ArticlePDF Available

The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive phase model of problem posing

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The aim of this study is to develop a descriptive phase model for problem-posing activities based on structured situations. For this purpose, 36 task-based interviews with pre-service primary and secondary mathematics teachers working in pairs who were given two structured problem-posing situations were conducted. Through an inductive-deductive category development, five types of activities (situation analysis, variation, generation, problem-solving, evaluation) were identified. These activities were coded in so-called episodes, allowing time-covering analyses of the observed processes. Recurring transitions between these episodes were observed, through which a descriptive phase model was derived. In addition, coding of the developed episode types was validated for its interrater agreement.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-021-10136-y
1 3
The process ofproblem posing: development ofadescriptive
phase model ofproblem posing
LukasBaumanns1 · BenjaminRott1
Accepted: 29 November 2021 /
© The Author(s) 2021
Abstract
The aim of this study is to develop a descriptive phase model for problem-posing activities
based on structured situations. For this purpose, 36 task-based interviews with pre-service
primary and secondary mathematics teachers working in pairs who were given two struc-
tured problem-posing situations were conducted. Through an inductive-deductive category
development, five types of activities (situation analysis, variation, generation, problem-
solving, evaluation) were identified. These activities were coded in so-called episodes,
allowing time-covering analyses of the observed processes. Recurring transitions between
these episodes were observed, through which a descriptive phase model was derived. In
addition, coding of the developed episode types was validated for its interrater agreement.
Keywords Problem posing· Phase model· Cognitive processes· Problem solving
1 Introduction
In re mathematica ars proponendi quaestionem pluris facienda est quam solvendi.
(Cantor, 1867, p. 26)
Transl.: In mathematics, the art of posing a question is of greater value than solving
it.
In his statement, Cantor emphasizes the importance of the ability to pose substantial
questions within mathematics. In fact, problem posing is considered a central activity of
mathematics (Hadamard, 1945; Halmos, 1980), and at the latest since the 1980s (Brown &
Walter, 1983; Butts, 1980; Kilpatrick, 1987), it is being investigated with growing interest
by mathematics education researchers. Since the 1990s, it has been widely used to identify
or assess mathematical creativity and abilities (Silver, 1994, 1997; Singer & Voica, 2015;
Van Harpen & Sriraman, 2013; Yuan & Sriraman, 2011). Silver (1997, p. 76) emphasizes
that to grasp such constructs, both products and processes of problem-posing activities can
be considered. However, a strong product orientation within research on problem posing
is noticeable (Bonotto, 2013; Singer etal., 2017; Van Harpen & Sriraman, 2013); that is,
* Lukas Baumanns
lukas.baumanns@uni-koeln.de
1 University ofCologne, Cologne, Germany
Published online: 27 December 2021
Educational Studies in Mathematics (2022) 110:251–269
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
studies aiming to assess mathematical creativity, for example, often focus on the posed
problems rather than the processes that led to them. This is noteworthy, since processes are
central to educational research. As Freudenthal (1991) states:
[T]he use of and the emphasis on processes is a didactical principle. Indeed, didac-
tics itself is concerned with processes. Most educational research, however, and
almost all of it that is based on or related to empirical evidence, focuses on states (or
time sequences of states when education is to be viewed as development). States are
products of previous processes. As a matter of fact, products of learning are more
easily accessible to observation and analysis than are learning processes which, on
the one hand, explains why researchers prefer to deal with states (or sequences of
states), and on the other hand why much of this educational research is didactically
pointless. (p. 87, emphases in original)
Although there are studies considering problem-posing processes (Headrick etal., 2020;
Ponte & Henriques, 2013), general knowledge about learners’ problem-posing processes
remains limited (Cai & Leikin, 2020). Only a few studies are dedicated to the development
of a phase model for problem posing (Cruz, 2006; Pelczer & Gamboa, 2009). Those mod-
els still hold the potential for sufficient generalization and validation. This knowledge could
help to develop a more sophisticated process-oriented perspective on problem posing. The
few studies that examine the general process of problem posing (Koichu & Kontorovich,
2013; Patáková, 2014; Pelczer & Rodríguez, 2011) may benefit from a validated phase
model. Such a model may also be useful for the effective educational use of problem pos-
ing in the classroom. This study aims to develop a valid and reliable category system that
allows analyzing problem-posing processes. These kinds of conceptual frameworks play
a central role in mathematics education research as they enable a better understanding of
thinking processes (Lester, 2005; Schoenfeld, 2000).
2 Theoretical background
2.1 Problem posing
There are two widespread definitions of problem posing which are used or referred to in
most studies on the topic. As a first definition, Silver (1994, p. 19) describes problem pos-
ing as the generation of new problems and reformulation of given problems. Silver con-
tinues that both activities can occur before, during, or after a problem-solving process. As
a second definition, Stoyanova and Ellerton (1996, p. 218) refer to problem posing as the
“process by which, on the basis of mathematical experience, students construct personal
interpretations of concrete situations and formulate them as meaningful mathematical
problems.” In the following, we adopt the definition of Silver (1994) as the differentiation
between the activities of generation and reformulation is beneficial for identifying differ-
ent activities in problem-posing processes. However, both definitions are not disjunctive or
contradictory but describe equivalent activities.
In both definitions, the term problem is used for any kind of mathematical task,
whether it is a routine or a non-routine problem (Pólya, 1966). For the former, “one has
ready access to a solution schema” (Schoenfeld, 1985b, p. 74), and for the latter, one
has no access to a solution schema. Thus, problem posing can lead to any kind of task
252 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
on the spectrum between routine and non-routine problems (Baumanns & Rott, 2019;
Baumanns & Rott, 2021a).
Stoyanova and Ellerton (1996) distinguish between free, semi-structured, and struc-
tured problem-posing situations depending on the degree of structure. A situation is an
ill-structured problem in the sense that its goal cannot be determined by all given ele-
ments and relationships (Stoyanova, 1997). Because this study focuses on structured
situations and Baumanns and Rott (2021a) encountered difficulties in distinguishing free
and semi-structured situations, in this article, we distinguish between unstructured and
structured situations. Unstructured situations form a spectrum of situations without an
initial problem. The given information of these situations reaches from nearly none (see
Table1, situation 1) to open situations with numerous given information, the structure
of which must be explored by using mathematical knowledge and mathematical con-
cepts (see Table1, situation 2). In structured situations, people are asked to pose fur-
ther problems based on a specific problem, for example, by varying its conditions (see
Table1, situation 3). The phase model developed in this article aims to describe prob-
lem-posing activities that are induced by situations like those in Table1. In particular,
the model is developed using structured situations.
Table 1 Unstructured and structured problem-posing situations
253The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
2.2 Process ofproblem posing—state ofresearch
Because products may be more accessible by analysis than processes (Freudenthal, 1991),
most problem-posing studies focus on posed problems (Bicer etal., 2020; Van Harpen
& Presmeg, 2013; Yuan & Sriraman, 2011). However, consideration of the processes
increases in recent studies (Cai & Leikin, 2020; Crespo & Harper, 2020; Headrick etal.,
2020; Koichu & Kontorovich, 2013; Patáková, 2014; Pelczer & Rodríguez, 2011). Ponte
and Henriques (2013), for example, examine the problem-posing process in investiga-
tion tasks among university students and found that problem posing and problem-solving
complement each other in generalizing or specifying conjectures to obtain more general
knowledge about the mathematics contents. Christou etal. (2005) describe four thinking
processes that occur within problem posing, namely editing, selecting, comprehending/
organizing, and translating quantitative information. They found the most able students are
characterized through editing and selecting processes. However, compared to the present
study, these activities do not tend to describe problem-posing processes by phases. Instead,
Christou etal. (2005) intend to characterize thinking processes in problem posing. Cifarelli
and Cai (2005) include problem posing in their model to describe the structure of math-
ematical exploration in open-ended problem situations. They identify a recursive process in
which reflection on aproblem’s solutions serves as the source of new problems.
The studies cited above differ from the present study as follows: They describe and
analyze only individual processes, they describe the problem-posing process in terms
of thinking processes rather than phases, or they consider problem-posing processes as
a sub-phase of a superordinate process. However, there is a lack of studies that attempt
to derive a general, descriptive phase model of observed problem-posing processes
themselves from numerous processes. For problem-solving research, the analysis of pro-
cesses through phase models has been established at least since Pólya’s (1945) and Sch-
oenfeld’s (1985b) seminal works. While their models are normative, which means they
function as advice on how to solve problems, newer empirical studies on the process of
problem-solving develop and investigate descriptive models, which means they portray
how problems are actually solved by participants (Artzt & Armour-Thomas, 1992; Rott
etal., 2021; Yimer & Ellerton, 2009). This study also focuses on descriptive models.
