ArticlePDF Available

Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy

Authors:

Abstract

Physical environment has long been recognized within occupational therapy as a key factor contributing to residents’ functional abilities. However, the specific aspects of the physical environment that matter and the extent to which they do so remain less understood. This paper reports a quantitative study of the relationship between a characteristic of the physical home environment—the degree of interconnectedness of its rooms—and the cognitive ability of adults. Working with demographic, health, and home layout data collected from a sample of community-dwelling older adults in Atlanta, Georgia (N=72, Mage=69.5), we found that the cognitive functioning determined by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score was significantly associated with the average connectivity and mean depth of the homes while controlling participants’ age and education. Regression analysis suggested home connectivity independently explained a little more than 4% of the variance in the MoCA scores. The results further revealed that the relationship may be better modeled using non-linear models, and that the increase in the numbers of circulation rings as average room connectivity rises may be partly, but not entirely, responsible for its association with cognitive ability. The study points to directions for further work, including causal modeling, based on recommendations that could be developed for homes to support older adults’ abilities to continue to reside in their own homes as they grow older.
*(Corresponding author) a(PhD), Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, USA, sonit.bafna@design.gatech.edu
b(PhD), Georgia State University, Atlanta, USA, kmaitra@gsu.edu, c(PhD), Let’s Talk Therapy, Duluth, Georgia, Atlanta,
USA, yoon@globaltherapysc.com, d(MArch), Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, USA, map339@cornell.edu
e(PhD), Georgia State University, Atlanta, USA, ychen102@gsu.edu, Article history: Received 20 November 2021, Accepted 13
December 2021, Published 31 December 2021, Copyright: © The Author(s). Distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License
Research Article
Online: www.drarch.org
Volume 2, Special Issue, (18-33), 2021
DOI: 10.47818/DRArch.2021.v2si033
JOURNAL OF DESIGN FOR RESILIENCE
IN ARCHITECTURE & PLANNING
Association between home layout connectivity and
cognitive ability in community dwelling older adults:
Implication for occupational therapy
Sonit Bafna*a
Kinsuk K. Maitrab
Yoonjeong Limc
Mansi Shahd
Yi-An Chene
Abstract
Physical environment has long been recognized within occupational therapy as a key
factor contributing to residents’ functional abilities. However, the specific aspects of
the physical environment that matter and the extent to which they do so remain less
understood. This paper reports a quantitative study of the relationship between a
characteristic of the physical home environmentthe degree of interconnectedness
of its roomsand the cognitive ability of adults. Working with demographic, health,
and home layout data collected from a sample of community-dwelling older adults in
Atlanta, Georgia (N=72, Mage=69.5), we found that the cognitive functioning
determined by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score was significantly
associated with the average connectivity and mean depth of the homes while
controlling participants’ age and education. Regression analysis suggested home
connectivity independently explained a little more than 4% of the variance in the
MoCA scores. The results further revealed that the relationship may be better
modeled using non-linear models, and that the increase in the numbers of circulation
rings as average room connectivity rises may be partly, but not entirely, responsible
for its association with cognitive ability. The study points to directions for further work,
including causal modeling, based on recommendations that could be developed for
homes to support older adults’ abilities to continue to reside in their own homes as
they grow older.
Keywords: aging in place, cognitive functioning, home connectivity
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 19
1. Introduction
The role of environment in its many forms, including the physical characteristics of space, is a
central concept in occupational therapy research, theory development, and practice. Kielhofner in
his Model of Human Occupation (Kielhofner et al., 2008) as well as Law in her Murriel Lecture (Law,
1991) discussed the role of space or physical built environment (BE) in facilitating optimal
occupational performance. But can the effects of the environment be shown to be systematic
enough for its role in occupational therapy to be practical and effective? And if such systematic
effects are found, can they be associated with specific features of the environment?
This question has recent begun to receive some attention from researchers in public health,
focusing particularly spatial layouts of residential neighborhoods. Over the last two decades,
several studies reported significant associations between features of neighborhood BE with
depression (Domènech-Abella et al., 2020; Pan et al., 2021), physical activity (Bonaccorsi et al.,
2020; Cleland et al., 2019; Yu et al., 2021), obesity, hypertension, and diabetes (Chandrabose et al.,
2019). Importantly, these are also established risk factors for dementia and/or Alzheimer’s disease
(AD) in older adults (Livingston et al., 2017; Roe et al., 2020). Recent research on BE and health is
beginning to find empirical evidence for an association between the layout of BE characteristics and
the cognitive health of older adult residents. For example, a 2015 study on neighborhood
connectivity and integration demonstrated a significant relationship between physical
neighborhood characteristics and continued cognitive performance (Watts et al., 2015). In 2019,
Besser et al. showed that the association between BE and cognition could be moderated by
apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype, a genetic risk factor for AD, thereby establishing a gene-
environment interaction (Besser et al., 2019). These studies proceed from the assumption that the
layout of streets can create significant differences of affordances to neighborhood services and
markets, walkable areas, and security between individual homes. The differential affordances can
lead to differences in cognitive behaviors and explain the observed association between BE layout
parameters and cognitive health. Decline in cognitive health is a key predictor of dementia and AD
(Livingston et al., 2017).
It is possible to argue that the association between BE and cognition is not just due to overt
behavioral consequences of differential opportunities of affordance but due to latent cognitive
factors as well. For example, a recent survey of research on designing facilities for individuals
suffering from dementia reports a few empirical studies exploring the idea that building
configuration or layout may influence residents’ orientation, wandering habits as well as social
interactions (Day, 2002). There is however lack of explicit theory as a result of which intriguing
empirical findings are left unexplained (Day, 2002: 380).
One basis for a possible theory is the fact that people do not rely merely on immediate
perceptual information or affordances from the environment in order to negotiate it, but also
construct and invoke cognitive maps of environments as part of their plans, strategies, and schemas
for conducting everyday activities (Downs, 1973; Gati & Tversky, 1984; Tversky, 2003; Tversky &
Hemenway, 1984). The cognitive map that we normally use to learn and navigate the layout of
space is constructed by firing in the hippocampal place cells in response to signals from the sensory-
motor cortex and visuo-spatial cortex (O’Keefe, 1978). Visuo-spatial memory is therefore especially
implicated in this ability. Any loss of functionality in visuo-spatial memory will impact both the
ability to form new cognitive maps and to navigate using existing maps. The evidence came from
the observation of severe deterioration of wayfinding performance when the participants were
distracted with tasks that interfered with visuo-spatial cognition (Hund, 2016). Some layouts, then,
may be easy for older adults to map cognitively and to negotiate, but other layouts may be not.
The relationship between the cognitive complexity of layouts and behavior competency must
exist at the level of the individual home as well. People construct cognitive maps of their homes as
much as they do so at the neighborhood level. A person, theoretically, uses these maps to locate
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 20
household and personal items, keep track of others in their homes, keep themselves oriented, and
order their lives through the day. The cognitive maps of the layout of the home are particularly
relevant in the case of older adults, who are likely to spend a considerable amount of their time in
their own homes and performing various activities of daily living. Older adults whose cognitive
abilities (memory and visuo-spatial cognition) suffer may find it difficult to construct cognitive maps
of their environments, and thus their ability to engage in the occupational performances of their
activities of daily living (ADLs).
