ArticlePDF Available

Abstract Thinking Facilitates Aggregation of Information

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Many situations in life (such as considering which stock to invest in, or which people to befriend) require averaging across series of values. Here, we examined predictions derived from construal level theory, and tested whether abstract compared with concrete thinking facilitates the process of aggregating values into a unified summary representation. In four experiments, participants were induced to think more abstractly (vs. concretely) and performed different variations of an averaging task with numerical values (Experiments 1-2 and 4), and emotional faces (Experiment 3). We found that the induction of abstract, compared with concrete thinking, improved aggregation accuracy (Experiments 1-3), but did not improve memory for specific items (Experiment 4). In particular, in concrete thinking, averaging was characterized by increased regression toward the mean and lower signal-to-noise ratio, compared with abstract thinking. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
Abstract Thinking Facilitates Aggregation of Information
Britt Hadar, Moshe Glickman, Yaacov Trope, Nira Liberman, and Marius Usher
Online First Publication, December 20, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001126
CITATION
Hadar, B., Glickman, M., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Usher, M. (2021, December 20). Abstract Thinking Facilitates
Aggregation of Information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001126
BRIEF REPORT
Abstract Thinking Facilitates Aggregation of Information
Britt Hadar
1, 2, 3
, Moshe Glickman
4, 5
, Yaacov Trope
6
, Nira Liberman
1
, and Marius Usher
1, 7
1
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel-Aviv University
2
Department of Psychology, Princeton University
3
Princeton School of Public and International Affairs, Princeton University
4
Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
5
Max Planck University College London Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, London, United Kingdom
6
Department of Psychology, New York University
7
Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel-Aviv University
Many situations in life (such as considering which stock to invest in, or which people to befriend)
require averaging across series of values. Here, we examined predictions derived from construal level
theory, and tested whether abstract compared with concrete thinking facilitates the process of aggregat-
ing values into a unified summary representation. In four experiments, participants were induced to
think more abstractly (vs. concretely) and performed different variations of an averaging task with nu-
merical values (Experiments 12 and 4), and emotional faces (Experiment 3). We found that the induc-
tion of abstract, compared with concrete thinking, improved aggregation accuracy (Experiments 13),
but did not improve memory for specific items (Experiment 4). In particular, in concrete thinking, aver-
aging was characterized by increased regression toward the mean and lower signal-to-noise ratio, com-
pared with abstract thinking.
Keywords: construal level theory, abstraction, numerical averaging, emotion perception
Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001126.supp
Imagine yourself in a new workplace, trying to figure out how
friendly are your new coworkers. To form impressions about their
dispositions, you sample their behavior (e.g., whether they greeted
you in the elevator or invited you to lunch), and average those sam-
ples into a general value-representation (Anderson, 1981). Similarly,
when watching products displayed in a shop (YamanashiLeibetal.,
2020), or a rapid stream of stock returns (Betsch et al., 2001;Vanunu
et al., 2019), one might extract an overall value estimation to guide
behavior. In all these cases, different exemplars that span across time
and space are collapsed into one representative value (e.g., I like a
lot this coworker, this shop is expensive, or the stock is profitable).
Averaging is a common and intuitive way of extracting a representa-
tive value from a stream of exemplars (Anderson, 1971;Kahneman,
2011). Indeed, the rapid extraction of ensemble information has been
documented in a wide array of ensemble properties ranging from
simple features such as size (Ariely, 2001;Chong et al., 2008;Chong
& Treisman, 2003;2005), orientation (Dakin & Watt, 1997;Parkes
et al., 2001), color (Maule et al., 2014), motion direction (Watama-
niuk & McKee, 1998;Watamaniuk et al., 1989),andmotionspeed
(Watamaniuk & Duchon, 1992); to more complex properties, such as
gender (Haberman & Whitney, 2007), identity (de Fockert & Wolf-
enstein, 2009;Neumann et al., 2013), emotional facial expressions
(Haberman et al., 2009), animacy (Yamanashi Leib et al., 2016),
gaze direction (Florey et al., 2016;Sweeny & Whitney, 2014), attrac-
tiveness (Post et al., 2012;van Osch et al., 2015;Walker & Vul,
2014), traits (Asch, 1946;Eyal et al., 2011;Hamilton & Sherman,
1996), and even when deciding whether a basketball playerscareer
earns him a place in the Hall of Fame (Brusovansky et al., 2019). For
Britt Hadar https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0863-8650
Britt Hadar and Moshe Glickman shared equal contribution. Nira
Liberman and Marius Usher joined senior authorship.
Data is available at: https://osf.io/y4uxd/.
This research was supported by a United States-Israel Binational
Science Foundation (BSF) Grant 2016090 to Nira Liberman and Yaacov
Trope. Nira Liberman and Britt Hadar were supported by Grant 524/17
by the Israel Science Foundation (ISF). Marius Usher was supported by
an ISF Grant 1413/17. The authors would like to thank Talya Rapp and
Yasmeen Shamshoum for their help in running the experiments.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Britt
Hadar, Department of Psychology, Princeton University, South Drive,
Princeton, NJ 08540, United States, or Moshe Glickman, Department of
Experimental Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way,
London, WC1H 0AP, United Kingdom. Email: britt.hadar@gmail.com or
mosheglickman345@gmail.com
1
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
©2021 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 0096-3445 https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001126
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
an extensive review of ensemble perception, the reader is referred to
Whitney and Yamanashi Leib (2018).
Recent research has supported the idea that average extraction is
automatic, or at least nonintentional (Betsch et al., 2001;Brusovan-
sky et al., 2018;Khayat & Hochstein, 2018), and is based on a popu-
lation-coding mechanism (Baek & Chong, 2020;Brezis et al., 2016;
Brezis et al., 2018). Several factors, including set size (Brezis et al.,
2015;Robitaille & Harris, 2011), and variance (Brezis et al., 2018;
Solomon, 2010) were found to affect the accuracy of averaging.
However, little is known about the impact of thinking-modes on the
accuracy of intuitive (i.e., without explicit symbolic calculation) aver-
aging. We specifically focus on concrete versus abstract thinking.
Research conducted within construal level theory (CLT), shows that
people form representations at varying degrees of abstraction (Gilead
et al., 2020;Liberman & Trope, 2014;Trope & Liberman, 2010). A
process of abstraction, in this view, occurs when distinctions between
exemplars are disregarded, and a dimension of similarity is high-
lighted that places them into a category. For example, categorizing
sandals and boots as footwear disregards waterproofand highlights
worn on feet.Averaging bears inherent relation to abstraction
because when a stream of exemplars is represented by an average,
differences between specific exemplars are disregarded, and their
commonality (their origin in a common distribution) is emphasized.
We reasoned that because averaging is a process of abstraction
wherein exemplars are integrated (with equal weights) into a single
representative value, then abstract thinking (compared with concrete
thinking) should enhance the accuracy of intuitive average estimation.
We tested this prediction in three experiments (Experiments 13) that
manipulated, within participants, abstract versus concrete thinking via
the why/howmindset manipulation, which was originally developed
by Freitas et al. (2004), and has been widely used since then. Indeed, a
review by Burgoon et al. (2013) mentions 11 published articles that
used this manipulation, and many more used it since then (Ding &
Keh, 2017;Efrat-Treister et al., 2020;Gilead et al., 2014;Hadar et al.,
2019;Hansen & Trope, 2013;Kille et al., 2017;Napier et al., 2018;
Spunt et al., 2016;Stillman et al., 2017;Yudkin et al., 2020). Experi-
ment 1 examined accuracy in a numerical averaging task (Brezis et al.,
2015;Malmi & Samson, 1983;Rosenbaum et al., 2021), and Experi-
ment 2 was a preregistered replication. Experiment 3 tested the effect
of abstract thinking on averaging of emotional faces (which prevents
the use of rule-based strategies). Experiment 4 was a control experi-
ment that tested the effect of abstract mindset on memory for specific
details. We predicted that abstract (compared with concrete) thinking
would improve accuracy of average extraction (Experiments 13), but
not memory for specific details (Experiment 4). We examined two
mechanisms that may explain the effect of abstract thought: signal-to-
noise ratio and temporal weighting biases.
