ArticlePDF Available

The Role of Face Masks in the Recognition of Emotions by Preschool Children

Authors:

Abstract

Since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, health policy requires staff working in preschool education to wear face masks. This has prompted worries about the ability of young children to recognize emotions and the possible impact on their development. Without face masks, preschoolers aged 36 to 72 months had a rate of correctly identified emotions on pictures of 11.8% to 13.1%.¹ Recent studies using photographs with digitally added face masks showed that participants had worse emotional recognition of the images with face masks; the first of these² tested preschoolers on a smartphone at home, and the second³ tested children aged 7 to 13 years. We therefore aimed to study the role of actual face masks on the recognition of joy, anger, and sadness in younger preschool children.
1
Title: The role of facemasks in the recognition of emotions by preschool children
Authors:
Juliane Schneider, MD, Developmental Unit, Neonatology, Department Woman-mother-child,
University hospital Lausanne, Switzerland
Vania Sandoz, MSc, Institute of Higher Education and Research in Healthcare, University of
Lausanne, Switzerland.
Lucile Equey, MD, Developmental Unit, Neonatology, Department Woman-mother-child, University
hospital Lausanne, Switzerland
Joanne Williams-Smith, MD, Developmental Unit, Neonatology, Department Woman-mother-child,
University hospital Lausanne, Switzerland
Antje Horsch, PhD, Prof., Institute of Higher Education and Research in Healthcare, University of
Lausanne, Switzerland.
Introduction: Since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, health policy requires staff working in
preschool education to wear facemasks. This has prompted worries about the ability of young children
to recognize emotions, and the possible impact on their development. Without facemasks, pre-
schoolers aged 36- 72 months had correct responses rate between 11.8 % and 13.1%[1]. Recent studies
using photos with digitally added facemasks showed worse recognition with facemasks, the first tested
pre-schoolers on a smartphone at home [2] and the second tested 7- 13- year old children [3] . We
therefore aimed to study the role of actual facemasks on the recognition of joy, anger and sadness in
younger preschool children.
Methods: The primary outcome of this cross-sectional experimental study was the rate of correct
responses using pictures of adults displaying joy, anger or sadness. With 15 actors with and without a
surgical facemask, we created a dataset of 90 pictures displaying joy, anger or sadness (10 women, 5
men, based on demographic information of childminders in local public day-care centres) (Figure 1).
We built the experiment with E-Prime® [4]. The ethics committee for human research of the Canton
Vaud approved the study (study number: 2020-02687) and accepted that with the pandemic situation
consent could be waived. Parents of children attending public day-care centres received written, oral
and filmed information, with the possibility to opt out. Children aged 36 to 72 months without treated
neurodevelopmental impairment were eligible to participate. They sat in front of a computer, with a
known caretaker if they wanted, and a trained paediatrician showed randomly the 90 pictures. Children
2
could either name the emotion, point on a card showing emoticons of these three emotions, or choose
the response options “I don’t know” or “quit the experiment”. The statistical analysis included a
comparison of the correct response rate in the different conditions with χ2 tests and bias corrected
Cramer’s V to calculate effect sizes.
Results: Data was collected in nine public day-care centres. The sample consisted of 276 children
(girls: 48.9%, mean age=52.4 months, SD=9.6). The test lasted a median of 6.74 min per child (IQR
4.22-9.26). The rate of “I don’t know” responses was 3.1 % and 2.2 % children stopped the
experiment prematurely but their responses were included. The global correct response rate was
68.8%, 70.6% without facemask vs 66.9% with facemask (χ2(1) = 37. 783, p < .001, V = 0.0385,
95%CI [0.0266, 0.0515]), with a difference for joy (94.8 vs 87.3%, χ2(1) = 140.260, p < .001, V =
0.1301, 95%CI [0.1090, 0.1521] ), sadness (54.1 vs 48.9, χ2(1) = 21.937, p < .001, V = 0.0505, 95%CI
[0.0266, 0.0515]), but not anger (62.2 vs 64.6%, χ2(1) = 2.7094, p = .0997, V = 0.0147, 95%CI
[0.0000, 0.0399]). There was no difference between boys and girls. The rate of correct responses
increased with age (χ2(2) = 136.680, p < .001, V = 0.07363, 95%CI [0.0615, 0.0864]) (Figure 2A).
