ArticlePDF Available

Effectiveness of Digital Forced-Choice Nudges for Voluntary Data Donation by Health Self-Trackers in Germany: Online Experiment

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: Health self-tracking is an evidence-based approach to optimize health and well-being for personal self-improvement through lifestyle changes. At the same time, user-generated health-related data can be of particular value for (health care) research. As longitudinal data, these data can provide evidence for developing better and new medications, diagnosing rare diseases faster, or treating chronic diseases. Objective: This quantitative study aims to investigate the impact of digital forced-choice nudges on the willingness of German health self-trackers to donate self-tracked health-related data for research. This study contributes to the body of knowledge on the effectiveness of nonmonetary incentives. Our study enables a gender-specific statement on influencing factors on the voluntary donation of personal health data and, at the same time, on the effectiveness of digital forced-choice nudges within tracking apps. Methods: We implemented a digital experiment using a web-based questionnaire by graphical manipulation of the Runtastic tracking app interface. We asked 5 groups independently to indicate their willingness to donate tracked data for research. We used a digital forced-choice nudge via a pop-up window, which framed the data donation request with 4 different counter values. We generated the counter values according to the specific target group needs identified from the research literature. Results: A sample of 919 was generated, of which, 625 (68%) were women and 294 (32%) were men. By dividing the sample into male and female participants, we take into account research on gender differences in privacy tendencies on the web and offline, showing that female participants display higher privacy concerns than male participants. A statistical group comparison shows that with a small effect size (r=0.21), men are significantly more likely (P=.04) to donate their self-tracked data for research if the need to take on social responsibility is addressed (the prosocial counter value in this case-contributing to society) compared with the control group without counter value. Selfish or pseudoprosocial counter values had no significant effect on willingness to donate health data among male or female health self-trackers in Germany when presented as a forced-choice nudge within a tracking app. Conclusions: Although surveys regularly reveal an 80% to 95% willingness to donate data on average in the population, our results show that only 41% (377/919) of the health self-trackers would donate their self-collected health data to research. Although selfish motives do not significantly influence willingness to donate, linking data donation to added societal value could significantly increase the likelihood of donating among male self-trackers by 15.5%. Thus, addressing the need to contribute to society promotes the willingness to donate data among male health self-trackers. The implementation of forced-choice framing nudges within tracking apps presented in a pop-up window can add to the accessibility of user-generated health-related data for research.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Original Paper
Effectiveness of Digital Forced-Choice Nudges for Voluntary Data
Donation by Health Self-trackers in Germany: Web-Based
Experiment
Katharina Pilgrim, PhD; Sabine Bohnet-Joschko, Prof Dr
Department of Management and Entrepreneurship, Faculty of Management, Economics and Society, Witten Herdecke University, Witten, Germany
Corresponding Author:
Katharina Pilgrim, PhD
Department of Management and Entrepreneurship
Faculty of Management, Economics and Society
Witten Herdecke University
Alfred-Herrhausen-Str 50
Witten, 58455
Germany
Phone: 49 2302926475
Email: katharina.pilgrim@uni-wh.de
Abstract
Background: Health self-tracking is an evidence-based approach to optimize health and well-being for personal self-improvement
through lifestyle changes. At the same time, user-generated health-related data can be of particular value for (health care) research.
As longitudinal data, these data can provide evidence for developing better and new medications, diagnosing rare diseases faster,
or treating chronic diseases.
Objective: This quantitative study aims to investigate the impact of digital forced-choice nudges on the willingness of German
health self-trackers to donate self-tracked health-related data for research. This study contributes to the body of knowledge on
the effectiveness of nonmonetary incentives. Our study enables a gender-specific statement on influencing factors on the voluntary
donation of personal health data and, at the same time, on the effectiveness of digital forced-choice nudges within tracking apps.
Methods: We implemented a digital experiment using a web-based questionnaire by graphical manipulation of the Runtastic
tracking app interface. We asked 5 groups independently to indicate their willingness to donate tracked data for research. We
used a digital forced-choice nudge via a pop-up window, which framed the data donation request with 4 different counter values.
We generated the counter values according to the specific target group needs identified from the research literature.
Results: A sample of 919 was generated, of which, 625 (68%) were women and 294 (32%) were men. By dividing the sample
into male and female participants, we take into account research on gender differences in privacy tendencies on the web and
offline, showing that female participants display higher privacy concerns than male participants. A statistical group comparison
shows that with a small effect size (r=0.21), men are significantly more likely (P=.04) to donate their self-tracked data for research
if the need to take on social responsibility is addressed (the prosocial counter value in this case—contributing to society) compared
with the control group without counter value. Selfish or pseudoprosocial counter values had no significant effect on willingness
to donate health data among male or female health self-trackers in Germany when presented as a forced-choice nudge within a
tracking app.
Conclusions: Although surveys regularly reveal an 80% to 95% willingness to donate data on average in the population, our
results show that only 41% (377/919) of the health self-trackers would donate their self-collected health data to research. Although
selfish motives do not significantly influence willingness to donate, linking data donation to added societal value could significantly
increase the likelihood of donating among male self-trackers by 15.5%. Thus, addressing the need to contribute to society promotes
the willingness to donate data among male health self-trackers. The implementation of forced-choice framing nudges within
tracking apps presented in a pop-up window can add to the accessibility of user-generated health-related data for research.
(J Med Internet Res 2022;24(2):e31363) doi: 10.2196/31363
KEYWORDS
quantified self; health self-tracking; digital nudge; data donation; health data; mobile phone
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 1https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
Introduction
Self-tracked Health Data for Research
As of April 2020, more than half a million enthusiastic health
self-trackers donated collected data to the federal government
research institute responsible for disease control and prevention
in Germany, via fitness wristbands or smartwatches [1]. As of
February 2021, data records donated have exceeded 170 million
sets, including pulse, blood pressure, weight, temperature,
physical activity, and information on sleep cycles. Regarding
the COVID-19 pandemic, heart rate measurement and physical
activity are of particular interest because an accelerated pulse
while decreasing activity is a likely fever indicator—a typical
COVID-19 symptom. At best, this fever monitor will serve as
an early warning system and predict the spread of coronavirus
in Germany even before case numbers from public health
authorities are available [2].
The approach described is only one instance of how
user-generated health-related data could add value to research
and support public health measures [3]. Moreover, this kind of
real-world data can support the development of pharmaceutical
innovation, accelerate rare disease diagnosis, and improve
chronic disease treatment [4-10]. For the self-tracked data to be
available for research, users have to share, more precisely,
donate their data actively to research institutions [11,12]. In
addition to observing regulatory, ethical, and privacy issues, it
is crucial to know, understand, and evaluate the motives of a
specific target group for data donation [13,14]. Research on
data donation typically uses a population cross-section sample.
Surveys indicate that 4 of 5 Germans are willing to donate their
digital health data anonymously and free of charge for medical
research [15]. As many as 95% of German and US social media
users would specifically donate their data to scientists at
universities and public research institutions [15,16]. The results
can potentially be highly biased, as individuals may
hypothetically signal their willingness to donate data and
perhaps not even engage in health self-tracking at all. Thus, on
the one hand, research lacks information on the willingness to
donate self-tracked health-related data among those who actually
engage in health self-tracking for personal reasons and with
individual goals. On the other hand, we need to determine
whether there are nonmonetary benefits for that target group
that can possibly influence the willingness to donate data
positively.
Research Question and Objectives
We argue that digital forced-choice nudging in general is an
appropriate tool to introduce behavioral change toward donating
personal health-related data among health self-trackers because
health self-trackers are very likely to share their self-tracked
data with third parties and primarily have other intentions for
tracking. Thus, data donation for research might be perceived
as rather unimportant and therefore present an appropriate field
for effective nudging.
This quantitative study aims to investigate whether a digital
forced-choice nudge can influence the willingness of German
health self-trackers to donate self-tracked health-related data
for research. We want to contribute to the body of knowledge
on the effectiveness of nonmonetary incentives derived from
the known needs of the target population. For this, we also
consider research on gender differences in privacy tendencies
on the web and offline. This shows that women display higher
privacy concerns than men. This is in line with both evolutionary
and social role theories. Therefore, we analyzed male and female
health self-trackers separately [17,18]. Our study thus enables
a gender-specific statement on influencing factors on the
voluntary donation of personal health data and, at the same time,
on the effectiveness of digital nudges for health self-trackers in
Germany.
Background and Theory
Nudging
When discussing the active decision to donate self-collected
health data and possible contributing factors, we already know
that various psychological effects influence individuals,
consciously or unconsciously, during their decision-making
process [19,20]. People often act impulsively, emotionally, or
simply out of habit [21]; they are not always able to calculate
the expected consequences of given options and therefore choose
the seemingly best available one [22].
In this context, nudges are tools for influencing behavior in
decision-making processes without resorting to prohibition,
commandments, or economic incentives [19,23,24]. Thus, a
nudge is a nonregulatory approach that attempts to motivate
individual behavior change through subtle alterations in the
choice environments that people face [19,25], whereas a
suggested benefit is embedded in the decision-making process
[26]. Thus, Karlsen and Andersen [27] define nudging as a term
for influencing decisions and behavior using suggestions,
positive reinforcement, and other noncoercive means. Löfgren
and Nordblom [28] show that the likelihood of a nudge having
an effect is higher for choices that the individual perceives as
rather unimportant and at a moment in time with limited
attention. In summary, the behavior of our study group, health
self-trackers in Germany, can potentially be influenced by a
nudge that promises a benefit based on existing target group
needs and is implemented at a point of limited attention.
