 1985. Coloniality In e Yellow Rumped
Cacique as a defense against nest predators. Auk. 102:
506–519.
Robinson, S. K. 1988. Foraging ecology and host relation-
ships of giant Cowbirds in southeastern Peru. Wilson
Bull. 100:224–235.
Rose, L. 1997. Vertebrate predation and food-sharing in
Cebus and Pan. Int. J. Primatol. 18: 727–765.
Sabbatini, G., Stammati, M, Tavares, M. C. H. and
Visalberghi, E. 2008. Behavioral exibility of a group
of Bearded Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in
the National Park of Brasília (Brazil): consequences of
cohabitation with visitors. Braz. J. Biol. 68(4): 685–693.
Siemers, B. M. 2000. Seasonal variation in food Resource
and forest strata use by Brown Capuchin Monkeys
(Cebus apella) in a disturbed forest fragment. Folia Pri-
matol. 71:181–184.
Torralvo, K., Rabelo, R. M., Andrade, A. and Botero-
Arias, R. 2017.Tool use by amazonian Capuchin Mon-
keys during predation on caiman nests in a high-pro-
ductivity forest. Primates 58(2): 279–283.
Watts, D. P. 2020. Meat eating by nonhuman primates: A
review and synthesis. J. Hum. Evol. 149: 102882.
PRELIMINARY INVESTIGATION INTO THE
DIFFERENCES IN VOCALISATIONS BETWEEN
WILD SAKI MONKEYS (PITHECIA SPP.)
Jen Muir
Adrian A. Barnett
Magdalena S. Svensson
Introduction
Information on primate vocalisations can be applied in
several ways, including: improving captive welfare, as a
census tool for cryptic species, or to investigate the im-
pacts of anthropogenic disturbance on species’ behaviour
(Delgado and van Shaik, 2000; Konrad and Geissman,
2006; Jacobsen et al., 2010). Vocalisations can be used
as a taxonomic tool, and structural dierences between
calls have been used to compare a wide variety of taxa,
including species of gibbon (Hylobatidae spp., Ruppell,
2010), marmosets (Callithrix spp., Mendes et al., 2009),
owls (Strigidae spp., Flint et al., 2015), wolves (Canis spp.,
Kershenbaum et al., 2016), and galagos (Galagidae spp.,
Svensson et al., 2017). Additionally, dierences in voca-
lisations across taxonomic groups can be used to help de-
termine genetic distances between species or investigate
why vocal behaviours evolved (Blumstein and Armitage,
1998; Ord and Garcia-Porta, 2012).
One primate group that recently has been revised ta-
xonomically is the genus Pithecia, the saki monkeys, in
which ve species were previously described: P. mona-
chus, P. aequatorialis, P. irrorata, P. albicans, and P. p i t he -
cia (Hershkovitz, 1987). Aer a review of morphological
data, the group was rearranged into 16 species, including
three subspecies elevated to full species and ve newly
described species (Marsh, 2014). It has been suggested
that taxonomic research should utilize several disciplines
in combination so as to decrease the risk of inaccurate
conclusions (Schlick-Steiner et al., 2010). In the case of
the sakis, the use of purely morphological features opens
the opportunity for the taxonomic conclusions to be tes-
ted by evidence based on genetic structure, behaviour,
ecology, or life history, among others.
In the current study, vocalisations produced by wild
golden-faced sakis, Pithecia chrysocephala, white-faced
sakis, P. pithecia, and Equatorial sakis, P. aequatorialis,
were compared to study how calls might dier between
closely related species (P. pithecia and P. chrysocephala),
and whether any features were conserved across more
evolutionarily separated species (P. chrysocephala and P.
pithecia compared to P. aequatorialis). Before the reclas-
sication of Pithecia by Marsh (2014), P. chrysocephala
was considered a subspecies of P. pithecia, and so these
taxa were expected to display similar vocalisations.
Methods
Study Species
Pithecia chrysocephala, P. pithecia, and P. aequatorialis all
occupy forest habitats, including Amazonian várzea, iga-
pó, and terra rme forests. However, the species occupy
dierent geographical areas (Fig. 1), with P. chrysocepha-
la occurring within the Brazilian states of Amazonas,
Pará, and Roraima, and P. pithecia found in the states of
Pará and Amapá, as well as in the countries of Venezuela,
Guyana, French Guiana and Suriname (Marsh, 2014). Pi-
thecia aequatorialis is present only in central Peru (Marsh
and Heymann, 2018).