Experiments
Mindset Manipulation
In all four experiments
1
we used the mindset induction manipu-
lation developed by Freitas et al. (2004); see Supplemental
Information (SI) for a detailed description of the manipulation. In
the abstract mindset condition, participants were presented with
four boxes, in which they answered four consecutive whyques-
tions, starting from, for example, Why maintain good physical
health. In the concrete mindset condition, participants answered
four consecutive howquestions, starting with the same behavior,
for example, How to maintain good physical health.(Figure 1A
presents the manipulation).
Averaging Task
In all three experiments, following each mindset induction, the
participants estimated the average of rapidly presented sequences of
values (Brezis et al., 2015;Malmi & Samson, 1983). The estimation
precision was measured via the Pearson correlation between the
actual and the estimated averages across all trials (for each participant
and mindset manipulation). Relying on correlation allows testing sen-
sitivity to relative changes in the variable, while allowing for general
biases.
2
In the General Discussion section, we also report analyses of
actual deviations (i.e., root mean square deviation [RMSD]).
Experiment 1
Method
Participants
Fifty-six Tel-Aviv University students (35 women, M
age
=
22.80, SD = 4.12) participated for a course credit. One participant
left before finishing the task and was excluded from analysis.
Materials
In the numerical averaging task, each trial began with a central
fixation cross (250 ms), after which a sequence of eight two-digit
numbers (between 10 and 90) was presented at a rate of 2 Hz and
participants were required to enter their estimation of the sequence
average on a visual analog scale ranging from 0100 (see Figure
1B, and SI for additional details). The numbers were drawn from a
Gaussian distribution (l=50þk, r= 20, k U[15,15]), with
no successive repetitions. Participants completed 20 practice trials
and 240 experimental trials.
Procedure
Participants first completed the practice trials of the numerical
averaging task, then the mindset manipulation (either the concrete
howor the abstract why). Then 60 trials of the numerical aver-
aging task, such that two whyblocks were completed consecu-
tively and then two howblocks were completed consecutively by
each participant. Whether a participants started with whyor
howblocks was counterbalanced between participants.
Results
Performance in the abstract and concrete thinking conditions was
quantified by computing the Pearson correlation between partici-
pantsestimations and actual mean of each sequence (see Figure
2ABfor an illustration of two representative participants). Pearson
1
Data is available at: https://osf.io/y4uxd/, see Hadar (2021).
2
For example, if the average estimations are accurate but shifted by a
constant, this would reduce the deviation score but preserve a high
correlation between real and the estimated averages. Such a situation
indicates a high discrimination sensitivity of the sequence-average.
2HADAR, GLICKMAN, TROPE, LIBERMAN, AND USHER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
correlations were transformed into Fishers Z-scores (z=arctanh
(r)) and submitted to 2 (mindset: abstract vs. concrete) 32(order:
abstract block first vs. concrete block first) mixed-design ANOVA
with mindset as a within-participants factor. The analysis revealed
the predicted effect of mindset, whereby the Fisher-transformed
correlations were significantly higher in the abstract mindset condi-
tion (M=.911,SD= .221) compared with the concrete mindset
condition (M=.859,SD =.214),F(1, 53) = 5.411, p=.024,h
p
2
=
.093. A main effect of order, F(1, 53) = 8.210, p=.006,h
p
2
=.134,
indicated that correlations were higher for participants who com-
pleted the concrete mindset condition first compared to those who
completed the abstract mindset condition first. The interaction was
not significant, F(1, 53) = .340, p= .562, indicating that the effect
of abstract thinking obtained irrespective of order (see Figure 2C
for the group-level [z-transformed] Pearson correlations collapsed
across order conditions). We confirmed that the difference between
the abstract and concrete conditions remain significant if we use a
nonparametric test to compare the raw correlations (i.e., without
using Fisher z-transformation). To this end, we conducted a two-
tailed paired-samples permutation test with 10,000 shuffles, which
yielded a significant result (p=.023).
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 was a preregistered replication
3
of Experiment 1
with several changes. First, we cut the number of trials by half.
Second, to generalize our finding across different underlying dis-
tributions, the values in Experiment 2 were sampled from a uni-
form distribution (in which all the numbers in the designated range
have the same probability to be sampled). This makes the estima-
tion more challenging by rendering inefficient heuristic strategies
(e.g., basing the average on only two to three random samples.)
Method
Participants
Sixty-two Tel-Aviv University students (37 women, M
age
=
23.69, SD = 3.27) participated in the experiment for course credit.
Two participants were excluded; one was not a native Hebrew
speaker, and one did not complete the manipulation forms.
Materials and Procedure
The procedure was the same as in Experiment 1, except that
participants completed only one block of 60 trials under each
mindset condition and the distribution from which numbers in
each sequence were sampled was uniform (U[45 þk, 55 þk],
kU[15, 15]) rather than Gaussian.
Results
The same analysis as in Experiment 1 revealed the predicted
effect of mindset, whereby Fisher-transformed correlations
were higher in the abstract mindset condition (M=1.091,SD=
.234) compared with the concrete mindset condition, (M=
1.041, SD = .278), F(1, 58) = 4.572, p= .037, h
p
2
= .073. Nei-
ther order F(1, 58) = .964, p= .330, nor the interaction of mind-
set and order were significant, F(1, 58) = .185, p=.669(see
Figure 2C). The same pattern of results was obtained if we ana-
lyzed the data using a paired-sampled permutation test similar
to that used in Experiment 1 (p= .013).
Experiment 3
The aim of Experiment 3 was to generalize the effect of abstrac-
tion on aggregation of information to more ecological and real-life
Figure 1
Event Sequence in Experiments 1 and 2
Note. (A) Mindset induction manipulation: Participants complete the abstract mindset manipulation (Why?) or the concrete
mindset manipulation (How?). (B) Participants then observe a sequence of eight numbers and estimate their average on an ana-
log scale (60 in each block). In Experiment 1 there were two blocks after a concrete mindset induction, two blocks after an
abstract mindset induction. In Experiment 2 there was only one block after each manipulation.
3
http://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=bd5245
ABSTRACTION FACILITATES AGGREGATION OF INFORMATION 3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
stimuli, which also prevents the use of rule-based computations. We
chose extraction of the mean emotion from a set of emotional faces
for several reasons. First, people may face a similar task on a daily
basis when trying to understand their partnersor coworkers
feelings. Second, finding a common process that applies to both sim-
ple stimuli (numbers in Experiments 1 and 2) as well as to complex
social stimuli (emotional faces) is not trivial and can shed light on a
broader spectrum of instances in which abstraction plays a role.
Figure 2
Results of Experiments 13
Note.(AB) Correlations between the sequence actual mean and the estimated responses of two representative participants
in the abstract (blue) and concrete (orange) thinking conditions. (C) Pearson correlations (z-transformed) by mindset condi-
tion (abstract vs. concrete) in Experiments 13. Blue and orange circles correspond to the correlations of each participant in
the abstract and concrete conditions, respectively. Gray circles correspond to the conditions means. The central mark on
each box-plot indicates the median, and the bottom and top edges indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively. *p,
.05. See the online article for the color version of this gure.
4HADAR, GLICKMAN, TROPE, LIBERMAN, AND USHER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Method
Participants
Sixty-two Tel-Aviv University students (38 women, M
age
=
22.88, SD = 2.54) participated in the experiment for course credit.
Two participants did not complete the manipulation forms and
were excluded.
Materials
Mindset Manipulation. We used the same mindset manipula-
tion as in Experiments 1 and 2.