Some researchers interpret problem posing as a problem-solving activity (Arıkan
& Ünal, 2015; Kontorovich etal., 2012; Silver, 1995), and there are several established
models of problem-solving processes (e.g., Mason etal., 1982; Pólya, 1945). Therefore,
it is a reasonable question whether a separate phase model for problem posing is needed.
From the observations of problem-solving and problem-posing activities within the present
study, we share the argument by Pelczer and Gamboa (2009) that the cognitive processes
involved in problem posing are of their own nature and cannot be adequately described by
the phase models of problem solving. For problem posing, Cai etal. (2015) state, “there
is not yet a general problem-posing analogue to well-established general frameworks for
problem solving such as Polya’s (1957) four steps” (p. 14).
To find existing research on this topic, we conducted a systematic literature review
(Baumanns & Rott, 2021a, 2021b). This review encompassed articles from high-ranked
journals of mathematics education, the Web of Science, PME proceedings, the 2013 and
2020 special issues in Educational Studies of Mathematics, the 2020 special issue in
International Journal of Educational Research, and two edited books on problem posing
(Felmer etal., 2016; Singer etal., 2015). From all reviewed articles, three were dedicated
to the development of general phases in problem posing similarly to the present study.
254 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
Cruz (2006) postulates a phase model based on a training program for teachers (see
Fig.1). For this reason, this phase model is preceded by educative needs and goals. Once
a concrete teaching goal has been set (1), the episode type of problem formulating begins
(2). This episode has a problem as its output which is then solved (3). If it cannot be solved,
the problem may have to be reformulated (4). A solvable problem is further developed
in the episode type problem improving (5). The complexity of the problem is adapted to
the learning group and compared with the goal (6 and 7). If the comparison shows that
the problem is not suitable, either further changes are made to the task (8) or the task is
rejected as unsuitable.
Pelczer and Gamboa (2009) distinguish five phases—setup, transformation, formula-
tion, evaluation, and final assessment—based on the analysis of problem-posing processes
in unstructured situations. The setup includes the definition of the mathematical context
of a situation and the reflection on the knowledge needed to understand the situation. This
assessment serves as a starting point for the subsequent process. During the transforma-
tion, the conditions of a problem are analyzed, and possibilities for modification are identi-
fied, reflected, and executed. In the formulation, all activities related to the formulation of
a task are summarized. This includes the consideration of different possible formulations
of the problem as well as an evaluation of these formulations. In the evaluation, a posed
problem is assessed in terms of various aspects, for example, whether it fulfills the initial
conditions or further modifications are needed. In the final assessment, the process of pos-
ing a problem is reflected upon, and the problem itself is evaluated, for example, in terms
of difficulty and interest. In their study, Pelczer and Gamboa (2009) compare experts’ and
novices’ problem-posing processes, identifying different trajectories, that is, transitions
between the stages. While experts more often go through recursive processes, processes of
novices are more linear and often occur without transformation and final assessment.
Koichu and Kontorovich (2013) developed four stages, observed in the context of two
successful problem-posing activities: (1) In the warming-up phase, typical problems spon-
taneously associated with the given situation are posed that serve as a starting point. (2) In
the phase searching for an interesting mathematical phenomenon, participants concentrate
on selected aspects of the given task to identify interesting aspects that can be used for
forthcoming problems. (3) Since the intention is to develop interesting problem formula-
tions, in the phase hiding the problem-posing process in the problem formulation, the pos-
ers try to disguise to the potential solvers in which way the task was created. (4) Finally, in
the reviewing phase, the posers evaluate the problems based on individual criteria such as
the degree of difficulty or appropriateness for a specific target group.
In general, Cruz’ (2006) phase model does not allow for sufficient generalization to
processes of sample groups that do not pursue school learning goals such as students or
mathematicians. The model by Pelczer and Gamboa (2009) has the potential to verify the
validity by checking objective coding. The stages by Koichu and Kontorovich (2013) are
developed on a small sample of two people and therefore need to be tested for applicability
to larger sample groups. All these potentials will be addressed in this article. In addition,
Fig. 1 Phase model of problem
posing by Cruz (2006)
255The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
although the models presented have certain similarities, they also show numerous charac-
teristic differences. In comparison, phase models for problem-solving (Artzt & Armour-
Thomas, 1992; Pólya, 1945; Rott etal., 2021; Schoenfeld, 1985b; Yimer & Ellerton, 2009)
share a very similar core structure. Thus, there is a conceptual and empirical need for a
generally applicable model for problem-posing research.
The need for developing a phase model for problem posing is, furthermore, based on
our general observation that the quality of the posed problems did not always match the
quality of the observed activity. In our opinion, it is therefore not enough to consider only
the products when, for example, problem posing is used to assess mathematical creativity.
Furthermore, developing a process-oriented framework serves as research for discussing
and analyzing these processes (Fernandez etal., 1994, p. 196).
2.3 Research questions
The research goal of this study is to develop a descriptive phase model for problem-posing
activities based on structured situations. The lack of phase models constitutes a desidera-
tum from which the following research questions emerge:
(1) Which recurring and distinguishable activities can be identified when dealing with
structured problem-posing situations?
(2) What is the general structure (i.e., sequence of distinguishable activities) of the
observed processes from which a descriptive phase model may be derived?
The goal of these research questions is to develop a descriptive phase model that allows
analyzing problem-posing processes. To evaluate the quality of this model, we draw on the
criteria by Schoenfeld (2000) that can be used for evaluating models in mathematics edu-
cation. As this type of coding is highly inferential (Rott etal., 2021; Schoenfeld, 1985b),
special emphasis is given to interrater agreement.
3 The study
3.1 Data collection
The present study is a generative study that aims to “generate new observation categories
and new elements of a theoretical model in the form of descriptions of mental structures
or processes that explain the data” (Clement, 2000, p. 557). For such studies, a less struc-
tured, qualitative approach is appropriate that is open to unexpected findings (Döring &
Bortz, 2016, p. 192), such as task-based interviews. Task-based interviews have particu-
larly been used in problem-solving research to gain insights into the cognitive processes
of participants (Konrad, 2010, p. 482). The interviews were conducted in pairs to create a
more natural communication situation and eliminate the constructed pressure to produce
something mathematical for the researcher (Schoenfeld, 1985a, p. 178). Johnson and John-
son (1999) also underline that cooperative learning groups such as pairs are “windows into
students’ minds” (p. 213). For this reason, the interviewer avoided intervening in the inter-
action process.
The interviews were conducted with 64 pre-service primary and secondary mathemat-
ics teachers (PST). The PSTs worked in pairs on one of two structured problem-posing
256 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
situations, either (A) Nim game or (B) Number pyramid, which are presented in Table2.
The participants were informed that both problemsolving and problem posing were cen-
tral. After the initial problemsolving, both situations stated: “Based on this task, pose as
many mathematical tasks as possible.” This open and restriction-free question should stim-
ulate a creative process. A common question of understanding from participants was, using
the example of situation (A), whether they should now pose further Nim games or were
also allowed to depart from them. This decision was left to the PSTs’ creativity.
In total, 15 processes of situation (A) and 17 processes of situation (B), ranging from
9 to 25min, have been recorded and analyzed. The processes ended when no ideas for
further problems emerged from the participants. In total, 7h and 46min of video material
were recorded and analyzed. Thus, the processes had an average length of 14.5min. Four
pairs of PSTs each were in the same room under authentic university seminar conditions. A
camera was positioned opposite the pairs capturing all the participants’ actions. To accus-
tom them to natural communication in front of the camera, short puzzles were performed
before problem posing.
3.2 Data analysis
For data analysis, we adapted Schoenfeld’s (1985b) verbal protocol analysis, originally
used to analyze problem-solving processes. This method is an event-based sampling. Com-
pared to time-based sampling, the processes are not divided into fixed time segments (e.g.,
30s), which are then coded. Instead, new codes are set when the participants’ behavior
changes. This method has two steps: At first, the recorded interviews are segmented into
“macroscopic chunks of consistent behavior” (Schoenfeld, 1985b, p. 292) that are called
episodes in which “an individual or a problem-solving group is engaged in one large task
[...] or closely related body of tasks in the service of the same goal” (Schoenfeld, 1985b, p.
292). In a second step, the episodes are then characterized in terms of content.
Table 2 Structured problem-posing situations used in this study
Situation
(
A)
Nim game
There are 20 stones on the table. Two players A and B ma y
alternately remove one or two stones from the table.