Data Organisation and Analysis
We collected vocalisation data for Pithecia chryso-
cephala in June-August 2018 in Manaus, Brazil (-3.083,
59.983) (for methodology and vocal repertoire see Muir
et al., 2019). We then gathered vocalisation data on two
other Pithecia species from previous published studies,
which we re-analysed and compared across species. For
P. pithecia, data from Henlines (2007) study conducted
in Isla Redonda, Venezuela (7.766, 62.883) was used,
and P. aequatorialis data came from Keirens (2012)
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 2021 33
study in the Tahuayo River Amazon Research Center,
Peru (-4.383, – 73.25). While Henlines (2007) study was
conducted before the reclassication of Pithecia species,
it is clear that this study did not include vocalisation
from P. chrysocephala as the in-situ part of the study was
conducted in Venezuela, far from the natural range of
this species, but within that P. pithecia. Our study and
the two previous studies each report between 5 and 12
distinct vocalisations per species (Table 1). One limita-
tion of these studies is that individual sakis could not be
distinguished during recordings and so pseudo-replica-
tion is a potential issue as it is unknown how much any
given individual contributed to the sample.
Table 1. e complete adult vocal repertoires of P. chrysocephala,
P. pithecia, and P. aequatorialis as recorded by Muir et al. (2019),
Henline (2007), and Keiran (2012). P. aequatorialis calls were not
put into groups within their study and so have been grouped ac-
cording to their similarity to the others based on their description
and/or behavioural context.
Group Pithecia
chrysocephala
Pithecia
pithecia
Pithecia aequatorialis
Whistles Whistle
(n = 778)
Pee (n = 20) Bird (n = 23)
See (n = 6) Chits (n = 61)
Chew (n=8) Chits/XX (n = 10)
Chucks Chuck
(n = 212)
Chuck
(n = 16)
Bark (n = 45)
Chits with Bark
(n = 6)
Churk
(n = 1)
Croak (n = 2)
Yip (n = 1)
Trills Trill (n = 253) Cheeyeep
(n = 29)
Trill (n = 5)
Seeyeep
(n = 5)
Peeyeep
(n = 2)
Warbled
Trill (n = 8)
Purrs Soft Growl
(n = 4)
Soft Growl
(n = 3)
Growl (n = 17)
Moans and
Alarms
roat Rattle
(n = 96)
roat
Rattle
(n = 9)
Scream (n = 3)
Z-trill
(n = 3)
Unknown Grunt (n = 1)
Results
From spectrograms, Pithecia chrysocephala and P. p i t h e -
cia display similar chuck calls, while P. chrysocephala and
P. aequatorialis show similar throat rattle/scream calls, as
well as juvenile cry/juvenile yip calls (Fig. 2, Table 2). Ad-
ditionally, these calls are similarly used in alarm, antago-
nistic, and parent-ospring contact, respectively.
One-sample t-tests suggested that Pithecia chrysocephala
and P. pithecia dier signicantly in the duration of their
chuck calls, t(217) = 2.11, p =.036, and in fundamental fre-
quency, t(217) = 15.191, p < .001. is was also suggested
for the duration, t(95) = 21.575, p < .001, and fundamen-
tal frequency, t(95) = 3.445, p = .001, of the throat rattle
call. Comparisons between the P. chrysocephala throat
rattle and P. aequatorialis scream were also conducted,
Figure 1. Location of study site and distributions of the studied
Pithecia species. Map created using spatial data obtained from
IUCN (Marsh et al. 2018a; Marsh et al. 2018b; Marsh and Hey-
mann 2018).
In our study calls were initially compared visually by
spectrograms from each paper, with similar calls then
being statistically compared in terms of acoustic mea-
surements. Similarity in terms of the behavioural con-
text of the calls was also considered. Call measurements
of duration and fundamental frequency were taken us-
ing the speech analysis program Praat (Boersma, 2001).
One sample t-tests were performed using SPSS v.25 to
compare call features between species as the raw data
was not available for P. pithecia and P. aequatorialis.
When making comparisons, the two calls within Hen-
line’s (2007) chucks group were averaged to allow for
a comparison with the graded chuck call found in P.
hrysocephala (Table 1). e whistle and trill groups were
not compared in this manner as the calls within them
appeared to dier from those of P. chrysocephala when
shown as spectrograms. e so growl group was also
not compared, due to a small sample size for both P.
pithecia and P. chrysocephala. From Keiran’s (2012) study
of P. aequatorialis, only the scream and juvenile yip calls
were similar enough on spectrograms to be compared
(to the P. chrysocephala throat rattle and juvenile cry,
respectively).