Emotion Averaging Task. We adopted the morphed faces
from Haberman et al. (2009). We used 50 different faces that were
created by linearly interpolating between two emotion extremes of
the same actor, taken from the Ekman gallery (Ekman & Friesen,
1976), such that each morphed face was one emotional unit sadder
than the one before it. Thus, the values of the faces ranged
between 1 (happiest) and 50 (saddest; see Figure 3). On each trial,
faces were randomly sampled from a uniform distribution (with
repetition).
Procedure
Participants were told that they will be presented with sequences
of emotional faces (all of the same person, Rachel). Each sequence
represents Rachels emotions on a given day. Participants were asked
to estimate Rachels mean emotion during each day. Participants
completed five practice trials, then completed the mindset induction
task, and then one block of the averaging task (68 trials). Participants
then completed the second mindset induction task (order was
counterbalanced between participants) and performed the second and
last block of the averaging task. Overall, participants completed 136
critical trials.
Results
The same analysis as in Experiments 1 and 2 revealed the pre-
dicted effect of mindset, wherein Fisher-transformed correlations
were higher in the abstract mindset condition (M= 1.008,SD=
.196) compared to the concrete mindset condition (M= .955, SD =
.191), F(1, 58) = 5.514, p= .022, h
p
2
= .087, (Figure 2C). Neither
order F(1, 58) = .607, p= .439, nor the interaction of mindset and
order were significant, F(1, 58) = .914, p= .343. The same pattern
of results was obtained if we analyze the data using a paired-
sampled permutation test (p= .035).
Computational Modeling
We begin by examining whether the participantsestimations
were more influenced by earlier items (i.e., primacy bias) or by
later items (i.e., recency bias). Previous studies have reported the
latter both for numerical values averaging task (Brezis et al., 2015;
Spitzer et al., 2017), and emotional faces (Hubert-Wallander &
Boynton, 2015). However, because different mechanisms may
underlie these two tasks (Haberman et al., 2015;Hubert-Wallander
& Boynton, 2015;Whitney & Yamanashi Leib, 2018), we ana-
lyzed the temporal weighting in each experiment separately. For
each experiment, we performed a temporal regression analysis, in
Figure 3
Illustration of the Emotion Averaging Task in Experiment 3
Note.Axation cross is presented for 500 ms, followed by a sequence of eight faces presented
for 500 ms each. Participants then estimate the average emotional expression using an analog scale,
presented until response. The authors adopted the morphed faces from Haberman et al. (2009).
ABSTRACTION FACILITATES AGGREGATION OF INFORMATION 5
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
which we predicted the response in each trial for Experiments 13
(Y), using the samples (X
i
) ranked by their temporal order.
Y¼woþX
n
i¼1
wiXi(1)
As shown in Figure 4, across Experiments 13 the mean normal-
ized weights of Samples 58 was higher than that of Samples 14
(Experiment 1: t(54) = 7.49, p,.001; Experiment 2: t(59) = 6.97,
p,.001; Experiment 3: t(59) = 9.11, p,.001), indicating a recency
bias. This motivated us to include a leak term (a parameter that con-
trols the extent to which earlier values are given less weight; Usher &
McClelland, 2001;Vanunu et al., 2019)insomeofourmodels.
We examined several models to gain better understanding of the
effects in Experiments 13. All models assume that the mean is
estimated based on noisy representations of the magnitudes of the
numbers (Experiments 1 and 2) or facial expressions (Experiment
3) presented in each sequence. Models are based on Equation 1
(with a Gaussian noise term), and approximate the neural popula-
tion-averaging model suggested by Brezis et al. (2015,2018; see
also Rosenbaum et al., 2021 for simulations showing the equiva-
lence between models based on Equation 1 and population-averag-
ing models). Because similar patterns of results were obtained in
Experiments 13, we collapsed them together.
In particular, we examined four models based on the mecha-
nism presented in Equation 1 (see Table 1). The first model
serves as a baseline, and includes equal temporal weights (i.e.,
no-leak) and a direct (unbiased) map of estimated values to the
response scale. The second model also assumes equal weights,
but includes two free-parameters: intercept and slope parameter
(b0and b1) which map between the participants internal
estimation to the external response scale; for slope parameters
that are smaller than 1, this reflects a regression to the mean.
Models 34 are similar to Models 12, but introduce leak
(unequal temporal weights). For each participant, the models
were fitted to the data of the abstract and concrete mindset condi-
tions separately, and were compared with each other using the
Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974).
As shown in Table 1, Model 4 decisively outperformed all other
models both in the concrete and abstract mindset conditions, sug-
gesting that the integration process was characterized by decreas-
ing weight to earlier-presented samples (recency bias), as well as
by biases in the mapping between presented numbers and the
response scale. To examine whether these processes varied
between conditions, we compared between conditions the best-fit-
ted leak, intercept and slope parameters of Model 4.
Toward this end, we performed three t-tests with mindset
(abstract vs. concrete) as independent variable, and leak, inter-
cept, and slope as dependent variables.
4
The first ttest revealed a
significant difference, t(174) = 3.48, p,.001, between the slope
of the abstract mindset condition (b1= .98) and the concrete
mindset condition (b1= .93), indicating a more accurate, less re-
gressive estimation mapping in the abstract condition. Similar
results were obtained for the intercept parameter, t(174) = 3.39,
p,.001, revealing that the intercept in the abstract mindset con-
dition (b0= .54) was less biased than the intercept in the con-
crete mindset condition (b0= 2.69). Finally, while the leak was
numerically lower in the concrete compared with the abstract condi-
tion, this difference was not statistically significant, t(174) = 1.59,
Figure 4
The Temporal-Weighting Prole (Standardized Regression Weights) of the Samples in Experiments
13
Note. In all experiments the evaluations of the participants were more inuenced by the recent items in the
sequence (i.e., recency bias). Error bars correspond to within-subjects SE. See the online article for the color
version of this gure.
4
For simplicity, the data in the analyses presented here were collapsed
across Experiments 13, see SI for a full two-way ANOVAs using mindset
and experiment as independent factors.
6HADAR, GLICKMAN, TROPE, LIBERMAN, AND USHER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
p= .12. For each condition we also computed a measure of signal-
to-noise ratio, by dividing the slope by the squared root of the
error-term ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
Xn
i¼1
ypredictedyobserved
ðÞ
2
n1
s
0
@1
A, which provides an estima-
tion of the scatter around the regression line (Figure 2AB). We
find that this measure of signal-to-noise ratio was higher in the
abstract mindset condition than in the concrete mindset condition, t
(174) = 3.83, p,.001.
Experiment 4
In three experiments we have shown that abstract mindset (com-
pared with concrete mindset) improves the accuracy by which infor-
mation is aggregated in average estimations. A remaining question,
however, is whether this improvement is unique to averaging, or is
rather a mere reflection of a general performance improvement.
Experiment 4 is a preregistered
5
examination of this question. We
modified the task from average-estimation to memory for individual
items. We hypothesized that abstract mindset would not improve per-
formance in this task, and might even impair performance, compared
with concrete mindset (which, compared with the abstract mindset,
may confer an advantage to the processing of individual elements).
Method
Participants
One-hundred and 57 participants were recruited from Prolific
6
(79 women; M
age
= 23.60, SD = 5.31), in exchange for $4. Partici-
pants were prescreened to those who currently hold a student sta-
tus (in order to keep the sample relatively comparable to the
previous lab-based experiments). Six participants were excluded,
five due to below-chance performance (i.e., accuracy rate ,.5),
and one for not completing the manipulation forms.
Materials and Procedure
Procedure generally followed that of Experiment 1 with few
changes. First, instead of asking about the mean value of the
sequence participants were presented after each sequence of num-
bers with two probe-numbers, and were asked to choose which one
of them has been presented in the sequence. One of these numbers
was randomly sampled from the eight numbers that were presented
in that sequence, and the other number was randomly sampled from
the same Gaussian distribution, but we made sure it would not be
any of the numbers in the sequence. Participants completed 10 prac-
tice trials, and 80 critical trials in each mindset condition (i.e., a
total of 160 critical trials), see Figure 5 for an illustration.