Nudges in Health Systems
Nudges are widely applied in health systems, with the ultimate
goal of a healthy population. According to Holland et al [29],
most nudging measures aim at a healthier diet, more exercise,
and the reduction of alcohol and tobacco consumption and are
thus frequently applied in preventive interventions. Especially
in the nutrition field, we find a body of evidence for the
effectiveness of nudging interventions: the use of nudges such
as food traffic lights, the prominent placement of healthy food
alternatives, or the transparent display of calories increases the
choice of the healthier option by an average of 16.3% in test
participants [30]. Okeke et al [31] investigated the impact of
haptic (digital detox) nudging (phone vibration) to reduce the
time spent on the web to improve users’well-being, successfully
reducing daily screen time by over 20%. The growing body of
evidence on nudging is also increasingly attracting the attention
of health care insurance providers. They can potentially realize
considerable savings by encouraging their insureds toward
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 2https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
targeted (healthier) behavior changes while offering the prospect
of bonuses in return.
Digital Nudges
With the extensive use of digital devices, decision-making in
digital choice environments (so-called digital nudging) has
emerged, especially in the field of e-commerce [27,32]. Digital
nudges affect value cocreation by (1) widening resource
accessibility, (2) extending engagement, or (3) augmenting
human actors’ agency [33]. Similar to offline nudges, ethical
considerations for digital nudges are already discussed in the
literature, focusing on topics such as preserving individuals’
freedom of choice or autonomy, transparent disclosure of
nudges, and individual (proself) and societal (prosocial)
goal-oriented justification of nudging [34]. Mirsch et al [32]
pointed out that digital nudging in general is a promising
research area with great potential for improvement and
opportunities, especially regarding user interface, user
experience, and digital service design questions. The future
potential is supported by findings from the study by Hummel
and Maedche [35], who discovered that today, only 62% of
digital nudging treatments are statistically significant. Regarding
the effect size of digital nudges, a quantitative review showed
no difference (median effect size of 21% depending on category
and context) compared with offline settings while offering new
perspectives of individualization [35]. However, research on
smart digital nudges in the health care or data donation field is
limited. Meske et al [36], for example, conclude that digital
nudging in hospitals can positively influence the use of
technology, new value creation, changes in structures, and
consequently financial dimensions of digital transformation,
supporting caregivers as well as caretakers [36]. Regarding
charity program participation, forced-choice nudges are found
to be the most efficient ones [37,38]; a review reveals that
overall default nudges are most effective, and precommitment
strategies are least effective [35].
Hypotheses Development
Overview
To test the effectiveness of digital forced-choice nudges for data
donation among health self-trackers in Germany, we need to
identify the needs of our test group to use the right triggers for
behavior change—in our experiment, donate tracked data. The
prospect of need satisfaction could be a potentially attractive
reward, which might encourage self-trackers to donate their
data in return. To this end, we will derive potentially attractive
counter values (benefits) for framing in a digital forced-choice
nudge based on the known prevailing motives and needs of our
target group and for data donation in general.
Need of Achievement and Power by Self-expertization
People with a diagnosed disease predominantly track vital signs
or biological parameters [39-42]. Intrinsic motivation to improve
disease management by advancing personal disease knowledge
and controlling health indicators (such as glucose or blood
pressure levels) is key for tracking [39-42]. Goals are controlling
symptoms and preventing or delaying disease progression.
People with self-perceived disease risk factors mainly track
their dietary and physical activities. Motives include potential
and subjectively perceived risk prevention (such as obesity) or
the desire to learn and promote a healthier lifestyle [43,44].
People without a diagnosed or self-perceived disease or
prevalence primarily track their nutrition and exercise
parameters. Self-design by performance optimization and
monitoring performance progress improvement is the motivation
behind [45-47]. A less relevant motive is self-entertainment,
which includes natural curiosity, a basic interest, and fun in data
collection and visualization [46,48,49].
Health self-trackers thus collectively possess a desire to use
digital technologies to optimize health and well-being via
self-monitoring [50,51]. Self-motivation, self-discipline, or the
desire for performance enhancement are motives found in every
user group [52,53] and can be labeled as self-expertization [54].
According to McClelland, these motives arise from the need
for achievement and power (over a disease or one’s own body)
[55-59]. As self-expertise is key in every subpopulation with
no regard to personal medical conditions, our first hypothesis
is as follows:
H1A: The prospect of receiving individualized tips to
improve one’s health has a positive influence on female
health self-trackers’ willingness to donate personal
self-collected health-related data for research.
H1B: The prospect of receiving individualized tips to
improve one’s health has a positive influence on male health
self-trackers’willingness to donate personal self-collected
health-related data for research.
Need of Self-actualization by Contributing to Society
Research on the motives to donate personal data, for example,
to charity, is consistent with findings on motives supporting
prosocial behavior, such as blood donation [60-63]. Donations
can positively impact self-image and sense of self, as the donor
receives appreciation and care in return [64]. Thus, individual
needs as well as the need for self-actualization are also satisfied
in this context [65]. Kalkman et al [14] pointed out that although
participants recognized the actual or potential benefits of data
donation for research, they expressed concerns about
confidentiality and data abuse [14]. Nonetheless, 2 positive
influences on the willingness to donate personal data exist:
social responsibility or sense of duty is the first and most
influential factor [66]. It refers to indirect reciprocity: giving
back to the community and expecting the same treatment in
return [67,68]. This altruistic motive is predominantly based on
perceived empathy—the willingness to help out of compassion
[69].
Second, an individual’s perception of the significance of their
own contributions to the community is crucial. Nudging
intervention can realize this by emphasizing benefits, such as
potentially accelerating the cure of disease by valid research
findings and improved therapeutic interventions [16,70].
Derived from identified positive influences on data donation in
general and underlying needs, our second hypothesis is as
follows:
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 3https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
H2A: The prospect of contributing to the community has
a positive influence on female health self-trackers’
willingness to donate personal self-collected health-related
data for research.
H2B: The prospect of contributing to the community has a
positive influence on male health self-trackers’willingness
to donate personal self-collected health-related data for
research.
Need of Recognition and Social Belonging
According to Gimpel et al [46], motives for sharing self-tracked
data, such as diet- and exercise-related or vital and biological
parameters, can be referred to as self-association. Cost-benefit
trade-offs regarding sharing tracked data are strongly linked to
situationally perceived experience [71]. On the basis of the
social exchange theory by Homan, users unconsciously or
consciously weigh the costs or disadvantages of disclosure
against personally perceived advantages [72]. Prospects of
satisfying feelings of belonging (to a community) and
identification with personalized and individual data outweigh
possible disadvantages, for example, receiving personalized
advertisements or privacy concerns [46]. Self-association can
be traced back to social and individual needs satisfaction
according to Maslow and Kruntorad [65], as individuals feel
an urge for recognition and belonging and, based on this, possess
a desire for esteem and prestige. We derive hypotheses 3 and 4
according to the identified motives for personal health-related
data sharing as follows:
H3A: The prospect of recognition by the peer group
(classification of personal tracking results) has a positive
influence on female health self-trackers’ willingness to
donate personal self-collected health-related data for
research.
H3B: The prospect of recognition by the peer group
(classification of personal tracking results) has a positive
influence on male health self-trackers’willingness to donate
personal self-collected health-related data for research.
H4A: The prospect of belonging to a peer group (via
personal donation activity) has a positive influence on
female health self-trackers’willingness to donate personal
self-collected health-related data for research.
H4B: The prospect of belonging to a peer group (via
personal donation activity) has a positive influence on male
health self-trackers’ willingness to donate personal
self-collected health-related data for research.
Methods
Study and Questionnaire Design
To test hypotheses 1 to 4 using digital forced-choice nudges,
we set up a web-based experiment with the questionnaire tool
LimeSurvey for health self-trackers in Germany. We generated
5 different mock-ups of the tracking app, Runtastic. We chose
the app for its continued popularity, familiarity across all age
groups, and duration in the market (since 2009—one of the first
apps for health self-tracking) [73-75]. These mock-ups depicted
the following situation:
the user has just tracked a run of 5.2 km with Runtastic. This
screen in particular is a characteristic of a frequently shared
status update on Facebook or Twitter [76]. It represents the
sharing of personal physical activity by predominantly
recreational athletes via social networks [76]. At this point in
the user journey within the app, users only want to see their
personal stats (and share them).
On completion, a pop-up window with call to action appeared.
The pop-up is a new built-in hurdle before reaching the desired
results. The person is in a state of physical exertion, having just
completed an intense sports session. Attention and interest in
pop-up content could be considered lower at this point [28].
