ArticlePDF Available

Solitude Skills and the Private Self

American Psychological Association
Qualitative Psychology
Authors:

Abstract

The construct of solitude skills suggests that successfully navigating the domain of solitude may require specific psychological resources, but this theoretical possibility has not yet been investigated empirically. Fourteen well-adjusted adults (Mage=49.5) participated in a qualitative study that examined their lived experiences with solitude and sought to identify the skills they utilized when engaging in positive solitude. Thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006) of narrative data resulted in the identification of eight solitude skills organized within three central concepts: Connect with Self included the skills of enjoying solitary activities, emotion regulation, and introspection; the skills included in Protect Time were making time to be alone, using that time mindfully, and validating one’s need for solitude; and the skills of Find a Balance included heeding signals to enter solitude and knowing when to exit solitude. These findings illuminate how the documented benefits of solitude are enacted and illustrate how solitude sustains the private self, which clinicians have argued promotes well-being (Modell, 1992). Knowledge of these skills may be valuable for those who volitionally enter solitude as well as those who find themselves in unwanted isolation. These findings lay the groundwork for future studies to examine whether the solitude skills identified here apply to other populations, and to explore the efficacy of solitude skills trainings in promoting psychological well-being or as a clinical intervention.
APPLIED ARTICLE
Solitude Skills and the Private Self
Virginia Thomas
Department of Psychology, Middlebury College
The construct of solitude skills suggests that successfully navigating the domain of solitude
may require specic psychological resources, but this theoretical possibility has not yet been
investigated empirically. Fourteen well-adjusted adults (M
age
=49.5) participated in a
qualitative study that examined their lived experiences with solitude and sought to identify
the skills they utilized when engaging in positive solitude. Thematic analysis (Braun &
Clarke, 2006) of narrative data resulted in the identication of eight solitude skills organized
within three central concepts: Connect with Self included the skills of enjoying solitary
activities, emotion regulation, and introspection; the skills included in Protect Time were
making time to be alone, using that time mindfully, and validating ones need for solitude;
and the skills of Find a Balance included heeding signals to enter solitude and knowing
when to exit solitude. These ndings illuminate how the documented benets of solitude are
enacted and illustrate how solitude sustains the private self, which clinicians have argued
promotes well-being (Modell, 1992). Knowledge of these skills may be valuable for those
who volitionally enter solitude as well as those who nd themselves in unwanted isolation.
These ndings lay the groundwork for future studies to examine whether the solitude skills
identied here apply to other populations, and to explore the efcacy of solitude skills
trainings in promoting psychological well-being or as a clinical intervention.
Keywords: solitude, well-being, private self, emotion regulation, thematic analysis
Humans are social and solitary creatures both.
We spend much of our lives engaged in relation-
ships with others, and the psychological literature
on attachment attests to the primacy of social
connection for well-being (cf. Bowlby, 1988).
Yet literature indicates that intentionally retreat-
ing from the social sphere is also benecial for
well-being, a point demonstrated in empirical
studies showing solitudes role in self-regulation
(Nguyen et al., 2018) and self-reection (Long &
Averill, 2003), and in the clinical literature
arguing that solitude is necessary to sustain the
authentic, private self (Modell, 1993).
Dening Solitude
Solitude has been dened as volitional time
spent by oneself that is generally used construc-
tively; in other words, alone time that is sought after
and utilized for the purpose of engaging in intrinsi-
cally motivated activities (Galanaki, 2013;Koch,
1994;Larson, 1990;Marcoen & Goossens, 1993).
Solitude is marked by an absence of communica-
tion and interaction with other people, typically
but not necessarilyinvolving physical separation
from others. For example, someone may be in a
public place but still be in solitude by choosing to
not interact with others (Burger, 1995).
Volition is important in this denition because
it indicates a desire to be alone, making solitude
distinct from loneliness. Loneliness is conceptu-
alized as an unwanted, negative state of real or
perceived social isolation, marked by a longing
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This article was published Online First December 20, 2021.
Virginia Thomas https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8751-
8213
The author wishes to thank Anjali Dutt and Margarita
Azmitia for reading drafts of this manuscript and providing
feedback throughout the writing process.
Correspondence concerning this article should be
addressed to Virginia Thomas, Department of Psychology,
Middlebury College, 276 Bicentennial Way #280, Middle-
bury, VT 05753, United States. Email: vthomas@middle
bury.edu
121
Qualitative Psychology
© 2021 American Psychological Association 2023, Vol. 10, No. 1, 121139
ISSN: 2326-3601 https://doi.org/10.1037/qup0000218
for companionship, rather than a desire to be
alone (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010). In contrast,
volitional solitude seeking increases the chance
that being alone will be a positive, benecial
experience (Larson, 1990;Lay et al., 2019;
Long & Averill, 2003).
Theoretical and empirical literature both
point to the benets of solitude throughout the
lifespan, as evidenced by the following examples.
Winnicott (1958) theorized that the capacity for
solitude begins in infancy and considered a per-
sons lack of anxiety when alone to be a develop-
mental achievement. Children who prefer solitary
play demonstrate higher attention spans and
display less emotional negativity and hostility
than their more sociable peers (Coplan & Armer,
2007;Youngblade et al., 1999). Solitude in ado-
lescence creates opportunities for identity develop-
ment, autonomy, and mood regulation (for a
review, see Goossens, 2014), and for college stu-
dents, opportunities for increased self-awareness
(Franzoi & Brewer, 1984), emotional renewal
(Long & Averill, 2003), and personal growth
(Thomas & Azmitia, 2019). In adulthood, comfort
with being alone correlated positively with satis-
faction in life and negatively with depression
(Larson & Lee, 1996), and older adultssolitude-
seeking behaviors have been shown to result in
positive affect (Lay et al., 2020).
However, there are risks involved with spend-
ing time alone. A preference for solitude has been
correlated with loneliness (Burger, 1995), and
excessive social withdrawal can create adjust-
ment difculties for children (Rubin et al.,
2009) and adolescents in particular (Bowker
et al., 2014;Wang et al., 2013). Time spent alone
has also been correlated with low mood (Larson,
1990), social anxiety (Alden & Auyeung, 2014),
and rumination (Lay et al., 2019).
In weighing the benets and risks of spending
time alone, it is important to state again that
aloneness is a multidimensional construct, one
that can include constructive solitude but also
states of unwanted loneliness and isolation
(Maes et al., 2015). Thus, it is critical to distin-
guish between solitude that is self-determined,or
autonomously chosen and generally positively
experienced, versus not self-determined, because
of their distinct correlations, respectively, to
enhanced or impaired psychosocial functioning
(Deci & Ryan, 2008;Thomas & Azmitia, 2019).
In other words, the states of anxious solitude
(Gazelle & Druhen, 2009 ) and positive solitude
(Ost Mor et al., 2020) are markedly distinct.
Positive solitude is not only volitional, but also
rich in meaning and enjoyment, whereas anxious
solitude is characterized by social inhibition and
peer rejection and is associated with poor psy-
chosocial outcomes.
Solitude can thus be viewed as an experiential
nichelled with both opportunities and dangers
(Larson, 1990, p. 156). Although much has been
written about the necessity of avoiding the dan-
gers of spending time alone, there is compara-
tively little articulation of how one might
maximize the opportunities. In other words,
how does one have a constructive, positive expe-
rience of solitude?
The Theoretical Construct of Solitude Skills
Decades ago, Rubenstein and Shaver (1982)
invoked the term solitude skills for the rst time,
suggesting that navigating the domain of solitude
and beneting from time alone may require spe-
cic psychological resources or abilities. Without
the skills to utilize solitude constructively, people
may go inward only to nd themselves lonely,
bored, or depressed.
The identication of skills in other psycholog-
ical domains has been well established; for exam-
ple, social skills trainings have been successful in
helping people move past social anxiety to func-
tion more readily in interpersonal relationships
and social groups (see Durlak et al., 2010 for a
review), and successful behavioral interventions
for depression include training individuals to use
adaptive coping skills during stressful experi-
ences (Puskar et al., 2007). By extension, it is
reasonable to expect that solitude skills trainings
could have analogous positive effects in the
intrapersonal sphere, promoting, for example,
identity development and self-regulation, and
potentially reducing experiences of loneliness
when individuals are alone.
However, to date psychologists have not inves-
tigated whether such skills exist, and if they do,
how they are enacted. Educators have suggested
the need for solitude skills curricula in our school
systems (Byrnes, 1983;Galanaki, 2005), but have
yet to articulate how children and adolescents can
develop and use these skills productively. In the
clinical arena, Rook (1988) differentiated
between loneliness that derives from a lack of
social skills versus a lack of solitude skills, but
these skills were not described. There are claims
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
122 THOMAS
that volitional solitude can serve as a treatment for
loneliness and depression (Burke, 1992;Knafo,
2012;Rook & Peplau, 1982;Young, 1982), yet
the skills needed to transform alone time into a
positive solitude experience rather than an isolat-
ing or terrifying one remain unspecied.
Aims and Research Question
Despite the lack of empirical evidence thus far,
the concept of solitude skills is theoretically
sound and worthy of investigation. The present
study was therefore designed to ll this empirical
gap and determine whether there is indeed evi-
dence for solitude skills, and in so doing afrm or
modify existing assumptions about such skills. In
short, the study sought to answer the following
research question: What are the skills individuals
utilize to experience solitude positively and con-
structively? Given the exploratory nature of the
research question, a qualitative study was de-
signed to identify and describe an initial set of
solitude skills, and no hypothesis testing was
proposed.
This research is timely because individuals are
increasingly spending more of their hours alone,
sometimes volitionally, as in the recent trend
worldwide trend of choosing to live alone
(Klinenberg, 2012) or involuntarily, as in the
case of being widowed (Dahlberg et al., 2018)
or socially isolated due to the coronavirus disease
(COVID-19) pandemic (Luchetti et al., 2020). In
addition, people of all ages are increasingly
spending more time alone online, and the costs
and benets of solitary digital technology have
been documented and regularly updated as these
technologies continue to evolve and become
more mainstream (see Burnell et al., 2021, for
a review). While some studies have found that
solitary online experiences can be useful, for
example in terms of relieving stress (Leung,
2015) or accessing mental health resources
(Hollis et al., 2020), other evidence indicates
that excessive smartphone use is a lonely, isolat-
ing experience rather than a socially connected
one (Kim, 2021;Thomas et al., 2021;Twenge
et al., 2019).
Loneliness is a problem with both societal and
individual consequences (Hawkley & Cacioppo,
2010), and if learning how to transform alone time
into a positive experience of solitude would help
to ameliorate loneliness or depression, as some
have suggested, then identifying and describing
specic solitude skills is essential. Furthermore,
an understanding of solitude skills would con-
tribute to a better understanding of what a capac-
ity for solitude looks like (Winnicott, 1958),
including how individuals who are procient in
solitude approach their alone time and extract
meaning from it, and would potentially increase
the likelihood that people who attempt solitude
will reap the benets.
Method
The present study was conducted in three
phases: (a) an online survey, which effectively
served as a screening tool for study participation;
(b) an experiential component of a solitude date;
and (c) a follow-up interview. Approval for the
study was granted by the academic Institutional
Review Board overseeing Human Subjects
Research (approval no. #HS2299).