If the layout induced cognitive challenges are not matched to their abilities, older adults tend to
respond with maladapted behavior. For example, reducing the levels of their occupational
activities, exhibiting learned helplessness, or showing negative affect. An idea that is well-
entrenched in environmental studies of aging in the form of competence-environmental press
model first developed by Nahemow and Lawton (1974).
Characteristics of layouts and cognitive ability
One way to manage the mismatch between competence and press that comes with aging is to
reduce the environmental press to match the lowered competence level. For example, layouts can
be simplified, all areas of the home put on a single floor, and ways found to reduce visual clutter in
the home. A useful study in this context is by Marquardt et al. (2011), who found that residents in
institutional homes with lower convexity scored higher on scores for ADLs. Convexity was
constructed as a measure on a ratio scale that combined the amount of fragmentation into convex
spaces and the proportion of spaces assigned specific labels, in which low convexity could be
interpreted as a setting that had fewer but proportionally more labeled spaces. They surmised that
low convexity would make the layout as a whole easier to memorize and so facilitate participation
in ADLs.
But another way is to create layouts that may offer just the right amount of challenge and act
as an enabler for behavioral adaptations that may even improve mental health. After all, certain
basic activities of living have to be conducted no matter what, and when compelled to do so in
environments that are challenging, but not to the extent that they prevent the activities from being
conducted, inhabitants of these environments may continue to engage in the necessary activity.
Engaging in challenging mental activity would then lead to improvement in their cognitive ability,
and the home layout may then be thought of as an active ingredient of therapeutic practice, playing
a passive role but more actively helping their residents stave off cognitive decline that comes with
age. Nahemow and Lawton (1974: 29-30) themselves suggested this line of thinking, demarcating
“a zone of maximum performance potential” with slightly challenging environmental press, in
which adaptation would take on positive effects (Figure 1).
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 21
Figure 1 The press-competence model describing the types of adaptive behavior and affect individuals exhibit under
varying conditions of environmental press and personal competence. Drawing after Nahemow and Lawton (1974).
There is a biological basis for this argument (termed “the enriched environments hypothesis” in
neuroscience literature) that cognitively challenging environments can lead to improvement in
cognitive function. However, the current evidence supporting it comes largely from animal models
(Cao et al., 2017; Fischer, 2016; Leggio et al., 2005). If such an effect can be demonstrated in humans
as well, it leads to the possibility of treating the home as a potential therapeutic aid or intervention
tool. Early results are encouraging (Leggio et al., 2005), but reviews of this work routinely raise two
kinds of questions that would need answering in order to devise effective treatments:
1) It is still not clear what exactly constitutes an enriched environment in the case of humans
(Kühn et al., 2017);
2) there is lack of theory on the basis of which treatments may be standardized (Ball et al., 2019;
Hertzog et al., 2008).
It is clear that the second of these questions depends on the knowledge required for the first.
Once some key properties of an enriched environment are understood, experiments could be
devised for developing the right level of complexity to create enriching environments and devising
standards for treatment.
Metrics for environmental complexity
Our study is intended to address the first question. More specifically, the question that
motivates our research is whether the complexity of layout can play a role in creating
environmental enrichment and, if so, what properties of layout could capture this enrichment.
Space syntax offers several indices to capture various characteristics of layouts derived from
different kinds of discretization schemes (Bafna, 2003; Hillier, 1984; Peponis, 1997; Turner et al.,
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 22
2001)
1
.The one that we favor in this case is a type of partitioning scheme known as the “boundary
map”, a very early analytical strategy (Hillier, 1984) that has gone out of favor in recent work (but
see Bafna, 2001). It is created simply by dividing the entire space of the home into discrete spaces
(rooms) using two rules: 1) there should exist a clear, sharp, and unambiguous transition between
two spaces, 2) the rooms so formed should have a distinct identity, established either through a
label or through specific elements of furnishing. Doorways (with or without doors) are typical of
such transitions, but there are other instances where a sharp threshold demarcates one room off
from anothera nook or alcove, off a larger room, or a change of ceiling height between two
spaces. The standard procedure, which we follow, is to treat the partitioned space as a graph in
which nodes and sometimes edges can be assigned numeric indices that depend upon their
positions within the graph.
In adopting the boundary map, we drop the typical requirement that the rooms formed by
partitioning be convex spaces. Defining both consistent (that is, unique) and efficient convex
partitions has been shown to be a problem without a solution in all cases (Peponis, 1997), and if
efficiency is sacrificed in order to ensure consistency, the resulting scheme produces spaces that do
not have a straightforward interpretation in terms of categories of rooms through which residents
conceive of their homes.
This procedure generates a partition that has several features to recommend it:
1. It creates graphs that can be interpreted in a straightforward manner: edges as
sharp transitions, either as doorways, or as thresholds, between distinct rooms, nodes as
rooms that can be described in terms of categories that form a part of residents’
conceptions of their homes.
2. The graphs, therefore, have a realist character; they represent not something
accidentally created in the process of shaping a building, as often happens in the case of
convex mapping schemes, but an organization that can conform closely to the residents’
cognitive maps of the spatial organization of their homes.
3. Most importantly, this approach gives us a way to create indices that are easily
computed, with measurement errors easily checked and corrected.
The metric that we think would capture the cognitive complexity of the layout most efficiently
is the average room connectivity. Computed as the average number of links associated with each
node in the boundary graph of the home, roughly as the number of doors per room in the house, it
can be taken directly as a measure of the degree of interconnectedness of the rooms of the layout.
If more doors are opened in the rooms of a house without increasing the number of rooms, then it
follows that the extra doors will create internal circuits or rings, that is, alternate pathways between
rooms. A house with a greater number of internal doors per room would offer greater variety for
movement trajectories or movement options within it and encourage a more active lifestyle. It
would, however, require a greater cognitive effort in the organization and execution of activities of
daily livingin the selection of routes through the home by comparing expected effort, for
instance, or in keeping track of other members of the household as they move around, or even in
making mental maps of the house. On the other hand, this effort cannot become prohibitively great
if the house is to maintain a sense of livability. Average room connectivity is therefore a good
measure of layout complexity: it is simple to compute; it can be directly associated with design
guidelines; and, within the range of normal houses, it is likely to vary from a value that is associated
1
Space syntax is an analytical approach to the description of built space that seeks to capture the sociologically relevant aspects of the
space of buildings and settlements. Those readers who are unfamiliar with space syntax will be able to find an accessible introduction in
Bafna (2003); those seeking a more in-depth explanation of its theoretical ideas that motivated it should consult Hillier and Hanson
(1984). Peponis and Wineman (2002) review the different questions in environment and behavior studies to which space syntax had
contributed, and so give a fairly comprehensive sense of its successes and challenges.
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 23
with very little cognitive effort, to one that is associated with just enough challenge to lead to
positive adaptations of the kind proposed by Nahemow and Lawton (1974).
2. Methods
Design and participants
This study is a cross-sectional design. Data were collected in three successive phases over 3
years. Seventy-one adults living within the greater Atlanta GA area participated in the study. These
participants were recruited via convenience sampling, consisting of fliers, and word-of-mouth.
Inclusion criteria were: (1) having an of age 60 or older, (2) living in their own home, (3) having no
known cognitive impairments, and (4) having the desire to stay in their own home. Exclusion criteria
included current orthopedic or neurological issues that prevented independent living. Any
participants who lacked adequate vision or were non-ambulatory were also excluded from this
study.
