ArticlePDF Available

On the idea of Learning Trajectories: Promises and Pitfalls

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
The Mathematics Enthusiast The Mathematics Enthusiast
Volume 8
Number 3
Number 3
Article 6
7-2011
On the idea of Learning Trajectories: Promises and Pitfalls On the idea of Learning Trajectories: Promises and Pitfalls
Susan B. Empson
Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/tme
Part of the Mathematics Commons
Let us know how access to this document bene?ts you.
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Empson, Susan B. (2011) "On the idea of Learning Trajectories: Promises and Pitfalls,"
The Mathematics
Enthusiast
: Vol. 8 : No. 3 , Article 6.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.54870/1551-3440.1229
Available at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/tme/vol8/iss3/6
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been
accepted for inclusion in The Mathematics Enthusiast by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks at University of
Montana. For more information, please contact scholarworks@mso.umt.edu.
TME, vol8, no.3, p .571
The Mathematics Enthusiast, ISSN 1551-3440, Vol. 8, no.3, pp.571-596
2011©Dept of Mathematical Sciences- The University of Montana & Information Age
Publishing
On the Idea of Learning Trajectories: Promises and Pitfalls1
Susan B. Empson2
The University of Texas at Austin
Learning mathematics is a complex and multidimensional if not an inherently
indeterminate process. A necessary goal of research on learning is to simplify
this complexity without sacrificing the ability of research to inform teaching. This
goal has been addressed in part by researchers focusing on how to represent
research on learning for teachers and on how to support teachers to use and
generate models of students’ learning (e.g., Franke, Carpenter, Levi, & Fennema,
et al., 2001; Hammer & Schifter, 2001; Simon & Tzur, 2004; Steffe, 2004). Recently,
the idea of learning trajectories has gained attention as a way to focus research on
learning in service of instruction and assessment. It is influencing curriculum
standards, assessment design, and funding priorities. In this paper – which grew
out of my response to Michael Battista’s keynote address on learning trajectories
at the last annual meeting of the North American chapter of Psychology in
Mathematics Education (Battista, 2010) – I examine the idea of learning
trajectories and speculate on its usefulness in mathematics education.
1 Thanks to Jennifer Knudsen of SRI International for invaluable feedback.
2 empson@mail.utexas.edu
Empson
The National Research Council (2007) described learning progressions as
“successively more sophisticated ways of thinking about a topic that can follow
one another as children learn about and investigate a topic” (p. 214). The recently
released Common Core Standards in Mathematics (CCSM) (2010), noted that the
“development of these Standards began with research-based learning
progressions detailing what is known today about how students’ mathematical
knowledge, skill, and understanding develop over time” (p. 4). The idea of
learning trajectories has a great deal of intuitive appeal and may offer a way to
bring coherence to how we think about learning and the curriculum. As research
on learning trajectories proliferates and is brought to bear on some of the most
vexing problems in teaching and learning mathematics, however, it is worth
considering what it foregrounds and what it may obscure.
In this paper, I briefly describe the origins of learning trajectories in
mathematics education and then consider three points for us to keep in mind as
we study learning and apply our findings to serve the purposes of
understanding and addressing the problems of practice.
1) The idea that learning progresses is not especially new. What do we know
about learning mathematics and how does it fit with the idea of a
trajectory?
2) Learning trajectories focus on specific domains of conceptual development
and may be limited in characterizing other valued aspects of the
mathematics curriculum.
TME, vol8, no.3, p .573
3) Learning in school is function of teaching. Too tight a focus on learning
trajectories may lead us to oversimplify or ignore critical drivers of
learning associated with teaching.
My goal in making these points is not to state the obvious but to foreground the
question of what the idea of learning trajectories affords us education researchers
and practitioners, and what it might obscure.
Origins of Learning Trajectories
The term learning trajectory appears to have been first used in mathematics
education in Marty Simon’s oft-cited 1995 paper, “Reconstructing Mathematics
Pedagogy from a Constructivist Perspective.” As I reread this paper, the most
important things I noticed – besides the fact that that the actual words “learning
trajectory” did not appear until 21 pages into the article – were that a) a learning
trajectory did not exist for Simon in the absence of an agent and a purpose and b)
it was introduced in the context of a theory of teaching. According to Simon, a
hypothetical learning trajectory is a teaching construct – something a teacher
conjectures as a way to make sense of where students are and where the teacher
might take them. It is hypothetical because an “actual learning trajectory is not
knowable in advance” (p. 135). Teachers are agents who hypothesize learning
trajectories for the purposes of planning tasks that connect students’ current
thinking activity with possible future thinking activity. A teacher might ask,
“What does this student understand? What could this student learn next and
Empson
how could they learn it?” and create a hypothetical learning trajectory as a way
to prospectively grapple with these questions.
The idea of learning progressions appears to have emerged first in the context
of science education and is now virtually synonymous with learning trajectory.
In a special issue of the Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics, and Technology
Education devoted to the topic of “long-term studies” of learning in science
education, Shapiro (2004) traced the notion of learning progression in part to
Rosalind Driver in her 1989 article, "Students' Conceptions and the Learning of
Science." In it, Driver drew attention to the increasing number of studies of the
development of children’s thinking in specific science domains that documented
patterns in what she called conceptual progressions and sequences of conceptual
progressions, which she termed conceptual trajectories (Shapiro, 2004, p. 3). In
contrast to Simon, the focus in that special issue of CJSMT was on describing
children’s learning as it had actually occurred under a given set of conditions,
rather than on a thought experiment about how it could occur. Neither of these
senses of learning trajectory – as a teacher-conjectured possible progression or a
researcher-documented progression of actual learners– predominates in current
conceptions of the notion.