Finally, the analysis of the mistakes showed that up to 25 % pre-schoolers confused anger and sadness
and up to 21% answered joy for anger or sadness (Figure 2B).
Discussion: Actual facemasks, depicted on static pictures, were significantly associated with emotion
recognition of healthy preschool children, although differences were small and effects sizes were weak
(Cramer’s V 0.2). Joy was more recognised and mistakenly chosen for anger or sadness, probably
due to a positivity bias in children[6]. Overall, participants of our study, who had been exposed to
facemasks for nearly a year, recognized emotions on pictures better than reported in previous research,
even with facemasks [1, 6]. This study has several limitations including the generalizability of its
findings using static pictures instead of live actors, and the validity of the outcomes. Investigating the
role of facemasks in relation to other aspects of development and for children with developmental
issues remains important, particularly in the wake of a fourth wave of the COVID-19 pandemic.
3
Acknowledgements: No funding. Dr Bickle Graz had full access to all the data in the study and takes
responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
References
1. Covic, A., N. von Steinbüchel, and C. Kiese-Himmel, Emotion Recognition in Kindergarten
Children. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 2020. 72(4): p. 273-281.
2. Gori, M., L. Schiatti, and M.B. Amadeo, Masking Emotions: Face Masks Impair How We
Read Emotions. Front Psychol, 2021. 12: p. 669432.
3. Ruba, A.L. and S.D. Pollak, Children’s emotion inferences from masked faces: Implications
for social interactions during COVID-19. PLOS ONE, 2020. 15(12): p. e0243708.
4. Psychology Software Tools, I., E-prime 3.0. 2016.
5. Garcia, S.E. and E.C. Tully, Children's recognition of happy, sad, and angry facial
expressions across emotive intensities. J Exp Child Psychol, 2020. 197: p. 104881.
6. Nguyen, T.T. and N.L. Nelson, Winners and losers: Recognition of spontaneous emotional
expressions increases across childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2021.
209: p. 105184.
Figure 1. Examples of pictures of the same actor showing (A) joy, (B), sadness, and (C) anger without
and with facemasks.
A B C
Figure 2B Responses for joy, anger and sadness, without and with facemasks (n= 276)
4
Figure 2A Rate of emotion recognition by age, emotion, and presence of facemask (n = 276)
Joy w/o
mask
Joy with
mask
Anger w/o
mask
Anger with
mask
Sadness w/o
mask
Sadness with
mask
Joy 94.8 87.3 13.9 14.8 14.8 20.8
Anger 1.3 4.9 62.9 64.6 24.6 23.6
Sadness 1.3 4.2 16.0 15 54.1 48.9
Does not know 0.4 1.4 5.0 3.2 4.3 4.5
Quit 2.2 2.2 2.2 2.3 2.2 2.2
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
Percentage of responses
Emotion and condition
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
3 years old 4 years old 5 years old
Percentage of correct responses
age
Joy without mask
Joy with mask
All emotions
without mask
All emotions with
mask
Anger without
mask
Anger with mask
Sadness without
mask
Sadness with mask
***
**
**
** p<0.01
*** p<0.001
... Only a limited number of studies have examined emotion recognition in preschool children, revealing that the capacity to interpret emotions from facial photographs tends to increase with age, from 3 to 6 years old [32,33]. Beck and colleagues [13,14] explored the identi cation of the NAO robot's emotional postures -de ned as static body postures -by adults and 11-to-13-years-old adolescents. ...
... In the present study, we decided to evaluate the most recognized NAO key postures in Beck and colleagues' study (corresponding to anger, sadness, fear, happiness) with younger children (i.e., in preschool children). As mentioned in previous studies on human faces [32,33], we hypothesized that older children would have better scores than younger one, regardless of whether they interacted with the robot or the human demonstrator. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The aim of the present study was to compare interactions of children aged between 3 and 6 years, with a NAO robot or an adult partner, in various body-focused tasks: comprehension and recognition of body parts labels, imitation of movements, and recognition of emotions in the postures of the agent. For each task, performances were appreciated through scores levels. We found no effect of the demonstrator type on our results: children of different ages responded similarly to the the human or the robot demonstrator. We found an effect of age, with the olderchildren having higher scores for the comprehension of body parts labels on the demonstrator’s body, the imitation of body parts sequences and the identification of emotional key postures. Results are discussed in light of the implications of the use of social robots such as the NAO one, in interactive and learning situations with typical children.