Variables
Demographic Variables: Age, gender, education, and income of all participants were collected.
As a comparison, the same measures of US population estimates were also collected from U.S.
Census Bureau data.
Variable characterizing cognitive ability: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is an
assessment that can be used to screen for cognitive impairment as well as to assess cognitive ability
and track cognitive changes over time (Koski et al., 2009). The MoCA has excellent internal
consistency (alpha = 0.83), excellent test-retest reliability (r = 0.92), and excellent concurrent
validity (r = 0.87) (Nasreddine et al., 2005). At a cutoff score of 26, the MoCA has a very high
sensitivity of detecting mild cognitive impairment (90%) and Alzheimer’s disease (100%), with a
somewhat lower specificity of 87% (Nasreddine et al., 2005). Lower than expected scores on MoCA
may therefore indicate reduced cognitive ability but not cognitive impairment as such.
Variables characterizing home layouts: Space syntactical metrics were used directly to create
variables characterizing different aspects of home layouts. Home layouts were recorded by
observers trained to draw maps of homes on-site and to record measurements of room-sizes. These
hand-drawn maps were then translated into diagrams in a generic CAD-format (.dxf) that recorded
individual rooms as polygons connected by centerlines that represented doors between them.
Depthmap version 10.14.00b (Copyright: University College London, Alasdair Turner, Eva Friedrich,
2010-11), was used to compute syntactical metrics.
Syntactical metrics are graph measures that are computed either for each node (that is, for each
room in the house), or for the entire house. Our study took the entire house as a unit of
measurement, and metrics that are computed at room level were aggregated and averaged to
produce house-level metrics. The main variable of interest, as described in the previous section,
was average room connectivitythis is computed simply as the mean number of links per node
that occur in the graph of the home. Two other syntactical metrics also featured as explanatory
variables in some of our models, but only as possible co-variates in our explorations to achieve
proper specifications of our models. One of these was the number of nodes (the number of rooms
obtained after partitioning the layout of the house), and the other, the average mean depth of all
the nodes (the mean of all shortest paths between all non-identical pairs of nodes of the graph
representing a house).
Analytical approach:
Our general analytical approach was to test different models for explaining variations in MoCA
scores, with a particular focus on average room connectivity as a predictor. We began by examining
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 24
each of the variables of interest for potential problems or to identify distinctive or unusual values.
MoCA scores were compared to published normative data to see if our sample had any unusual
characteristics. Of the explanatory variables, both demographic variables and the syntactical indices
were also compared to available public data in order in order to get a sense of the
representativeness of our sample. Correlations between explanatory variables were examined to
give us some information about how to set up multiple regression models that we would eventually
use. After this preliminary work, we constructed a series of simple regressions to check how each
of our proposed predictors performed individually and then finally constructed several multiple
regression models testing different assumptions about both nature of the relationship between the
outcome and explanatory variableswhether it was best taken to be linear or non-linearand
whether the specific characteristics of the outcome variable called for particular types of modeling
approaches. Throughout our approach focused on exploration of data and discovery rather than on
proving or disproving specific hypotheses.
The analyses and data-transformations were performed using a number of libraries available for
the R programming language, using Rstudio as the IDE (R Core Team [2020]. R: A language and
environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).The
results reported here used the base library of R as well as libraries ARM [Andrew Gelman and Yu-
Sung Su, 2020, Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models, (R package
version 1.11-2)], CAR [John Fox and Sanford Weisberg, 2019, An R Companion to Applied Regression,
Third Edition (Thousand Oaks CA: Sage)], Betareg [Achim Zeileis, Francisco Cribari-Neto, et al. 2021,
Betareg: Beta Regression (R package version 3.1-4)], and Splines2 [Wang W, Yan J, 2021, Splines2:
Regression Spline Functions and Classes (R package version 0.4.5)]. Graphics were all produced
using the base R package.
3. Results
Preliminaries
Our sample included data from 71 individuals, of which 26 were men and 45 women. All were,
by sampling design, older adults ranging in age from 60 to 92 years, distributed around a mean of
69.5 years (with a standard deviation of 8.8 years). As Table 1 shows, the level of education in our
sample was relatively high. Only 2 individuals reported education level without a high school
diploma, and 38 individuals (53%) reported having obtained a bachelor’s degree or higher. In
comparison, 54% of adults older than 65, residing in Atlanta, have a bachelor’s degree (U.S. Census
Bureau, 2019b). The income distribution in our sample (Table 1), with a median income category of
($50,000 to $74,000) also conformed quite closely to that of the city of Atlanta population (median
income = 65,385; U.S. Census Bureau, 2019a).
Table 1 Characteristics of the study sample, compared with national demographic characteristics (US Census Bureau,
2019a and 2019b)
Sample Data
Population Estimates (US)
N (%)
Mean (SD)
Range
%
Mean
Age
69.5 (8.8)
60-92
NA
Gender
Male
26 (37%)
46%
Female
45 (63%)
54%
Education
No High School
2 (2%)
12.43%
High School
31 (43%)
48.39%
College Degree
38 (53%)
39.17%
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 25
Income
Less than $20,000
7 (9%)
12 %
$20,000 to $34,999
10 (14%)
11.3 %
$35,000 to $49,999
11 (16%)
9.8 %
$50,000 to $74,999
12 (17%)
15.4 %
$75,000 to $99,999
6 (9%)
11 %
Over $100,000
25 (35%)
29.1 %
Av. Room Connectivity
1.99 (1.7)
1.61-2.36
NA
Av. Mean Depth
3.10 (0.71)
1.87-4.82
NA
MoCA30 Score
25.27 (3.46)
15-30
26 (2.3)
The MoCA scores of our sample had a mean of 25.3, with a standard deviation of 3.5 points.
Thirty-three individuals (46%) scored below the standard cut-off value of 26 that is used to identify
individuals with possible cognitive impairment. In a study to gather normative data, Rosetti et al.
(Rossetti et al., 2011) obtained a much lower mean value of 23.4 (sd = 4.0) from a sample of 2356
individuals. However, the sample included individuals between 18 and 85 years, who were not
screened for any diagnosed cognitive impairment. Another normative study (Borland et al. 2017)
with a more comparable sample of 758 adults with an age range (65-85 years) to our sample, which
also excluded any individual with severe cognitive impairment, had a mean score of 26 (sd = 2.3),
with 37.3% reporting scores below 26, reports data that conform quite well with our sample. Our
sample, in short, did not exhibit any unusual characteristics with respect to the MoCA scores.
The average connectivity scores of the homes of the individuals in our sample ranged from 1.61
to 2.36. The range appears very small but is actually well within expected limits. It can be
interpreted somewhat roughly as the average number of doorways per room of the house. Given
contemporary privacy norms, most rooms in a typical single-family house will have one doorway, a
few may have two, and very few other spaces, such as hallways, corridors, or lobbies have more
than three doorways opening into them. The number of spaces (all distinct and separable spaces in
the home that can accommodate at least one person conducting some daily activity) in these homes
ranged from 6 to 27, showing that there was a fairly good degree of variation in the size of the
homes in our sample.
Models
A simple OLS regression of MoCA scores on the average connectivity of the individuals’ homes
showed that the connectivity was able to explain 5% of their variation (p = 0.039; df = 1 & 69). The
corresponding coefficient was 5.09 MoCA score units, with a standard error of 2.42. MoCA scores
have been found to be quite susceptible to age and education in most normative studies; there is
more equivocal evidence about the role of gender in determining them. We checked with this with
a series of simple OLS regressions, with each of these variables taken as independent regressors.