Since 2004, there has been a groundswell of research that explicitly identifies
itself as concerned with learning trajectories or progressions, as reflected in
conferences and special journal issues (Clements & Sarama, 2004; Duncan &
Hmelo-Silver, 2009), reports (Catley, Lehrer, & Reiser, 2005; Cocoran, Mosher, &
TME, vol8, no.3, p .575
Rogat, 2009; Daro, Mosher, & Cocoran, 2011), and books (Clements & Sarama,
2009). A report by the Center for Continuous Improvement in Instruction (Daro,
et al., 2011) treats learning trajectories as interchangeable with learning
progressions, reflecting the general trend.
Because the metaphor of trajectory implies a sequenced path, researchers who
focus explicitly on learning trajectories have taken pains to draw attention to
their multidimensional character. For example, Clements and Sarama (2004)
defined learning trajectories as complex constructions that include “the
simultaneous consideration of mathematics goals, models of children’s thinking,
teachers’ and researchers’ models of children’s thinking, sequences of
instructional tasks, and the interaction of these at a detailed level of analysis of
processes” (p. 87). Confrey and colleagues (2009) defined them as “researcher-
conjectured, empirically-supported description[s] of the ordered network of
experiences a student encounters through instruction … in order to move from
informal ideas … towards increasingly complex concepts over time” (p. 2).
Three Points to Keep in Mind
Learning Trajectories are Not Really New – So What does the Metaphor Buy Us?
The idea that students’ learning progresses in some way as a result of
instruction is at the very heart of the enterprise of mathematics education.
Researchers have been studying students’ mathematics thinking and what it
could mean for that thinking to progress in identifiable ways since long before
the term learning trajectories was introduced. Chains of inquiry focused on
Empson
children’s mathematics learning – we could call these research trajectories – have
stretched over decades. For example, Glenadine Gibbs’s (1956) study of students’
thinking about subtraction word problems helped to pave the way for later
researchers such as Carpenter and Moser (1984) to create frameworks portraying
the development of children’s thinking about addition and subtraction, and for
Carpenter, Fennema, and Peterson to study how teachers used this information
about children’s thinking to teach for understanding (Carpenter, et al., 1989;
Carpenter, et al., 1999). Les Steffe and John Olive’s recent (2010) book on
Children’s Fractional Knowledge detailing the evolution of children’s conceptual
schemes for operating on fractions synthesized two decades’ worth of prior
research, as did Karen Fuson’s findings on the development children’s multidigit
operations (1992). None this work mentioned learning trajectories as such, but
each focused on elucidating the development of children’s understanding and
identifying major conceptual advances.
Why then talk about learning trajectories now? The metaphor emphasizes the
orderly development of children’s thinking and draws our attention to learning
targets and possible milestones along the way.
To what extent is this kind of assumption about learning warranted? That is,
in what sense does children’s mathematics learning follow predictable
trajectories? Some domains appear to readily lend themselves to analysis in
terms of a pathway, such as the development of young children’s counting skills
(Gelman & Gallistel, 1986). The progression of children’s strategies for addition
TME, vol8, no.3, p .577
and subtraction story problems from direct modeling, to counting, to the use of
derived and recalled facts also has been well established (Carpenter et al., 1999;
Carpenter, 1985; Fuson, 1992). Yet even given such a robust progression in a
basic content domain, how and when – and sometimes whether – children come
to understand and use these strategies depends on a variety of factors differing
from classroom to classroom and from child to child. Trying to represent
research on learning in terms of trajectories quickly gets complicated, even for as
fundamental a concept as rational number (e.g., Figure 2 “Learning Trajectories
Map for Rational Number Reasoning,” in Confrey, Maloney, Nguyen, et al.,
2009) or measurement (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Battista’s (2010) representation of one student’s actual learning path in
measurement
Other research suggests that the development of much of children’s thinking
is more piece-meal and context-dependent than representations of learning
trajectories might lead us believe (DiSessa, 2000; Greeno & MMAP, 1998). For
Empson
example, in a cross-sectional, cross-cultural study, Liu and Tang (2004) found
differences in progressions of students’ conceptions of energy in Canada and
China over several years of schooling, which they attributed to differences in
curriculum and instruction in each country. The topic of rational numbers in
mathematics has an ample research base that illustrates, in some cases
meticulously, how children’s thinking about fractions could progress (Behr,
Harel, Post, & Lesh, 1992; Davydov & Tsvetkovich, 1991; Empson & Levi, 2011;
Hackenberg, 2010; Steffe & Olive, 2010; Streefland, 1991; Tzur, 1999). Taken
collectively this research does not appear to converge on a single trajectory of
learning.
Why might this be? In practice, learning cannot be separated from tasks and
the instructional context; the “selection of learning tasks and the hypotheses
about the process of student learning are interdependent” (Simon & Tzur, 2004,
p. 93). What children learn is sensitive to the context in which they learn it – a
context that is constituted by many factors, including most immediately the
types of instructional tasks and how teachers organize students’ engagement
with these tasks.
For example, in classrooms where part-whole tasks (Fig. 2a) dominate
instruction on fractions, children learn to think about fractions in terms of
counting parts rather than as magnitudes (Thompson & Saldanha, 2003).
Students are likely to think about 5/8 as “5 out of 8” and of 8/5 as an impossible
fraction. In classrooms where teachers have students solve and discuss equal
TME, vol8, no.3, p .579
sharing tasks (Fig. 2b), children learn to think about fractions in terms of
relationships between quantities and later in terms of a multiplicative
relationship between numerator and denominator (Empson, Junk, Dominguez, &
Turner, 2005; Empson & Levi, 2011). They are more likely to think of 5/8, for
example, as 5 groups each of size 1/8, instead of “5 out of 8.” In classrooms
where teachers engage students in reasoning about multiplicative comparisons
of measures (Fig. 2c), students learn to think about fractions as a ratio of
measures (Brousseau, Brousseau, & Warfield, 2004; Davydov &Tsvetkovich,
1991; Steffe & Olive, 2010). Children learn to interpret 5/8 as a multiplicative
comparison between 5 and 8. Both of these latter types of tasks – equal sharing
and measuring – coupled with norms for engaging in tasks that put a premium
on intellectual effort and agency (Hiebert & Grouws, 2007) – appear to constitute
productive approaches to learning fractions.