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 202134
with results suggesting that they dier signicantly in
fundamental frequency, t(95) = 55.401, p < .001, but not
in duration, t(95) = .671, p = .504. e P. chrysocephala
juvenile cry and P. aequatorialis juvenile yip call diered
signicantly in duration, t(107) = 15.619, p < .001and
fundamental frequency, t(107) = 35.503, p < .001.
Figure 2. Spectrograms of the vocal repertoires of Pithecia chrysocephala, P. pithecia, and P.
aequatorialis, with visually similar calls. Calls include: P. pithecia chuck (a), P. chrysocephala
chuck (b), P. chrysocephala throat rattle (c), P. aequatorialis scream (d), P. chrysocephala juvenile
cry (e) and P. aequatorialis juvenile yip (f).
Table 2. Structural properties of structurally and contextually similar calls between Pithecia chry-
socephala, P. pithecia, and P. aequatorialis.
Species Compared Calls Compared Mean Duration in
Seconds
Mean Fundamental
Frequency in Hz
P. chrysocephala
P. pithecia
Chuck
Chuck
0.352
0.335
6307.10
6507.15
P. chrysocephala
P. aequatorialis
roat rattle
Scream
1.537
1.554
3029.84
784.38
P. chrysocephala
P. aequatorialis
Juvenile cry
Juvenile yip
0.419
0.158
6486.16
766.73
Discussion
At a glance, structural properties between Pithecia pithe-
cia calls in Henline (2007) and those of the current study
of P. chrysocephala are very similar, with chucks being the
shortest calls, throat rattles the longest, chucks and trills
at the highest frequencies, and so growls at the lowest.
However, few calls between these species were sucient-
ly similar on spectrograms to be compared statistically.
All visually similar calls among P. chrysocephala, P. pi t he -
cia and P. aequatorialis were found to have dierences in
their structure in terms of their fundamental frequency.
ese preliminary ndings add to the morphological evi-
dence presented by Marsh (2014) on the distinctness of
Pithecia taxa, supporting the classication of P. chryso-
cephala as a separate species from P. pithecia, rather than
a subspecies. However, as only one population of each
species was studied, the possibility of variations within
populations of a single evolutionary unit must be consi-
dered before any more substantial claims can be made.
e calls of Pithecia aequatorialis appeared notably less
similar to P. chrysocephala than P. pithecia, potentially
reecting the greater evolutionary distance between
these species. Phylogenetic dierences in call struc-
ture across a genus have been previously observed in
a number of primate taxa, including sportive lemurs,
Lepilemur spp., in which species that were less closely
related to each other had more distinctly dierent calls
(Mendez-Cárdenas et al., 2008). Similar results have
been obtained with titi monkeys, Callicebinae sp., (Ar-
det et al., 2018). It is also possible that these calls are
conserved across the genus, similar to how the startle
call is conserved across mouse lemurs, Microcebus spp.
(Zimmerman, 2012).
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 2021 35
Future studies could usefully investigate dierences
among Pithecia populations and examine the inuence
of habitat dierences on vocalisations. It would also be
interesting to investigate Pithecia vocal behaviour in the
context of all pitheciids, as several similarities are evi-
dent throughout this group (Bezerra et al., 2017). For
example, the tcho calls, whistles, and loud screams of gol-
den-backed uacaris, Cacajao ouakar y, are similar to the
P. chrysocephala chucks, whistles, and throat rattles res-
pectively (Bezerra et al., 2010). Likewise, juvenile B-calls
(an alarm call) of black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus
nigrifrons, are similar to juvenile P. chrysocephala peeps,
as are the juvenile purrs of red-bellied titi monkeys, Plec-
turocebus moloch, to juvenile golden faced saki trews
(Moynihan, 1966; Berthet et al., 2018). Additionally, the
chucks and intergroup call/throat rattle of Plecturocebus
moloch and Pithecia chrysocephala resemble each other
(Moynihan, 1966; Caselli et al., 2014). More wide-sca-
le comparisons across all pitheciid species could also be
conducted to examine the social and ecological eects
on the evolution of their communication, and why such
calls appear to be similar. However, a full phylogenetic
comparison of Pithecia vocalisations is currently limited
by a lack of data, and so further studies of Pithecia and
pitheciid species are recommended. e development of
a pitheciid vocalisation database as suggested by Bezerra
et al. (2017) would be ideal for this purpose.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Makiko Take and Tianara So-
broza for their assistance in Brazil. We would also like to
thank Instituto National de Pesquisas da Amazonia for
providing the opportunity to carry out this project, and
for Santander for providing a Student Project Grant.
