ArticlePDF Available

The Theories of the Development of Students: A Factor to Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation

Frontiers
Frontiers in Psychology
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Empathy represents an essential prerequisite for developing effective interpersonal behavior and maintaining interpersonal relationships. Education is a result of teacher-student interaction, and, therefore, it is worth noting that teaching empathy is critical for the development of students and the professional growth of teachers. Recently, researchers began to explore the influential factors of empathy (e.g., empathic mindsets) based on motivation. Beyond their empathic attitudes, teachers also have a mindset toward the development of students. A survey study was adopted to explore the relationship between the theories of the growth of students and teacher empathy. Four hundred and eighty-four Chinese teachers completed the student development scale, the teacher empathic motivation scale, and the teacher empathy scale. The mediation model results showed that the theories of the development of students could significantly predict teacher empathy and teacher empathic motivation. The teacher empathic motivation mediated the positive relationship between the theories of the development of students and teacher empathy. This study proposes a new concept and method for teacher empathy intervention in future.
This content is subject to copyright.
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 16 November 2021
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736656
Edited by:
Mohamed A. Ali,
Grand Canyon University,
United States
Reviewed by:
Shaoying Gong,
Central China Normal University,
China
Qing Zhao,
Institute of Psychology, Chinese
Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
*Correspondence:
Weijian Li
xlxh@zjnu.cn
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Educational Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 05 July 2021
Accepted: 26 October 2021
Published: 16 November 2021
Citation:
Ge Y, Li W, Chen F, Kayani S and
Qin G (2021) The Theories of the
Development of Students: A Factor
to Shape Teacher Empathy From
the Perspective of Motivation.
Front. Psychol. 12:736656.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736656
The Theories of the Development of
Students: A Factor to Shape Teacher
Empathy From the Perspective of
Motivation
Yabo Ge1,2,3, Weijian Li1*, Fangyan Chen3, Sumaira Kayani1and Guihua Qin1
1Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China, 2Key Laboratory of Intelligent Education Technology
and Application of Zhejiang Province, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China, 3Teachers College, Jinhua Polytechnic,
Jinhua, China
Empathy represents an essential prerequisite for developing effective interpersonal
behavior and maintaining interpersonal relationships. Education is a result of teacher-
student interaction, and, therefore, it is worth noting that teaching empathy is critical
for the development of students and the professional growth of teachers. Recently,
researchers began to explore the influential factors of empathy (e.g., empathic mindsets)
based on motivation. Beyond their empathic attitudes, teachers also have a mindset
toward the development of students. A survey study was adopted to explore the
relationship between the theories of the growth of students and teacher empathy.
Four hundred and eighty-four Chinese teachers completed the student development
scale, the teacher empathic motivation scale, and the teacher empathy scale. The
mediation model results showed that the theories of the development of students could
significantly predict teacher empathy and teacher empathic motivation. The teacher
empathic motivation mediated the positive relationship between the theories of the
development of students and teacher empathy. This study proposes a new concept
and method for teacher empathy intervention in future.
Keywords: teacher empathy, empathic propensity, motivation, empathic motivation, teacher education
INTRODUCTION
Empathy, an interpersonal phenomenon, refers to sharing in and understanding the thoughts, and
feelings of other people, and caring for their welfare (Zaki, 2014;Preston and Waal, 2017;Yang
et al., 2018;Weisz and Cikara, 2021). It is broadly believed that empathy is a critical ingredient
in interpersonal processes (Main et al., 2017;Amicucci et al., 2021). Moreover, related studies
have noted an impairment of social functioning consequent upon the empathy deficit in a series
of neuropsychiatric conditions (Shimoni et al., 2012;Laisney et al., 2013;Schreiter et al., 2013;
Yang et al., 2018). As is generally known, teaching is a social interaction that involves a pupil
and a teacher (Holper et al., 2013), which is also inseparable from empathy (Swan and Riley,
2015). Teacher empathy, which involves cognitive and affective elements (Tettegah and Anderson,
2007;Swan and Riley, 2015;Goroshit and Hen, 2016), involves comprehensively understanding
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 2
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
the situation of students sharing the positive and negative
emotions of students, and expressing care for the students
through actions (Berkovich and Eyal, 2015;Meyers et al.,
2019;Ronen, 2020). Currently, there is an enhanced awareness
of the importance of teacher empathy in teacher education
(Swan and Riley, 2015).
The Value of Culturing for Teacher
Empathy
It is generally accepted that teacher empathy is significantly
correlated with the development of students and teachers.
Accumulating evidence suggests that, on the one hand,
teacher empathy can promote the academic achievement of
students (Cadima et al., 2010;Warren, 2018;Ronen, 2020),
their motivation for learning (Cooper, 2004), teacher-student
relationship (Wubbels and Brekelmans, 2005;Stojiljkovi´
c et al.,
2012), and the overall classroom atmosphere (Cooper, 2010).
Additionally, teacher empathy is not merely meant to encourage
student engagement in learning but to help achieve social
justice across diverse backgrounds (Bullough, 2019). Therefore,
arguably, education is not complete without teacher empathy; if
not, teachers are teaching to transmit content instead of teaching
the students (Swan and Riley, 2015). On the other hand, teacher
empathy plays a vital role in promoting the development of
students and is a crucial feature of the identity of teachers, which
can promote their professional growth (Stojiljkovi´
c et al., 2012;
Zhu et al., 2019). For example, empathy effectively facilitates
the specialization of teachers through establishing positive
teacher-student relationships and a relaxed teaching atmosphere
(Stojiljkovi´
c et al., 2012). Moreover, some investigators noted
that empathy had been long considered central to the teaching
profession (Jaber et al., 2018).
Overall, empathy-building interventions for teachers have
a clear adaptive function for social interaction, both for the
students and the teachers. Thus far, empathy interventions have
focused on building perspective-taking strategies and increasing
empathy-expression strategies (Weisz and Zaki, 2017). However,
these intervention techniques have not yielded impressive results
(Waller et al., 2020). Therefore, it is essential to identify other
factors that shape teacher empathy.
Empathy and Motivation
Empathy is not always automatic but is rather context-
dependent (Zaki, 2014). Furthermore, empathy, like many other
psychological phenomena, involves a motivational component
(Weisz et al., 2020). Empathic motivation is goal-directed,
an internal force that drives people toward and away from
social connections (Weisz and Zaki, 2018). More importantly,
Keysers and Gazzola (2014) proposed that the ability-propensity
distinction is crucial to characterizing empathy. According to
this theory, there may be variations in empathy owing to not
only the ability difference but also the motivation difference.
Similarly, Ferguson et al. (2020) suggested that empathy is a
choice and can be evoked in multiple ways. Therefore, similar
to empathic ability, empathic motivation serves a significant role
in the process of empathy. The relevant theory of empathic
motivation provides a new insight into empathy culturing as to
date, many empathic cultivations focus on developing the ability
of people to empathize via experience-based and expression-
based interventions (Weisz and Zaki, 2017).
Teacher Empathy: The Potential Role of
Teachers’ Beliefs
It is novel to identify the factors that shape teacher empathy
based on motivation. Belief is one of the most critical factors
that influence motivation, such as the achievement goal theory
(Dweck, 1996). Recently, more and more researchers have
focused more on beliefs, which play an essential role in empathy
and empathic motivation, and have gained many advances
(Schumann et al., 2014;Weisz et al., 2020;Gandhi et al., 2021).
For example, Weisz et al. (2020) found that the participants
who had a more robust belief about the malleability of empathy
exhibited greater empathic motivation and empathic accuracy.
Similarly, Gandhi et al. (2021) reported that individuals who
believed that empathy was changeable exhibited more empathetic
behavior (less aggression).
Likewise, various educational researchers suggest that
teachers’ beliefs affect their classroom practice (Kagan, 1992;
Fang, 1996;Mansour, 2009). For example, Wang and Yang
(2021) found that most pre-service STEM teachers hold the
reality of development and the possibility of developing beliefs
about migrant students. Moreover, teachers’ beliefs can also
affect student motivation through classroom practice. Heyder
et al. (2020) suggested that the more teachers believed that math
requires innate ability, the lower the intrinsic motivation of
students with low-achieving was. These studies also show that
there is an intimate relationship between teacher beliefs and
teacher practices. Therefore, just like in the general domain
of empathy studies, we believe that the empathy and empathic
motivation of teachers, belonging to the practices of teachers, may
also be influenced by the beliefs of the teachers. As mentioned
above, empathic motivation is a crucial influencing factor of
empathy. Collectively, we speculate empathic motivation would
play a mediating role in the relationship between the beliefs and
empathy of teachers.
