ArticlePDF Available

Daily television exposure, parent conversation during shared television viewing and socioeconomic status: Associations with curiosity at kindergarten

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objective To examine the main and interactive effects of the amount of daily television exposure and frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing on parent ratings of curiosity at kindergarten, and to test for moderation by socioeconomic status (SES). Study design Sample included 5100 children from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort. Hours of daily television exposure and frequency of parent screen-time conversation were assessed from a parent interview at preschool, and the outcome of early childhood curiosity was derived from a child behavior questionnaire at kindergarten. Multivariate linear regression examined the main and interactive effects of television exposure and parent screen-time conversation on kindergarten curiosity and tested for moderation by SES. Results In adjusted models, greater number of hours of daily television viewing at preschool was associated with lower curiosity at kindergarten (B = -0.14, p = .008). More frequent parent conversation during shared screen-time was associated with higher parent-reported curiosity at kindergarten with evidence of moderation by SES. The magnitude of association between frequency of parent conversation during television viewing and curiosity was greater for children from low SES environments, compared to children from high SES environments: (SES ≤ median): B = 0.29, p < .001; (SES > median): B = 0.11, p < .001. Conclusions Higher curiosity at kindergarten was associated with greater frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing, with a greater magnitude of association in low-SES families. While the study could not include measures of television program content, digital media use and non-screen time conversation, our results suggest the importance of parent conversation to promote early childhood curiosity, especially for children with socioeconomic disadvantage.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation
during shared television viewing and
socioeconomic status: Associations with
curiosity at kindergarten
Prachi E. ShahID
1,2
*, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek
3
, Todd B. Kashdan
4
, Kristen Harrison
5
,
Katherine Rosenblum
2
, Heidi M. WeeksID
6
, Priya Singh
7
, Niko Kaciroti
8
1Division of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, Department of Pediatrics, University of Michigan Medical
School, Ann Arbor, MI, United States of America, 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan Medical
School, Ann Arbor, MI, United States of America, 3Department of Psychology, Temple University,
Philadelphia, PA, United States of America, 4Department of Psychology and Center for the Advancement of
Well-Being, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, United States of America, 5Department of
Communication and Media, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States of America, 6Department of
Nutritional Sciences, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States of
America, 7Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati Medical School, Cincinnati, OH, United States
of America, 8Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI,
United States of America
*prachis@umich.edu
Abstract
Objective
To examine the main and interactive effects of the amount of daily television exposure and
frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing on parent ratings of curi-
osity at kindergarten, and to test for moderation by socioeconomic status (SES).
Study design
Sample included 5100 children from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort.
Hours of daily television exposure and frequency of parent screen-time conversation were
assessed from a parent interview at preschool, and the outcome of early childhood curiosity
was derived from a child behavior questionnaire at kindergarten. Multivariate linear regres-
sion examined the main and interactive effects of television exposure and parent screen-
time conversation on kindergarten curiosity and tested for moderation by SES.
Results
In adjusted models, greater number of hours of daily television viewing at preschool was
associated with lower curiosity at kindergarten (B = -0.14, p = .008). More frequent parent
conversation during shared screen-time was associated with higher parent-reported curios-
ity at kindergarten with evidence of moderation by SES. The magnitude of association
between frequency of parent conversation during television viewing and curiosity was
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 1 / 15
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Shah PE, Hirsh-Pasek K, Kashdan TB,
Harrison K, Rosenblum K, Weeks HM, et al. (2021)
Daily television exposure, parent conversation
during shared television viewing and
socioeconomic status: Associations with curiosity
at kindergarten. PLoS ONE 16(10): e0258572.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572
Editor: Susan Engel, Williams College, UNITED
STATES
Received: January 31, 2020
Accepted: September 30, 2021
Published: October 28, 2021
Copyright: ©2021 Shah et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: Data cannot be
shared publicly because the data were obtained
from a restricted use dataset. Data are available
from the Institute of Educational Statistics, from
the US Department of Education’s National Center
for Education Statistics (NCES) (contact via https://
nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/licenses.asp) for
researchers who meet the criteria for access to
confidential data. To elaborate, this manuscript
utilizes restricted data from the Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B). The
greater for children from low SES environments, compared to children from high SES envi-
ronments: (SES median): B = 0.29, p <.001; (SES >median): B = 0.11, p <.001.
Conclusions
Higher curiosity at kindergarten was associated with greater frequency of parent conversa-
tion during shared television viewing, with a greater magnitude of association in low-SES
families. While the study could not include measures of television program content, digital
media use and non-screen time conversation, our results suggest the importance of parent
conversation to promote early childhood curiosity, especially for children with socioeco-
nomic disadvantage.
Introduction
Curiosity, an important foundation for scientific innovation [1], is characterized by the drive
to seek out new information [2], desire to explore [3], and joy in learning [4,5]. Higher curios-
ity has been associated with numerous adaptive outcomes in childhood including more robust
word acquisition [6], enhanced learning and exploration [7] and higher academic achievement
[8,9], highlighting the potential importance of fostering curiosity from an early age. Our previ-
ous work found a positive association between higher curiosity and higher academic achieve-
ment, with a greater magnitude of benefit for children with socioeconomic disadvantage [10],
raising the possibility that promoting curiosity in young children may be one way to mitigate
the achievement gap associated with poverty [11]. To foster curiosity in early childhood, it is
necessary to consider the modifiable contexts that may promote or inhibit its expression.
One potential modifiable factor associated with differences in early childhood outcomes is
the amount of daily television exposure. While there is an increasing interest in the role of digi-
tal media exposure on child development, televisions are in 98% of all homes, and television
viewing remains the dominant screen activity of young children, accounting for 72% of all
screen time [12], making television exposure a relevant developmental context in young chil-
dren. Children are exposed to an average of 1–4 hours of television per day [13,14], with
higher exposure in children who are economically disadvantaged [15,16]. In previous screen-
time research with infants, toddlers and preschoolers, more television exposure has been asso-
ciated with impaired self- regulation [17,18], lower language outcomes [19,20], and lower
cognitive development [21,22], however, association with curiosity has not been examined,
and is a gap in the literature. Screen media exposure, including television, can displace explor-
atory activities such as play and parent-child interactions [23] that are thought to be necessary
for the cultivation of curiosity [24]. We therefore sought to test the hypothesis that higher daily
television exposure would be associated with lower curiosity (Hypothesis 1). We also consid-
ered that the association between the amount of television exposure on early childhood curios-
ity may be attenuated in children with higher SES, who may have other resources to foster
curiosity, compared with low SES children. Therefore, we sought to test whether the associa-
tion between higher daily television exposure and early childhood curiosity was moderated by
SES, with a greater magnitude of effect seen in low SES /under-resourced families (Hypothesis
2).
In addition, because development unfolds through reciprocal interactions between children
and their parents, the quality of early dyadic experiences may also play a role in fostering curi-
osity. Previous work has demonstrated the benefits of parent conversation during shared
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 2 / 15
ECLS-B is a nationally representative, population-
based longitudinal study sponsored by the Institute
of Educational Statistics, from the US Department
of Education’s National Center for Education
Statistics (NCES). While the ECLS-B is a publicly
available dataset, the data used for this analysis
comes from the restricted ECLS-B dataset, which
requires special access and permission from
NCES, prior to accessing the data. The PI (senior
author (PES)) had to enter into a data-use
agreement with the Institute for Educational
Statistics / NCES prior to receiving access for the
restricted use data. Per the requirements of the
NCES, the data cannot be freely shared with other
investigators, and interested investigators must
enter into a data use agreement with the NCES
prior to accessing the restricted-use data from the
Institution for Educational Statistics. Due to NCES’s
confidentiality legislation, ECLS-B case-level data
are available only to qualified researchers who are
granted a restricted-use data license from NCES.
Information regarding how to obtain a restricted-
data license for the ECLS-B can be found at https://
nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/licenses.asp.
Funding: This work was supported by grants from
the University of Michigan, the Zero to Three
Academy of Fellows, and NICHD (K08HD078506)
to PES, and from the Zero to Three Academy of
Fellows to KR. Study sponsors had no role in the
study design; the collection, analysis, and
interpretation of data; the writing of the report; or
the decision to submit the manuscript for
publication.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
television viewing on language development, with more frequent conversation moderating the
adverse impact of heavy television exposure [25]. Previous research has also demonstrated that
parent-child conversation facilitates children’s thinking, learning and exploration (i.e., behav-
ioral indicators of curiosity) through pedagogical exchanges [26]. As such, we hypothesized
that more frequent parent conversation during shared television viewing may be associated
with higher curiosity (Hypothesis 3a) and may moderate the association between higher televi-
sion exposure and curiosity (Hypothesis 3b). Furthermore, because the amount and quality of
language that young children hear also varies by socioeconomic status [27], (e.g., the 30-mil-
lion-word-gap) [28,29], we theorized that there may be a similar “curiosity gap” among low
income children who are exposed to less conversation. We hypothesized that, consistent with a
cumulative risk model [30], socioeconomic disadvantage in combination with less frequent
parental conversation may confer an added risk for lower curiosity, with greatest effects seen
in children from low SES families (Hypothesis 4). The overarching aim of this work was to
identify modifiable factors in the early caregiving environment (e.g., amount of early television
viewing, frequency of parent conversation) which may be important for the promotion of
early childhood curiosity, and to examine whether these factors were associated with differen-
tial effects in children from under-resourced families. Results from this work will help inform
anticipatory guidance to promote early childhood curiosity in at risk populations.