Second, following each critical block, participants reported their
current motivation (I was motivated to succeed in the numerical
task), stress (I feel stressed at the moment), and mood (I have
a positive mood at the moment), on a 5-point scale ranging from
1(strongly disagree)to5(strongly agree).
Results
Manipulation Check
In order to validate that the manipulation elicited the intended
mindset in each condition, we used an automated text analysis tool
(TATE; Simchon, 2019), which rates the concreteness level of words
accordingtoanormeddictionary(Brysbaert et al., 2014). Higher
scores indicate higher concreteness level. Concreteness scores were
submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA, and revealed that as
expected, textsconcreteness level in the concrete mindset condition
was higher (M= 2.943, SD = .321) than in the abstract mindset con-
dition (M=2.483,SD = .312), F(1, 149) = 217.268, p,.001, h
p
2
=
.593. This analysis confirms that the experimental manipulation
made participants use abstract versus concrete language, in the
abstract versus concrete conditions as intended.
Accuracy
Memory-accuracy was submitted to the same analyses as in
Experiments 13. In contrast to our previous experiments (and in
contradiction to a general improvement account of our previous
results), there was no advantage for the abstract condition relative
to the concrete condition. Rather, the analysis revealed a marginal
effect of mindset favoring the concrete mindset condition.
Table 1
Model Comparison
Model number Model Abstract mindset (aggregated AIC) Concrete mindset (aggregated AIC)
1
yi¼Xn
i¼1
Xi
nþei
97,902 98,942
2
yi¼b0þb1Xn
i¼1
Xi
nþei
94,458 94,920
3
yi¼Xn
i¼1
1k
ðÞ
niXi
Xn
i¼1
1k
ðÞ
ni
þei
97,527 98,545
4
yi¼b0þb1Xn
i¼1
1k
ðÞ
niXi
Xn
i¼1
1k
ðÞ
ni
þei
93,927 94,394
Note. Note that AIC differences higher than 10 are considered decisive evidence in favor of the model with the lower numerical value (bold values indi-
cate the best fits).
5
https://aspredicted.org/blind2.php
6
https://prolic.co/
ABSTRACTION FACILITATES AGGREGATION OF INFORMATION 7
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Accuracy in the abstract mindset condition was slightly lower
(M= .709,SD= .100) than in the concrete mindset condition (M=
.721,SD= .093), F(1, 149) = 3.78, p= .054, h
p
2
= .025, see Figure
5The effect of order was not significant, F(1, 149) = .414, p=
.521, but the interaction of mindset and order was significant, F(1,
149) = 9.194, p= .003, h
p
2
= .058. In addition, motivation (p= .639),
stress (p= .425), and mood (p= .189) did not significantly differ
between the two mindset conditions.
Taken together, these results indicate that the improved per-
formance in the abstract mindset condition in Experiments 13
is unlikely to reflect a general effect on any type of performance
(e.g., due to higher motivation, better attention, or lesser fa-
tigue). Rather, as we discuss in the General Discussion, this pat-
tern of findings supports the notion that abstract versus concrete
processing differentially favor gist extraction versus attention to
details.
General Discussion
In three experiments, we examined how abstract versus con-
crete thinking affected aggregation of information, and in the
fourth experiment we examined how it affects memory for spe-
cific items. As predicted, we found that abstract thinking
improved aggregation accuracy compared with concrete think-
ing, whereas it did not improve accuracy in the memory task. In
Experiments 1 and 2, participants performed a numerical averag-
ing task adopted from Brezis et al. (2015), whereas in Experi-
ment 3 they performed a variation of the averaging task with
emotional faces (Haberman et al., 2009). The results were tested
using Pearson correlations as a measure of estimation precision,
but similar results obtained with deviations between correct and
estimated values (RMSD).
7
Finally, in Experiment 4 we ruled
out a general (e.g., motivational or attentional) account for the
improved performance in Experiments 13. When the task was
changed to remembering individual items, the abstract mindset
condition showed no advantage but rather a small decrement rel-
ative to the concrete mindset condition.
To better understand the processes that drive the improved aver-
age estimations in the abstract mindset, we examined several com-
putational models, assuming that the estimation of the mean is
based on transformation of numerical values (Dehaene, 1992;
Dehaene & Cohen, 1991), or emotional faces (Holmes & Lourenco,
2011) into analog/magnitude representations. We found an overall
recency effectthis did not differ significantly between the abstract
and concrete conditions. Rather, our best fitting model indicated
that concrete thinking resulted in higher levels of regression toward
the mean (lower slopes) compared with abstract thinking. One way
to understand the increased regression to the mean in the concrete
condition is viewing it as a compensatory strategy in face of uncer-
tainty or difficulty. This possible explanation assumes that partici-
pants in the concrete condition experienced more difficulty and/or
uncertainty in their estimations and as a result provided a response
that was closer to the mean of the entire block (Anobile et al., 2012;
Figure 5
Task and Results of Experiment 4
Note. (A) After each sequence of numbers, participants were presented with two probe-numbers and asked to
choose which one of them has been presented in the sequence. (B) Memory accuracy rates by mindset condition.
Blue and orange circles correspond to the accuracy rates of each participant in the abstract and concrete condi-
tions, respectively. Gray circles correspond to the conditions means. The central mark on each box-plot indicates
the median, and the bottom and top edges indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively. p,.10. See the
online article for the color version of this gure.
7
Across the three experiments, the RMSD for the abstract thinking was
smaller than that of the concrete thinking, t(174) = 2.86, p= .004. In
addition, there was a high correlation between the two precision measures,
Pearson correlation and RMSD, across participants and conditions (r=
.61, p,.001)
8HADAR, GLICKMAN, TROPE, LIBERMAN, AND USHER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Jazayeri & Shadlen, 2010). Indeed, previous work suggests that
participants have access to their averaging uncertainty as their esti-
mation confidence decreases with the variance of the sequence
(Rosenbaum et al., 2021). Future studies could examine the specific
strategy that gives rise to more regressive responses in the concrete
processing condition: Is it the case that participants in this condition
represent (a limited number of) individual samples rather than keep-
ing track of a running average? Do they fail to keep track of the
boundaries between different trials? Do they provide a defaultre-
gressive middle-of-the-scale response on a larger proportion of the
trials? All of these (nonmutually exclusive) strategies would pro-
duce more regressive responses in the concrete than the abstract
condition.
Finally, the measure of signal-to-noise ratio (slope/error-term)
was higher for the abstract condition, indicating that abstract think-
ing created a less noisy representation of the sequences of values in
each trial. We find this pattern of results noteworthy, because it indi-
cates that abstract processing gave rise to better, more accurate inte-
gration, that at the same time was not characterized by increased
information loss (as would be predicted, e.g., if abstraction would
lead to heuristic, less elaborate processing).
Another prediction derived from CLT is that an abstract mindset
would improve performance when gist extraction or filtering-out
irrelevant information is beneficial (e.g., Hadar et al., 2019), but not
when one needs to retain the exact details. The results of Experiment
4 are consistent with this prediction. However, future research with
a larger sample of participants and stimuli types will be needed to
examine this hypothesis more closely.
In conclusion, people may face a need to average series of val-
ues when they consider which stock to buy, which people to
befriend, or which student shows more promising performance.
Understanding the contextual features that contribute to the accu-
racy of the averaging process could thus inform not only theories
of decision-making and (social) cognition, but also educators
and policymakers.
References
Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE
Transactions on Automatic Control,19(6), 716723. https://doi.org/10
.1109/TAC.1974.1100705
Anderson, N. H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change. Psycholog-
ical Review,78(3), 171206. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030834
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory
(Vol. 578). Academic Press.
Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M., & Burr, D. C. (2012). Linear mapping of
numbers onto space requires attention. Cognition,122(3), 454459.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.11.006
Ariely, D. (2001). Seeing sets: Representation by statistical properties.
Psychological Science,12(2), 157162. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467
-9280.00327
Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. JournalofAbnormal
and Social Psychology,41,258290. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0055756
Baek, J., & Chong, S. C. (2020). Ensemble perception and focused atten-
tion: Two different modes of visual processing to cope with limited
capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,27(4), 602606. https://doi
.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01718-7
Betsch, T., Plessner, H., Schwieren, C., & Gütig, R. (2001). I like it but I
dont know why: A value-account approach to implicit attitude
formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,27(2), 242253.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201272009
Brezis, N., Bronfman, Z. Z., & Usher, M. (2015). Adaptive spontaneous
transitions between two mechanisms of numerical averaging. Scientific
Reports,5(1), 10415. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep10415
Brezis, N., Bronfman, Z. Z., Jacoby, N., Lavidor, M., & Usher, M. (2016).
Transcranial direct current stimulation over the parietal cortex improves
approximate numerical averaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
28(11), 17001713. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00991
Brezis, N., Bronfman, Z. Z., & Usher, M. (2018). A perceptual-like pop-
ulation-coding mechanism of approximate numerical averaging. Neu-
ral Computation,30(2), 428446. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco_a
_01037
Brusovansky, M., Glickman, M., & Usher, M. (2018). Fast and effec-
tive: Intuitive processes in complex decisions. Psychonomic Bulletin
&Review,25(4), 15421548. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018
-1474-1
Brusovansky, M., Vanunu, Y., & Usher, M. (2019). Why we should quit
while were ahead: When do averages matter more than sums? Decision,
6(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000087
Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings
for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research
Methods,46(3), 904911. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5
Burgoon, E. M., Henderson, M. D., & Markman, A. B. (2013). There are
many ways to see the forest for the trees: A tour guide for abstraction.
Perspectives on Psychological Science,8(5), 501520. https://doi.org/10
.1177/1745691613497964
Chong, S. C., & Treisman, A. (2003). Representation of statistical proper-
ties. Vision Research,43(4), 393404. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042
-6989(02)00596-5
Chong, S. C., & Treisman, A. (2005). Statistical processing: Computing
the average size in perceptual groups. Vision Research,45(7), 891900.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.004
Chong, S. C., Joo, S. J., Emmanouil, T. A., & Treisman, A. (2008). Statis-
tical processing: Not so implausible after all. Perception & Psychophy-
sics,70(7), 13271334. https://doi.org/10.3758/pp.70.7.1327
Dakin, S. C., & Watt, R. J. (1997). The computation of orientation statis-
tics from visual texture. Vision Research,37(22), 31813192. https://doi
.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00133-8
de Fockert, J., & Wolfenstein, C. (2009). Rapid extraction of mean identity
from sets of faces. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Experimental Psychology,62(9), 17161722. https://doi.org/10
.1080/17470210902811249
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition,44(1-2),
142. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1991). Two mental calculation systems: A case
study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsycholo-
gia,29(11), 10451054. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(91)90076-K
Ding, Y., & Keh, H. T. (2017). Consumer reliance on intangible versus
tangible attributes in service evaluation: The role of construal level.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,45(6), 848865. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s11747-017-0527-8
Efrat-Treister, D., Daniels, M. A., & Robinson, S. L. (2020). Putting time
in perspective: How and why construal level buffers the relationship
between wait time and aggressive tendencies. Journal of Organizational
Behavior,41(3), 294309. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2433
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Measuring facial movement. Environ-
mental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior,1(1), 5675. https://doi
.org/10.1007/BF01115465
Eyal, T., Hoover, G. M., Fujita, K., & Nussbaum, S. (2011). The effect of
distance-dependent construals on schema-driven impression formation.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,47(1), 278281. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.007
ABSTRACTION FACILITATES AGGREGATION OF INFORMATION 9
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Florey, J., Clifford, C. W., Dakin, S., & Mareschal, I. (2016). Spatial limi-
tations in averaging social cues. Scientific Reports,6(1), 32210. https://
doi.org/10.1038/srep32210
Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract
and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding othersself-regula-
tory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,40(6),
739752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.003
Gilead, M., Liberman, N., & Maril, A. (2014). From mind to matter: Neural
correlates of abstract and concrete mindsets. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience,9(5), 638645. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst031
Gilead, M., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2020). Above and beyond the con-
crete: The diverse representational substrates of the predictive brain. Be-
havioral and Brain Sciences,43,e121.https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525
X19002000
Haberman, J., Brady, T. F., & Alvarez, G. A. (2015). Individual differen-
ces in ensemble perception reveal multiple, independent levels of en-
semble representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
144(2), 432446. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000053
Haberman, J., & Whitney, D. (2007). Rapid extraction of mean emotion
and gender from sets of faces. Current Biology,17(17), R751R753.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.039
Haberman, J., Harp, T., & Whitney, D. (2009). Averaging facial expression
over time. Journal of Vision,9(11), 1. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.11.1
Hadar, B., Luria, R., & Liberman, N. (2019). Concrete mindset impairs fil-
tering in visual working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
26(6), 19171924. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01625-6
Hadar, B. (2021, June 2). Abstract thinking facilitates aggregation of infor-
mation.osf.io/y4uxd/
Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Perceiving persons and groups.
Psychological Review,103(2), 336355. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033
-295X.103.2.336
Hansen, J., & Trope, Y. (2013). When time flies: How abstract and con-
crete mental construal affect the perception of time. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: General,142(2), 336347. https://doi.org/10
.1037/a0029283
Holmes,K.J.,&Lourenco,S.F.(2011).Commonspatialorganization
of number and emotional expression: A mental magnitude line. Brain
and Cognition,77(2), 315323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011
.07.002
Hubert-Wallander, B., & Boynton, G. M. (2015). Not all summary statis-
tics are made equal: Evidence from extracting summaries across time.
Journal of Vision,15(4), 5. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.4.5
Jazayeri, M., & Shadlen, M. N. (2010). Temporal context calibrates inter-
val timing. Nature Neuroscience,13(8), 10201026. https://doi.org/10
.1038/nn.2590
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Khayat, N., & Hochstein, S. (2018). Perceiving set mean and range: Auto-
maticity and precision. Journal of Vision,18(9), 23. https://doi.org/10
.1167/18.9.23
Kille, D. R., Eibach, R. P., Wood, J. V., & Holmes, J. G. (2017). Who
cant take a compliment? The role of construal level and self-esteem in
accepting positive feedback from close others. Journal of Experimen-
tal Social Psychology,68(1), 4049. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp
.2016.05.003
Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2014). Traversing psychological distance.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences,18(7), 364369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.tics.2014.03.001
Malmi, R. A., & Samson, D. J. (1983). Intuitive averaging of categorized
numerical stimuli. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
22(5), 547559. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90337-7
Maule, J., Witzel, C., & Franklin, A. (2014). Getting the gist of multiple
hues: Metric and categorical effects on ensemble perception of hue. Jour-
nal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, Image Science, and
Vision,31(4), A93A102. https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.31.000A93
Napier,J.L.,Luguri,J.B.,Dovidio,J.F.,&Oltman,K.A.(2018).
Construing the essence: The effects of construal level on genetic
attributions for individual and social group differences. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin,44(9), 13951407. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0146167218768799
Neumann, M. F., Schweinberger, S. R., & Burton, A. M. (2013). Viewers
extract mean and individual identity from sets of famous faces. Cognition,
128(1), 5663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.006
Parkes, L., Lund, J., Angelucci, A., Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. (2001).
Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision.
