ArticlePDF Available

Future-relevant memories are not selectively strengthened during sleep

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Overnight consolidation processes are thought to operate in a selective manner, such that important (i.e. future-relevant) memories are strengthened ahead of irrelevant information. Using an online protocol, we sought to replicate the seminal finding that the memory benefits of sleep are enhanced when people expect a future test [Wilhelm et al., 2011]. Participants memorised verbal paired associates to a criterion of 60 percent (Experiment 1) or 40 percent correct (Experiment 2) before a 12-hour delay containing overnight sleep (sleep group) or daytime wakefulness (wake group). Critically, half of the participants were informed that they would be tested again the following day, whereas the other half were told that they would carry out a different set of tasks. We observed a robust memory benefit of overnight consolidation, with the sleep group outperforming the wake group in both experiments. However, knowledge of an upcoming test had no impact on sleep-associated consolidation in either experiment, suggesting that overnight memory processes were not enhanced for future-relevant information. These findings, together with other failed replication attempts, show that sleep does not provide selective support to memories that are deemed relevant for the future.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Future-relevant memories are not selectively
strengthened during sleep
Jennifer E. AshtonID
1
*, Scott A. Cairney
1,2
1Department of Psychology, University of York, York, United Kingdom, 2York Biomedical Research
Institute, University of York, York, United Kingdom
*jennifer.ashton@york.ac.uk
Abstract
Overnight consolidation processes are thought to operate in a selective manner, such that
important (i.e. future-relevant) memories are strengthened ahead of irrelevant information.
Using an online protocol, we sought to replicate the seminal finding that the memory benefits
of sleep are enhanced when people expect a future test [Wilhelm et al., 2011]. Participants
memorised verbal paired associates to a criterion of 60 percent (Experiment 1) or 40 percent
correct (Experiment 2) before a 12-hour delay containing overnight sleep (sleep group) or
daytime wakefulness (wake group). Critically, half of the participants were informed that
they would be tested again the following day, whereas the other half were told that they
would carry out a different set of tasks. We observed a robust memory benefit of overnight
consolidation, with the sleep group outperforming the wake group in both experiments. How-
ever, knowledge of an upcoming test had no impact on sleep-associated consolidation in
either experiment, suggesting that overnight memory processes were not enhanced for
future-relevant information. These findings, together with other failed replication attempts,
show that sleep does not provide selective support to memories that are deemed relevant
for the future.
Introduction
It is now well established that sleep supports the consolidation of newly acquired declarative
memories [14]. Sleep consistently results in better memory retention when compared to
equivalent periods of wakefulness [1,2,510] and these behavioural effects have been associated
with specific features of sleep (e.g. slow-wave sleep [1113]). Contemporary models of sleep-
associated consolidation suggest that newly formed memories are reactivated during sleep,
prompting their migration from hippocampus to neocortex for long-term storage [1,2,13,14].
However, new memories do not all profit equally from sleep (i.e. some memories benefit more
from sleep than others), and the factors that determine to what extent a memory will benefit
from overnight consolidation are not fully understood.
There is growing evidence to suggest that sleep offers special protection to salient informa-
tion. For example, the benefits of sleep are enhanced for memories that are considered emo-
tionally negative [1519], that are associated with monetary reward [20] or that are deemed
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 1 / 14
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Ashton JE, Cairney SA (2021) Future-
relevant memories are not selectively strengthened
during sleep. PLoS ONE 16(11): e0258110. https://
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110
Editor: Bradley R. King, University of Utah, UNITED
STATES
Received: June 9, 2021
Accepted: September 18, 2021
Published: November 4, 2021
Copyright: ©2021 Ashton, Cairney. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: Study data are
available on the Open Science Framework database
(osf.io/3nqpd).
Funding: This work was supported by a Medical
Research Council Career Development Award (MR/
P020208/1) to S.A.C. The funders had no role in
study design, data collection and analysis, decision
to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
relevant for the future [2123]. The perceived relevance of newly learned information can be
manipulated experimentally by controlling test expectancy; that is, whether or not an individ-
ual expects their memory to be assessed. In their seminal study, Wilhelm et al. [24] trained par-
ticipants on a declarative memory task before a delay containing sleep or wakefulness.