Age was found to predict about 23% of the variation in MoCA scores (p = 1.17 x 10-5; df = 1 & 69).
Education (taken as an ordinal variable with three levels) was found to predict 9% of the variation
(p = 0.004, df = 1 & 69). It is conventional to treat education as a categorical variable, but our sample
data showed a consistent and linear increase in the MoCA scores between all three categories taken
in order, thus supporting the case for treating it as a single regressor on an ordinal scale. Gender
accounted for just about 1% of the variation in scores without reaching significance (p = 0.83, df =
1 & 69). Income level was also found to be a potential predictor of MoCA scores, explaining nearly
12% of the variation (p = 0.002, df = 1 & 69), but income level is correlated with education in our
sample (as it should be), and when the model was adjusted for age and education, the effect of
income disappeared (coefficient = 0.29, s.e. = 0.21, p = 0.17).
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 26
Learning from these exploratory models, we decided to adjust for both age and education, but
not for gender or income, in modeling MoCA scores by average connectivity. An OLS regression
model (see Figure 2) with three predictorsage, education, and average connectivityproduced
R2 = 0.34, (p = 7.28 x 10-7, df = 3 & 67). All three predictors reached the set significance level of 0.5
(see Appendix 1a for details of the model). The estimation of the coefficient associated with average
connectivity reduced slightly (4.61, in comparison with 5.09 from the regression with average
connectivity as the sole regressor), but it came with a gain in efficiency (s.e. = 2.08, in comparison
to 2.42 in the simple OLS regression). A comparison model of the MoCA scores with only age and
education level as predictors, produced an R2 = 0.31 (p = 1.53 x 10-6, df = 2 and 68). This means that
average connectivity was able to account for an additional 3% of the overall variation in the MoCA
scores.
Figure 2 Estimated MoCA scores expected from given the average room connectivity values of their homes, plotted over
actual values. The estimates are derived from the multiple linear regression model reported in Table 2. 2a shows the line
of best fit for 65 years old individuals educated up to high school; grey lines, representing other possible fits that might
explain the given data, indicate the uncertainty of the estimates. 2b shows estimated best fit lines for selected ages for a
high school educated individual, and 2c shows estimated best fit lines for each of the three levels of educational categories
for a median aged individuals from the study sample. Note that in all diagrams, observed values are jittered along the x-
axis to reveal overlapped datapoints, and so may not match each other precisely.
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 27
Basic diagnostic plots (Figure 3) of the regression model with age, education and average
connectivity as predictors did not reveal any serious concerns. As the plots show, very slight
heteroscedasticity as well as a slight departure from normality (hinting towards a negative
skewness) in the residuals at the extreme range. Both these points reflect the truncated character
of the MoCA scale at the upper endthe fact that MoCA scores of our sample cluster near the top,
but cannot go past the highest score of 30. The observations are not a severe threat to the validity
of the model for our data, but we discuss one possible way to address them in the section below
for the more general case. There are no points with unusually high leverage.
Figure 3 Diagnostic plots for the model presented in Appendix, Table 2.
Cognitive ability as a non-linear function of connectivity
It is reasonable to question whether the assumption that the relationship between the outcome
variable and our main predictor can be expressed as a simple linear function. MoCA scores are
constrained to lie between 0 and 30, and a large majority of healthy adults are expected to score
near the higher end. In fact, the actual distribution of the scores that we modeled is only moderately
skewed (skewness = -0.85); the residuals from our model still retain some skewness (-0.51), as our
diagnostic plots (Figure 3) show. A somewhat related worry is the possible presence of
heteroscedasticity as a result of the truncation of scores. To ameliorate this worry, we also tested
a beta-regression model, rescaling MoCA scores as fractions of the maximum values, and modeling
it as a beta distributed variable that ranges between 0 and 1 (Appendix 1b). The AIC value of 149.6
of the beta regression model is much lower as compared to AIC of 131.0 for an ordinary linear
regression model of MoCA scores on the same scale, so it does offer improved efficiency. But within
the range of connectivity values normally obtained in residences, the parameter estimates
themselves close enough to the linear model to make the simpler linear model acceptable (Figure
4a; Appendix 1b).
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 28
Figure 4 Models exploring non-linear associations between MoCA scores and Average room connectivity 4a compares
two fit curves; the dotted curve is the best fit line obtained for 70 years old individuals educated up to high school from
a generalized linear model with gaussian distribution and identity link (in essence, an ordinary least squares multiple
regression model); the solid curve is the best fit line from a beta regression model for individuals with the same
characteristics (details in Appendix, Table 3); MoCA scores for both fits were rescaled to lie between 0 and 1. 4b shows
the pattern of association between MoCA scores of individuals and the average connectivity of their homes revealed by
spline regression. The modeled curve shown here is a constructed using a b-spline basis (polynomials of degree = 3; df =
4), showing variation in MoCA scores predicted by average connectivity for a median aged individuals from the study
sample with a high school education.
But there are other more substantive reasons to consider possibilities of non-linear relationship
between connectivity and MoCA scores. Looking more closely at the scattergram in Figure 3, we
can that a straight line may not best capture the change in MoCA scores as connectivity changes. In
particular, there seems to be a less clear indication of a consistent positive association between the
two for homes with average room connectivity below 2. This could be just a characteristic of our
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 29
sample, but there are reasonable theoretical grounds to believe that the pattern is general. From
Euler’s well-known theorem for polygons, we know that connectivity values greater than 2 can only
arise if there are rings in the graphs, that is, if more than one paths exist between some pairs of
nodes (Bafna et al., 2019). Below this threshold of 2, the drop in connectivity values is entirely a
function of the number of rooms in the home; above this threshold, the connectivity depends upon
a combination of the number of rooms and the number of circuits. We tested the hypothesis of
non-linear relationship by modeling MoCA scores on our three predictors using a b-spline of 3
degrees for the relationship with average room connectivity, with 4 degrees of freedom. The
estimated model produces an R2 value of 0.36 (F = 7.46, Df = 6 and 64, p = 4.3 x 10-6), explaining 2%
more of the variation, as compared to the model with a linear relationship (Figure 4b). To see if this
increase in the explained proportion of variance is entirely due to average connectivity, we first
modeled the MoCA scores with age and education as predictors, and then modeled the residuals
from this fit, using only average connectivity as a predictor; the R2 value obtained was 0.06, which
was 0.02 more than the proportion of variance explained by average connectivity alone in the OLS
model. There are clear indications, therefore, that the relationship that exists between average
connectivity and MoCA scores is not linear; however, a much larger sample and a stronger theory
are required for exploring this relationship further.
4. Discussion
We must caution any enthusiastic readers against the inference that our findings offer empirical
proof that layout can influence cognitive ability. Studies of this kind, based on observational data
collected from the field, are only designed to test the extent to which the data are consistent with
some hypothesized stochastic model that describes the posited relationship between predictor and
outcome variables. The limited sample size of our study, which reflects the considerable resource
costs needed to collect layout data on homes, also calls for caution in inference. But matters are
somewhat ameliorated by the fact that our sample seems to be fairly representative of the US
population in its demographic character (Table 1), giving us some confidence in the estimates
generated from it.
Keeping these limitations in mind, we can make three reasonable inferences from the findings.
