Content uploaded by Virginia Thomas
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Virginia Thomas on May 04, 2023
Content may be subject to copyright.
APPLIED ARTICLE
Solitude Skills and the Private Self
Virginia Thomas
Department of Psychology, Middlebury College
The construct of solitude skills suggests that successfully navigating the domain of solitude
may require specific psychological resources, but this theoretical possibility has not yet been
investigated empirically. Fourteen well-adjusted adults (M
age
=49.5) participated in a
qualitative study that examined their lived experiences with solitude and sought to identify
the skills they utilized when engaging in positive solitude. Thematic analysis (Braun &
Clarke, 2006) of narrative data resulted in the identification of eight solitude skills organized
within three central concepts: Connect with Self included the skills of enjoying solitary
activities, emotion regulation, and introspection; the skills included in Protect Time were
making time to be alone, using that time mindfully, and validating one’s need for solitude;
and the skills of Find a Balance included heeding signals to enter solitude and knowing
when to exit solitude. These findings illuminate how the documented benefits of solitude are
enacted and illustrate how solitude sustains the private self, which clinicians have argued
promotes well-being (Modell, 1992). Knowledge of these skills may be valuable for those
who volitionally enter solitude as well as those who find themselves in unwanted isolation.
These findings lay the groundwork for future studies to examine whether the solitude skills
identified here apply to other populations, and to explore the efficacy of solitude skills
trainings in promoting psychological well-being or as a clinical intervention.
Keywords: solitude, well-being, private self, emotion regulation, thematic analysis
Humans are social and solitary creatures both.
We spend much of our lives engaged in relation-
ships with others, and the psychological literature
on attachment attests to the primacy of social
connection for well-being (cf. Bowlby, 1988).
Yet literature indicates that intentionally retreat-
ing from the social sphere is also beneficial for
well-being, a point demonstrated in empirical
studies showing solitude’s role in self-regulation
(Nguyen et al., 2018) and self-reflection (Long &
Averill, 2003), and in the clinical literature
arguing that solitude is necessary to sustain the
authentic, private self (Modell, 1993).
Defining Solitude
Solitude has been defined as volitional time
spent by oneself that is generally used construc-
tively; in other words, alone time that is sought after
and utilized for the purpose of engaging in intrinsi-
cally motivated activities (Galanaki, 2013;Koch,
1994;Larson, 1990;Marcoen & Goossens, 1993).
Solitude is marked by an absence of communica-
tion and interaction with other people, typically—
but not necessarily—involving physical separation
from others. For example, someone may be in a
public place but still be in solitude by choosing to
not interact with others (Burger, 1995).
Volition is important in this definition because
it indicates a desire to be alone, making solitude
distinct from loneliness. Loneliness is conceptu-
alized as an unwanted, negative state of real or
perceived social isolation, marked by a longing
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This article was published Online First December 20, 2021.
Virginia Thomas https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8751-
8213
The author wishes to thank Anjali Dutt and Margarita
Azmitia for reading drafts of this manuscript and providing
feedback throughout the writing process.
Correspondence concerning this article should be
addressed to Virginia Thomas, Department of Psychology,
Middlebury College, 276 Bicentennial Way #280, Middle-
bury, VT 05753, United States. Email: vthomas@middle
bury.edu
121
Qualitative Psychology
© 2021 American Psychological Association 2023, Vol. 10, No. 1, 121–139
ISSN: 2326-3601 https://doi.org/10.1037/qup0000218
for companionship, rather than a desire to be
alone (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010). In contrast,
volitional solitude seeking increases the chance
that being alone will be a positive, beneficial
experience (Larson, 1990;Lay et al., 2019;
Long & Averill, 2003).
Theoretical and empirical literature both
point to the benefits of solitude throughout the
lifespan, as evidenced by the following examples.
Winnicott (1958) theorized that the capacity for
solitude begins in infancy and considered a per-
son’s lack of anxiety when alone to be a develop-
mental achievement. Children who prefer solitary
play demonstrate higher attention spans and
display less emotional negativity and hostility
than their more sociable peers (Coplan & Armer,
2007;Youngblade et al., 1999). Solitude in ado-
lescence creates opportunities for identity develop-
ment, autonomy, and mood regulation (for a
review, see Goossens, 2014), and for college stu-
dents, opportunities for increased self-awareness
(Franzoi & Brewer, 1984), emotional renewal
(Long & Averill, 2003), and personal growth
(Thomas & Azmitia, 2019). In adulthood, comfort
with being alone correlated positively with satis-
faction in life and negatively with depression
(Larson & Lee, 1996), and older adults’solitude-
seeking behaviors have been shown to result in
positive affect (Lay et al., 2020).
However, there are risks involved with spend-
ing time alone. A preference for solitude has been
correlated with loneliness (Burger, 1995), and
excessive social withdrawal can create adjust-
ment difficulties for children (Rubin et al.,
2009) and adolescents in particular (Bowker
et al., 2014;Wang et al., 2013). Time spent alone
has also been correlated with low mood (Larson,
1990), social anxiety (Alden & Auyeung, 2014),
and rumination (Lay et al., 2019).
In weighing the benefits and risks of spending
time alone, it is important to state again that
aloneness is a multidimensional construct, one
that can include constructive solitude but also
states of unwanted loneliness and isolation
(Maes et al., 2015). Thus, it is critical to distin-
guish between solitude that is self-determined,or
autonomously chosen and generally positively
experienced, versus not self-determined, because
of their distinct correlations, respectively, to
enhanced or impaired psychosocial functioning
(Deci & Ryan, 2008;Thomas & Azmitia, 2019).
In other words, the states of anxious solitude
(Gazelle & Druhen, 2009 ) and positive solitude
(Ost Mor et al., 2020) are markedly distinct.
Positive solitude is not only volitional, but also
rich in meaning and enjoyment, whereas anxious
solitude is characterized by social inhibition and
peer rejection and is associated with poor psy-
chosocial outcomes.
Solitude can thus be viewed as an “experiential
niche”filled with both opportunities and dangers
(Larson, 1990, p. 156). Although much has been
written about the necessity of avoiding the dan-
gers of spending time alone, there is compara-
tively little articulation of how one might
maximize the opportunities. In other words,
how does one have a constructive, positive expe-
rience of solitude?
The Theoretical Construct of Solitude Skills
Decades ago, Rubenstein and Shaver (1982)
invoked the term solitude skills for the first time,
suggesting that navigating the domain of solitude
and benefiting from time alone may require spe-
cific psychological resources or abilities. Without
the skills to utilize solitude constructively, people
may go inward only to find themselves lonely,
bored, or depressed.
The identification of skills in other psycholog-
ical domains has been well established; for exam-
ple, social skills trainings have been successful in
helping people move past social anxiety to func-
tion more readily in interpersonal relationships
and social groups (see Durlak et al., 2010 for a
review), and successful behavioral interventions
for depression include training individuals to use
adaptive coping skills during stressful experi-
ences (Puskar et al., 2007). By extension, it is
reasonable to expect that solitude skills trainings
could have analogous positive effects in the
intrapersonal sphere, promoting, for example,
identity development and self-regulation, and
potentially reducing experiences of loneliness
when individuals are alone.
However, to date psychologists have not inves-
tigated whether such skills exist, and if they do,
how they are enacted. Educators have suggested
the need for solitude skills curricula in our school
systems (Byrnes, 1983;Galanaki, 2005), but have
yet to articulate how children and adolescents can
develop and use these skills productively. In the
clinical arena, Rook (1988) differentiated
between loneliness that derives from a lack of
social skills versus a lack of solitude skills, but
these skills were not described. There are claims
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
122 THOMAS
that volitional solitude can serve as a treatment for
loneliness and depression (Burke, 1992;Knafo,
2012;Rook & Peplau, 1982;Young, 1982), yet
the skills needed to transform alone time into a
positive solitude experience rather than an isolat-
ing or terrifying one remain unspecified.
Aims and Research Question
Despite the lack of empirical evidence thus far,
the concept of solitude skills is theoretically
sound and worthy of investigation. The present
study was therefore designed to fill this empirical
gap and determine whether there is indeed evi-
dence for solitude skills, and in so doing affirm or
modify existing assumptions about such skills. In
short, the study sought to answer the following
research question: What are the skills individuals
utilize to experience solitude positively and con-
structively? Given the exploratory nature of the
research question, a qualitative study was de-
signed to identify and describe an initial set of
solitude skills, and no hypothesis testing was
proposed.
This research is timely because individuals are
increasingly spending more of their hours alone,
sometimes volitionally, as in the recent trend
worldwide trend of choosing to live alone
(Klinenberg, 2012) or involuntarily, as in the
case of being widowed (Dahlberg et al., 2018)
or socially isolated due to the coronavirus disease
(COVID-19) pandemic (Luchetti et al., 2020). In
addition, people of all ages are increasingly
spending more time alone online, and the costs
and benefits of solitary digital technology have
been documented and regularly updated as these
technologies continue to evolve and become
more mainstream (see Burnell et al., 2021, for
a review). While some studies have found that
solitary online experiences can be useful, for
example in terms of relieving stress (Leung,
2015) or accessing mental health resources
(Hollis et al., 2020), other evidence indicates
that excessive smartphone use is a lonely, isolat-
ing experience rather than a socially connected
one (Kim, 2021;Thomas et al., 2021;Twenge
et al., 2019).