Whoever makes the last move wins. Can player A, who
starts, win safely? Based on this task, pose as many
mathematical tasks as possible.
(cf. Schupp, 2002, p. 92)
(
B)
Number pyramid
In the following numb er pyramid, which number is in 8th place from the right in the
67th line?
Based on this task, pose as many mathematical tasks as possible.
(cf. Stoyanova, 1997, p. 70)
257The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
To answer the first research question, verbal protocol analyses were employed in terms
of inductive category development (Mayring, 2014, pp. 79–87), meaning that the episode
types were developed data-derived. The descriptions of the episode types were addition-
ally concretized in a theory-based manner. For that, the above-mentioned conceptual and
empirical findings of problem-posing research (Cruz, 2006; Pelczer & Gamboa, 2009; Sil-
ver, 1994), as well as findings of research on phase models in problemsolving (Pólya,
1945; Schoenfeld, 1985b), were used. This procedure aims to develop exclusive and
exhaustive codes (Cohen, 1960), that is, episode types, that can be assigned to the observed
problem-posing processes.
To answer the second research question, recurring sequences of the episode types were
identified to develop a general phase model. Both general sequences in the observed pro-
cesses, as well as conceptual insights about problem-posing activities in general, were con-
sidered. To analyze the interrater agreement, an independent second coder was trained. At
first, the second coder was given the coding manual and a process to code without further
comment. For this first coding, cases of doubt were discussed within 2h of training. After
this training, the second coder analyzed about 2h and 23 min of the total video material
of 7h and 46min which means 10 randomly chosen processes out of 32. Thus, the second
coder analyzed about 30.7% of the total video material. Finally, cases of doubt of coding
were discussed via consensual validation. These codings were used to calculate the inter-
rater agreement to the author’s coding.
The interrater agreement was calculated with the EasyDIAg algorithm by Holle and
Rein (2015). EasyDIAg provides an algorithm that converts two codes of an event-based
sampling data set into an agreement table from which Cohen’s kappa (Cohen, 1960) is cal-
culated through an iterative proportional fitting algorithm. Furthermore, in contrast to the
classical Cohen’s kappa, EasyDIAg provides an interrater agreement score for each value
of a category. EasyDIAg considers raters’ agreement on segmentation and categorization
as well as the temporal overlap of the raters’ annotations. This makes this algorithm par-
ticularly suitable for assessing the interrater agreement of the event-based sampling data
set at hand. For the agreement, we used an overlap criterion of 60% as suggested by Holle
and Rein (2015). In the online supplement, we provide an example analysis of a process
that was coded by the authors and the second rater followed by the calculation of the inter-
rater agreement in this manner.
4 Results
First, to retrace the inductive-deductive category development, the problem-posing process
of the Nim game by Theresa and Ugur will be described in order to refer back to it when
describing the developed episode types. The individual episodes are described without
labelling them. The given periods indicate the minutes and seconds (mm:ss) of the respec-
tive episodes. The recorded time starts with the first attempt at posing problems after the
initial problem has been solved. Compared to other participants, Theresa and Ugur get the
solution of the Nim game quickly and without assistance.
Episode 1 (00:00–00:49): Theresa and Ugur first read the task that should initiate the
problem posing. Ugur considers whether new tasks should now be posed in relation to the
solution strategy of working backwards. Theresa considers whether the stones should be
the focus of new tasks. Afterward, both reflect again on their solution strategy and consider
to what extent they can use it for new tasks.
258 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
Episode 2 (00:49–02:14): Then other games like Connect Four or Tic-tac-toe, which
may have a winning strategy similar to the Nim game, are collected.
Episode 3 (02:14–05:50): Both participants want to figure out whether there is a
winning strategy for Tic-tac-toe. After about 3min, they assume that an optimal game
always results in a draw. They return to the Nim game and ponder whether player B also
has a chance to win safely. They conclude that player B can only win if player A does
not make the first move according to the winning strategy.
Episode 4 (05:50–07:43): They pose the task of how many stones are necessary for
player B to win safely. Afterward, the text of the task is formulated. They also ask how
many moves player A needs in order to win.
Episode 5 (07:43–09:03): The last-mentioned question of episode 4 is solved and
also generalized. Ugur says, you find the number of moves of player A by going from
the number of stones to the next higher number divisible by three, and then dividing this
number by three.
Episode 6 (09:03–09:44): Ugur suggests increasing the number of stones that can
be removed from the table. Specifically, he suggests that one to three stones can be
removed. Meanwhile, Theresa writes down these ideas.
Episode 7 (09:44–10:32): Theresa writes down the previously posed problems with-
out working on the content of the formulations.
Episode 8 (10:32–13:48): Both play the variation of the Nim game raised in episode
6. They express that they want to develop a winning strategy for this variation. They
quickly realize that player B can safely win the game since multiples of four are now
winning numbers and the 20 stones that are on the table at the beginning are already
divisible by four. They validate this strategy afterward. At the last minute, the newly
posed variation is also evaluated as exciting.
Episode 9 (13:48–14:13): Ugur wants to generalize the game further and poses the
task of how to win when the players can remove one to n stones. Theresa asks Ugur if
his goal is a general formula.
Episode 10 (14:13–15:48): This task is then solved by Ugur by transferring the struc-
ture of the solution of the initial problem to the generalization. Ugur formulates that if
you are allowed to remove one to n
1 stones, the player who has the turn must bring
the number of stones to n by his turn to win safely.
Episode 11 (15:48–16:42): Subsequently, both work on a suitable formulation for
this generalized task.
Episode 12 (16:42–18:28): Theresa notes that solving the initial problem is challeng-
ing and therefore suggests providing help for pupils. Theresa suggests that it might help
when the pupils first develop a winning strategy for the simple case that the players can
only remove one stone. Ugur suggests further help cards which can be requested by the
pupils themselves if they get stuck.
Episode 13 (18:28–19:50): Theresa wants to focus on new tasks again. They move
away from the initial problem and use the stones to create an iconic representation of
the triangular numbers (1, 3, 6, ...). They formulate the task to find a general formula to
calculate the n-th triangular number.
Episode 14 (19:50–21:33): Theresa puts the stones in rows of three so that the struc-
ture that leads to the winning strategy is more visible. She evaluates this presentation by
emphasizing the usefulness of this method for extensions of the Nim game with more
than 20 stones on the table. The process comes to an end as Theresa and Ugur, when
asked by the interviewer, agree not to generate any more ideas.
259The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
4.1 Category development ofepisode types inproblem posing
Using the described evaluation method, five episode categories were developed which
allow the observed processes to be described in a time-covering manner. These episode
categories are situation analysis, variation, generation, problem-solving, and evaluation. In
the following, the developed categories of episode types are described. The episodes of the
process by Theresa and Ugur (T&U) described above are assigned to these episode types
for a better comprehension of the episode types. In addition, we provide further anchor
examples in the online supplement. Subsequently, indications are given for coding the indi-
vidual categories. Finally, the categories are discussed regarding the state of research.
4.1.1 Situation analysis
Description During the situation analysis, the posers capture single or multiple conditions
of the initial task. They usually recognize which conditions are suitable and to what extent,
to create a new task by variation (changing or omitting single or multiple conditions) or
generation (constructing single or multiple new conditions). In addition, the subsequent
investigation of the initial task’s solution is summarized in this episode. This also includes
the creation of clues or supporting tasks that lead to the solution of the initial task.
In the process of T&U, episode 1 is coded as situation analysis as the participants still
reflect on their solution strategy. Also, episode 12 is coded as situation analysis because
both PSTs try to come up with ideas on how to support students with solving the initial
problem. A further example of other participants who capture the conditions of the initial
problem can be found in the online supplement.
Coding instructions It is not always clear when the posers are engaged in reading (see
non-content-related episodes below) or have already moved on to situation analysis.
Simultaneous coding is possible here. The creation of supporting tasks, which are sup-
posed to assist in solving the initial problem, is interpreted as an analytical examination of
the situation and is therefore coded as situation analysis.
4.1.2 Variation
Description During variation, single or multiple conditions of the initial task or a task
previously posed in the process are changed or omitted. No additional conditions are con-
structed. In addition, writing down and formulating the respective task is included under
this episode.
In the process of T&U, episodes 4, 6, 9, and 11 are coded as variation. In episode 6, for
example, Ugur varies one specific rule of the Nim game and states that the players are now
allowed to remove one to three stones from the table. In episode 9, this is further general-
ized by variation.