Nature Neuroscience,4(7), 739744. https://doi.org/10.1038/89532
Post, R. B., Haberman, J., Iwaki, L., & Whitney, D. (2012). The frozen
face effect: Why static photographs may not do you justice. Frontiers in
Psychology,3(1), 22. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00022
Robitaille, N., & Harris, I. M. (2011). When more is less: Extraction of
summary statistics benefits from larger sets. Journal of Vision,11(12),
18. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.12.18
Rosenbaum, D., Glickman, M., & Usher, M. (2021). Extracting sum-
mary statistics of rapid numerical sequences. Frontiers in Psychol-
ogy,12, 693575. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.693575
Simchon, A. (2019). TATE: Text analysis tools for English (R package
version 3.0) [Computer software]. https://github.com/almogsi/TATE
Solomon, J. A. (2010). Visual discrimination of orientation statistics in
crowded and uncrowded arrays. Journal of Vision,10(14), 19. https://
doi.org/10.1167/10.14.19
Spitzer, B., Waschke, L., & Summerfield, C. (2017). Selective over-
weighting of larger magnitudes during noisy numerical comparison.
Nature Human Behaviour,1(8), 145. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562
-017-0145
Spunt, R. P., Kemmerer, D., & Adolphs, R. (2016). The neural basis of
conceptualizing the same action at different levels of abstraction. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,11(7), 11411151. https://doi
.org/10.1093/scan/nsv084
Stillman, P. E., Medvedev, D., & Ferguson, M. J. (2017). Resisting tempta-
tion: Tracking how self-control conflicts are successfully resolved in
real time. Psychological Science,28(9), 12401258. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0956797617705386
Sweeny, T. D., & Whitney, D. (2014). Perceiving crowd attention: Ensem-
ble perception of a crowds gaze. Psychological Science,25(10),
19031913. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614544510
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological
distance. Psychological Review,117(2), 440463. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0018963
Usher, M., & McClelland, J. L. (2001). The time course of perceptual
choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychological Review,
108(3), 550592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.550
vanOsch,Y.,Blanken,I.,Meijs,M.H.,&vanWolferen,J.(2015).A
groups physical attractiveness is greater than the average attractive-
ness of its members: The group attractiveness effect. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,41(4), 559574. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167215572799
Vanunu, Y., Pachur, T., & Usher, M. (2019). Constructing preference from
sequential samples: The impact of evaluation format on risk attitudes.
Decision,6(3), 223236. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000098
Walker, D., & Vul, E. (2014). Hierarchical encoding makes individuals in
a group seem more attractive. Psychological Science,25(1), 230235.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613497969
Watamaniuk, S. N., & Duchon, A. (1992). The human visual system aver-
ages speed information. Vision Research,32(5), 931941. https://doi
.org/10.1016/0042-6989(92)90036-I
Watamaniuk, S. N., & McKee, S. P. (1998). Simultaneous encoding of
direction at a local and global scale. Perception & Psychophysics,60(2),
191200. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206028
10 HADAR, GLICKMAN, TROPE, LIBERMAN, AND USHER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Watamaniuk, S. N., Sekuler, R., & Williams, D. W. (1989). Direction per-
ception in complex dynamic displays: The integration of direction infor-
mation. Vision Research,29(1), 4759. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042
-6989(89)90173-9
Whitney, D., & Yamanashi Leib, A. (2018). Ensemble perception. Annual
Review of Psychology,69(1), 105129. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev
-psych-010416-044232
Yamanashi Leib, A., Chang, K., Xia, Y., Peng, A., & Whitney, D. (2020).
Fleeting impressions of economic value via summary statistical representa-
tions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,149(10), 18111822.
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000745
Yamanashi Leib, A., Kosovicheva, A., & Whitney, D. (2016). Fast ensem-
ble representations for abstract visual impressions. Nature Communica-
tions,7(1), 13186. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13186
Yudkin, D. A., Liberman, N., Wakslak, C., & Trope, Y. (2020). Better off
and far away: Reactions to othersoutcomes depends on their distance.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,156,1323.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.10.004
Received September 7, 2020
Revision received June 1, 2021
Accepted July 9, 2021 n
ABSTRACTION FACILITATES AGGREGATION OF INFORMATION 11
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Many studies have investigated how construal levels affect cognitive processing across disciplines. Research has shown that high construal levels facilitate information aggregation (Hadar et al., 2022). In this study, all participants were instructed to complete a construal-level priming manipulation task. ...
... We determined the sample size based on multiple studies that employed the same task and data analysis methods, establishing a minimum of 55 participants per experimental condition (e.g., Guest et al., 2016;Hadar et al., 2022;Honke & Kurtz, 2019). We recruited 131 undergraduate students for the study (63 males and 68 females, mean age = 21.55 years, SD = 1.11 years). ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined whether a high-level construal mindset promotes categorizing information according to thematic relations. In three experiments, the construal-level priming task was used to initiate a high-level versus low-level construal mindset, and then all participants were asked to complete the triad task, which is a task measuring the preference to classify. The results of Experiments 1 and 2 have shown that regardless of whether it was a set of artificially produced objects (Experiment 1) or a set of natural objects (Experiment 2), the high-level construal mindset group exhibited a higher proportion of thematic responses in the triad task. Experiment 3 transformed the stimulus set of the triad task into a set that consisted of many, larger, published, and controlled/optimized stimuli. The results of the experiment still showed that the high-level construal mindset group exhibited a higher proportion of thematic responses in the triad task. The findings suggest that a high-level construal mindset promotes categorizing information based on thematic relations.
... In Study 2 we conducted a similar analysis, except that experimental condition was a between-participant factor. We found a marginally significant interaction, In the Supplementary materials we report additional modeling analyses that are commonly used within the value psychophysics tradition 26,27 . ...
... Just like an abstract category (e.g., dogs) represents different exemplars (e.g., poodle, Airedale-Terrier, Chihuahua) by glossing over how they are different from each other, a mean represents different instances (the exemplars over which the mean was computed) by glossing over their variability 34 . Most relevant to the notion that averaging is an instance of abstraction is a recent set of studies 26 which demonstrated that participants estimated the average of numerical values and of facial expressions with greater accuracy after being induced with an abstract mindset than after A) Study 1 (within-participants) B) Study 2 (Between-participants) www.nature.com/scientificreports/ being induced with a concrete mindset (by repeatedly generating abstract goals for performing an action vs. concrete means to perform it, respectively). ...
Article
Full-text available
Humans learn both directly, from own experience, and via social communication, from the experience of others. They also often integrate these two sources of knowledge to make predictions and choices. We hypothesized that when faced with the need to integrate communicated information into personal experience, people would represent the average of experienced exemplars with greater accuracy. In two experiments, Mturk users estimated the mean of consecutively and rapidly presented number sequences that represented bonuses ostensibly paid by different providers on a crowdsource platform. Participants who expected integrating these values with verbal information about possible change in bonuses were more accurate in extracting the means of the values compared to participants who did not have such expectation. While our study focused on socially communicated information, the observed effect may potentially extend to other forms of information integration. We suggest that expected integration of experience with additional information facilitates an abstract representation of personal experiences.
... This is due to conversational search providing integrated and summarized information tailored to specific questions [40]. It aligns better with the high-level abstract thinking used by users in these situations [33,75]. Conversely, web search provides a list of results, requiring users to filter and summarize information themselves. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Conversational search offers an easier and faster alternative to conventional web search, while having downsides like lack of source verification. Research has examined performance disparities between these two systems in different settings. However, little work has considered the effects of variations within a given search task. We hypothesize that psychological distance - one''s perceived closeness to a target event - affects information needs in search tasks, and investigate the corresponding effects on user preferences between web and conversational search systems. We find that with greater psychological distances, users perceive conversational search as more credible, useful, enjoyable, and easy to use, and demonstrate increased preference for this system. We reveal qualitative reasons for these differences and provide design implications for search system designers.