Materials and methods
Study design and sample
Data were drawn from the restricted data of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth
Cohort (ECLS-B), a nationally representative, population-based longitudinal study sponsored
by the US Department of Education’s National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). The
ECLS-B is based on a nationally representative probability sample of children born in the
United States in 2001. Data were collected from children and their parents at age 9 months,
24-months, preschool and kindergarten timepoints, and included parent interviews, and direct
and indirect child assessments across multiple settings [31]. Our sample excluded children
with congenital and chromosomal abnormalities, and included children born at 22–41 weeks
gestation who had kindergarten behavioral data from which we could derive a measure of curi-
osity. Our study utilized data from birth, 24-months, preschool and kindergarten timepoints,
with a final sample of 5100 children. This study was considered exempt by the Institutional
Review Board because it involved the use of a publicly available dataset with de-identified par-
ticipants who could not be linked to the data.
Measures
Outcomes. Curiosity. Because the ECLS-B did not have a measure to examine curiosity,
we derived a measure of curiosity from an existing assessment of child behavior available in
the dataset, which included questions from the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavioral Scales
Second Edition (PKBS-2) and Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). While we were limited by
the questions that were available the parent PKBS-2 questionnaire at the kindergarten time-
point, we drew from previous theoretical work and behavioral descriptions of curiosity in
young children [3238] to select question items that most closely aligned with characteristics
of curiosity. While there is no single definition of curiosity [33], there are certain behavioral
characteristics of curiosity that are widely accepted, including, (1) the thirst for knowledge,and
the drive to understand what one does not know [34]; (2) an exploratory drive to seek novelty
[35]; (3) an openness to new experiences [36]; and, in young children, (4) innovation in explor-
atory play [37,38]. Four question items from the PKBS-2 which aligned with these
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 3 / 15
characteristics of curiosity were chosen for our “curiosity factor.” The individual question
items included (1) shows eagerness to learn new things (i.e., thirst for knowledge); (2) likes to
try new things (i.e., drive for novelty); (3) easily adjusts to a new situation (i.e., openness to new
experiences); and (4) shows imagination in work and play (i.e., innovation in exploratory play).
At the kindergarten timepoint, parents were asked to report the frequency of behaviors
observed in the previous 3 months on a 5-point Likert scale (1, never to 5, very often). Items
were reverse coded as appropriate such that higher scores indicated more positive behaviors. A
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to assure reliability and to calculate the
appropriate loading values for deriving our curiosity factor. Standardized scoring of the curios-
ity factor was conducted, and good internal consistency was demonstrated (α= 0.70, M = 0.07,
SD = 1.2) [10]. Individual question items, loading coefficients, and model fit indices for our
curiosity factor are shown in S3 Appendix.
Predictors. Hours of television viewing. Hours of television viewing at preschool were
determined from a parent questionnaire at the preschool timepoint. Parents were asked
. . .about how many hours of television does [your child] watch at home per day,” with
responses ranging from 0–24 hours. Respondents who answered “N/A” to this question were
not included in the analysis. Because most children (96%) reportedly watched 6 hours or fewer
of television per day, the hours of daily television exposure were capped at 6+ hours, reducing
the influence of a few statistical outliers.
Parent conversation during shared television viewing. Parent conversation during television
viewing was determined from a parent questionnaire at the preschool timepoint. Parents were
asked, “In a typical week,when your family watches TV together,how often do you or another
family member talk with [your child] about the TV programs?” Responses were coded categori-
cally as 1 = never, 2 = hardly ever, 3 = sometimes, or 4 = often. Parents were not asked to
report on the amount of time a child watched television without adults, thus we were unable to
control for the amount of time children watched television without adult co-viewing.
Relatedly, there was also no measure of overall (non-television) parental language for the
entire sample. As such, we were unable to control for non-screen time parental language. To
address this limitation, using a subsample of 500 parent-child dyads with available data on a
structured reading task, we examined the association between parent conversation during TV
viewing and parent conversation during the reading task. We found a positive association for
the frequency of television-related parent conversation and elaborative parent language during
the reading task characterized by use of open-ended questions (p = .008) and relating the book
to the child’s experience (p = .03). Based on this subsample analysis, we considered that televi-
sion-related parent conversation may also reflect the quality of the language environment in
the home. For the purpose of this study, we considered the amount of parent conversation
during shared television viewing to serve as a proxy for the amount of language in the caregiv-
ing environment.
Covariates. In our primary analyses, we included sociodemographic variables that might
be associated with the amount of television viewing and curiosity. Specifically, we controlled
for maternal age, race/ethnicity, marital status (married/unmarried), maternal education
(<high school; high school graduate; >high school), and poverty (<185% federal poverty
line; 185% federal poverty line). The latter two variables were integrated into a composite
measure of household socioeconomic status (SES) at kindergarten [31]. We also controlled for
child sex, child age, the type of childcare/preschool experience (no non-parental care; relative/
nonrelative home-based care; center-based care), and average number of hours of childcare/
center-based care per day. Because lower developmental skills [39] and inability to delay grati-
fication [18] have been associated with increased television exposure, additional covariates
included a measure of infant development at 24-months from the Bayley Short-Form Research
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 4 / 15
Edition, and parent-report of delay of gratification at 24-months (“My child is able to wait
dichotomized as “no/yes”). Of note, there was no measure of the content of television pro-
gramming available in the dataset (i.e., educational programming vs. entertainment), so we
were not able to control for television content in our analyses.
Statistical analyses
All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 [40] (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Maternal and
child characteristics were examined using descriptive statistics. Multivariate linear regression
utilizing the SURVEYREG (SAS) procedure allowed for tests of associations between hours of
daily television viewing, frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing
and kindergarten curiosity in linear and non-linear (quadratic) models, with minimal differ-
ences between the linear and quadratic models. We included covariates related to television
viewing, parent conversation, and curiosity to adjust for theoretically justified confounds. For
our primary analyses, in adjusted models, we tested the association between the hours of televi-
sion viewing and curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 1), and whether the association
between hours of television viewing and curiosity was moderated by SES (Hypothesis 2). We
examined whether the amount of parent conversation during shared television viewing at pre-
school was associated with early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3a), and whether the amount
of parent conversation moderated the association between the amount of television viewing
and early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3b). Finally, we examined whether the association
between parent conversation during television viewing and curiosity at kindergarten was mod-
erated by SES (i.e., our test of a cumulative risk hypothesis) (Hypothesis 4). In all our modera-
tion analyses, we included the interaction term in the final step of the multivariate regression
models. When the interaction was statistically significant (p <.05), we performed a stratified
analysis of the association between the predictor and curiosity, adjusting for covariates.
Because of the complex sample design, sample weights and the Jackknife method [41] were
used to account for stratification, clustering and unit non-response, thereby allowing the
weighted results to be generalized to the population of U.S. children born in 2001. In accord
with the NCES requirements for ECLS-B data use, reported numbers were rounded to the
nearest 50.
Results
Sample characteristics
Of the 6350 children who had behavioral (curiosity) data at kindergarten, 5100 children had
television-viewing data at preschool and all covariates, which served as our analytic sample.
The 5100 children in our final sample did not differ from the 1250 children who were excluded
(due to missing data) on most demographic characteristics. However, children who were
excluded were more likely to be non-White, have lower SES, have higher 24-month develop-
ment, watched fewer hours/day of television, and attended childcare/preschool more hours
per day. At the preschool timepoint, parents reported that children watched an average of 2.5
hours of television per day, and almost half of parents (49.8%) reported talking with their chil-
dren “often” when viewing television together. After applying sample weights, the maternal
and child characteristics were generalizable to the US population in 2001. The sample charac-
teristics for the weighted sample are shown in Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the
amount of television viewing and parent conversation during shared television viewing are
shown in Table 2.