Coding instructions For the identification of variation, the What-If-Not-strategy by
Brown and Walter (2005) should be used. The first step of this strategy is intended to
extract the conditions of a problem. The Nim game, for example, has at least the following
five conditions: (1) 20 stones, (2) two players, (3) alternating moves, (4) one or two stones
260 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
are removed, and (5) whoever empties the table wins. This analysis should be done before
coding. Omitting or varying these analyzed conditions will be coded as variation. Also,
omitting or varying conditions of a previously posed problem is coded as variation.
4.1.3 Generation
Description During generation, tasks are raised by constructing new conditions to the
given initial task or a task previously posed in the process. Due to the possible change in
the task structure, posers sometimes explain the new task. In addition, writing down and
formulating the respective task is summarized under this episode type. Also, free associa-
tions, in which tasks similar to the initial task are reminded, are coded as generation.
In the process of T&U, episodes 2 and 13 are coded as generation. In episode 13, for
example, they move further away from the Nim game and use the stones to ask questions
about dot patterns.
Coding instructions The episode types variation and generation are not always clearly
distinguishable from each other. Although the coding focuses on the activity of the poser
and not on the emerged task, it can help to examine the characteristics of a task resulting
from variation or generation. In the case of a varied task, the question or the solution struc-
ture often remains unchanged. In the case of a generated task, there is usually a fundamen-
tally different task whose solution often requires different strategies.
4.1.4 Problem solving
Description Problemsolving describes the activity in which the posers solve a task that
they have previously posed. If a non-routine problem has been posed, the respondents go
through a shortened problem-solving process in which the phases of devising and carrying
out the plan (Pólya, 1945) are the main focus. In some cases, the posers omit to carry out
the plan if the plan already provides sufficient information on the solvability and complex-
ity of the posed problem. If a routine problem has been posed, the solution is usually not
explained, since the method of solution is known. However, longer phases of solving rou-
tine tasks are also coded as problemsolving.
In the process of T&U, episodes 3, 5, 8, and 10 are coded as problemsolving as the par-
ticipants are engaged in solving their posed problems.
Coding instructions Although solving a routine problem should be differentiated from
solving a non-routine problem, both activities are labelled with the same code. However,
the commentary of the coding should specify whether an episode is an activity of solving a
routine or a non-routine problem.
4.1.5 Evaluation
Description In the evaluation, the posers assess the posed tasks based on individually
defined criteria. In the processes observed, posers asked whether the posed problem is
solvable, well-defined, similar to the initial task, appropriate for a specific target group, or
interesting for themselves to solve. On the basis of this evaluation, the posed task is then
accepted or rejected.
261The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
In the process of T&U, episode 14 is coded as evaluation, and in episode 8, there is
a simultaneous coding of problem-solving and evaluation. In episode 8, for example, the
participants are initially engaged in problem-solving. Towards the end of this episode, they
both assess their posed problem based on their interest in solving it.
Coding instructions Often, evaluative statements are made about the course of an episode
of problem-solving, since the criteria for the evaluation of a posed problem (e.g., solvabil-
ity or interest) are based on sufficient knowledge about the solution of the posed prob-
lem. In such cases, the episode types of problem-solving and evaluation cannot be sepa-
rated empirically, which is why simultaneous coding is permitted. The criterion for this
simultaneous coding is that during an episode of problem-solving, an evaluative statement
must come within a 30-s window for a simultaneous coding to be made. For example, if
at least one evaluative statement falls during the first 30s of a problem-solving episode,
both types of episodes are coded simultaneously. If at least one evaluative statement also
falls within the following 30s of problem-solving, both episode types are again encoded
simultaneously.
4.1.6 Non‑content‑related episode types
When participants, for example, ran out of ideas or became distracted during the inter-
view, they engage in the following non-content-related activities. Such activities were
also identified in descriptive models of problemsolving (Rott etal., 2021). In the pro-
cess of T&U, episode 7 was coded as non-content-related episode.
Reading The episode of reading consists of reading the situation text as well as a shorter
exchange about what has been read to make sure that the text is understood. Since the par-
ticipants have usually already solved the initial task of the situation, the reading takes place
rather in between.
Writing In the episode of writing, posers write down the text of a problem they have
already worked out orally. Also, the posers write down the solution of a previously posed
problem. Writing is only coded if no solution or problem formulation is being worked on in
terms of content (e.g., specify the problem text).
Organization Organization includes all activities in which the poser is working on the
situation, but where no content-related work is apparent. This includes, for example, the
lengthy production of drawings.
Digression The episode digression is encoded when the posers are not engaged with the
situation. This may include informal conversations with the other person about topics that
are not related to the task (e.g., weekend activities) or looking out of the window for a long
time.
Other All episodes that cannot be assigned to any other episode type are coded as other.
262 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
4.1.7 Discussion
To provide a theoretical justification of the data-driven episode types of problem posing,
we want to connect the five episode types with the presented state of research on problem-
posing phase models.
Situation analysis In Pelczer’s and Gamboa’s (2009) phase model, we find aspects of situ-
ation analysis in their transformation stage. One sub-process of this transformation stage is
the analysis of the problem’s characteristics. Terminologically, the episode name is based
on Schoenfeld’s (1985a, 1985b) analysis, because we observed that, similar to problem-
solving, posers identify what possibilities for problem posing the given situations provide
through their conditions.
Variation Pelczer and Gamboa (2009) have aspects of variation in the stage of formula-
tion in which a problem is written down and the formulation is evaluated. Problem formu-
lating can also be found in the model by Cruz (2006). The principle of variation also plays
a central role in problemsolving. Schoenfeld (1985b), for example, suggests posing modi-
fied problems by replacing or varying the conditions of a particular problem that is difficult
to solve.
Generation Koichu and Kontorovich (2013) consider spontaneously associated problems
related to a given problem-posing situation in their model, yet this is only one aspect of
the generation described above. The distinction between variation and generation is theo-
retically already conceptualized by Silver (1994). In empirical studies on problem posing,
there are so far no objective criteria that enable distinct identification of both activities. The
phase model at hand proposes criteria for this distinction.
Problem‑solving Cruz (2006) explicitly mentions problemsolving as a stage in his prob-
lem-posing phase model. In the model by Pelczer and Gamboa (2009), problemsolving is
implicit in the evaluation phase, in which the posed problem is assessed and modified. This
is presumably done based on the solution of it.
Evaluation The stage of evaluation in the phase model by Pelczer and Gamboa (2009)
shares the same name and has similar characteristics. Cruz (2006) implicitly considers
evaluation when the posers improve the posed problem when they deem it not suitable
for a specific learning group. The activity of evaluation is closely related to the metacog-
nitive activity of the regulation of cognition (Flavell, 1979; Schraw & Moshman, 1995).
In research on problem posing, there are hardly any studies that investigate metacognitive
behavior, yet some frameworks implicitly include aspects of it. Kontorovich etal. (2012),
for example, consider aptness by means of fitness, suitableness, and appropriateness of a
posed problem.
4.2 Derivation ofadescriptive phase model forproblem posing
There is no predetermined order of episode types which means there can be transitions
from any episode type to any other. However, there is a kind of “natural order” in which
episode types appear in most processes and in which transitions often occur. This has
263The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
been indicated by the order in which the episode types were presented in Sect.4.1. It was
observed that first the conditions of a situation are grasped (situation analysis) and then
new tasks are posed through variation or generation; these tasks are solved in order to
evaluate them based on the solution. Of course, we did not observe exactly this order in
every process, but across the participants and the different problem-posing situations, parts
of this superordinate pattern were identified. Often the situation analysis was observed at
the beginning of the process and at the end of a longer phase of variation. Also typical
were frequent changes between variation or generation and problemsolving (sometimes
in combination with evaluation). Furthermore, problem posing was identified as a cycli-
cal activity. Several participants were observed to revise or to further vary their previously
posed problems. Figure2 shows the T&U’S process following Schoenfeld’s (1985b) illus-
trations of problem-solving processes. Several characteristic transitions can be observed in
this process. The vertical lines shown in this figure indicate points in time when a new task
(either by variation or by generation) was posed.
From these theoretically justifiable as well as empirically observable patterns in the
sequence of episodes, the descriptive phase model shown in Fig.3 was derived. It contains
Fig. 2 Example of a timeline chart of the problem-posing process by Theresa and Ugur as described in
Sect.4 following the illustrations by Schoenfeld (1985b)
Fig. 3 Descriptive phase model for problem posing based on structured situations
264 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
all five content-related episodes as a complete graph. All transitions indicated by arrows
can occur and have been observed empirically in the study. However, not all episode types
need to occur in a process. Several participants were observed to revise or to further vary
their previously posed problems. In addition, in most cases, not only one but several prob-
lems are posed in numerous cycles. The model reflects this observation through its cyclic
structure. The model is used to represent all these possible paths within the problem-pos-
ing process.