Loneliness is a problem with both societal and
individual consequences (Hawkley & Cacioppo,
2010), and if learning how to transform alone time
into a positive experience of solitude would help
to ameliorate loneliness or depression, as some
have suggested, then identifying and describing
specific solitude skills is essential. Furthermore,
an understanding of solitude skills would con-
tribute to a better understanding of what a capac-
ity for solitude looks like (Winnicott, 1958),
including how individuals who are proficient in
solitude approach their alone time and extract
meaning from it, and would potentially increase
the likelihood that people who attempt solitude
will reap the benefits.
Method
The present study was conducted in three
phases: (a) an online survey, which effectively
served as a screening tool for study participation;
(b) an experiential component of a solitude date;
and (c) a follow-up interview. Approval for the
study was granted by the academic Institutional
Review Board overseeing Human Subjects
Research (approval no. #HS2299).
Recruitment
Purposive sampling was utilized to recruit
adults who were positively oriented to solitude
and had multiple life experiences of spending
extensive time alone. To increase the likelihood
of transferability of results, the study avoided
sampling extreme solitaires, such as self-
identified hermits, recluses, or individuals living
in remote areas for months or years at a time (see
Storr, 1988). Rather, this study recruited partici-
pants between the ages of 30 and 60 who were not
yet retired to target adults were actively engaged
in societal roles, with commitments to work and
family or friends in their daily lives, yet who still
sought out solitude within those constraints; a
time of life identified as the generativity stage in
Erikson’s life stage model (1997). Because of
solitude’s noteworthy associations with creativ-
ity and spirituality (Koch, 1994;Storr, 1988), an
announcement about the study was posted on an
online creative writing network, local to a city on
the West Coast of the U.S., and at a Buddhist
meditation center located on the East Coast of the
United States, with an additional snowball effect
of word of mouth. The recruitment flyer opened
with “A call to those who enjoy being alone”with
an invitation to participate in a research study to
“share your experiences of solitude,”and briefly
listed the three phases of the study and a con-
tact email.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 123
Recruited participants completed an online
survey where they gave their written consent to
participate. The online survey served as a screen-
ing tool to assess their status as solitude experts.A
review of expertise in a variety of domains
(Hatano & Oura, 2003) distinguished experts
from novices in the following ways: experts
have rich knowledge of the domain in question;
experts have gained this knowledge over years of
experience with the domain; and their knowledge
and experience over time have been accompanied
by socioemotional changes (for an example of
this approach in the field of creativity, see
Kaufman et al., 2005). The prestudy survey,
administered online, inquired about the fre-
quency and duration of time spent alone by choice
in their daily lives and assessed their affinity for
solitude through two psychometric scales: the
Preference for Solitude Scale (Burger, 1995)
and the Motivation for Solitude Scale Short-
Form (Thomas & Azmitia, 2019). Additionally,
participants completed the UCLA Loneliness
Scale, Short-Form (Hays & DiMatteo, 1987)to
avoid oversampling a lonely population and
potentially conflating positive solitude skills
with coping strategies for loneliness.
Participants
Fifteen participants were recruited and com-
pleted the prestudy survey; one participant
declined to complete the remainder of the study.
This resulted in an analytic sample of 14 adults,
aged 33–61 (M
age
=49.5). Eight participants (six
women; two men) identified as creative writers
and were employed in a related profession (e.g.,
literature professor, book editor, writing tutor).
Six participants (three women; three men) iden-
tified as contemplatives who engaged in regular
meditation practice and held full- or part-time
staff positions at a contemplative retreat center
grounded in Buddhist teachings. Participants
identified as White (N=11), Japanese/French
Canadian (N=1), White/Irish (N=2), and
claimed their nation of origin as Australia
(N=1), Canada (N=1), and the United States
(N=12), although all were living in the U.S. at
the time of data collection. Eight were in long-
term relationships, and three were parents of
children currently living with them.
On average, participants reported inten-
tionally spending an average of 30.1 hr alone
per week for pleasure, beyond time spent alone
for work purposes. Nine of the fourteen parti-
cipants had spent at least two consecutive days
alone at some point in the past 2 years. Alto-
gether, the sample showed high preference for
solitude (M=10.29, SD =1.68; maximum
possible score of 12), high self-determined moti-
vation for solitude (M=25.43, SD =5.26;
maximum possible score of 32), and moderately
low loneliness (M=17.0, SD =4.45; maxi-
mum possible score of 32). Thus, on the whole,
participants spent significant amounts of time
alone in their daily lives, despite their work,
social, or family commitments, exhibited a high
preference for solitude, were motivated for soli-
tude for self-determined reasons, and were not
overly lonely.
Procedure
Solitude Date
After completing the online survey, partici-
pants agreed to spend one full day alone on a
solitude date (Manalastas, 2011) at a time of their
choosing. Rather than place constraints or rules
on where or how participants spent their time, no
instructions were provided for how to spend their
day in solitude and each person made their own
choices about internet use, phone use, and being
inside or outside of their homes. The rationale for
the lack of instruction stemmed from the research
goal of examining how solitude was enacted in
daily life and what skills were used to enjoy such
time alone, without intervention or influence from
the author of the study. By permitting participants
to exercise their own agency on their solitude
date, unencumbered by a set of external expecta-
tions or rules, the author aimed to more accurately
examine solitude in vivo.
Participants were provided with a worksheet to
complete at the end of their solitude date, which
was an adapted form of the Day Reconstruction
Method (Kahneman et al., 2004). The worksheet
invited participants to jot notes about their day in
solitude, including the various activities they
engaged in, their moods throughout the day,
and things they thought about during their time
alone. Use of the worksheet was optional, and
participants were informed that the worksheet’s
purpose was only as a memory aid for the subse-
quent interview, and the worksheet would not be
collected nor analyzed.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
124 THOMAS
Interview
Within 48 hr of completing their solitude date
each participant engaged in a semi-structured,
private interview by phone with the author, which
lasted approximately 1 hr and was audio re-
corded. The author began each interview by
describing the purpose of the study, which was
to understand how people experience solitude,
including both positive and negative experiences
of being alone, with a particular interest in hearing
from individuals like themselves who frequently
sought out solitude. In an effort to be transparent
and build rapport, the author briefly shared that
she herself had a long history of appreciating
solitude, although sometimes found it challeng-
ing to acquire alone time given the demands of
parenthood while working full-time, and there-
fore was also interested in how participants bal-
anced their current commitments and
relationships with their desire for solitude. The
author thus positioned herself as both academi-
cally and personally invested in the research
question, and in a similar stage of life as many
of the participants, while also approaching the
interview with an attitude of openness and
curiosity.
The interview questions were constructed using
a two-pronged approach: the first half of the
interview elicited from the participants the activi-
ties, thoughts, and feelings experienced during
their recent solitude date; the second half asked
them to share and reflect on their experiences of
solitude more generally, past and present. This
two-pronged approach allowed the interviewer to
examine the phenomenology of the solitude expe-
rience through a “micro”lens, by probing for
specifics of how participants navigated a recent
episode of time alone, as well as through a
“macro”lens, by listening to participants share,
reflect on, and evaluate their lived experiences of
solitude in a diverse range of contexts over time.
The semistructured format of the interview and the
predominance of open-ended questions allowed
for conversational dialogue and ample opportu-
nity for follow-up questions, clarifications, and
requests for illustrative examples.
Given that the meanings of solitude may vary
from person to person, each participant was
asked to define solitude and how they would
differentiate it from loneliness. Participants
were unanimous in affirming that there were
distinct differences between these two states.
They described solitude as primarily an enjoy-
able state of being alone, typically engaged in by
choice and accompanied by positive connota-
tions, often using metaphors of value and full-
ness, such as “a gift,”“enriching,”and “feeling
nourished.”In contrast, they described loneli-
ness in negative terms, such as feeling “empty,”
“hollow,”and “disconnected.”For instance,
Mika
1
differentiated these states as such:
Loneliness is a longing. My heart wants something
that’s not there. And it’s a pull, I feel somewhat melan-
choly or like I’m missing something. Solitude is—it’s
like (sigh), it’s a peace, I’m—I’m whole and complete
and my heart feels content being with me, like I don’t
feel the need or the pull for something outside of myself.
There’s a sense of wholeness and fullness.
Thus, by first eliciting from participants their
own definition of the phenomenon under investi-
gation, the author could more confidently ensure
that subsequent questions were built on a more
mutual understanding of what the term soli-
tude meant.
To avoid influencing the participants to view
their solitude experiences through the lens of
skills, the term itself was never used in the inter-
view. Rather, open-ended questions about their
lived experiences of solitude, both positive and
negative, were used to see if concepts related to
the theoretical proposal of solitude skills arose
organically in the interview. For instance, parti-
cipants were asked what motivated them to seek
solitude, how they benefited from solitude, and
what they enjoyed about being alone. Given prior
literature pointing out potential pitfalls of soli-
tude, in particular low mood and negative affect
(Goossens, 2014;Lay et al., 2019), participants
were also asked about any difficulties they faced
around solitude, in particular whether they expe-
rienced negative emotions such as loneliness or
boredom when alone and if so, how they re-
sponded. Finally, they were asked whether and
how their enjoyment of solitude affected other
people, and what messages they had received
about solitude from their family, friends, and
cultures in which they had lived.