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 5 / 15
Tests of association between hours of daily television viewing and child
curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 1), moderation by SES (Hypothesis
2), and main and moderating effects of parent conversation during shared
television viewing (Hypotheses 3a and 3b)
In adjusted models, higher daily television viewing at preschool was associated with lower curi-
osity at kindergarten (B = -0.14, p = .008) (Hypothesis 1, S1 Appendix). The association
between the hours of daily television viewing at preschool and kindergarten curiosity was not
moderated by SES (p = .22) (Hypothesis 2). In adjusted models, we also found that more fre-
quent parent screen-time conversation was associated with higher curiosity at kindergarten (p
<.001), (Hypothesis 3a, S1 Appendix), but that more frequent parent conversation did not
moderate the association between the amount of television exposure and early childhood curi-
osity (p = .23) (Hypothesis 3b).
Table 1. Maternal and child characteristics.
Maternal Characteristics Mean, SD or Weighted %
Age (years) 27.4, 4.6
Race/ethnicity
White/Non-Hispanic 57.2%
Black/Non-Hispanic 14.5%
Hispanic 22.9%
Asian 2.9%
Other 2.4%
Marital Status
Married 68.0%
Unmarried 32.0%
Socioeconomic indicators calculated from measures of education and income at Kindergarten:
Maternal Education
Less than high school 17.8%
High school graduate 29.5%
>High School 52.8%
Below poverty threshold (<185% federal poverty line) 44.2%
At or above poverty threshold (185% federal poverty line) 55.8%
Child Characteristics Mean, SD, or Weighted %
Gender
Male 50.5%
Female 49.5%
Preschool-age Child Care/Preschool Experience
Parental care only (no childcare) 19.9%
Relative/non-relative care 21.4%
Center based care (Preschool or Head Start) 58.7%
Hours per day of Child-care or Preschool (hours) 4.5, 5.9
Age at Preschool (months) 52.5 (5.5)
Age at Kindergarten (months) 68.2, 7.4
24 Month Cognitive Development (T-score) 50.1, 16.3
Ability to delay gratification at 24 months (yes) 33.7%
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study, Birth Cohort. Selected years 2001–2007
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.t001
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 6 / 15
Tests of association between parent conversation during shared television
viewing and child curiosity at kindergarten, and moderation by
socioeconomic status (Hypothesis 4)
We then examined whether the association between the frequency of parent screen-time con-
versation at preschool and kindergarten curiosity was moderated by socioeconomic status
(SES) (Hypothesis 4). We found evidence of moderation by SES, (S2 Appendix), and pro-
ceeded to examine this association further by stratifying by lower SES (median) and higher
SES (>median), adjusting for the a priori covariates. We found differences in parent-reported
curiosity between families from high and low levels of SES, for each category of parent conver-
sation (never,hardly ever,sometimes,often), with stronger association among families from
under-resourced environments (i.e., low SES) (Table 3). To test and confirm the linear trend
between parent conversation and curiosity, in this model only, we then tested the association
with parent conversation coded as a continuous variable (1–4). The linear trend demonstrated
that the effect of more frequent parent conversation on curiosity was stronger among low SES
families (B = 0.29, p <.001) compared with high SES families (B = 0.11, p <.001) (Fig 1).
Frequency of parent conversation during shared television viewing and
associations with characteristics of childhood curiosity
To further examine the psychometrics of our measure of curiosity and consider the value of
each question item, we conducted a post hoc analysis to determine if there were specific fea-
tures of childhood curiosity that were susceptible to the effects of parent conversation. We ran
four models, examining the association between the frequency of parent conversation (as a
continuous variable), and each curiosity question item as our outcome, adjusting for a priori
covariates. In these models, more frequent parent conversation was positively associated with
each curiosity question item, with the greatest magnitude of association demonstrated by
shows imagination in work and play,” (B = 0.14, p <.001) (Table 4). The relatively similar
findings across items suggests that our curiosity measure tends to act as a unified construct.
Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of amount of television viewing and parent conversation during shared televi-
sion viewing.
(Weighted %) Total Sample Low SES High SES
Hours of Television Viewing/Day
0 hours 1.6% 1.0% 2.3%
1 hour 35.7% 28.1% 44.5%
2 hours 31.8% 31.5% 32.1%
3 hours 13.8% 16.7% 10.4%
4 hours 6.8% 8.9% 4.3%
5 hours 4.6% 5.9% 3.1%
6+ hours 5.8% 7.9% 3.3%
Parent Conversation During Shared TV Viewing
Often 49.8% 46.5% 53.6%
Sometimes 38.8% 42.1% 35.0%
Hardly Ever 8.6% 8.5% 8.7%
Never 2.8% 2.9% 2.7%
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study, Birth Cohort. Selected years 2001–2007
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.t002
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 7 / 15
Discussion
This is the first study examining associations among amount of daily television exposure, fre-
quency of parent conversation during shared television viewing at preschool, socioeconomic
Table 3. Adjusted associations of daily television viewing, parent conversation during shared television viewing
and curiosity at kindergarten, stratified by higher and lower socioeconomic status (SES).
Lower SES Higher SES
Step 3 Results (with interaction, stratified by SES) B (SE) B (SE)
Hours of television viewing/day (linear term) -0.16 (0.06)-0.14 (0.07)
Hours of television viewing/day (quadratic term) 0.02 (0.01)0.01 (0.01)
Parent Conversation during shared TV viewing
Often 0.89 (0.17)0.36 (0.14)
Sometimes 0.55 (0.17) 0.23 (0.14)
Hardly ever 0.40 (0.18)0.18 (0.17)
Never (REF) ——— ———
B coefficients are unstandardized betas.
: p <.05; : p <.01; : p <.001.
Analyses controlled for SES at kindergarten, maternal age, race/ethnicity, child cognitive development (24 months),
child’s age, child’s sex, child’s ability to delay gratification.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study, Birth Cohort. Selected years 2001–2007.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.t003
Fig 1. Frequency of parent conversation during shared media viewing at preschool and mean curiosity at
kindergarten, stratified by higher and lower socioeconomic status (SES). B coefficients are unstandardized betas.
Bars indicate parent conversation during TV viewing examined as a categorical variable. Lines indicate parent
conversation during TV viewing examined as a continuous variable, to test and confirm the linear trend. : p <.05; :
p<.01. SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort. Selected years 2001–2007.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.g001
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 8 / 15
status, and parent-report of curiosity at kindergarten using a nationally representative sample.
In adjusted analyses, we found that higher daily television viewing at preschool had a small but
significant association with lower curiosity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 1), but that this associ-
ation was not moderated by socioeconomic status (SES) (Hypothesis 2). We found that more
frequent parent conversation during shared television viewing was associated with higher curi-
osity at kindergarten (Hypothesis 3a), but that more frequent parent conversation during
shared television viewing did not moderate the association between the amount of television
exposure and early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3b). While we found an association
between higher television viewing at preschool and lower parent-report curiosity at kindergar-
ten, we were not able to include measures of the content of the television programming.
Because the opportunities for conversation and scaffolding may differ if dyads are watching
educational TV versus other type of programming, our inability to include the content of the
television programming in our analyses (due to the constraints of the dataset) limits the
interpretability of the association between the amount of television viewing and kindergarten
curiosity.
We found an association between the amount of parent conversation during shared televi-
sion watching at preschool and early childhood curiosity (Hypothesis 3a) with evidence of
moderation by SES (Hypothesis 4). In both high and low SES families, parents who reported
higher amounts of conversation also rated their children as being more curious, with a greater
magnitude of association in children from under-resourced families. We have several possible
explanations to account for these findings. One interpretation is that parents who report
engaging in more conversation may also be more attuned to children’s expression of curiosity
(e.g., children’s asking of questions, and engagement in pedagogical exchanges in conversa-
tion), and thus they also report their children as having higher curiosity. However, while
greater parental conversational exchanges have been associated with more question-asking
from their children [42], this explanation does not explain why the magnitude of association
between the frequency of parent conversation and curiosity would be greater in low SES chil-
dren. One possible explanation is that some parents may engage in frequent conversation with
their children in settings other than television, but allow their children watch television alone,
which may explain why there is an attenuated association between conversations during
shared television watching and curiosity for higher SES parents. An alternate explanation is
that while the “cumulative risks” of socioeconomic disadvantage and less frequent parental
conversation may confer an added risk for lower curiosity [30], the same children who are
more vulnerable to suboptimal development (e.g. “lower curiosity”) may also be more suscepti-
ble to the effects of more stimulating caregiving environments (e.g. more frequent parent
Table 4. Association between frequency of parent conversation during shared media viewing and each curiosity
question item at kindergarten.
Curiosity Question B (SE) p
Likes to try new things 0.11 (0.02) <.001
Shows imagination in work and play 0.14 (0.02) <.001
Shows eagerness to learn new things 0.11 (0.02) <.001
Easily adjusts to a new situation 0.12 (0.02) <.001
B coefficients are unstandardized betas.