To check the interrater agreement, 30.7% of the total video material of 7h and 46min
was coded by a second independent rater and combined into an agreement table (see
Table3) using the EasyDIAg algorithm (Holle & Rein, 2015). As explained in Sect.4.1.5,
the episode types of problemsolving and evaluation have empirically often been observed
simultaneously, which is why simultaneous coding was allowed. We have, therefore, con-
sidered this simultaneous coding as a separate category for the verification of interrater
agreement. If the start or end of a process was coded differently in time by the two raters,
there are unlinked events in the agreement which are coded as X. The entry X–X in Table3
can, therefore, not occur empirically.
With a Cohen’s kappa of κ = 0.81, the interrater agreement is almost perfect (Landis &
Koch, 1977, p. 165). This high level of agreement is particularly gratifying as the evalua-
tion method is a highly subjective and interpretative procedure, yet the developed catego-
ries are capable of consistent coding. As anticipated, the biggest coding differences are
observed for the categories variation and generation as well as the distinction between the
categories of problemsolving, problemsolving and evaluation, and evaluation. The kappa
calculated for the separate categories are (with the abbreviations from Table3 as indices)
κSA = 0.87, κV = 0.83, κG = 0.72, κPS = 0.87, κPS/E = 0.73, κE = 0.49, and κO = 97.
5 Discussion
This study aimed to develop a valid and reliable model to describe and analyze problem-
posing processes. Schoenfeld (2000) provides eight criteria for evaluating models in math-
ematics education: (i) descriptive power, (ii) explanatory power, (iii) scope, (iv) predictive
Table 3 Agreement table for all seven categories of episodes as determined by EasyDIAg. The %overlap
parameter was set to 60%. Abbreviations: SA situation analysis, V variation, G generation, PS problem-
solving, PS/E problemsolving and evaluation (simultaneous coding), E evaluation, O others, X no match)
Rater 1
SA V G PS PS/E E OX Totalp
Rater 2
SA 22 0400 0002
60
.10
V2680 00 0227
40
.28
G0630 40 0004
00
.15
PS 000322 0003
40
.13
PS/E 044226 2003
80
.14
E00002200
40
.02
O00000042 04
20
.16
X0310000–
40
.02
Total24 81 39 38 30 4442 2621.00
p
0.09 0.31 0.15 0.14 0.11 0.02 0.17 0.01 1.00 0.81
265The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
power, (v) rigor and specificity, (vi) falsifiability, (vii) replicability, and (viii) multiple
sources of evidence. Criteria (i), (iii), (v), and (vii) will be outlined to discuss the potential
and limitations of the presented framework.
Regarding research question (1), five content-related episode types—situation analysis,
variation, generation, problemsolving, and evaluation—were identified inductively which
enable objective coding through their operationalization. The episode types of the devel-
oped phase model enable a specific descriptive perspective on all observed problem-posing
processes in the study in a time-covering manner. This description, we argue, provides a
better understanding of problem-posing processes in general (i). Furthermore and with
regard to research question (2), from the observed processes, a general structure in terms of
the sequence of the episodes was identified from which we were able to derive a descrip-
tive process model for problem posing. The high interrater agreement attests to the replica-
bility of the model (vii). The participants of the study were heterogeneous and ranged from
PSTs in the first bachelor’s semester for primary school to PSTs in the 3rd master’s semes-
ter for high school. Equally heterogeneous were the processes that could nevertheless be
analyzed by the developed model (iii). The detailed descriptions, coding instructions, and
theoretical classifications provide specificity to the terms. In the online supplement, anchor
examples serve for additional specification (v).
The model developed here provides additional insights compared to existing models
(e.g., Cruz, 2006; Pelczer & Gamboa, 2009): It distinguishes the episode types variation
and generation empirically which Silver (1994) already conceptualized theoretically. Addi-
tionally, the model encompasses non-content-related episodes for the description that have
also been identified in descriptive models of problem-solving (Rott etal., 2021).
The phase model can now be used to characterize, for example, different degrees of
quality of the problem-posing process which is still a recent topic in problem-posing
research and for which considering the products and processes seems advisable (Kon-
torovich & Koichu, 2016; Patáková, 2014; Rosli etal., 2013; Singer & Voica, 2017). Thus,
as in problem-solving research (cf. Schoenfeld, 1985b), a comparison between experts and
novices might be a fruitful approach to identify different types of problem posers. Further-
more and following the process-oriented research on problem-solving (Rott etal., 2021), it
would be conceivable that the process of posing routine tasks proceeds differently than the
process of posing non-routine problems.
Finally, possible limitations to the generalizability of the developed model will be
addressed. In general, the model offers one possible perspective on problem-posing pro-
cesses. Depending on the selected problem-posing situation, sample, or study design, it
cannot be ruled out that slightly different or even additional episode types may also occur.
We also find other perspectives on problem-posing processes in research (e.g., Headrick
etal., 2020). This study considers two specific structured situations with a non-routine ini-
tial problem. However, the developed phase model has also been successfully applied to
situations with routine initial problems and other mathematical contents within bachelor
and master theses. With small changes, the model was also successfully applied to pro-
cesses based on unstructured situations in several master theses. Moreover, this study has
PSTs as a sample. The phase model was successfully applied in bachelor and master the-
ses to other sample groups such as school students and teachers (iii). Therefore, there are
strong indications that support the generalizability of the phase model, which could still be
clarified in follow-up studies.
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 021- 10136-y.
266 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
Funding Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Declarations
We declare that this work has not been submitted for publication elsewhere.
Competing interests The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Com-
mons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly
from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
Arıkan, E. E., & Ünal, H. (2015). Investigation of problem-solving and problem-posing abilities of sev-
enth-grade students. Educational Sciences – Theory & Practice, 15(5), 1403–1416.https:// doi. org/ 10.
12738/ estp. 2015.5. 2678
Artzt, A. F., & Armour-Thomas, E. (1992). Development of a cognitive-metacognitive framework for pro-
tocol analysis of mathematical problem solving in small groups. Cognition and Instruction, 9(2), 137–
175. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1207/ s1532 690xc i0902_3
Baumanns, L., & Rott, B. (2021a). Rethinking problem-posing situations: A review. Investigations in Math-
ematics Leaning, 13(2), 59–76. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 19477 503. 2020. 18415 01
Baumanns, L., & Rott, B. (2021b). Developing a framework for characterizing problem-posing activities: A
review. Research in Mathematics Education. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 14794 802. 2021. 18970 36
Bicer, A., Lee, Y., Perihan, C., Capraro, M. M., & Capraro, R. M. (2020). Considering mathematical crea-
tive self-efficacy with problem posing as a measure of mathematical creativity. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 105(3), 457–485. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 020- 09995-8
Bonotto, C. (2013). Artifacts as sources for problem-posing activities. Educational Studies in Mathematics,
83(1), 37–55. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 012- 9441-7
Brown, S. I., & Walter, M. I. (1983). The art of problem posing. Franklin Institute Press.
Brown, S. I., & Walter, M. I. (2005). The art of problem posing (3rd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Butts, T. (1980). Posing problems properly. In S. Krulik & R. E. Reys (Eds.), Problem solving in school
mathematics (pp. 23–33). NCTM.
Cai, J., Hwang, S., Jiang, C., & Silber, S. (2015). Problem-posing research in mathematics education: Some
answered and unanswered questions. In F. M. Singer, N. F. Ellerton, & J. Cai (Eds.), Mathematical
problem posing. From research to effective practice (pp. 3–34). Springer. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
978-1- 4614- 6258-3_1
Cai, J., & Leikin, R. (2020). Affect in mathematical problem posing: Conceptualization, advances, and
future directions for research. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 105(3), 287–301. https:// doi. org/
10. 1007/ s10649- 020- 10008-x
Cantor, G. (1867). De aequationibus secundi gradus indeterminatis. Berlin, Schultz. Christou, C., Mou-
soulides, N., Pittalis, M., Pitta-Pantazi, D., & Sriraman, B. (2005). An empirical taxonomy of prob-
lem posing processes. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 37(3), 149–158. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s11858- 005- 0004-6
Christou, C., Mousoulides, N., Pittalis, M., Pitta-Pantazi, D., & Sriraman, B. (2005). An empirical taxon-
omy of problem posing processes. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 37(3), 149–158. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1007/ s11858- 005- 0004-6
Cifarelli, V. V., & Cai, J. (2005). The evolution of mathematical explorations in open-ended problem-solv-
ing situations. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 24, 302–324.