How much pizza is left on the plate?
(a)
8 children want to share 10 candy bars so that each one gets
the same amount. How many candy bars can each child
have?
Empson
(b)
How long is B compared to A? A compared to B?
(c)
Figure 2. Examples of types of tasks to teach fractions: a) part-whole, b) equal
sharing (with sample solution), and c) measurement
Within the context of documenting regularities and patterns in the
development of children’s thinking, however, it’s important to recognize
individual children’s ways of reasoning and the significant contributions this
reasoning could make to a group’s learning. To return to my research on equal
sharing, for example, we found that students frequently produced strategies for
solving problems that were, from the perspective of a trajectory, “out of
sequence” and presented rich learning opportunities for other students (e.g.,
Turner et al., in press). There was a progression in what students learned but
“deviations” were consistent and numerous, and, I am suggesting, fruitful – not
anomalies to be ignored but significant occurrences that teachers could use to
advance everyone’s learning.
Consider first a simple progression of strategies for equal sharing (Empson &
TME, vol8, no.3, p .581
Levi, 2011). To figure out how much one person got if 8 people were sharing 6
burritos equally, a child using a basic strategy might draw all 6 burritos, decide
to split each burrito into 8 pieces, and give each person 1 piece from each burrito
for a total of 6 pieces. A more sophisticated strategy would involve imagining
that each burrito could be split into 8 pieces and mentally combining those pieces
to conclude that one person’s share consisted of 6 groups of 1/8 burrito or 6/8
burrito. Ultimately, children come to the understanding that the problem can be
represented by 6÷8, which is the same as 6/8.
Within this simplified progression, there are several other ways to solve the
problem that do not fall into a sequence and do not appear as an inevitable
consequence of development. These other strategies were a function of specific
quantities in a problem as well as what tools children were using and children’s
prior knowledge. For example, a fifth grader solved the problem by reducing it
to an equivalent ratio involving 1 1/2 burritos and 2 children, which she easily
solved by finding half of 1 and half of 1/2 and combining the amounts (Fig. 3).
Another fifth grader used a similar strategy, but used cubes to represent each
quantity (8 total cubes for sharers, 6 total cubes for burritos), specifically
highlighting the ratio character of the strategy. These strategies were
appropriated by several children who saw them as more efficient and they
provided an opportunity for the teachers to address concepts of fraction and
ratio equivalence.
Empson
Figure 3. A solution for 6÷8 involving the equivalent ratios 3 for 4 and 1 1/2 for 2
As students’ understanding develops and diversifies, they become more
likely to see and make connections between their ways of thinking and different
ways of thinking expressed by their fellow students. Making these connections
enriches learners’ understanding and cultivates their ability to recognize and
pursue new avenues of reasoning independently of the teacher’s direction and to
monitor their thinking. The balance in instruction between supporting students’
agentic initiative and aiming to instill specific conceptions can be difficult to
manage. Indeed, some researchers have cautioned that representing learning as
progressive sequences of content understanding could lead teachers to direct
students through the sequences at the expense of allowing students to “express,
test, and revise their own ways of thinking” (Lesh & Yoon, 2004, p. 206; Sikorski
& Hammer, 2010). At the same time, other research suggests that, at the right
level of abstraction, representations of the progressive development of students’
understanding can enhance teachers’ ability to respond to students’ thinking in
ways that open up or are generative of new possibilities (e.g., Franke et al. 2001).
TME, vol8, no.3, p .583
In either case, it’s important to recognize that research on learning in specific
mathematics domains has a long history that, while concerned with progress,
may not fit easily into the idea of a single trajectory.
Learning Trajectories Involve Specific Domains of Conceptual Development – So Their
Reach May be Limited
Researchers have made the study of mathematics learning more tractable
by focusing in particular on conceptual development in specific content domains,
represented by sets of well-defined, interrelated tasks. Steffe and Olive’s (2010)
research on the development of fraction concepts and Clements and Sarama’s
(2009) research on children’s understanding of measurement are examples of
such an approach. This work, like a great deal of the research in mathematics
education including my own, is informed by a Piagetian-like view of learning, if
not in its emphasis on levels, then certainly in its emphasis on a conceptual
trajectory, in which less sophisticated concepts give way to more sophisticated
concepts. Because this work is based on children’s thinking about specific types
of tasks, its power lies in its capacity to inform teachers’ use and interpretation of
these tasks to foster students’ conceptual development in a coherent unit of
study (e.g., Fennema et al, 1996; Simon & Tzur, 2004).
However powerful, these kinds of portrayals of learning necessarily
represent only one dimension or a small set of what we value as a field about
mathematics and wish for students to learn. Learning is a multidimensional
process, comprised of a variety of intertwined cognitive and social processes. In
Empson
particular, since the publication of Everyone Counts (National Research Council,
1989) and the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards (National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics, 1988), mathematics educators have increasingly focused on
teaching students to engage in practices such as posing and solving problems
(Hiebert et al., 1996), constructing models (Lesh & Doerr, 2003), and making
convincing arguments (Lehrer & Schauble, 2007) – that is, to do mathematics.
Doing mathematics involves a complex and integrated set of content
understanding and disciplinary practices (Bass, 2011; Kilpatrick, Swafford, &
Findell, 2001) as well as the ability to monitor the interplay between these things
(Schoenfeld, 1992).