Analyses controlled for hours of daily television viewing, maternal age, race/ethnicity, child cognitive development
(24 months), SES at kindergarten, child’s age, child’s sex, child’s ability to delay gratification.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study, Birth Cohort. Selected years 2001–2007.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572.t004
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 9 / 15
conversation) [43]. This suggests a potential “differential susceptibility” to the quality of the
caregiving environment, whereby low-SES children may reap added benefits from language-
promotive environments. Prior research has demonstrated how the quality of the linguistic
environment in the home (e.g. quality and quantity of language stimulation) can mitigate the
effects of socioeconomic disparities (i.e., poverty) on brain structure and later language and lit-
eracy outcomes [44,45]. Our results similarly suggest that the quality of the early linguistic
environment (characterized by more frequent parent conversation during shared TV viewing)
while promotive of higher curiosity in all children, may be especially beneficial to foster curios-
ity in children with socioeconomic disadvantage.
These findings have implications for the anticipatory guidance provided to parents. There
is some evidence suggesting that children with low curiosity fail engage with their environ-
ments in ways that foster motivation, achievement, and more specifically, academic develop-
ment [46]. Building on our previous work which suggested that higher curiosity can help
narrow the achievement gap associated with poverty [10], our results suggest that one potential
way to foster curiosity is through facilitating conversational exchanges between children and
their parents around moments of shared activity, especially for children from low socioeco-
nomic environments. This aligns with previous language-related research which demonstrates
that socioeconomically disadvantaged children preferentially benefit from greater child-
directed speech and conversational exchanges [27,45,47,48]. Our findings also highlight the
importance of parental scaffolding for child engagement and learning. In the same way that
parental engagement with children around shared play with toys facilitates children’s learning
and exploration [49], we found that parent conversation (as measured around shared televi-
sion viewing) could be similarly scaffolding, associated with higher expressions of child
curiosity.
Prior research has demonstrated that children learn best in environments that are interac-
tive, encouraging turn-taking, dialogic exchanges and intrinsically motivated questions [47,
50]. Our results similarly attest to this, but with an important consideration for children with
socioeconomic disadvantage. While incremental increases in the frequency of parent conver-
sation were associated with higher curiosity for all children, for children from under-resourced
(i.e., low SES) environments, only parents who often engaged in conversation around shared
television viewing had children whose curiosity scores were above the mean. Conversely, chil-
dren from more-resourced environments (i.e., high SES) had curiosity scores above the mean
even if parents hardly ever conversed when viewing television together. The “curiosity gap”
between higher and lower SES children was greatest when parents “never” or “hardly ever
engaged in television-related conversation but was not observed when conversational
exchanges occurred “often.” This suggests that for children from under-resourced environ-
ments, more frequent parent conversation may help enable the expression of curiosity. One
implication is that parents from low SES environments might benefit from anticipatory guid-
ance regarding the importance of dialogic (back and forth) conversation to promote inquisi-
tiveness and learning. Such guidance may include interventions similar to “parent coaching”
to facilitate conversational exchanges to promote early language development [51]. At present,
because the dominant screen activity of low-income children involves television viewing [52],
and because television viewing is essentially non-conversational and non-interactive [53], fos-
tering opportunities for conversational exchanges around television viewing (in addition to
other shared activities) may be one potential naturalistic intervention [47].
Our results also indicate that more frequent parent conversation was associated with parent
reports of higher imagination at kindergarten. The topics eliciting a child’s curiosity are often
related to a child’s idiosyncratic interests [54], and are revealed in the context of responsive,
interactive exchanges [55]. Because we hypothesize that conversation around shared television
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 10 / 15
viewing likely included pedagogical exchanges, (e.g., “What do you think is going on? Why do
you think that happened?”), our results suggest the possibility that more frequent conversation
(in all contexts, not just television viewing) can promote imaginative expression (one of the
underpinnings of curiosity [56]) at kindergarten. Interventions to promote dialogic exchanges
and language-rich caregiver-child interactions have been shown to be beneficial for early
imagination and learning, and may be similarly promotive for early childhood curiosity [57,
58].
Our study had several strengths and limitations. Strengths include the use of a nationally
representative sample which included a child behavior questionnaire from which we could
derive a measure of curiosity, whose results are generalizable to the population. One limitation
is that our study used parent self-reports to measure the amount of television viewing and par-
ent conversation, and our curiosity factor was derived from a single parent-report behavioral
measure at the kindergarten timepoint. As such, we acknowledge the potential bias and shared
method variance associated with parent report measures. In addition, although a subsample
analysis indicated that parents who engaged in more frequent television-related conversation
were more likely to use elaborative language, there was no independent measure of non-televi-
sion parent-child conversation for the entire sample, so we were unable to control for non-
screen time language. Although there was a teacher-report of child behavior at kindergarten, it
did not include all the “curiosity” items, so we could not examine curiosity across reporters. In
addition, the dataset did not contain information regarding the content of the television pro-
grams watched, which is a potential confounder which we were not able to include in our anal-
yses. We also acknowledge that while we found significant associations between the hours of
television viewing, frequency of parent conversation and parent reports of curiosity, our effect
sizes were small. Finally, while the ECLS-B is a rich dataset and among the only longitudinal
cohorts from the United States, the data are older, and did not include measures of smart-
phones and other portable technologies on which television programming may be watched,
along with more conversational media such as video-chatting, which is an additional limita-
tion. Future research should consider examining these associations in relation to use of con-
versational and non-conversational digital media across screen platforms. Future research
should also examine other features of curiosity that might help mitigate the poverty achieve-
ment gap [59], and consider other adaptive outcomes associated with early childhood curiosity
[60]. Despite these limitations, we believe that our results have some important implications
for caregivers and pediatricians.
Conclusions
Our results suggest that more frequent parent-child conversations around television viewing
(which may be a proxy for other conversational exchanges) are associated with higher curios-
ity, especially in children with socioeconomic disadvantage. This highlights the importance of
parents engaging in reciprocal conversations around topics and experiences of mutual interest
[47], and suggests the importance of finding opportunities foster conversational exchanges in
the context of daily routines (e.g., even when watching television). Aligning with the American
Academy of Pediatrics’ recommendations on media [61,62], parents can be counseled on the
value of parental instructive dialogue during television viewing (e.g. “active mediation”) [63],
as an opportunity to promote inquiry [64]. Parent-child conversations that are guided by
active mediation have been associated with more adaptive social-emotional development in
young children, with a greater magnitude of effect in children from low-income families [65].
Our work extends this line of research and highlights the benefits of active mediation on early
childhood curiosity. Because parent conversation around television viewing is likely related to
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 11 / 15
parent conversation in the home, our results also suggest the importance of fostering opportu-
nities for dialogic exchanges around all topics (not just television), especially for children from
environments of socioeconomic disadvantage [27].
Supporting information
S1 Appendix. Adjusted associations of daily television viewing, parent conversation during
shared television viewing and curiosity (Step 1 –main effects).
(DOCX)
S2 Appendix. Adjusted associations of daily television viewing, parent conversation during
shared television viewing and curiosity, moderated by SES (Step 2- moderation results,
prior to median split).
(DOCX)
S3 Appendix. Derived curiosity factor from the ECLS-B.
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Prachi E. Shah, Todd B. Kashdan, Katherine Rosenblum.
Data curation: Prachi E. Shah, Heidi M. Weeks, Niko Kaciroti.
Formal analysis: Heidi M. Weeks, Niko Kaciroti.
Funding acquisition: Prachi E. Shah.
Investigation: Prachi E. Shah.
Methodology: Prachi E. Shah, Katherine Rosenblum.
Project administration: Heidi M. Weeks.
Resources: Prachi E. Shah.
Supervision: Prachi E. Shah, Katherine Rosenblum.
Writing – original draft: Prachi E. Shah, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Todd B. Kashdan, Katherine
Rosenblum, Heidi M. Weeks, Priya Singh.
Writing – review & editing: Prachi E. Shah, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Todd B. Kashdan, Kristen
Harrison, Katherine Rosenblum, Heidi M. Weeks, Priya Singh, Niko Kaciroti.
References
1. Pennock RTM, J. D., editor Virtues of U.S. scientists guiding scientific practice. Symposia Presented at
the AAAS Annual Meeting; 2016; Washington, D.C.
2. Loewenstein G. The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin.
1994; 116(1):75–98.
3. Jirout J, Klahr D. Children’s scientific curiosity: In search of an operational definition of an elusive con-
cept. Developmental Review. 2012; 32(2):125–60.
4. Nakamura J, Csikszentmihalyi M. The concept of flow. In. Snyder CR & Lopez SJ (Eds.), Handbook of
positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2002.