Clement, J. (2000). Analysis of clinical interviews: Foundations and model viability. In A. Kelly & R. Lesh
(Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 547–589). Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
267The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measure-
ment, 20(1), 37–46. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 00131 64460 02000 104
Crespo, S., & Harper, F. K. (2020). Learning to pose collaborative mathematics problems with secondary
prospective teachers. International Journal of Educational Research, 102. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
ijer. 2019. 05. 003
Cruz, M. (2006). A mathematical problem-formulating strategy. International Journal for Mathematics
Teaching and Learning, 79–90.
Döring, N., & Bortz, J. (2016). Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation in den Sozial- und Humanwissenschaf-
ten (5th ed.). Springer.
Felmer, P., Pehkonen, E., & Kilpatrick, J. (Eds.). (2016). Posing and solving mathematical problems.
advances and new perspectives. Springer. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ 978-3- 319- 28023-3
Fernandez, M. L., Hadaway, N., & Wilson, J. W. (1994). Problem solving: Managing it all. The Mathemat-
ics Teacher, 87(3), 195–199.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. A new area of cognitive-developmental
inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0003- 066X. 34. 10. 906
Freudenthal, H. (1991). Revisiting mathematics education. Dordrecht, Kluwer. Hadamard, J. (1945). The
psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Dover Publications.
Halmos, P. R. (1980). The heart of mathematics. The American Mathematical Monthly, 87(7), 519–524.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 2307/ 23214 15
Headrick, L., Wiezel, A., Tarr, G., Zhang, X., Cullicott, C., Middleton, J., & Jansen, A. (2020). Engagement
and affect patterns in high school mathematics classrooms that exhibit spontaneous problem posing:
An exploratory framework and study. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 105, 435–456. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 020- 09996-7
Holle, H., & Rein, R. (2015). EasyDIAg: A tool for easy determination of interrater agreement. Behavior
Research Methods, 47(3), 837–847. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 014- 0506-7
Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (1999). Learning together and alone: Cooperative, competitive and individual-
istic learning. Prentice Hall.
Kilpatrick, J. (1987). Problem formulating: Where do good problems come from? In A. H. Schoenfeld (Ed.),
Cognitive science and mathematics education (pp. 123–147). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Koichu, B., & Kontorovich, I. (2013). Dissecting success stories on mathematical problem posing: A
case of the billiard task. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 83(1), 71–86. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s10649- 012- 9431-9
Konrad, K. (2010). Lautes Denken. In G. Mey & K. Mruck (Eds.), Handbuch Qualitative Forschung in der
Psychologie (pp. 476–490). VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.
Kontorovich, I., & Koichu, B. (2016). A case study of an expert problem poser for mathematics competi-
tions. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 14(1), 81–99. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1007/ s10763- 013- 9467-z
Kontorovich, I., Koichu, B., Leikin, R., & Berman, A. (2012). An exploratory framework for handling the
complexity of mathematical problem posing in small groups. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior,
31(1), 149–161. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jmathb. 2011. 11. 002
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biomet-
rics, 33(1), 159–174. https:// doi. org/ 10. 2307/ 25293 10
Lester, F. K. (2005). On the theoretical, conceptual, and philosophical foundations for research in mathemat-
ics education. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 37(6), 457–467. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ BF026 55854
Mason, J., Burton, L., & Stacey, K. (1982). Thinking mathematically (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall.
Mayring, P. (2014). Qualitative content analysis. Theoretical foundation, basic procedures and software
solution. https:// nbn- resol ving. org/ urn: nbn: de: 0168- ssoar- 395173
Patáková, E. (2014). Expert recurrence of linear problem posing. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sci-
ences, 152, 590–595. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. sbspro. 2014. 09. 248
Pelczer, I., & Gamboa, F. (2009). Problem posing: Comparison between experts and novices. In M. Tze-
kaki, M. Kaldrimidou, & H. Sakonidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33th conference of the International
group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Vol. 4 (pp. 353–360). PME.
Pelczer, I., & Rodríguez, F. G. (2011). Creativity assessment in school settings through problem posing
tasks. The Mathematics Enthusiast, 8(1 & 2), 383–398.
Pólya, G. (1945). How to solve it. University Press.
Pólya, G. (1966). On teaching problem solving. In The Conference Board of the Mathematical Sciences
(Ed.), The role of axiomatics and problem solving in mathematics (pp. 123–129). Ginn.
Ponte, J. P., & Henriques, A. (2013). Problem posing based on investigation activities by university students.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 83, 145–156. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 012- 9443-5
268 L. Baumanns, B. Rott
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1 3
Rosli, R., Goldsby, D., & Capraro, M. M. (2013). Assessing students’ mathematical problem-solving and
problem-posing skills. Asian Social Science, 9(16), 54–60. https:// doi. org/ 10. 5539/ ass. v9n16 p54
Rott, B., Specht, B., & Knipping, C. (2021). A descriptive phase model of problemsolving processes. ZDM
– Mathematics Education. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s11858- 021- 01244-3
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985a). Making sense of “out loud” problem-solving protocols. The Journal of Math-
ematical Behavior, 4, 171–191.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985b). Mathematical problem solving. Academic Press.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (2000). Purposes and methods of research in mathematics education. Notices of the AMS,
47, 641–649.
Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology Review, 7(4), 351–
371. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ BF022 12307
Schupp, H. (2002). Thema mit Variationen. Aufgabenvariationen im Mathematikunterricht. Franzbecker.
Silver, E. A. (1994). On mathematical problem posing. For the Learning of Mathematics, 14(1), 19–28.
Silver, E. A. (1995). The nature and use of open problems in mathematics education: Mathematical and
pedagogical perspectives. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 27, 67–72.
Silver, E. A. (1997). Fostering creativity through instruction rich in mathematical problem solving and prob-
lem posing. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 29(3), 75–80. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s11858- 997- 0003-x
Singer, F. M., & Voica, C. (2015). Is problem posing a tool for identifying and developing mathematical cre-
ativity? In F. M. Singer, N. F. Ellerton, & J. Cai (Eds.), Mathematical problem posing. From research
to effective practice (pp. 141–174). Springer.
Singer, F. M., Ellerton, N. F., & Cai, J. (Eds.). (2015). Mathematical problem posing. Springer.
Singer, F. M., & Voica, C. (2017). When mathematics meets real objects: How does creativity interact with
expertise in problem solving and posing? Creativity and Giftedness, 75–103,. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
978-3- 319- 38840-3_6
Singer, F. M., Voica, C., & Pelczer, I. (2017). Cognitive styles in posing geometry problems: Implications
for assessment of mathematical creativity. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 49(1), 37–52. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1007/ s11858- 016- 0820-x
Stoyanova, E. (1997). Extending and exploring students’ problem solving via problem posing (Doctoral dis-
sertation). Edith Cowan University.
Stoyanova, E., & Ellerton, N. F. (1996). A framework for research into students’ problem posing in school
mathematics. In P. C. Clarkson (Ed.), Technology in mathematics education (pp. 518–525). Mathemat-
ics Education Research Group of Australasia.
Van Harpen, X. Y., & Presmeg, N. C. (2013). An investigation of relationships between students’ math-
ematical problem-posing abilities and their mathematical content knowledge. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 83(1), 117–132.
Van Harpen, X., & Sriraman, B. (2013). Creativity and mathematical problem posing: An analysis of high
school students’ mathematical problem posing in china and the USA. Educational Studies in Math-
ematics, 82(2), 201–221. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 012- 9419-5
Xie, J., & Masingila, J. O. (2017). Examining interactions between problem posing and problem solving
with prospective primary teachers: A case of using fractions. Educational Studies in Mathematics,
96(1), 101–118. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10649- 017- 9760-9
Yimer, A., & Ellerton, N. F. (2009). A five-phase model for mathematical problem solving: Identifying syn-
ergies in pre-service-teachers’ metacognitive and cognitive actions. ZDM – Mathematics Education,
42(2), 245–261. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s11858- 009- 0223-3
Yuan, X., & Sriraman, B. (2011). An exploratory study of relationships between students’ creativity and
mathematical problem-posing abilities. In B. Sriraman & K. H. Lee (Eds.), The elements of creativity
and giftedness in mathematics (pp. 5–28). Sense Publishers.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.