Analysis
Interviews were transcribed verbatim for anal-
ysis and pseudonyms were assigned to protect
participants’identities. The interviews were
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
1
All participant names are pseudonyms.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 125
analyzed using a phenomenologically informed
thematic analysis, which focuses on the subjec-
tive experiences and perspectives of a small group
of participants on a given topic, analyzes narra-
tives thematically rather than idiographically to
identify meaningful patterns in the dataset, and
utilizes an organic, iterative coding process
(Braun & Clarke, 2006;Braun et al., 2019).
Thematic analysis (TA) is a flexible approach
that allows for active engagement with the data to
identify shared meanings across the dataset and
interpret them in a coherent and compelling way,
while not being constrained by a particular epis-
temological framework or methodological
tradition.
The flexibility of TA thus accommodates the
phenomenological aspect of this study, that is
closely examining individuals’lived experiences
of a specific and often underexplored phenomena,
as well as supports the aim of generating theory—
in this case, articulating a set of solitude skills
both theoretically consistent with previous litera-
ture on the benefits of solitude and potentially
transferable to populations beyond the study
sample. Finally, TA’s active and reflexive
approach (Braun et al., 2019) to coding qualita-
tive interviews reflects a post-positivist paradigm
(Guba & Lincoln, 1994), and is especially useful
when examining exploratory research questions
that do not involve hypothesis testing. A phenom-
enologically informed approach to thematic anal-
ysis was therefore determined to be the
appropriate analytic method for the present study.
Prior to coding, familiarization of the data took
place through a process of immersive reading of
each manuscript. Coding categories were not
developed a priori, given the lack of empirical
literature on solitude skills; rather, codes were
generated during analysis in an iterative, inter-
pretive process drawn from intensive, multiple
readings of each transcript.
Inductive coding occurred during subsequent
readings of the transcripts, taking first the form of
captions or phrases which captured the essence of
the content. These initial codes were provisional
and semantic in nature; care was taken to stay
close to the meaning of the participants’original
words. Next, semantic coding transitioned to
latent coding, a more interpretive process that
considered the semantic codes as the surface
features of underlying conceptual patterns across
the dataset (Braun et al., 2019), and fidelity to the
meaning of the participants’words was preserved
through the linkage of transcript line numbers to
each code.
Using thematic mapping (Braun & Clarke,
2006), latent codes were then clustered theoreti-
cally into candidate themes. An iterative process
was used to promote candidate themes to a final
set of themes; as new candidate themes were
constructed from the latent codes, transcripts
were reviewed again to determine whether the
candidate themes indeed reflected the meaning of
the narratives. During this process, candidate
themes were revised, clarified, or dropped.
Afinal set of themes was determined based on
their prevalence within the dataset, theoretical
importance, and relevance to the research ques-
tion (i.e., solitude skills). Further review of the
analysis revealed that the final set of eight themes,
or skills, when viewed as a whole, could be seen
as comprising three larger categories, each repre-
senting a central organizing concept, that both
integrated and expanded on the extant literature.
Results
This study investigated the exploratory ques-
tion: What are the skills required to experience
solitude positively and constructively? Analysis
yielded evidence for eight themes (i.e., skills)
clustered within three central organizing con-
cepts: Connect with Self, Protect Time, and
Find a Balance (see Table 1). Each skill is
described, supported by thick description from
participant narratives.
The Skills of Connecting with the Self
The first three skills showcased participants’
ability to connect with themselves in three as-
pects: behaviorally, by enjoying meaningful
solitary activities (Skill 1); emotionally, by
experiencing and regulating various emotional
states (Skill 2); and cognitively, by engaging in
introspection and self-insight (Skill 3).
Skill 1: Enjoyment of Solitary Activities
These adults rarely experienced boredom dur-
ing solitude, and they explained its absence as a
function of deriving satisfaction and meaning
from solitary pursuits. This enabled them to
genuinely enjoy time not only by themselves,
but also with themselves. Participants explicitly
described their solitude as “enjoying my own
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
126 THOMAS
company”(Doug), “happy with my own com-
pany”(Catherine), and “enjoying being by
myself”(Laurie). A conversation with Diana
illustrated this skill:
I am never, ever bored in solitu de ::: because in solitude
I can follow whatever it is that is interesting. I find my
thoughts interesting and if my thoughts aren’t interesting,
well there’s always a book, you know ::: and if I’m, if
I’m not in a reading mood, well I can take a bath, or I
could, you know, dance, or I could water the plants. I
mean ::: there’s nothing about being alonethat’s boring.
Frequently the activities mentioned by parti-
cipants were everyday pleasures such as reading,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Table 1
Eight Solitude Skills Within Three Central Organizing Concepts
Central organizing
concept Theme Definition of theme Example of theme
Connect with self 1. Enjoyment of solitary
activities
Ability to find pleasure and
meaning in solitary activities;
enjoying one’s own company
“I feel nourished by myself
meditating or being creative or
going on a long bike ride by
myself. And then if I’m doing
those things, it’s like I need no
one else, I need nothing else, I
can just be happy and content
with what I’m doing.”
Connect with self 2. Emotion regulation Ability to identify feelings,
tolerate distress, and regulate
emotions
“As my body relaxed, my mind
quieted, I had a lot of tears, I had
an emotional release that felt
really cleansing, even though it
was uncomfortable to feel
sadness.”
Connect with self 3. Introspection Ability to engage in self-reflection
and insight
“I process what I’m needing or
going through at the time in my
life, it’s important for me to just
to kind of step back and reflect
on everything.”
Protect time 4. Make time for solitude Ability to carve out time for
solitude, and if needed,
negotiate for it
“During the week when I’m
working, and I go to the gym or
Ifill my day with stuff, I still
have several hours alone to
recharge or to prepare myself,
and that’s important to me.
Protect time 5. Be mindful of how time
in solitude is spent
Ability to use solitude
intentionally or constructively,
free of distractions or
obligations to others
“[I tell myself] don’tgoonthe
computer because you’re going
to get distracted ::: I just try to
be very disciplined about it so
that I don’t feel interrupted or
empty.”
Protect time 6. Validate need for
solitude
Ability to validate one’s need for
solitude despite judgments from
self, others, or society
“There is kind of this feeling you
know, in our world, in our
society, that if you like to spend
a lot of time alone, there’s
something kind of wrong with
you. And I don’t agree with that
at all.”
Find a balance 7. Heed internal signals to
enter solitude
Ability to notice and act on
internal sensations that signal a
need to enter solitude
“I just feel easily irritated and I just
want everyone to leave me
alone, and so then I’m like, oh
okay, I need to put myself in
time-out.”
Find a balance 8. Know when to exit
solitude
Ability to recognize when solitude
has served its purpose and act
on the desire to re-enter social
life
“I’m feeling restless or lonely :::
I’ve gotten what I need from
being alone, and okay, now
who can I go meet?”
SOLITUDE SKILLS 127
walking in nature, engaging in a hobby, or fol-
lowing their own thoughts in silence. The content
of the solitary activity did not appear to be
significant; what mattered was that the activity
was intrinsically pleasurable and meaningful
when experienced alone. Philip made this point
clear when he said:
There isn’t enough time in any day to do the things I
want to do ::: there are a million things I want to read,
there’s a million things I want to watch, and I have my
[screenwriting] project to work on, and I’ve got all
these—so there’s always stuff. And again, I’m not
talking about, like, distractions, they’re really just like
rewarding things that I want to focus my attention on.
The relationship between this skill and its
central organizing concept of Connect with Self
is illustrated by Doug, who attributed previous
experiences of boredom in solitude to “lack of
self-contact, like I’m disconnected from myself.”
He contrasted that with his current experience of
solitude for the present study: “I’m just kind of
content to be with myself ::: I wasn’t longing or
pining or missing anything, relationally. And I
was really connected and present with my own
experience.”
Skill 2: Emotion Regulation
This skill described a willingness to experience
a range of emotions during solitude, tolerate
distress caused by difficult emotions, and process
those emotions until a shift or insight occurred.
Participants identified both positive and negative
emotional states during their solitude experi-
ences, but this skill centered on their ability to
approach rather than avoid the difficult emotions
that sometimes emerged, including grief, anger,
loneliness, despair, confusion, and anxiety.
Some participants, like Shelby, were startled
by initial negative emotions that surfaced but
were able to accept them:
As my body relaxed, my mind quieted. I had a lot of
tears, I had an emotional release that felt really cleans-
ing, even though it was uncomfortable to feel sadness
::: I could feel in my chest this feeling like my heart is
breaking ::: I also had a process of ::: just kind of
being really welcoming towards whatever was arriving.
Others, like Laurie, intentionally sought soli-
tude in order to process emotions they were
already feeling:
[If I’m feeling] a little bit insecure or a little bit fragile, I
definitely want to be alone ::: I guess [solitude] lets me
be with [my emotions] in a way where I don’t have to
explain them—it feels like if I’m alone I’m allowed
more time to just kind of let it, let it be ::: and then it
usually just, I kind of move on, move on from it.