The ability to engage in mathematical practices such as the ones above is
as critical as content knowledge to a well-developed capacity to think
mathematically, but it is less amenable to analysis in terms of sequences of
development. For example, students engaged in mathematical modeling or
problem solving may draw on multiple content domains and work
collaboratively on tasks that have many possible resolutions such that the
solutions they produce appear to follow no predictable trajectory over time.
Examples of such tasks include creating simulations of disease spread (Stroup,
Ares, & Hurford, 2005), optimizing the occupancy of a hotel during tourist
season (Aliprantis & Carmona, 2003), and designing a template to generate a
quilt pattern (Lesh & Doerr, 2003). These kinds of tasks and thinking practices
pose considerable challenge for researchers seeking to codify and systematically
TME, vol8, no.3, p .585
represent learning in terms of a trajectory, because of the variety of
understanding and practices that students bring to bear in the their solutions.
Learning trajectories may be limited in what they can and cannot specify
in terms of learning mathematics over time; and in particular, they may not be
applicable to certain critical aspects of the mathematics curriculum. Catley,
Lehrer, and Reiser (2005) recognized this potential limitation when they argued
that “scientific concepts are never developed without participation in specialized
forms of practice” and “concepts are contingent on these practices” (p. 4) – such
as the ones listed in the Common Core Standards in Mathematics (2010). Among
others, these practices include making sense of problems and persevering in
solving them; using appropriate tools strategically; attending to precision; and
looking for and making use of structure (CCSM, 2010, pp. 6-8). Most, if not all,
current characterizations of learning trajectories do not address the practices that
engender the development of concepts – although it’s worth thinking about
alternative ways to characterize curriculum standards and learning trajectories
that draw teachers’ attention to specific aspects of students’ mathematical
practices as well as the content that might be the aim of that practice.
What is a reasonable unit of students’ mathematical activity for teachers to
notice? If a unit is too small or requires a great deal of inference (e.g., a mental
operation), then teachers in their moment-to-moment decision-making may not
be able to detect it and respond to it; likewise if a unit is too broad or stretches
over too long a period of time (e.g., “critical thinking”), teachers may not
Empson
recognize it when they are seeing it. The most productive kinds of units of
mathematical activity would allow teachers to see and respond to clearly defined
instances of student’s thinking during instruction and to gather information
about students’ progress relative to instructional goals. For example, in research
in elementary mathematics, strategies and types of reasoning are productive units
because we know that teachers can learn to differentiate students’ strategies and
use what they learn about students’ thinking to successfully guide instruction
(e.g., Fennema, et al., 1996). Catley and colleagues (2005) proposed “learning
performances” as a way to represent the “cognitive processes and associated
practices linked to particular standards” (p. 5). Formative assessments that
include a variety of points of access and possible solutions and that require
students to engage in various mathematics practices could also yield rich
information about students’ understanding of and engagement in mathematics
(cf., Aliprantis & Carmona, 2003; Lesh & Doerr, 2003). The important thing is to
take into account the interplay of practices and content in students’ learning over
time.
Teaching is Integral to Learning and Learning Trajectories
Learning school mathematics depends on teaching. To support learning,
teachers need be able to “understand, plan, and react instructionally, on a
moment-to-moment basis, to students' developing reasoning” and coordinate
these interactions with learning goals (Battista, 2010). Similarly, Daro and
colleagues (2011) concluded that:
TME, vol8, no.3, p .587
Teachers are going to have to find ways to attend more closely and regularly
to each of their students during instruction to determine where they are in
their progress toward meeting the standards, and the kinds of problems they
might be having along the way. Then teachers must use that information to
decide what to do to help each student continue to progress, to provide
students with feedback, and help them overcome their particular problems to
get back on a path to success. (Daro et al., 2011, p. 15)
We know very little about how teachers do these things, in contrast to what
we know about children’s learning, whether it falls under the rubric of learning
trajectory research or not. As teachers interact with students and decide how to
proceed, there are many types of decisions to be made – how to gather
information about children’s thinking, how to respond to it appropriately in the
moment, how to design tasks that extend it, and even what to pay attention to.
With the right tools, teachers have access to the most up-to-date information
about each student, what they understand and are able to do, their disposition,
their history, and so on, and can make decisions based on their own informed
understanding of these things and their relationships. Good tools, such as
formative assessment frameworks in particular, enhance this knowledge and
support teachers to engage in the active, contingent process of creating
instructional trajectories informed by knowledge of actual children’s learning.
Further, learning mathematics in school takes work and depends
fundamentally on interpersonal relationships of trust and respect, which cannot
be designed into a tool or a list of learning goals. Teaching is a relational act and
the relationship between the teacher and the student is at the center of students’
learning in school (Gergen, 2009; Grossman & McDonald, 2008). These
Empson
relationships can have a profound effect on what students learn and how they
come to see themselves.
In the face of what can seem like a tidal wave of top-down mandates, I
suggest that we mathematics educators keep sight of the fact that teaching is
driven essentially by interpersonal relationships and happens from the bottom
up, beginning with the teacher and the student relating to each other and the
content. We need to be sure that teachers are equipped with knowledge of the
domain and its learning milestones without forgetting that both teachers and
students are active agents in learning.
Closing Thoughts
“Clearly … the trajectories followed by those who learn will be extremely
diverse and may not be predictable” (Lave & Wenger, 1991)
In choosing to focus on learning trajectories, we embrace a metaphor that, for
all its appeal, implies that learning unfolds following a predictable, sequenced
path. Everyone knows it is not that simple; researchers and educators alike
acknowledge the complexity of learning. As Simon (1995) emphasized, learning
trajectories are essentially provisional. We can think of them as the provisional
creation of teachers who are deliberating about how to support students’
learning and we can think of them as the provisional creation of researchers
attempting to understand students’ learning and to represent it in a way that is
useful for teachers, curriculum designers, and test makers.