269The process of problem posing: development of a descriptive…
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center
GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers
and authorised users (“Users”), for small-scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all
copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By accessing,
sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of
use (“Terms”). For these purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and
students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and
conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal subscription. These Terms will prevail over any
conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription (to
the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of
the Creative Commons license used will apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may
also use these personal data internally within ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share
it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not otherwise
disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies
unless we have your permission as detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial
use, it is important to note that Users may not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale
basis or as a means to circumvent access control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any
jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association
unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a
systematic database of Springer Nature journal content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a
product or service that creates revenue, royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as
part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal content cannot be
used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large
scale into their, or any other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not
obligated to publish any information or content on this website and may remove it or features or
functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature may revoke
this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content
which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or
guarantees to Users, either express or implied with respect to the Springer nature journal content and
all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law, including
merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published
by Springer Nature that may be licensed from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a
regular basis or in any other manner not expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer
Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... Matemática escolhida para evitar a aplicação direta do conteúdo. Apesar disso, esses seis grupos conseguiram realizar a reelaboração pretendida, de modo que revelaram atenção na análise da situação para as condições do problema (Baumanns;Rott, 2022). Essa dificuldade e superação foi evidenciada nos estudos de Maia-Afonso (2021) ISSN 1980-4415 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980 ...
... Matemática escolhida para evitar a aplicação direta do conteúdo. Apesar disso, esses seis grupos conseguiram realizar a reelaboração pretendida, de modo que revelaram atenção na análise da situação para as condições do problema (Baumanns;Rott, 2022). Essa dificuldade e superação foi evidenciada nos estudos de Maia-Afonso (2021) ISSN 1980-4415 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980 ...
... Um exemplo disso é o significado do termo Juros Compostos (conhecimento semântico), de modo que para a pergunta do problema escolhido, o grupo G9 não colocou esse termo e sim deixou da seguinte forma: Sabendo que o juros incide sobre o montante anterior, quantos reais terá após 90 dias de aplicação? (Questionamento do G9, 2022).Isso mostra que o grupo fez a decodificação do esquema de juros composto(Singer; Voica, 2013) e depois o substituiu por sua ideia agindo na variação da condição dada(Baumanns;Rott, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Resumo O artigo tem como objetivo analisar as dificuldades de licenciandos em Matemática na escolha de problemas na abordagem do Ensino-Aprendizagem de Matemática via Resolução de Problemas – EAMvRP. Realizamos um estudo qualitativo e de campo com 33 estudantes, os quais receberam uma formação para aprender e propor sequências de ensino tendo o problema como ponto de partida na perspectiva do EAMvRP. Os resultados mostraram que as duas dificuldades mais evidenciadas foram para reestruturar o problema escolhido (elaborado e reelaborado) e para situá-lo em contextos próximos à realidade dos alunos da escola. Além das dificuldades relacionadas à identificação de estratégias, utilização dos conhecimentos prévios e revisão de alternativas/questões nos problemas, esses resultados indicam a interrelação necessária para a ação de escolha do problema. Concluímos que superar as dificuldades na escolha de problemas está diretamente associado à constituição contínua e conjunta de conhecimentos pedagógico e matemático.
... Overall, literature has highlighted the role of the invention as an opportunity to promote flexible thinking and lead to a deeper understanding of mathematical contents (Baumanns & Rott, 2022). Within the context of the development of algebraic thinking, more studies are focusing on the invention of algebraic problems (e.g. ...
... Literature has highlighted the role of the invention as an opportunity to promote flexible thinking and lead to a deeper understanding of mathematical contents (Baumanns & Rott, 2022). However, it is essential to consider that there is not only one type of invention task, based on the ideas of Stoyanova (2000), who referred to problem-solving, distinguishing three categories of invention tasks: (a) free situations, (b) semi-structured situations, and (c) structured situations. ...
... Managing to invent a pattern with two different types of structures evidences cognitive flexibility in students. This highlights the role of the invention as an opportunity to promote flexible thinking and lead to a deep understanding of mathematical contents (Baumanns & Rott, 2022). ...
... Problem posing can also promote creativity and diverse and flexible thinking (Bicer et al., 2020). In addition, the stimuli given to students to prompt problem posing can differ in their degree of structure (Baumanns & Rott, 2022;Cai & Jiang, 2017;Christou et al., 2005;Stoyanova et al., 1996). ...
Article
Full-text available
Problems encountered in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM) contexts cannot be adequately described or solved with the knowledge of a single discipline. Instead, a high level of inter-and transdisciplinary knowledge and methods is required to overcome them. These help to pose problems about the complex challenges and solve them in a creative way. The better the knowledge within one and of different disciplines is interlinked, the more targeted questions can be formulated and answered. Mathematical concepts serve as the foundation for many of the pressing problems of our time. At the same time, these problems offer a wide range of opportunities for individualized exploration. They are equally suitable for students with different interests and levels of ability, as everyone can identify and work on an individual problem within the given context. However, numerous studies have shown that posing adequate mathematical problems must be learnt as well as knowledge from different disciplines is not automatically linked or transferred to other situations. The ability to grasp the formal structure of a problem, recognize problems, and find connecting problems are characteristics of mathematically gifted children and young people that need to be promoted. In our theory-based contribution , we use a concrete context to illustrate which possible mathematically rich problems can be posed by students of different ages and abilities. This approach facilitates the development of their individual abilities according to their interests and potential.
... Yet, the effectiveness of these simplifications varied, with ChatGPT occasionally altering the problem's content significantly. Additionally, ChatGPT proactively challenged teachers to craft problems that met specific educational objectives, indicating its utility in encouraging thoughtful task formulation as an integral part of the problem-posing process (Baumanns & Rott, 2022). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The use of AI tools in the professional development of teachers is evolving into an important part of education. Despite growing interest, there is a gap in understanding how AI, specifically ChatGPT, can support mathematics teachers, for example, when posing problems of varying degrees of difficulty. This study aims to explore how interactions with ChatGPT assist pre-service mathematics teachers (PSTs) in posing problems of various levels of difficulty. Five PSTs were asked to create one easy, moderate, and difficult problem based on a given problem. The interactions with ChatGPT were exploratively analyzed using qualitative methods. The analysis identified three themes (collaboration, pedagogical content knowledge, and support in formulation), offering initial insights into nuances of lesson planning and contributing to our understanding of human-AI interaction in education.
... Baumanns and Rott propose that problem solving is a personalized learning process in which students construct and create meaningful problems based on their own learning experiences [23]. Generating questions is a cognitive strategy and a postulated cognitive strategy. ...
Article
Distance learning lacks person-to-person interaction, which makes learning less interesting. While peer interaction can gain the support, assistance, and discussion of peer empathy, which can effectively motivate learning. This study proposes a remote peer live learning strategy, which allows learners to conduct peer learning anytime and anywhere via the Internet and uses peer-to-peer video methods to conduct peer-to-peer live teaching. Explores the influence of adding peer factors on their learning effectiveness and learning motivation. The findings of this study are as follows: (1) The learners who use peer live teaching strategy, regardless of whether they are high or low achievement, can effectively improve their learning effectiveness, learning motivation and reduce their cognition compared with those who use general distance teaching. (2) Students who utilized the peer-to-peer live teaching platform exhibited notably higher levels of technology acceptance compared to their counterparts enrolled in the conventional distance learning system.
Article
Full-text available
Problem generation is a versatile technique for creating and organizing mathematical problems. The first experimental study on problem posing was done in 1989 by Edward Silver with funding from the US National Science Foundation. Since then, this field has grown rapidly and gained attention from researchers. This study aims to investigate how problem posing is conceptualized and studied empirically in mathematics education research. This study was conducted based on two databases, Scopus and Web of Science. There are two phrases for search terms: problem posing and mathematical. Eligibility criteria to include literature in the study are: articles published in peer-reviewed journals in English, between the years 2017 and 2022, about mathematics, and are empirical research. The findings of the study show that from a conceptual perspective, problem posing is defined as the creation of new problems and the reorganization of existing problems based on the given situation. There are several approaches that can be used in problem posing, such as the use of games, manipulatives, or real-life applications. Problem posing also involves four main processes, namely editing, selection, comprehension, and the translation process. The findings of this study are expected to provide a more complete view of the concept of problem posing and provide guidance for teaching and learning mathematics in the classroom.