Each pop-up window had a recommendation to the
user—donating the tracked data to research, followed by
information that motivates and helps him choose the suggested
behavior—1 of 4 different nudges (N1 to N4) [27]. Presented
nudges refer to hypotheses H1A to H4B. N1 is a framing nudge
with egoistic benefits, promising individual tips for data
donation based on the need for achievement and power through
self-experimentation. N2 promises a contribution to society and
is thus our prosocial (framing) nudge based on the need for
self-actualization by contributing to society. N3 promises the
comparison of one’s own results with other users (social norm
framing), as the second egoistic nudge, built on the identified
need for recognition. N4 promises joining the data-for-science
community, indicated by a badge within the app, and is thus a
pseudoprosocial nudge that also uses social norm framing
(belonging to a community). The counter value is based on the
identified needs of social belonging and the desire for prestige.
Finally, N0 is the control group, with no offered counter value.
Willingness to donate was queried by assessing the likelihood
of donating on an 11-point scale, ranging from 0% to 100%
(How likely are you to click Donate Now?). Only one of the
pop-up windows was randomly included in each questionnaire.
In addition to three sociodemographic parameters, gender, age,
and education, we added 2 items as inclusion criteria to the
questionnaire. We started by querying devices used for tracking
health-related data (smartphone, smartwatch, fitness tracker,
or none), allowing multiple answers as well as the frequency of
accessing the tracked data (daily, weekly, monthly, fever, or
never).
Recruitment
The recruitment strategy included digital social media channels,
such as Facebook, Instagram, LinkedIn, Xing, and Twitter.
Facebook groups dedicated to fitness and nutrition topics as
well as Instagram stories of female fitness microinfluencers
represented key channels. To question active health self-trackers
in Germany that engage with their tracked data, the defined
exclusion criteria were (1) if participants never used a tracking
device and (2) if participants never actively or consciously
tracked any health-related data.
Data Processing and Statistical Analysis
After collection, the data preparation included cleaning and
organizing the raw data set in Excel (Microsoft Inc). We
diligently checked for errors to eliminate incomplete
questionnaires. Data processing involved encoding text format
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 4https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
data into numeric indicator variables. Ultimately, our sample
included exclusively ordinal-scaled variables suitable for
statistical analysis in SPSS. We divided the sample into female
and male participants to examine gender-specific differences.
To perform appropriate statistical analysis for the experimental
evaluation, we checked the data distribution. In addition, we
checked for variance homogeneity within the 5 different test
groups for each gender. To validate experimental hypotheses
H1 to H4, we compared our 5 groups with each other for men
and women separately. Regarding the data set characteristics,
we applied the Kruskal-Wallis and Mann-Whitney Utests as
nonparametric tests to compare independent samples with
homogeneous variances. The Kruskal-Wallis test can evaluate
whether there is an actual effect of group affiliation in the first
step. With a subsequent post hoc test, we checked which of the
groups differed significantly. We used the Dunn-Bonferroni
test.
Results
Sample
We collected 1091 questionnaires in January and February 2021.
Following our defined exclusion criteria, we excluded 5.04%
(55/1091) of observations because of participants not using a
tracking device and 0.73% (8/1091) of observations because
they did not track any health-related data actively or consciously.
Furthermore, we removed 9.99% (109/1091) of incomplete
questionnaires. The final sample consisted of 919 participants.
Our data set was not normally distributed but left-sided
(skewness=0.087) and compressed (kurtosis=1.431; Table 1).
The sample included 68% (625/919) women and 32% (294/919)
men. Overall, 45.1% (414/919) of the participants were aged
between 18 and 34 years, 46% (423/919) of the participants
were aged between 35 and 54 years, and 8.9% (82/919) of the
participants were aged >55 years. Overall, only 0.2% (2/919)
of participants did not finish high school. Overall, 4.5% (41/919)
of the participants were still in school or high school graduates
with no additional formal education. In addition, 43.5%
(400/919) of the participants were attending or had graduated
from college, and 51.8% (476/919) of the participants were
going to or had graduated from a university. In terms of tracking
frequency, 85.4% (785/919) of the participants reported daily
tracking, 10.8% (99/919) of the participants tracked weekly,
1.4% (13/919) of the participants tracked monthly, and 2.4%
(22/919) of the participants tracked less than once per month.
We found that 60.3% (554/919) of our sample used a smartwatch
for health self-tracking. Overall, 43.2% (397/919) of the
participants used a smartphone, and 33% (303/919) of the
participants used a fitness tracker (multiple answers were
possible).
The sample (N=919) was divided into 5 test groups with N0 as
186 (20.2%; control group without nudge) and 4 different nudge
groups (N1=183, 19.9%; N2=163, 17.7%; N3=199, 21.7%; and
N4=188, 20.5%; women and men combined).
Taking gender differences in privacy concerns into account, we
split the sample into female (Table 2) and male (Table 3)
participants for further analysis.
Table 1. Data distribution.
ValuesProbability to donate (men + women)
Participants, n (%)
919 (100)Valid
0 (0)Missing
0.087 (0.081)Value, skewness (SE)
1.431 (0.161)Value, kurtosis (SE)
Table 2. Description of the female sample—divided into the 5 test groups (n=625).a
Value, mean (SD; SE; range; 95% CI)Participants, n (%)Nudge
41.18 (36.113; 3.205; 0-100; 34.84-47.52)127 (20.3)0
43.91 (31.336; 2.922; 0-100; 38.12-49.70)115 (18.4)1
47.97 (34.901; 3.213; 0-100; 41.60-54.33)118 (18.9)2
45.30 (34.574; 2.832; 0-100; 39.70-50.90)149 (23.8)3
34.83 (36.295; 3.370; 0-100; 28.15-41.50)116 (18.6)4
aTotal: mean 42.77, SD 34.879; SE 1.395; range 0-100; 95% CI 40.03-45.51.
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 5https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
Table 3. Description of the male sample—divided into the 5 test groups (n=294).a
Value, mean (SD; SE; range; 95% CI)Participants, n (%)Nudge
47.80 (36.863; 4.799; 0-100; 38.19-57.40)59 (20.1)0
48.24 (36.608; 4.439; 0-100; 39.37-57.10)68 (23.1)1
63.33 (33.439; 4.985; 0-100; 53.29-73.38)45 (15.3)2
56.20 (34.279; 4.848; 0-100; 46.46-65.94)50 (17)3
41.25 (35.759; 4.214; 0-100; 32.85-49.65)72 (26.3)4
aTotal: mean 50.10, SD 36.112; SE 2.106; range 0-100; 95% CI 45.96-54.25.
Outcomes of Web-Based Experiment: Nudging Female
Health Self-trackers
The female sample contained 625 questionnaires. Regardless
of the nudge applied, 77% (481/625) of women surveyed were
willing to donate their tracked data for research (with a
probability between 10% and 100%).
The Levene test indicates homogeneous variances for female
health self-trackers (Table 4).
Using the Kruskal-Wallis test, we examined whether the
probability of data donation is the same across the 5 sample
groups. As a result, we had to reject the nil hypothesis because
significant differences (P=.03) exist across at least 2 groups.
As a post hoc test, we applied the Dunn–Bonferroni test for
pairwise group comparison to identify the groups with
significant differences. Ultimately, we found no significant
difference in the likelihood of donating data between the control
group 0 and the 4 test groups. Accordingly, we had to reject the
hypotheses H1A, H2A, H3A, and H4A for female health
self-trackers, as none of the nudges exerted a significant positive
influence on the willingness to donate data. However, there was
a significant difference between group 2, the social nudge, and
group 4, the prosocial nudge (P=.03; Figure 1; Table 5).
Table 4. Levene test of homogeneity of variances (women; N=625).
Significance (Pvalue)Levene statistic (df)Parameters
Probability to donate (women)
.092.007 (4,620)On the basis of the mean
.261.323 (4,620)On the basis of the median
.261.323 (4,548.352)On the basis of the median and with adjusted df
.121.864 (4,620)On the basis of the trimmed mean
Figure 1. Boxplots for willingness to donate among the 5 test groups (women).
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 6https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
Table 5. Pairwise comparison of nudges (women; N=625).
Adjusted significanceb
(Pvalue)
Significance (Pvalue)Standard test
statistic
Test statistic (SE)Participants, n (%)
Sample 1-sample 2a
.99.151.43733.037 (22.986)243 (38.9)4-0
.25.032.23552.646 (23.552)231 (36.9)4-1
.09.012.59157.423 (22.161)265 (42.4)4-3
.03.0032.96769.431 (23.401)234 (37.4)4-2
.99.40.85119.609 (23.038)242 (38.7)0-1
.99.261.12824.386 (21.615)276 (44.2)0-3
.99.111.59036.394 (22.884)245 (39.2)0-2
.99.83.2154.777 (22.215)264 (42.2)1-3
.99.47.71616.785 (23.452)233 (37.3)1-2
.99.59.54412.008 (22.055)267 (42.7)3-2
aEach row tests the null hypothesis that the sample 1 and sample 2 distributions are the same. Asymptotic significances (2-sided tests) are displayed.
The significance level is P=.05.
bSignificance values have been adjusted by the Bonferroni correction for multiple tests.
The post hoc test results and plot visualization suggest a
potential significant difference between groups 1 and 4 and
between groups 3 and 2. We further applied a median-based
pairwise group comparison using the Mann–Whitney Utest.
The results indicate significant differences between groups 1
and 4 (N=231; P=.01) and between groups 3 and 4 (N=265;
P=.01).