TME, vol8, no.3, p .589
I firmly believe that a critical part of our mission as researchers is to produce
something that is of use to the field and serves as a resource for teachers and
curriculum designers to optimize student learning. No doubt this includes
creating, testing, and refining empirically based representations of students’
learning for teachers to use in professional decision-making and, further,
investigating ways to support teachers’ decision-making without stripping
teachers of the agency needed to hypothesize learning trajectories for individual
children as they teach. This focus would add a layer of complexity to our
research on learning and invite us to think seriously about how to support
teachers to incorporate knowledge of children’s learning into their purposeful
decision-making about instruction. Further, I suggest we consider, in the end,
“Whose responsibility is it to construct learning trajectories?” (Steffe, 2004, p.
130). If we researchers can figure out how to supply teachers with knowledge
frameworks and formative assessment tools to facilitate their work, teachers will
be able to exercise this responsibility with increasing skill, professionalism, and
effectiveness.
Because of the growing popularity of learning trajectories in education circles,
it is worth thinking hard about the role of learning trajectory representations in
teaching, and in particular, whether a learning trajectory can exist meaningfully
apart from the relationship between a teacher and a student at a specific time and
place. Simon’s (1995) perspective on teaching and learning suggests not. As the
field moves forward with research on learning trajectories and strive for
Empson
coherence in learning across the grades, I would like to remain mindful of both
the affordances and constraints this particular type of representation offers for
teachers and students alike.
TME, vol8, no.3, p .591
References
Aliprantis, C. & Carmona, G. (2003). Introduction to an economic problem: A
models and modeling perspective. In R. Lesh & H. Doerr (Eds.) Beyond
constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics problem solving,
learning, and teaching, 255-264. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bass, H. (2011). A vignette of doing mathematics: A meta-cognitive tour of the
production of some elementary mathematics. The Montana Mathematics
Enthusiast,8(1-2), 3-34.
Battista, M. (2010). Representations of learning for teaching: Learning
progressions, learning trajectories, and levels of sophistication. In P. Brosnan,
D. B. Erchick, & L. Flevares (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-Second Annual
Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol. pp. 60-71. Columbus, OH: The Ohio
State University.
Behr, M., Harel, G., Post, T. & Lesh, R. (1992). Rational number, ratio, and
proportion. In D. Grouws (Ed.). Handbook of research on mathematics teaching
and learning (pp. 296 -333). New York: Macmillan.
Brousseau, G., Brousseau, N., & Warfield, V. (2004). Rationals and decimals as
required in the school curriculum Part 1: Rationals as measurements. Journal
of Mathematical Behavior, 23, 1-20.
Empson
Carpenter, T. P. (1985). Learning to add and subtract: An exercise in problem
solving. In E. A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematical problem solving:
Multiple research perspectives (pp. 17-40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Franke, M., Levi, L. & Empson, S. B. (1999).
Children’s Mathematics: Cognitively Guided Instruction. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann.
Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Peterson, P., Chiang, C. & Loef, M. (1989). Using
knowledge of children's mathematics thinking in classroom teaching: An
experimental study. American Educational Research Journal, 26(4), 499 – 531.
Carpenter, T. P. & Moser, J. (1983). The acquisition of addition and subtraction
concepts. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), The acquisition of mathematical
concepts and processes (pp. 7 - 44). New York: Academic.
Catley, K. M., Lehrer, R., & Reiser, B. J. (2005). Tracing a prospective learning
progression for developing understanding of evolution. Paper commissioned
by the National Academies Committee on Test Design for K-12 Science
Achievement.
Clements, D. & Sarama, J. (2004). Learning trajectories in mathematics education.
Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6(2), 81-89.
Clements, D. &Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and teaching early math: The learning
trajectories approach. New York: Routledge.
Common Core State Standards Initiative (2010). Mathematics. Washington, DC:
Council of Chief State School Officers & National Governers Association
TME, vol8, no.3, p .593
Center for Best Practices. Downloaded 8/15/10 from
http://www.corestandards.org/the-standards/mathematics.
Confrey, J., Maloney, A., Nguyen, K., Mojica, G., & Myers, M. (2009).
Equipartitioning/splitting as a foundation of rational number reasoning. In
M. Tzekaki, M. Kaldrimidou & C. Sakonidis (Eds.) Proceedings of the 33rd
Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
Vol. 1. Thessaloniki, Greece: PME.
Daro, P., Mosher, F., & Cocoran, T. (2011). Learning trajectories in mathematics: A
foundation for standards, curriculum, assessment, and instruction. Philadelphia,
PA: Consortium for Policy Research in Education.
Davydov, V. V. & Tsvetkovich, Z. H. (1991). The object sources of the concept of
fractions. In L. Steffe (Ed.) Soviet studies in mathematics, vol. 6: Psychological
abilities of primary school children in learning mathematics (J. Teller, Trans.).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (Original work
published in 1969)
DiSessa, A. (2000). Changing minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Driver, R. (1989). Students’ conception and learning of science. International
Journal of Science Education, 11(5), 481-490.
Duncan, R. & Hmelo-Silver, C. (2009). Learning progressions: Aligning
curriculum, instruction, and assessment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
46(6), 606-609.
Empson
Empson, S. B., Junk, D., Dominguez, H., & Turner, E. E. (2005). Fractions as the
coordination of multiplicatively related quantities: A cross-sectional study of
children’s thinking. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 63, 1-28
Empson, S. B. & Levi, L. (2011). Extending children’s mathematics: Fractions and
decimals. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Fennema, E., Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M., Levi, L., Jacobs, V. & Empson, S. B.
(1996). Mathematics instruction and teachers' beliefs: A longitudinal study of
using children's thinking. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(4),
403-434.
Franke, M. L., Carpenter, T. P., Levi, L. & Fennema, E. (2001). Capturing teachers’
generative change: A follow-up study of professional development in
mathematics. American Educational Research Journal, 38, 653-689.