The Current Study
As stated above, many investigators have paid increasing
attention to the role of beliefs in empathy and empathic
motivation in recent years (Schumann et al., 2014;Weisz et al.,
2020). For example, Schumann et al. (2014) defined the empathic
mindset as a malleable mindset through which believing empathy
can be developed and a fixed theory through which believing
empathy cannot be set. The results suggest that people who
had a malleable mindset expended greater empathic motivation
in challenging contexts than those who believed in a fixed
theory. Similarly, as alluded to earlier, Weisz et al. (2020)
found the same results. These findings suggested that empathic
belief, a motivation-based intervention, is an essential factor
that shapes empathy.
Fives and Buehl (2012) divided the beliefs of teachers into six
categories, in which beliefs about students refer to the views about
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 3
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
linguistic difference, capacity, learning, and the development of
students. Researchers believe that beliefs about students are the
most critical influencing factor of teacher practices, which is in
a relatively central position (Wang and Yang, 2021). In other
words, teachers, in the educational context, have not only their
beliefs about themselves but also beliefs about students (e.g.,
the development of student abilities). According to Dweck et al.
(1995) the theory of mindsets, teachers may have two different
beliefs about the development of the ability of students. Teachers
with malleable beliefs of the ability of students may think that
the ability of students is unstable and can be enhanced through
acquired efforts. On the contrary, teachers with fixed mindsets of
the abilities of students may believe that their abilities are fixed
and unchangeable. This raises the question of whether teacher
empathy and teachers’ empathic motivation are influenced by
their beliefs about the development of students.
Similar to beliefs about oneself, in this study, we expected
teacher beliefs about the development of students to play
an essential role in the empathic motivation and empathy
of teachers. A mediation model was established to test these
hypotheses; Figure 1 presents the conceptual model. This study’s
primary contribution was to first investigate the role of teacher
beliefs about the ability development of students under teacher
empathy based on the motivation perspective.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Based on the model complexity (e.g., n= 5–10 per estimated
parameter) (Bentler and Chou, 1987), the reasonable sample
size for this investigation was between 285 and 570. A total
of 495 Chinese teachers were recruited from kindergarten,
primary school, junior high school, and senior high school in
Zhejiang province. Eleven participants were excluded from the
data screening process as they chose the same option on all the
scales. The valid sample included 484 Chinese teachers [84.1%
female; 67 no-titles (13.8%), 163 secondary title (33.7%), 194
primary title (40.1%), and 67 senior title (13.8%); 18 kindergarten
teachers (3.7%), 320 primary school teachers (66.1%), 83 junior
high school teachers (17.1%), and 63 senior high school teachers
(13.0%)]. The average age was (mean ±SD) 35.81 ±8.27 years
old; the average teaching experience was (mean ±SD) 14.78
±10.98 years.
Measures
Demographic Information Questionnaire
The sociodemographic characteristics of the participants were
evaluated using a questionnaire that included the following
information: gender, age, teaching experience, and title (i.e.,
“What is your teaching experience in terms off years?”).
Theories of the Development of Students
To construct a more accurate evaluation method of the teacher’s
mindset regarding the development of students (TOS), we
adapted a three-item measure from an existing measure of the
implicit theory of personality (Dweck et al., 1995) and theories of
empathy (Schumann et al., 2014). The questionnaire was scored
on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree),
followed by three statements related to the teacher’s mindset
of the ability of students. The statements were: “The level of
Students’ learning ability is stable to some extent, and students are
not able to change it. Students can indeed learn new knowledge,
but they cannot improve their learning ability. That is Students’
learning ability is unlikely to change.” After statistical analysis,
the internal consistency of the questionnaire was highly reliable
(Cronbachs α= 0.86).
Motivation for Teacher Empathy
Empathic motivation is a very abstract concept with motivational
content or driving direction. This study focuses on the driving
direction of motivation. It is self-edited to be suitable for
the educational situation in which teachers are approaching
the intensity of the empathic motivation, that is, the teachers’
empathic motivation (TEM) questionnaire. The questionnaire is
adapted from the study of Schumann et al. (2014), and includes
three statements (e.g., “when a student is in a bad mood, “I
want to know what they are thinking that moment, “I am
willing to share in their bad feelings, and “I want to comfort
them.”). Participants responded to the questionnaire on a 7-
point agreement scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree).
A statistical analysis showed that this questionnaire has a high
internal consistency coefficient (Cronbachs α= 0.83).
Teacher Empathy
The Chinese version of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI-
C) (Huang et al., 2011) was revised and normalized for Chinese
(Sun et al., 2017) to assess empathy (Likert 5-point, from 0 to
4). This scale version is widely used in Chinese culture (Sun
et al., 2017;Chen et al., 2018). To construct a more accurate
evaluation method, we composed an empathy questionnaire
(IRI for Chinese teachers’ empathy, IRI-CT) to evaluate teacher
empathy (TE) based on IRI-C. Specifically, we changed the
statements of IRI-C to fit the current study purpose (i.e.,
empathic target). For example, we revised IRI-C “I often have
tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me.”
and “I sometimes find it difficult to see things” from the “other
person’s point of view.” to “I often have tender, concerned feelings
for students less fortunate than me.” and “I sometimes find it
difficult to see things from the ‘Students” point of view” (see
Supplementary Appendix).
IRI-CT, which is similar to the IRI-C, also assesses four aspects
of teacher empathy, namely, empathic concern (i.e., TE-EC, seven
items), perspective-taking (i.e., TE-PT, seven items), fantasy (i.e.,
TE-FS, seven items), and personal distress (i.e., TE-PD, seven
items). Participants rated their agreement or disagreement with
28 items on a 7-point scale (1 = does not describe me well,
7 = describes me very well). To validate the four-factor model,
a confirmatory factor analyses was used to assess the model
fit (x2/df = 3.98, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.73, TLI = 0.70,
SRMR = 0.10), which shows a moderate structure validity. The
correlations between the IRI-CT subscales and related constructs
were significant (Zhao et al., 2018). For instance, TE-EC was
negatively correlated with verbal aggression (the subscale of AQ)
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 4
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
FIGURE 1 | The conceptual model of the relationships between teachers’ beliefs about Students’ development, teachers’ empathic motivation, and teacher
empathy.
(r=0.15, p<0.001), difficulty in identifying one’s own feelings
(the subscale of TAS-20) (r=0.14, p<0.01) and positively
correlated with prosocial tendencies (PTM) (r= 0.41, p<0.001),
relational needs (the subscale of BPNS) (r= 0.30, p<0.001). An
internal consistency analysis revealed that the adapted scale of IRI
has moderate reliability (α= 0.74). All the subscales of the IRI-CT
demonstrated good internal consistencies (ranging from 0.57 to
0.71, see Table 1), which was consistent with other studies based
on the Chinese versions of IRI (Zhao et al., 2021).
Procedures
This study was conducted through a web-based survey via a
Chinese survey website.1All the participants in the questionnaire
survey were volunteers and were asked to read the introduction
to the study. The participants were then immediately instructed
to fill out a demographic information questionnaire, and
complete the whole task carefully. Data including demographic
information, theories of the development of students, the
empathetic motivation of teachers, and the subscales, such as
TE-EC, TE-PT, TE-FS, and TE-PD were collected. The survey
could not be submitted if any questions had not been answered,
like other studies (Zhao et al., 2021). Hence, there were no
missing values. They took approximately 8 min to complete all
the assessments. Once they completed the questionnaire, the
participants were debriefed about the purpose of this study and
thanked for their participation. The Zhejiang Normal University
Review Board approved the current research procedures.
Data Analysis
The data analysis was as follows. First, SPSS (version 23.0) was
used to calculate the descriptive statistics and partial correlations
of crucial variables in this study. One recent cross-cultural study
reported the culture-sex interaction effect for both trait and
state empathy with Australian and Chinese subjects (Zhao et al.,
2021). Therefore, partial correlation analyses (i.e., to control for
the following covariates: gender, age, teaching experience, and
title) were conducted to examine the relationships between the
theories of the development of students (TOS), the empathic
motivation of teachers, teacher empathy (TE), the empathic
concern of teacher empathy (TE-EC), the perspective-taking of
1https://www.wjx.cn
teacher empathy (TE-PT), the fantasy of teacher empathy (TE-
FS), and the personal distress of teacher empathy (TE-EC).
Second, the hypothesized mediation model was tested using
the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2013). The hypothesized
mediation [the theories of the development of students the
empathic motivation of teachers (mediator) teacher empathy,
TE-EC, TE-PT, TE-FS, and TE-PD, respectively] was tested using
model 4 of the PROCESS macro for SPSS (version 3.3, see
Hayes, 2013). For the current analyses, a meaningful indirect
effect was identified depending on whether zero was outside the
95% confidence interval (CI) of the indirect effect (Field, 2013).
Gender, age, teaching experience, and title were controlled for.