... The negative side effects of wearing masks on reading emotions have been investigated in several studies since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic (Pavlova & Sokolov, 2022). However, these findings rely heavily on empirical studies with adult participants, with the exception of nine studies (Bourke et al., 2023;Carbon & Serrano, 2021;Chester et al., 2022;Cuzzocrea et al., 2023;Gil & Le Bigot, 2023;Giordano et al., 2022;Gori et al., 2021;Ruba & Pollak, 2020;Schneider et al., 2022). It is questionable whether the results for adult participants can be generalized to school-aged children. ...
... Post-2020, as mentioned above, nine studies have shown that face masks reduce children's emotion recognition performance. These studies have been conducted in the USA (Carbon & Serrano, 2021;Chester et al., 2022;Giordano et al., 2022;Ruba & Pollak, 2020) and Europe (Bourke et al., 2023;Cuzzocrea et al., 2023;Gil & Le Bigot, 2023;Gori et al., 2021;Schneider et al., 2022). The bias of the sample was also common in previous studies conducted before the COVID-19 pandemic (Gagnon et al., 2014;Guarnera et al., 2017Guarnera et al., , 2015Roberson et al., 2012), in which emotion was recognized from parts of the face (e.g., trimmed eyes or mouth). ...
Article
The negative side effects of mask-wearing on reading facial emotional cues have been investigated in several studies with adults post-2020. However, little is known about children. This study aimed to determine the negative influence of mask-wearing on reading emotions of adult faces by Japanese school-aged children, compared to Japanese adults. We also examined whether this negative influence could be alleviated by using a transparent face mask instead of an opaque one (surgical mask). The performance on reading emotions was measured using emotion categorization and emotion intensity rating tasks for adult faces. As per the findings, the accuracy of emotion recognition in children was impaired for various facial expressions (disgust, fear, happy, neutral, sad, and surprise faces), except for angry faces. Conversely, in adults, it was impaired for a few facial expressions. The perceived intensity for happy faces with a surgical mask was weaker in both children and adults than in those without the mask. A negative influence of wearing surgical masks was generally not observed for faces wearing a transparent mask in both children and adults. Thus, negative side effects of mask-wearing on reading emotions are observed for more facial expressions in children than in adults; transparent masks can help remedy these.
... The negative impact of face masks on emotional recognition also exists in children. Schneider et al. (2022) found the recognition accuracy of 3-5-year-old children for three facial expressions (happiness, anger, and sadness) were impacted by face mask. Roberson et al. (2012) found that as children aged their accuracy in recognizing unmasked facial expressions gradually increased, but their accuracy in recognizing masked facial expressions remained at a lower level. ...
... Most of the studies recruited young and healthy participants (Miyazaki and Kawahara, 2016;Freud et al., 2020;Olivera-La Rosa et al., 2020;Marini et al., 2021;Langbehn et al., 2022), but the effects of masks in atypical people, who are in the stages of growth or functional deterioration respectively, may be greater than those on young and healthy people. As mentioned earlier, face masks impede facial recognition in children (Roberson et al., 2012;Schneider et al., 2022). However, most of these studies were conducted at the beginning of an infectious disease outbreak (COVID-19), when child participants only lived in mask-wearing interactions environments for short periods. ...
Article
Full-text available
The pandemic has made wearing masks commonplace, prompting researchers to investigate their effects on interpersonal perception. Findings indicate masks obstruct face identification and expression recognition, with lower face cues being most affected. When judging attractiveness, masks can enhance the appeal of less attractive faces, but reduce the appeal of more attractive faces. Trust and speech perception outcomes are inconclusive. Future studies could focus on individual differences in how masks influence our perception of others.
... Many individuals report not wearing masks because they experience difficulties communicating when masked 31,32 . Recent work has shown that individuals are less accurate judging masked facial expressions including in adults 33-37 , in children [38][39][40] , and across cultures 41 . While accuracy is one important aspect of facial emotion communication, little research has examined whether masks may also influence the speed of this communication, or underlying processes such as evidence accumulation. ...