Fuson, K. (1992). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D.
Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics education. New York:
Macmillan.
Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. R. (1986). The child’s understanding of number. Boston:
Harvard University Press.
Gergen, K. J. (2009). Relational being: Beyond self and community. New York:
Oxford.
Gibb, E. G. (1956). Children’s thinking in the process of subtraction. The Journal of
Experimental Education, 25(1), 71-80.
TME, vol8, no.3, p .595
Greeno, J. & Middle-School Math through Applications. (1998). The situativity of
knowing, learning, and research. American Psychologist, 53(1), 5-26.
Grossman, P. & McDonald, M. (2008). Back to the future: Directions for research
in teaching and teacher education. American Educational Research Journal, 45(1),
184-205.
Hackenberg, A. (2010). Students’ reasoning with reversible multiplicative
relationships. Cognition and Instruction, 28(4), 383-432.
Hammer, D. & Schifter, D. (2001). Practices of inquiry in teaching and research.
Cognition and Instruction, 19, 441-478.
Hiebert, J., Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Fuson, K., Human, P., Murray, H.,
Olivier, A. & Wearne, D. (1996). Problem solving as a basis for reform in
curriculum and instruction: The case of mathematics. Educational Researcher,
25(4), 12-21.
Hiebert, J. & Grouws, D. (2007). The effects of classroom mathematics teaching
on students’ learning. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on teaching
and learning mathematics, Vol. I (pp. 371-404). Reston, VA: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J. & Findell, B. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn
mathematics. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation.
New York: Cambridge.
Empson
Leach, J. (1998). Rosalind Driver (1941 – 1997): A tribute to her contribution to
science education. In M. Meheut & G. Rebmann (Eds.) Proceedings of the 4th
European Science Education Summerschool, Marly-le-Roi, France.
Lehrer, R. & Schauble, L. (2007). A developmental approach for supporting the
epistemology of modeling. Modeling and applications in mathematics education,
vol. 10, part 3, sec. 3.1, 153-160.
Lesh, R. & Doerr, H. (Eds.) (2003). Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling
perspectives on mathematics teaching, learning, and problem solving. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Liu, X. & Tang, L. (2004). The progression of students’ conceptions of energy: A
cross-grade, cross-cultural study. Canadian Journal Science, Mathematics, and
Technology Education, 4(1), 43-57.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1988). Curriculum and evaluation
standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: author.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for
school mathematics. Reston, VA: author.
National Research Council. (1989). Everyone counts: A report to the nation on the
future of mathematics education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
National Research Council. (2001). Knowing what students know: The science and
design of educational assessment. Pelligrino, J., Chudowsky, N., and Glaser, R.,
(Eds.). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
TME, vol8, no.3, p .597
Resnick, L. & Ford, W. (1981). The psychology of mathematics for instruction.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schoenfeld, A. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving,
metacognition, and sense making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), The
handbook of research in mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334-370). New
York: Macmillan Publishing Company.
Shapiro, B. (2004). Developing understanding: Research on science learning and
teaching over time. Canadian Journal Science, Mathematics, and Technology
Education, 4(1), 1-6.
Sikorski, T-R. & Hammer, D. (2010). A critique of how learning progressions
research conceptualizes sophistication and progress. In Gomez, K., Lyons, L.,
& Radinsky, J. (Eds.) Learning in the Disciplines: Proceedings of the 9th
International Conference of the Learning Sciences (ICLS 2010) - Volume 2, Full
Papers, 1032-1039. International Society of the Learning Sciences: Chicago IL.
Simon, M. (1995). Reconstructing mathematics pedagogy from a constructivist
perspective. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(2), 114-145.
Simon, M. & Tzur, R. (2004). Explicating the role of tasks in conceptual learning:
An elaboration of a hypothetical learning trajectory. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning, 6(2), 91-104.
Smith, C. L., Wiser, M., Anderson, C. W., & Krajcik, J. (2006). Implications of
research on children’s learning for standards and assessment: A proposed
Empson
learning progression for matter and the atomic-molecular theory. Measurement,
14(1&2), 1-98.
Steffe, L. & Olive, J. (2010). Children’s fractional knowledge. New York: Springer.
Streefland, L. (1991). Fractions in realistic mathematics education. Boston: Kluwer.
Thompson, P. & Saldanha, L. (2003). Fractions and multiplicative reasoning. In J.
Kilpatrick, W. G. Mateo, & D. Schifter (Eds.) A research companion to principles
and standards for school mathematics (pp. 95-113). Reston, VA: National Council
of Teachers of Mathematics.
Turner, E. E., Dominguez, H., Maldonado, L., & Empson, S. B. (in press). English
learners’ participation in mathematical discussion: Shifting positionings,
dynamic identities. In R. Guttiérez, Equity, identity, and power. Special issue of
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education.
... Critical arguments pointing out that LPs do not accommodate all children's developmental trajectories often overlook the probabilistic nature of statistical methods used in LPbased assessments, which account for individual differences. Future critiques should focus on the extent to which measurement models are effective in accommodating such variability, rather than on simplistic notions that LPs impose fixed or "orderly" developmental trajectories (e.g., Empson, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated concerns about potential racial/ethnic bias in the Desired Results Developmental Profile (DRDP), a widely used observational assessment in early childhood education (ECE). Specifically, we examined whether the learning progressions (LPs) underlying the DRDP items exhibit differential applicability to children from culturally and linguistically diverse (CLD) families—comparing groups of children identified as Latino/a, Black, or White. Using Rasch person fit analyses with a large public preschool sample (N = 80,058), we tested the hypothesis that greater positive misfit for Latino/a and Black children would indicate bias. Contrary to this hypothesis, our findings revealed comparable person fit distributions across the three racial/ethnic groups, suggesting no evidence of racial/ethnic bias in the DRDP. Additionally, the study identified key trends, including higher positive misfit among children in special education, likely reflecting greater intra-individual variability, and slightly lower misfit for children identified as female compared to males. Person fit improved over the course of the academic year, underscoring the potential influence of raters and contexts on consistency. Teacher-level variance emerged as the largest contributor to person fit variance, highlighting the need for ongoing professional development and support to ensure consistency. The results support the cultural sensitivity of the DRDP and emphasize the importance of continued research on rater effects and potential biases in early learning assessment. This study also demonstrates the promise of person fit metrics as a tool to enhance assessment validity, equity, and instructional planning in ECE contexts serving children from diverse families.