Jen Muir, School of Social Sciences, Oxford Brookes
University, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK, E-mail: <jenmuir93@
gmail.com>, Adrian A. Barnett, Amazon Mammal Re-
search Group, Biodiversity Studies, Instituto National de
Pesquisas da Amazonia, Manaus, Brazil, and Magdalena
S. Svensson, School of Social Sciences, Oxford Brookes
University, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK.
References
Adret, P., Dingess, K. A., Caselli, C. B., Vermeer, J., Mar-
tínez, J., Luna Amancio, J. C., ... & Di Fiore, A. 2018.
Duetting patterns of titi monkeys (Primates, Pithe-
ciidae: Callicebinae) and relationships with phylo-
geny.Animals 8: 178.
Berthet, M., Neumann, C., Mesbahi, G., Cäsar, C., and Zu-
berbühler, K. 2018. Contextual encoding in titi monkey
alarm call sequences. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72: 8.
Bezerra, B. M., Souto, A. S., and Jones, G. 2010. Vocal
repertoire of golden-backed uakaris (Cacajao melano-
cephalus): call structure and context. Int. J. Primatol. 31:
759–778.
Bezerra, B., Casar, C., Jerusalinsky, L., Barnett, A., Bas-
tos, M., Souto, A., and Jones, G. 2017. Pitheciid vocal
communication: what can we say about what they are
saying? Ethnobiol. Cons. 6: 1–23.
Blumstein, D. T., & Armitage, K. B. 1998. Why do yellow-
-bellied marmots call? Anim. Behav. 56: 1053–1055.
Boersma, P. 2001. Praat, a system for doing phonetics by
computer. Glot Int. 5: 341–345.
Caselli, C. B., Mennill, D. J., Bicca‐Marques, J. C., and
Setz, E. Z. 2014. Vocal behaviour of black‐fronted titi
monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons): Acoustic properties
and behavioral contexts of loud calls. Am. J. Primatol.
76: 788–800.
Delgado Jr., R. A., and Van Schaik, C. P. 2000. e beha-
vioural ecology and conservation of the orangutan
(Pongo pygmaeus): a tale of two islands. Evo. Anthropol.
9: 201–218.
Flint, P. Whaley, D., Kirwan, G. M., Charalambides, M.,
Schweizer, M. and Wink, M. 2015. Reprising the taxo-
nomy of Cyprus scops owl Otus (scops) cyprius, a ne-
glected island endemic. Zootaxa 4040: 301–316.
Henline, W. T. 2007. Vocal repertoire of white-faced sakis
(Pithecia pithecia). Master’s thesis, Eastern Kentucky
University, USA.
Hershkovitz, P. 1987. e taxonomy of South American
sakis, genus Pithecia (Cebidae, Platyrrhini): a prelimi-
nary report and critical review with the description of
a new species and a new subspecies. Am. J. Primatol.
12: 387–468.
Jacobsen, K. R., Mikkelsen, L. F., and Hau, J. 2010. e
eect of environmental enrichment on the behaviour
of captive tued capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Lab
Anim. 39: 269.
Kershenbaum, A., Root-Gutteridge, H., Habib, B., Koler-
-Matznick, J., Mitchell, B., Palacios, V., and Waller, S.
2016. Disentangling canid howls across multiple spe-
cies and subspecies: Structure in a complex communi-
cation channel. Behav. Processes. 124: 149–157.
Kieran, T. J. 2012. Identity and vocal repertoire of a Pe-
ruvian saki monkey (Pithecia aequatorialis). Master’s
thesis, Winthrop University, USA.
Konrad, R., and Geissmann, T. 2006. Vocal diversity and
taxonomy of Nomascus in Cambodia. Int. J. Primatol.
27: 713.
Marsh, L. K. 2014. A taxonomic revision of the saki
monkeys, Pithecia (Desmarest, 1804). Neotrop. Prima-
tes. 21: 1–165.
Marsh, L. K., Boubli, J., Mittermeier, R. A., Rohe, F.,
Urbani, B. & de Azevedo, R. 2018a. Pithecia pithecia.
e IUCN Red List of reatened Species 2018: e.
T43942A17991915. Website: http://dx.doi.org/10.2305/
IUCN.UK.2018-2.RLTS.T43942A17991915.en. Acces-
sed 20 January 2021.
Marsh, L. K., Mittermeier, R.A., Rohe, F. & de Azevedo,
R.2018b.Pithecia chrysocephala.e IUCN Red List of
reatened Species 2018: e.T43943A17991938.Websi-
te: http://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2018-2.RLTS.