The first, is that our data are consistent with the assumption that the relationship between features
of home layout and their elderly residents’ cognitive ability has a systematic component. Second,
assuming that our sample data are representative, we can form at least a provisional judgment
about the size of the associative effect we ought to expect. In our data, the average amount of
difference in scores predicted over a standard deviation of interconnectivity found in houses
about one unit on the MoCA scalewas of the same order as that predicted by a decade of
difference in ages. But the non-deterministic componentthe between-subjects variance about
the expected differenceis still relatively high; average room connectivity accounts for 4% of the
variation in cognitive ability that is not accounted for by other factors. Third, our data clearly
indicate that increase in cognitive ability is closely associated with increased occurrence of rings in
traversal routes around the home, but that the increase of rings alone cannot account for the
predicted difference in cognitive ability; it is possible that along with the increase in rings there is
also an increase in perceived complexity of the home as the degree of interconnectivity increases,
since larger homes with more rings also increase the difficulty of forming hierarchical conceptions
of layouts.
If the results of study hold in further study, and if the substantive size of effects is judged to be
therapeutically useful, our study offers support for a different approach into designing domestic
environments for the elderly. Rather than designing home environments to only to reduce the
environmental press, the design of environments may be utilized as a way to actively promote their
health. As we had discussed in the introduction, the idea of devising a day-to-day living environment
that offers the right level of cognitive challenges that lead to improved cognitive ability, or forestalls
cognitive decline that comes with age, has been in the air since the 1970s. A key stumbling-block
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 30
has been the lack of ability to define what features of the environment can offer the right kind of
enrichment for humans. Our findings lead towards the idea that the interconnected of layouts may
be a basic feature of enriched environments.
Naturally, more work is needed to confirm these findings, to ensure that the associations they
capture are consistent, and to establish reliable effect sizes, so that practical decisions about health
outcomes may be made. The thrust of such work, if it develops, ought to be towards developing
and clarifying the underlying theory rather than just seeking to establish more precise empirical
associations with increasingly complex models.
Funding
This work was supported by Georgia State University’s Byrdine F. Lewis College of Nursing & Health
Professions Intramural Grant Program.
Acknowledgement
We would like to acknowledge our participants for their time and contribution. The following graduate
students assisted in data collections as a part of their Master’s group project: Audrey Reiner, Benjamin Allen,
Marianna Thomas, Kaitlyn Barker, Katherine Moss, and Nicole Sweeny.
References
Bafna, S. (2001). Geometrical Intuitions of Genotypes. Space syntax: 3rd International Symposium, Georgia
Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA.
Bafna, S. (2003). Space Syntax:A Brief Introduction to Its Logic and Analytical Techniques. Environment and
Behavior, 35(1), 17-29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916502238863
Bafna, S., Maitra, K., Lim, Y., Newman, W. E., & Shah, M. (2019). The Home as a Therapeutic Environment.
12th International Space Syntax Symposium, Beijing, Jiaotong University.
Ball, N. J., Mercado, E., & Orduña, I. (2019, 2019-March-06). Enriched Environments as a Potential Treatment
for Developmental Disorders: A Critical Assessment [Review]. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(466).
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00466
Besser, L., Galvin, J. E., Rodriguez, D., Seeman, T., Kukull, W., Rapp, S. R., & Smith, J. (2019, 2019/11/01/).
Associations between neighborhood built environment and cognition vary by apolipoprotein E
genotype: Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Health & Place, 60, 102188.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2019.102188
Bonaccorsi, G., Manzi, F., Del Riccio, M., Setola, N., Naldi, E., Milani, C., Giorgetti, D., Dellisanti, C., & Lorini, C.
(2020, 08). Impact of the Built Environment and the Neighborhood in Promoting the Physical Activity
and the Healthy Aging in Older People: An Umbrella Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health, 17(17).
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176127
Cao, M., Pu, T., Wang, L., Marshall, C., He, H., Hu, G., & Xiao, M. (2017, 2017/08/01/). Early enriched
physical environment reverses impairments of the hippocampus, but not medial prefrontal cortex, of
socially-isolated mice. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 64, 232-243.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2017.04.009
Chandrabose, M., Rachele, J. N., Gunn, L., Kavanagh, A., Owen, N., Turrell, G., Giles-Corti, B., & Sugiyama, T.
(2019, 01). Built environment and cardio-metabolic health: systematic review and meta-analysis of
longitudinal studies. Obes Rev, 20(1), 41-54. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12759
Cleland, C., Reis, R. S., Ferreira Hino, A. A., Hunter, R., Fermino, R. C., Koller de Paiva, H., Czestschuk, B., &
Ellis, G. (2019, 05). Built environment correlates of physical activity and sedentary behaviour in older
adults: A comparative review between high and low-middle income countries. Health Place, 57, 277-
304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2019.05.007
Day, K., & Calkins, M. (2002). Design and dementia. In A. C. R. Bechtel (Ed.), The new environmental
psychology handbook (pp. pp. 374-393). John Wiley and Sons.
Domènech-Abella, J., Mundó, J., Leonardi, M., Chatterji, S., Tobiasz-Adamczyk, B., Koskinen, S., Ayuso-
Mateos, J. L., Haro, J. M., & Olaya, B. (2020, 03). Loneliness and depression among older European
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 31
adults: The role of perceived neighborhood built environment. Health Place, 62, 102280.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2019.102280
Downs, R. M., & Stea, D. (1973). Image & environment: Cognitive mapping and spatial behavior.
AldineTransaction.
Fischer, A. (2016, 2016/05/01/). Environmental enrichment as a method to improve cognitive function.
What can we learn from animal models? Neuroimage, 131, 42-47.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.039
Gati, I., & Tversky, A. (1984, Jul). Weighting common and distinctive features in perceptual and conceptual
judgments. Cogn Psychol, 16(3), 341-370. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(84)90013-6
Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment Effects on Adult Cognitive
Development:Can the Functional Capacity of Older Adults Be Preserved and Enhanced? Psychological
Science in the Public Interest, 9(1), 1-65. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01034.x
Hillier, B., Hanson, J., & Peponis, J. (1984). What Do We Mean by Building Function? . In I. C. J. S. Powell, & S.
Lera (Ed.), Designing for Building Utilisation (pp. pp. 6172). Spon.
Hund, A. M. (2016, 2016/03/01/). Visuospatial working memory facilitates indoor wayfinding and direction
giving. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 233-238.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.008
Kielhofner, G., Lippincott, W., & Wilkins. (2008). Model of human occupation : theory and application.
http://ot.lwwhealthlibrary.com.p.atsu.edu/book.aspx?bookid=1089
Koski, L., Xie, H., & Finch, L. (2009, Sep). Measuring cognition in a geriatric outpatient clinic: Rasch analysis of
the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol, 22(3), 151-160.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988709332944
Kühn, S., Düzel, S., Eibich, P., Krekel, C., Wüstemann, H., Kolbe, J., Martensson, J., Goebel, J., Gallinat, J.,
Wagner, G. G., & Lindenberger, U. (2017, 2017/09/20). In search of features that constitute an “enriched
environment” in humans: Associations between geographical properties and brain structure. Scientific
Reports, 7(1), 11920. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-12046-7
Law, M. (1991, Oct). 1991 Muriel Driver lecture. The environment: a focus for occupational therapy. Can J
Occup Ther, 58(4), 171-180. https://doi.org/10.1177/000841749105800404
Leggio, M. G., Mandolesi, L., Federico, F., Spirito, F., Ricci, B., Gelfo, F., & Petrosini, L. (2005, 2005/08/30/).