... However, we can accurately perceive our surroundings at a glance. Previous studies attribute the process of perceiving multiple items presented simultaneously or sequentially to ensemble coding, in which observers extract summary statistics (e.g., mean, variance, the shape of a distribution); the common and intuitive way to accomplish this is averaging from a group of items [1][2][3]. Ensemble coding is applicable to a variety of visual dimensions ranging from low-level properties (e.g., size [4][5][6], orientation [7,8], and hue [9,10]) to highlevel properties (e.g., facial expression [11][12][13], biological motion [14], and even economic value [15]). ...
Article
Full-text available
Ensemble coding allows observers to form an average to represent a set of elements. However, it is unclear whether observers can extract an average from a cross-category set. Previous investigations on this issue using low-level stimuli yielded contradictory results. The current study addressed this issue by presenting high-level stimuli (i.e., a crowd of facial expressions) simultaneously (Experiment 1) or sequentially (Experiment 2), and asked participants to complete a member judgment task. The results showed that participants could extract average information from a group of cross-category facial expressions with a short perceptual distance. These findings demonstrate cross-category ensemble coding of high-level stimuli, contributing to the understanding of ensemble coding and providing inspiration for future research.
... This differs from tasks designed to support the very existence of EP as a process and used to demonstrate that EP can operate independently from the encoding of individual items. In a recent study (Hadar et al., 2022), people were more accurate at averaging numerical values or facial emotions after the induction of abstract thinking, compared to concrete thinking. Trait propensity to reason abstractly (e.g., Vallacher & Wegner, 1989) could be related to the domain-general component of EP. ...
Article
Full-text available
People can report summary statistics for various features about a group of objects. One theory is that different abilities support ensemble judgments about low-level features like color versus high-level features like identity. Existing research mostly evaluates such claims based on evidence of correlations within and between feature domains. However, correlations between two identical tasks that only differ in the type of feature that is used can be inflated by method variance. Another concern is that conclusions about high-level features are mostly based on faces. We used latent variable methods on data from 237 participants to investigate the abilities supporting low-level and high-level feature ensemble judgments. Ensemble judgment was measured with six distinct tests, each requiring judgments for a distinct low-level or high-level feature, using different task requirements. We also controlled for other general visual abilities when examining how low-level and high-level ensemble abilities relate to each other. Confirmatory factor analyses showed a perfect correlation between the two factors, suggesting a single ability. There was a unique relationship between these two factors beyond the influence of object recognition and perceptual speed. Additional results from 117 of the same participants also ruled out the role of working memory. This study provides strong evidence of a general ensemble judgment ability across a wide range of features at the latent level and characterizes its relationship to other visual abilities.
... Indeed, a more thorough processing of anchor information increases anchoring effects (e.g., Bodenhausen et al., 2000). On the other hand, more abstract and high-level construal seems to facilitate global processing of sequential information and its comparison with a prototypical and abstract representation of the event (Eyal et al., 2011;Hadar et al., 2022;Kalkstein et al., 2018;Nussbaum et al., 2003). ...
... More specifically, abstraction engenders two distinct yet related processes (see Gilead et al., 2020 for a review of the functions of abstraction): One process is filtering out of irrelevant information, such that distracting or irrelevant details are omitted, making it easier to concentrate on the critical parameters of the problem 21 . The second process is integration of information, such that objects are grouped together to form a broader category 22,23 . Both processes might assist mental operations that are critical for solving complex problems. ...
Article
Full-text available
Performance on standardized academic aptitude tests (AAT) can determine important life outcomes. However, it is not clear whether and which aspects of the content of test questions affect performance. We examined the effect of psychological distance embedded in test questions. In Study 1 ( N = 41,209), we classified the content of existing AAT questions as invoking proximal versus distal details. We found better performance with proximal compared to distal questions, especially for low-achieving examinees. Studies 2 and 3 manipulated the distance of questions adapted from AATs and examined three moderators: overall AAT score, working-memory capacity, and presence of irrelevant information. In Study 2 ( N = 129), proximity (versus distance) improved the performance of low-achieving participants. In Study 3 ( N = 1744), a field study, among low-achieving examinees, proximity improved performance on questions that included irrelevant information. Together, these results suggest that the psychological distance that is invoked by the content of test questions has important consequences for performance in real-life high-stakes tests.
Article
Full-text available
Artificial intelligence (AI) technologies are rapidly advancing, enhancing human capabilities across various fields spanning from finance to medicine. Despite their numerous advantages, AI systems can exhibit biased judgements in domains ranging from perception to emotion. Here, in a series of experiments (n = 1,401 participants), we reveal a feedback loop where human–AI interactions alter processes underlying human perceptual, emotional and social judgements, subsequently amplifying biases in humans. This amplification is significantly greater than that observed in interactions between humans, due to both the tendency of AI systems to amplify biases and the way humans perceive AI systems. Participants are often unaware of the extent of the AI’s influence, rendering them more susceptible to it. These findings uncover a mechanism wherein AI systems amplify biases, which are further internalized by humans, triggering a snowball effect where small errors in judgement escalate into much larger ones.
Article
Full-text available
Although the focus of research for decades, there is a surprising lack of consensus on what is (and what is not) self-control. We review some of the most prominent theoretical models of self-control, including those that highlight conflicts between smaller-sooner versus larger-later rewards, “hot” emotions versus “cool” cognitions, and efficient automatic versus resource-intensive controlled processes. After discussing some of their shortcomings, we propose an alternative approach based on tenets of construal level theory (Trope et al., 2021) that integrates these disparate models while also providing novel insights. Specifically, we model self-control as a problem of regulatory scope—the range of considerations one accounts for in any decision or behavior. Self-control conflicts occur when the pursuit of specific local opportunities threatens the ability to address motivational priorities that span a broader array of time, places, individuals, and possibilities. Whereas a more contractive consideration of relevant concerns may prompt indulgence in temptation, a more expansive consideration of concerns should not only help people identify the self-control conflict but also successfully resolve it. We review empirical evidence that supports this new framework and discuss implications and new directions. This regulatory framework not only clarifies what is and what is not self-control but also provides new insights that can be leveraged to enhance self-control in all its various forms.
Article
Poverty impedes children's executive function (EF). Therefore, it is necessary to mitigate the negative effect of poverty by developing efficient interventions to improve poor children's cognitive function. In three studies, we examined whether high-level construals can improve EF among poor children in China. In Study 1, we observed a positive relationship between family socioeconomic status and children's EF, which was moderated by construal level (n = 206; M age = 9.71; 45.6% girls). In Study 2a, we experimentally induced high- versus low-level construals and found that poor children with high-level construals exhibited better EF than those with low-level construals (n = 65; M age = 11.32; 47.7% girls). However, the same intervention did not affect the performance of affluent children in Study 2b (n = 63; M age = 10.54; 54% girls). Moreover, we found that the interventional effects of high-level construals improved the ability of children living in poverty to make healthy decisions and delayed gratification in Study 3 (n = 74; M age = 11.10; 45.9% girls). These findings may have implications for using high-level construals as an effective intervention to improve poor children's EF and cognitive capacity.
Article
Full-text available
We examine the ability of observers to extract summary statistics (such as the mean and the relative-variance) from rapid numerical sequences of two digit numbers presented at a rate of 4/s. In four experiments (total N = 100), we find that the participants show a remarkable ability to extract such summary statistics and that their precision in the estimation of the sequence-mean improves with the sequence-length (subject to individual differences). Using model selection for individual participants we find that, when only the sequence-average is estimated, most participants rely on a holistic process of frequency based estimation with a minority who rely on a (rule-based and capacity limited) mid-range strategy. When both the sequence-average and the relative variance are estimated, about half of the participants rely on these two strategies. Importantly, the holistic strategy appears more efficient in terms of its precision. We discuss implications for the domains of two pathways numerical processing and decision-making.