... The ability to fluently add and subtract within 20, solve one-step word problems, and use efficient strategies for numbers up to 1000 support a strong grasp of basic operations. Number sense and numeracy are built upon understanding the structure of three-digit numbers, comparing numbers, and recognizing the amount of tens and ones in a two-digit number (Empson, 2011 ...
Experiment Findings
Full-text available
LXD Research conducted a study to evaluate the impact of the Zaner-Bloser Kickstart: Number Sense program, a math intervention designed to enhance foundational math skills among first and second-grade students in an Oregon school district over a three-year period from spring 2019 to Spring 2023. The program was implemented in two schools, engaging 130 first graders, 163 second graders, and 538 third graders. The student population included 15% Hispanic/Latino students, 8% English Language Learners, 15% students with diagnosed disabilities, and approximately 70% of students qualifying for Free/Reduced Price lunch. The district represents at least 19 different language backgrounds, reflecting a diverse and multicultural student body. The intervention comprised daily 20-minute group lessons focused on developing critical math skills, conducted by instructors without prior math teaching experience. The program's effectiveness was evaluated by comparing third-grade math proficiency rates between Kickstart students and their peers from comparison schools of similar size across the state, utilizing publicly available proficiency scores from the Oregon Statewide Assessments (OSAS) as the benchmark. Key findings demonstrated that Zaner-Bloser Kickstart: Number Sense students consistently outperformed their counterparts in comparison schools over multiple academic years. Specifically, in the 2021-2022 academic year, 43% of all Kickstart students achieved math proficiency, compared to 25% in comparison schools. In the 2022-2023 academic year, the proficiency rate for all Kickstart students was 44%, compared to 25% in comparison schools. Additionally, first graders' proficiency according to school-delivered assessments increased from 25% in 2021-2022 to 63% in 2022-2023, while second graders' proficiency rose from 36% to 62% over the same period. These results suggest that the Zaner-Bloser Kickstart: Number Sense program is highly effective in significantly improving early math outcomes, fostering a solid foundation in math for young learners, and potentially bridging educational gaps. The study's rigorous methodology and findings support Zaner-Bloser Kickstart: Number Sense as a comprehensive program that meets the criteria for ESSA Level 2. [This report was produced with Zaner-Bloser.]
... "In all cases, however, it involves the focused and informed study of the development of students' mathematical thinking in specific content domains, and it is grounded in teachers' practice" (Carpenter & Franke, 2004, p. 51). Frameworks for types of word problems and associated progressions of student strategies for solving problems are salient features of CGI, although these frameworks are featured and used differently in different CGI-based programs (Empson, 2011;Schoen et al., 2022;Schoen et al., 2023). ...
... The research in this field first started in the field of science and then gradually expanded to various disciplines. However, in some mathematics education studies, learning progressions and learning trajectory have no clear distinction, meaning learning progressions are virtually synonymous with learning trajectory (Empson, 2011). The following text uses learning trajectory to express. ...
Article
Full-text available
Learning trajectory describes the student’s learning progress or steps in one area through which teaching and learning can be linked. The cognitive diagnostic model (CDM) is an emerging evaluation theory in education measures. Researchers can determine students’ mastery of fine-grained knowledge points by describing the learning trajectory based on CDM. The present study is based on the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) 2019, particularly, 21 mathematical items in the fourth grade. It analyzes the response data of 2,854 students from 17 countries from 10 attributes based on CDM. This study explores students’ different learning trajectories in the content field by analyzing the relationships between knowledge states and attribute patterns. The study found that the 17 countries differ in learning trajectories but have commonalities. The learning starting points of 17 countries can be roughly divided into two categories and geometry attribute and fractions and decimals attribute are the last two attributes to master.
... Asimismo, las dificultades que presentan los EPM en cuanto al contenido matemático pueden ser producto de los significados de las fracciones que predominan en la enseñanza primaria. Por ejemplo, el significado de fracción como relación parte-todo podría promover el uso de estrategias aditivas; mientras que el significado de fracción como razón podría favorecer el uso de estrategias multiplicativas (Empson, 2011). La dificultad de los EPM con la anticipación de estrategias multiplicativas también puede deberse a que es más fácil visualizar y representar las diferencias entre dos cantidades cuando se comparan aditivamente, en contraste a cuando se trata de la razón entre estas cantidades. ...
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this research is to characterize how primary prospective teachers, one year after a teaching experiment, recognize different stages of progression anticipating solving strategies for measurement division problems with fractions used by elementary school children. The 41 participants were in the seventh semester of the Education Teaching Degree (eight semesters) during the 2018-2019 academic year. The analysis considered the type of strategies used and whether they evidenced the idea of progression. The results show three categories in the use of the idea of progression when anticipating answers to measurement division problems: They (a) do not use the idea of progression; (b) partially use the idea of progression; (c) use the idea of progression. Despite its difficulty, it is possible to begin to develop the idea of progression by anticipating strategies in the training of future teachers.