T43943A17991938.en.Accessed 20 January 2021.
Neotropical Primates 27(1), July 202136
Marsh, L. K. and Heymann, E. W. 2018. Pithecia aequato-
rialis. e IUCN Red List of reatened Species 2018:
e.T17402A17971831. Website: https://www.iucnre-
dlist.org/species/17402/17971831. Accessed 21 January
2021.
Mendes, S. L., Vielliard, J. M. E., & De Marco, P. 2009. e
vocal identity of the Callithrix species (Primates, Calli-
trichidae). In:e Smallest Anthropoids, S. M. Ford, L.
M. Porter and L. C. Davis (eds.),pp. 63–84. Springer,
Boston, MA.
Méndez‐Cárdenas, M., Randrianambinina, B., Rabesan-
dratana, A., Rasoloharijaona, S., and Zimmermann,
E. 2008. Geographic variation in loud calls of sportive
lemurs (Lepilemur ssp.) and their implications for con-
servation. Am. J. Primatol. 70: 828-838.
Moynihan, M. 1966. Communication in the titi monkey,
Callicebus. J. Zool. 150: 77–127.
Muir, J., Barnett, A., & Svensson, M. S. 2019. e vocal
repertoire of golden-faced sakis, Pithecia chrysocepha-
la, and the relationship between context and call struc-
ture. Int. J. Primatol, 40(6): 721–743.
Ord, T. J., and Garcia-Porta, J. 2012. Is sociality requi-
red for the evolution of communicative complexity?
Evidence weighed against alternative hypotheses in di-
verse taxonomic groups. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 367:
1811–1828.
Ruppell, J. C. 2010. Vocal diversity and taxonomy of No-
mascus in central Vietnam and southern Laos. Int. J.
Primatol. 31: 73–94.
Schlick-Steiner, B. C., Steiner, F. M., Seifert, B., Stauer,
C., Christian, E., and Crozier, R. H. 2010. Integrative
taxonomy: a multisource approach to exploring biodi-
versity. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 55: 421–438.
Svensson, M. S., Bersacola, E., Mills, M. S., Munds, R.
A., Nijman, V., Perkin, A., ... and Bearder, S. K. 2017. A
giant among dwarfs: a new species of galago (Primates:
Galagidae) from Angola. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 163:
30–43.
Zimmermann, E. 2012. Primate serenades: call variation,
species diversity, and adaptation in nocturnal strep-
sirhines. In: Leaping Ahead: Advances in Prosimian
Biology, J. C. Masters, M. Gamba, and F. Genin, (eds.),
pp.287–295. Springer, New York.
FIRST RECORD OF BROWN HOWLER MONKEYS
IN SÃO PEDRO DA ALDEIA, RIO DE JANEIRO,
BRAZIL
Fabio Mostacato Bastos
Bruno Coutinho Kurtz
Luís Fernando Faulstich Neves
Leonardo de Carvalho Oliveira
Introduction
e discovery of a threatened species in a new locality
provides important information to reassess its extent of
occurrence, area of occupancy, and conservation status
(IUCN, 2019). In times where human-induced defau-
nation in forest fragments is of major scientic concern
(Canale et al., 2012; Dirzo et al., 2014; Galleti et al.,
2016) and outbreaks of yellow fever virus are severely
impacting populations of wild non-human primates in
Brazil’s Atlantic Forest (Holzmann et al., 2010; Almeida
et al., 2012; Bicca-Marques et al., 2017), the discovery of
a group of threatened primate species in a small fragment
of Atlantic Forest should be celebrated.
We report here the rst record of a group of brown howler
monkeys, Alouatta guariba clamitans, in São Pedro da
Aldeia, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. A. guariba is the primate
species with most records and with the largest distribu-
tion in the Atlantic Forest (Culot et al., 2019). In Rio
de Janeiro state brown howlers inhabit the coastal and
the northern regions (Gregorin, 2006), with populations
occurring in at least 23 protected areas (Bicca-Marques
et al., 2018). e deforestation and fragmentation of the
southern and southeastern forests have played an impor-
tant role in decreasing its current distribution compared
to its historical occurrence (Bicca-Marques et al., 2018).
It is currently listed as a Vulnerable species on the IUCN
Red List of reatened Species, with ongoing population
decline (Jerusalinsky et al., 2020).