Environmental enrichment promotes improved spatial abilities and enhanced dendritic growth in the
rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 163(1), 78-90.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.04.009
Livingston, G., Sommerlad, A., Orgeta, V., Costafreda, S. G., Huntley, J., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S.,
Burns, A., Cohen-Mansfield, J., Cooper, C., Fox, N., Gitlin, L. N., Howard, R., Kales, H. C., Larson, E. B.,
Ritchie, K., Rockwood, K., Sampson, E. L., Samus, Q., Schneider, L. S., Selbæk, G., Teri, L., & Mukadam,
N. (2017, Dec). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet, 390(10113), 2673-2734.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6
Marquardt, G., Johnston, D., Black, B. S., Morrison, A., Rosenblatt, A., Lyketsos, C. G., & Samus, Q. M. (2011).
Association of the Spatial Layout of the Home and ADL Abilities Among Older Adults With Dementia.
American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementias®, 26(1), 51-57.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317510387584
Nahemow, L., & Lawton, M. P. (1974). Towards an Ecological Theory of Adaptation and Aging. In W. F. E
Preiser (Ed.), Environmental Design Research (pp. pp. 2432). Dowden, Hutchinson, and Ross.
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., &
Chertkow, H. (2005, Apr). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild
cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc, 53(4), 695-699. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-
5415.2005.53221.x
O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford University Press.
Pan, H., Liu, Y., & Chen, Y. (2021, Jan). The health effect of perceived built environment on depression of
elderly people in rural China: Moderation by income. Health Soc Care Community, 29(1), 185-193.
https://doi.org/10.1111/hsc.13081
Peponis, J., & Wineman, J. (2002). Spatial Structure of Environment and Behavior. In R. B. Bechtel & A.
Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (p. 27`1-291). J. Wiley & Sons.
Peponis, J., Wineman, J., Rashid, M., Kim, S. H., & Bafna, S. (1997). On the Description of Shape and Spatial
Configuration inside Buildings: Convex Partitions and Their Local Properties. Environment and Planning
B: Planning and Design, 24(5), 761-781.
S. Bafna, K.K. Maitra, Y. Lim, M. Shah, Y. Chen / Association between home layout connectivity and cognitive ability in community
dwelling older adults: Implication for occupational therapy
Page | 32
Roe, J., Mondschein, A., Neale, C., Barnes, L., Boukhechba, M., & Lopez, S. (2020). The Urban Built
Environment, Walking and Mental Health Outcomes Among Older Adults: A Pilot Study. Front Public
Health, 8, 575946. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2020.575946
Rossetti, H. C., Lacritz, L. H., Cullum, C. M., & Weiner, M. F. (2011, Sep 27). Normative data for the Montreal
Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample. Neurology, 77(13), 1272-1275.
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e318230208a
Turner, A., Doxa, M., O'Sullivan, D., & Penn, A. (2001). From Isovists to Visibility Graphs: A Methodology for
the Analysis of Architectural Space. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design, 28(1), 103-121.
https://doi.org/10.1068/b2684
Tversky, B. (2003). Structures Of Mental Spaces:How People Think About Space. Environment and Behavior,
35(1), 66-80. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916502238865
Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1984, Jun). Objects, parts, and categories. J Exp Psychol Gen, 113(2), 169-197.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6242749
U.S. Census Bureau. (2019a). Selected Economic Characteristics, 2015 - 2019 American Community Survey
5-Year Data Profile. Retrived from:
https://data.census.gov/cedsci/table?tid=ACSDP5Y2019.DP2003&g=2310M2500US12060.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2019b). Selected Social Characteristics in the United States, 2015 - 2019 American
Community Survey 5-Year Data Profile. Retrieved from:
https://data.census.gov/cedsci/table?tid=ACSDP5Y2019.DP2002&g=2310M2500US12060.
Watts, A., Ferdous, F., Diaz Moore, K., & Burns, J. M. (2015). Neighborhood Integration and Connectivity
Predict Cognitive Performance and Decline. Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, 1, 2333721415599141.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2333721415599141
Yu, J., Yang, C., Zhang, S., Zhai, D., Wang, A., & Li, J. (2021, Jan). The Effect of the Built Environment on Older
Men's and Women's Leisure-Time Physical Activity in the Mid-Scale City of Jinhua, China. Int J Environ
Res Public Health, 18(3). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18031039
Resume
Dr. Bafna studies the principles that shape the built environment and govern its relationship with social,
cultural, and imaginative life. He has published empirical studies on the impact of space on human behavior,
cognition, social organization, and health, as well as critical studies of architectural works that explore topics
in representation, aesthetics, and interpretation. As a member of the school of architecture faculty at Georgia
Tech, he teaches courses in architectural theory, analysis and interpretation, and research methods. He is
currently serving as the director of the PhD program in Architecture in the College of Design. Current projects
include a book titled Imaginative Reasoning in the Shaping of Buildings.
Dr. Maitra is a Professor and the Chair and Professor of the Department of Occupational Therapy (OT) at
Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA. He is an occupational therapist and a cognitive neuroscientist.
Currently, he is working to understand the relationship between space cognition and the occupation of living
in community-living older adults. He publishes in peer-reviewed national and international journals and
regularly presents in national and international forums. Dr. Maitra received research support from federal
agencies like the National Science Foundation (NSF) and the National Parkinson Foundation besides
intramural fundings. He is a Fellow of the American Occupational Therapy Association, and he served as a
Board of Trustees of the American Occupational Therapy Foundation for several years.
Dr. Lim is a pediatric occupational therapist at Let’s Talk Therapy, Duluth, Georgia. She is also a lead clinical
instructor and supervises fieldwork students from Occupational Therapy programs. Her clinical expertise is in
strengths-based approach to autism and behavior and provides family-centered practice. Dr. Lim is a member
of American Occupational Therapy Association.
Mansi is an architect and urban designer with 11 years of experience spanning practice, research, and
teaching in India, the US, and Europe. She was mentored by Balkrishna Doshi the 2018 Pritzker Laureate at
his design laboratory in Ahmedabad, India from 2010-2014. Her research interests lie in understanding
architecture’s interaction with multiple disciplines such as psychology, linguistics, and anthropology. As a
Journal of Design for Resilience in Architecture & Planning, 2021, 2(Special Issue), 18-33
Page | 33
visiting research scholar at Georgia Institute of Technology, she has been a part of several projects exploring
the relationship between human behavior and the environment. She is currently a participant in a joint project
between the architecture and psychology schools at Georgia Tech investigating the role of physical
environments in post-pandemic work settings.
Yi-An Chen is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Occupational Therapy at Georgia State University
(GSU). Chen’s research interests mainly focus on upper extremity rehabilitation for people with neurological
disorders (e.g., stroke and Parkinson’s disease). She has been collaborating with experts and researchers from
Georgia Tech and Emory University on several different projects, with the common goal to provide patient-
centered training or design to allow better independent life of patients. Chen had her clinical training and
earned a Master’s degree in Occupational Therapy. She further pursued her Ph.D. in Biokinesiology at the
University of Southern California, concentrating on neurorehabilitation. She completed her postdoc training
at GSU Physical Therapy Department, focusing on telerehabilitation in stroke.