Crucially, half of the participants were informed that they would be re-tested in a later session,
whereas the other half were told that they would complete a different set of tasks. Among par-
ticipants that slept (but not those who remained awake), declarative memory retention was
better for individuals who expected a test, as compared to those who did not, suggesting that
sleep had selectively strengthened future-relevant information.
However, other findings concerning the selective influences of sleep have been somewhat
contradictory. A number of studies have failed to observe a preferential benefit of sleep on
emotionally negative relative to neutral memories [6,2530], and others have observed no
impact of monetary reward on sleep-associated consolidation [3133]. In recent work, knowl-
edge of a future test had no impact on the overnight retention of declarative or nondeclarative
memories [34], and failed to influence the neural correlates of memory retention after sleep
[35]. The circumstances by which future expectations affect the salience of newly acquired
information, and the subsequent impacts on sleep-associated memory processing, therefore
remain unclear.
To address this issue, we sought to replicate the seminal findings of Wilhelm et al. [24] in
an online protocol and test the hypothesis that the benefits of sleep for memory are amplified
when newly learned information is deemed relevant for the future. In Experiment 1, partici-
pants completed verbal paired associates training (encoding and baseline test) before a
12-hour retention interval containing overnight sleep or daytime wakefulness. Critically, after
training, half of the participants were informed that they would be tested again in the following
session, whereas the other half were told to expect a different set of tasks. All participants were
re-tested 12 hours later, and again after 7 days, allowing us to study the impact of test expec-
tancy on sleep-associated consolidation and to determine the extent to which these effects are
maintained in the longer term. Because participants received feedback on their final round of
baseline testing, we expected to observe an improvement in memory performance between the
baseline and follow-up tests. Importantly, however, we also expected that the extent of this
improvement would be greater after sleep than wakefulness and, furthermore, that this sleep-
associated memory gain would be augmented for individuals who had knowledge of a future
test.
There is growing evidence to suggest that sleep favours the consolidation of weakly-
encoded relative to strongly-encoded memories [3638]. We therefore reasoned that if base-
line performance levels are too high, then knowledge of a future test might not augment sleep-
associated consolidation. We tested this possibility in our second, pre-registered experiment
(osf.io/3k6ej), by adjusting our paired associates memory paradigm to reduce pre-sleep
performance.
Experiment 1
Methods
Participants. Two-hundred and thirty adults (163 women, mean ±SD age: 24.89 ±3.66
years) were recruited online via Prolific (https://app.prolific.co/) and reported to be living in
the UK with English as their first language. On the days of the experiment, participants were
asked to abstain from alcohol and avoid taking naps. Informed consent was obtained from all
participants in line with the Research Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology at
the University of York. Only participants who expected/did not expect a memory test in line
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 2 / 14
with their respective condition were included in the final analysis (e.g. individuals in the unex-
pected condition who reported to have expected a follow-up test after the baseline test were
excluded; see Follow-up questionnaires below). The number of participants excluded from
each condition was: sleep-expected n= 12, sleep-unexpected n= 19, wake-expected n= 10,
wake-unexpected n= 25. A further two participants were excluded; one for reporting to have
used pen and paper to memorise the stimuli, and another who reported napping during the
day of the study. This resulted in a final sample of 162 participants (117 women, mean ±SD
age: 25.04 ±3.74 years) who were allocated to one of four conditions: sleep-expected (n= 42),
sleep-unexpected (n= 39), wake-expected (n= 50) or wake-unexpected (n= 31). Assignment
to the expected and unexpected conditions was determined by the time that participants
signed up for the study (AM or PM), which alternated between testing days. Evening sign ups
were always allocated to the sleep group and morning sign ups were always allocated to the
wake group.