... Participants were not only less accurate, but also slower at identifying masked expressions, because masks slowed the speed with which they accumulated evidence for emotion. Here, by applying drift-diffusion modeling (DDM) 14,1617 , this work extends recent evidence that individuals judge expressions less accurately with face masks [33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41] , by demonstrating that masks also influence the speed of these judgments, and revealing evidence accumulation as the mechanism underlying both of these effects. This suggests that individuals may struggle to perceive masked expressions under time pressure, such as when they quickly pass other people in the grocery store or other brief interactions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Face masks slow the spread of SARS-CoV-2, but it has been unknown how masks might reshape social interaction. One important possibility is that masks may influence how individuals communicate emotion through facial expressions. Here, we clarify to what extent—and how—masks influence facial emotion communication, through drift–diffusion modeling (DDM). Over two independent pre-registered studies, conducted three and 6 months into the COVID-19 pandemic, online participants judged expressions of 6 emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise) with the lower or upper face “masked” or unmasked. Participants in Study 1 (N = 228) correctly identified expressions above chance with lower face masks. However, they were less likely—and slower—to correctly identify these expressions relative to without masks, and they accumulated evidence for emotion more slowly—via decreased drift rate in DDM. This pattern replicated and intensified 3 months later in Study 2 (N = 264). These findings highlight how effectively individuals still communicate with masks, but also explain why they can experience difficulties communicating when masked. By revealing evidence accumulation as the underlying mechanism, this work suggests that time-sensitive situations may risk miscommunication with masks. This research could inform critical interventions to promote continued mask wearing as needed.
... Among college students, fathers, and their children, our findings support our hypothesis that the emotional recognition accuracy of masked faces is lower than unmasked faces. This is consistent with other studies that found reduced emotional accuracy to masked faces in children (Chester et al., 2023;Schneider et al., 2022) and adults (Freud et al., 2020;Rizzato et al., 2022). Since emotion recognition is important to socioemotional development and to forming and maintaining relationships, it will be important for future studies to examine whether this impaired accuracy is associated with social behavior and children's long-term development. ...
Article
Full-text available
Facial emotion recognition is vital for human social behavior. During the COVID-19 pandemic, face masks were widely adopted for viral mitigation and remain crucial public health tools. However, questions persist about their impact on emotion recognition and neural processing, especially in children, parents, and young adults. We developed the Masked Affective and Social Cognition task, featuring masked and unmasked faces displaying fear, sadness, and anger. We recruited three racial and ethnically diverse samples: 119 college students, 30 children who entered school age at the beginning of the pandemic, and 31 fathers of the aforementioned children. Of the latter two groups, 41 participants (n = 23 fathers, 18 children) did the Masked Affective and Social Cognition task during a neuroimaging scan, while the remaining 20 participants (n = 8 fathers, 12 children) who were not eligible for scanning completed the task during their lab visit. Behaviorally, we found that participants recognized emotions less accurately when viewing masked faces and also found an interaction of emotion by condition, such that accuracy was particularly compromised by sad masked faces. Neurally, masked faces elicited greater activation in the posterior cingulate, insula, and fusiform gyrus. Anterior insula and inferior frontal gyrus activation were driven by sad, masked faces. These results were consistent across age groups. Among fathers, activation to sad masked faces was associated with stress and depression. Overall, our findings did not depend on previous mask exposure or timing of participation during the pandemic. These results have implications for understanding face emotion recognition, empathy, and socioemotional neurodevelopment.
... During the pandemic, the effects of mask-wearing by children were frequently discussed (e.g., 14,15 ). However, little is known about the mimicry of child faces by adults. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional mimicry, the imitation of others’ emotion expressions, is related to increased interpersonal closeness and better interaction quality. Yet, little research has focused on the effect of face masks on emotional mimicry and none on (masked) child faces. To address this gap, we conducted an online experiment (N = 235, German sample, adult perceivers). Masks reduced emotion recognition accuracy for all expressions, except in the case of anger in masked child faces, where perceived anger was even increased. Perceived interpersonal closeness was reduced for masked happy and sad faces. For both child and adult expressers, masks reduced facial mimicry of happy expressions, with no mask effects for sadness and anger expression. A stronger mask effect on facial happiness mimicry of child faces was mediated by the degree of emotion recognition accuracy. Smiles shown by masked children were not recognized well, likely due to the absence of wrinkles around the eyes in child faces. Independent of masks, sadness shown by children was mimicked even more strongly than when shown by adults. These results provide evidence for facial mimicry of child expressions by adult perceivers and show that the effects of face masks on emotion communication may vary when children wear them.