Further, the models were tested using 5,000 bootstrap samples.
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and the partial correlation
analyses (i.e., to control for covariates) for the key variables.
As expected, the theories of the Students’ development were
positively correlated with the empathic motivation of teachers,
teacher empathy, TE-EC and TE-PT, but negatively correlated
with TE-PD. Additionally, the empathic motivation of teachers
was significantly and positively related to teacher empathy, TE-
EC, TE-PT, and TE-FS. The above preliminary analyses indicate
a close relationship with the theories of Students’ development,
empathic motivation, and teacher empathy (including subscales),
which is also the basis of the mediation model analysis.
Mediation Model
Two univariate outliers (i.e., an outlier for each of TEM, TE-
PD) were identified (z-scores >3.29) and excluded (Zhao et al.,
2019, 2020). As indicated in Figure 2A, for teacher empathy, the
total effects model with the theories of Students’ development
and beforementioned covariates (i.e., gender, age, teaching
experience, and title) showed that the regression coefficient for
the theories of the development of the students was significant
(b= 0.42, SE = 0.18, p= 0.02, 95% CI = [0.06,0.78]), which
indicated that the total effect of the theories of the development
of the students on teacher empathy was significant. Further, the
mediation model controlled for the covariates was tested. The
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 5
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
TABLE 1 | Descriptive statistics and partial correlations for key variables.
Variable M±SD 1234567
1 Theories of the development of students 12.50 ±3.36 0.86
2 The empathic motivation of teachers 16.87 ±2.99 0.31*** 0.83
3 Teacher empathy (TE) 134.88 ±13.64 0.13*** 0.43 *** 0.74
4 Teacher empathy-EC (TE-EC) 38.38 ±4.84 0.25*** 0.51 *** 0.64*** 0.57
5 Teacher empathy-PT (TE-PT) 37.88 ±4.95 0.37*** 0.55 *** 0.51*** 0.54 *** 0.70
6 Teacher empathy-FS (TE-FS) 28.49 ±6.60 0.03 0.18*** 0.74*** 0.20*** 0.07 0.71
7 Teacher empathy-PD (TE-PD) 30.13 ±5.89 0.24*** 0.08 0.53*** 0.02 0.20 *** 0.37*** 0.71
Cronbach’s alpha for each scale is in bold and in the diagonals. ***p <0.001.
results showed that the empathic motivation of teachers was
significantly predicted by the theories of the development of the
students (b= 0.26, SE = 0.04, p<0.001, 95% CI = [0.19,0.34]) and
that teacher empathy was significantly predicted by the empathic
motivation of teachers (b= 1.93, SE = 0.19, p<0.001, 95%
CI = [1.55, 2.31]). Further, the predictive relationship between
the theories of Students’ development and teacher empathy was
not significant when teachers’ empathic motivation was included
in the regression equation (b=0.09, SE = 0.17, p= 0.62, 95%
CI = [0.43,0.26]). The indirect effect of the empathic motivation
of teachers was (b= 0.51, SE = 0.10, 95% CI = [0.33,0.72]). There
is a meaningful indirect effect, as mentioned above when the zero
was outside the 95% confidence interval (CI) of the indirect effect
(Field, 2013). Therefore, the relationship between the theories of
Students’ development and teacher empathy was mediated by the
empathic motivation of teachers.
The abovementioned analytic approach will also be used for
subscales of teacher empathy including TE-EC, TE-PT, TE-FS,
and TE-PD. The results showed that the empathic motivation of
teachers has a mediating effect between the theories of Students’
development and teacher empathy subscales except for TE-PD;
namely, TE-EC (the indirect effect b= 0.19, SE = 0.04, 95%
CI = [0.13,0.27], see Figure 2B), TE-PT (the indirect effect
b= 0.21, SE = 0.04, 95% CI = [0.14,0.28], see Figure 2C), TE-
FS (the indirect effect b= 0.10, SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [0.04,0.18],
see Figure 2D) and TE-PD (the indirect effect b= 0.01, SE = 0.02,
95% CI = [0.04,0.06], see Figure 2E).
DISCUSSION
Teacher empathy plays a vital role in the Students’ development
and the professional development of teachers (Stojiljkovi´
c
et al., 2012). It is the foundation of the empathy culture to
identify the factors that shape this psychological variable. This
study investigates the role of teachers’ beliefs about Students
development in the empathic motivation and empathy of
teachers via the mediation model. Our finding suggests that the
theories of Students’ development were positively associated with
the empathic motivation and teacher empathy. The empathic
motivation of teachers was positively associated with teacher
empathy, and mediated the positive relationship between the
theories of Students’ development and the teacher empathy.
Therefore, the belief of teachers about Students’ development
may be an essential characteristic of empathic motivation that
contributes to teacher empathy in the educational context.
The Theories of the Development of
Students and Teacher Empathy
To the best of our knowledge, no previous study investigates
the direct link between the theories of Students’ development
and teacher empathy. However, some evidence from previous
research indicates that the beliefs of teachers affect teacher
practice, such as teaching effectiveness (Jordan et al., 2010).
Additionally, Lavigne (2014) suggests that teachers develop
beliefs about students as part of the teacher identity process,
and focus more on student understanding and achievement.
Besides, the research found that the teachers’ self-confidence
was significantly associated with their Students’ self-confidence
(Larina and Markina, 2020). Consistent with these previous
findings among educational context, we observed a significant
association between the teachers’ belief (i.e., the theories of
Students’ development) and teachers practices (i.e., teacher
empathy and three subscales). In other words, teachers who have
a malleable mindset of the ability of students were more likely
to exhibit high empathic concern and perspective-taking, and
low personal distress toward students, while fantasy does not.
One possible explanation for this is that fantasy was designed
to evaluate a person’s propensity to appreciate the emotions of
fictitious characters in movies, plays, or books (i.e., “When I
watch a good movie, I can very easily put myself in the place
of a leading character”) (Davis, 1980); some researchers assert
that fantasy does not evaluate empathy per se (Baron-Cohen and
Wheelwright, 2004) and hence exclude it from the data analysis
(Zhao et al., 2019). Therefore, fantasy may not accurately reflect
teacher empathy, which is to comprehensively understand the
situation of students, share the positive and negative emotions
of students, and express care for them through their actions
(Berkovich and Eyal, 2015;Meyers et al., 2019;Ronen, 2020).
The Mediating Roles of Empathic
Motivation
More importantly, we discovered the mediating roles of the
empathic motivation of teachers in explaining why the theories
of the development of students further generate teacher empathy.
Previous research indicates a close relationship between the
teachers’ beliefs and their practice. This study expands this work
to explore the mechanism in these psychological variables (e.g.,
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 6
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
FIGURE 2 | Models of teacher empathic motivation (TEM) as a mediator
variable for the relationship between the theories of student development
(TOS), teacher empathy (TE; A), empathic concern of teacher empathy
(TE-EC; B), perspective taking of teacher empathy (TE-PT; C), fantasy of
teacher empathy (TE-FS; D) and personal distress of teacher empathy
(TE-PD, E). Non-standardized coefficients are reported. The solid lines
represent significant coefficients, and the dashed line means insignificant
effects. ***p< 0.001.
teachers’ beliefs, teacher empathy). The relationship between
teachers’ beliefs and motivation has been confirmed by numerous
studies (Dweck et al., 1995;Blackwell et al., 2007;Dweck, 2012;
Yeager et al., 2013). We further investigate why the theories
of Students’ development are particularly predictive of the
empathic motivation of teachers. Empathy is, to our knowledge,
felt as a cognitive cost (e.g., the uncertainty associated with
inferring information about another person’s experience), which
causes people to avoid adopting empathy (Cameron et al., 2019;
Ferguson et al., 2020, 2021). Therefore, empathy, an instrumental
process, is tied to real-world rewards (e.g., affiliation, positive
affect, etc.). This association-power motivates the adoption of
empathic behavior (Zaki, 2014;Ferguson et al., 2021). When a
teacher has a fixed mindset of the ability of students, they may
believe that the abilities of students are fixed and unchangeable.
The little reward results from empathy because no matter how
hard the teacher tries to practice, the students will not change
significantly. Therefore, as a primary outcome, teachers exhibit
less empathy in that devoid of an obvious reward, and empathy is
not significant for them (Ferguson et al., 2021).