Our sample size was calculated using an effect size reported in Wilhelm et al. [24]. The
effect of interest (d= 0.86) was derived from a t-test comparing memory retention after sleep
in individuals who did and did not expect a test. We determined that a minimum sample size
of n= 120 (n= 30 per condition) would be necessary for 95% power (one-tailed, alpha = .05)
to detect an effect of this magnitude. Data collection continued until our desired sample size
was met, with each participant providing a full and usable data set.
Procedure. A schematic of the study is shown in Fig 1. All participants completed verbal
paired associates training (encoding and baseline memory test) in the morning (between 7am
and 9am; wake groups) or evening (between 7pm and 9pm; sleep groups). Participants then
returned 12 hours and 7 days after encoding to complete follow-up tests.
Critically, after the baseline test, half of the participants (in both the wake and sleep groups)
were informed that their memory for the pairs would be tested in the next session (expected
condition), whereas the other half were told they would carry out a different set of tasks (unex-
pected condition).
Encoding and baseline assessment. The paired associates task required participants to learn
40 semantically-related word pairs (e.g. horizonsun). On each encoding trial, a randomly
selected word pair was presented in the centre of the screen for 5 s, followed by an inter-stimu-
lus interval of 200 ms. Participants were instructed to commit each pair to memory by imagin-
ing a scenario in which the words were interacting (for example, one might imagine the sun
coming up over the horizon in the example above). Baseline memory performance was
assessed immediately after encoding using a cued-recall procedure (for all 40 word pairs). On
each trial, the first word (cue) of a randomly selected pair was presented on the centre left of
the screen and participants were required to type the associated second word (target). Partici-
pants had 10 s to provide each response and submitted their answer by pressing ‘enter’ on the
Fig 1. Study procedure. All participants completed a verbal paired associates task (encoding and baseline test) in the
morning (wake groups) or evening (sleep groups). Afterwards, participants were informed that their memory for the
word pairs would be tested in the next session (expected condition) or were told to expect a different set of tasks
(unexpected condition). All participants were tested again 12 hours and 7 days after encoding.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.g001
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 3 / 14
keyboard. Following each response, and irrespective of accuracy, the cue and target words
were displayed together for 2 s. Participants were instructed to use this feedback to relearn any
pairs that they may have recalled incorrectly. This cued-recall procedure was repeated for a
second time if participants had not reached a criterion of 60% correct responses in the first
baseline test (all 40 word pairs were re-tested). Participants completed the test a maximum of
two times (55% of participants completed a second test). The proportion of participants who
completed a second test did not differ between the sleep and wake groups, X
2
(1, N= 162) =
0.62, p= .430.
Expectancy manipulation and retention interval. Following the baseline memory assess-
ment, participants in the ‘expected’ groups were told that they would be retested in the next
session, whereas participants in the ‘unexpected’ groups were told they would perform a differ-
ent set of tasks. To avoid active rehearsal of the word pairs, all participants completed a puzzle
matching game for 5 minutes before finishing the session. Participants then entered a 12-hour
retention interval, which took place across the day (wake groups) or night (sleep groups). Par-
ticipants in the wake groups followed their usual daytime routine and abstained from taking
naps. Participants in the sleep groups followed their usual home sleep routine. Participants
were not given any explicit instructions about the tasks that they would perform at the final fol-
low-up (7 days later).
Memory retrieval. Participants returned 12 hours and 7 days after the first session. During
these follow-up sessions, their memory for all 40 word pairs was retested using the same cued-
recall procedures as at the baseline session, with the exception that no feedback was provided.
Follow-up questionnaires. Participants completed questionnaires probing the amount of
sleep that they had achieved during the 12-hour delay (sleep groups), or whether they had
taken any naps (wake groups). Critically, all participants reported whether or not they had
expected to be retested on the word pairs after the baseline test. Participants in the unexpected
conditions who reported to have expected a test were excluded, as were participants in the
expected conditions who reported to have not expected a test. As outlined above (see Partici-
pants), the majority of individuals excluded on this basis were those in the unexpected condi-
tion who reported to have expected another test after the baseline test. A total of 66
participants were excluded on this basis in Experiment 1 (see Participants).