In summary, for female health self-trackers, no offered counter
value, based on egoistic, social, or prosocial motives or needs,
has a significant positive or negative influence on the willingness
to donate tracked data for research. However, the prospect of
showing one’s donation behavior to other users has a negative
effect on willingness to donate data in direct comparison with
groups that receive a prosocial or a self-serving return for their
data donation.
Outcomes of Web-Based Experiment: Nudging Male
Health Self-trackers
Overall, 79.9% (235/294) of the male respondents would donate
their data (with a probability between 10% and 100%),
regardless of the nudge queried (3% more than female
respondents).
The Levene test indicates homogeneous variances for male
health self-trackers (Table 6).
Testing for differences among groups in terms of donation
probability also revealed a significant difference among male
participants (P=.02) using the Kruskal-Wallis test.
A pairwise group comparison also revealed a significant
difference between groups 2 (social nudge) and 4 (prosocial
nudge; P=.02; Figure 2; Table 7).
The Mann-Whitney Utest revealed significant differences
between groups 1 and 2 (P=.04), between groups 0 and 2
(P=.04), and between groups 3 and 4 (P=.02).
As with female health self-trackers, we had to reject hypotheses
H1B, H3B, and H4B for men as well. Thus, the prospect of a
self-serving benefit or displaying data donation activity, named
pseudoprosocial in our experiment, has no significant positive
or negative influence on willingness to donate.
On the other hand, the prospect of making a prosocial
contribution significantly influences the likelihood of donating
self-tracked (health) data with a small effect size (r=0.21;
Z=2.087; N=104). Accordingly, we could not reject hypothesis
H2B. Comparing means, nudging male health self-trackers in
their donating decision-making process with a prosocial nudge
would increase the willingness to donate data by 15.5%. At the
same time, for male health self-trackers, we found evidence that
open data donation significantly reduces the probability of
donation compared with a secret donation (nudge 2), as well
as compared with test groups 1 and 3, receiving a personal
benefit. In addition, a group comparison of participants receiving
personal tips and participants merely making a prosocial
contribution reveals again that prosocial reasons are superior
to selfish ones and increase the likelihood of donating data in
direct comparisons.
By dividing the sample into male and female participants, we
considered gender differences because of the known privacy
concern differences between men and women. We also split the
sample into different groups regarding age, education, devices
used for tracking health-related data, and tracking frequency
during analysis. The results show that there are no significant
increases or decreases in the willingness to donate self-tracked
health-related data when the sample is divided into the stated
groups. A correlation analysis did not show any significant
positive or negative correlations with the willingness to donate
any other variable but gender (P=.001).
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 7https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
Table 6. Levene test of homogeneity of variances (men; N=294).
Significance (Pvalue)Levene statistic (df)Parameters
Probability to donate (men)
.480.873 (4,289)On the basis of the mean
.341.127 (4,289)On the basis of the median
.341.127 (4,275.393)On the basis of the median and with adjusted df
.420.975 (4,289)On the basis of the trimmed mean
Figure 2. Boxplots for willingness to donate among the 5 test groups (men).
Table 7. Pairwise comparison of nudges (men; N=294).
Adjusted significanceb
(Pvalue)
Significance (Pvalue)Standard test
statistic
Test statistic (SE)Participants, n (%)
Sample 1-sample 2a
.99.301.04315.441 (14.806)131 (44.6)4-0
.99.221.21817.372 (14.258)140 (47.6)4-1
.22.022.29435.604 (15.522)122 (41.5)4-3
.02.0023.16150.646 (16.022)117 (39.8)4-2
.99.90.1291.932 (15.001)127 (43.2)0-1
.99.211.24420.163 (16.207)109 (37.1)0-3
.35.042.11035.205 (16.687)104 (35.4)0-2
.99.251.16118.231 (15.708)118 (40.1)1-3
.40.042.05433.274 (16.203)113 (38.4)1-2
.99.39.86815.042 (17.325)95 (32.3)3-2
aEach row tests the null hypothesis that the sample 1 and sample 2 distributions are the same. Asymptotic significances (2-sided tests) are displayed.
The significance level is P=.05.
bSignificance values have been adjusted by the Bonferroni correction for multiple tests.
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 8https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
Discussion
Principal Findings and Comparison With Previous
Work
As apps represent digital environments in which users have to
make decisions on an ongoing basis, a neutral choice
presentation is impossible from a behavioral economics point
of view. User interface design is crucial and influences users’
app interactions. Developers need to know and understand user
decisions and motives, as well as the inferable effects of design
on user decision-making, to support desired actions. Our results
show that digital nudges addressing the right user needs seem
to be an action-triggering operation. Specifically, a digital
forced-choice prosocial framing nudge, presented in a pop-up
window, can increase the willingness to donate data among
male health self-trackers by 15.5%. The results are in line with
research on the effectiveness of digital nudging in general (20%)
as well as on the effectiveness of offline nudges in health care
(16%) [30,35]. We need to further examine how the nudge itself
(framing), the presentation (pop-up window vs no pop-up
window), and timing of the digital nudge impact nudge
effectiveness. In general, however, developers and researchers
should consider digital nudges addressing the need for social
responsibility when asking for data donation within this specific
user group.
The experimental results confirm the findings of previous
research on the motives to donate personal data for the health
area. Specifically, for health self-trackers, our study confirms
the results of Skatova and Goulding [66] and Mujcic and
Leibbrandt [68], who found social responsibility or duty to be
the strongest predictor of willingness to donate personal data.
Our experiment demonstrated that the prospect of doing
something good for society positively influences willingness
to donate personal health data. Arguably, the act of donating
has a positive impact on self-image and sense of self. Unlike
blood donation, which provides immediate reciprocal value to
the donor in the form of appreciation and caring (during
donation; Schiefer [64]) by those present, our results suggest
the opposite for health data donation. The public donation of
data and its visibility to third parties, especially peers, can
potentially discourage health self-trackers from making this
donation—they prefer to do so confidentially. Belonging to an
ever more similar group within the group of self-trackers, in
this case, people who donate their health data have no positive
influence on willingness to donate data and more of a negative
influence. Showing charity, therefore, negatively influences
willingness to donate data. This finding should be taken into
account when framing a data donation plea, for example, by
explicitly referring to donor anonymity.
Compared with polls regarding willingness to donate data, our
results show a significantly lower willingness among active
health self-trackers. In contrast to population surveys, which
put a willingness to donate data at 80% or social media users’
willingness at 95% [15,16], only 41% (377/919) of surveyed
health self-trackers would be willing to donate their tracked
data to research with a probability above 50% and only 10%
(92/919) with 100% probability. This discrepancy suggests that
individuals who actively collect health-related data for a
particular purpose value it more. It is important to keep this in
mind when designing approaches to access health-related,
self-tracked data. Research surveying a population cross-section
disregards the hypothetical character of questions and answers,
and thus, results can be biased and lead to ineffective measures.
Our results also show that women, who are evolutionarily more
concerned about protecting their privacy, do not respond to any
of the nudges presented adding to the findings by Tifferet [17]
and Farinosi and Taipale [18], who found these gender
differences, especially in social media users. Regarding the
existing gender gap in clinical trials, addressed by Karp and
Reavey [77], research needs to investigate which motives, needs,
and nudges can increase access to women’s health data equally.
Limitations
The findings have a number of limitations. Owing to recruiting
primarily via social media and fitness influencers, the sample
includes a disproportionately large number of younger and
higher educated people as well as mostly female compared with
male health self-trackers (2/3 to 1/3), which can bias our results.
Thus, the sample may not represent all German health
self-trackers. Furthermore, people engaging in social media and
following a call to action from influencers on Instagram and
from peers in fitness Facebook groups (participating in the
experiment in this case) have fewer privacy concerns and are
more likely to share their health-related information with others.
We did not perform ex ante power calculations to determine
the sample size. Future research may therefore repeat the survey
with a larger sample via additional recruitment channels to
assess the reliability of our findings. In addition, the chosen
geographic focus (Germany) might have biased the results
because of cultural differences in terms of relevance and general
attitudes toward privacy [78]. Further studies could consider
international cross-cultural comparisons to verify the validity
of our findings for a global app market.
Participants’ nonreproducible or untruthful answers can also
limit the results. The reasons could be the chosen and not clearly
comprehensible scale levels or phrasing of the individual benefit.
Future experiments could also imbed nudges in a different
environment (a different app) and use an even more realistic
situation with clickable mock-ups. Our hypothetical request for
web-based health data donation may not represent reality. Future
experiments could implement a real data donation tool within
a popular health self-tracking app to verify our results.
Conclusions
The growing trend toward digital health and increasing
acceptance as well as the use of health apps such as fitness
trackers, digital check-ups, and nutrition apps will contribute
to a significant increase in nudging measures. This study could
aid access to health data for research and long-term care
improvement.
Although selfish motives do not significantly influence
willingness to donate, linking data donation to added societal
value could significantly increase the likelihood of donating
among male self-trackers by 15.5%. Thus, addressing the need
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 9https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
to contribute to society promotes willingness to donate data
among male health self-trackers and should be emphasized when
designing campaigns to donate health data. The implementation
of forced-choice framing nudges within tracking apps presented
in a pop-up window can add to the accessibility of
user-generated health-related data for research.
Acknowledgments
The study was conducted as part of the ATLAS project, which is funded by the Ministry of Economic Affairs, Innovation,
Digitalization, and Energy of North Rhine-Westphalia (funding code: ITG-1-1).