Recruitment
Purposive sampling was utilized to recruit
adults who were positively oriented to solitude
and had multiple life experiences of spending
extensive time alone. To increase the likelihood
of transferability of results, the study avoided
sampling extreme solitaires, such as self-
identied hermits, recluses, or individuals living
in remote areas for months or years at a time (see
Storr, 1988). Rather, this study recruited partici-
pants between the ages of 30 and 60 who were not
yet retired to target adults were actively engaged
in societal roles, with commitments to work and
family or friends in their daily lives, yet who still
sought out solitude within those constraints; a
time of life identied as the generativity stage in
Eriksons life stage model (1997). Because of
solitudes noteworthy associations with creativ-
ity and spirituality (Koch, 1994;Storr, 1988), an
announcement about the study was posted on an
online creative writing network, local to a city on
the West Coast of the U.S., and at a Buddhist
meditation center located on the East Coast of the
United States, with an additional snowball effect
of word of mouth. The recruitment yer opened
with A call to those who enjoy being alonewith
an invitation to participate in a research study to
share your experiences of solitude,and briey
listed the three phases of the study and a con-
tact email.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 123
Recruited participants completed an online
survey where they gave their written consent to
participate. The online survey served as a screen-
ing tool to assess their status as solitude experts.A
review of expertise in a variety of domains
(Hatano & Oura, 2003) distinguished experts
from novices in the following ways: experts
have rich knowledge of the domain in question;
experts have gained this knowledge over years of
experience with the domain; and their knowledge
and experience over time have been accompanied
by socioemotional changes (for an example of
this approach in the eld of creativity, see
Kaufman et al., 2005). The prestudy survey,
administered online, inquired about the fre-
quency and duration of time spent alone by choice
in their daily lives and assessed their afnity for
solitude through two psychometric scales: the
Preference for Solitude Scale (Burger, 1995)
and the Motivation for Solitude Scale Short-
Form (Thomas & Azmitia, 2019). Additionally,
participants completed the UCLA Loneliness
Scale, Short-Form (Hays & DiMatteo, 1987)to
avoid oversampling a lonely population and
potentially conating positive solitude skills
with coping strategies for loneliness.
Participants
Fifteen participants were recruited and com-
pleted the prestudy survey; one participant
declined to complete the remainder of the study.
This resulted in an analytic sample of 14 adults,
aged 3361 (M
age
=49.5). Eight participants (six
women; two men) identied as creative writers
and were employed in a related profession (e.g.,
literature professor, book editor, writing tutor).
Six participants (three women; three men) iden-
tied as contemplatives who engaged in regular
meditation practice and held full- or part-time
staff positions at a contemplative retreat center
grounded in Buddhist teachings. Participants
identied as White (N=11), Japanese/French
Canadian (N=1), White/Irish (N=2), and
claimed their nation of origin as Australia
(N=1), Canada (N=1), and the United States
(N=12), although all were living in the U.S. at
the time of data collection. Eight were in long-
term relationships, and three were parents of
children currently living with them.
On average, participants reported inten-
tionally spending an average of 30.1 hr alone
per week for pleasure, beyond time spent alone
for work purposes. Nine of the fourteen parti-
cipants had spent at least two consecutive days
alone at some point in the past 2 years. Alto-
gether, the sample showed high preference for
solitude (M=10.29, SD =1.68; maximum
possible score of 12), high self-determined moti-
vation for solitude (M=25.43, SD =5.26;
maximum possible score of 32), and moderately
low loneliness (M=17.0, SD =4.45; maxi-
mum possible score of 32). Thus, on the whole,
participants spent signicant amounts of time
alone in their daily lives, despite their work,
social, or family commitments, exhibited a high
preference for solitude, were motivated for soli-
tude for self-determined reasons, and were not
overly lonely.
Procedure
Solitude Date
After completing the online survey, partici-
pants agreed to spend one full day alone on a
solitude date (Manalastas, 2011) at a time of their
choosing. Rather than place constraints or rules
on where or how participants spent their time, no
instructions were provided for how to spend their
day in solitude and each person made their own
choices about internet use, phone use, and being
inside or outside of their homes. The rationale for
the lack of instruction stemmed from the research
goal of examining how solitude was enacted in
daily life and what skills were used to enjoy such
time alone, without intervention or inuence from
the author of the study. By permitting participants
to exercise their own agency on their solitude
date, unencumbered by a set of external expecta-
tions or rules, the author aimed to more accurately
examine solitude in vivo.
Participants were provided with a worksheet to
complete at the end of their solitude date, which
was an adapted form of the Day Reconstruction
Method (Kahneman et al., 2004). The worksheet
invited participants to jot notes about their day in
solitude, including the various activities they
engaged in, their moods throughout the day,
and things they thought about during their time
alone. Use of the worksheet was optional, and
participants were informed that the worksheets
purpose was only as a memory aid for the subse-
quent interview, and the worksheet would not be
collected nor analyzed.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
124 THOMAS
Interview
Within 48 hr of completing their solitude date
each participant engaged in a semi-structured,
private interview by phone with the author, which
lasted approximately 1 hr and was audio re-
corded. The author began each interview by
describing the purpose of the study, which was
to understand how people experience solitude,
including both positive and negative experiences
of being alone, with a particular interest in hearing
from individuals like themselves who frequently
sought out solitude. In an effort to be transparent
and build rapport, the author briey shared that
she herself had a long history of appreciating
solitude, although sometimes found it challeng-
ing to acquire alone time given the demands of
parenthood while working full-time, and there-
fore was also interested in how participants bal-
anced their current commitments and
relationships with their desire for solitude. The
author thus positioned herself as both academi-
cally and personally invested in the research
question, and in a similar stage of life as many
of the participants, while also approaching the
interview with an attitude of openness and
curiosity.
The interview questions were constructed using
a two-pronged approach: the rst half of the
interview elicited from the participants the activi-
ties, thoughts, and feelings experienced during
their recent solitude date; the second half asked
them to share and reect on their experiences of
solitude more generally, past and present. This
two-pronged approach allowed the interviewer to
examine the phenomenology of the solitude expe-
rience through a microlens, by probing for
specics of how participants navigated a recent
episode of time alone, as well as through a
macrolens, by listening to participants share,
reect on, and evaluate their lived experiences of
solitude in a diverse range of contexts over time.
The semistructured format of the interview and the
predominance of open-ended questions allowed
for conversational dialogue and ample opportu-
nity for follow-up questions, clarications, and
requests for illustrative examples.
Given that the meanings of solitude may vary
from person to person, each participant was
asked to dene solitude and how they would
differentiate it from loneliness. Participants
were unanimous in afrming that there were
distinct differences between these two states.
They described solitude as primarily an enjoy-
able state of being alone, typically engaged in by
choice and accompanied by positive connota-
tions, often using metaphors of value and full-
ness, such as a gift,”“enriching,and feeling
nourished.In contrast, they described loneli-
ness in negative terms, such as feeling empty,
hollow,and disconnected.For instance,
Mika
1
differentiated these states as such:
Loneliness is a longing. My heart wants something
thats not there. And its a pull, I feel somewhat melan-
choly or like Im missing something. Solitude isits
like (sigh), its a peace, ImIm whole and complete
and my heart feels content being with me, like I dont
feel the need or the pull for something outside of myself.
Theres a sense of wholeness and fullness.
Thus, by rst eliciting from participants their
own denition of the phenomenon under investi-
gation, the author could more condently ensure
that subsequent questions were built on a more
mutual understanding of what the term soli-
tude meant.
To avoid inuencing the participants to view
their solitude experiences through the lens of
skills, the term itself was never used in the inter-
view. Rather, open-ended questions about their
lived experiences of solitude, both positive and
negative, were used to see if concepts related to
the theoretical proposal of solitude skills arose
organically in the interview. For instance, parti-
cipants were asked what motivated them to seek
solitude, how they beneted from solitude, and
what they enjoyed about being alone. Given prior
literature pointing out potential pitfalls of soli-
tude, in particular low mood and negative affect
(Goossens, 2014;Lay et al., 2019), participants
were also asked about any difculties they faced
around solitude, in particular whether they expe-
rienced negative emotions such as loneliness or
boredom when alone and if so, how they re-
sponded. Finally, they were asked whether and
how their enjoyment of solitude affected other
people, and what messages they had received
about solitude from their family, friends, and
cultures in which they had lived.
Analysis
Interviews were transcribed verbatim for anal-
ysis and pseudonyms were assigned to protect
participantsidentities. The interviews were
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
1
All participant names are pseudonyms.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 125
analyzed using a phenomenologically informed
thematic analysis, which focuses on the subjec-
tive experiences and perspectives of a small group
of participants on a given topic, analyzes narra-
tives thematically rather than idiographically to
identify meaningful patterns in the dataset, and
utilizes an organic, iterative coding process
(Braun & Clarke, 2006;Braun et al., 2019).
Thematic analysis (TA) is a exible approach
that allows for active engagement with the data to
identify shared meanings across the dataset and
interpret them in a coherent and compelling way,
while not being constrained by a particular epis-
temological framework or methodological
tradition.
The exibility of TA thus accommodates the
phenomenological aspect of this study, that is
closely examining individualslived experiences
of a specic and often underexplored phenomena,
as well as supports the aim of generating theory
in this case, articulating a set of solitude skills
both theoretically consistent with previous litera-
ture on the benets of solitude and potentially
transferable to populations beyond the study
sample. Finally, TAs active and reexive
approach (Braun et al., 2019) to coding qualita-
tive interviews reects a post-positivist paradigm
(Guba & Lincoln, 1994), and is especially useful
when examining exploratory research questions
that do not involve hypothesis testing. A phenom-
enologically informed approach to thematic anal-
ysis was therefore determined to be the
appropriate analytic method for the present study.
Prior to coding, familiarization of the data took
place through a process of immersive reading of
each manuscript. Coding categories were not
developed a priori, given the lack of empirical
literature on solitude skills; rather, codes were
generated during analysis in an iterative, inter-
pretive process drawn from intensive, multiple
readings of each transcript.
Inductive coding occurred during subsequent
readings of the transcripts, taking rst the form of
captions or phrases which captured the essence of
the content. These initial codes were provisional
and semantic in nature; care was taken to stay
close to the meaning of the participantsoriginal
words. Next, semantic coding transitioned to
latent coding, a more interpretive process that
considered the semantic codes as the surface
features of underlying conceptual patterns across
the dataset (Braun et al., 2019), and delity to the
meaning of the participantswords was preserved
through the linkage of transcript line numbers to
each code.
Using thematic mapping (Braun & Clarke,
2006), latent codes were then clustered theoreti-
cally into candidate themes. An iterative process
was used to promote candidate themes to a nal
set of themes; as new candidate themes were
constructed from the latent codes, transcripts
were reviewed again to determine whether the
candidate themes indeed reected the meaning of
the narratives. During this process, candidate
themes were revised, claried, or dropped.
Anal set of themes was determined based on
their prevalence within the dataset, theoretical
importance, and relevance to the research ques-
tion (i.e., solitude skills). Further review of the
analysis revealed that the nal set of eight themes,
or skills, when viewed as a whole, could be seen
as comprising three larger categories, each repre-
senting a central organizing concept, that both
integrated and expanded on the extant literature.
Results
This study investigated the exploratory ques-
tion: What are the skills required to experience
solitude positively and constructively? Analysis
yielded evidence for eight themes (i.e., skills)
clustered within three central organizing con-
cepts: Connect with Self, Protect Time, and
Find a Balance (see Table 1). Each skill is
described, supported by thick description from
participant narratives.
The Skills of Connecting with the Self
The rst three skills showcased participants
ability to connect with themselves in three as-
pects: behaviorally, by enjoying meaningful
solitary activities (Skill 1); emotionally, by
experiencing and regulating various emotional
states (Skill 2); and cognitively, by engaging in
introspection and self-insight (Skill 3).
Skill 1: Enjoyment of Solitary Activities
These adults rarely experienced boredom dur-
ing solitude, and they explained its absence as a
function of deriving satisfaction and meaning
from solitary pursuits. This enabled them to
genuinely enjoy time not only by themselves,
but also with themselves. Participants explicitly
described their solitude as enjoying my own
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
126 THOMAS
company(Doug), happy with my own com-
pany(Catherine), and enjoying being by
myself(Laurie). A conversation with Diana
illustrated this skill:
I am never, ever bored in solitu de ::: because in solitude
I can follow whatever it is that is interesting. I nd my
thoughts interesting and if my thoughts arent interesting,
well theres always a book, you know ::: and if Im, if
Im not in a reading mood, well I can take a bath, or I
could, you know, dance, or I could water the plants. I
mean ::: theres nothing about being alonethats boring.