Furthermore, previous studies indicate that there is a positive
relationship between empathic motivation and empathy (Zaki,
2014;Cameron et al., 2017a, 2019;Cameron, 2018;Weisz and
Zaki, 2018;Ferguson et al., 2020;Weisz et al., 2020). For example,
researchers suggested that empathy, a motivational phenomenon,
is a process of decision-making based on values (Cameron
et al., 2019;Ferguson et al., 2020). In addition, Keysers and
Gazzola (2014) suggest that empathic ability and propensity
affect empathy. In other words, empathic motivation may be an
important cause of empathic variations. Consistent with these
previous findings and theories, this study observed a significant
positive association between empathic motivation and teacher
empathy, including empathic concern, perspective-taking, and
fantasy for students while personal distress was not affected by
empathic motivation. To our knowledge, it is generally accepted
that empathy involves two information processes, namely, top-
down and bottom-up. The former refers to self-regulation, while
the latter refers to the automatic process (Decety and Lamm,
2006;Fan et al., 2011;Cameron et al., 2017b). Personal distress
is a component of bottom-up processes, which is a self-oriented
automatic aversive response to the suffering of other people
(Lopez-Perez et al., 2014), and may not be influenced by cognitive
control (i.e., motivation). Therefore, this is a possible explanation
for the fact that the personal distress of teacher empathy was not
significantly predicted by the empathic motivation of teachers
and the mediation effect for the relationship between theories of
Students’ development and teacher empathy-PD.
Limitations and Implications
This study has several limitations. First, this study employed
only self-report measures, which might be susceptible to response
bias (e.g., social desirability). Moreover, this study was cross-
sectional in design. The interpretations of the causal relationship
between theories of students development, empathic motivation
and empathy should be considered carefully. There is a need
for future studies to examine the results using an experimental
design. Second, although we try our best to recruit more Chinese
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 7
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
teachers, the number of subjects in this study is still relatively
small. Future research needs to adopt multiple approaches (for
example, a combination of online and offline surveys) to expand
the number of subjects. Third, some cross-cultural studies show
that there are cultural differences in empathy (Zhao et al., 2019,
2021). However, we did not collect information about the cultures
(i.e., teachers’ perceived professional ethics, social expectation,
and educational level) that may affect teacher empathy. The
relationships between cultures and teacher empathy could also be
a new direction for future research. Finally, although Cronbachs
alpha coefficient of both the questionnaire of theories of Students’
development and the empathic motivation of teachers was high,
and suggest a satisfactory internal consistency, we were unable
to evaluate the construct validity as the number of items was
too small to analyze statistically. To address these problems,
a multidimensional questionnaire about the theories of the
development of students and the empathic motivation of teachers
needs to be developed in future studies.
Despite these limitations, the current study primarily
contributed to investigating the relationship between the theories
of the development of students, empathic motivation and teacher
empathy and identifying an essential factor that shapes teacher
empathy. This study has an important theoretical implication.
Although the term “empathy” is significantly difficult to define
(Assmann and Detmers, 2016;Zembylas et al., 2020), it is
often viewed as an “ability” (Decety and Lamm, 2006;Shamay-
Tsoory et al., 2009;Batson, 2011;Lockwood et al., 2017).
Recently, however, research in the field of clinical psychology
has challenged this conclusion, suggesting that individuals with
mental disorders may not be impaired by their ability to
empathize, but rather a lack of empathic motivation (Meffert
et al., 2013;Gillespie et al., 2014). Furthermore, the theory of
dissociating the ability and propensity for empathy has been
proposed by Keysers and Gazzola (2014), which upholds that
empathic behavior is influenced not only by the capacity for
empathy but also by the motivation to empathize. Therefore, the
perspective of empathy research should shift from the “ability”
framework to the “ability motivation” framework. However,
there is still a lack of empirical research on the relationship
between empathic motivation and empathy; this study fills
this research gap.
Beyond its theoretical contribution, this study also has
practical implications. The empathy brain is plastic and
provides a theoretical basis for the cultivation of empathy
(Hein et al., 2016). Many empathic interventions focus
on developing people’s ability to empathize via experience-
based and expression-based interventions (Weisz and Zaki,
2017). However, empathy results from a combination of
empathic capacities and motivation (Keysers and Gazzola,
2014). Therefore, studies have increasingly begun to focus on
the intervention of empathic motivation via the change of
mindsets (Schumann et al., 2014;Weisz, 2018), norms (Weisz
et al., 2020), rewards (Ferguson et al., 2020), etc. The purpose
of these interventions is to engage the empathic motivation
of observers. However, the field of teacher empathy training
mainly focuses on improving the ability to be empathetic
(Jaber et al., 2018;Ronen, 2020). This study addresses the vital
gap in the empathic motivation intervention of teachers by
identifying the essential factor, which is the teachers’ beliefs about
students (i.e., theories of Students’ development), that shape
this psychological variable. Helping teachers improve empathic
motivation through psychological interventions (e.g., changing
the theories of Students’ development) should be an explicit goal
for teacher education programs.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the
influence of the theories of Students’ development on empathic
motivation and follow upon and expand the work of Schumann
et al. (2014) and Weisz et al. (2020), which found that
the empathy mindsets of individuals can significantly predict
empathic motivation.
CONCLUSION
Central to the teaching profession, teacher empathy can promote
the development of students and the professional growth of
teachers. Based on the motivation perspective, our finding may
suggest that the teachers’ beliefs of the ability of students could
predict empathic motivation and teacher empathy. Moreover,
empathic motivation plays a mediating role in the theories of
the development of students to expect teacher empathy, which
requires more research for validation. Providing new ideas and
methods to cultivate empathy for teachers, in this study, is a
primary contribution.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be
made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
ETHICS STATEMENT
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and
approved by the Zhejiang Normal University Review Board. The
patients/participants provided their written informed consent to
participate in this study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
YG designed the current study, collected the data, analyzed the
data, and wrote this manuscript. WL proposed the research idea
and demonstrate the feasibility of the method. SK participated
in language polishing to ensure manuscript quality. FC and GQ
joined the data analysis and the manuscript writing. All authors
contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
FUNDING
This work was supported by the National Natural Science
Foundation of China (No. 31871124) and Project supported
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 8
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
Open Research Fund of College of Teacher Education, Zhejiang
Normal University (No. jykf20012).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to all the teachers who took part in the current
study and would like to thank Junjie Zhang and Linqiu Xie for
assisting with data collection. We thank Associate Editor MA and
reviewers for their excellent feedback and guidance throughout
the review process.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.
2021.736656/full#supplementary-material
REFERENCES
Amicucci, G., Tempesta, D., Salfi, F., D’Atri, A., Viselli, L., Gennaro, L., et al. (2021).
The effect of 5 nights of sleep restriction on empathic propensity. J. Sleep Res.
30:e13325. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13325
Assmann, A., and Detmers, I. (2016). Empathy and its Limits. London: Palgrave
Macmillan UK.
Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An
investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism,
and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/B:
JADD.0000022607.19833.00
Batson, C. (2011). Altruism in humans: Oxford scholarship online. Altruism Hum.
10, 1–336. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195341065.001.0001
Bentler, P. M., and Chou, C. P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling.
Sociol. Methods Res. 16, 78–117. doi: 10.1177/0049124187016001004
Berkovich, I., and Eyal, O. (2015). Educational Leaders and Emotions: An
International Review of Empirical Evidence 1992-2012. Rev. Educat. Res. 85,
129–167. doi: 10.3102/0034654314550046
Blackwell, L., Trzesniewski, K., and Dweck, C. (2007). Implicit theories of
intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: a longitudinal
study and an intervention. Child Dev. 78, 246–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.
2007.00995.x
Bullough, R. V. (2019). Empathy, teaching dispositions, social justice and
teacher education. Teachers Teaching 25, 507–522. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2019.
1602518
Cadima, J., Leal, T., and Burchinal, M. (2010). The quality of teacher-student
interactions: Associations with first graders’ academic and behavioral outcomes.
J. School Psychol. 48, 457–482. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2010.09.001
Cameron, C. D. (2018). Motivating empathy: Three methodological
recommendations for mapping empathy. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass
12:e12418. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12418
Cameron, C. D., Hutcherson, C. A., Ferguson, A. M., Scheffer, J. A., Hadjiandreou,
E., and Inzlicht, M. (2019). Empathy is hard work: People choose to avoid
empathy because of its cognitive costs. J. Exp. Psychol. General 148, 962–976.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000595
Cameron, C. D., Spring, V. L., and Todd, A. R. (2017b). The empathy impulse:
A multinomial model of intentional and unintentional empathy for pain. Am.
Psychol. Associat. 17, 395–411. doi: 10.1037/emo0000266
Cameron, C. D., Cunningham, W., Saunders, B., and Inzlicht, M. (2017a). The
ends of empathy: Constructing empathy from value-based choice. [Preprint].
doi: 10.31234/osf.io/d99bp
Chen, W., Feng, H., Lv, C., and Lu, J. (2018). Relationships between empathy,
gender, and testosterone levels in older adults. Soc. Behav. Personal. 46, 1895–
1908. doi: 10.2224/sbp.6884
Cooper, B. (2004). Empathy, interaction, and caring: teachers roles in a constrained
environment. Pastoral Care Educat. 22:00299.x. doi: 10.1111/j.0264-3944.2004.