Study site
Our study site (22°43'20.02"S, 42°07'25.37"W) is a small
patch of lowland seasonal semideciduous forest with 36
ha. It has an elliptical shape, with altitude varying from 7
to 35 m above sea level. Locally known as Ilha dos Maca-
cos (Monkeys’ Island), it is connected to other fragments
of Atlantic Forest totaling about 418 ha (Fig. 1). e site
lies in a swampy plain bordered by two small rivers that
drain northward up to the basin’s main river, Rio Una.
Grassy elds characterize the landscape. e rainy sea-
son (December-May) turns most of the plain into wet-
lands. e forest fragment studied lies in a higher terrain
so that waters reach only part of its border. Cattle ran-
ching, eucalyptus forestry and agriculture characterize
local land use around the fragment (Bastos, 2020). e
local climate is an interface between two Köppen-Geiger
climate classes, Aw and BSh (Barbiéri, 1984, 1997), and
the precipitation is between 900-1,000 mm per year (Pin-
to et al., 2011).
Results and discussion
During our survey focused on oristic and phytosocio-
logical data collection (Bastos, 2020), we unexpectedly
heard howls at the study site. erefore, we decided to
collect ad libitum data (Altmann, 1974) on all monkey
observations during every visit. We registered howler
vocalizations in 10 of 31 visits. e records encompas-
sed two years, from May 2018 to February 2020. e
only visualization was in January 2020. We observed an
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Vocalizations are a vital form of communication. Call structure and use may change depending on emotional arousal, behavioral context, sex, or social complexity. Pithecia chrysocephala (golden-faced sakis) are a little-studied Neotropical species. We aimed to determine the vocal repertoire of P. chrysocephala and the influence of context on call structure. We collected data June–August 2018 in an urban secondary forest fragment in Manaus, Amazonian Brazil. We took continuous vocal recordings in 10-min blocks with 5-min breaks during daily follows of two groups. We recorded scan samples of group behavior at the start and end of blocks and used ad libitum behavioral recording during blocks. We collected 70 h of data and analyzed 1500 calls. Lowest frequencies ranged 690.1–5879 Hz in adults/subadults and 5393.6–9497.8Hz in the only juvenile sampled. We identified eight calls, three of which were juvenile specific. We found that, while repertoire size was similar to that of other New World monkeys of similar group size and structure, it also resembled those with larger group sizes and different social structures. The durations of Chuck calls were shorter for feeding contexts compared to hostile, but frequencies were higher than predicted if call structure reflects motivation. This finding may be due to the higher arousal involved in hostile situations, or because P. chrysocephala use Chuck calls in appeasement, similar to behavior seen in other primates. Call structures did not differ between sexes, potentially linked to the limited size dimorphism in this species. Our findings provide a foundation for further investigation of Pithecia vocal behavior and phylogeny, as well as applications for both captive welfare (stress relief) and field research (playbacks for surveys).
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Titi monkeys—a diversified group of pair-bonded, territorial neotropical primates exhibiting biparental care—produce elaborate, powerful vocal duets used for long-range communication. While the callicebine taxonomy has been centered mainly on the biogeography, morphology, anatomy, and genetics of titi populations, vocal attributes have received little attention as potentially informative markers of phylogenetic relationships. We conducted acoustic analysis of callicebine loud calls recorded from ten species of titis at sites in Bolivia, Peru, and Ecuador and found four distinct patterns of duetting that only partially match three major clades identified in recent molecular genetic studies. In particular, we found that the loud calls of the San Martin titi monkey, P. oenanthe, and the Urubamba brown titi, P. urubambensis, strikingly differ from putative relatives within the donacophilus lineage. Our findings highlight interplay between genes and environment on the expression of vocal behavior and suggest that closer interaction between taxonomists, ethologists, and molecular biologists should be rewarding in resolving the callicebine phylogeny. Such concerted efforts, in turn, will most likely generate valuable recommendations for the conservation of some endangered populations of titi monkeys, such as the vocally distinctive San Martin titi. Abstract Long-range vocal communication in socially monogamous titi monkeys is mediated by the production of loud, advertising calls in the form of solos, duets, and choruses. We conducted a power spectral analysis of duets and choruses (simply “duets” hereafter) followed by linear discriminant analysis using three acoustic parameters—dominant frequency of the combined signal, duet sequence duration, and pant call rate—comparing the coordinated vocalizations recorded from 36 family groups at 18 sites in Bolivia, Peru and Ecuador. Our analysis identified four distinct duetting patterns: (1) a donacophilus pattern, sensu stricto, characteristic of P. donacophilus, P. pallescens, P. olallae, and P. modestus; (2) a moloch pattern comprising P. discolor, P. toppini, P. aureipalatii, and P. urubambensis; (3) a torquatus pattern exemplified by the duet of Cheracebus lucifer; and (4) the distinctive duet of P. oenanthe, a putative member of the donacophilus group, which is characterized by a mix of broadband and narrowband syllables, many of which are unique to this species. We also document a sex-related difference in the bellow-pant phrase combination among the three taxa sampled from the moloch lineage. Our data reveal a presumptive taxonomic incoherence illustrated by the distinctive loud calls of both P. urubambensis and P. oenanthe within the donacophilus lineage, sensu largo. The results are discussed in light of recent reassessments of the callicebine phylogeny, based on a suite of genetic studies, and the potential contribution of environmental influences, including habitat acoustics and social learning. A better knowledge of callicebine loud calls may also impact the conservation of critically endangered populations, such as the vocally distinctive Peruvian endemic, the San Martin titi, P. oenanthe.