5. McCombs BL. The learner-centered psychological principles: A framework for balancing academic
achievement and social-emotional learning outcomes. In: Zins JE, Weissberg RP, Wang MC, Wallberg
HJ, editors. Building academic success on social and emotional learning: What does the research say.
23. New York: Teachers College Press; 2004.
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 12 / 15
6. Ackermann L, Hepach R, Mani N. Children learn words easier when they are interested in the category
to which the word belongs. Developmental science. 2019:e12915. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12915
PMID: 31618505
7. Stahl AE, Feigenson L. Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science.
2015; 348(6230):91. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa3799 PMID: 25838378
8. Lepper MR, Corpus JH, Iyengar SS. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom:
age differences and academic correlates. Journal of educational psychology. 2005; 97(2):184.
9. Kashdan TB, Yuen M. Whether highly curious students thrive academically depends on perceptions
about the school learning environment: A study of Hong Kong adolescents. Motivation and Emotion.
2007; 31(4):260–70.
10. Shah PE, Weeks HM, Richards B, Kaciroti N. Early childhood curiosity and kindergarten reading and
math academic achievement. Pediatric research. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-018-0039-3
PMID: 29884846
11. Hassinger-Das B, Hirsh-Pasek K. Appetite for knowledge: curiosity and children’s academic achieve-
ment. Pediatric research. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-018-0099-4 PMID: 29967534
12. Rideout V. The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight.2017 August 16, 2019.
13. Tandon PS, Zhou C, Lozano P, Christakis DA. Preschoolers’ total daily screen time at home and by
type of child care. The Journal of pediatrics. 2011; 158(2):297–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.
2010.08.005 PMID: 20980020
14. Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Huberman HS, Alvir J, et al. Infant
television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeco-
nomic status households. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2008; 162(5):411–7. https://
doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.162.5.411 PMID: 18458186
15. Lapierre MA, Piotrowski JT, Linebarger DL. Background television in the homes of US children. Pediat-
rics. 2012; 130(5):839–46. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2581 PMID: 23027166
16. Yang-Huang J, van Grieken A, Moll HA, Jaddoe VWV, Wijtzes AI, Raat H. Socioeconomic differences
in children’s television viewing trajectory: A population-based prospective cohort study. PloS one. 2017;
12(12):e0188363. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188363 PMID: 29211770
17. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Peterson KE, Brophy-Herb HE, Horodynski MA, Contreras D, et al. Media Expo-
sure in Low-Income Preschool-Aged Children Is Associated with Multiple Measures of Self-Regulatory
Behavior. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics: JDBP. 2018; 39(4):303–9. https://doi.
org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000560 PMID: 29538186
18. Radesky JS, Silverstein M, Zuckerman B, Christakis DA. Infant Self-Regulation and Early Childhood
Media Exposure. Pediatrics. 2014; 133(5):e1172. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-2367 PMID:
24733868
19. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Berkule S, Fierman AH, Brockmeyer C, Mendelsohn AL. Infant media expo-
sure and toddler development. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2010; 164(12):1105–11.
https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.235 PMID: 21135338
20. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between media viewing andlanguage devel-
opment in children under age 2 years. The Journal of pediatrics. 2007; 151(4):364–8. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.071 PMID: 17889070
21. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association Between Screen Time and Children’s
Performance on a Developmental Screening Test. JAMA pediatrics. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1001/
jamapediatrics.2018.5056 PMID: 30688984
22. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Children’s television viewing and cognitive outcomes: a longitudinal
analysis of national data. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2005; 159(7):619–25. https://
doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.7.619 PMID: 15996993
23. Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, Schmidt ME, Anderson DR. The impact of background television
on parent-child interaction. Child Dev. 2009; 80(5):1350–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.
01337.x PMID: 19765004
24. Yogman M, Garner A, Hutchinson J, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. The Power of Play: A Pediatric Role
in Enhancing Development in Young Children. Pediatrics. 2018; 142(3). https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.
2018-2058 PMID: 30126932
25. Mendelsohn AL, Brockmeyer CA, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Berkule-Silberman SB, Tomopoulos S. Do
Verbal Interactions with Infants During Electronic Media Exposure Mitigate Adverse Impacts on their
Language Development as Toddlers? Infant Child Dev. 2010; 19(6):577–93. https://doi.org/10.1002/
icd.711 PMID: 21593996
26. Yu Y, Bonawitz E, Shafto P. Pedagogical Questions in Parent-Child Conversations. Child Dev. 2019;
90(1):147–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12850 PMID: 28617972
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 13 / 15
27. Hirsh-Pasek K, Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Owen MT, Golinkoff RM, Pace A, et al. The Contribution of
Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success. Psychological science.
2015; 26(7):1071–83. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493 PMID: 26048887
28. Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children: Paul
H Brookes Publishing; 1995.
29. Golinkoff RM, Hoff E, Rowe ML, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Hirsh-Pasek K. Language Matters: Denying the
Existence of the 30-Million-Word Gap Has Serious Consequences. Child Dev. 2019; 90(3):985–92.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13128 PMID: 30102419
30. Atkinson L, Beitchman J, Gonzalez A, Young A, Wilson B, Escobar M, et al. Cumulative risk, cumulative
outcome: a 20-year longitudinal study. PloS one. 2015; 10(6):e0127650. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0127650 PMID: 26030616
31. Snow K, Derecho A, Wheeless S, Lennon J, Rosen J, Rogers J, et al. Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) Kindergarten 2006 and 2007 Data File User’s Manual (2010–010). In:
Statistics NCfE, editor. Washington, D.C.: Institute of Educational Sciences, U.S. Department of Edu-
cation; 2009.
32. Piotrowski JT, Litman JA, Valkenburg P. Measuring epistemic curiosity in young children. Infant and
Child Development. 2014; 23(5):542–53.
33. Kidd C, Hayden Benjamin Y. The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity. Neuron. 2015; 88(3):449–
60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010 PMID: 26539887
34. Hall GS, Smith TL. Curiosity and Interest. Pedag Sem. 1903; X:315–58.
35. Berlyne DE. A theory of human curiosity. British Journal of Psychology General Section. 1954; 45
(3):180–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1954.tb01243.x PMID: 13190171
36. Maw W, Maw E. The measurement of curiosity in elementary school children. A summary Cooperative
Research Project. 1964(801).
37. Cook C, Goodman ND, Schulz LE. Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’
exploratory play. Cognition. 2011; 120(3):341–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.003 PMID:
21561605
38. Schulz LE, Bonawitz EB. Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is
confounded. Developmental psychology. 2007; 43(4):1045. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.
1045 PMID: 17605535
39. Lin L-Y, Cherng R-J, Chen Y-J, Chen Y-J, Yang H-M. Effects of television exposure on developmental
skills among young children. Infant Behavior and Development. 2015; 38:20–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.infbeh.2014.12.005 PMID: 25544743
40. Inc. SI. Base SAS(R) 9.4 Procedures Guide: Statistical Procedures. In: Inc. SI, editor. Cary, NC 2014.
41. Wicklin R. Statistical programming with SAS/IML software: SAS Institute; 2010.
42. Verdine BN, Zimmermann L, Foster L, Marzouk MA, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K, et al. Effects of geo-
metric toy design on parent–child interactions and spatial language. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly. 2019; 46:126–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.03.015 PMID: 30555211
43. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psy-
chological Bulletin. 2009; 135(6):885. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376 PMID: 19883141
44. Merz EC, Maskus EA, Melvin SA, He X, Noble KG. Socioeconomic disparities in language input are
associated with children’s language-related brain structure and reading skills. Child development. 2020;
91(3):846–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13239 PMID: 30919945
45. Pace A, Luo R, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and
language development. Annual Review of Linguistics. 2017; 3(1):285–308.
46. Maw WH, Maw EW. Self-Concepts of High- and Low-Curiosity Boys. Child Development. 1970; 41
(1):123–9. PMID: 5490675
47. Zimmerman FJ, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Christakis DA, Xu D, Gray S, et al. Teaching by listening:
The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics. 2009; 124(1):342–9.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2267 PMID: 19564318
48. Vernon-Feagans L, Bratsch-Hines ME, Investigators FLPK. Caregiver–child verbal interactions in child
care: A buffer against poor language outcomes when maternal language input is less. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly. 2013; 28(4):858–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.08.002 PMID:
24634566
49. Bigelow AE, MacLean K, Proctor J. The role of joint attention in the development of infants’ play with
objects. Developmental science. 2004; 7(5):518–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00375.x
PMID: 15603284
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 14 / 15
50. Hirsh-Pasek K, Zosh JM, Golinkoff RM, Gray JH, Robb MB, Kaufman J. Putting education in “educa-
tional” apps: Lessons from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2015;
16(1):3–34.