Participants echoed each other in sharing that
these difficult emotions did not scare them nor
discourage them from solitude. Rather than
abandon solitude, they chose to stay alone. As
Diana described, “IfeelthatIspendmostofmy
regular life without having enough time to be
really sad and that almost every time I’m alone,
the initial thing that happens is that I go through
intense sadness and then, and then I proceed.”
Participants utilized various strategies to navi-
gate these emotions, from physical movement to
self-reflection, and shared stories of successfully
moving through to a more emotionally balanced
state. In Diana’scase,“I thought through the
sadness as far as it needed to go and then it just
evaporated.”
In particular, participants described how they
worked through any loneliness that arose so that
they could return to a more positive experience of
being alone. For example, Audrey shared:
If I’m lonely and I need to get back to the solitude I
might go out to the ocean for a long walk because there’s
something about hearing the waves, seeing the waves,
seeing those little birds scurry across the sand, just
looking at the sand trying to find sand dollars, especially
watching the waves, I find it very reassuring and then I
just feel so much better and I can hear myself.
Skill 3: Introspection
These adults talked about solitude as an oppor-
tunity to take a step back from their everyday lives
to reflect and gain insight about themselves. This
was often framed as a benefit of solitude; for
example, time alone provided the chance for Nora
to “get a new perspective on life”and served as
time for reflection for Laurie “when I feel like
there’s something I kind of want to think about or
just kind of sit with for a while.”
Introspection included both a curiosity about
their inner lives as well as a willingness to exam-
ine their own behaviors, feelings, and thoughts.
For Scott, this skill was central to his experience
of solitude: “To me it’s useful to be silent because
then you can see yourself clearly, that’s the whole
point of it.”
For Philip, introspection helped him under-
stand his authentic self. Although he acknowl-
edged that humans are social creatures and in fact
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
128 THOMAS
identified himself as a very sociable person, he
seemed to view his truest self as something
accessible in its most authentic form when
engaged in solitary introspection.
You can’t know who you are if you don’t spend time
completely alone with yourself ::: I think you really get
in touch with what’s going on inside of you, how you’re
feeling, who you are, what your opinions are, what your
beliefs are, knowing how you react to things, yeah, your
internal values, your internal—knowing just everything
about yourself that has nothing to do with other people’s
reactions or who they are. But if we’re all just plugged
into each other, there’s so much other noise that’s
intruding on listening to yourself, your own instincts,
your own feelings. It’s like you lose that if you’re not
spending time alone.
Altogether, the first three skills belong in the
central organizing concept of Connect with Self—
a connection that appeared important for these
participants’well-being. However, this state of
self-connection depended on them being able to
protect their time in solitude.
The Skills of Protecting One’s Time
The second central organizing concept, Protect
Time, included the themes of making time for
solitude (Skill 4), using that time mindfully (Skill
5), and validating their need for time alone
(Skill 6).
Skill 4: Make Time for Solitude
Participants expressed the need to be disci-
plined and strategic about securing time in soli-
tude. For example, Audrey shared:
After college, in my twenties, I started drawing. I would
draw every night for four hours and so I think I would
rearrange friends around that time. Because I’d say, well
I have to draw, I need to draw for four hours, I have to
draw, I spend every night drawing for four hours, and so
I can’t see you tonight (laughs) ::: It’s just this thing
that I did and it was like feeding myself. I had to–I had to
do this thing for four hours.
Audrey frequently described solitude as “nour-
ishment”and as an experience akin to being “well
fed.”For her, carving out 4 hr every night to draw
in solitude was as necessary as eating dinner, and
she has continued to practice this skill in her 30s
and 40s, declining social invitations when they
interfere with a need to be alone.
Matt made this skill apparent by discussing his
difficulties with it. For him, being a husband and a
father meant he needed to negotiate for solitude if
he wanted to make time for it:
I think it’s one of those things that you go through
negotiations, or you go through like an evolution in
terms of how you deal with it in a relationship. I think
[my wife] is more understanding of that need [for
solitude] now than ever, like you know, the longer
we’ve been together the more she’s realized, you
know, this is just who he is, this is just something he
needs, and she understands that it’s good for me and so
therefore it’s good for us. Right, like I’ll be better to be
around, we’ll both be happier if I get some time—but it’s
always a negotiation because she wants the opposite.
However, the skill of making time for solitude
was especially difficult when close relationships
were unsupportive. For example, Maeve histori-
cally found herself in relationships with people
who “did not get it”and who in fact “took it
personally”when she spent time alone. “I ended
so many relationships because I couldn’tfigure
out how to negotiate how badly I needed time for
myself,”she recalled. In order to secure her
solitude in subsequent relationships, Maeve
had to do three things: “explain”her need for
solitude, “ask for it,”and “set the boundaries.”
Skill 5: Be Mindful of how Time in Solitude
Is Spent
This skill described the measures participants
took to make their time in solitude “well spent,”
rather than “wasting”it, to quote Philip. For them,
constructive solitude was dependent on being
mindful that their time alone was volitional,
uninterrupted, and intentional—which included
being free of distractions, especially the internet.
Mika illustrated this when she described her
solitude date for the present study:
Facebook—it can be a real waste of time, and I just
decided that that was not going to be a part of the day. So
not having that as an outlet really brought me back to
what do I want to do right now, like what’s, what’san
opportunity, what’s an available opportunity for me
right now at this moment, instead of just sort of being
swept along by my mind’s preoccupation with the
Internet or with the computer ::: There was more
mindfulness around pretty much everything that I did.
I was much more in touch with myself, I was much more
aware of my mind racing and then me, you know,
wanting to come back to being present.
In most cases, participants shared that they
worked hard to prevent other people from inter-
rupting them, whether it be retreating to a
secluded place or asking people not to call
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 129
them during a certain time of day. However, the
more difficult aspect of securing uninterrupted
solitude was monitoring their own propensity to
interrupt or distract themselves. Audrey attrib-
uted her lack of interruptions while in solitude to
being mindful about what she wanted in the
moment:
I don’t get interrupted very often and (pause) and I think
that I’m, I try to pay really close attention to how I’m
feeling because if I’m feeling in a time of solitude it feels
like, like there’s this cushion around me and, and so I
can say, okay I have this cushion around me, now is a
good time to, to just end up writing or draw or play
music. And then I, then if I want to call somebody, I
really have to say to myself, okay if you call this person,
you’re going to break out of the solitude and you know,
you just have to be aware that it might take a little while
to get back into it.
Finally, the skill of being mindful involved
participants entering the space of solitude with
clear intentions about what they wanted to expe-
rience or accomplish, andnot allowing themselves
to get distracted by meaningless activities. Maeve
described how she distinguished whether or not
her solitude was “worthy”:
Sometimes you know, I just want to lie on the couch and
watch a movie, but I do know like if I go too far with that
like, there’s a certain way in which distractions take me
into that disconnected place in myself ::: I really feel
like part of the reason that I need solitude and part of the
way that solitude can be so helpful for me is that
because, it’s because it allows me to go more deeply
into things that are truly meaningful to me on like a spirit
and soul level, and I don’t, I don’t just like to throw that
time away.
Skill 6: Validate Need for Solitude
This skill described the ability to validate one’s
need for solitude despite judgments from self,
others, or society. Although these participants
emphasized throughout their interviews how nec-
essary time in solitude was for them, they also
acknowledged that it went against social norms.
They were aware that the culture of the United
States in which they all currently lived placed a
high value on socializing and viewed people who
prefer to be alone in deficit terms; “weird,”“anti-
social,”and “selfish”were the most frequent
terms used by participants.
Responses to this cultural bias against solitude
fell on a spectrum. On one end, they diagnosed the
culture as misguided or biased. This helped them
affirm their need for solitude as normal and
natural. For example, Matt shared that he not
only validates his own need for solitude but tries
to be an example for his daughter:
I think American culture is highly skewed towards
extroverts, and that’s kind of seen as the norm, and
the ideal. And so I think solitude is always kind of
looked at with suspicion ::: I think it’s good for [my
daughter] to see solitude as something that’s not nega-
tive, as something that can certainly be positive, and it’s
positive for me ::: I think it’s good that she’s able to see
different ways of being, that there’s not one way that’s
right, or one way that’s ideal, that different people have
different needs. And it doesn’t mean that you’re antiso-
cial or weird or don’t have good people skills or
whatever.
On the other end of the spectrum, they some-
times struggled with these internalized cultural
messages of solitude as signaling deficiency.
During these times, they were unable to consis-
tently validate their desire to be alone when they
needed it. Two participants were mothers of
school-aged children, and they often felt guilty
or selfish for seeking solitude because of per-
ceived gendered expectations that they be emo-
tionally available to others at all times. In addition
to self-judgments, many participants worried
about how other people would react to their
need for solitude. For example, Doug said for
much of his life, “The fear was that they would be
upset with me, like they would take it personally
somehow ::: the hesitancy to assert that [need
for solitude] for fear of some kind of retaliation or
rejection.”
Catherine shared how she learned over time to
validate her desire for solitude:
Ten years ago when I was less comfortable with my own
company than I am now ::: going to a party, say, that I
didn’t necessarily want to go to, but I went anyway, and
then waking up and being tired the next morning and
mad at myself ::: choosing to be with people that I
didn’t really want to be with. And then letting myself
down. Yeah, and then I finally thought, wait a minute,
this is my life. I’m not going to break these promises to
myself anymore. If I want to be at home by myself, I’m
going to be at home by myself. And just tell people, no
thank you. Yeah. And the world did not end.