... Children are particularly encouraged to wear masks because of their weak immune systems. However, the limited facial information visible in people wearing masks can affect children's social skills, which are heavily influenced by their ability to read facial expressions [1,2]. Recent studies have explored methods to identify faces and recognize facial expressions when a mask partially covers the face, particularly when divided into top and bottom parts. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has significantly impacted society, having led to a lack of social skills in children who became used to interacting with others while wearing masks. To analyze this issue, we investigated the effects of masks on face identification and facial expression recognition, using deep learning models for these operations. The results showed that when using the upper or lower facial regions for face identification, the upper facial region allowed for an accuracy of 81.36%, and the lower facial region allowed for an accuracy of 55.52%. Regarding facial expression recognition, the upper facial region allowed for an accuracy of 39% compared to 49% for the lower facial region. Furthermore, our analysis was conducted for a number of facial expressions, and specific emotions such as happiness and contempt were difficult to distinguish using only the upper facial region. Because this study used a model trained on data generated from human labeling, it is assumed that the effects on humans would be similar. Therefore, this study is significant because it provides engineering evidence of a decline in facial expression recognition; however, wearing masks does not cause difficulties in identification.
... Ruba and Pollack found some loss of emotional information transmission, comparable to that found when the subject wore sunglasses [26]. Schneider et al. found evidence of emotion misrecognition, and Gori et al. found that mask use interfered with the ability to accurately infer facial expressions at any age, but particularly in toddlers [27,28]. ...
Article
Full-text available
“First, do no harm” [...]
Article
Rationale Masks have been widely used as a preventative tool during the COVID‐19 pandemic. However, the use of masks by children has been controversial, with international guidelines recommending a risk‐based approach to national policymakers. Aims and objectives We aimed to conduct a systematic review that explores children's experiences of mask‐wearing, drawing on an evidence base that describes mask‐wearing in different contexts including air pollution, and to prevent the spread of infectious disease. Methods We searched MEDLINE, Embase and PsycINFO in June 2021, with repeat searches in August 2022 and January 2024, for primary research studies exploring children's experiences of masks. Included studies reported on participants between 4 and 14 years (inclusive), with no restrictions on language where an English translation was available. Two reviewers independently screened titles and abstracts and reviewed full texts, with discrepancies resolved by a third reviewer. We used the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool for quality appraisal and narrative synthesis to identify key findings. We also conducted stakeholder consultation (Patient and Public Involvement (PPI)) with nine children, where they submitted annotated drawings of their preferred masks to complement our review findings. Results We screened 982 titles and abstracts and reviewed 94 full texts. 45 studies were included in the synthesis. Children's experiences of mask‐wearing were influenced by their perceived necessity, social norms around their use and parental attitudes. Challenges related to mask‐wearing were described, including difficulty reading facial expressions and physical discomfort. Children found it easier to wear masks when sitting and in cooler environments, and they benefited from unmasking during outdoor break time at school. As part of the PPI consultation, children highlighted the importance of mask design and the environmental impact of masks. Conclusion Children's experiences of mask‐wearing were varied and context‐dependent, with several mask‐design challenges raised. Future policy on mask‐wearing needs to consider the context in which mask‐wearing would be most beneficial, and how local adaptations to policy can respond to children's needs.
Article
Full-text available
Aim: The purpose of the present study was to investigate the evolution of attention and emotional recognition abilities in preschool and elementary school children with normal and abnormal development. Methods: The current research was a descriptive and comparative study. The research population included all hyperactive children under treatment at Mashavah Rouhana and Tivat centers and preschool and primary school children in schools in District 1, 2 and 3 of Tehran in the academic year of 2017-2018. Among these children, 410 children were selected using a multi-stage random sampling method. To collect data, the continuous performance test of Rossold et al. (1956) and Ekman's emotion recognition test (2007) were used. To analyze the data, one-way analysis of variance and Tukey's post hoc test were used by SPSS-22. Results: The results of the analysis of variance showed that the participants in two groups, normal and hyperactive, have different ability to pay attention and recognize emotions, and this difference is significant in all components at the 0.001 level. Conclusion: According to the results of the present study, attention and emotional recognition in children play an important role in improving their social communication and academic performance. Therefore, examining and strengthening it in primary school ages around 4 to 8 years old and even up to 12 years old when the transformation of the frontal parts is complete, plays an important role in academic success.