... HLT constitutes a learning process construction that anticipates student learning. It is grounded in learning objectives, students' level of understanding, and mathematical activity choices (Conner et al., 2017;Empson, 2011;Fuadiah, 2017). Learning trajectories are designed based on the previously identified learning obstacles. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study presents the development of mathematics instructional materials using the Didactical Design Research (DDR) framework, aiming to enhance the Pedagogical Content Knowledge (PCK) of prospective mathematics teachers in fractional number operations. The research adopted the ADDIE model and encompassed Analysis, Design, Development, Implementation, and Evaluation phases. The evaluation involved expert validators specializing in Didactical Design Research (DDR), subject matter, media, and educators. The resulting Semester Lesson Plan (RPS) assessment scores and teaching materials demonstrated a "Very Good" categorization, indicating their validity and suitability for field testing. Notable findings highlight epistemological, ontogenetic, and didactic obstacles in students' understanding of quadratic inequalities. The instructional materials showed incremental effectiveness during classroom implementation, as evidenced by increasing percentages of successful sessions. This research's implications for the future include the potential adaptation of the DDR framework across diverse educational contexts, exploring technological integration, and enhancing educators' proficiency in implementing DDR-based pedagogies. By bridging theoretical foundations with practical application, this study contributes to advancing mathematics education by fostering innovative instructional materials grounded in Didactical Design Research.
... As LTs have proliferated across educational communities, some scholars expressed concerns with issues of equity and diversity inherent in their conceptualization, development, and implementation. Some have argued that, though mathematics learning is multidimensional and occurs through connections across multiple domains, trajectories reduce learning to a hierarchical, linear path (Empson, 2011;Lesh & Yoon, 2004). Anderson and colleagues (2012) reported that researchers and other leaders in science and mathematics education have raised a number of concerns about trajectories. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to shed light on how static and dynamic proportionality is treated by authors of teaching materials, on national tests and by teachers and students in the classroom, as well as how students encounter mathematics tasks where proportional reasoning is an option. The research is based on two sets of empirical data. In concrete terms, the thesis includes three studies examining three themes that relate to proportionality in classroom interaction and in texts. The first study analyses how proportionality is presented in some Swedish textbooks, in curricular texts and national course tests in mathematics for students in upper secondary school. The second study is a case study of how a teacher instructs and explains a task in a class in Grade 6, where proportional reasoning is a possible solution technique. Finally, the third study concerns how students in Grade 6 handle proportional reasoning when they encounter a patterning task involving proportional relationships. The analyses of textbooks and national course tests show that proportionality is handled differently in these two settings in the context of “Mathematics A” at the upper secondary school. About a quarter of the tasks in the textbooks and the national course tests involved proportionality tasks of one specific kind (missing value). Other types of proportionality tasks were infrequent. The results of the classroom studies show that students are able to engage in early forms of proportional reasoning before being taught about proportionality as a mathematical concept. The concept of learning trajectory is used to identify situations in the learning process during instruction where students meet obstacles and need scaffolding and teacher support. It is shown how a teacher dealing with a mathematical task involving mixtures of liquids encounters a task that has the possibility of making proportional reasoning visible for the students, and how she struggles to make the modelling required intelligible to herself and to the students. The instructional strategy of using everyday problems as a basis for learning implies that the initial modelling phase becomes crucial, and the students have to be aware of the conditions and limitations under which proportional reasoning is applicable. In conclusion, students engage in early forms of proportional reasoning well ahead of formal instruction. The difficulties they experience as they are to develop their proficiency, and where they require support from the teacher, concern how to model the familiar, everyday situations they encounter in exercises in mathematically precise and productive ways. In addition, in textbooks and national course tests proportionality is presented in a standardized, and rather simplified, form, and it is not sufficiently connected to the various areas of mathematics teaching and learning where it is applicable.
Article
Full-text available
The results of the preliminary study show that students' mathematical literacy skills on the topic probability was low. This is caused by teachers' conservative teaching methods and students who are not familiar with literacy matters. The presentation of opportunity material is not fully adequate to guide students in learning. This research aims to develop learning designs with a Realistic Mathematic Education (RME) approach with local Jakarta culture on opportunity material using the Gravemeijer and Cobb model which consists of three stages, namely experimental preparation), experimental implementation), and retrospective analysis. The research subject was Health Vocational School Mulia Karya Husada class XI. The learning design in question is a learning path known as Hypothetical Learning Trajectory (HLT). HLT is a learning flow consisting of learning objectives, learning activities, and learning process hypotheses to predict how students think and understanding will develop in the context of learning activities. The research results show that there was an increase in students mathematical literacy abilities.
Book
Children's Fractional Knowledge elegantly tracks the construction of knowledge, both by children learning new methods of reasoning and by the researchers studying their methods. The book challenges the widely held belief that children's whole number knowledge is a distraction from their learning of fractions by positing that their fractional learning involves reorganizing not simply using or building upon their whole number knowledge. This hypothesis is explained in detail using examples of actual grade-schoolers approaching problems in fractions including the schemes they construct to relate parts to a whole, to produce a fraction as a multiple of a unit part, to transform a fraction into a commensurate fraction, or to combine two fractions multiplicatively or additively. These case studies provide a singular journey into children's mathematics experience, which often varies greatly from that of adults. Moreover, the authors' descriptive terms reflect children's quantitative operations, as opposed to adult mathematical phrases rooted in concepts that do not reflect-and which in the classroom may even suppress-youngsters' learning experiences. Highlights of the coverage: Toward a formulation of a mathematics of living instead of beingOperations that produce numerical counting schemes Case studies: children's part-whole, partitive, iterative, and other fraction schemes Using the generalized number sequence to produce fraction schemes Redefining school mathematics This fresh perspective is of immediate importance to researchers in mathematics education. With the up-close lens onto mathematical development found in Children's Fractional Knowledge, readers can work toward creating more effective methods for improving young learners' quantitative reasoning skills. © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010 All rights reserved.