Alertness. Alertness was assessed at the beginning of each session using the Stanford Sleepi-
ness Scale (SSS [39]) and a Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT). During the PVT, participants
were presented with a blank grey screen. At random intervals, a red cross appeared in the cen-
tre of the screen and participants were required to press the keyboard space bar as quickly as
possible. Inter-stimulus intervals were randomly distributed from 2 to 10 s and the task lasted
for a total of 3 minutes.
Analysis. Memory retention at each follow-up test was defined as the percentage of cor-
rectly recalled word pairs, with performance on the final round of the baseline test set to 100%
[24]. For example, if a participant recalled 30 out of 40 words at baseline and then 35 out of 40
words at the 12-h follow-up, then their 12-h retention score would be ~117% ([35/30]100).
Owing to the feedback provided during the final baseline assessment, cued-recall performance
at the follow-up tests was expected to be above 100%. Retention scores at the 12-hour and
7-day tests were applied to separate 2 (Delay: Sleep/Wake) 2 (Expectancy: Expected/Unex-
pected) ANOVAs. To assess the strength of non-significant effects, a corresponding factorial
Bayesian ANOVA was calculated for each test with the same factors as above. By convention,
the strength of evidence for the null hypothesis, in comparison to the experimental hypothesis,
is regarded as noteworthy if the Bayes Factor (BF
01
) is >3 [40].
Alertness in each session was measured using scores provided on the SSS [39]. Vigilance
was assessed by the number of attentional lapses (i.e. trials with a response time [RT] >500
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 4 / 14
ms) made during the PVT [41]. These measures were subjected to separate 2 (Delay: Sleep/
Wake) 2 (Expectancy: Expected/Unexpected) ANOVAs at each session. All analyses were
completed in R[42] using the Rstatix [43] package.
Results
Baseline memory performance
Across all conditions, baseline memory performance (calculated as the percentage of correctly
recalled word pairs in the final baseline test) was 77% ±0.95 (mean ±SEM; see Table 3). A 2
(Delay: Sleep/Wake) 2 (Expectancy: Expected/Unexpected) ANOVA showed no group dif-
ferences at baseline (Delay: F(1,158) = 0.10, p= .320, Expectancy: F(1, 158) = 0.12, p= .726)
and no DelayExpectancy interaction (F(1, 158) = 0.02, p= .900). Nine participants failed to
reach the performance criterion of 60% after two attempts. The lowest score among these par-
ticipants was 37.5% and the average score was 48%. Our results did not change when we
repeated the analysis without these participants (all p>.05; also see analysis of memory reten-
tion below).
Memory retention
Memory retention at the 12-hour test was better after sleep than wakefulness (F(1, 158) = 9.67,
p= .002, η
p2
= 0.06, Fig 2A). However, performance was not influenced by knowledge of a
future test (F(1, 158) = 0.63, p= .428) and there was no DelayExpectancy interaction (F(1,
158) = 0.07, p= .786). Sleep-associated memory benefits were not therefore amplified for indi-
viduals who expected a test. In line with this interpretation, our Bayesian ANOVA yielded a
DelayExpectancy BF
01
of 4.00, indicating moderate evidence in favour of the null hypothesis.
The same pattern of results was observed at the 7-day test (Fig 2B), with an overall benefit
of sleep on memory retention (F(1, 158) = 4.30, p= .040, η
p2
= 0.03). Knowledge of a future
test did not influence performance (F(1, 158) = 1.96, p= .163) and there was no
DelayExpectancy interaction (F(1, 158) = 1.34, p= .249, BF
01
= 2.47).
These results remained the same when participants who did not meet the 60% baseline per-
formance criterion were removed from the analysis. Only a main effect of Delay was observed
at the 12-hour (F(1,149) = 16.93, p<.001, η
p2
= 0.10) and 7-day test (F(1,149) = 4.68, p= .032,
η
p2
= 0.03) was observed. There were no main effects of Expectancy and no DelayExpectancy
interactions at either test (all p>.05).