Authors' Contributions
KP conducted the literature research, conducted the quantitative study, and analyzed and discussed the data. SB-J provided content
support for the process and reviewed the results. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
References
1. Amft O, Lopera Gonzalez LI, Lukowicz P, Bian S, Burggraf P. Wearables to fight COVID-19: from symptom tracking to
contact tracing. IEEE Pervasive Comput 2020;19(4):53-60. [doi: 10.1109/mprv.2020.3021321]
2. Corona data donation app 2.0: donate data and answer surveys - your contribution against Corona. Robert Koch-Institut.
2021. URL: https://www.rki.de/DE/Content/InfAZ/N/Neuartiges_Coronavirus/Corona-Datenspende-allgemein.html
[accessed 2020-12-04]
3. Kakkanatt C, Benigno M, Jackson VM, Huang PL, Ng K. Curating and integrating user-generated health data from multiple
sources to support healthcare analytics. IBM J Res Dev 2018;62(1):2:1-2:7. [doi: 10.1147/jrd.2017.2756742]
4. Shaw DM, Gross JV, Erren TC. Data donation after death: a proposal to prevent the waste of medical research data. EMBO
Rep 2016;17(1):14-17 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.15252/embr.201541802] [Medline: 26658488]
5. Strotbaum V, Pobiruchin M, Schreiweis B, Wiesner M, Strahwald B. Your data is gold - data donation for better healthcare?
Inf Technol 2019;61(5-6):219-229. [doi: 10.1515/itit-2019-0024]
6. Gay V, Leijdekkers P. Bringing health and fitness data together for connected health care: mobile apps as enablers of
interoperability. J Med Internet Res 2015;17(11):e260 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.2196/jmir.5094] [Medline: 26581920]
7. Sharon T. Self-tracking for health and the quantified self: re-articulating autonomy, solidarity, and authenticity in an age
of personalized healthcare. Philos Technol 2017;30(1):93-121. [doi: 10.1007/s13347-016-0215-5]
8. Niknejad N, Ismail WB, Mardani A, Liao H, Ghani I. A comprehensive overview of smart wearables: the state of the art
literature, recent advances, and future challenges. Eng Appl Artif Intell 2020;90:103529. [doi:
10.1016/j.engappai.2020.103529]
9. Lupton D. Data mattering and self-tracking: what can personal data do? Continuum 2020;34(1):1-13. [doi:
10.1080/10304312.2019.1691149]
10. Lupton D. Self-tracking modes: reflexive self-monitoring and data practices. SSRN J 2014:1-19. [doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2483549]
11. Bietz MJ, Bloss CS, Calvert S, Godino JG, Gregory J, Claffey MP, et al. Opportunities and challenges in the use of personal
health data for health research. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2016;23(e1):e42-e48 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.1093/jamia/ocv118]
[Medline: 26335984]
12. Chen J, Bauman A, Allman-Farinelli M. A study to determine the most popular lifestyle smartphone applications and
willingness of the public to share their personal data for health research. Telemed J E Health 2016;22(8):655-665. [doi:
10.1089/tmj.2015.0159] [Medline: 26958742]
13. Bietz M, Patrick K, Bloss C. Data donation as a model for citizen science health research. Citiz Sci Theory Pract 2019;4(1):6.
[doi: 10.5334/cstp.178]
14. Kalkman S, van Delden J, Banerjee A, Tyl B, Mostert M, van Thiel G. Patients' and public views and attitudes towards the
sharing of health data for research: a narrative review of the empirical evidence. J Med Ethics 2022;48(1):3-13 [FREE Full
text] [doi: 10.1136/medethics-2019-105651] [Medline: 31719155]
15. Semler SC. "Datenspende“ für die medizinische Forschung: ergebnisse einer aktuellen umfrage. Medizininformatik-initiative.
2019. URL: https://www.tmf-ev.de/DesktopModules/Bring2mind/DMX/Download.aspx?EntryId=32081&PortalId=0
[accessed 2021-01-07]
16. Grajales F, Clifford D, Loupos P, Okun S, Quattrone S, Simon M, et al. Social networking sites and the continuously
learning health system: a survey. NAM Perspectives 2014;4(1):1-26. [doi: 10.31478/201401d]
17. Tifferet S. Gender differences in privacy tendencies on social network sites: a meta-analysis. Comput Hum Behav
2019;93:1-12. [doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.11.046]
18. Farinosi M, Taipale S. Who can see my stuff? Online self-disclosure and gender differences on Facebook. OBS*
2018;12(1):53-71. [doi: 10.15847/obsobs12120181129]
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 10https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
19. Mirsch T, Lehrer C, Jung R. Digital nudging: altering user behavior in digital environments. In: Proceedings der 13th
Internationalen Tagung Wirtschaftsinformatik. 2017 Presented at: WI '17; February 12-15, 2017; St. Gallen.
20. Weinmann M, Schneider C, vom Brocke J. Digital nudging. Bus Inf Syst Eng 2016;58(6):433-436. [doi:
10.2139/ssrn.2708250]
21. Hechter M, Kanazawa S. Sociological rational choice theory. Annu Rev Sociol 1997;23(1):191-214. [doi:
10.1146/annurev.soc.23.1.191]
22. Müller H. Spielen hilft nicht immer. die grenzen des rational-choice-ansatzes und der platz der theorie kommunikativen
handelns in der analyse internationaler beziehungen. Zeitschrift für internationale beziehungen 1995;2(2):371-391 [FREE
Full text]
23. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. NUDGE: improving decision about health, wealth, and happiness. London: Penguin; 2009:185-198.
24. Sunstein CR. Nudging: a very short guide. J Consum Policy 2014;37(4):583-588. [doi: 10.1007/s10603-014-9273-1]
25. Oliver A. From nudging to budging: using behavioural economics to inform public sector policy. J Soc Policy
2013;42(4):685-700. [doi: 10.1017/s0047279413000299]
26. Chong D, Druckman JN. Framing theory. Annu Rev Polit Sci 2007;10(1):103-126. [doi:
10.1146/annurev.polisci.10.072805.103054]
27. Karlsen R, Andersen A. Recommendations with a nudge. Technologies 2019;7(2):45. [doi: 10.3390/technologies7020045]
28. Löfgren Å, Nordblom K. A theoretical framework of decision making explaining the mechanisms of nudging. J Econ Behav
Organ 2020;174:1-12. [doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.03.021]
29. Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, Jebb SA, Kelly MP, Nakamura R, et al. Altering micro-environments to change
population health behaviour: towards an evidence base for choice architecture interventions. BMC Public Health 2013;13:1218
[FREE Full text] [doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-1218] [Medline: 24359583]
30. Arno A, Thomas S. The efficacy of nudge theory strategies in influencing adult dietary behaviour: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. BMC Public Health 2016;16:676 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3272-x] [Medline: 27475752]
31. Okeke F, Sobolev M, Dell N, Estrin D. Good vibrations: can a digital nudge reduce digital overload? In: Proceedings of
the 20th international conference on human-computer interaction with mobile devices and services. New York: Association
for Computing Machinery; 2018 Presented at: MobileHCI '18; September 3-6, 2018; Barcelona p. 1-12. [doi:
10.1145/3229434.3229463]
32. Mirsch T, Lehrer C, Jung R. Making digital nudging applicable: the digital nudge design method. In: Proceedings of the
39th international conference on information systems.: Association for Information Systems. AIS Electronic Library
(AISeL); 2018 Presented at: ICIS '18; December 13-16, 2018; San Francisco.
33. Mele C, Russo Spena T, Kaartemo V, Marzullo ML. Smart nudging: how cognitive technologies enable choice architectures
for value co-creation. J Bus Res 2021;129:949-960. [doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.09.004]
34. Lembcke TB, Engelbrecht N, Brendel AB, Kolbe L. To nudge or not to nudge: ethical considerations of digital nudging
based on its behavioral economics roots. In: Proceedings of 27th European conference on information systems. 2019
Presented at: ECIS '19; June 8-14, 2019; Stockholm.