Appendix
Table 2 Summary details for ordinary least squares multiple regression model of MoCA with Age, Education
(categorical) and Average Room Connectivity of Home as co-predictors
Call:
lm(formula = Moca30 ~ Age + Educ2_ord + Hs_connect, data = all_data)
Coefficients:
Estimate
Std. Error
t value
Pr(>|t|)
(Intercept)
24.09
4.97
4.85
0.00
Age
-0.17
0.04
-4.13
0.00
Educ2_ord
1.51
0.66
2.30
0.02
Hs Connect
4.61
2.08
2.21
0.03
Residual standard error: 2.8 on 67 degrees of freedom
Multiple R-squared: 0.37, Adjusted R-squared: 0.34
F-statistic: 13 on 3 and 67 DF, p-value: 7.3e-07
Table 3 Summary details of Beta Regression model of MoCA (rescaled) with Age, Education (categorical) and Average
Room Connectivity of Home as co-predictors; the model for the estimation of means was specified with a logit link
function; an intercept-only model with an identity link was specified for the estimation of the precision parameter
Call:
betareg(formula = Moca_beta_scl ~ Age + Educ2_ord + Hs_connect, data = all_data)
Coefficients (mean model with logit link):
Estimate
Std. Error
z value
Pr(>|z|)
(Intercept)
1.38
1.17
1.18
0.24
Age
-0.04
0.01
-4.55
0.00
Educ2_ord
0.32
0.15
2.10
0.04
Hs_connect
1.27
0.51
2.52
0.01
Phi coefficients (precision model with identity link):
Estimate
Std. Error
z value
Pr(>|z|)
(phi)
13.52
2.28
5.92
0.00
Log-likelihood: 80 on 5 Df
Pseudo R-squared: 0.35
Number of iterations: 19 (BFGS) + 2 (Fisher scoring)
... When working in urban environments and understanding how accessibility might differ between people, the general differentiation made is rather between measures, either different centrality measures, different radii, or different distance concepts such as angular and geometric distance or primary and dual graphs (Hillier and Iida, 2005;Marshall et al., 2018;Hillier and Hanson, 1984;Feng and Zhang, 2019). There is also other research related to the overall question of Universal design and spatial configuration (Grzeschik et al., 2021;Bafna et al., 2021), which is of importance for our work, but which tends to focus on understanding what kinds of configurations are more easy or difficult to navigate for different people, not directly comparing differences in spatial models for different individuals within the same environment. In some methodological perspectives, our research builds upon and develops these kinds of approaches, but in other respects our approach is quite different as we consider the models to be different for different individuals. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Syntactic analysis focuses analyzing space from a general perspective, where connections of ‘visibility and access’ are analyzed through different geometrical abstractions. In universal design research, the point of origin is that what is ‘visible’ or ‘accessible’ is different for different people depending on for example age or disability. This paper addresses challenges in the way models are made, used and theorized in space syntax research as capturing or describing relations between people and environment, specifically from the perspective that if people are different, so are their respective ‘connections of access and visibility’, and potentially their subsequent patterns of centrality and accessibility. Such difference, as recognized in universal design research, may appear on both the local (molecular) and the global (molar) scale; i.e., the effects of accessibility difference can be on a local scale of whether a relative detour is needed or not, but also on global structures of centrality in a city or municipality as a whole. However, there is need for structured understanding of the ‘molar’ level, and the interrelation between scales. Following Gibson’s theory that affordances exist neither in the environment nor in species, but in the specific relation between them, this paper engages with how such differences can be empirically, methodologically and theoretically understood based on empirical research in the city of Uppsala, Sweden. This work utilizes municipal data and input from disability rights organizations to explore the problem, and presents important considerations for both research and practice as comes to inclusiveness in spatial analysis and accessibility.
Article
In response to the growing importance of AI-driven residential design and the lack of dedicated evaluation metrics, we propose the Residential Floor Plan Assessment (RFP-A), a comprehensive framework tailored to architectural evaluation. RFP-A consists of multiple metrics that assess key aspects of floor plans, including room count compliance, spatial connectivity, room locations, and geometric features. It incorporates both rule-based comparisons and graph-based analysis to ensure design requirements are met. A comparison of RFP-A and existing metrics was conducted both qualitatively and quantitatively, and it was revealed that RFP-A provides more robust, interpretable, and computationally efficient assessments of the accuracy and diversity of generated plans. We evaluated the performance of six existing floor plan generation models using RFP-A, showing that, surprisingly, only HouseDiffusion and FloorplanDiffusion achieved accuracies above 90%, while other models scored below or around 60%. We further conducted a quantitative comparison of diversity, revealing that FloorplanDiffusion, HouseDiffusion, and HouseGAN each demonstrated strengths in different aspects—graph structure, spatial location, and room geometry, respectively—while no model achieved consistently high diversity across all dimensions. In addition, existing metrics can not reflect the quality of generated designs well, and the diversity of the generated designs depends on both the model input and structure. Our study not only enhances the assessment of generated floor plans but also aids architects in utilizing numerous generated designs effectively.
Book
Full-text available
This research report is in Swedish and presents findings from a research project on universal design from a systems perspective. This perspective includes “systems” in several aspects: planning systems, political systems, organizations, social systems, and spatial systems. The focus is on planning systems and spatial systems, addressing chal- lenges and queries in how to understand molar (larger scale, system) perspectives of universal design as interlinked with molecular (local) aspects. It contains a general discussion and conclusion of the project as a whole and three studies, focusing on process and governance (by Björn Hellström), developing models and methods (by Ann Legeby), and investigating universal design challenges in relation to system world, life world, and material world (by Daniel Koch) respectively. It contains a brief English summary at the end.
Article
Full-text available
Physical activity has been suggested to be beneficial in preventing disease and improving body function in older people. Older people’s leisure-time physical activity (LTPA) is affected by various factors, especially environmental factors. However, the differences in the association between older people’s LTPA and the built environment in different sex groups remain unclear. Perceived built environment scores and older people’s LTPA were collected for 240 older people in Jinhua using the Neighborhood Environment Walkability Scale and International Physical Activity Questionnaire, respectively. A linear regression method was used to analyze the associations between older people’s LTPA and the built environment in men, women, and all participants. The results showed that land use mix diversity was associated with LTPA in older people for both sexes. In men, LTPA was also associated with access to services. However, in women, LTPA was associated with residential density, street connectivity, and crime safety. The relationship varied when demographic variables were incorporated into the regression analysis. Those results indicated that a shorter perceived distance from home to destination would motivate older people to engage more in LTPA. Older people’s LTPA was affected by various built environment factors according to different sex groups. Women’s LTPA was generally more sensitive to the built environment. More studies are needed to confirm the association between LTPA in older people and the built environment in men and women in mid- or small-sized Chinese cities in the future.