To capture the change in retention between the 12-hour and 7-day tests, memory retention
at the 7-day test was re-analysed, with performance in the 12-hour test set to 100%. When ana-
lysed with the same ANOVA as described above, there were no differences between conditions
(Delay: F(1, 158) = 0.95, p= .330; Expectancy: F(1, 158) = 0.99, p= .321), and no
DelayExpectancy interaction was observed (F(1, 158) = 2.25, p= .136).
Self-reported sleep
Participants in the sleep groups reported the number of hours that they had slept between
encoding and the 12-hour test. Mean (±SEM) hours slept was comparable between the sleep-
expected (6.58 ±0.25) and the sleep-unexpected (7.01 ±0.21) conditions (t(79) = 1.28, p=
.203). To ensure that our results were not influenced by poor sleepers (i.e. those who reported
less than 6 h of sleep) we repeated our main analyses without these participants (n = 13). The
foregoing findings were replicated, with only a significant memory benefit of sleep emerging
at the 12-hour (F(1, 145) = 10.15, p= .002, η
p2
= 0.07) and 7-day test (F(1, 145) = 4.27, p= .041,
η
p2
= .03). All other effects and interactions were non-significant (p>.05).
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 5 / 14
No significant correlation was observed between self-reported sleep and task performance
in either the sleep-expected (12-hour test: r= 0.06, p= .686; 7-day test: r= 0.05, p= .779) or
sleep-unexpected condition (12-hour test: r= 0.20, p= .200; 7-day test: r= 0.26, p= .109).
Alertness
Alertness levels, as indicated by the SSS (Table 1) were comparable across the Delay and
Expectancy conditions at the baseline test and 7-day test (all p>.05). Interestingly, at the
12-hour test, the sleep group rated themselves as less alert than the wake group (F(1, 158) =
26.55, p<.001, η
p2
= 0.14). This pattern of results is however contrary to what would be
expected if alertness levels were driving the retention advantage for the sleep (vs wake) groups,
and therefore cannot explain the purported benefit of sleep for memory. There was no main
effect of Expectancy and no DelayExpectancy interaction at the 12-hout test (all p>.05).
Performance in the PVT (Table 2) was comparable across the Delay and Expectancy condi-
tions at the baseline and 12-hour tests (all p>.05). At the 7-day test, we observed a
DelayExpectancy interaction (F(1,158) = 7.69, p= .006, η
p2
= 0.05). Post-hoc comparisons
show that this effect was driven by a greater number of attentional lapses in the wake-expected
condition compared to the sleep-expected condition (p
bonf
= .015). No other post-hoc compar-
isons were significant (p
bonf
>.05). There were no main effects of Delay or Expectancy at the
7-day test (all p>.05).
Experiment 2
In Experiment 1, we failed to replicate the finding that sleep selectively strengthens memories
that are deemed relevant for the future [24]. One potential explanation for this failed replica-
tion is that baseline performance levels were too high to allow knowledge of a future test to
Fig 2. Experiment 1 Memory retention. Performance at the 12-hour (A) and 7-day (B) tests in Experiment 1.
Performance is indicated by the percentage of word pairs recalled at each test, with performance on the final baseline
test set to 100%. Data are shown as mean ±SEM; p<.05, ��p<.01.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.g002
Table 1. Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS) scores for each condition and each session of Experiment 1.
Baseline 12-hour test 7-day test
Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected
Sleep 2.64 ±0.14 3.00 ±0.11 3.24 ±0.24 3.51 ±0.23 3.02 ±0.22 3.44 ±0.25
Wake 2.94 ±0.14 2.94 ±0.25 2.24 ±0.11 2.45 ±0.22 2.90 ±0.11 3.07 ±0.20
Data are presented as means ±SEM.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.t001
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 6 / 14
augment sleep-associated consolidation. Baseline memory performance in our first experi-
ment was 77%, although true performance levels were likely far higher due to the feedback
provided at the final baseline testing round. Indeed, accuracy levels at the 12-hour test
exceeded baseline performance despite there being no further exposure to the word pairs. To
address the possibility that memories were encoded too strongly to permit a selective benefit
of sleep on future-relevant information, we ran a second, pre-registered experiment (osf.io/
3k6ej) in which baseline performance levels were reduced relative to Experiment 1.