00299.x
Cooper, B. (2010). In search of profound empathy in learning relationships:
Understanding the mathematics of moral learning environments. J. Moral
Educat. 39, 79–99. doi: 10.1080/03057240903528717
Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in
empathy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 10:85.
Decety, J., and Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of
social neuroscience. ScientificWorldJournal 6, 1146–1163. doi: 10.1100/tsw.20
06.221
Dweck, C. (1996). Capturing the dynamic nature of personality. J. Res. Personal.
30, 348–362. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1996.0024
Dweck, C. (2012). Mindsets and human nature: promoting change in the middle
east, the schoolyard, the racial divide, and willpower. Am. Psychol. 67, 614–622.
doi: 10.1037/a0029783
Dweck, C., Chiu, C. Y., and Hong, Y. Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role
in judgments and reactions: a word from two perspectives. Psychol. Inquiry 6,
267–285. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0604_1
Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., Greck, M., de, and Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a
core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 903–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009
Fang, Z. (1996). A review of research on teacher beliefs and practices. Educat. Res.
38, 47–65. doi: 10.1080/0013188960380104
Ferguson, A. M., Cameron, C. D., and Inzlicht, M. (2020). Motivational effects
on empathic choices. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 90:104010. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.
104010
Ferguson, A., Cameron, D., and Inzlicht, M. (2021). When does empathy feel good?
Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 39, 125–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.011
Field, A. (2013). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. London: Sage.
Fives, H., and Buehl, M. (2012). Spring cleaning for the “messy” construct
of teachers’ beliefs: What are they? Which has been examined? What
can they tell us? Am. Psychol. Associat. 2, 471–499. doi: 10.1037/1327
4-019
Gandhi, A. U., Dawood, S., and Schroder, H. S. (2021). Empathy mind-
set moderates the association between low empathy and social aggression.
J. Interpers. Violence 36, 1679–1697. doi: 10.1177/0886260517747604
Gillespie, S., Mccleery, J., and Oberman, L. (2014). Spontaneous versus deliberate
vicarious representations: Different routes to empathy in psychopathy and
autism. Brain J. Neurol. 137:awt364. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt364
Goroshit, M., and Hen, M. (2016). Teachers’ empathy: Can it be predicted by self-
efficacy? Teachers Teaching 22, 805–818. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2016.1185818
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional
Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: The Guilford
Press.
Hein, G., Engelmann, J. B., Vollberg, M. C., and Tobler, P. N. (2016). How
learning shapes the empathic brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 113, 80–85.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514539112
Heyder, A., Weidinger, A. F., Cimpian, A., and Steinmayr, R. (2020). Teachers’
belief that math requires innate ability predicts lower intrinsic motivation
among low-achieving students. Learn. Instruct. 65:101220. doi: 10.1016/j.
learninstruc.2019.101220
Holper, L., Goldin, A. P., Shalóm, D. E., Battro, A. M., Wolf, M., and Sigman, M.
(2013). The teaching and the learning brain: A cortical hemodynamic marker of
teacher-student interactions in the Socratic dialog. Int. J. Educat. Res. 59, 1–10.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2013.02.002
Huang, X., Li, W., Sun, B., Haide, C., and Davis, M. (2011). The validation of the
interpersonal reactivity index for Chinese teachers from primary and middle
schools. J. Psychoeducat. Assess. 30, 194–204. doi: 10.1177/0734282911410588
Jaber, L. Z., Southerland, S., and Dake, F. (2018). Cultivating epistemic empathy
in pre-service teacher education. Teaching Teacher Educat. 72, 13–23. doi: 10.
1016/j.tate.2018.02.009
Jordan, A., Glenn, C., and McGhie-Richmond, D. (2010). The Supporting Effective
Teaching (SET) project: The relationship of inclusive teaching practices to
teachers’ beliefs about disability and ability and their roles as teachers. Teaching
Teacher Educat. 26, 259–266. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2009.03.005
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
fpsyg-12-736656 November 10, 2021 Time: 12:23 # 9
Ge et al. Shape Teacher Empathy From the Perspective of Motivation
Kagan, D. (1992). The implication of research on teacher belief. Educat. Psychol.27,
65–90. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2701_6
Keysers, C., and Gazzola, V. (2014). Dissociating the ability and propensity for
empathy. Trends Cognit. Sci. 18, 163–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.011
Laisney, M., Bon, L., Guiziou, C., Daluzeau, N., Eustache, F., and Desgranges, B.
(2013). Cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in mild to moderate Alzheimer’s
disease. J. Neuropsychol. 7, 107–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-6653.2012.02038.x
Larina, G., and Markina, V. (2020). Hidden mechanisms of differentiation:
teachers’ beliefs about student diversity. J. Mathemat. Teacher Educat. 23,
463–482. doi: 10.1007/s10857-019- 09436-1
Lavigne, A. L. (2014). Beginning teachers who stay: Beliefs about students. Teaching
Teacher Educat. 39, 31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2013.12.002
Lockwood, P. L., Ang, Y.-S., Husain, M., and Crockett, M. J. (2017). Individual
differences in empathy are associated with apathy-motivation. Sci. Rep. 7:17293.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017- 17415-w
Lopez-Perez, B., Carrera, P., Ambrona, T., and Oceja, L. (2014). Testing the
qualitative differences between empathy and personal distress: Measuring core
affect and self-orientation. Soc. Sci. J. 51, 676–680. doi: 10.1016/j.soscij.2014.08.
001
Main, A., Walle, E. A., Kho, C., and Halpern, J. (2017). The interpersonal functions
of empathy: A relational perspective. Emot. Rev. 9, 358–366. doi: 10.1177/
1754073916669440
Mansour, N. (2009). Science teachers beliefs and practices: Issues, implications,
and research agenda. Int. J. Environ. Sci. Educat. 4, 25–48.
Meffert, H., Gazzola, V., Boer, J., Bartels, A., and Keysers, C. (2013). Reduced
spontaneous but relatively normal deliberate vicarious representations in
psychopathy. Brain J. Neurol. 136, 2550–2562. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt190
Meyers, S., Rowell, K., Wells, M., and Smith, B. C. (2019). Teacher empathy: A
model of empathy for teaching for student success. Coll. Teaching 67, 160–168.
doi: 10.1080/87567555.2019.1579699
Preston, S. D., Waal, F. B. M. (2017). Only the PAM explains the personalized
nature of empathy. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18:769. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.140
Ronen, I. K. (2020). Empathy awareness among pre-service teachers: the case of
the incorrect use of the intuitive rule “same a–same b.”. Int. J. Sci. Mathemat.
Educat. 18, 183–201. doi: 10.1007/s10763-019-09952-9
Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., and aan het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults
with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 1–16.
doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
Schumann, K., Zaki, J., and Dweck, C. S. (2014). Addressing the empathy deficit:
Beliefs about the malleability of empathy predict effortful responses when
empathy is challenging. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 107, 475–493. doi: 10.1037/
a0036738
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for
empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in
inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain J. Neurol.
132(Pt 3), 617–627. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn279
Shimoni, H., Weizman, A., Hegesh, R., and Raviv, A. (2012). Theory of mind,
the severity of autistic symptoms and parental correlates in children and
adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Psychiatry Res. 197, 85–89. doi: 10.1016/j.
psychres.2012.02.021
Stojiljkovi´
c, S., Djigi´
c, G., and Zlatkovi´
c, B. (2012). Empathy and teachers’ roles.
Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 69, 960–966. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.021
Sun, B., Luo, Z., Zhang, W., Li, W., and Li, X. (2017). Age-related differences in
affective and cognitive empathy: self-report and performance-based evidence.
Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 25, 1–18. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1360835
Swan, P., and Riley, P. (2015). Social connection: empathy and mentalization
for teachers. Pastoral Care Educat. 33, 220–233. doi: 10.1080/02643944.2015.
1094120
Tettegah, S., and Anderson, C. J. (2007). Pre-service teachers’ empathy and
cognitions: Statistical analysis of text data by graphical models. Contemp.
Educat. Psychol. 32, 48–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.010
Waller, R., Wagner, N. J., Barstead, M. G., Subar, A., Petersen, J. L., Hyde, J. S.,
et al. (2020). A meta-analysis of the associations between callous-unemotional
traits and empathy, prosociality, and guilt. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 75:101809. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101809
Wang, J., and Yang, M. (2021). A study on t he relationshipbetween preser vice stem
teachers’ beliefs about migrant students and teachers roles in chinese urban
schools. Educat. Urban Soc. 53, 206–230. doi: 10.1177/0013124520927673
Warren, C. A. (2018). Empathy, teacher dispositions, and preparation for
culturally responsive pedagogy. J. Teacher Educat. 69, 169–183. doi: 10.1177/
0022487117712487
Weisz, E. (2018). Building empathy through psychological interventions. Stanford:
Stanford University.