Research
Full-text available
Dissertação apresentada ao Programa de Pós-Graduação em Educação Matemática e Tecnológica da Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, como requisito parcial para obtenção do título de mestre em Educação Matemática e Tecnológica. Área de concentração: Ensino das Ciências e da Matemática. Esta pesquisa tem como questão norteadora a indagação de como os estudantes podem desenvolver o pensamento matemático com estratégias de formulação de problemas algébricos recreativos? O objeto de estudo foi a reformulação de problemas algébricos recreativos no 9º Ano, tendo como objetivo geral investigar como os estudantes desenvolvem estratégias no processo de reformulação de problemas algébricos recreativos na sala de aula. Os objetivos específicos: investigar como os estudantes concebem a formulação de problemas algébricos recreativos; identificar as estratégias das reformulações de problemas algébricos recreativos desenvolvidas pelos estudantes; analisar como será desenvolvido o pensamento algébrico durante o processo de reformulação de problemas. A pesquisa foi desenvolvida com estudantes de uma escola da rede pública municipal de ensino em Pernambuco, utilizando como metodologia de pesquisa, a abordagem qualitativa, estudos de caso, de dois estudantes, em atividades de formulação de problemas algébricos recreativos. Estes eram baseadas nos textos, que apresentam o Problema adaptado das Pérolas do Rajá, do livro O Homem que Calculava, de Malba Tahan, O Cavalo e a Mula, do livro de Yakov Perelman. Foram realizadas duas entrevistas semiestruturadas, uma no início da pesquisa e outra após as duas sessões de formulação de problemas algébricos recreativos e um questionário. Fizemos a análise dos dados produzidos com categorias a priori que são as concepções dos estudantes sobre a formulação de problemas algébricos recreativos, estratégias de formulação dos problemas algébricos recreativos e o desenvolvimento do pensamento algébrico durante o processo de formulação de problemas. E as categorias a posteriori, emergiram: Formulação de Exercícios; e A linguagem lógico matemática utilizada. Os resultados sugerem, em ambos os casos, que os estudantes, ao utilizarem as estratégias de formulação de problemas, formularam exercícios e ainda não transitaram o seu pensamento matemático da aritmética para a álgebra. As concepções de ambos sobre a formulação de problemas algébricos recreativos diferiram. No Caso 1, o estudante não tem clareza sobre a álgebra e nem interesse por recreações matemáticas em problemas contextualizados. Por sua vez, no Caso 2, a estudante demonstra alguma compreensão sobre a álgebra, se diverte com a Matemática e a vê em contextos.
Article
We designed a programme consisting of initial problem posing related to a problem about oil drums, teaching activities to solve the problem and further problem posing to investigate the change in pre-service mathematics teachers’ problem-posing performances and the potential role of teaching activities in these performances. The participants’ problem-posing fluency and flexibility scores showed statistically significant improvement over the course of the cycle. Although there was an improvement in problem-posing rarity scores, it was not statistically significant. Understanding the visual model and discovering the geometric properties of the problem were identified as categories that were effective in improving the participants’ performances.
Article
Full-text available
This research aims to determine the practicality of the problem-posing approach with the STAD setting in mathematics learning. Practicality can be seen based on the implementation of learning by the teacher and student responses. This research is development research that focuses on producing a problem-posing approach design with an STAD setting. The subjects of this research were 8th-grade students. The data collection instruments used in this research were teacher observation sheets and student response questionnaires. Problem posing approach STAD setting used in this learning includes (1) identifying learning objectives and motivating students, (2) forming groups, (3) presenting problems, (4) proposing new problems based on the problems given, (5) guiding students' answers by evaluating the problem-solving process (6) feedback back and, (7) give appreciation. The research results show that the learning design implementation is in the good category, and students' responses to the learning process are in the positive category. Thus, the problem-posing approach with the STAD setting in mathematics learning is in the practical category.
Article
Full-text available
This article aims to develop a framework for the characterisation of problem-posing activities. The framework links three theoretical constructs from research on problem posing, problem solving, and psychology: (1) problem posing as an activity of generating new or reformulating given problems, (2) emerging tasks on the spectrum between routine and non-routine problems, and (3) metacognitive behaviour in problem-posing processes. These dimensions are first conceptualised theoretically. Afterward, the application of these conceptualised dimensions is demonstrated qualitatively using empirical studies on problem posing. Finally, the framework is applied to characterise problem-posing activities within systematically gathered articles from high-ranked journals on mathematics education to identify focal points and under-represented activities in research on problem posing.
Article
Full-text available
Complementary to existing normative models, in this paper we suggest a descriptive phase model of problem solving. Real, not ideal, problem-solving processes contain errors, detours, and cycles, and they do not follow a predetermined sequence, as is presumed in normative models. To represent and emphasize the non-linearity of empirical processes, a descriptive model seemed essential. The juxtaposition of models from the literature and our empirical analyses enabled us to generate such a descriptive model of problem-solving processes. For the generation of our model, we reflected on the following questions: (1) Which elements of existing models for problem-solving processes can be used for a descriptive model? (2) Can the model be used to describe and discriminate different types of processes? Our descriptive model allows one not only to capture the idiosyncratic sequencing of real problem-solving processes, but simultaneously to compare different processes, by means of accumulation. In particular, our model allows discrimination between problem-solving and routine processes. Also, successful and unsuccessful problem-solving processes as well as processes in paper-and-pencil versus dynamic-geometry environments can be characterised and compared with our model.
Article
Full-text available
In research on mathematical problem posing, a broad spectrum of different situations is used to induce the activity of posing problems. This review aims at characterizing these so-called problem-posing situations by conducting three consecutive analyses: (1) By analyzing the openness of potential problem-posing situations, the concept of ‚mathematical posing' is concretized. (2) The problem-posing situations are assigned to the categories free, semi-structured, and structured by Stoyanova and Ellerton to illustrate the distribution of situations used in research. (3) Finally, the initial problems of the structured problem-posing situations are analyzed with regard to whether they are routine or non-routine problems. These analyses are conducted on 271 potential problem-posing situations from 241 systematically gathered articles on problem posing. The purpose of this review is to provide a framework for the identification of differences between problem-posing situations.
Article
Full-text available
Spontaneous problem posing (SPP) is presented as a phenomenon central to mathematical thinking, wherein learners generate problems without formal prompting, and where posed problems originate from the learner’s motivation to improve their knowledge. Because of this, they may serve as important markers of productive mathematical engagement—in particular, affective engagement—both for problem posers and their classroom communities (D’Mello & Graesser in Learning and Instruction, 22(2), 145–157, 2012). We report on a study which utilized mixed methods to examine SPP and associated affective engagement of students in four early high school mathematics classrooms in two geographic regions of the USA. For each classroom, we used observational and experience sampling methods to examine the patterns of affect problem posers and their peers experienced at individual and group levels, respectively, on a day with at least one instance of SPP observed compared with a day with no observed instances of SPP. Results show evidence of positive affect among problem posers, while their peers reported fewer negative emotions about mathematics tasks on days with SPP than on days without SPP. Moreover, a detailed analysis of two spontaneous problem posers revealed that they showed a desire to extend their own mathematical thinking, expressed dissatisfaction with their current knowledge, and directed their problems primarily towards the teacher. Results are discussed regarding the need to support students’ confidence in their original mathematical ideas in class, and for reducing negative emotional responses to mathematics tasks.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to reveal both the effects of problem-posing interventions on the mathematical creative ability of students and how students’ creative self-efficacy in mathematics was related to their mathematical creative ability. Elementary school students (n = 205) were randomly assigned to one of two groups: problem-posing or control. Results showed the mathematical creativity for the problem-posing group increased (p < 0.05) more than for students in the control group (d = 0.77). Results from the Confirmatory Factor Analysis showed that mathematical creativity was a higher order factor that included mathematical creative ability and mathematical creative self-efficacy as first-order factors. Among the implications for this is that integrating problem-posing activities into elementary school mathematics instruction can foster mathematical creativity.
Article
The quality of mathematics problems has been a concern to mathematics educators. There are many frameworks to support teachers’ posing problems to students, however, these frameworks have tended to focus primarily on their cognitive demand (e.g., Smith & Stein, 1998). Rarely are mathematics problems considered in relation to their social and participation dimensions, namely whether or not they are designed to support students’ collaborative problem solving (Featherstone et al., 2011; Horn, 2012; Lotan, 2003). In this article the authors explore secondary prospective teachers’ (PTs) analysis and adaptations of mathematics problems after they are introduced to frameworks that foreground collaboration and equity in the teaching and learning of mathematics. Implications are offered to future teachers of mathematics and mathematics education researchers.
Book
Updated and expanded, this second edition satisfies the same philosophical objective as the first -- to show the importance of problem posing. Although interest in mathematical problem solving increased during the past decade, problem posing remained relatively ignored. The Art of Problem Posing draws attention to this equally important act and is the innovator in the field. Special features include: •an exploration ofthe logical relationship between problem posing and problem solving •a special chapter devoted to teaching problem posing as a separate course •sketches, drawings, diagrams, and cartoons that illustrate the schemes proposed a special section on writing in mathematics. © 1990 by Stephen I. Brown and Marion I. Walter. All rights reserved.