35. Hummel D, Maedche A. How effective is nudging? A quantitative review on the effect sizes and limits of empirical nudging
studies. J Behav Exp Econ 2019;80:47-58. [doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2019.03.005]
36. Meske C, Amojo I, Poncette AS, Balzer F. The potential role of digital nudging in the digital transformation of the healthcare
industry. In: 21st HCII: international conference on human-computer interaction. 2019 Presented at: HCII '19; July 26-31,
2019; Orlando p. 323-336. [doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-23538-3_25]
37. Meske C, Amojo I, Mohr P. Digital nudging to increase usage of charity features on e-commerce platforms. In: 15th
international conference on business informatics. 2020 Presented at: WI '20; March 9-11, 2020; Potsdam. [doi:
10.30844/wi_2020_k5-meske]
38. Liebig G, Rommel J. Active and forced choice for overcoming status quo bias: a field experiment on the adoption of “no
junk mail” stickers in Berlin, Germany. J Consum Policy 2014;37(3):423-435. [doi: 10.1007/s10603-014-9264-2]
39. Karkar R, Zia J, Vilardaga R, Mishra SR, Fogarty J, Munson SA, et al. A framework for self-experimentation in personalized
health. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2016;23(3):440-448 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.1093/jamia/ocv150] [Medline: 26644399]
40. Lee HR, Panont WR, Plattenburg B, de la Croix JP, Patharachalam D, Abowd G. Asthmon: empowering asthmatic children's
self-management with a virtual pet. In: CHI'10 extended abstracts on human factors in computing systems. 2010 Presented
at: CHI EA '10; April 10-15, 2010; Atlanta p. 3583-3588. [doi: 10.1145/1753846.1754022]
41. Walters DL, Sarela A, Fairfull A, Neighbour K, Cowen C, Stephens B, et al. A mobile phone-based care model for outpatient
cardiac rehabilitation: the care assessment platform (CAP). BMC Cardiovasc Disord 2010;10:5 [FREE Full text] [doi:
10.1186/1471-2261-10-5] [Medline: 20109196]
42. Klasnja P, Hartzler A, Powell C, Phan G, Pratt W. Health weaver mobile: designing a mobile tool for managing personal
health information during cancer care. AMIA Annu Symp Proc 2010;2010:392-396 [FREE Full text] [Medline: 21347007]
43. Miller AD, Mynatt ED. StepStream: a school-based pervasive social fitness system for everyday adolescent health. In:
Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems. New York: Association for Computing
Machinery; 2014 Presented at: CHI '14; April 16-May 1, 2014; Toronto p. 2823-2832. [doi: 10.1145/2556288.2557190]
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 11https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
44. Cordeiro F, Epstein DA, Thomaz E, Bales E, Jagannathan AK, Abowd G, et al. Barriers and negative nudges: exploring
challenges in food journaling. Proc SIGCHI Conf Hum Factor Comput Syst 2015;2015:1159-1162 [FREE Full text] [doi:
10.1145/2702123.2702155] [Medline: 26894233]
45. Choe EK, Lee NB, Lee B, Pratt W, Kientz JA. Understanding quantified-selfers' practices in collecting and exploring
personal data. In: 32nd annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems. New York: Association for
Computing Machinery; 2014 Presented at: CHI '14; April 26-May 1, 2014; Toronto p. 1143-1152. [doi:
10.1145/2556288.2557372]
46. Gimpel H, Nißen M, Görlitz R. Quantifying the quantified self: a study on the motivations of patients to track their own
health. In: Proceedings of 34th international conference on information systems. 2013 Presented at: ICIS '13; December
15-18, 2013; Milan.
47. Swan M. Sensor mania! The Internet of things, wearable computing, objective metrics, and the quantified self 2.0. J Sens
Actuator Netw 2012;1(3):217-253. [doi: 10.3390/jsan1030217]
48. Li I, Dey AK, Forlizzi J. Understanding my data, myself: supporting self-reflection with ubicomp technologies. In:
Proceedings of the 13th international conference on ubiquitous computing. New York: Association for Computing Machinery;
2011 Presented at: UbiComp '11; September 17-21, 2011; Beijing p. 405-414. [doi: 10.1145/2030112.2030166]
49. Budzinski O, Schneider S. Smart fitness: Ökonomische effekte einer digitalisierung der selbstvermessung. List Forum
2017;43:89-124. [doi: 10.1007/s41025-017-0059-5]
50. Lupton D. ‘It’s made me a lot more aware’: a new materialist analysis of health self-tracking. Media Int Aust
2019;171(1):66-79. [doi: 10.1177/1329878x19844042]
51. Fox NJ. Personal health technologies, micropolitics and resistance: a new materialist analysis. Health (London)
2017;21(2):136-153. [doi: 10.1177/1363459315590248] [Medline: 26216896]
52. Sanders R. Self-tracking in the digital era: biopower, patriarchy, and the new biometric body projects. Body Soc
2017;23(1):36-63. [doi: 10.1177/1357034x16660366]
53. Walz SP, Deterding S. Foucault's fitbit: governance and gamification. In: Walz SP, Deterding S, editors. The gameful
world: approaches, issues, applications. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2014:339-358.
54. Heyen NB. From self-tracking to self-expertise: the production of self-related knowledge by doing personal science. Public
Underst Sci 2020;29(2):124-138 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.1177/0963662519888757] [Medline: 31778095]
55. McClelland D. Achievement motivation theory. In: Miner JB, editor. Organizational behavior 1: essential theories of
motivation and leadership. Milton park: Routledge; 2005:46-60.
56. Fotopoulou A, O’Riordan K. Training to self-care: fitness tracking, biopedagogy and the healthy consumer. Health Sociol
Rev 2016;26(1):54-68. [doi: 10.1080/14461242.2016.1184582]
57. Lentferink AJ, Oldenhuis HK, de Groot M, Polstra L, Velthuijsen H, van Gemert-Pijnen JE. Key components in eHealth
interventions combining self-tracking and persuasive eCoaching to promote a healthier lifestyle: a scoping review. J Med
Internet Res 2017;19(8):e277 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.2196/jmir.7288] [Medline: 28765103]
58. Salamati F, Pasek ZJ. Personal wellness: complex and elusive product and distributed self-services. Procedia CIRP
2014;16:283-288. [doi: 10.1016/j.procir.2014.02.016]
59. Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Prevention of Mental Disorders, Mrazek PJ, Haggerty RJ. Reducing risks for
mental disorders: frontiers for preventive intervention research. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press; 1994:1-32.
60. Skatova A, Ng E, Goulding J. Data donation: Sharing personal data for public good. In: Digital economy all hands meeting
2014. 2014 Presented at: BESiDE '14; December 3-5, 2014; London. [doi: 10.13140/2.1.2567.8405]
61. Evans R, Ferguson E. Defining and measuring blood donor altruism: a theoretical approach from biology, economics and
psychology. Vox Sang 2013;106(2):118-126. [doi: 10.1111/vox.12080] [Medline: 24117697]
62. Penner LA, Dovidio JF, Piliavin JA, Schroeder DA. Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Annu Rev Psychol
2005;56:365-392. [doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070141] [Medline: 15709940]
63. de Waal FB. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu Rev Psychol 2008;59:279-300. [doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625] [Medline: 17550343]
64. Schiefer G. Motive des blutspendens: tiefenpsychologische untersuchung mit gestaltungsoptionen für das marketing von
nonprofit-organisationen des blutspendewesens. Cham: Springer; 2007:93-119.
65. Maslow AH, Kruntorad P. Motivation und persönlichkeit. Hamburg: Rowohlt; 1981:49-61.
66. Skatova A, Goulding J. Psychology of personal data donation. PLoS One 2019;14(11):e0224240 [FREE Full text] [doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0224240] [Medline: 31747408]
67. Godin G, Conner M, Sheeran P, Bélanger-Gravel A, Germain M. Determinants of repeated blood donation among new and
experienced blood donors. Transfusion 2007;47(9):1607-1615. [doi: 10.1111/j.1537-2995.2007.01331.x] [Medline:
17725724]
68. Mujcic R, Leibbrandt A. Indirect reciprocity and prosocial behaviour: evidence from a natural field experiment. Econ J
2018;128(611):1683-1699. [doi: 10.1111/ecoj.12474]
69. Titchener EB. Lectures on the experimental psychology of the thought-processes. Basingstoke: Macmillan; 1909:157-194.
70. Hummel P, Braun M, Dabrock P. Data donations as exercises of sovereignty. In: Krutzinna J, Floridi L, editors. The ethics
of medical data donation. Cham: Springer; 2019:23-54.
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 12https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
71. Liao Y. Sharing personal health information on social media: balancing self-presentation and privacy. In: Proceedings of
the 10th international conference on social media and society. New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 2019
Presented at: SMSociety '19; July 19-21, 2019; Toronto p. 194-204. [doi: 10.1145/3328529.3328560]
72. Krasnova H, Spiekermann S, Koroleva K, Hildebrand T. Online social networks: why we disclose. J Inf Technol
2010;25(2):109-125. [doi: 10.1057/jit.2010.6]
73. Fröhlich G. Die Geschichte medienbasierter Selbsttechnologien von Rousseau bis Runtastic. In: Gentzel P, Krotz F, Wimmer
J, Winter R, editors. Das vergessene Subjekt. Cham: Springer; 2019:207-226.
74. Monroy Antón A, Rodríguez Rodríguez B. Runtastic PRO app: an excellent all-rounder for logging fitness. Br J Sports
Med 2016;50(11):705-706. [doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-094940] [Medline: 26294442]
75. Ittner L, Novak F. Über den Faktor Zeit: Warum Timing alles ist – und es nur selten Sinn macht, ausschließlich auf
organisches Wachstum zu setzen. In: Business Angel Hansi Hansmann. Wiesbaden: Springer; 2018:61-66.