Weisz, E., and Cikara, M. (2021). Strategic regulation of empathy. Trends Cognit.
Sci. 25, 213–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.12.002
Weisz, E., and Zaki, J. (2017). Empathy building interventions: A review of existing
work and suggestions for future directions. Oxford Handb. Compass. Sci. 2017,
205–217.
Weisz, E., and Zaki, J. (2018). Motivated empathy: A social neuroscience
perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 24, 67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.05.005
Weisz, E., Ong, D. C., Carlson, R. W., and Zaki, J. (2020). Building empathy
through motivation-based interventions. Emotion [Prperint]. doi: 10.1037/
emo0000929
Wubbels, T., and Brekelmans, M. (2005). Two decades of research on teacher-
student relationships in class. Int. J. Educat. Res. 43, 6–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.
2006.03.003
Yang, C.-C., Khalifa, N., and Völlm, B. (2018). The effects of repetitive transcranial
magnetic stimulation on empathy: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Psychol. Med. 48, 737–750. doi: 10.1017/S003329171700232X
Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., and Dweck, C. S. (2013). An implicit theories
of personality intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to
victimization and exclusion. Child Dev. 84, 970–988. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12003
Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: A motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1608–1647.
doi: 10.1037/a0037679
Zembylas, M., Loukaides, L., and Antoniou, P. (2020). Teachers’ understandings
of empathy in teaching about the holocaust in Cyprus: The emotional risks of
identification and the disruptive potential of ’empathic unsettlement.’. Teaching
Teacher Educat. 89:103007. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2019.103007
Zhao, Q., Neumann, D. L., Cao, X., Baron-Cohen, S., Sun, X., Cao, Y., et al. (2018).
Validation of the empathy quotient in mainland china. J. Personal. Assess. 100,
333–342. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2017.1324458
Zhao, Q., Neumann, D. L., Cao, Y., Baron-Cohen, S., Yan, C., Chan, R. C. K., et al.
(2019). Culture-sex interaction and the self-report empathy in Australians and
mainland Chinese. Front. Psychol. 10:396. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00396
Zhao, Q., Neumann, D. L., Yan, C., Djekic, S., and Shum, D. H. K. (2021). Culture,
sex, and group-bias in trait and state empathy. Front. Psychol. 12:561930. doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2021.561930
Zhao, Q., Ren, Q., Sun, Y., Wan, L., and Hu, L. (2020). Impact factors of empathy
in mainland Chinese youth. Front. Psychol. 11:688. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.
00688
Zhu, J., Wang, X. Q., He, X., Hu, Y.-Y., Li, F., Liu, M.-F., et al. (2019). Affective and
cognitive empathy in pre-teachers with strong or weak professional identity: An
ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:175. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00175
Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a
potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors
and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of
the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in
this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or
endorsed by the publisher.
Copyright © 2021 Ge, Li, Chen, Kayani and Qin. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academicpractice. No
use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with theseterms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9November 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 736656
... It can increase teachers' understanding and appreciation of the situation of particular groups [27], which in turn influences teachers' attitudes toward special groups [28] and, ultimately, their attitudes toward inclusive education. Furthermore, it has been stated that empathy affects teachers' willingness and teaching motivation [29], subsequently affecting their attitudes towards children with special needs and inclusive education [30]. Based on these arguments, this study proposed the below hypothesis: ...
... Teaching motivation is an intrinsic drive for individuals to engage in education, and people with high levels of empathy are more likely to be motivated to engage in education, leading them to be more engaged and passionate about their work [29]. To a certain extent, it can predict teachers' enthusiasm for the profession and their tendency to improve their professionalism [31]. ...
... The results of this study validated research hypotheses 2, 3, and 4. This study supports the previous findings [29,64,65]. Research indicates that individuals with higher levels of empathy are more capable of understanding others' situations and emotions and thus feel more socially responsible and actively engage in social activities that promote social justice [66]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recently, inclusive education has become the direction of special education development. Inclusive education requires that teachers’ educational philosophies and approaches meet the needs of all students. Since pre-service teachers are future educators in training, it is vital to investigate their perspectives on inclusive education. Pre-service teachers’ attitudes, ability and views of inclusive education have an impact on the success of inclusive education practice. However, little literature has examined the influence of psychological mechanism of pre-service teachers toward inclusive education, i.e., the impact of empathy on pre-service teachers’ attitudes toward inclusive education and the mediating effects of teaching motivation and inclusive education efficacy toward the aforementioned relationship. Thus, this study employed Stimulus-Organism-Response’s (S-O-R) model to underpin the research framework that examines the relationship between pre-service teachers’ empathy and attitudes toward inclusive education and the mediating effects of teaching motivation and inclusive education efficacy in the Chinese context. Quantitative data through survey questionnaires was collected from 480 Chinese pre-service teachers and analysed using PLS-SEM. Findings suggested that pre-service teachers’ empathy directly predicted teaching motivation, inclusive education efficacy, and attitudes toward inclusive education. Teaching motivation and inclusive education efficacy together play a chain mediating role in the relationship between empathy and pre-service teachers’ attitudes toward inclusive education. The results of this study offer a theoretical framework that explains in detail the psychological mechanisms underlying the effects of pre-service teachers’ empathy, teaching motivation, and inclusive education efficacy on attitudes towards inclusive education. The research findings also provide practical guidance for the professional development of teachers, talent cultivation, and the development of inclusive education in higher education.
... 41 Teaching is a social interaction between pupils and teachers, 42,43 and teachers have mindsets about both themselves and their students (eg students' abilities). 44,45 Intriguingly, these mindsets have motivational effects. Ge et al divided the theories of student abilities according to two different mindsets. ...
... These researchers found that teachers' mindsets regarding students' abilities could significantly predict teacher empathy and that empathic motivation mediated this relationship. 45 According to the empathic motivation framework theory and empathic propensity-ability dissociating theory, empathy is often a motivating phenomenon, and empathic motivation is a significant predictor of empathy. 34,35 Thus, we propose that kindergarten teachers' mindsets toward children could predict their compassion fatigue and that motivation for teacher empathy plays a mediating role in this relationship. ...
... This study investigated the relationships among Chinese kindergarten teachers' mindsets toward children, compassion fatigue, motivation for teacher empathy, job stress, and social support. Building on previous studies, 34,35,40,45,47,50,53 a moderated mediating model was proposed (see Figure 1) to assess the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 1: Kindergarten teachers' compassion fatigue is negatively predicted by their mindsets toward children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Kindergarten teachers who empathize with toddlers experience a great risk of burnout and emotional disturbance. This is referred to as compassion fatigue, in which teachers’ empathy experience is reduced. This study proposed a moderated mediation model to identify the risks of compassion fatigue and its protective factors for developing evidence-based clinical interventions. Methods In this cross-sectional study, self-report measures were administered to 1049 kindergarten teachers to observe their mindsets toward children, motivation for teacher empathy, job stress, social support, and compassion fatigue. The PROCESS macro (SPSS 23.0) was used to assess the moderated mediation model. Results The results demonstrated that motivation for teacher empathy mediated the negative relationship between kindergarten teachers’ mindsets toward children and compassion fatigue. Moreover, job stress and social support moderated the relationship between kindergarten teachers’ mindsets toward children and motivation for teacher empathy. However, this effect was not observed in the negative relationship between kindergarten teachers’ mindsets toward children and compassion fatigue. Conclusion The proposed moderated mediation model was found to be valid. Furthermore, the study findings have practical implications for developing evidence-based interventions for addressing kindergarten teachers’ compassion fatigue.
... /fpsyg. . study showed that teachers who exhibit a malleable mindset with respect to the ability of students also exhibited greater empathic motivation, thus leading to a stronger experience of empathy (Ge et al., 2021). Additionally, some studies have shown that interventions based on empathic motivation can effectively enhance individuals' empathic responses (Hess et al., 2017;. ...
... Empathic motivation reflects an individual's willingness to empathize with others. Three items adapted from the research of Ge et al. (2021) ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Teacher empathy has been proven to be highly relevant to both the educational process and outcomes. Therefore, exploring its influencing factors and developing effective cultivation strategies are highly importance. The present study aimed to examine the effects of teacher-perceived student likability on teacher empathy and to further explore the role of empathy motivations. Methods In Study 1, 138 primary and secondary school teachers (mean age = 38.0 ± 8.8 years) reported their anticipated emotional exhaustion, empathic motivation, and empathic reaction when they read a text that described a negative event involving either a disliked or liked student. In Study 2, another 221 primary and middle school teachers (mean age = 34.8 ± 10.1 years) took part in an intervention designed to activate empathic motivation. Results The results of study 1 showed that teachers felt less empathy in the former context. In addition, anticipated emotional exhaustion and empathic motivation serially mediated the effect of teacher-perceived student likability on teacher empathy. The results of Study 2 showed that teachers' empathy toward disliked students improved and that the likable empathy bias was eliminated when empathic motivation was primed. Discussion These findings suggest that empathic motivation plays a crucial role in likable empathy bias among teachers in that it not only functions as a key mechanism underlying this bias but also emerges as a potential pathway for mitigating such bias. Our research has important theoretical and practical significance.