Frequently the activities mentioned by parti-
cipants were everyday pleasures such as reading,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Table 1
Eight Solitude Skills Within Three Central Organizing Concepts
Central organizing
concept Theme Definition of theme Example of theme
Connect with self 1. Enjoyment of solitary
activities
Ability to nd pleasure and
meaning in solitary activities;
enjoying ones own company
I feel nourished by myself
meditating or being creative or
going on a long bike ride by
myself. And then if Im doing
those things, its like I need no
one else, I need nothing else, I
can just be happy and content
with what Im doing.
Connect with self 2. Emotion regulation Ability to identify feelings,
tolerate distress, and regulate
emotions
As my body relaxed, my mind
quieted, I had a lot of tears, I had
an emotional release that felt
really cleansing, even though it
was uncomfortable to feel
sadness.
Connect with self 3. Introspection Ability to engage in self-reection
and insight
I process what Im needing or
going through at the time in my
life, its important for me to just
to kind of step back and reect
on everything.
Protect time 4. Make time for solitude Ability to carve out time for
solitude, and if needed,
negotiate for it
During the week when Im
working, and I go to the gym or
Ill my day with stuff, I still
have several hours alone to
recharge or to prepare myself,
and thats important to me.
Protect time 5. Be mindful of how time
in solitude is spent
Ability to use solitude
intentionally or constructively,
free of distractions or
obligations to others
[I tell myself] dontgoonthe
computer because youre going
to get distracted ::: I just try to
be very disciplined about it so
that I dont feel interrupted or
empty.
Protect time 6. Validate need for
solitude
Ability to validate ones need for
solitude despite judgments from
self, others, or society
There is kind of this feeling you
know, in our world, in our
society, that if you like to spend
a lot of time alone, theres
something kind of wrong with
you. And I dont agree with that
at all.
Find a balance 7. Heed internal signals to
enter solitude
Ability to notice and act on
internal sensations that signal a
need to enter solitude
I just feel easily irritated and I just
want everyone to leave me
alone, and so then Im like, oh
okay, I need to put myself in
time-out.
Find a balance 8. Know when to exit
solitude
Ability to recognize when solitude
has served its purpose and act
on the desire to re-enter social
life
Im feeling restless or lonely :::
Ive gotten what I need from
being alone, and okay, now
who can I go meet?
SOLITUDE SKILLS 127
walking in nature, engaging in a hobby, or fol-
lowing their own thoughts in silence. The content
of the solitary activity did not appear to be
signicant; what mattered was that the activity
was intrinsically pleasurable and meaningful
when experienced alone. Philip made this point
clear when he said:
There isnt enough time in any day to do the things I
want to do ::: there are a million things I want to read,
theres a million things I want to watch, and I have my
[screenwriting] project to work on, and Ive got all
theseso theres always stuff. And again, Im not
talking about, like, distractions, theyre really just like
rewarding things that I want to focus my attention on.
The relationship between this skill and its
central organizing concept of Connect with Self
is illustrated by Doug, who attributed previous
experiences of boredom in solitude to lack of
self-contact, like Im disconnected from myself.
He contrasted that with his current experience of
solitude for the present study: Im just kind of
content to be with myself ::: I wasnt longing or
pining or missing anything, relationally. And I
was really connected and present with my own
experience.
Skill 2: Emotion Regulation
This skill described a willingness to experience
a range of emotions during solitude, tolerate
distress caused by difcult emotions, and process
those emotions until a shift or insight occurred.
Participants identied both positive and negative
emotional states during their solitude experi-
ences, but this skill centered on their ability to
approach rather than avoid the difcult emotions
that sometimes emerged, including grief, anger,
loneliness, despair, confusion, and anxiety.
Some participants, like Shelby, were startled
by initial negative emotions that surfaced but
were able to accept them:
As my body relaxed, my mind quieted. I had a lot of
tears, I had an emotional release that felt really cleans-
ing, even though it was uncomfortable to feel sadness
::: I could feel in my chest this feeling like my heart is
breaking ::: I also had a process of ::: just kind of
being really welcoming towards whatever was arriving.
Others, like Laurie, intentionally sought soli-
tude in order to process emotions they were
already feeling:
[If Im feeling] a little bit insecure or a little bit fragile, I
denitely want to be alone ::: I guess [solitude] lets me
be with [my emotions] in a way where I dont have to
explain themit feels like if Im alone Im allowed
more time to just kind of let it, let it be ::: and then it
usually just, I kind of move on, move on from it.
Participants echoed each other in sharing that
these difcult emotions did not scare them nor
discourage them from solitude. Rather than
abandon solitude, they chose to stay alone. As
Diana described, IfeelthatIspendmostofmy
regular life without having enough time to be
really sad and that almost every time Im alone,
the initial thing that happens is that I go through
intense sadness and then, and then I proceed.
Participants utilized various strategies to navi-
gate these emotions, from physical movement to
self-reection, and shared stories of successfully
moving through to a more emotionally balanced
state. In Dianascase,I thought through the
sadness as far as it needed to go and then it just
evaporated.
In particular, participants described how they
worked through any loneliness that arose so that
they could return to a more positive experience of
being alone. For example, Audrey shared:
If Im lonely and I need to get back to the solitude I
might go out to the ocean for a long walk because theres
something about hearing the waves, seeing the waves,
seeing those little birds scurry across the sand, just
looking at the sand trying to nd sand dollars, especially
watching the waves, I nd it very reassuring and then I
just feel so much better and I can hear myself.
Skill 3: Introspection
These adults talked about solitude as an oppor-
tunity to take a step back from their everyday lives
to reect and gain insight about themselves. This
was often framed as a benet of solitude; for
example, time alone provided the chance for Nora
to get a new perspective on lifeand served as
time for reection for Laurie when I feel like
theres something I kind of want to think about or
just kind of sit with for a while.
Introspection included both a curiosity about
their inner lives as well as a willingness to exam-
ine their own behaviors, feelings, and thoughts.
For Scott, this skill was central to his experience
of solitude: To me its useful to be silent because
then you can see yourself clearly, thats the whole
point of it.
For Philip, introspection helped him under-
stand his authentic self. Although he acknowl-
edged that humans are social creatures and in fact
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
128 THOMAS
identied himself as a very sociable person, he
seemed to view his truest self as something
accessible in its most authentic form when
engaged in solitary introspection.
You cant know who you are if you dont spend time
completely alone with yourself ::: I think you really get
in touch with whats going on inside of you, how youre
feeling, who you are, what your opinions are, what your
beliefs are, knowing how you react to things, yeah, your
internal values, your internalknowing just everything
about yourself that has nothing to do with other peoples
reactions or who they are. But if were all just plugged
into each other, theres so much other noise thats
intruding on listening to yourself, your own instincts,
your own feelings. Its like you lose that if youre not
spending time alone.
Altogether, the rst three skills belong in the
central organizing concept of Connect with Self
a connection that appeared important for these
participantswell-being. However, this state of
self-connection depended on them being able to
protect their time in solitude.
The Skills of Protecting Ones Time
The second central organizing concept, Protect
Time, included the themes of making time for
solitude (Skill 4), using that time mindfully (Skill
5), and validating their need for time alone
(Skill 6).
Skill 4: Make Time for Solitude
Participants expressed the need to be disci-
plined and strategic about securing time in soli-
tude. For example, Audrey shared:
After college, in my twenties, I started drawing. I would
draw every night for four hours and so I think I would
rearrange friends around that time. Because Id say, well
I have to draw, I need to draw for four hours, I have to
draw, I spend every night drawing for four hours, and so
I cant see you tonight (laughs) ::: Its just this thing
that I did and it was like feeding myself. I had toI had to
do this thing for four hours.
Audrey frequently described solitude as nour-
ishmentand as an experience akin to being well
fed.For her, carving out 4 hr every night to draw
in solitude was as necessary as eating dinner, and
she has continued to practice this skill in her 30s
and 40s, declining social invitations when they
interfere with a need to be alone.
Matt made this skill apparent by discussing his
difculties with it. For him, being a husband and a
father meant he needed to negotiate for solitude if
he wanted to make time for it:
I think its one of those things that you go through
negotiations, or you go through like an evolution in
terms of how you deal with it in a relationship. I think
[my wife] is more understanding of that need [for
solitude] now than ever, like you know, the longer
weve been together the more shes realized, you
know, this is just who he is, this is just something he
needs, and she understands that its good for me and so
therefore its good for us. Right, like Ill be better to be
around, well both be happier if I get some timebut its
always a negotiation because she wants the opposite.
However, the skill of making time for solitude
was especially difcult when close relationships
were unsupportive. For example, Maeve histori-
cally found herself in relationships with people
who did not get itand who in fact took it
personallywhen she spent time alone. I ended
so many relationships because I couldntgure
out how to negotiate how badly I needed time for
myself,she recalled. In order to secure her
solitude in subsequent relationships, Maeve
had to do three things: explainher need for
solitude, ask for it,and set the boundaries.
Skill 5: Be Mindful of how Time in Solitude
Is Spent
This skill described the measures participants
took to make their time in solitude well spent,
rather than wastingit, to quote Philip. For them,
constructive solitude was dependent on being
mindful that their time alone was volitional,
uninterrupted, and intentionalwhich included
being free of distractions, especially the internet.
Mika illustrated this when she described her
solitude date for the present study:
Facebookit can be a real waste of time, and I just
decided that that was not going to be a part of the day. So
not having that as an outlet really brought me back to
what do I want to do right now, like whats, whatsan
opportunity, whats an available opportunity for me
right now at this moment, instead of just sort of being
swept along by my minds preoccupation with the
Internet or with the computer ::: There was more
mindfulness around pretty much everything that I did.
I was much more in touch with myself, I was much more
aware of my mind racing and then me, you know,
wanting to come back to being present.
In most cases, participants shared that they
worked hard to prevent other people from inter-
rupting them, whether it be retreating to a
secluded place or asking people not to call
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 129
them during a certain time of day. However, the
more difcult aspect of securing uninterrupted
solitude was monitoring their own propensity to
interrupt or distract themselves. Audrey attrib-
uted her lack of interruptions while in solitude to
being mindful about what she wanted in the
moment:
I dont get interrupted very often and (pause) and I think
that Im, I try to pay really close attention to how Im
feeling because if Im feeling in a time of solitude it feels
like, like theres this cushion around me and, and so I
can say, okay I have this cushion around me, now is a
good time to, to just end up writing or draw or play
music. And then I, then if I want to call somebody, I
really have to say to myself, okay if you call this person,
youre going to break out of the solitude and you know,
you just have to be aware that it might take a little while
to get back into it.
Finally, the skill of being mindful involved
participants entering the space of solitude with
clear intentions about what they wanted to expe-
rience or accomplish, andnot allowing themselves
to get distracted by meaningless activities. Maeve
described how she distinguished whether or not
her solitude was worthy:
Sometimes you know, I just want to lie on the couch and
watch a movie, but I do know like if I go too far with that
like, theres a certain way in which distractions take me
into that disconnected place in myself ::: I really feel
like part of the reason that I need solitude and part of the
way that solitude can be so helpful for me is that
because, its because it allows me to go more deeply
into things that are truly meaningful to me on like a spirit
and soul level, and I dont, I dont just like to throw that
time away.
Skill 6: Validate Need for Solitude
This skill described the ability to validate ones
need for solitude despite judgments from self,
others, or society. Although these participants
emphasized throughout their interviews how nec-
essary time in solitude was for them, they also
acknowledged that it went against social norms.
They were aware that the culture of the United
States in which they all currently lived placed a
high value on socializing and viewed people who
prefer to be alone in decit terms; weird,”“anti-
social,and selshwere the most frequent
terms used by participants.