51. Ferjan Ramirez N, Lytle SR, Fish M, Kuhl PK. Parent coaching at 6 and 10 months improves language
outcomes at 14 months: A randomized controlled trial. Developmental science. 2019; 22(3):e12762.
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12762 PMID: 30318708
52. Fletcher EN, Whitaker RC, Marino AJ, Anderson SE. Screen Time at Home and School among Low-
Income Children Attending Head Start. Child Indic Res. 2014; 7(2):421–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12187-013-9212-8 PMID: 24891924
53. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM, Xu D, et al. Audible Television
and Decreased Adult Words, Infant Vocalizations, and Conversational Turns: A Population-Based
Study. JAMA pediatrics. 2009; 163(6):554–8.
54. Alvarez AL, Booth AE. Motivated by Meaning: Testing the Effect of Knowledge-Infused Rewards on
Preschoolers’ Persistence. Child Development. 2014; 85(2):783–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.
12151 PMID: 24033222
55. Dore RA, Zosh JM, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Chapter 4—Plugging Into Word Learning: The Role
of Electronic Toys and Digital Media in Language Development. In: Blumberg FC, Brooks PJ, editors.
Cognitive Development in Digital Contexts. San Diego: Academic Press; 2017. p. 75–91.
56. Arasteh JD. CREATIVITY AND RELATED PROCESSES IN THE YOUNG CHILD: A REVIEW OF THE
LITERATURE. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development.
1968; 112(1):77–108.
57. Hassinger-Das B, Bustamante AS, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Learning landscapes: Playing the way
to learning and engagement in public spaces. Education Sciences. 2018; 8(2):74.
58. Hadani HS, Winthrop R, Hirsh-Pasek K. Playful Learning Landscapes: Convergence of Education and
City Planning. Powering a Learning Society During an Age of Disruption: Springer, Singapore; 2021. p.
151–64. https://doi.org/10.5603/PJNNS.a2021.0017 PMID: 33559873
59. Kashdan TB, Stiksma MC, Disabato DD, McKnight PE, Bekier J, Kaji J, et al. Thefive-dimensional curi-
osity scale: Capturing the bandwidth of curiosity and identifying four unique subgroups of curious peo-
ple. Journal of Research in Personality. 2018; 73:130–49.
60. Kashdan TB, Sherman RA, Yarbro J, Funder DC. How Are Curious People Viewed and How Do They
Behave in Social Situations? From the Perspectives of Self, Friends, Parents, and Unacquainted
Observers. Journal of Personality. 2013; 81(2):142–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.
00796.x PMID: 22583101
61. American Academy of Pediatrics. Children, Adolescents, and the Media. Pediatrics. 2013; 132(5):958.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-2656 PMID: 28448255
62. American Academy of Pediatrics. Media and Young Minds. Pediatrics. 2016; 138(5):e20162591.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2591 PMID: 27940793
63. Harrison K, Liechty JM, The SKP. US Preschoolers’ Media Exposure and Dietary Habits: The primacy
of television and the limits of parental mediation. Journal of Children and Media. 2012; 6(1):18–36.
64. Coyne SM, Radesky J, Collier KM, Gentile DA, Linder JR, Nathanson AI, et al. Parenting and Digital
Media. Pediatrics. 2017; 140(Supplement 2):S112. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758N PMID:
29093044
65. Rasmussen EE, Shafer A, Colwell MJ, White S, Punyanunt-Carter N, Densley RL, et al. Relation
between active mediation, exposure to Daniel Tiger’s Neighborhood, and US preschoolers’ social and
emotional development. Journal of Children and Media. 2016; 10(4):443–61.
PLOS ONE
Daily television exposure, parent conversation, SES and curiosity
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258572 October 28, 2021 15 / 15
... Whether usage occurs with someone else could also be important. Shah et al. (2021) found that greater conversation between caregivers and their children during screen time was associated with greater levels of curiosity, and this was particularly the case for children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds. ...
... Caregivers speak more and with a richer vocabulary to their toddlers during storybook reading than during TV co-viewing (Hanson et al., 2021). Shah et al. (2021) found that greater conversation between caregivers and their children during screen time was associated with greater levels of curiosity, and this was particularly the case for children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds. Those watching alone in the current study could perhaps be more independent and engaged in more curiositydriven behavior. ...
... Time spent on screens could replace children's time conversing with others, which could affect language development (Hanson et al., 2021). Time spent on screens could also lead to a reliance on bottom-up processing (Essex et al., 2022), and replace curiositydriven behavior (Shah et al., 2021). It is not entirely certain that it is screen time per se that is driving the findings or a lack of other activities. ...
Article
Preschool children's exposure to screen media and associations with sleep, language, and cognition were investigated along with the time of day of screen exposure, content type, and whether use occurred with someone. Caregivers of Australian children, aged 2 years, 11 months to 5 years, 11 months, answered questions online about the durations children engaged with entertainment, relaxing/calming, and educational content. Fifty-nine percent of children engaged with relaxing/calming content and 86 % with educational, but all children engaged with entertainment content, which became the focus of the analyses. Average daily durations engaged with relaxing and educational content were 1 h each and 2 h for entertainment content. Longer time spent engaged with entertainment content was associated with shorter sleep duration and poorer sleep quality. The interaction between screen time and usage at night vs. daytime only was non-significant suggesting that the association with sleep duration was similar regardless of time of day of usage. Greater screen time also predicted lower communication and problem solving scores, and more attention difficulties. Engaging in screen content with someone else was associated with poorer problem solving skills, whereas engaging alone was associated with better problem solving. The findings here indicate that preschoolers largely engage in entertainment content and this has implications for their sleep even when screen engagement predominantly occurs during the day. Greater screen time also has implications for cognitive and language development raising questions about the time children spend on screens that could be spent on activities that better support development.
... A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to assure reliability and to calculate the appropriate loading values for deriving our curiosity factor. Standardized scoring of the curiosity factor was conducted, and good internal consistency was demonstrated (α = 0.70, M = 0.07, SD = 1.2) (Shah et al., 2021). ...
... We theorize that these same "built environments, " especially in disadvantaged communities, can foster early childhood curiosity by creating interactive opportunities to engage in conversational exchanges that are dotted with questions (Gaudreau et al., 2021). Our previous research found an association between more frequent parent conversation (during share television viewing) and higher kindergarten curiosity, with a greater magnitude of association in children from low-SES families (Shah et al., 2021). Research from Playful Learning Landscapes including Urban Thinkscape and other projects like Parkopolis (Bustamante et al., 2020); a life-sized human board game) and Fractionball (Bustamante et al., 2022); a recrafted basketball court designed to promote fraction talk) demonstrate that transforming neighborhood spaces into areas which prioritize caregiver-child interactions, facilitate language-learning opportunities which are promotive of school readiness and question asking-a behavior associated with curiosity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Curiosity is an important social-emotional process underlying early learning. Our previous work found a positive association between higher curiosity and higher academic achievement at kindergarten, with a greater magnitude of benefit for children with socioeconomic disadvantage. Because characteristics of the early caregiving and physical environment impact the processes that underlie early learning, we sought to examine early environmental experiences associated with early childhood curiosity, in hopes of identifying modifiable contexts that may promote its expression. Methods Using data from a nationally representative sample of 4,750 children from the United States, this study examined the association of multi-level ecological contexts (i.e., neighborhood safety, parenting quality, home environment, and center-based preschool enrollment) on early childhood curiosity at kindergarten, and tested for moderation by socioeconomic status. Results In adjusted, stratified models, children from lower-resourced environments (characterized by the lowest-SES tertile) manifested higher curiosity if they experienced more positive parenting, higher quality home environments, and if they lived in “very safe” neighborhoods. Discussion We discuss the ecological contexts (i.e., parenting, home, and neighborhood environments) that are promotive of early childhood curiosity, with an emphasis on the role of the neighborhood safety and the “neighborhood built environment” as important modifiable contexts to foster early childhood curiosity in lower-resourced families.
... According to the recent study of Gaudreau, Hirsh-Pasek,and Golinkoff (2022), children posed a lower number of questions in the cell phone compared to the control condition. Shah et al. (2021) observed a positive relationship between heightened curiosity levels in kindergarten children and a higher occurrence of parental conversation during shared television viewing. This association exhibited a more pronounced effect within low socioeconomic status (SES) families. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of 10 popular very young learner (VYL) videos on YouTube. The purpose of the study was to investigate the suitability of these videos for children aged 2-6 years in terms of their educational content, visual and auditory quality, and engagement value. Content analysis was used to analyze the videos. A total of 2 raters were conducted content analysis for the study. They were asked to rate each video on a rubric. The study concludes that careful consideration must be given when selecting videos for VYLs, as their effectiveness depends on several factors, such as age appropriateness, educational content, and visual and auditory quality. The results show that most YouTuber videos used multimedia learning principles effectively in their videos, with varying degrees of success.