... Teacher empathy was assessed by the Chinese version of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI-C) (Huang et al. 2012). In line with previous studies (Ge et al. 2021), we adapted the statements of IRI-C to suit the teacher role, such as empathic target. For example, "I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the other person's point of view" was revised to "I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the students' point of view." ...
Article
The field of education has long emphasized the importance of kindergarten teachers perceiving and understanding students' thoughts and feelings and expressing concern, commonly referred to as teacher empathy. However, little work has focused on the “cost of caring,” such as compassion fatigue, particularly among kindergarten teachers, which poses a significant threat to their physical and mental health. To identify the factors that shape compassion fatigue and explore the mechanisms underlying these relationships, a serial mediation model was proposed involving mindsets of empathy, teacher empathy, professional identity, and compassion fatigue. In the present study, self‐report measures were administered to 2256 kindergarten teachers to assess the major variables. The serial mediation model was evaluated using the Process macro software, and the results showed that compassion fatigue can be significantly and negatively predicted by mindsets of empathy, with teacher empathy and professional identity mediating this relationship among kindergarten teachers. These findings substantially contribute to the understanding of compassion fatigue among kindergarten teachers and provide valuable implications for its interventions.
... Further, psychological resilience was associated with empathetic tendencies in teachers of refugee children (Üzar-Özçetin et al., 2022). Empathetic skills motivate teachers to problem-solve collaboratively and sensitively, strengthening the teacher-student relationship (Ge et al., 2021), and improving attitudes (Navarro-Mateu, 2019). Therefore, future studies could investigate factors associated with psychological resilience in teachers who have experienced personal trauma. ...
Article
Full-text available
Childhood trauma can result in developmental and psychosocial problems leaving teachers struggling to manage the effects of students’ trauma and potentially leading to increased burnout. The present study investigated whether teachers’ attitudes towards teaching trauma-affected students and prior experience with trauma predicted teacher burnout. Five types of prior experience were informed by the multidimensional model of attitude strength: the extent and valence (i.e., how favourable or unfavourable the experience was) of direct teaching experience, the extent and valence of personal experience, and the extent of indirect experience (trauma-training). The study investigated whether the relationships between prior experiences and burnout were mediated by teachers’ attitudes, controlling for teacher age. Australian mainstream teachers (N = 536) were recruited to an online survey through snowball sampling on social media. Results showed that attitudes significantly mediated the relationships between all experience variables with burnout, except for the extent of personal experience. More favourable attitudes were predicted by more direct experience (contrary to the hypothesised direction) and indirect experience (as hypothesised). Regarding valence of experience, exploratory analyses found more favourable direct and personal experiences predicted more favourable attitudes. Supporting the hypotheses, all mediations found more favourable attitudes predicted less burnout, while more personal experience predicted greater burnout. These cross-sectional findings suggest that greater experience teaching trauma-affected students, trauma-training, and fostering favourable perceptions of teachers’ personal trauma may protect teachers from burnout. Future research using longitudinal designs is needed to support causal effects between teachers’ experiences, attitudes, and burnout.
... Professional collaboration among teachers also helps create an empathetic relationship among teaching staff which translates to their relationships or interactions with students (Campbell, 2022;McGowan et al., 2021). Lastly, teacher motivation and job satisfaction in a way dictate how they interact with students and teaching quality (Ge et al., 2021;Parchomiuk, 2019). ...
Chapter
Students with disabilities (SWDs) are among the most marginalized group of learners who may struggle to acquire the knowledge and skills needed to provide authentic solutions to real-life problems. As a result, policymakers and educators have recognized rethinking design for inclusiveness as a means to provide support for and meet the needs of such learner cohorts. This rethink has resulted in the adoption or adaption of the design-based learning (DBL) approach which seeks to improve the learning experience of SWDs through five iterative design thinking (DT) processes: empathize, define, ideate, prototype, and test. It is important to note that the prevailing literature has largely taken a generic look at the application of the DT processes in inclusive settings. This chapter thus critically focuses on the first process of DT (empathize) to facilitate deep engagement between designers and SWDs as a strategy for reducing instructional barriers and aiding the development of their problem-solving skills. The theoretical analysis of the literature concludes with a proposed model which highlights four key factors (environmental, classroom, teacher, and student factors) as enablers for instructors to demonstrate empathy in inclusive settings. Moving forward, we recommend a participatory design approach through the consideration of the four interrelated factors in the proposed model when constructing the educational process of SWDs. Also, a supportive and disability-friendly learning environment should be created for SWDs in terms of architectural and instructional design.
... Cooper [20] mentions that teacher empathy is vital to successful learning; he also argues that empathetic relationships between students and teachers create a better and more supportive classroom, which in turn improves the learning process. Based on the motivation perspective, Ge et al. [21] findings might indicate that the teachers' beliefs of the ability of students could predict empathic motivation and teacher empathy. O'Connell Schmakel [24] demonstrates that teacher empathy is related to motivational support and that it can improve student engagement in academics. ...
... The Chinese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI-C) [31], was revised and normalized for a Chinese sample to assess empathy (Likert 5-point, from 0 to 4) [32]. This scale version is widely used in Chinese culture [32,54,55]. In the current study, the Cronbach's α value for the four subscales (i.e., EC, PT, FS, and PD) was .51, ...
Article
Full-text available
Empathy involves both empathic ability and empathic motivation. An important topic has been how to measure empathic ability and motivation simultaneously in both clinical and non-clinical samples and across different cultures. The Empathy Components Questionnaire (ECQ) is a self-report questionnaire that measures empathic ability and motivation in a questionnaire. The current study aimed to validate the Mandarin Chinese version of the ECQ (ECQ-Chinese) in three Chinese samples. In study 1, a total of 538 Chinese participants (Sample 1) completed the ECQ-Chinese via an online survey, and existing measures of empathy and related constructs which were used for criterion validity. In study 2, a total of 104 participants (Sample 2) were recruited again from sample 1 and completed the ECQ-Chinese three weeks later to investigate test-retest reliability. In study 3, a further 324 participants (Sample 3) completed the ECQ-Chinese for confirmatory factor analysis. The results showed that the ECQ-Chinese has a good internal consistency reliability, split-half reliability, and criterion validity (Study 1), and a good test-retest reliability (Study 2). Further, Study 3 found that a 22-item ECQ-Chinese consisting of five subscales had a good construct validity, convergence validity and discriminate validity, demonstrating it to be a suitable tool for the measurement of empathic ability and motivation in Chinese samples and to carry out cross-cultural studies of empathy and its components.
Article
Full-text available
Teachers’ emotional intelligence is one of the prime indicators of their improved social skill and social competence at workplace. Due to rapid changes in the current times, the role of school teachers has become very challenging. Different factors affect their teaching effectiveness. Emotional intelligence is one such important factor which has been less explored in the context of elementary education. The present study examines how emotional intelligence mediates the relations of social skills and social competence of teachers. A total of 210 teachers participated in the study from elementary schools of Punjab, Pakistan. A quantitative measure was administered to collect data on five-point Likert type scale. Mean score Pearson’s r, regression and path analysis were conducted to analyze the data. The results showed a high mean score which was above 4.00 for teachers’ emotional intelligence, social skills and social competence. The correlation between all the three variables indicated social skills having significant correlation with social competence (r value=.766 and p< 0.000), social skills with emotional intelligence (r value = .802 and p<0.000) and social competence with emotional intelligence (r value =.805 and p<0.000). The study showed that emotional intelligence partially mediated the relationships between social skills and social competency. Based on the results it is concluded that emotional intelligence is a strong predictor of social skills and social competence of teachers in elementary education context.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Empathy is an attribute that plays an essential role in the dentist–patient therapeutic relationship, clinical care, and treatment adherence, along with providing other benefits. The main objective of this research was to establish the validity, reliability, and invariance of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy and then characterize the empathy levels of students and teachers at a dental school. Materials and Methods: An observational and cross-sectional study analyzed a sample of undergraduate students and professors from the Universidad Andrés Bello Faculty of Dentistry (Chile) (n = 1727 and n = 267, respectively). The Empathy Scale for Health Professionals (HP) and the same scale for students (HPS) were applied. Results: The Jefferson Empathy Scale presents adequate psychometric properties. The empathy measure has adequate reliability and construct validity, confirming a fit of the three-factor empathy model to the data. The measurement is invariant within the university campus, the sex of the student, and between teacher and student. Teachers present greater empathy than students except in the Perspective Adoption dimension. Conclusions: The Jefferson Empathy Scale is reliable, valid, and invariant among Chilean dental students and professors. Students do not differ from their professors in the cognitive component of empathy, but they present a lower score in the affective component and global empathy. It is inferred that students can develop the affective component of empathy in their interactions with their professors, increasing their overall empathy. Understanding and fostering empathy in dental students and professors can significantly improve patient care and treatment adherence and increase patient and dentist satisfaction.