Article
Full-text available
Many primates produce one type of alarm call to a broad range of events, usually terrestrial predators and non-predatory situations, which raises questions about whether primate alarm calls should be considered ‘functionally referential’. A recent example is black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus nigrifrons, which emit sequences of B-calls to terrestrial predators or when moving towards or near the ground. In this study, we reassess the context specificity of these utterances, focussing both on their acoustic and sequential structure. We found that B-calls could be differentiated into context-specific acoustic variants (terrestrial predators vs. ground-related movements) and that call sequences to predators had a more regular sequential structure than ground-related sequences. Overall, these findings suggest that the acoustic and temporal structure of titi monkey call sequences discriminate between predator and non-predatory events, fulfilling the production criterion of functional reference. Significance statement Primate terrestrial alarm calls are at the centre of an ongoing debate about meaning in animal signals. Primates regularly emit one alarm call type to ground predators but often also to various non-predatory events, raising questions about the referential nature of these signals. In this study, we report observational and experimental data from wild titi monkeys and show that terrestrial alarm calls are usually given in sequences of acoustically distinct variants composed in structurally distinct ways depending on the external event. These differences are salient and could help recipients to distinguish the nature of the call eliciting event. Since most previous studies on animal alarm calls have not checked for acoustic variants within different call classes, it may be premature to conclude that primate terrestrial calls do not meet the criteria of functional reference.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Based on vocalization recordings of an unknown galago species, our main objectives were to compare morphology and call structure with known closely-related taxa and describe a new species of galago. Materials and methods: We conducted field surveys in three forest habitats along the escarpment region in western Angola (Kumbira Forest, Bimbe Area, and Northern Scarp Forest), and examined galago specimens from museums worldwide. We digitized and analyzed calls using Avisoft SASLab Pro software. We also compared museum specimens from Angola with other Galago and Galagoides specimens, and conducted comparative analyses (ANOVA and between group principle component analysis) based on a set of twelve linear measurements of skulls and teeth. Results: We describe the new species to which we give the name Angolan dwarf galago, Galagoides kumbirensis sp. nov. The new species has a loud and characteristic crescendo call, used by other Galagoides spp. (sensu stricto) in West Africa to attract companions and repel rivals. However, this call shows species-typical differences from its closest relatives. Galagoides kumbirensis sp. nov. is also distinguished by differences in the skull morphology, pelage color and facial markings, as well as a larger body size, similar to that of Galago moholi, which is not known to be sympatric. Conclusion: This discovery points to the importance of Angolan forests as refuges for endemic biodiversity. These forests are under severe threat from overexploitation, and there is an urgent need to establish conservation measures and designate protected areas.
Article
Full-text available
The endemic Cyprus Scops Owl Otus (scops) cyprius has been treated as a subspecies of the widespread Eurasian Scops Owl O. scops since at least the 1940s. However, its song is distinct from that of all other subspecies of O. scops in being double-noted, rather than single-noted. Its plumage also differs, most obviously in being consistently darker than other subspecies and in lacking a rufous morph. However, it shows no biometric differences from O. s. cycladum and southern populations of O. s. scops. It is also unusual among scops (s. l.) populations in being at least partially resident, although two specimens showing characters of this taxon were collected in Israel in early spring, and the numbers of birds that are resident on Cyprus appear to vary, with few recent winter records. It differs from O. s. scops by one synapomorphic nucleotide exchange in the analysed mitochondrial marker, indicating a recent separation. Given that large numbers of O. s. scops and O. s. cycladum pass through Cyprus on spring migration, and that the latter breeds in adjacent countries, it seems probable that cycladum would colonize the island, but for the presence of cyprius. That it does not do so, and that cyprius retains its distinctive song and plumage, suggests that isolating mechanisms exist. We recommend that cyprius be considered specifically distinct, as are other distinctively voiced insular Otus populations.