Responses to this cultural bias against solitude
fell on a spectrum. On one end, they diagnosed the
culture as misguided or biased. This helped them
afrm their need for solitude as normal and
natural. For example, Matt shared that he not
only validates his own need for solitude but tries
to be an example for his daughter:
I think American culture is highly skewed towards
extroverts, and thats kind of seen as the norm, and
the ideal. And so I think solitude is always kind of
looked at with suspicion ::: I think its good for [my
daughter] to see solitude as something thats not nega-
tive, as something that can certainly be positive, and its
positive for me ::: I think its good that shes able to see
different ways of being, that theres not one way thats
right, or one way thats ideal, that different people have
different needs. And it doesnt mean that youre antiso-
cial or weird or dont have good people skills or
whatever.
On the other end of the spectrum, they some-
times struggled with these internalized cultural
messages of solitude as signaling deciency.
During these times, they were unable to consis-
tently validate their desire to be alone when they
needed it. Two participants were mothers of
school-aged children, and they often felt guilty
or selsh for seeking solitude because of per-
ceived gendered expectations that they be emo-
tionally available to others at all times. In addition
to self-judgments, many participants worried
about how other people would react to their
need for solitude. For example, Doug said for
much of his life, The fear was that they would be
upset with me, like they would take it personally
somehow ::: the hesitancy to assert that [need
for solitude] for fear of some kind of retaliation or
rejection.
Catherine shared how she learned over time to
validate her desire for solitude:
Ten years ago when I was less comfortable with my own
company than I am now ::: going to a party, say, that I
didnt necessarily want to go to, but I went anyway, and
then waking up and being tired the next morning and
mad at myself ::: choosing to be with people that I
didnt really want to be with. And then letting myself
down. Yeah, and then I nally thought, wait a minute,
this is my life. Im not going to break these promises to
myself anymore. If I want to be at home by myself, Im
going to be at home by myself. And just tell people, no
thank you. Yeah. And the world did not end.
Catherines words suggest that an essential
component of this skill is validating to oneself
not necessarily to othersthe need for solitude
and learning how to ignore or reframe any related
internalized judgment or guilt. Aaron echoed
this sentiment when he said, The most impor-
tant thing is giving myself permission [to
be alone].
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
130 THOMAS
The Skills of Finding a Balance
The nal two skills involved creating a healthy
balance of time in solitude versus time in the
social world by noticing internal cues that sig-
naled a need to enter (skill 7) or exit (skill 8)
solitude.
Skill 7: Heed Internal Signals to Enter Solitude
This skill was two-fold; it entailed rst the
ability to notice internal cues, whether they be
physical sensations or emotions, and interpret
them as signals to enter solitude, and second,
to act on those signals by moving into solitude.
The internal cues were indicators of overstimula-
tion, and were most frequently described as feel-
ing overwhelmed, exhausted, or easily irritated
with others. Doug summarized it as:
Its like this sense of under my collar is too warm. And
that I cant hear myself think, you know ::: the emo-
tions, and the irritation ::: this sense of exasperation, I
think overwhelmed is in there somewhere, like Ive just
got to go be alone ::: I want to take care of myself and
go back to sort of quietly reconnect or see whats
happening ::: I kind of lost myself here somewhere
so lets go be by myself again to connect with myself.
It was clear from these participantsstories that
interpreting these signals in a timely and accurate
way was a skill they learned over time. Maeve
described it as such:
I get excessively irritable ::: literally my system just
shuts down ::: I will keep going as long as I can but it
feels like it comes at a really high cost, you know :::
Ive learned to recognize the early warning signs of
[Im] going to have a meltdown, right. I start to notice
that I feel overwhelmed, I start to complain about my
surroundings, I start to tell myself stories about how bad
everything is. Im like, I know whats happened here,
Ive just been getting too tired, Im getting too far away
from myself and I just need some time, I need to get out,
I need to go for a walk in nature by myself, whatever. So,
I think its you know either that Im feeling overstimu-
lated or that Im feeling like for whatever reason I
havent really been attending to, you know, myself
on a spiritual or creative level with those things.
Skill 8: Know When to Exit Solitude
Just as participants recognized internal signals
that prompted them to seek solitude, they likewise
identied when they needed to exit solitude.
These signals fell into two primary categories:
boredom and loneliness. Philip recognized when
his solitude has served its purposes when, Im
feeling restless or lonely ::: Ive gotten what I
need from being alone, and okay, now who can I
go meet?Participants emphasized that it was
essential to discern whether these feelings sig-
naled a real need to rejoin the social world, or
whether they instead represented a need to go
deeper into their solitude. Maeve elaborated on
the necessity of discernment when loneliness or
boredom arose:
Ill get restless when Im alone and sometimes Ill be
like, is this restlessness because Im interested in going
deeper in my own process with myself? Because like if I,
if I get restless and I havent felt like I really settled with
myself then I think that its not really the sign that I have
to go out and be with people. But you know, if Ive been
alone a while and I feel like Ive come to a subtle place
that sort ofIrst just wait to see what happens and
then you know, what feels like a really natural urge just
to get out and connect more comes to me.
Participants reported that they were glad to
socially reconnect when they felt they had suf-
cient time in solitude, and in fact remarked that
spending time alone enhanced those relation-
ships. Audrey noted, I cant be with people
effectively unless Ive spent time in solitude,
and Laurie said, my solitude helps my interac-
tions.She elaborated by saying:
[Solitude is] how I kind of recharge, and I think if Im
feeling, if Im feeling overwhelmed or if I feel I dont
have enough time for myself, then I will be cranky and I
will be sort of brusque ::: I just dont want to really be
around people. Whereas if Ive had some solitude, yeah,
I think it does [help]. Because then when Im around
people Im glad to be around people.
The balance of solitude and sociability that
resulted from the use of these nal two skills
appeared to foster connection not only with
themselves, but also with others.
Discussion
The set of solitude skills identied in the
present study supported an experience of positive
solitude for these participants and contributes to
an understanding of how individuals can success-
fully navigate the domain of solitude and reap the
benets. Furthermore, this study integrates prior
ndings from disparate developmental and clini-
cal traditions and extends that research in
new ways.
Behaviorally, it appears that individuals who
have the capacity to be alone know how to engage
in intrinsically enjoyable activities. Boredom
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 131
signals a failure to engage ones attention on
meaningful activities (Westgate, 2020), which
explains the notable absence of boredom experi-
enced by these participants during their solitude;
they were expert in their ability to nd meaning
and pleasure in solitary activities. Burger (1995)
remarked on a similar absence of boredom in the
individuals he surveyed who scored high in a
preference for solitude. The lack of boredom may
be partially explained by these participantsten-
dency to become immersed in whatever they were
doingwhether it be a creative project, an intro-
spective process, or an appreciation for the beauty
of their surroundings. This is consistent with the
proposal that, Solitude provides a situation that
is suited to deep absorption(Larson, 1990,
p. 165). The identication of this rst skill is
also consistent with research that has linked the
frequency and enjoyment of solitude experiences
with a high solitropic orientation, which denotes a
high desire to engage in solitary activities, rather
than with a low sociotropic orientation, which
denotes a low desire to afliate with others (Leary
et al., 2003). Furthermore, given that the partici-
pants appeared intrinsically motivated to engage
in their chosen activities, their solitude was self-
determined, increasing the likelihood of a posi-
tive experience of solitude (Deci & Ryan, 2008;
Thomas & Azmitia, 2019).
Emotionally, these participants were skillful in
traversing the internal landscape of their feelings.
The strategies they used most closely align with
integrative emotion regulation (IER; Roth et al.,
2019) rather than the emotion regulation model
proposed by Gross (2015), in which cognitive and
behavioral strategies such as reappraisal and
selection are utilized to inuence emotional
states. In contrast, the IER model views emotions
as important sources of information about ones
inner life, and these emotions are brought into full
awareness in a nonreactive, receptive manner
akin to mindfulness and acceptance-based thera-
pies (Baer, 2003;Blackledge & Hayes, 2001;
Keng et al., 2011). The participants exhibited
this integrative approach to emotion regulation,
showing an openness to a broad range of emo-
tions and a willingness to explore what those
emotions signied about their current goals,
needs, or relationships.
Furthermore, these participants exhibited a
capacity for emotion differentiation (Kashdan
et al., 2015), dened as an ability to put into
words the various complex emotions one is
feeling. Before one can successfully regulate
an emotion, it helps to know exactly what that
emotion is. Also known as emotional granular-
ity, this ability represents an adaptive response to
stress. Furthermore, they demonstrated distress
tolerance when the emotions they felt were
uncomfortable or negatively valenced (Simons
& Gaher, 2005). Rather than eeing solitude
when these unpleasant emotions arose, they
were able to tolerate that discomfort and explore
those emotions, after which they described feel-
ing an emotional release or renewal. These nd-
ings support the speculation that negative moods
that surface during solitude may be masking
important emotional and cognitive processes
underneath and is consistent with research over
multiple adult and adolescent samples demon-
strating that moods typically lower during soli-
tude but then rebound to higher than normal
levels upon exiting solitude (Larson, 1990).
Taken together, this set of emotional capacities
served to increase participantsconnection with
themselves.
It must be noted that these participants were not
limited to regulating difcult emotions; they also
reported feeling a range of positive emotions
when alone, including gratitude, joy, content-
ment, peace, and fulllment. Their frequent re-
ferences to positive emotionality during solitude
are indicative of positive emotion regulation
(Silton et al., 2020), a process known to enhance
well-being and reduce depression. Long et al.
(2003) found that the ability to be emotionally
creativeto adapt and navigate challenging emo-
tional situationscorrelated signicantly with
the frequency of positive solitude experiences.
Cognitively, participants demonstrated the
skill of introspection, which involves the ability
to reect on ones own behavior, attitudes, be-
liefs, or place in the world. This supports previous
work showing that people use solitude for exis-
tential contemplation (Long & Averill, 2003) and
to gain a reective perspective (Koch, 1994), in
addition to engaging in the self-reection neces-
sary for identity development (Goossens &
Marcoen, 1999). The introspection exhibited in
these narratives often took the form of a dialecti-
cal relationship of the self-interacting with the
self, as when Audrey said, I can hear myself.
This is reected in William James' (1890) concept
of the Iand the me,in which the Irepre-
sents the self as knower, containing the qualities
of self-awareness and agency, and the me
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
132 THOMAS
represents the self as an object of knowledge.
James argued that this internal dialogue between a
subjective self and the self as social object is
essential for maintaining a continuity of being and
ensuring congruence between our values and
behavior.
Rather than engage in ruminative self-
reection, a behavior known to be generally
maladaptive (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000), these par-
ticipantsintrospection took the form of self-
insight, a meta-cognitive process that supports
subjective well-being (Harrington & Loffredo,
2010;Stein & Grant, 2014) and plays a role in
self-regulation (Silvia & Phillips, 2014). Alto-
gether, the identication of this skill lends cre-
dence to Byrnes' (1983) speculation that to benet
from solitude, one needs to develop an ability for
introspection in addition to being able to sur-
mount initial feelings of anxiety that may surface
when alone.
To summarize thus far, the rst three skills of
enjoying solitary activities, emotion regulation,
and introspection can be viewed as the substance
of the solitude skillset. They represent abilities
that are exercised during time alone, thus creating
an experience of positive solitude, dened as rich
in meaning and enjoyment (Ost Mor et al., 2020).
Viewed as such, solitude may function less as a
happiness promoter in the hedonic sense (i.e.,
feeling good, experiencing pleasant emotions)
and more as a well-being promoter (i.e., gaining
insight, experiencing the full spectrum of emo-
tions) in the eudaimonic sense (Ryff, 1989).
The remaining ve skills can be viewed as the
structural support of positive solitude in two
ways: rst, in the practical sense of securing
time for solitude and being mindful to avoid
distractions during that time; second, in the psy-
chological sense of heeding internal signals to
enter and exit solitude when needed, and validat-
ing this need for solitude even in the face of
internal or external critics. The identication of
these skills extends existing literature and chal-
lenges core assumptions. For instance, knowing
when to enter and exit solitude helped each
participant create what Larson (1990) called their
optimal balanceof social and solitary engage-
ment (p. 158) and support the theoretical argu-
ment that a balance of solitude and relationship is
a necessary ingredient for well-being in adult-
hood (Littman-Ovadia, 2019).