Article
Full-text available
Empathy is sharing and understanding others’ emotions. Recently, researchers identified a culture–sex interaction effect in empathy. This phenomenon has been largely ignored by previous researchers. In this study, the culture–sex interaction effect was explored with a cohort of 129 participants (61 Australian Caucasians and 68 Chinese Hans) using both self-report questionnaires (i.e., Empathy Quotient and Interpersonal Reactivity Index) and computer-based empathy tasks. In line with the previous findings, the culture–sex interaction effect was observed for both trait empathy (i.e., the generalized characteristics of empathy, as examined by the self-report questionnaires) and state empathy (i.e., the on-spot reaction of empathy for a specific stimulus, as evaluated by the computer-based tasks). Moreover, in terms of state empathy, the culture–sex interaction effect further interacted with stimulus traits (i.e., stimulus ethnicity, stimulus sex, or stimulus emotion) and resulted in three- and four-way interactions. Follow-up analyses of these higher-order interactions suggested that the phenomena of ethnic group bias and sex group favor in empathy varied among the four culture–sex participant groups (i.e., Australian female, Australian male, Chinese female, and Chinese male). The current findings highlighted the dynamic nature of empathy (i.e., its sensitivity toward both participant traits and stimulus features). Furthermore, the newly identified interaction effects in empathy deserve more investigation and need to be verified with other Western and Asian populations.
Article
Full-text available
Empathy is associated with adaptive social and emotional outcomes; as such, a crucial outstanding question is whether it can be bolstered in ways that make practical differences in people’s lives. Most empathy-building efforts address one’s ability to empathize, increasing empathy by training skills like perspective taking. However, empathy is more than the ability to share and understand others’ feelings; it also reflects underlying motives that drive people to experience or avoid it. As such, another strategy for increasing empathy could focus on shifting relevant motives. Here we explored this idea, leveraging two intervention techniques (mindsets and social norms) to increase motivation to empathize. Two hundred ninety-two first-year college students were randomly assigned to one of three intervention conditions—malleable mindset, social norms, or a combination of the two—or a control condition. Eight weeks later, participants in the intervention conditions endorsed stronger beliefs about empathy’s malleability and exhibited greater empathic accuracy when rating others’ positive emotions as compared to the control condition. They also reported having made a greater number of friends since starting college. The interventions did not affect outcomes related to intergroup processes or empathic accuracy when rating others’ negative emotions, indicating a boundary condition for these interventions. This experiment underscores the potential of motivation-based empathy interventions to generate positive, real-world impact.
Article
Full-text available
Empathy was investigated in 592 Mainland Chinese youth using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Participants’ empathy-related information covering demographic traits, emotional wellness, as well as academic and social problems were recorded. Results of Classification and Regression Tree (CART) analysis showed that emotional empathy, cognitive empathy, and empathy-related personal distress was impacted by inherited traits (e.g., sex), acquired traits (e.g., study major), and a combination of both aspects, respectively. Moreover, empathy was found to be higher in youth in a vulnerable social position (i.e., outlander, female, and ethnic minority) than those in a dominant one (i.e., local, male, and ethnic majority). It was also found that personal distress, rather than empathy, was significantly correlated with academic, social, and emotional problems in the youth cohort. Hence, the current study provided an innovative observation of the relationships between empathy, personal distress, ethnicity, social vulnerability, wellness, study major, and other key characteristics in Mainland Chinese youth.
Article
Empathy has many benefits. When we are willing to empathize, we are more likely to act prosocially (and receive help from others in the future), to have satisfying relationships, and to be viewed as moral actors. Moreover, empathizing in certain contexts can actually feel good, regardless of the content of the emotion itself—for example, we might feel a sense of connectedness after empathizing with and supporting a grieving friend. Does this feeling come from empathy itself, or from its real and implied consequences? We suggest that the rewards that flow from empathy confound our experience of it, and that the pleasant feelings associated with engaging empathy are extrinsically tied to the results of some action, not to the experience of empathy itself. When we observe people’s decisions related to empathy in the absence of these acquired rewards, as we can in experimental settings, empathy appears decidedly less pleasant.
Article
Literature supports the existence of a significant relationship between sleep quality/quantity and empathy. However, empathic ability and empathic propensity are distinct constructs. Expression of empathic propensity depends on the subjective cognitive costs attributed to the empathic experience. Studies on the effects of the experimental reduction in sleep duration on empathic behaviour are still lacking. Therefore, we investigated the consequences of 5 consecutive nights of sleep restriction on empathic propensity. A total of 42 university students (mean [SEM] age 24.09 [0.65] years; 22 females) underwent a cross-over design consisting of 5 consecutive nights of regular sleep and 5 consecutive nights of sleep restriction with a maximum of 5 hr sleep/night. After each condition, all participants were evaluated using the Empathy Selection Task, a new test assessing the motivated avoidance of empathy for its associated cognitive costs. The results showed different effects of sleep restriction depending on the habitual way of responding in the empathic context. Participants with baseline high levels of empathic propensity reduced their empathic propensity after prolonged sleep restriction. Differently, participants who tended to avoid empathising already in the habitual sleep condition maintained their empathic behaviour unchanged after sleep curtailment. In conclusion, inter-individual variability should be taken into account when evaluating the effects of sleep restriction on empathic propensity. People with habitual higher tendency to empathise could choose to avoid empathic experience following several consecutive nights of inadequate sleep.
Article
Empathy is an integral part of socioemotional well-being, but recent research has highlighted some of its downsides. Here we examine literature that establishes when, how much, and what aspects of empathy promote specific outcomes. After reviewing a theoretical framework that characterizes empathy as a suite of separable components, we examine evidence showing how dissociations of these components affect important socioemotional outcomes and describe emerging evidence suggesting that these components can be independently and deliberately regulated. Finally, we advocate for an approach to a multicomponent view of empathy that accounts for the interrelations among components. This perspective advances scientific conceptualization of empathy and offers suggestions for tailoring empathy to help people realize their social, emotional, and occupational goals.
Article
Empathy often feels automatic, but variations in empathic responding suggest that, at least some of the time, empathy is affected by one's motivation to empathize in any particular circumstance. Here, we show that people can be motivated to engage in (or avoid) empathy-eliciting situations with strangers, and that these decisions are driven by subjective value-based estimations of the costs (e.g., cognitive effort) and benefits (e.g., social reward) inherent to empathizing. Across seven experiments (overall N = 1348), and replicating previous work (Cameron et al., 2019), we found a robust empathy avoidance effect. We also find support for the hypothesis that individuals can be motivated to opt-in to situations requiring empathy that they would otherwise avoid. Participants were more likely to opt into empathy-eliciting situations if 1) they were incentivized monetarily for doing so (Experiments 1a and 1b), and 2) if a more familiar and liked empathy target was available (Experiments 2a and 2b). Framing empathy as explicitly related to one's moral character and reputation did not motivate participants to engage in empathy (Experiment 3a and 3c), though these null results may be due to a weak manipulation. These findings suggest that empathy can be motivated in multiple ways, and is a process driven by context-specific value-based decision making.
Article
The survival and development of migrant students in urban areas were always the focus of all sectors of society. Science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) subjects are most likely to cause learning difficulties for migrant students, and the beliefs of STEM teachers about migrant students and their role closely affect classroom teaching and after-school support and tutoring. This study focuses on 268 STEM-oriented preservice teachers majoring in elementary education in a normal university in Beijing, China. A questionnaire investigation has been conducted on their beliefs about migrant students’ and teachers’ role in urban elementary schools with metaphor method. The results show that there are significant differences among all the preservice STEM teachers at grade levels, and most of them hold the reality of development and the possibility of development beliefs about migrant students, and the facilitating orientation beliefs about teachers’ role as well. Grade factors are influenced by their curriculum, and beliefs about migrant students show an excessive trend from existence orientation to development orientation with the increasing grade levels. There is a significant correlation between beliefs about migrant students’ and teachers’ role, and preservice teachers with the reality of development beliefs about students are more inclined to the facilitating orientation beliefs about teachers’ role.