Catherine’s words suggest that an essential
component of this skill is validating to oneself—
not necessarily to others—the need for solitude
and learning how to ignore or reframe any related
internalized judgment or guilt. Aaron echoed
this sentiment when he said, “The most impor-
tant thing is giving myself permission [to
be alone].”
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
130 THOMAS
The Skills of Finding a Balance
The final two skills involved creating a healthy
balance of time in solitude versus time in the
social world by noticing internal cues that sig-
naled a need to enter (skill 7) or exit (skill 8)
solitude.
Skill 7: Heed Internal Signals to Enter Solitude
This skill was two-fold; it entailed first the
ability to notice internal cues, whether they be
physical sensations or emotions, and interpret
them as signals to enter solitude, and second,
to act on those signals by moving into solitude.
The internal cues were indicators of overstimula-
tion, and were most frequently described as feel-
ing overwhelmed, exhausted, or easily irritated
with others. Doug summarized it as:
It’s like this sense of under my collar is too warm. And
that I can’t hear myself think, you know ::: the emo-
tions, and the irritation ::: this sense of exasperation, I
think overwhelmed is in there somewhere, like I’ve just
got to go be alone ::: I want to take care of myself and
go back to sort of quietly reconnect or see what’s
happening ::: I kind of lost myself here somewhere
so let’s go be by myself again to connect with myself.
It was clear from these participants’stories that
interpreting these signals in a timely and accurate
way was a skill they learned over time. Maeve
described it as such:
I get excessively irritable ::: literally my system just
shuts down ::: I will keep going as long as I can but it
feels like it comes at a really high cost, you know :::
I’ve learned to recognize the early warning signs of
[I’m] going to have a meltdown, right. I start to notice
that I feel overwhelmed, I start to complain about my
surroundings, I start to tell myself stories about how bad
everything is. I’m like, I know what’s happened here,
I’ve just been getting too tired, I’m getting too far away
from myself and I just need some time, I need to get out,
I need to go for a walk in nature by myself, whatever. So,
I think it’s you know either that I’m feeling overstimu-
lated or that I’m feeling like for whatever reason I
haven’t really been attending to, you know, myself
on a spiritual or creative level with those things.
Skill 8: Know When to Exit Solitude
Just as participants recognized internal signals
that prompted them to seek solitude, they likewise
identified when they needed to exit solitude.
These signals fell into two primary categories:
boredom and loneliness. Philip recognized when
his solitude has served its purposes when, “I’m
feeling restless or lonely ::: I’ve gotten what I
need from being alone, and okay, now who can I
go meet?”Participants emphasized that it was
essential to discern whether these feelings sig-
naled a real need to rejoin the social world, or
whether they instead represented a need to go
deeper into their solitude. Maeve elaborated on
the necessity of discernment when loneliness or
boredom arose:
I’ll get restless when I’m alone and sometimes I’ll be
like, is this restlessness because I’m interested in going
deeper in my own process with myself? Because like if I,
if I get restless and I haven’t felt like I really settled with
myself then I think that it’s not really the sign that I have
to go out and be with people. But you know, if I’ve been
alone a while and I feel like I’ve come to a subtle place
that sort of—Ifirst just wait to see what happens and
then you know, what feels like a really natural urge just
to get out and connect more comes to me.
Participants reported that they were glad to
socially reconnect when they felt they had suffi-
cient time in solitude, and in fact remarked that
spending time alone enhanced those relation-
ships. Audrey noted, “I can’t be with people
effectively unless I’ve spent time in solitude,”
and Laurie said, “my solitude helps my interac-
tions.”She elaborated by saying:
[Solitude is] how I kind of recharge, and I think if I’m
feeling, if I’m feeling overwhelmed or if I feel I don’t
have enough time for myself, then I will be cranky and I
will be sort of brusque ::: I just don’t want to really be
around people. Whereas if I’ve had some solitude, yeah,
I think it does [help]. Because then when I’m around
people I’m glad to be around people.
The balance of solitude and sociability that
resulted from the use of these final two skills
appeared to foster connection not only with
themselves, but also with others.
Discussion
The set of solitude skills identified in the
present study supported an experience of positive
solitude for these participants and contributes to
an understanding of how individuals can success-
fully navigate the domain of solitude and reap the
benefits. Furthermore, this study integrates prior
findings from disparate developmental and clini-
cal traditions and extends that research in
new ways.
Behaviorally, it appears that individuals who
have the capacity to be alone know how to engage
in intrinsically enjoyable activities. Boredom
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 131
signals a failure to engage one’s attention on
meaningful activities (Westgate, 2020), which
explains the notable absence of boredom experi-
enced by these participants during their solitude;
they were expert in their ability to find meaning
and pleasure in solitary activities. Burger (1995)
remarked on a similar absence of boredom in the
individuals he surveyed who scored high in a
preference for solitude. The lack of boredom may
be partially explained by these participants’ten-
dency to become immersed in whatever they were
doing—whether it be a creative project, an intro-
spective process, or an appreciation for the beauty
of their surroundings. This is consistent with the
proposal that, “Solitude provides a situation that
is suited to deep absorption”(Larson, 1990,
p. 165). The identification of this first skill is
also consistent with research that has linked the
frequency and enjoyment of solitude experiences
with a high solitropic orientation, which denotes a
high desire to engage in solitary activities, rather
than with a low sociotropic orientation, which
denotes a low desire to affiliate with others (Leary
et al., 2003). Furthermore, given that the partici-
pants appeared intrinsically motivated to engage
in their chosen activities, their solitude was self-
determined, increasing the likelihood of a posi-
tive experience of solitude (Deci & Ryan, 2008;
Thomas & Azmitia, 2019).
Emotionally, these participants were skillful in
traversing the internal landscape of their feelings.
The strategies they used most closely align with
integrative emotion regulation (IER; Roth et al.,
2019) rather than the emotion regulation model
proposed by Gross (2015), in which cognitive and
behavioral strategies such as reappraisal and
selection are utilized to influence emotional
states. In contrast, the IER model views emotions
as important sources of information about one’s
inner life, and these emotions are brought into full
awareness in a nonreactive, receptive manner
akin to mindfulness and acceptance-based thera-
pies (Baer, 2003;Blackledge & Hayes, 2001;
Keng et al., 2011). The participants exhibited
this integrative approach to emotion regulation,
showing an openness to a broad range of emo-
tions and a willingness to explore what those
emotions signified about their current goals,
needs, or relationships.
Furthermore, these participants exhibited a
capacity for emotion differentiation (Kashdan
et al., 2015), defined as an ability to put into
words the various complex emotions one is
feeling. Before one can successfully regulate
an emotion, it helps to know exactly what that
emotion is. Also known as emotional granular-
ity, this ability represents an adaptive response to
stress. Furthermore, they demonstrated distress
tolerance when the emotions they felt were
uncomfortable or negatively valenced (Simons
& Gaher, 2005). Rather than fleeing solitude
when these unpleasant emotions arose, they
were able to tolerate that discomfort and explore
those emotions, after which they described feel-
ing an emotional release or renewal. These find-
ings support the speculation that negative moods
that surface during solitude may be masking
important emotional and cognitive processes
underneath and is consistent with research over
multiple adult and adolescent samples demon-
strating that moods typically lower during soli-
tude but then rebound to higher than normal
levels upon exiting solitude (Larson, 1990).
Taken together, this set of emotional capacities
served to increase participants’connection with
themselves.
It must be noted that these participants were not
limited to regulating difficult emotions; they also
reported feeling a range of positive emotions
when alone, including gratitude, joy, content-
ment, peace, and fulfillment. Their frequent re-
ferences to positive emotionality during solitude
are indicative of positive emotion regulation
(Silton et al., 2020), a process known to enhance
well-being and reduce depression. Long et al.
(2003) found that the ability to be emotionally
creative—to adapt and navigate challenging emo-
tional situations—correlated significantly with
the frequency of positive solitude experiences.
Cognitively, participants demonstrated the
skill of introspection, which involves the ability
to reflect on one’s own behavior, attitudes, be-
liefs, or place in the world. This supports previous
work showing that people use solitude for exis-
tential contemplation (Long & Averill, 2003) and
to gain a reflective perspective (Koch, 1994), in
addition to engaging in the self-reflection neces-
sary for identity development (Goossens &
Marcoen, 1999). The introspection exhibited in
these narratives often took the form of a dialecti-
cal relationship of the self-interacting with the
self, as when Audrey said, “I can hear myself.”
This is reflected in William James' (1890) concept
of the “I”and the “me,”in which the “I”repre-
sents the self as knower, containing the qualities
of self-awareness and agency, and the “me”
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
132 THOMAS
represents the self as an object of knowledge.
James argued that this internal dialogue between a
subjective self and the self as social object is
essential for maintaining a continuity of being and
ensuring congruence between our values and
behavior.