Article
Full-text available
Visual processing is limited: we cannot exhaustively analyze every object in a scene in a brief glance. However, ensemble perception affords the visual system a rapid shortcut to efficiently evaluate multiple objects. Ensemble processing has been widely tested across basic features. However, ensemble perception could be especially important and valuable for processes that are normally thought to require cognitive deliberative effort. One typical high-level cognitive process that humans engage in frequently is evaluating the value of objects. Here, we presented brief displays of consumer products to human observers and measured their visual sensitivity to the average value of the sets. We found that participants were sensitive to the average value of sets of products even when they did not have explicit memory for every item in the display. Our results show that value judgments can be based on ensemble information. Although value is thought to be an inferential concept, ensemble processing affords the brain a heuristic to efficiently assign value to entire sets of objects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
We spend a substantial part of our daily life waiting, and unfortunately, wait time can fuel aggressive tendencies. Our study examines the relationship between wait time, perceived wait time, and aggressive tendencies from a construal level perspective. In Study 1, we found that the higher the construal level, the stronger the relationship between actual and perceived wait time and the stronger relationship between perceived wait time and aggressive tendencies. In Study 2, we manipulated construal level and found that power explains the moderating impact of construal on the wait ‐ aggressive tendencies relationships. Results demonstrate the role of construal in explaining both perceived wait time and aggressive responses to long wait times, suggesting that mental construal influences both the psychological experience of time and the subsequent reaction to that experience. Overall, these results contribute to research on subjective time perspective by enhancing the knowledge and understanding of the determinants and effects of perceived wait time.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, scientists have increasingly taken to investigate the predictive nature of cognition. We argue that prediction relies on abstraction, and thus theories of predictive cognition need an explicit theory of abstract representation. We propose such a theory of the abstract representational capacities that allow humans to transcend the “here-and-now”. Consistent with the predictive cognition literature, we suggest that the representational substrates of the mind are built as a hierarchy , ranging from the concrete to the abstract; however, we argue that there are qualitative differences between elements along this hierarchy, generating meaningful, often unacknowledged, diversity. Echoing views from philosophy, we suggest that the representational hierarchy can be parsed into: modality-specific representations, instantiated on perceptual similarity; multimodal representations, primarily instantiated on the discovery of spatiotemporal contiguity; and categorical representations, primarily instantiated on social interaction. These elements serve as the building blocks of complex structure s discussed in cognitive psychology (e.g., episodes, scripts) and are the inputs for mental representations that behave like functions, typically discussed in linguistics (i.e., predicators ). We support our argument for representational diversity by explaining how the elements in our ontology are all required in order to account for humans’ predictive cognition (e.g., in subserving logic-based prediction; in optimizing the trade-off between accurate and detailed predictions) and by examining how the neuroscientific evidence coheres with our account. In doing so, we provide a testable model of the neural bases of conceptual cognition, and highlight several important implications to research on self-projection, reinforcement learning, and predictive-processing models of psychopathology.
Article
Full-text available
The cognitive mechanism underlying decisions based on sequential samples has been found to be affected by whether multiple alternatives are evaluated together or whether each alternative is evaluated individually. In this experiment, we examined whether evaluation format can also lead to different preference orders among risky alternatives. We hypothesized that because of differences in computational demands posed by the 2 evaluation formats, there would be differences in the type of the cognitive mechanism deployed: a risk-return mechanism, that trades off the mean reward and risk of an alternative, or a selective-accumulator mechanism, that sums the rewards of each alternative, with a higher weight to more extreme payoffs. Each participant rated the same set of alternatives (sequences of payoffs from slot machines) in both a one-by-one and a grouped evaluation format. The mean and the variance of the payoff distributions of each alternative were varied orthogonally. As predicted, in the grouped (but not in the one-by-one) condition, the impact of the variance on participants’ ratings interacted with the mean payoff. Specifically, participants were risk averse for alternatives with a low mean payoff and risk seeking for alternatives with a high mean payoff. Computational modeling showed that the majority of participants were best described by a risk–return model in the one-by-one condition but by a selective-accumulator model in the grouped condition. Our results underline the importance of studying the cognitive foundations of risk attitudes in order to understand how they are shaped by the structure of a given decision task.
Article
The visual system has a limited capacity for dealing with complex and redundant information in a scene. Here, we propose that a distributed attention mode of processing is necessary for coping with this limit, together with a focused attention mode of processing. The distributed attention mode provides a statistical summary of a scene, whereas the focused attention mode provides relevant information for object recognition. In this paper, we claim that a distributed mode of processing is necessary because (1) averaging performance improves with increased set-sizes, (2) even unselected items are likely to contribute to averaging, and (3) the assumption of variable capacity limits in averaging over different set-sizes is not plausible. We then propose how the averaging process can access multiple items over the capacity limit of focused attention. The visual system can represent multiple items as population responses and read out relevant information using the two modes of attention. It can summarize population responses with a broad application of a Gaussian profile (i.e., distributed attention) and represent its peak as the mean. It can focus on relevant population responses with a narrow application of a Gaussian profile (i.e., focused attention) and select important information for object recognition. The two attention modes of processing provide a framework for incorporating two seemingly opposing fields of study (ensemble perception and selective attention) and a unified theory of a coping strategy with our limited capacity.
Article
Two studies tested whether a mindset manipulation would affect the filtering of distractors from entering visual working memory (VWM). In Study 1, participants completed a concrete mindset manipulation (by repeatedly describing how to perform an action), an abstract mindset manipulation (by repeatedly describing why to perform an action), and a baseline condition (no manipulation). In Study 2, some participants completed a concrete mindset manipulation, whereas others completed an abstract manipulation. Filtering efficiency was estimated by a change-detection task that included a condition with distractors alongside targets. We derived our prediction from construal-level theory (CLT), according to which concrete representations retain information regardless of its relevance, whereas abstract representations retain the relevant and omit the irrelevant elements of an input array. In a task that requires attending to task-relevant targets and ignoring task-irrelevant distractors in a visual array, concrete processing should impair performance relative to abstract processing. We therefore predicted that a concrete mindset would reduce filtering efficiency as compared to an abstract mindset. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that a concrete mindset manipulation reduced filtering efficiency, as compared to both an abstract mindset manipulation (Studies 1 and 2) and the baseline condition (Study 1). These results suggest a new factor that may contribute to both individual differences and situational variation in working memory performance.
Article
To compensate for the limited visual information that can be perceived and remembered at any given moment, many aspects of the visual world are represented as summary statistics. We acquire ensemble representations of element groups as a whole, spreading attention over objects, for which we encode no detailed information. Previous studies found that different features of items (from size/orientation to facial expression/biological motion) are summarized to their mean, over space or time. Summarizing is economical, saving time and energy when the environment is too rich and complex to encode each stimulus separately. We investigated set perception using rapid serial visual presentation sequences. Following each sequence, participants viewed two stimuli, member and nonmember, indicating the member. Sometimes, unbeknownst to participants, one stimulus was the set mean, and or the nonmember was outside the set range. Participants preferentially chose stimuli at/near the mean, a "mean effect," and more easily rejected out-of-range stimuli, a "range effect." Performance improved with member proximity to the mean and nonmember distance from set mean and edge, though they were instructed only to remember presented stimuli. We conclude that participants automatically encode both mean and range boundaries of stimulus sets, avoiding capacity limits and speeding perceptual decisions.
Article
The present research links a nonsocial, contextual influence (construal level) to the tendency to endorse genetic attributions for individual and social group differences. Studies 1 to 3 show that people thinking in an abstract (vs. concrete) mind-set score higher on a measure of genetic attributions for individual and racial group differences. Study 4 showed that abstract (vs. concrete) construal also increased genetic attributions for novel groups. Study 5 explored the potential downstream consequences of construal on intergroup attitudes, and found that abstract (vs. concrete) construal led people to endorse genetic attributions in general and this was associated with increased anti-Black prejudice.