However, the present ndings go further by
describing how this balance was achieved.
Participants were attuned to their inner signals,
such as exhaustion and irritability, that prompted
them to seek solitude and remedy the effects of
overstimulation. Likewise, they had to discern
when signals to exit solitude were trustworthy; for
example, whether loneliness and boredom were
invitations to go deeper into solitude or cues that
the company of others was indeed necessary.
Moreover, to create an optimal balance they
had to exercise the skill of making time for
solitude, and often had to negotiate with others
to obtain it. Because support for solitude was
often missing from their cultural upbringing,
participants developed these skills by trial and
error. In these ways, the skills of knowing when to
enter and exit solitude provide greater articulation
of the dynamic relationship of solitude and socia-
bility outlined in the extant literature, which has
demonstrated that when a person becomes over-
taxed by the social world, a retreat into solitude
can be rejuvenating and emotionally regulating,
with even brief periods of solitude having a
deactivating effect on high arousal states, both
positive (e.g., excitement) and negative (e.g.,
anger; Buchholz & Helbraun, 1999;Larson,
1990;Nguyen et al., 2018).
Yet, these narratives simultaneously challenge
the assumption that solitudes function is ulti-
mately in service to our more fundamental social
nature, essentially providing a temporary haven
to withdraw and self-regulate, after which one can
rejoin society better equipped to relate positively
with others (Larson, 1990;Nguyen et al., 2018).
Rather, several participants ipped this script,
asserting that solitude was their natural state
or their defaultand expressed confusion during
the interview when asked what motivated them to
be alone. For instance, Scott replied: Motiva-
tion? I would need motivation to be around
people. I would say being by myself is natural
::: And if youre looking for some reason, like I
dontI dont have a reason. Its just kind of the
way I like to be.His response made clear the
assumption embedded in the interview question:
namely, that one would need a motivation to be
alone, as though solitude-seeking needed to be
justied.
The centrality of solitude for these participants
is reminiscent of the recently proposed construct
of aloneliness, dened as a negative feeling state
that arises when a person perceives that they are
not spending enough time alone (Coplan et al.,
2019), as when Mika said that when she gets too
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 133
far away from solitude, I get lonely for myself,
like I feel lonely and miss the connection to
myself when I get really busy.Altogether, rather
than solitude serving a subsidiary role to social
interaction, these ndings instead suggest that for
some individuals time alone may be equal, or
preferable, to social relationships.
Solitude Skills and the Private Self
As a whole, the solitude skills demonstrated by
these participants, and the function of self-
connection they appear to serve, nd a home in
the construct of the private self, developed by
clinical psychologist Arnold Modell (1992,
1993), building on what Winnicott (1965)
described as the true self. The chief characteristics
of the private self are authenticity and an internal
coherency of being, drawing on William James'
(1890) articulation of the psychological necessity
to maintain a continuity of self. The private self is
supported by the capacity to generate meaning
from personal experience. While meanings are
also generated through social interaction and
participation in the public sphere, Modell
(1993) argued that occasional retreat from social
roles and societal obligations is necessary to make
contact with an affective core where private
thoughts, feelings, and meanings are processed
and recategorized. The private self is the source
of autonomy(Modell, 1992, p. 3), sustained by
the freedom to be oneself when alone and exercise
the agency to engage in vital personal interests
(p. 3) that are meaningful and nourishing.
These qualities of the private self are concep-
tually linked to the rst three solitude skills
identied in the present study; enjoyment of
solitary activities, emotion regulation, and intro-
spection all served to deepen participantscon-
nection with themselves, or in Modells terms, to
sustain the private self. This theoretical lens
provides context for someone like Nora, whose
very experience of self depended on time in
solitude: If I didnt have solitude, I wouldnt
have me.The protective skills of carving out
time to be alone, using that time mindfully, and
validating ones need for solitude all encourage
optimal conditions to make contact with the
private self. Finally, embedded in Modells con-
ception of the private self is a recognition of
the importance of social relationships, and he
aligns with Winnicott (1965) in arguing that
supportive attachment relationships are what
make the private self possible. The nal two skills
illustrate Modells emphasis on the importance of
abalance between alternate periods of related-
ness and disengagement(1992, p. 5).
Modell argued that even when one is forced
into solitude, individuals who successfully navi-
gate that state do so because they rely on internal
resources of the private self to sustain them-
selves from within ::: [and] nd sources of
nourishment at the core of their being(1993,
p. 126). The theoretical construct of the private
self was echoed in remarkably similar ways
throughout participant interviews, including me-
taphors of feeling nourished by time alone. It is
also consistent with Larsons(1990) review of his
decades of empirical research: The ability to be
alone without succumbing to panic or disturbed
means of coping confers a measure of indepen-
dence from social denitions of self. It holds open
the chance for self-renewal ::: to reect on their
experience and make choices congruent with
personal values and feelings(p. 177).
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions
The present ndings represent an important
advance in the literature on solitude by identify-
ing a preliminary set of skills that foster the state
of positive solitude. Until now, the concept of
solitude skills was noted as a theoretical possibil-
ity but never investigated empirically (Byrnes,
1983;Galanaki, 2005;Rook, 1988;Rubenstein &
Shaver, 1982).
Given the exploratory nature of the research
question, the open-ended, semi-structured inter-
view method undertaken in the present study,
combined with a phenomenologically oriented,
inductive approach to thematic analysis, proved
to be a fruitful methodology to map the hereto-
fore uncharted territory of solitude skills. This
method facilitated an advance from theoretical
speculation about solitude skills to empirical
identication of a preliminary set of skills uti-
lized by adults in daily life and opens the door for
quantitative and mixed-methods studies to ver-
ify or amend these skills in other populations and
test their efcacy in clinical and educational
settings.
Despite the advantages of qualitative methods,
the interview does not act as a awless conduit of a
participants inner world. Rather, the social interac-
tion embedded in the interviewerparticipant dyad
can introduce demand characteristics and power
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
134 THOMAS
differences, leaving the participant vulnerable to
misinterpretation (see Potter & Hepburn, 2005,
for a critique of this method). Moreover, the
conversational nature of the interview with its
reliance on oral expression is potentially at odds
with the topic in question, inherently limiting
participantsability to accurately convey the
private experience of solitude in the context of
interpersonal dialogue. Future research may want
to utilize nonoral qualitative methods, such as
diary studies or written reections, to bypass
some of the constraints imposed by conversa-
tional interviews.
In addition, the eight skills identied here were
drawn from a select sample that is not represen-
tative of the population at large, and attempts to
generalize should be undertaken with the follow-
ing caveats in mind. First, the solitude skills
demonstrated by these adults may be develop-
mentally constrained. For example, children and
adolescents may not have the cognitive capacity
necessary for introspection, nor have the agency
necessary for carving out time to be alone. Sec-
ond, the adults in this study were employed and
well-educated; they had access to access to pri-
vate rooms and the autonomy to go places on their
own. In short, they had space and freedom. Many
people of all ages lack the ability to access space
and time for solitude, whether due to poverty,
displacement, housing situation, and untold other
reasons (Koch, 1994). Such developmental and
socio-economic factors need to be taken into
consideration when theorizing or applying from
this data set. Third, these individuals were in
solitude volitionally. There may be additional,
or different, skills required when solitude is
involuntarily, as in the case of the millions of
individuals worldwide who were isolated in their
homes due to the stay-at-home orders during the
COVID-19 pandemic. This study was conducted
prior to the onset of the pandemic, and it is
unknown how enforced social isolation may
impact the development or enactment of solitude
skills.
Despite these limitations, the present study lays
the groundwork for a more comprehensive inves-
tigation of the role these skills may play in psy-
chological well-being. As Levitt (2021) has noted,
qualitative research lends itself to generalization
to the phenomenon, rather than to the population,
and thus future research can investigate how
positive solitude, and the skills that support it,
is enacted across various contexts. Conducting
additional qualitative and mixed-methods studies
with more diverse samples can illuminate whether
and how these skills are manifested in other
populations; a nonexhaustive list of additional
variables includes age, psychological adjustment,
prior trauma, and ones culturalcontext. Empirical
questions about the efcacy of solitude skills
could be tested using quasi-experimental or inter-
vention methods. In addition, solitude skills train-
ings could be adapted to therapeutic settings;
clinicians have already speculated that the capac-
ity for solitude could serve as a clinical interven-
tion for patients who suffer from loneliness,
depression, and poor relationships (Burke,
1992;Knafo, 2012;Young, 1982).
Finally, although individual differences such
as extraversion likely play a role in how much
solitude one prefers (Burger, 1995), the actual
skills to utilize solitude constructively may not
depend on individual differences. Research has
shown that many expert behaviors typically
attributed to innate personality characteristics
are actually activities practiced by individuals,
activities which are demonstrated and reinforced
by the larger culture. These are called repertoires
of practice (Gutiérrez & Rogoff, 2003), and
denote experiential skills that are learned and
practiced over time. Viewed as a repertoire of
practice, engaging in solitude is not the exclusive
province of individuals who naturallyknow
how to be alone. Rather, positive solitude be-
comes potentially accessible for the individual
who learns to practice and adapt various solitude
skills to meet particular needs or achieve cer-
tain ends.
Conclusion
Fostering the private self in solitude is critical
for supporting mental health and well-being
(Modell, 1993), and the ndings from this study
suggest that certain skills assist individuals in
successfully navigating solitude and reaping the
benets. The capacity to be content when alone is
a developmental achievement; indeed Winnicott,
in his landmark paper on the subject, called it one
of the most important signs of maturity in emo-
tional development(Winnicott, 1958, p. 416).
The solitude skills identied in the present
study may represent some of the psychological
resources necessary to sustain an authentic, pri-
vate self, with the added implication that they are
valuable not only for those who volitionally enter
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 135
solitude, but also for those who nd themselves in
unwanted isolation. Applying these skills could
position individuals to leverage their time alone
for the purposes of self-regulation and self-
reection, increasing the likelihood of experienc-
ing positive solitude. Whether this preliminary set
of solitude skills can be found in populations
beyond the study sample, whether these skills
can be learned, and whether the practice of such
skills can enhance psychological functioning are
all empirical questions worth investigating.
Doing so would further our understanding of
the role positive solitude has in adjustment and
well-being.
References
Alden, L. E., & Auyeung, K. W. (2014). Social
anxiety disorder and emotional solitude. In R.
Coplan & J. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook of
solitude: Psychological perspectives on social iso-
lation, social withdrawal, and being alone
(pp. 391408). Wiley Blackwell.
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as clinical
intervention: A conceptual and empirical review.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,10(2),
125143. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpg015
Blackledge, J. T., & Hayes, S. C. (2001). Emotion
regulation in acceptance and commitment therapy.
Journal of Clinical Psychology,57(2), 243255.
https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(200102)57:2<243::
AID-JCLP9>3.0.CO;2-X
Bowker, J. C., Nelson, L. J., Markovic, A., & Luster,
S. (2014). Social withdrawal during adolescence
and emerging adulthood. In R. Coplan & J. Bowker
(Eds.), The handbook of solitude: Psychological
perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal,
and being alone (pp. 167183). Wiley.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-Child attach-
ment and healthy human development. Basic Books.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic
analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology,3(2), 77101. https://doi.org/10.1191/
1478088706qp063oa
Braun, V., Clarke, V., Hayeld, N., & Terry, G. (2019).
Thematic analysis. In P. Liamputtong (Ed.), Hand-
book of research methods in health social sciences
(pp. 843860). Springer Nature Singapore. https://
doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5251-4_103
Buchholz, E., & Helbraun, E. (1999). A psychobio-
logical developmental model for an alonetime
need in infancy. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic.
63(2), 143158.