Article
Full-text available
The benefits of walking in older age include improved cognitive health (e.g., mental alertness, improved memory functioning) and a reduced risk of stress, depression and dementia. However, research capturing the benefits of walking among older people in real-time as they navigate their world is currently very limited. This study explores cognitive health and well-being outcomes in older people as they walk in their local neighborhood environment. Residents from an independent living facility for older people (mean age 65, n = 11) walked from their home in two dichotomous settings, selected on the basis of significantly different infrastructure, varying levels of noise, traffic and percentage of green space. Employing a repeated-measures, cross over design, participants were randomly allocated to one of two groups, and walked on different days in an urban busy “gray” district (a busy, built up commercial street) vs. an urban quiet “green” district (a quiet residential area with front gardens and street trees). Our study captured real-time air quality and noise data using hand-held Airbeam sensors and physiologic health data using a smart watch to capture heart rate variability (a biomarker of stress). Cognitive health outcome measures were a pre- and post-walk short cognitive reaction time (SRT) test and memory recall of the route walked (captured via a drawn mental map). Emotional well-being outcomes were a pre- and post-walk mood scale capturing perceived stress, happiness and arousal levels. Findings showed significant positive health benefits from walking in the urban green district on emotional well-being (happiness levels) and stress physiology (p < 0.05), accompanied by faster cognitive reaction times post-walk, albeit not statistically significant in this small sample. Cognitive recall of the route varied between urban gray and urban green conditions, as participants were more likely to rely on natural features to define their routes when present. The environmental and physiologic data sets were converged to show a significant effect of ambient noise and urban conditions on stress activation as measured by heart rate variability. Findings are discussed in relation to the complexity of combining real-time environmental and physiologic data and the implications for follow-on studies. Overall, our study demonstrates the viability of using older people as citizen scientists in the capture of environmental and physiologic stress data and establishes a new protocol for exploring relationships between the built environment and cognitive health in older people.
Article
Full-text available
(1) Background: The aim of this study is to establish which specific elements of the built environment can contribute to improving the physical activity of self-sufficient, noninstitutionalized and living in the city adults > 65 years. (2) Methods: An extensive literature search was conducted in several database. Umbrella review methodology was used to include the reviews that presented a sufficient methodological quality. (3) Results: Eleven reviews were included. The elements positively associated with physical activity in older adults were: walkability; residential density/urbanization; street connectivity; land-use mix-destination diversity; overall access to facilities, destinations and services; pedestrian-friendly infrastructures; greenery and aesthetically pleasing scenery; high environmental quality; street lighting; crime-related safety; traffic-related safety. The elements that were negatively associated with physical activity were: poor pedestrian access to shopping centers; poor pedestrian-friendly infrastructure and footpath quality; barriers to walking/cycling; lack of aesthetically pleasing scenery; crime-related unsafety; unattended dogs; inadequate street lighting and upkeep; traffic; littering, vandalism, decay; pollution; noise. (4) Conclusions: Evidence shows that specific elements of the built environment can contribute to promoting older people’s physical activity. The city restructuring plans should take into consideration these factors.
Article
Full-text available
The beneficial effects of enriched environments have been established through a long history of research. Enrichment of the living conditions of captive animals in the form of larger cages, sensory stimulating objects, and opportunities for social interaction and physical exercise, has been shown to reduce emotional reactivity, ameliorate abnormal behaviors, and enhance cognitive functioning. Recently, environmental enrichment research has been extended to humans, in part due to growing interest in its potential therapeutic benefits for children with neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs). This paper reviews the history of enriched environment research and the use of enriched environments as a developmental intervention in studies of both NDD animal models and children. We argue that while environmental enrichment may sometimes benefit children with NDDs, several methodological factors need to be more closely considered before the efficacy of this approach can be adequately evaluated, including: (i) operationally defining and standardizing enriched environment treatments across studies; (ii) use of control groups and better control over potentially confounding variables; and (iii) a comprehensive theoretical framework capable of predicting when and how environmental enrichment will alter the trajectory of NDDs.
Article
Full-text available
Enriched environments elicit brain plasticity in animals. In humans it is unclear which environment is enriching. Living in a city has been associated with increased amygdala activity in a stress paradigm, and being brought up in a city with increased pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (pACC) activity. We set out to identify geographical characteristics that constitute an enriched environment affecting the human brain. We used structural equation modelling on 341 older adults to establish three latent brain factors (amygdala, pACC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)) to test the effects of forest, urban green, water and wasteland around the home address. Our results reveal a significant positive association between the coverage of forest and amygdala integrity. We conclude that forests may have salutogenic effects on the integrity of the amygdala. Since cross-sectional data does not allow causal inference it could also be that individuals with high structural integrity choose to live closer to forest.
Article
This study explored the health effect of perceived built environment on depression of the Chinese elderly people in rural areas. A multifaceted approach in examining perceived built environment was used. A sample of 670 older residents in rural area of China were interviewed, and data were collected mainly for exploratory factor analysis, confirmatory factor analysis and moderation analysis. Results showed a reliable construct of perceived built environment which was developed by this study. A significant moderating role of personal income per month in the significantly negative relationship between perceived built environment and depression was confirmed. That is, the health effect of perceived built environment on depression became weakened among people of higher income levels. This study added evidence for the health effect of perceived built environment on depression among Chinese elderly people. Additionally, individual income was identified as an important influencing factor in such effect. Discussion on the negative relationship between perceived built environment and depression, as well as the moderating effect of personal income in such relationship were presented.
Article
Depression and loneliness act in a synergistic way among older adults. We tested two indicators of the perceived neighborhood built environment (BE) as moderators of the association between these conditions in older Eu-ropean adults. Positive perceptions of neighborhood BE were related to lower levels of loneliness but not to major depressive disorder (MDD). Reporting low BE usability was significantly related to a higher likelihood of feeling lonely except for those suffering from MDD, whereas reporting low BE walkability was significantly related with a high likelihood of loneliness particularly among those with MDD. Therefore, improving neighborhood BE and, specifically, its walkability, might result in a reduction in the prevalence of loneliness.
Article
We examined whether neighborhood built environment (BE) and cognition associations in older adults vary by apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype, a genetic risk factor for Alzheimer's disease (AD). We conducted a cross-sectional analysis of 4091 participants. Neighborhood characteristics included social and walking destination density (SDD, WDD), intersection density, and proportion of land dedicated to retail. Individuals were categorized as APOE ε2 (lower AD risk), APOE ε4 (higher AD risk), or APOE ε3 carriers. Among APOE ε2 carriers, greater proportion of land dedicated to retail was associated with better global cognition, and greater SDD, WDD, intersection density, and proportion of land dedicated to retail was associated with better processing speed. These associations were not observed in APOE ε3 or ε4 carriers. APOE ε2 carriers may be more susceptible to the potentially beneficial effects of denser neighborhood BEs on cognition; however, longitudinal studies are needed.
Article
Built environment attributes may be related to cardio‐metabolic diseases (e.g. type 2 diabetes, heart disease and stroke) and their risk factors, potentially by influencing residents' physical activity. However, existing literature reviews on the built environment and health for the most part focus on obesity as the outcome and rely on cross‐sectional studies. This systematic review synthesized current evidence on longitudinal relationships between built environment attributes and cardio‐metabolic health outcomes among adults and on the potential mediating role of physical inactivity. By searching eight databases for peer‐reviewed journal articles published in the English language between January 2000 and July 2016, the review identified 36 articles. A meta‐analysis method, weighted Z‐test, was used to quantify the strength of evidence by incorporating the methodological quality of the studies. We found strong evidence for longitudinal relationships of walkability with obesity, type 2 diabetes and hypertension outcomes in the expected direction. There was strong evidence for the impact of urban sprawl on obesity outcomes. The evidence on potential mediation by physical activity was inconclusive. Further longitudinal studies are warranted to examine which specific built environment attributes influence residents' cardio‐metabolic health outcomes and how physical inactivity may be involved in these relationships.