... It is reported that blue light exposure from electronic devices at night suppresses melatonin secretion and is associated with late bedtimes [18][19][20]. On the other hand, talking with parents while watching television made children more curious [21] and improved their learning from experience [22]. Not only the duration and timing of media viewing, but also the type of media consumption is important. ...
Article
Background Good sleep is essential for children’s healthy growth. In 2005, we conducted a questionnaire survey on children’s sleep habits and their background, targeting parents who attended health checkups for their 3-year-old children in Asahikawa City, Hokkaido. In 2020, we performed a secondary survey, including additional questions regarding media usage. We analyzed changes in children’s sleep environment by comparing the results of both surveys. Methods Children from 500 families (n = 420; 219 males, 201 females; mean age, 3.6 years) who underwent 3.5-year-old health checkups (per the changed schedule in 2015) in Asahikawa City from July 2020 to November 2020 and their parents who had completely answered the questionnaire were included. Results The proportion of children who used childcare support system such as nursery schools or kindergarten increased from 30% in the previous survey to 95% in the present survey. The mean nocturnal sleep duration of children was 9.33 h in the present survey, 0.77 h shorter than that in the previous survey; similar to the previous survey results, it was significantly short (8.71 h) in children who went to bed after 10 PM. Moreover, it was significantly short in children who watched television for more than two hours or used media within two hours before going to bed or if parents used smartphones or watched motion pictures for >30 min/day. The rate of consulting pediatricians regarding sleep problems decreased from 3% to 2.4%. Conclusion Parents’ lifestyles greatly influenced children’s sleep habits in 2020. Pediatricians should actively participate in managing children’s sleep problems.
... Studies show an inverse association between parental education, particularly maternal education, and the time of exposure in children, particularly at earlier ages. Low socioeconomic status, single-parent families and high parental exposure habits are also associated with an increase in children's exposure time (9,10,11) . ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To characterize the habits of screen exposure time in a sample of infants and preschoolers and to assess if there is a relationship between the proportion of early childhood excessive screen exposure time and the presence of psychopathology and parental concerns. Methods: A cross-sectional cohort study was conducted with 38 infants and preschoolers in a Child and Adolescent Psychiatric outpatient unit and children followed exclusively in Primary Health Care in the same geographic area (Vila Nova de Gaia/ Espinho Hospital Center). Information was collected from a self-report questionnaire filled by the caregiver between October 1st, 2018, and June 30th, 2019. Results: Screen time was analyzed and organized in two groups: the H group (screen time higher than recommended) and R group (within the recommended), according to the American Academy of Pediatrics. The need for referral to a Child and Adolescent Psychiatry appointment and the presence of parental behavior concerns related to behavior changes during early childhood are significantly associated with screen time, with a greater proportion within the H group (71.8% (n=15) vs. 31.3% (n=6), p=0.006 for the appointment and 61.1% (n=13) vs. 25% (n=4), p=0.032 for behavior concerns). There is also a tendency towards a higher percentage of overweight/obesity, sleep and food-related concerns in the H group. Only 45% of the total sample fulfilled the recommendations regarding screen exposure (p value ≤0.05). Conclusion: The study found an association between screen exposure time above the recommended and presence of psychopathology and parental concern for behavioral changes. These findings were statistically significant.
Article
מגיל צעיר ילדוֺת וילדים היום מוקפים באמצעי תקשורת דיגיטליים, ניידים ואינטראקטיביים. החשיפה ההולכת וגוברת של פעוטות למסכים עשויה להיות הזדמנות להתפתחות, משחק ולמידה, אך עלולה להיות גם מזיקה. הורים לפעוטות מדווחים על היעדר כלים להעריך את התאמתם של תכנים ברשת ושל אמצעי התקשורת הדיגיטליים לילדיהם. המחקר הנוכחי בחן את מעורבותם הפעילה של ההורים בתוכנית לימודים ניסיונית שנערכה בגן לגילאי שלוש עד ארבע, המבוססת על צילום במצלמות דיגיטליות. הורי הילדים קיבלו עליהם לבצע מטלות צילומיות משותפות עם ילדיהם, והמחקר בחן את תרומת התוכנית לילדים ולמבוגרים שבסביבתם בטיפוח אוריינות חזותית. שאלת המחקר הייתה כיצד תיווכו הגננת וההורים לילדים את השימוש במצלמה לצורכי למידה והנאה וכיצד הם תופסים ומעריכים את תצלומי הפעוטות. שאלה נוספת הייתה כיצד תיווך משותף זה, שיוצר רצף חינוכי, תורם לרכישת מיומנויות חזותיות ודיגיטליות של הילדים והילדות. המחקר התבסס על כלי מחקר איכותניים מגוונים שכללו תיעוד מצולם של הילדים בווידאו ובתמונות סטילס, שיחות עם ההורים ועם הגננת וניתוח של תוכנית הלימודים הניסיונית ותוצריה הצילומיים. הראיונות והתצפיות מעידים כי הצלחת התוכנית קשורה לתיווך החינוכי ולשיתוף האינטגרלי של ההורים בצילום עם הילדים. ומכאן עולה המסקנה כי תהליך חינוכי מתמשך שבו פעוטות חווים בבית ובגן תהליכים מקבילים המחזקים אלה את אלה מניב אוריינות חזותית ודיגיטלית מיטיבה. תיווך הורי משתתף, כמו זה המוצע כאן בהקשר של צילום, יכול להיות מיושם גם ביחס לאמצעים דיגיטליים אחרים
Article
Infants engage in prosocial behaviors from very young ages and their prosocial behavior is clearly present by the second year of life. Physiological regulation has been related to infants' emerging prosocial behavior, with infants with better regulatory skills displaying increased prosocial behavior. Media use and media content has also been related to infants emerging prosocial behavior. The current study examined individual differences in infants' vagal regulation (as physiological marker of regulatory skills, N = 269) while engaging with emotionally salient media and how individual differences in vagal regulation were related to parental and child empathy, prosocial behavior, and media use. The study included parent-infant dyads co-viewing a brief educational but emotionally salient video clip, an observational prosocial sticker and sharing tasks, and parental reports of infants' prosocial media use, empathy, and parents' own prosocial media use, prosocial behavior, and empathy. Most infants displayed an increase in RSA while co-viewing the media clip relative to a neutral baseline. Infants with higher RSA (a sign of better regulation) during co-viewing displayed more prosocial behavior in the sticker and pen drop tasks, were rated as more empathic, and had primary caregivers who engaged with more prosocial media than infants who showed a decrease in RSA (e.g., more immature regulatory skills) during co-viewing. Results suggest the importance of vagal regulation in the presence of others' emotions for infants emerging prosocial behavior. Results also suggest that parents' own prosocial media viewing may indirectly influence on the way infants' respond physiologically to others’ emotions.
Chapter
Full-text available
Playful Learning Landscapes (PLL) has become a viable, evidence-based approach for addressing inequity in learning by merging architectural design and placemaking with the science of learning. PLL embeds learning opportunities in places where families regularly go (e.g., bus stops, supermarkets, and laundromats) and transforms them into engaging and enriched learning hubs. Learning outside the classroom in museums, libraries, and with digital media has been growing in importance and was further accentuated due to the coronavirus disease. As a result, the role of families and the community has been elevated as a critical component of education. To address gaps in school readiness and achievement, policy makers have largely focused on formal learning environments. But these efforts neither address nor harness the 80% of time young children spend outside of the classroom with their families. PLL fills this gap and offers one way of harnessing innovation to help reduce education inequality and promote individual and community development—all key factors for an effective learning society.
Article
Full-text available
The overall pattern of vocabulary development is relatively similar across children learning different languages. However, there are considerable differences in the words known to individual children. Historically, this variability has been explained in terms of differences in the input. Here, we examine the alternate possibility that children's individual interest in specific natural categories shapes the words they are likely to learn – a child who is more interested in animals will learn a new animal name easier relative to a new vehicle name. Two‐year‐old German‐learning children (N = 39) were exposed to four novel word‐object associations for objects from four different categories. Prior to the word learning task, we measured their interest in the categories that the objects belonged to. Our measure was pupillary change following exposure to familiar objects from these four categories, with increased pupillary change interpreted as increased interest in that category. Children showed more robust learning of word‐object associations from categories they were more interested in relative to categories they were less interested in. We further found that interest in the novel objects themselves influenced learning, with distinct influences of both category interest and object interest on learning. These results suggest that children's interest in different natural categories shapes their word learning. This provides evidence for the strikingly intuitive possibility that a child who is more interested in animals will learn novel animal names easier than a child who is more interested in vehicles.