Burger, J. (1995). Individual differences in preference for
solitude. Journal of Research in Personality,29(1),
85108. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1995.1005
Burke, N. (1992). College psychotherapy and the
development of a capacity for solitude. Journal
of College Student Psychotherapy,6(2), 5986.
https://doi.org/10.1300/J035v06n02_08
Burnell, K., George, M. J., & Underwood, M. K.
(2021). New media and solitude: Implications for
peer relations. In R. J. Coplan, J. C. Bowker, & L. J.
Nelson (Eds.), The handbook of solitude: Psycholog-
ical perspectives on social isolation, social with-
drawal, and being alone (2nd ed., pp. 254267).
Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119576457.ch18
Byrnes, D. A. (1983). Life skills in solitude and silence
in the school. Education,104(1), 9699.
Coplan, R. J., & Armer, M. (2007). A multitudeof
solitude: A closer look at social withdrawal and
nonsocial play in early childhood. Child Develop-
ment Perspective: Society for Research in Child
Development,1(1), 2632. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1750-8606.2007.00006.x
Coplan, R. J., Hipson, W. E., Archbell, K. A., Ooi,
L. L., Baldwin, D., & Bowker, J. C. (2019). Seeking
more solitude: Conceptualization, assessment, and
implications of aloneliness. Personality and Indi-
vidual Differences,148,1726. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.paid.2019.05.020
Dahlberg, L., Agahi, N., & Lennartsson, C. (2018).
Lonelier than ever? Loneliness of older people over
two decades. Archives of Gerontology and Geria-
trics,75,96103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger
.2017.11.004
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination
theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, devel-
opment, and health. Canadian Psychology,49(3),
182185. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012801
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., & Pachan, M. (2010).
A meta-analysis of after-school programs that seek
to promote personal and social skills in children and
adolescents. American Journal of Community Psy-
chology,45, 294309. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10464-010-9300-6
Erikson, J. (1997). The life cycle completed. Norton.
Franzoi, S. L., & Brewer, L. C. (1984). The experience
of self-awareness and its relation to level of self-
consciousness: An experiential sampling study.
Journal of Research in Personality,18(4), 522
540. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(84)90010-2
Galanaki, E. (2005). Solitude in the school: A ne-
glected facet of childrens development and educa-
tion. Childhood Education,81(3), 128132. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2005.10522255
Galanaki, E. (2013). Solitude in children and adoles-
cents: A review of the research literature. Psychol-
ogy and Education: An Interdisciplinary Journal.
Special Issue: Loneliness and Solitude in Children
and Adolescents,50(34), 7988.
Gazelle, H., & Druhen, M. J. (2009). Anxious solitude
and peer exclusion predict social helplessness,
upset affect, and vagal regulation in response to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
136 THOMAS
behavioral rejection by a friend. Developmental
Psychology,45(4), 10771096. https://doi.org/10
.1037/a0016165
Goossens, L. (2014). Afnity for aloneness in adoles-
cence and preference for solitude in childhood. In
R. Coplan & J. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook of
solitude: Psychological perspectives on social iso-
lation, social withdrawal, and being alone
(pp. 150166). Wiley.
Goossens, L., & Marcoen, A. (1999). Adolescent
loneliness, self-reection, and identity: From
individual differences to developmental processes.
In K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness
in childhood and adolescence (pp. 225243).
Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10
.1017/CBO9780511551888.011
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status
and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry,26(1), 1
26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Competing
paradigms in qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin
& Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of Qualitative
Research (pp. 105117). Sage Publications.
Gutiérrez, K. D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways
of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of prac-
tice. Educational Researcher,32(5), 1925. https://
doi.org/10.3102/0013189X032005019
Harrington, R., & Loffredo, D. A. (2010). Insight,
rumination, and self-reection as predictors of well-
being. The Journal of Psychology,145(1), 3957.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2010.528072
Hatano, G., & Oura, Y. (2003). Commentary: Recon-
ceptualizing school learning using insight from exper-
tise research. Educational Researcher,32(8), 2629.
https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X032008026
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness
matters: A theoretical and empirical review of con-
sequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral
Medicine,40(2), 218227. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12160-010-9210-8
Hays, R. D., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1987). A short-form
measure of loneliness. Journal of Personality
Assessment,51(1), 6981. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15327752jpa5101_6
Hollis, C., Livingstone, S., & Sonuga-Barke, E.
(2020). Editorial: The role of digital technology
in children and young peoples mental healthA
triple-edged sword? Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,61(8), 837
841. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13302
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology,
volume 1. In Frederick Burkhardt (Ed.). Harvard
University Press, 1981.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A.,
Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method
for characterizing daily life experience: The day
reconstruction method. Science,306(5702), 1776
1780. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572
Kashdan, T. B., Feldman Barrett, L., & McKnight,
P. E. (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation:
Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving
distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in
Psychological Science,24(1), 1016. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550708
Kaufman, J. C., Gentile, C. A., & Baer, J. (2005). Do
gifted student writers and creative writing experts
rate creativity the same way? Gifted Child
Quarterly,49(3), 260265. https://doi.org/10
.1177/001698620504900307
Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011).
Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A
review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology
Review,31(6), 10411056. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.cpr.2011.04.006
Kim, J.-H. (2021). Getting even lonelier? Psychologi-
cal well-being and problematic use of media in the
over-connected society. In R. J. Coplan, J. C.
Bowker, & L. J. Nelson (Eds.), The Handbook of
solitude: Psychological perspectives on social
isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone
(2nd ed., pp. 240253). Wiley. https://doi.org/10
.1002/9781119576457.ch17
Klinenberg, E. (2012). Going solo: The extraordinary
rise and surprising appeal of living alone. Penguin.
Knafo, D. (2012). Alone together: Solitude and the
creative encounter in art and psychoanalysis. Psy-
choanalytic Dialogues: The International Journal
of Relational Perspectives,22(1), 5471. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10481885.2012.646605
Koch, P. (1994). Solitude: A philosophical encounter.
Open Court Publishing.
Larson, R. (1990). The solitary side of life: An exami-
nation of the time people spend alone from childhood
to old age. Developmental Review,10(2), 155183.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(90)90008-R
Larson, R., & Lee, M. (1996). The capacity to be alone
as a stress buffer. The Journal of Social Psychology,
136(1), 516. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545
.1996.9923024
Lay, J. C., Pauly, T., Graf, P., Biesanz, J. C., &
Hoppmann, C. A. (2019). By myself and liking
it? Predictors of distinct types of solitude experi-
ences in daily life. Journal of Personality,87(3),
633647. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12421
Lay, J. C., Pauly, T., Graf, P., Mahmood, A., &
Hoppmann, C. A. (2020). Choosing solitude: Age
differences in situational and affective correlates of
solitude-seeking in midlife and older adulthood.
The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychologi-
cal Sciences and Social Sciences,75(3), 483493.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby044
Leary, M. R., Herbst, K. C., & McCrary, F. (2003).
Finding pleasure in solitary activities: Desire for
aloneness or disinterest in social contact? Person-
ality and Individual Differences,35(1), 5968.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00141-1
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 137
Leung, L. (2015). Using tablet in solitude for stress
reduction: An examination of desire for aloneness,
leisure boredom, tablet activities, and location of
use. Computers in Human Behavior,48, 382391.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.068
Levitt, H. M. (2021). Qualitative generalization, not to
the population but to the phenomenon: Reconcep-
tualizing variation in qualitative research. Qualita-
tive Psychology,8(1), 95110. https://doi.org/10
.1037/qup0000184
Littman-Ovadia, H. (2019). Doing-being and relation-
ship-solitude: A proposed model for a balanced life.
Journal of Happiness Studies,20, 19531971.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0018-8
Long, C., & Averill, J. (2003). Solitude: An explora-
tion of the benets of being alone. Journal for the
Theory of Social Behaviour,33(1), 2144. https://
doi.org/10.1111/1468-5914.00204
Long, C. R., Seburn, M., Averill, J. R., & More, T. A.
(2003). Solitude experiences: Varieties, settings,
and individual differences. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,29(5), 578583. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029005003
Luchetti, M., Lee, J. H., Aschwanden, D., Sesker, A.,
Strickhouser, J. E., Terracciano, A., & Sutin, A. R.
(2020). The trajectory of loneliness in response to
COVID-19. American Psychologist,75(7), 897
908. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000690
Maes, M., Van den Noortgate, W., & Goossens, L.
(2015). A reliability generalization study for a
multidimensional loneliness scale: The loneliness
and aloneness scale for children and adolescents.
European Journal of Psychological Assessment,
31(4), 294301. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-
5759/a000237
Manalastas, E. J. (2011). An exercise to teach the
psychological benets of solitude: The date with
the self. Philippine Journal of Psychology,44(1),
95106. http://www.ejournals.ph/form/cite.php?
id=3945
Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1993). Loneliness,
attitude towards aloneness, and solitude: Age dif-
ferences and developmental signicance during
adolescence. In S. Jackson & H. Rodriguez-Tome
(Eds.), Adolescence and its Social Worlds (pp. 197
227). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Modell, A. H. (1992). The private self and private space.
In Chicago Institute for Psychoanalysis (Eds.), The
Annual of Psychoanalysis (Vol. 20, pp. 114). The
Analytic Press.
Modell, A. H. (1993). The private self. Harvard
University Press.
Nguyen, T. T., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2018).
Solitude as an approach to affective self-regulation.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,44(1),
92106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217733073
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in
depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive
symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
109(3), 504511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-
843X.109.3.504
Ost Mor, S., Palgi, Y., & Segel-Karpas, D. (2020). The
denition and categories of positive solitude: Older
and younger adultsperspectives on spending time
by themselves. International Journal of Aging &
Human Development. Advance online publication.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0091415020957379
Potter, J., & Hepburn, A. (2005). Qualitative inter-
views in psychology: Problems and possibilities.
Research in Qualitative Psychology,2(4), 281307.
https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088705qp045oa
Puskar, K., Sereika, S., & Tusaie-Mumford, K. (2007).
Effect of the Teaching Kids to Cope (TKC) program
on outcomes of depression and coping among rural
adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psy-
chiatric Nursing,16(2), 7180. https://doi.org/10
.1111/j.1744-6171.2003.tb00350.x
Rook, K. S. (1988). Toward a more differentiated view
of loneliness. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of per-
sonal relationships (pp. 571589). Wiley.
Rook, K. S., & Peplau, L. A. (1982). Perspectives on
helping the lonely. In L. Peplau & D. Perlman
(Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory,
research and therapy (pp. 351378). Wiley.
Roth, G., Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2019).
Integrative emotion regulation: Process and develop-
ment from a self-determination theory perspective.
Development and Psychopathology,31, 945956.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000403
Rubenstein, C., & Shaver, P. (1982). In search of
intimacy. Delacorte Press.
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., & Bowker, J. C. (2009).
Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of
Psychology,60, 141171. https://doi.org/10.1146/
annurev.psych.60.110707.163642
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it?
Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy,57(6), 10691081. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.57.6.1069
Silton, R. L., Kahrilas, I. J., Skymba, H. V., Smith, J.,
Bryant, F. B., & Heller, W. (2020). Regulating
positive emotions: Implications for promoting
well-being in individuals with depression. Emotion,
20(1), 9397. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000675
Silvia, P. J., & Phillips, A. G. (2014). Evaluating
self-reection and insight as self-conscious traits.
Personality and Individual Differences,50(2), 234237.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.035
Simons, J. S., & Gaher, R. M. (2005). The distress
tolerance scale: Developmentand validation of a self-
report measure. Motivation and Emotion,29(2),
83102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-005-7955-3
Stein, D., & Grant, A. M. (2014). Disentangling the
relationships among self-reection, insight, and
subjective well-being: The role of dysfunctional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
138 THOMAS
attitudes and core self-evaluations. The Journal of
Psychology,148(5), 505522. https://doi.org/10
.1080/00223980.2013.810128
Storr, A. (1988). Solitude: A return to the self.