Article
Full-text available
To date, COVID-19 has spread across the world, changing our way of life and forcing us to wear face masks. This report demonstrates that face masks influence the human ability to infer emotions by observing facial configurations. Specifically, a mask obstructing a face limits the ability of people of all ages to infer emotions expressed by facial features, but the difficulties associated with the mask’s use are significantly pronounced in children aged between 3 and 5 years old. These findings are of essential importance, as they suggest that we live in a time that may potentially affect the development of social and emotion reasoning, and young children’s future social abilities should be monitored to assess the true impact of the use of masks.
Article
Full-text available
To slow the progression of COVID-19, the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) and the World Health Organization (WHO) have recommended wearing face coverings. However, very little is known about how occluding parts of the face might impact the emotion inferences that children make during social interactions. The current study recruited a racially diverse sample of school-aged (7- to 13-years) children from publicly funded after-school programs. Children made inferences from facial configurations that were not covered, wearing sunglasses to occlude the eyes, or wearing surgical masks to occlude the mouth. Children were still able to make accurate inferences about emotions, even when parts of the faces were covered. These data suggest that while there may be some challenges for children incurred by others wearing masks, in combination with other contextual cues, masks are unlikely to dramatically impair children’s social interactions in their everyday lives.
Article
Full-text available
Background/aims: Recognition and understanding of emotions are essential skills in nonverbal communication and in everyday social functioning. These are already evident in infancy. We aimed to compare how young children recognize facial emotional expressions from static faces versus vocal emotional expressions from speech prosody. Methods: Participants were 313 kindergarten children (162 girls, mean age = 51.01, SD 9.65 months; range 36-72). The design consisted of a visual and an auditory block (with 45 randomized trials each). Children were seated in front of a 14-inch laptop monitor and received visual stimuli (photos of faces) or auditory stimuli (spoken sentences) via loudspeakers. Results: Recognizing emotions from looking at static faces was found to be easier compared to interpreting emotions transmitted by speech prosody alone. The ability to interpret emotions from both faces and speech prosody increased with age. It was easier to identify a "happy" emotion from a facial expression than an "angry" or "sad" one, whereas a "sad" emotion could be more easily recognized from speech prosody alone than facial imagery alone. Girls were significantly better than boys in identifying "sad" facial expressions. Conclusion: The results of the study are discussed in terms of educational implications for nonverbal communication.
Article
Research using posed emotional expressions is problematic because they lack ecological validity. Adults’ recognition of spontaneous real-world expressions may require the inclusion of postural information. Whether posture improves children’s recognition of real-world expressions was unknown. Younger children (n = 30; 5- to 7-year-olds), older children (n = 30; 8- to 10-year-olds), and adults (n = 30) judged whether tennis players had won or lost a point. Images showed one of three cue types: Head-only, Body-only, or Head–Body expressions. Recognition of expressions improved with age; older children and adults performed better than younger children. In addition, recognition of Body-only and Head–Body cues was better than Head-only cues for all ages. Spontaneous expression recognition improved throughout childhood and with the inclusion of postural information.
Article
Sharing emotional experiences is a key task that requires accurate recognition of peers’ emotions during middle childhood. Existing research suggests that children are proficient at discerning emotion from facial expressions during middle childhood, but this research has focused on recognition of adults’ intense emotional expressions. In this study, facial emotion recognition for children’s happy, sad, and angry expressions across low, medium, and high intensities was measured in a sample of 7- to 10-year-old children (N = 80; 53% female) to quantify overall accurate recognition as well as inaccuracies, including identifying an emotion as present when it is not (false alarms) and failing to identify an emotion when present (miss rate). Children’s recognition accuracy for low-threshold happiness, sadness, and anger was quite poor but improved in a cubic fashion as expression intensity increased, with dramatic improvements across medium-intensity expressions, and little further improvement across high-intensity expressions. A positivity bias was evident; children were more accurate at recognizing happiness than at recognizing sadness and anger, rarely failed to identify happiness when present, and tended to mislabel expressions as happy rather than as angry or sad. Children were generally better at recognizing anger compared with sadness but were more accurate at recognizing subtle sadness compared with anger, which appeared to be due to children missing subtle anger when present. The findings are discussed with regard to the functionality of others’ happiness for signaling positive socializing opportunities, anger for signaling threatening interactions, and sadness for prompting prosocial action and with regard to how children’s facial emotion recognition may affect general socioemotional development.