Article
Full-text available
The mechanisms underlying socioeconomic disparities in children's reading skills are not well understood. This study examined associations among socioeconomic background, home linguistic input, brain structure, and reading skills in 5‐to‐9‐year‐old children (N = 94). Naturalistic home audio recordings and high‐resolution, T1‐weighted MRI scans were acquired. Children who experienced more adult–child conversational turns or adult words had greater left perisylvian cortical surface area. Language input mediated the association between parental education and left perisylvian cortical surface area. Language input was indirectly associated with children's reading skills via left perisylvian surface area. Left perisylvian surface area mediated the association between parental education and children's reading skills. Language experience may thus partially explain socioeconomic disparities in language‐supporting brain structure and in turn reading skills.
Article
Full-text available
Children from under-resourced communities regularly enter formal schooling lagging behind their peers. These deficits in areas such as language development, reading readiness, and even in the kind of spatial skills that predict later mathematical knowledge, may persist throughout their lifespan. To address such gaps, policymakers have focused largely on schooling as the great equalizer. Yet, children only spend 20% of their waking hours in school. How can developmental scientists and educators address this "other 80%" for the benefit of children's development? One answer is the Learning Landscapes initiative, which involves crafting carefully planned play experiences that focus on learning outcomes, particularly for children and families from under-resourced communities. Playful learning, a broad pedagogical approach featuring child-directed play methods, provides a unique way to foster learning and engagement organically within the built environment. Learning Landscapes already incorporates several well-documented projects. The Ultimate Block Party brought over 50,000 people to Central Park to engage in playful learning activities. Supermarkets became hotspots for caregiver-child interaction by simply adding prompts for caregiver-child interaction through signage in everyday "trapped" experiences. Urban Thinkscape transformed a bus stop and adjacent lot into a hub for playful learning while families were waiting for public transportation. Finally, Parkopolis is a life-size human board game that fosters STEM and reasoning skills in public spaces. This paper reflects on data from these projects while reflecting on lessons learned and future directions.
Article
Full-text available
Children from under-resourced communities regularly enter formal schooling lagging behind their peers. These deficits in areas such as language development, reading readiness, and even in the kind of spatial skills that predict later mathematical knowledge, may persist throughout their lifespan. To address such gaps, policymakers have focused largely on schooling as the great equalizer. Yet, children only spend 20% of their waking hours in school. How can developmental scientists and educators address this “other 80%” for the benefit of children’s development? One answer is the Learning Landscapes initiative, which involves crafting carefully planned play experiences that focus on learning outcomes, particularly for children and families from under-resourced communities. Playful learning, a broad pedagogical approach featuring child-directed play methods, provides a unique way to foster learning and engagement organically within the built environment. Learning Landscapes already incorporates several well-documented projects. The Ultimate Block Party brought over 50,000 people to Central Park to engage in playful learning activities. Supermarkets became hotspots for caregiver-child interaction by simply adding prompts for caregiver-child interaction through signage in everyday “trapped” experiences. Urban Thinkscape transformed a bus stop and adjacent lot into a hub for playful learning while families were waiting for public transportation. Finally, Parkopolis is a life-size human board game that fosters STEM and reasoning skills in public spaces. This paper reflects on data from these projects while reflecting on lessons learned and future directions.
Article
Importance: Excessive screen time is associated with delays in development; however, it is unclear if greater screen time predicts lower performance scores on developmental screening tests or if children with poor developmental performance receive added screen time as a way to modulate challenging behavior. Objective: To assess the directional association between screen time and child development in a population of mothers and children. Design, setting, and participants: This longitudinal cohort study used a 3-wave, cross-lagged panel model in 2441 mothers and children in Calgary, Alberta, Canada, drawn from the All Our Families study. Data were available when children were aged 24, 36, and 60 months. Data were collected between October 20, 2011, and October 6, 2016. Statistical analyses were conducted from July 31 to November 15, 2018. Exposures: Media. Main outcomes and measures: At age 24, 36, and 60 months, children's screen-time behavior (total hours per week) and developmental outcomes (Ages and Stages Questionnaire, Third Edition) were assessed via maternal report. Results: Of the 2441 children included in the analysis, 1169 (47.9%) were boys. A random-intercepts, cross-lagged panel model revealed that higher levels of screen time at 24 and 36 months were significantly associated with poorer performance on developmental screening tests at 36 months (β, -0.08; 95% CI, -0.13 to -0.02) and 60 months (β, -0.06; 95% CI, -0.13 to -0.02), respectively. These within-person (time-varying) associations statistically controlled for between-person (stable) differences. Conclusions and relevance: The results of this study support the directional association between screen time and child development. Recommendations include encouraging family media plans, as well as managing screen time, to offset the potential consequences of excess use.
Article
Previous studies reveal an association between particular features of parental language input and advances in children's language learning. However, it is not known whether parent coaching aimed to enhance specific input components would (i) successfully increase these components in parents’ language input, and (ii) result in concurrent increases in children's language development. The present randomized controlled trial assigned families of typically developing 6‐month‐old infants to Intervention (parent coaching) and Control (no coaching) groups. Families were equivalent on socioeconomic status, infants’ gender, and infants’ age. Parent coaching took place when infants were 6 and 10 months of age, and included quantitative and qualitative linguistic feedback on the amount of child directed speech, back‐and‐forth interactions, and parentese speech style. These variables were derived from each family's first‐person LENA recordings at home. Input variables and infant language were measured at 6, 10, and 14 months. Parent coaching significantly enhanced language input as measured by two social interaction variables: percentage of speech directed to the child and percentage of parentese speech. These two variables were correlated, and were both related to growth in infant babbling between 6 and 14 months. Intervention infants showed greater growth in babbling than control infants. Furthermore, at 14 months, intervention infants produced significantly more words than control infants, as indicated by LENA recordings and parent report via the MacArthur‐Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory. Together, these results indicate that parent coaching can enrich specific aspects of parental language input, and can immediately and positively impact child language outcomes. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Children need to develop a variety of skill sets to optimize their development and manage toxic stress. Research demonstrates that developmentally appropriate play with parents and peers is a singular opportunity to promote the social-emotional, cognitive, language, and self-regulation skills that build executive function and a prosocial brain. Furthermore, play supports the formation of the safe, stable, and nurturing relationships with all caregivers that children need to thrive. Play is not frivolous: it enhances brain structure and function and promotes executive function (ie, the process of learning, rather than the content), which allow us to pursue goals and ignore distractions. When play and safe, stable, nurturing relationships are missing in a child’s life, toxic stress can disrupt the development of executive function and the learning of prosocial behavior; in the presence of childhood adversity, play becomes even more important. The mutual joy and shared communication and attunement (harmonious serve and return interactions) that parents and children can experience during play regulate the body’s stress response. This clinical report provides pediatric providers with the information they need to promote the benefits of play and and to write a prescription for play at well visits to complement reach out and read. At a time when early childhood programs are pressured to add more didactic components and less playful learning, pediatricians can play an important role in emphasizing the role of a balanced curriculum that includes the importance of playful learning for the promotion of healthy child development.
Article
Sperry, Sperry, and Miller (2018) aim to debunk what is called the 30‐million‐word gap by claiming that children from lower income households hear more speech than Hart and Risley (1995) reported. We address why the 30‐million‐word gap should not be abandoned, and the importance of retaining focus on the vital ingredient to language learning—quality speech directed to children rather than overheard speech, the focus of Sperry et al.'s argument. Three issues are addressed: Whether there is a language gap; the characteristics of speech that promote language development; and the importance of language in school achievement. There are serious risks to claims that low‐income children, on average, hear sufficient, high‐quality language relative to peers from higher income homes.
Article
Curiosity is critical for the development of knowledge, even from the earliest of ages. From infants’ motivation to explore their immediate physical environment to kindergarteners questioning why the sky is blue, curiosity drives our acquisition of new understandings about the wider world. Subject to many definitional debates, curiosity is both a state and a trait related to the need for new knowledge and the drive to explore to find it. In the current issue of Pediatric Research, Shah, Weeks, Richards, and Kaciroti examine the relations among kindergarten children’s curiosity and their academic achievement in both reading and mathematics in the article “Early Childhood Curiosity and Kindergarten Reading and Math Academic Achievement.” Using a large, representative sample of children enrolled in kindergarten in the United States, the authors found that higher levels of curiosity were associated with greater mathematics and reading achievement in kindergarten. Interestingly, this relation was not moderated by the biological sex of the children or measurements of their attention and persistence (effortful control). However, for children experiencing poverty, curiosity was more highly associated with reading and mathematics achievement than for children from higher socioeconomic backgrounds. Throughout their article, Shah and colleagues demonstrate the potential importance of curiosity for supporting children’s academic achievement—and highlight the possibility that it may benefit children to participate in activities that foster their creativity in early childhood.