Free Press.
Thomas, V., & Azmitia, M. (2019). Motivation mat-
ters: Development and validation of the Motivation
for Solitude Scale-Short Form (MSS-SF). Journal
of Adolescence,70(1), 3342. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.adolescence.2018.11.004
Thomas, V., Balzer Carr, B., Azmitia, M., Whittaker,
S. (2021). Alone and online: Understanding the
relationships between social media, solitude, and
psychological adjustment. Psychology of Popular
Media,10(2), 201211. https://doi.org/10.1037/
ppm0000287
Twenge, J. M., Spitzberg, B. H., & Campbell, W. K.
(2019). Less in-person social interaction with peers
among U.S. adolescents in the 21st century and
links to loneliness. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships,36(6), 18921913. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0265407519836170
Wang, J. M., Rubin, K. H., Laursen, B., Booth-La-
Force, C., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2013). Preference-
for-solitude and adjustment difculties in early and
late adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology,42(6), 834842. https://
doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.794700
Westgate, E. (2020). Why boredom is interesting. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science,29(1), 33
40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884309
Winnicott, D. W. (1958). The capacity to be alone. The
International Journal of Psycho-Analysis,39,
416420.
Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes
and the facilitating environment. The Hogarth Press.
Young, J. E. (1982). Loneliness, depression, and
cognitive therapy: Theory and application. In L.
Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A source-
book of current theory, research and therapy
(pp. 379405). Wiley.
Youngblade, L. M., Berlin, L. J., & Belsky, J. (1999).
Connections among loneliness, the ability to be
alone, and peer relationships in young children. In
K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness in
Childhood and Adolescence (pp. 135152).
Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10
.1017/CBO9780511551888.007
Received March 19, 2021
Revision received August 30, 2021
Accepted September 14, 2021
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!
Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be
available online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at https://my.apa.org/
portal/alerts/ and you will be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become
available!
SOLITUDE SKILLS 139
... Yalnızlık tercihi diğer yalnızlık yapılarının aksine olumlu çıktılarla betimlenmektedir (Çelikbaş ve Tatar, 2021). Yalnızlık tercihinin olumlu çıktısı olarak özellikle iyi oluş öne sürülmektedir (Burger, 1995;Thomas, 2023). Yalnız geçirilen zaman içerisinde ve sonrasında düşük uyarılma pozitif etkisi (huzur, gevşeme, rahatlama, psikolojik dinginlik) görülmektedir. ...
... Ayrıca tercih edilen yalnızlıkta duygusal bileşenler dışında psikolojik çıktı olarak yaratıcılıkta, yenilenmede ve dikkatte artışlar görülmektedir. Bu pozitif çıktılar doğrultusunda yalnızlık tercihinin iyi oluş düzeyini arttırdığı bildirilmektedir (Thomas, 2023). Ancak her ne kadar yalnızlık tercihinin pozitif çıktılara sebep olduğu ifade edilse de bu pozitif çıktılar ölçme sonuçlarına her zaman yansımamaktadır (Coplan ve ark, 2021;Özdoğan ve ark., 2023). ...
... İlgili yazında yalnızlık algısının iyi oluş ve benlik saygısı ile negatif yönde ilişkili olduğu görülmektedir (Skoko ve ark., 2024). Bunun dışında yalnızlık tercihinin bu yapılarla pozitif yönde ilişkili olduğu bildirilmektedir (Thomas, 2023). Ayrıca yalnızlık algısının olumsuz sonuçları açısından yalnızlık tercihinin önemli bir rol üstlendiği belirtilmektedir. ...
Article
This study aims to examine the mediating role of preference for solitude and the moderating role of self-esteem in the relationship between loneliness and well-being. A total of 338 individuals (183 males and 155 females) aged between 18 and 55 years (M = 26.272  7.125 year) participated in this study. Data were collected using the Loneliness Scale, the Preference for Solitude Scale, the Personal Well-Being Scale, and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. For data analysis, frequency analysis, descriptive statistics, Pearson correlation analysis, and regression analysis using the SPSS Macro extension were conducted to test the moderated mediation model. The findings indicated that loneliness was positively associated with preference for solitude and negatively associated with self-esteem and personal well-being. While preference for solitude was negatively related to personal well-being, no significant relationship was found between preference for solitude and self-esteem. A positive correlation was observed between personal well-being and self-esteem. Additionally, preference for solitude mediated the relationship between loneliness and personal well-being. Furthermore, self-esteem moderated the relationship between preference for solitude and personal well-being. When self-esteem was one standard deviation above the mean, the direction of the relationship between preference for solitude and personal well-being became positive, and the strength of this relationship increased. However, when self-esteem was one standard deviation below the mean, the strength of the relationship increased, but the direction remained negative. The findings of this study were discussed in relation to the existing literature and compared with previous research.
... Well-being can be both hedonic (e.g., happiness) and eudaimonic (e.g., meaning) in nature, with some research showing solitude may have stronger associations with the latter form (Larson and Csikszentmihalyi 1978;Ost Mor, Palgi, and Segel-Karpas 2021;Thomas 2023a;Weinstein, Nguyen, and Hansen 2021). We asked participants to evaluate their well-being generalized over the course of the day by answering nine questions drawn from the Mental-Health Continuum Short Form (Keyes et al. 2008), a scale that measures both forms of well-being not as traits but as states that fluctuate based on a changing environment. ...
... Participant reports of well-being were generalized over the whole day and reported at end of each day, so it is unknown whether reduced well-being levels remained constant over the course of the day or whether they fluctuated before and after solitude (falling due to stress and then rising after time alone), which is what we would expect if voluntary solitude indeed has a stress-relieving function, or whether they fluctuated in the opposite direction. In the literature, some experimental (Nguyen, Ryan, and Deci 2018), experience sampling (Larson and Csikszentmihalyi 1978), and qualitative (Thomas 2023a(Thomas , 2023b) studies have indicated that moderate amounts of volitional solitude have a mood-regulating effect, allowing people to process difficult problems or feelings in private, after which they emerge feeling emotionally restored and better prepared to socially interact. Still other work has shown that time in solitude does not ameliorate stress and may in fact exacerbate ill-being (Larson and Lee 1996;Pauly et al. 2017). ...
... Although this study provides a clearer picture of the associations between the traits of introversion and sensitivity with solitary motivation and daily behavior, there are likely other individual differences and developmental factors that also play a role, and that moreover may determine the extent to which a person experiences benefits from time alone. Qualitative studies have begun to identify potential candidates, including the capacities for introspection and creativity, the development of a private self, and exposure to the benefits of solitude early in one's life, which may serve as a socializing factor (Ost Mor, Palgi, and Segel-Karpas 2021;Thomas 2023a;Weinstein, Hansen, and Nguyen 2023). A small but growing literature has begun to examine the role of culture in shaping solitary motivations and behaviors, with most studies focused on comparing populations from individualistic cultures which emphasize personal freedom and independence (e.g., the United States) with those from collectivistic cultures which emphasize social harmony and community (e.g., China); similar to the introversion literature, results have been mixed with some studies showing solitude is experienced more positively in individualistic cultures while other studies show the opposite pattern (Jiang et al. 2019;Lay, Fung et al. 2020;Lay, Pauly et al. 2020;Van Zyl, Dankaert, and Guse 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The state of solitude may be desirable and beneficial particularly for individuals who are highly sensitive and introverted. Methods To test these predictions, we surveyed a nationally representative US sample of 301 adults and a sample of 99 undergraduates on their levels of sensory processing sensitivity and assessed introversion with the Big Five Inventory and the multifaceted STAR Introversion Scale. Participants then reported the frequency and duration of their volitional solitude, stress levels, and subjective well‐being across 10 consecutive days. Results Results revealed that Social Introversion and sensitivity significantly predicted higher motivations for solitude, both self‐determined and not. Thinking Introversion also predicted higher self‐determined solitude, but BFI introversion showed no relationship with either motivation. Social Introversion and sensitivity predicted higher frequency of solitude in daily life and longer duration of solitary episodes; BFI Introversion and Restrained Introversion showed the opposite pattern for both outcomes. Finally, stress was positively associated with daily solitude frequency, and in turn, solitude frequency was negatively associated with same‐day well‐being; there were no interaction effects with personality traits. Discussion These findings suggest that introversion, as measured by the STAR Introversion Scale, and sensitivity contribute significantly to solitary motivation; however, solitude appears to be sought after by people in times of stress regardless of their scores on these traits.
... Many participants reported feeling more 'in tune' with themselves during alone time, free from social judgment. This sense of selfconnection and privacy was considered a key factor in their solitude experience [31]. However, solitude also posed risks, such as boredom, a sense of unful llment, and concerns about wasting time. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Retirement marks a significant life transition, often accompanied by the loss of structured work routines and an increase in alone time. How this solitude is experienced—positively or negatively—varies depending on individual and environmental factors. Methods: This study interviewed 8 pre-retirees and 15 retirees (N = 23) using semi-structured individual and focus group interviews. Guided by the Theory of Planned Behaviour framework, we asked participants to reflect on their retirement transition, attitudes toward solitude, social influences on their alone time, and their perceived control over managing solitude during retirement. Results: Thematic analysis revealed key themes, including shifts in identity, balancing social connection and alone time, and the role of autonomy and financial stability in shaping solitude experiences. While some participants embraced solitude as an opportunity for personal growth and well-being, others faced challenges such as boredom and social isolation. Conclusions: Findings suggest that achieving a balance between alone time and social engagement is critical to fostering positive experiences of solitude in retirement.
... In a planned live-interaction experiment, test the core hypothesis that a supportive social context characterized by empathic listening could simultaneously reduce positive expectations of being in solitude while enhancing the actual solitude experience. By doing so, the current research integrated recent findings in listening research (e.g., Kluger & Itzchakov, 2022;Yip & Fisher, 2022), solitude research (e.g., Thomas, 2023), and self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2017) to examine the impact of empathic listening on speakers' solitude. ...
Article
Full-text available
A live discussion experiment was designed to test the effects of highly empathic (vs. moderately empathic) listening on solitude experiences. Participants were assigned to three conditions in which they: 1) Discussed a negative personal experience with a confederate (ostensibly another participant) exhibiting highly empathic listening; 2) Discussed an experience with a confederate exhibiting moderately empathic listening; or, 3) Engaged in a positive reframing exercise. Building on previous listening theory (Weinstein et al., 2022) and research (Itzchakov & Weinstein, 2021; Itzchakov, Weinstein, et al., 2022). We then assessed the two posited mechanisms of autonomy and relatedness and tested the expectations to be in solitude. All participants were instructed to spend ten minutes alone, phones off, and distractions stored away. While highly empathic listening enhanced participants' (i.e. speakers) autonomy and relatedness need satisfaction compared to the other two conditions and predicted initial increases in self- and social-connection, it did not subsequently improve solitude experiences, with no direct effects found predicting self-connection, peaceful affect, loneliness, or self-insight. Indirect effects linked empathic listening to self-connection and self-insight through autonomy satisfaction. While empathic listening fosters immediate psychological need satisfaction in social contexts, deeper listening interventions may be necessary to improve subsequent solitude periods.
... To test these possibilities we explored the impact on both relatedness -the experience that one is close and connected to one's partner, and autonomy-the feeling that one can express oneself fully and behave congruently -during silence (e.g., as two indicators of relationship quality. A third psychological need posited by SDT -competence (feeling effective in activities and able to pursue and achieve meaningful goals; -is less relevant for understanding silence because it is typically related to external task or goal pursuit (Elliot et al., 2002); silence, in contrast, involves 'just being' with one's partner, a distinct and often not goal-directed experience (Thomas, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Silence shared between partners is a rich and understudied feature of romantic relationships. Within relationships, silence may be experienced in meaningfully different ways as a function of the motivations underlying it. These internally rich experiences may affect partners differently than silence that occurs spontaneously (i.e., without intentional initiation). In four studies, we tested the motives of silence and corresponding affect and relationship quality, operationalized through psychological need satisfactions and inclusion of other into self. Studies relied on complementary methods to explore the phenomenon of silence, namely cross-sectional, daily diary, and experimental designs. Findings across studies showed that intrinsically motivated silence was felt with more positive affect and less negative affect, and that relationships were closer and more need satisfying during intrinsically motivated moments of silence. Introjected and externally motivated silences, on the other hand, were often linked to more negative affect and lower relational outcomes. Spontaneous moments of silence were not consistently linked to affect or need satisfaction.