Article
Full-text available
For more than 200 years, the taxonomy of Pithecia has been floating on the misunderstanding of a few species, in particular P. pithecia and P. monachus. In this revision, historical names and descriptions are addressed and original type material is examined. For every museum specimen, all location, collection, and museum data were recorded, and photographs and measurements of each skin, skull, mount, or fluid specimen were taken. The revision is based on work conducted in 36 museums in 28 cities from 17 countries in North America, South America, Europe, and Japan, resulting in the examination of 876 skins (including mounts and fluids), 690 skulls, and hundreds of photographs taken by the author and by colleagues in the field of living captive and wild sakis of all species, and through internet searches. Per this revision, there are 16 species of Pithecia: five currently recognized, three reinstated, three elevated from subspecies level, and five newly described.
Article
The variation in ecological traits in pitheciids allow investigation of vocal communication across a range of social and acoustic circumstances. In this review, we present a summary of the history of pitheciid vocal studies, and review i) the status of current knowledge of pitheciid vocal repertoire sizes, ii) how much we understand about the context of different acoustic signals, and iii) how can we potentially use our knowledge of vocalizations in animal welfare practices. The repertoires described for titi monkeys and sakis have the expected sizes for these genera, considering their relatively small social group sizes. However, uacari groups can contain over 100 individuals, and a larger vocal repertoire than the ones described would be expected, which could be a consequence of the fissionfusion social system where the large group divides into smaller subgroups. Nevertheless, vocal repertoires exist for only about 12% of the pitheciid species and nothing is known, for example, concerning call ontogeny. We hope that this study will act as a reference point for researchers interested in investigating vocal behaviour in pitheciids, thus, optimising both funding focus and, researcher's time and effort. Also, we hope to help defining methodologies and strategies for the conservation and management of pitheciid monkeys.
Thesis
I studied the vocalizations of wild saki monkeys (Pithecia spp.) in the Área de Conservacíon Regional Comunal de Tamshiyacu-Tahuayo (ACRCTT), Peru. Sakis are little studied in the wild and occupy dense, seasonally flooded, lowland rainforest, which makes extended visual contact with them difficult. Their vocalizations can be used to find groups, localize and track individuals, identify group activities when visibility is poor, and to help identify species. Vocalizations were recorded ad libitum as MP3 files using a parabolic stereo microphone attached to a digital linear recorder, then analyzed using Raven Pro 1.3 bioacoustic software. Behavioral data on individuals and groups and habitat information was noted when possible. I identified 11 main call types with at least 4 call variations, verified using the Kruskal-Wallis test followed by Dunn’s Multiple Comparisons post hoc test (Zar 1984) on call attributes of the minimum and maximum frequency, frequency range, and duration. I compared these calls with spectrographs of calls of P. pithecia (Buchanan 1978, Henline 2007), P. monachus (Buchanan 1978) and P. aequatorialis (Sounds of Neotropical Rainforest Mammals, Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology) from published sources. Using the calls of the species that aurally and visually resembled the unidentified Pithecia sp. an ANOVA was performed comparing P. aequatorialis calls with Pithecia calls from the ACRCTT, identifying the species in the reserve as potentially being P. aequatorialis. Based on geographic range, physical descriptions and vocalization analysis, I concluded that P. aequatorialis is the most likely candidate for the previously unidentified saki species in the ACRCTT. Pithecia aequatorialis has not been previously been reported south of the Amazon River and its vocalizations have not been described previously. With the genus Pithecia undergoing review, vocalizations may provide an important aid in species identification.
Chapter
Interspecific divergence in acoustic signaling systems is a major focus of biodiversity and evolutionary research, but empirical data for primates are rare. Our research team compared communication calls of four cryptic, genetically defined mouse lemur (Microcebus) species uttered in comparable social situations. We found that calls vary interspecifically to different extents depending on the context. The acoustic pattern of calls given in the startle context was conserved across species, whereas calls given in agonistic and mating contexts showed significant interspecific variation. Uniformity in the acoustic pattern of calls across species can be explained by similar physiological and environmental constraints; and divergences have been explained by several hypotheses including natural and sexual selection. Because of remarkable species-specific differences in frequency contours, advertisement calls are the most useful diagnostic tools for noninvasive species and subspecies identification.