Rather than engage in ruminative self-
reflection, a behavior known to be generally
maladaptive (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000), these par-
ticipants’introspection took the form of self-
insight, a meta-cognitive process that supports
subjective well-being (Harrington & Loffredo,
2010;Stein & Grant, 2014) and plays a role in
self-regulation (Silvia & Phillips, 2014). Alto-
gether, the identification of this skill lends cre-
dence to Byrnes' (1983) speculation that to benefit
from solitude, one needs to develop an ability for
introspection in addition to being able to sur-
mount initial feelings of anxiety that may surface
when alone.
To summarize thus far, the first three skills of
enjoying solitary activities, emotion regulation,
and introspection can be viewed as the substance
of the solitude skillset. They represent abilities
that are exercised during time alone, thus creating
an experience of positive solitude, defined as rich
in meaning and enjoyment (Ost Mor et al., 2020).
Viewed as such, solitude may function less as a
happiness promoter in the hedonic sense (i.e.,
feeling good, experiencing pleasant emotions)
and more as a well-being promoter (i.e., gaining
insight, experiencing the full spectrum of emo-
tions) in the eudaimonic sense (Ryff, 1989).
The remaining five skills can be viewed as the
structural support of positive solitude in two
ways: first, in the practical sense of securing
time for solitude and being mindful to avoid
distractions during that time; second, in the psy-
chological sense of heeding internal signals to
enter and exit solitude when needed, and validat-
ing this need for solitude even in the face of
internal or external critics. The identification of
these skills extends existing literature and chal-
lenges core assumptions. For instance, knowing
when to enter and exit solitude helped each
participant create what Larson (1990) called their
“optimal balance”of social and solitary engage-
ment (p. 158) and support the theoretical argu-
ment that a balance of solitude and relationship is
a necessary ingredient for well-being in adult-
hood (Littman-Ovadia, 2019).
However, the present findings go further by
describing how this balance was achieved.
Participants were attuned to their inner signals,
such as exhaustion and irritability, that prompted
them to seek solitude and remedy the effects of
overstimulation. Likewise, they had to discern
when signals to exit solitude were trustworthy; for
example, whether loneliness and boredom were
invitations to go deeper into solitude or cues that
the company of others was indeed necessary.
Moreover, to create an optimal balance they
had to exercise the skill of making time for
solitude, and often had to negotiate with others
to obtain it. Because support for solitude was
often missing from their cultural upbringing,
participants developed these skills by trial and
error. In these ways, the skills of knowing when to
enter and exit solitude provide greater articulation
of the dynamic relationship of solitude and socia-
bility outlined in the extant literature, which has
demonstrated that when a person becomes over-
taxed by the social world, a retreat into solitude
can be rejuvenating and emotionally regulating,
with even brief periods of solitude having a
deactivating effect on high arousal states, both
positive (e.g., excitement) and negative (e.g.,
anger; Buchholz & Helbraun, 1999;Larson,
1990;Nguyen et al., 2018).
Yet, these narratives simultaneously challenge
the assumption that solitude’s function is ulti-
mately in service to our more fundamental social
nature, essentially providing a temporary haven
to withdraw and self-regulate, after which one can
rejoin society better equipped to relate positively
with others (Larson, 1990;Nguyen et al., 2018).
Rather, several participants flipped this script,
asserting that solitude was their “natural state”
or their “default”and expressed confusion during
the interview when asked what motivated them to
be alone. For instance, Scott replied: “Motiva-
tion? I would need motivation to be around
people. I would say being by myself is natural
::: And if you’re looking for some reason, like I
don’t—I don’t have a reason. It’s just kind of the
way I like to be.”His response made clear the
assumption embedded in the interview question:
namely, that one would need a motivation to be
alone, as though solitude-seeking needed to be
justified.
The centrality of solitude for these participants
is reminiscent of the recently proposed construct
of aloneliness, defined as a negative feeling state
that arises when a person perceives that they are
not spending enough time alone (Coplan et al.,
2019), as when Mika said that when she gets too
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 133
far away from solitude, “I get lonely for myself,
like I feel lonely and miss the connection to
myself when I get really busy.”Altogether, rather
than solitude serving a subsidiary role to social
interaction, these findings instead suggest that for
some individuals time alone may be equal, or
preferable, to social relationships.
Solitude Skills and the Private Self
As a whole, the solitude skills demonstrated by
these participants, and the function of self-
connection they appear to serve, find a home in
the construct of the private self, developed by
clinical psychologist Arnold Modell (1992,
1993), building on what Winnicott (1965)
described as the true self. The chief characteristics
of the private self are authenticity and an internal
coherency of being, drawing on William James'
(1890) articulation of the psychological necessity
to maintain a continuity of self. The private self is
supported by the capacity to generate meaning
from personal experience. While meanings are
also generated through social interaction and
participation in the public sphere, Modell
(1993) argued that occasional retreat from social
roles and societal obligations is necessary to make
contact with an affective core where private
thoughts, feelings, and meanings are processed
and recategorized. The private self is “the source
of autonomy”(Modell, 1992, p. 3), sustained by
the freedom to be oneself when alone and exercise
the agency to engage in “vital personal interests”
(p. 3) that are meaningful and nourishing.
These qualities of the private self are concep-
tually linked to the first three solitude skills
identified in the present study; enjoyment of
solitary activities, emotion regulation, and intro-
spection all served to deepen participants’con-
nection with themselves, or in Modell’s terms, to
sustain the private self. This theoretical lens
provides context for someone like Nora, whose
very experience of self depended on time in
solitude: “If I didn’t have solitude, I wouldn’t
have me.”The protective skills of carving out
time to be alone, using that time mindfully, and
validating one’s need for solitude all encourage
optimal conditions to make contact with the
private self. Finally, embedded in Modell’s con-
ception of the private self is a recognition of
the importance of social relationships, and he
aligns with Winnicott (1965) in arguing that
supportive attachment relationships are what
make the private self possible. The final two skills
illustrate Modell’s emphasis on the importance of
a“balance between alternate periods of related-
ness and disengagement”(1992, p. 5).
Modell argued that even when one is forced
into solitude, individuals who successfully navi-
gate that state do so because they rely on internal
resources of the private self to “sustain them-
selves from within ::: [and] find sources of
nourishment at the core of their being”(1993,
p. 126). The theoretical construct of the private
self was echoed in remarkably similar ways
throughout participant interviews, including me-
taphors of feeling nourished by time alone. It is
also consistent with Larson’s(1990) review of his
decades of empirical research: “The ability to be
alone without succumbing to panic or disturbed
means of coping confers a measure of indepen-
dence from social definitions of self. It holds open
the chance for self-renewal ::: to reflect on their
experience and make choices congruent with
personal values and feelings”(p. 177).
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions
The present findings represent an important
advance in the literature on solitude by identify-
ing a preliminary set of skills that foster the state
of positive solitude. Until now, the concept of
solitude skills was noted as a theoretical possibil-
ity but never investigated empirically (Byrnes,
1983;Galanaki, 2005;Rook, 1988;Rubenstein &
Shaver, 1982).
Given the exploratory nature of the research
question, the open-ended, semi-structured inter-
view method undertaken in the present study,
combined with a phenomenologically oriented,
inductive approach to thematic analysis, proved
to be a fruitful methodology to map the hereto-
fore uncharted territory of solitude skills. This
method facilitated an advance from theoretical
speculation about solitude skills to empirical
identification of a preliminary set of skills uti-
lized by adults in daily life and opens the door for
quantitative and mixed-methods studies to ver-
ify or amend these skills in other populations and
test their efficacy in clinical and educational
settings.
Despite the advantages of qualitative methods,
the interview does not act as a flawless conduit of a
participant’s inner world. Rather, the social interac-
tion embedded in the interviewer–participant dyad
can introduce demand characteristics and power
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
134 THOMAS
differences, leaving the participant vulnerable to
misinterpretation (see Potter & Hepburn, 2005,
for a critique of this method). Moreover, the
conversational nature of the interview with its
reliance on oral expression is potentially at odds
with the topic in question, inherently limiting
participants’ability to accurately convey the
private experience of solitude in the context of
interpersonal dialogue. Future research may want
to utilize nonoral qualitative methods, such as
diary studies or written reflections, to bypass
some of the constraints imposed by conversa-
tional interviews.
In addition, the eight skills identified here were
drawn from a select sample that is not represen-
tative of the population at large, and attempts to
generalize should be undertaken with the follow-
ing caveats in mind. First, the solitude skills
demonstrated by these adults may be develop-
mentally constrained. For example, children and
adolescents may not have the cognitive capacity
necessary for introspection, nor have the agency
necessary for carving out time to be alone. Sec-
ond, the adults in this study were employed and
well-educated; they had access to access to pri-
vate rooms and the autonomy to go places on their
own. In short, they had space and freedom. Many
people of all ages lack the ability to access space
and time for solitude, whether due to poverty,
displacement, housing situation, and untold other
reasons (Koch, 1994). Such developmental and
socio-economic factors need to be taken into
consideration when theorizing or applying from
this data set. Third, these individuals were in
solitude volitionally. There may be additional,
or different, skills required when solitude is
involuntarily, as in the case of the millions of
individuals worldwide who were isolated in their
homes due to the stay-at-home orders during the
COVID-19 pandemic. This study was conducted
prior to the onset of the pandemic, and it is
unknown how enforced social isolation may
impact the development or enactment of solitude
skills.