76. Stragier J, Evens T, Mechant P. Broadcast yourself: an exploratory study of sharing physical activity on social networking
sites. Media Int Aust 2015;155(1):120-129. [doi: 10.1177/1329878x1515500114]
77. Karp NA, Reavey N. Sex bias in preclinical research and an exploration of how to change the status quo. Br J Pharmacol
2019;176(21):4107-4118 [FREE Full text] [doi: 10.1111/bph.14539] [Medline: 30418665]
78. Tsoi HK, Chen L. From privacy concern to uses of social network sites: a cultural comparison via user survey. In: IEEE
3rd international conference on privacy, security, risk and trust and IEEE 3rd international conference on social computing.:
IEEE; 2011 Presented at: SocialCom and PASSAT '11; October 9-11, 2011; Boston p. 457-464. [doi:
10.1109/passat/socialcom.2011.71]
Edited by R Kukafka; submitted 21.06.21; peer-reviewed by M Salimi, T Karen, O El-Gayar, J Nafziger; comments to author 22.09.21;
revised version received 17.11.21; accepted 17.12.21; published 21.02.22
Please cite as:
Pilgrim K, Bohnet-Joschko S
Effectiveness of Digital Forced-Choice Nudges for Voluntary Data Donation by Health Self-trackers in Germany: Web-Based
Experiment
J Med Internet Res 2022;24(2):e31363
URL: https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363
doi: 10.2196/31363
PMID:
©Katharina Pilgrim, Sabine Bohnet-Joschko. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research
(https://www.jmir.org), 21.02.2022. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete
bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license
information must be included.
J Med Internet Res 2022 | vol. 24 | iss. 2 | e31363 | p. 13https://www.jmir.org/2022/2/e31363 (page number not for citation purposes)
Pilgrim & Bohnet-JoschkoJOURNAL OF MEDICAL INTERNET RESEARCH
XSL
FO
RenderX
... Examples of digital nudging can include pop-up notifications, visual cues, recommendation algorithms, and other interactive elements that guide users towards a specific action or decision. Digital nudges can utilise a variety of online and information technologies, digital user interfaces, and design elements such as push notifications (Özdemir 2020) on mobile apps (Pilgrim and Bohnet-Joschko 2021), social networks (Allem et al. 2017), gaming (Arafan 2021;Lewis 2014), ecommerce platforms (Meske, Amojo, and Mohr 2020), online verification tools (Schneider et al. 2017), recommendation tools (Karlsen and Andersen 2019), and location-based services (Acquisti et al. 2017; Barev and Janson 2019). From a company's perspective, a significant advantage of digital nudges is that they are relatively inexpensive and can change behaviour quickly (Özdemir 2020). ...
Article
The developing research area of digital nudges has garnered increasing attention over the past decade. However, with the acceleration of technological change, there is a need to identify how digital nudges are evolving with changing digital environments. For example, digital choice architectures can be personalised with algorithms, dark nudges, and real-time data. This paper presents a systematic narrative literature review on the evolution of digital nudges. The investigation for this review uncovered several themes, including subsets of digital nudges such as AI-powered nudges and dark nudges, as well as calls for ethics, transparency, and accountability when employing digital nudges. The findings provide a basis for a revised definition of digital nudges, a taxonomy wheel that clarifies relationships between digital nudge terminology, and valuable insights for practitioners to consider when designing and implementing digital nudges.
... Examples of nudges include changing the default settings of physician ordering templates or electronic health records, introducing posters as tangible point-of-decision reminders, and modifying chair placement to subliminally encourage physicians to sit by the bedside and practice etiquette-based care [14][15][16]. Nudges can involve psychological rewards, provide economic-driven benefits, or play into humanistic tendencies to do good, which can increase willingness to bring medications to primary care visits, increase appointment scheduling in patient portals, or increase participation in research studies that longitudinally track health data [17][18][19]. Crafting an effective nudge requires mapping the digital, physical, and social environments; denudging any problem behavior; targeting specific cognitive heuristics; identifying any existing misaligned nudges; developing clear measurable goals with termination plans for failure; and performing rapid sprints to test the minimally viable nudge (Multimedia Appendix 1). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background In today’s digital age, web-based apps have become integral to daily life, driving transformative shifts in human behavior. “AgileNudge+” (Indiana University Center for Health Innovation and Implementation Science) is a web-based solution to simplify the process of positive behavior change using nudging as an intervention. By integrating knowledge from behavioral economics with technology, AgileNudge+ organizes multiple steps, simplifies complex tasks, minimizes errors by enhancing user engagement, and provides resources for creating and testing nudge interventions. Objective This paper aimed to outline the design process, methodologies, and usefulness of “AgileNudge+” for the development of evidence-based nudges. It used a mixed methods approach to evaluate the software’s interface usability and usefulness for creating and testing nudge interventions. Methods AgileNudge+ was developed through iterative processes integrating principles from behavioral economics and user-centered design. The content of AgileNudge+ operationalizes an Agile science–based process to efficiently design, embed, and disseminate evidence-based nudges that encourage positive behavior change without limiting choice. Using a mixed methods approach, we tested AgileNudge+ software’s ability to organize and simplify the nudge intervention process, allowing a diverse range of scholars with limited knowledge of Agile science to use nudges. Usability testing assessed the tool’s usefulness and interface with a sample of 18 health care professionals, each asked to interact with the software and create a nudge intervention to solve a problem within their professional project’s sphere. Results The study was funded in August 2022, with data collection occurring from June 2023 to July 2024. As of July 2024, we have enrolled 18 participants. Quantitative results found a mean usefulness rating of AgileNudge+ of 3.83 (95% CI 3.00-4.66). Qualitative results highlighted ways to modify the language used in AgileNudge+ to be more comprehensible to a diverse user base and promoted modifications to the software that facilitate real-time assistance and prioritize time efficiency in user interactions. Feedback further supported the positive impact of gamification on participant motivation when using the software. Conclusions AgileNudge+ is an effective assistive tool for simplifying the positive behavior change process using nudge interventions, with tailored content and interactions to meet users’ needs and demands. Building onto the current design, future iterations of AgileNudge+ will use artificial intelligence to process large volumes of data while reducing the time and mental energy required to scan for existing cognitive biases and nudge prototypes. The software is also being upgraded to build on current gamification efforts, encouraging more sustained motivation by increasing the temporal resolution of the digital interface. These modifications stay true to the agility and user-centered aspects of AgileNudge+, emphasizing the novelty of the constantly evolving software design process.
... Despite the importance of this data, its sensitive nature poses significant obstacles to its collection [17], [21], [22], [35], [71], [92]. Prior work indicates that individuals are generally willing to donate their data for altruistic purposes [5], [5], [84], [95], [98], though there is a notable reluctance when it comes to their health data specifically [70], [72]. However, willingness to donate personal health data increases if the donor or their close family members are directly affected by the disease under investigation [14], [41]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Voluntary donation of private information for altruistic purposes, such as advancing research, is common. However, concerns about data misuse and leakage may deter individuals from donating their information. While prior research has indicated that Privacy Enhancement Technologies (PETs) can alleviate these concerns, the extent to which these techniques influence willingness to donate data remains unclear. This study conducts a vignette survey (N=485) to examine people's willingness to donate medical data for developing new treatments under four privacy guarantees: data expiration, anonymization, use restriction, and access control. The study explores two mechanisms for verifying these guarantees: self-auditing and expert auditing, and evaluates the impact on two types of data recipient entities: for-profit and non-profit institutions. Our findings reveal that the type of entity collecting data strongly influences respondents' privacy expectations, which in part influence their willingness to donate data. Respondents have such high expectations of the privacy provided by non-profit entities that explicitly stating the privacy protections provided makes little adjustment to those expectations. In contrast, statements about privacy bring respondents' expectations of the privacy provided by for-profit entities nearly in-line with non-profit expectations. We highlight the risks of these respective results as well as the need for future research to better align technical community and end-user perceptions about the effectiveness of auditing PETs and to effectively set expectations about the efficacy of PETs in the face of end-user concerns about data breaches.
... Digital nudges can positively influence consumer decisions, improved online product recommendations, and increased user engagement (Auf der Landwehr et al., 2021;Eigenbrod & Janson, 2018;Seo, 2020). Digital nudges utilize online and information technologies, digital user interfaces, and elements such as push notifications (Özdemir, 2019), mobile apps (Pilgrim & Bohnet-Joschko, 2021), social networks (Allem et al., 2017), gaming (Amojo, 2022), ecommerce platforms (Meske et al., 2020), and location-based services (Acquisti et al., 2017;Barev, T. J. & Janson, A., 2019). Examples of digital nudging can include pop-up notifications, visual cues, recommendation algorithms, and other interactive elements that guide users toward a specific action or decision. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This dissertation explores how knowledge of digital nudges impacts consumer decisions, and how consumer preferences based on that knowledge impacts design decisions. Paper 1 presents a systematic narrative literature review on the evolution of digital nudge literature. This investigation uncovers several themes and provides a basis for a revised definition of digital nudges and a taxonomy wheel of digital nudges. Paper 2 (co-authored with Jeffrey Livingston, Jonathan Ericson, and Patrick McHugh) investigates how knowledge about digital nudges impacts consumer preferences to have them within the digital experiences they use. This paper highlights how knowledge of digital nudges impact consumer online shopping preferences. Consumers (N = 331) were given two options for a homepage, product page, and checkout page within an e-commerce experience. Using an experiment with consumers as subjects, we discover that consumers consider themselves more knowledgeable about digital nudges after the experiment rather than before. Additionally, findings indicate that consumers are more inclined to avoid web pages incorporating dark patterns into their design. Paper 3 (co-authored with Jeffrey Livingston, Jonathan Ericson, and Patrick McHugh) complements the consumer paper. It investigates how much designers know about digital nudges and whether designers would be less apt to use digital nudges and dark patterns in their designs if they knew more about those digital nudges and if they knew which ones consumers would prefer to avoid. In this study, we examine whether and how designers might be influenced to avoid using digital nudges that might be considered manipulative or that consumers find objectionable. Designers (N = 353) were given two options for a homepage, product page, and checkout page within an e-commerce experience. We found that designers originally considered themselves to be more knowledgeable about digital nudges before the study than after it. Additionally, our findings indicate that designers are more inclined to avoid using digital nudges if they know that the consumer does not prefer them within a page experience.