... Similarly, in line with Self Determination Theory, further studies could explore the relationships between motivations for interpersonal and solitary behaviors, motivations for SM use, and PSMU, to shed light on the underlying motives leading to PMSU. In addition, it could be that promoting "solitude skills" (Thomas, 2021) through validation of one's solitary activities in educational settings, could benefit and protect from loneliness and PSMU. These observations highlight the need to gather new evidence, in order to develop new interventions focusing both on PSMU and the well-being, especially among young adults, who seems to seek solitude more than other age groups Nguyen et al., 2019;Thomas & Azmitia, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This study was conducted to summarize existing studies on the association between solitary experiences and problematic social media use (PSMU) among young adults. Method: A systematic review was performed according to the PRISMA guidelines, implemented in Scopus, Web of Science, PubMed and PsycINFO. We selected studies if they presented original data, assessed solitary experiences and PSMU in young adults (i.e., 18-30 age range), were published in peer reviewed journals between 2004 and 2023, and were written in English. Results: After duplicate removal, 1,841 eligible studies were found. From these, 12 articles were selected, encompassing 4,009 participants. Most studies showed a positive association between general loneliness and PSMU. Some of these suggested that this relationship varies based on the facets of loneliness, other potential variables, and the type of social media. No mediating factors were found. Few studies assessed solitary experiences other than general loneliness, highlighting the need for a multidimensional perspective on solitary experience in investigating PSMU. Conclusions: Implications and future research orientations are discussed.
... In addition to motivation for solitary behaviors, specific individual skills could play a central role in PS. Specifically, a recent study (Thomas, 2023b), through qualitative methods, has identified various solitude skills, which were categorized into three main themes, i.e., connecting with self (which includes skills such as introspection and emotion regulation), protecting personal time (which involves skills such as validating one's solitary time), and finding a balance (which include skills such as heeding one's feelings to enter a state of solitude). Based on these results, PS not only reflects an individual's disposition, but can also be understood as a capacity that can be modified and implemented (Palgi et al., 2021b). ...
Article
Positive Solitude (PS) is the decision to use one’s time for a meaningful activity, with a choice made by and for oneself. The purpose of this study is to validate the Italian version of the Positive Solitude Scale (PSS) developed by Palgi et al. (2021a). Data were collected through the administration of a questionnaire to 1,120 Italian adults. Confirmatory factor analyses showed a bi-factor structure, indicating that the scale can be considered as unidimensional. Moreover, the Italian PSS demonstrated good internal consistency, supporting the scale’s good reliability. Then, PS was associated with improved psychological well-being, increased mindfulness, higher levels of positive effects and lower loneliness. However, we found no association between PS and depressive symptomatology. These results support a good convergent, discriminant, and divergent validity of the scale. Finally, this study enhances the understanding of PS in the Italian context providing a valuable tool for future research.
Article
Spending time alone is a virtually inevitable part of daily life that can promote or undermine well-being. Here, we explore how the language used to describe time alone - such as "me-time", "solitude", or "isolation" - influences how it is perceived and experienced. In Study 1 (N = 500 U.S adults), participants evaluated five common labels for time alone. Descriptive and narrative evidence revealed robust interindividual variability and significant mean differences in how these labels were evaluated. Overall, "me-time" was rated most positively, and "isolation" was rated least positively (but not negatively). In Study 2, we experimentally manipulated the linguistic framing of time alone, describing it as either "me-time" or "isolation". Participants (N = 176 U.S undergraduates) then spent 30 min physically alone without in-person or digital interaction. Notably, positive affect increased for "me-time" participants but decreased for "isolation" participants. Negative affect decreased in both conditions, but the magnitude of the decrease was greater after "me-time". People's beliefs about being alone improved after "me-time" but not after "isolation". Further, we explored participants' behaviours and thoughts while alone. These findings demonstrate meaningful variation in how people perceive different time alone labels and provide preliminary evidence that simple linguistic shifts may enhance subjective experiences of time alone.
Article
Daily need satisfaction for relatedness (social connection), autonomy (volitional self‐congruent action), and competence (self‐efficacy) fosters well‐being, but those findings primarily reflect experiences during social interactions. A three‐week daily diary study ( N = 178) explored psychological need satisfaction in two key everyday contexts: solitude and social. Holding constant the benefits of need‐satisfying social contexts, autonomy satisfaction during solitude was key to peaceful affect, whereas competence satisfaction during solitude contributed to lower loneliness; both psychological needs in solitude contributed to evaluating the day as more satisfying. Relatedness‐deficiency in solitude did not contribute to loneliness; instead, those who were relatedness‐deficient in social interactions felt lonely. Further, need satisfaction in solitude compensated for deficient needs in social contexts, reducing loneliness (autonomy and relatedness) and increasing day satisfaction (all needs) when social contexts failed to satisfy needs. Findings suggest daily solitude can shape daily well‐being and further attention is needed to understand and ultimately improve everyday solitude.
Article
Full-text available
Several studies have shown that people who engage in ruminative responses to depressive symptoms have higher levels of depressive symptoms over time, after accounting for baseline levels of depressive symptoms. The analyses reported here showed that rumination also predicted depressive disorders, including new onsets of depressive episodes. Rumination predicted chronicity of depressive disorders before accounting for the effects of baseline depressive symptoms but not after accounting for the effects of baseline depressive symptoms. Rumination also predicted anxiety symptoms and may be particularly characteristic of people with mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
A question that qualitative researchers are asked frequently is how they justify generalizing their finding to populations. In this article, I argue that this question results from a misunderstanding of generalization that conflates of the logic and mechanics of statistical generalization with that inherent in the process typically used in qualitative methods, which does not rely on probability sampling procedures. To clarify the differences in these processes, I propose the concept of qualitative generalization. It is built upon the work of scholars who have identified the logic of qualitative research as rooted in a cycle of inferential processes that identify forms of stability and variation in their data. Instead of using probability sampling to capture variability in samples which reflects that in a population, qualitative researchers use this cycle to develop a map of variation in their data, which reflects the practice and experience of the phenomena under study—a logic describing generalization to the phenomenon, not the population. The initial application of this self-correcting cycle of inferences underpins the later stage of transferability of findings by readers. The framework of methodological integrity is considered to explain how research goals, epistemological perspectives, and study characteristics (e.g., diversity) influence the identification of variation and, ultimately, qualitative generalization. This framework orients researchers to identify variation that can increase fidelity and utility, supporting qualitative generalization. The formulation of qualitative generalization proposed is seen as congruent with existing practices across a variety of qualitative traditions and with reasoning intrinsic to qualitative methods.
Article
Full-text available
This paper attempts to develop a better understanding of the positive solitude (PS) phenomenon and its meaning among age groups, as well as formulate a unanimous definition for PS. A qualitative study (N = 124) was conducted. Participants were gerontology professionals and laypeople. Interviews were conducted and content was analyzed in order to understand the essence of PS, themes, and subthemes. Two major categories were found: the essence and the components of the PS experience. Seven PS content categories and three meta-themes were raised: (1) PS is a matter of choice; (2) PS is satisfying and enjoyable; and (3) PS is meaningful. Differences in PS categories between younger and older adults were found. The meta-themes served as the foundation of a new definition. PS has different attributes in old age. A unified definition of PS may help distinguishing between PS and other forms of being alone. Recognizing and facilitating the PS experience among older adults might be beneficial and contribute to their quality of life.
Article
Full-text available
The rapid expansion of access to, and engagement with, the Internet and digital technology over the past 15 or so years has transformed the social, educational and therapeutic space occupied by children and young people in contemporary society in remarkable ways. First, it has created previously unimaginable opportunities for learning and development and personal exploration and growth. Second, it seems that the very same qualities and characteristics of the Internet that make these positive contributions possible, such as its immediacy, portability, intimacy, unconstrained reach and lack of supervision and regulation of content, has opened children and young people up to a range of serious social, intellectual and mental health risks. Finally, over and above these 'effects', the digital space is increasingly successfully being harnessed for the identification and treatment of mental health problems. Accordingly, the Internet is not so much a double‐, as a triple‐edged sword, with regard to children’s mental health.
Article
Full-text available
Social distancing and “stay-at-home” orders are essential to contain the coronavirus outbreak (COVID-19), but there is concern that these measures will increase feelings of loneliness, particularly in vulnerable groups. The present study examined change in loneliness in response to the social restriction measures taken to control the coronavirus spread. A nationwide sample of American adults (N = 1,545; 45% women; ages 18 to 98, M = 53.68, SD = 15.63) was assessed on three occasions: in late January/early February 2020 (before the outbreak), in late March (during the President’s initial “15 Days to Slow the Spread” campaign), and in late April (during the “stay-at-home” policies of most states). Contrary to expectations, there were no significant mean-level changes in loneliness across the three assessments (d = .04, p > .05). In fact, respondents perceived increased support from others over the follow-up period (d = .19, p < .01). Older adults reported less loneliness overall compared to younger age groups but had an increase in loneliness during the acute phase of the outbreak (d = .14, p < .05). Their loneliness, however, leveled off after the issuance of stay-at-home orders. Individuals living alone and those with at least one chronic condition reported feeling lonelier at baseline but did not increase in loneliness during the implementation of social distancing measures. Despite some detrimental impact on vulnerable individuals, in the present sample, there was no large increase in loneliness but remarkable resilience in response to COVID-19.
Chapter
FULL TEXT NOT AVAILABLE, PLEASE DO NOT REQUEST, ANY REQUESTS WILL BE DECLINED This chapter maps the terrain of thematic analysis (TA), a method for capturing patterns ("themes") across qualitative datasets. We identify key concepts and different orientations and practices, illustrating why TA is often better understood as an umbrella term, used for sometimes quite different approaches, than a single qualitative analytic approach. Under the umbrella, three broad approaches can be identified: a "coding reliability" approach, a "codebook" approach, and a "reflexive" approach. These are often characterized by distinctive-sometimes radically different-conceptualizations of what a theme is, as well as methods for theme identification and development, and indeed coding. We then provide practical guidance on completing TA within our popular (reflexive) approach to TA, discussing each phase of the six-phase approach we have developed in relation to a project on men, rehabilitation, and embodiment. We conclude with a discussion of key concerns related to ensuring the TA you do-within whatever approach-is of the highest quality. © Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019. All rights are reserved.
Chapter
Loneliness is experienced by children, adolescents and adults across varied cultures. In the early 1960s and 1970s, some authorities in the field of psychology did not believe that children experienced loneliness. This book ushers in a new wave of theory and research examining the phenomena of loneliness during childhood and adolescence. The book represents a thorough examination of the topic: the chapters range from the role of attachment in children's loneliness, differences between being alone and loneliness, the significance of divided self and identity achievement in adolescents' loneliness, and the link between loneliness and maladjustment during adolescence. This volume should stimulate research into loneliness during childhood and adolescence for many years to come.
Chapter
Loneliness is experienced by children, adolescents and adults across varied cultures. In the early 1960s and 1970s, some authorities in the field of psychology did not believe that children experienced loneliness. This book ushers in a new wave of theory and research examining the phenomena of loneliness during childhood and adolescence. The book represents a thorough examination of the topic: the chapters range from the role of attachment in children's loneliness, differences between being alone and loneliness, the significance of divided self and identity achievement in adolescents' loneliness, and the link between loneliness and maladjustment during adolescence. This volume should stimulate research into loneliness during childhood and adolescence for many years to come.