Despite these limitations, the present study lays
the groundwork for a more comprehensive inves-
tigation of the role these skills may play in psy-
chological well-being. As Levitt (2021) has noted,
qualitative research lends itself to generalization
to the phenomenon, rather than to the population,
and thus future research can investigate how
positive solitude, and the skills that support it,
is enacted across various contexts. Conducting
additional qualitative and mixed-methods studies
with more diverse samples can illuminate whether
and how these skills are manifested in other
populations; a nonexhaustive list of additional
variables includes age, psychological adjustment,
prior trauma, and one’s culturalcontext. Empirical
questions about the efficacy of solitude skills
could be tested using quasi-experimental or inter-
vention methods. In addition, solitude skills train-
ings could be adapted to therapeutic settings;
clinicians have already speculated that the capac-
ity for solitude could serve as a clinical interven-
tion for patients who suffer from loneliness,
depression, and poor relationships (Burke,
1992;Knafo, 2012;Young, 1982).
Finally, although individual differences such
as extraversion likely play a role in how much
solitude one prefers (Burger, 1995), the actual
skills to utilize solitude constructively may not
depend on individual differences. Research has
shown that many expert behaviors typically
attributed to innate personality characteristics
are actually activities practiced by individuals,
activities which are demonstrated and reinforced
by the larger culture. These are called repertoires
of practice (Gutiérrez & Rogoff, 2003), and
denote experiential skills that are learned and
practiced over time. Viewed as a repertoire of
practice, engaging in solitude is not the exclusive
province of individuals who “naturally”know
how to be alone. Rather, positive solitude be-
comes potentially accessible for the individual
who learns to practice and adapt various solitude
skills to meet particular needs or achieve cer-
tain ends.
Conclusion
Fostering the private self in solitude is critical
for supporting mental health and well-being
(Modell, 1993), and the findings from this study
suggest that certain skills assist individuals in
successfully navigating solitude and reaping the
benefits. The capacity to be content when alone is
a developmental achievement; indeed Winnicott,
in his landmark paper on the subject, called it “one
of the most important signs of maturity in emo-
tional development”(Winnicott, 1958, p. 416).
The solitude skills identified in the present
study may represent some of the psychological
resources necessary to sustain an authentic, pri-
vate self, with the added implication that they are
valuable not only for those who volitionally enter
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 135
solitude, but also for those who find themselves in
unwanted isolation. Applying these skills could
position individuals to leverage their time alone
for the purposes of self-regulation and self-
reflection, increasing the likelihood of experienc-
ing positive solitude. Whether this preliminary set
of solitude skills can be found in populations
beyond the study sample, whether these skills
can be learned, and whether the practice of such
skills can enhance psychological functioning are
all empirical questions worth investigating.
Doing so would further our understanding of
the role positive solitude has in adjustment and
well-being.
References
Alden, L. E., & Auyeung, K. W. (2014). Social
anxiety disorder and emotional solitude. In R.
Coplan & J. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook of
solitude: Psychological perspectives on social iso-
lation, social withdrawal, and being alone
(pp. 391–408). Wiley Blackwell.
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as clinical
intervention: A conceptual and empirical review.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,10(2),
125–143. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpg015
Blackledge, J. T., & Hayes, S. C. (2001). Emotion
regulation in acceptance and commitment therapy.
Journal of Clinical Psychology,57(2), 243–255.
https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(200102)57:2<243::
AID-JCLP9>3.0.CO;2-X
Bowker, J. C., Nelson, L. J., Markovic, A., & Luster,
S. (2014). Social withdrawal during adolescence
and emerging adulthood. In R. Coplan & J. Bowker
(Eds.), The handbook of solitude: Psychological
perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal,
and being alone (pp. 167–183). Wiley.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-Child attach-
ment and healthy human development. Basic Books.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic
analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology,3(2), 77–101. https://doi.org/10.1191/
1478088706qp063oa
Braun, V., Clarke, V., Hayfield, N., & Terry, G. (2019).
Thematic analysis. In P. Liamputtong (Ed.), Hand-
book of research methods in health social sciences
(pp. 843–860). Springer Nature Singapore. https://
doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5251-4_103
Buchholz, E., & Helbraun, E. (1999). A psychobio-
logical developmental model for an “alonetime”
need in infancy. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic.
63(2), 143–158.
Burger, J. (1995). Individual differences in preference for
solitude. Journal of Research in Personality,29(1),
85–108. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1995.1005
Burke, N. (1992). College psychotherapy and the
development of a capacity for solitude. Journal
of College Student Psychotherapy,6(2), 59–86.
https://doi.org/10.1300/J035v06n02_08
Burnell, K., George, M. J., & Underwood, M. K.
(2021). New media and solitude: Implications for
peer relations. In R. J. Coplan, J. C. Bowker, & L. J.
Nelson (Eds.), The handbook of solitude: Psycholog-
ical perspectives on social isolation, social with-
drawal, and being alone (2nd ed., pp. 254–267).
Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119576457.ch18
Byrnes, D. A. (1983). Life skills in solitude and silence
in the school. Education,104(1), 96–99.
Coplan, R. J., & Armer, M. (2007). A “multitude”of
solitude: A closer look at social withdrawal and
nonsocial play in early childhood. Child Develop-
ment Perspective: Society for Research in Child
Development,1(1), 26–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1750-8606.2007.00006.x
Coplan, R. J., Hipson, W. E., Archbell, K. A., Ooi,
L. L., Baldwin, D., & Bowker, J. C. (2019). Seeking
more solitude: Conceptualization, assessment, and
implications of aloneliness. Personality and Indi-
vidual Differences,148,17–26. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.paid.2019.05.020
Dahlberg, L., Agahi, N., & Lennartsson, C. (2018).
Lonelier than ever? Loneliness of older people over
two decades. Archives of Gerontology and Geria-
trics,75,96–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger
.2017.11.004
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination
theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, devel-
opment, and health. Canadian Psychology,49(3),
182–185. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012801
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., & Pachan, M. (2010).
A meta-analysis of after-school programs that seek
to promote personal and social skills in children and
adolescents. American Journal of Community Psy-
chology,45, 294–309. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10464-010-9300-6
Erikson, J. (1997). The life cycle completed. Norton.
Franzoi, S. L., & Brewer, L. C. (1984). The experience
of self-awareness and its relation to level of self-
consciousness: An experiential sampling study.
Journal of Research in Personality,18(4), 522–
540. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(84)90010-2
Galanaki, E. (2005). Solitude in the school: A ne-
glected facet of children’s development and educa-
tion. Childhood Education,81(3), 128–132. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2005.10522255
Galanaki, E. (2013). Solitude in children and adoles-
cents: A review of the research literature. Psychol-
ogy and Education: An Interdisciplinary Journal.
Special Issue: Loneliness and Solitude in Children
and Adolescents,50(3–4), 79–88.
Gazelle, H., & Druhen, M. J. (2009). Anxious solitude
and peer exclusion predict social helplessness,
upset affect, and vagal regulation in response to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
136 THOMAS
behavioral rejection by a friend. Developmental
Psychology,45(4), 1077–1096. https://doi.org/10
.1037/a0016165
Goossens, L. (2014). Affinity for aloneness in adoles-
cence and preference for solitude in childhood. In
R. Coplan & J. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook of
solitude: Psychological perspectives on social iso-
lation, social withdrawal, and being alone
(pp. 150–166). Wiley.
Goossens, L., & Marcoen, A. (1999). Adolescent
loneliness, self-reflection, and identity: From
individual differences to developmental processes.
In K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness
in childhood and adolescence (pp. 225–243).
Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10
.1017/CBO9780511551888.011
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status
and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry,26(1), 1–
26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Competing
paradigms in qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin
& Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of Qualitative
Research (pp. 105–117). Sage Publications.
Gutiérrez, K. D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways
of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of prac-
tice. Educational Researcher,32(5), 19–25. https://
doi.org/10.3102/0013189X032005019
Harrington, R., & Loffredo, D. A. (2010). Insight,
rumination, and self-reflection as predictors of well-
being. The Journal of Psychology,145(1), 39–57.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2010.528072
Hatano, G., & Oura, Y. (2003). Commentary: Recon-
ceptualizing school learning using insight from exper-
tise research. Educational Researcher,32(8), 26–29.
https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X032008026
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness
matters: A theoretical and empirical review of con-
sequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral
Medicine,40(2), 218–227. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12160-010-9210-8
Hays, R. D., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1987). A short-form
measure of loneliness. Journal of Personality
Assessment,51(1), 69–81. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15327752jpa5101_6
Hollis, C., Livingstone, S., & Sonuga-Barke, E.
(2020). Editorial: The role of digital technology
in children and young people’s mental health—A
triple-edged sword? Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,61(8), 837–
841. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13302
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology,
volume 1. In Frederick Burkhardt (Ed.). Harvard
University Press, 1981.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A.,
Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method
for characterizing daily life experience: The day
reconstruction method. Science,306(5702), 1776–
1780. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572
Kashdan, T. B., Feldman Barrett, L., & McKnight,
P. E. (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation:
Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving
distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in
Psychological Science,24(1), 10–16. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550708
Kaufman, J. C., Gentile, C. A., & Baer, J. (2005). Do
gifted student writers and creative writing experts
rate creativity the same way? Gifted Child
Quarterly,49(3), 260–265. https://doi.org/10
.1177/001698620504900307
Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011).
Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A
review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology
Review,31(6), 1041–1056. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.cpr.2011.04.006
Kim, J.-H. (2021). Getting even lonelier? Psychologi-
cal well-being and problematic use of media in the
over-connected society. In R. J. Coplan, J. C.
Bowker, & L. J. Nelson (Eds.), The Handbook of
solitude: Psychological perspectives on social
isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone
(2nd ed., pp. 240–253). Wiley. https://doi.org/10
.1002/9781119576457.ch17
Klinenberg, E. (2012). Going solo: The extraordinary
rise and surprising appeal of living alone. Penguin.
Knafo, D. (2012). Alone together: Solitude and the
creative encounter in art and psychoanalysis. Psy-
choanalytic Dialogues: The International Journal
of Relational Perspectives,22(1), 54–71. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10481885.2012.646605
Koch, P. (1994). Solitude: A philosophical encounter.
Open Court Publishing.
Larson, R. (1990). The solitary side of life: An exami-
nation of the time people spend alone from childhood
to old age. Developmental Review,10(2), 155–183.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(90)90008-R
Larson, R., & Lee, M. (1996). The capacity to be alone
as a stress buffer. The Journal of Social Psychology,
136(1), 5–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545
.1996.9923024
Lay, J. C., Pauly, T., Graf, P., Biesanz, J. C., &
Hoppmann, C. A. (2019). By myself and liking
it? Predictors of distinct types of solitude experi-
ences in daily life. Journal of Personality,87(3),
633–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12421
Lay, J. C., Pauly, T., Graf, P., Mahmood, A., &
Hoppmann, C. A. (2020). Choosing solitude: Age
differences in situational and affective correlates of
solitude-seeking in midlife and older adulthood.
The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychologi-
cal Sciences and Social Sciences,75(3), 483–493.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby044
Leary, M. R., Herbst, K. C., & McCrary, F. (2003).
Finding pleasure in solitary activities: Desire for
aloneness or disinterest in social contact? Person-
ality and Individual Differences,35(1), 59–68.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00141-1
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SOLITUDE SKILLS 137
Leung, L. (2015). Using tablet in solitude for stress
reduction: An examination of desire for aloneness,
leisure boredom, tablet activities, and location of
use. Computers in Human Behavior,48, 382–391.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.068
Levitt, H. M. (2021). Qualitative generalization, not to
the population but to the phenomenon: Reconcep-
tualizing variation in qualitative research. Qualita-
tive Psychology,8(1), 95–110. https://doi.org/10
.1037/qup0000184
Littman-Ovadia, H. (2019). Doing-being and relation-
ship-solitude: A proposed model for a balanced life.
Journal of Happiness Studies,20, 1953–1971.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0018-8
Long, C., & Averill, J. (2003). Solitude: An explora-
tion of the benefits of being alone. Journal for the
Theory of Social Behaviour,33(1), 21–44. https://
doi.org/10.1111/1468-5914.00204
Long, C. R., Seburn, M., Averill, J. R., & More, T. A.
(2003). Solitude experiences: Varieties, settings,
and individual differences. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,29(5), 578–583. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029005003
Luchetti, M., Lee, J. H., Aschwanden, D., Sesker, A.,
Strickhouser, J. E., Terracciano, A., & Sutin, A. R.
(2020). The trajectory of loneliness in response to
COVID-19. American Psychologist,75(7), 897–
908. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000690
Maes, M., Van den Noortgate, W., & Goossens, L.
(2015). A reliability generalization study for a
multidimensional loneliness scale: The loneliness
and aloneness scale for children and adolescents.
European Journal of Psychological Assessment,
31(4), 294–301. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-
5759/a000237
Manalastas, E. J. (2011). An exercise to teach the
psychological benefits of solitude: The date with
the self. Philippine Journal of Psychology,44(1),
95–106. http://www.ejournals.ph/form/cite.php?
id=3945
Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1993). Loneliness,
attitude towards aloneness, and solitude: Age dif-
ferences and developmental significance during
adolescence. In S. Jackson & H. Rodriguez-Tome
(Eds.), Adolescence and its Social Worlds (pp. 197–
227). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Modell, A. H. (1992). The private self and private space.
In Chicago Institute for Psychoanalysis (Eds.), The
Annual of Psychoanalysis (Vol. 20, pp. 1–14). The
Analytic Press.
Modell, A. H. (1993). The private self. Harvard
University Press.
Nguyen, T. T., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2018).
Solitude as an approach to affective self-regulation.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,44(1),
92–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217733073
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in
depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive
symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
109(3), 504–511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-
843X.109.3.504
Ost Mor, S., Palgi, Y., & Segel-Karpas, D. (2020). The
definition and categories of positive solitude: Older
and younger adults’perspectives on spending time
by themselves. International Journal of Aging &
Human Development. Advance online publication.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0091415020957379
Potter, J., & Hepburn, A. (2005). Qualitative inter-
views in psychology: Problems and possibilities.
Research in Qualitative Psychology,2(4), 281–307.
https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088705qp045oa
Puskar, K., Sereika, S., & Tusaie-Mumford, K. (2007).
Effect of the Teaching Kids to Cope (TKC) program
on outcomes of depression and coping among rural
adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psy-
chiatric Nursing,16(2), 71–80. https://doi.org/10
.1111/j.1744-6171.2003.tb00350.x
Rook, K. S. (1988). Toward a more differentiated view
of loneliness. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of per-
sonal relationships (pp. 571–589). Wiley.
Rook, K. S., & Peplau, L. A. (1982). Perspectives on
helping the lonely. In L. Peplau & D. Perlman
(Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory,
research and therapy (pp. 351–378). Wiley.
Roth, G., Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2019).
Integrative emotion regulation: Process and develop-
ment from a self-determination theory perspective.
Development and Psychopathology,31, 945–956.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000403
Rubenstein, C., & Shaver, P. (1982). In search of
intimacy. Delacorte Press.
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., & Bowker, J. C. (2009).
Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of
Psychology,60, 141–171. https://doi.org/10.1146/
annurev.psych.60.110707.163642
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it?
Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy,57(6), 1069–1081. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.57.6.1069
Silton, R. L., Kahrilas, I. J., Skymba, H. V., Smith, J.,
Bryant, F. B., & Heller, W. (2020). Regulating
positive emotions: Implications for promoting
well-being in individuals with depression. Emotion,
20(1), 93–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000675
Silvia, P. J., & Phillips, A. G. (2014). Evaluating
self-reflection and insight as self-conscious traits.
Personality and Individual Differences,50(2), 234–237.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.035
Simons, J. S., & Gaher, R. M. (2005). The distress
tolerance scale: Developmentand validation of a self-
report measure. Motivation and Emotion,29(2),
83–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-005-7955-3
Stein, D., & Grant, A. M. (2014). Disentangling the
relationships among self-reflection, insight, and
subjective well-being: The role of dysfunctional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
138 THOMAS
attitudes and core self-evaluations. The Journal of
Psychology,148(5), 505–522. https://doi.org/10
.1080/00223980.2013.810128
Storr, A. (1988). Solitude: A return to the self.
Free Press.
Thomas, V., & Azmitia, M. (2019). Motivation mat-
ters: Development and validation of the Motivation
for Solitude Scale-Short Form (MSS-SF). Journal
of Adolescence,70(1), 33–42. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.adolescence.2018.11.004
Thomas, V., Balzer Carr, B., Azmitia, M., Whittaker,
S. (2021). Alone and online: Understanding the
relationships between social media, solitude, and
psychological adjustment. Psychology of Popular
Media,10(2), 201–211. https://doi.org/10.1037/
ppm0000287
Twenge, J. M., Spitzberg, B. H., & Campbell, W. K.
(2019). Less in-person social interaction with peers
among U.S. adolescents in the 21st century and
links to loneliness. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships,36(6), 1892–1913. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0265407519836170
Wang, J. M., Rubin, K. H., Laursen, B., Booth-La-
Force, C., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2013). Preference-
for-solitude and adjustment difficulties in early and
late adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology,42(6), 834–842. https://
doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.794700
Westgate, E. (2020). Why boredom is interesting. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science,29(1), 33–
40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884309
Winnicott, D. W. (1958). The capacity to be alone. The
International Journal of Psycho-Analysis,39,
416–420.
Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes
and the facilitating environment. The Hogarth Press.
Young, J. E. (1982). Loneliness, depression, and
cognitive therapy: Theory and application. In L.
Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A source-
book of current theory, research and therapy
(pp. 379–405). Wiley.
Youngblade, L. M., Berlin, L. J., & Belsky, J. (1999).
Connections among loneliness, the ability to be
alone, and peer relationships in young children. In
K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness in
Childhood and Adolescence (pp. 135–152).
Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10
.1017/CBO9780511551888.007
Received March 19, 2021
Revision received August 30, 2021
Accepted September 14, 2021 ▪
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!
Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be
available online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at https://my.apa.org/
portal/alerts/ and you will be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become
available!
SOLITUDE SKILLS 139
A preview of this full-text is provided by American Psychological Association.
Content available from Qualitative Psychology
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.