... The FanFit app is a vehicle for investigating the expanding role of self-tracking data in shaping individual health-related behaviours, knowledge, and interpersonal relationships. The paper frames smartphone app interventions as an extension of self-tracking surveillance and enhanced physical activity [8][9][10][11], in which real-world fitness activities and app emotional affordances interact with discursive forms of fitness anchored in each user's allegiance to their sports club. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The last decade has seen a dramatic shift toward the study of fitness surveillance, thanks in part to the emergence of mobile health (mHealth) apps that allow users to track their health through a variety of data-driven insights. This study examines the adoption trends and community mediation of the mobile fitness application ‘FanFit’, a platform aimed at promoting physical activity among sports fans by creating a fitness app branded to their favourite team for health promotion. Objective: Our study looked at the impact of a specially designed mobile app (FanFit) as a digital health intervention for initiating and maintaining physical activity as part of football club membership. Our analysis indicates that app users will adopt healthier behaviours as a result of the app’s sense of fan community and behaviour change. Methods: The findings reported here are based on an implementation of the FanFit app and, in particular, on those who participated in a more in-depth study (n = 30). These participants were Rangers FC supporters with a mix of genders (n = 19 males and n = 11 females). Focus groups and interviews were conducted with participants to ascertain users’ perspectives on the most effective methods for nudging users toward adopting and maintaining a pattern of fitness behaviours. Results: The findings show that the user community was interested in fitness and wanted to live a ‘healthy lifestyle,’ which was augmented and fuelled by the app’s competitive architecture design. Furthermore, the data reveal a new fan-health discourse about a person’s developing wants, talents, and identities as embodied beings. Conclusions: We have developed and presented valid links between the use of sports club apps and health programmes. The app could be useful for sports programmes and club providers looking for mHealth applications that provide community support through fan discourse with opportunities for both male and female fans.
Article
Full-text available
Digital trace data retrieved via data donations holds significant potential for the study of individual behavior. However, data donation studies may be subject to bias. Researchers therefore need to quantify and address systematic error in digital trace data. To complement a-posteriori error correction methods like statistical modeling, we tested how ex-ante approaches, in particular survey design strategies, may help address bias in data donation studies. We conducted two preregistered experiments, one with a convenience sample of students (NI = 345) and one with a convenience sample from an online access panel (NII = 2,039). In both experiments, we analyzed the effects of survey design strategies - technical support during data donation, personalized incentives, and highlighting the societal relevance of participants' data-on nonresponse rates and nonresponse bias. Our results indicate that while data donation studies are prone to both, our ex-ante strategies could not effectively decrease nonresponse rates or nonresponse bias. Overall, our study underlines the need to (a) make bias in digital trace data more transparent and (b) advance research on error correction methods.
Article
Full-text available
Self-tracking technologies and practices offer ways of generating vast reams of personal details, raising questions about how these data are revealed or exposed to others. In this article, I report on findings from an interview-based study of long-term Australian self-trackers who were collecting and reviewing personal information about their bodies and other aspects of their everyday lives. The discussion focuses on the participants' understandings and practices related to sharing their personal data and to data privacy. The contextual elements of self-tracked sharing and privacy concerns were evident in the participants' accounts and were strongly related to ideas about why and how these details should be accessed by others. Sharing personal information from self-tracking was largely viewed as an intimate social experience. The value of self-tracked data to contribute to close face-to-face relationships was recognized and related aspects of social privacy were identified. However, most participants did not consider the possibilities that their personal information could be distributed well-beyond these relationships by third parties for commercial purposes (or what has been termed “institutional privacy”). These findings contribute to a more-than-digital approach to personal data sharing and privacy practices that recognizes the interplay between digital and non-digital practices and contexts. They also highlight the relational and social dimensions of self-tracking and concepts of data privacy.
Article
Full-text available
People make decisions and take actions to improve their viability everyday, and they increasingly turn to artificial intelligence (AI) to assist with their decision making. Such trends suggest the need to determine how AI and other cognitive technologies affect value co-creation. An integrative framework, based on the service-dominant logic and nudge theory, conceptualizes smart nudging as uses of cognitive technologies to affect people's behaviour predictably, without limiting their options or altering their economic incentives. Several choice ar-chitectures and nudges affect value co-creation, by (1) widening resource accessibility, (2) extending engagement , or (3) augmenting human actors' agency. Although cognitive technologies are unlikely to engender smart outcomes alone, they enable designs of conditions and contexts that promote smart behaviours, by amplifying capacities for self-understanding, control, and action. This study offers a conceptualization of actors' value co-creation prompted by AI-driven nudged choices, in terms of re-institutionalizing processes that affect agency and practices.
Article
Full-text available
We present a theoretical model to clarify the underlying mechanisms that drive individual decision making and responses to behavioral interventions, such as nudges. The model provides a theoretical framework that comprehensively structures the individual decision-making process applicable to a wide range of choice situations. We also identify the mechanisms behind the effectiveness of behavioral interventions-in particular, nudges-based on this structured decision-making process. Hence, the model can be used to predict under which circumstances, and in which choice situations, a nudge is likely to be effective.
Article
Full-text available
This article explores the production and type of knowledge acquired in the course of specific digital self-tracking activities that resemble research and are common among followers of the Quantified Self movement. On the basis of interviews with self-trackers, it is shown that this knowledge can be characterised as a verified and practical self-knowledge, and that science in the form of scientific sources, methods and quality criteria plays a key role in its production. It is argued that this self-related knowledge can be conceptualised as self-expertise, and its production as personal science. The article then discusses the implications for the science-society relationship. In contrast to self-tracking data, so far self-knowledge has hardly caused any resonance in science, although science currently appears open to the insights from single subject (N-of-1) research. As a new mode of public engagement with science, personal science instead mainly leads to an individual self-expertisation.
Chapter
How a situation or question is proposed can influence what people think of it. People react differently to “If you quit smoking, you will live longer” than to “If you don’t quit smoking, you will die sooner.” Although both statements provide the same information,...
Article
THE NEW CORONAVIRUS pandemic has promoted the new development of mobile and wearable computing in unprecedented ways. We discuss how on-body devices can help to fight the pandemic and may stay as a toolset to effectively deal with infectious diseases in the future. WHY WEARABLES? Researchers and health policy managers turned to smartphones and on-body devices mostly for their ubiquity, i.e., to offer health and safety-related information to a large share of the population or gather patient responses. Yet, smartphones and wearables enable fast data and information flow, which is particularly relevant for the rapid infectious character of SARS-CoV-2. We observe that continuous sensor and behavior data of smartphones and on-body devices are important as virus testing is associated with effort, cost, and provides only one-time information , and global immunization is still far away. We focus here on already existing and newly created wearable devices and smartphone apps for everyday use, but we exclude clinical and laboratory measurement systems, e.g., for heart and respiratory assessment. For researchers and public health authorities, symptom screening and tracking based on continuous sensor data from wearables and smartphones can provide insight into a possible infection of individuals. Also important are methods for social distancing and digital contact tracing, devices and apps for managing quarantine/self-isolation, and clinical management. To round-off this overview, we summarize lessons learnt from selected initiatives.
Article
Smart wearables have gained considerable attention from Information Systems (IS) academics, business managers, and health practitioners. In spite of the availability of authentic research studies of smart wearables, there is still a lack of systematic review on the different aspects of smart wearables concept to find the current state of research, particularly from the perspective of IS field. Therefore, the predominant aim of this research is to review smart wearables literature, recent advances, and future challenges. Accordingly, a systematic literature review was conducted to explore smart wearables by reviewing previous studies from 2010 to 2019. For covering all related papers during these years, an integrated review protocol consisting of automatic and manual stages was pursued. 244 papers were identified to address smart wearables issues and challenges. According to the findings, it is observed that smart wearable studies have increased dramatically during the last years. Moreover, the results show that current studies covered different research themes which are related to smart wearables area, particularly user behavior, technology-focused, security and privacy, design, and social acceptability. Furthermore, based on the results of the weight analysis technique, perceived usefulness, attitude toward technology, social influence, and privacy concerns are identified as the best predictors of smart wearables adoption. Additionally, the results show that the Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) is the most commonly adopted theory in the smart wearable studies. The findings of this review would assist academics to realize the existing limitations and gaps as well as the future works for smart wearables research studies.
Article
Today, medical data such as diagnoses, procedures, imaging reports and laboratory tests, are not only collected in context of primary research and clinical studies. In addition, citizens are tracking their daily steps, food intake, sport exercises, and disease symptoms via mobile phones and wearable devices. In this context, the topic of “data donation” is drawing increased attention in science, politics, ethics and practice. This paper provides insights into the status quo of personal data donation in Germany and from a global perspective. As this topic requires a consideration of several perspectives, potential benefits and related, multifaceted challenges for citizens, patients and researchers are discussed. This includes aspects such as data quality & accessibility, privacy and ethical considerations.