Methods
Participants. Two-hundred and thirteen adults (126 women, mean ±SD age: 24.45 ±3.86
years) were recruited using the same procedures as described in Experiment 1. As before, only
participants who expected/did not expect a memory test in line with their respective condi-
tions were included in our analysis (a total of 82 participants were excluded on this basis,
sleep-expected n= 5, sleep-unexpected n= 32, wake-expected n= 8, wake-unexpected n= 37).
A further seven participants were excluded from the analysis for either taking a daytime nap
during the ‘wake’ delay of the experiment (n= 6) or reporting to have obtained no sleep during
the ‘sleep’ delay (n= 1). This resulted in a final sample of 124 participants (78 women,
mean ±SD age: 24.32 ±4.05 years) who were allocated equally across conditions (n= 31 in
each condition).
Procedure. There were three key modifications to the procedures: 1) the number of sti-
muli was increased from 40 to 100 word pairs, 2) the criterion participants were required to
meet during the baseline assessment was reduced from 60% to 40% correct responses, and 3)
only a 12-hour follow-up test was included (because no changes were observed between the
12-hour and 7-day tests in Experiment 1). Sixty-eight percent of participants met the baseline
performance criterion on the first round of testing, whereas the remaining participants met
this criterion on the second testing round. The proportion of participants who completed a
second test did not differ between the sleep and wake groups, X
2
(1, N= 124) = 1.83, p= .176.
Results
Baseline memory performance
A 2 (Delay: Sleep/Wake) 2 (Expectancy: Expected/Unexpected) 2 (Experiment: 1/2)
ANOVA confirmed that baseline memory performance was significantly lower in Experiment
2 (64% ±1.29; ±SEM) relative to Experiment 1 (77%; F(1, 278) = 65.08, p<.001, η
p2
= 0.19,
Table 3). All other main effects and interactions were not significant (p>.05), and this was
also the case when baseline performance in Experiment 2 was assessed in isolation (Delay: F(1,
120) = 0.01, p= .917; Expectancy: F(1, 120) = 0.54, p= .464); DelayExpectancy: (F(1, 120) =
0.005, p= .946).
Table 2. Percentage of PVT trials with attentional lapses (RT >500 ms) in Experiment 1.
Baseline 12-hour test 7-day test
Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected
Sleep 7.19 ±1.32 9.81 ±2.32 11.64 ±2.05 11.57 ±1.65 10.40 ±2.26 14.82 ±1.76
Wake 10.03 ±2.08 8.86 ±1.97 13.24 ±2.63 9.52 ±2.30 20.38 ±2.90 11.04 ±2.14
Data are presented as means ±SEM.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.t002
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 7 / 14
Memory retention
Our findings mirrored those of Experiment 1. Memory retention at the 12-hour test was
higher after sleep than wakefulness (F(1, 120) = 5.27, p= .023, η
p2
= 0.04, Fig 3). However, per-
formance was not influenced by knowledge of a future test (F(1, 120) = 0.17, p= .682) and
there was no DelayExpectancy interaction (F(1, 120) = 0.45, p= .504). A Bayesian ANOVA
yielded a DelayExpectancy BF
01
of 3.42, indicating moderate evidence in favour of the null
hypothesis. As before, our findings suggest that sleep-associated memory benefits were not
amplified for individuals who expected a test.
Self-reported sleep
Mean (±SEM) hours slept was comparable between the sleep-expected (7.05 ±0.25) and the
sleep-unexpected (6.69 ±0.29) conditions (t(60) = 0.93, p= .359). Again, to ensure that our
results were not influenced by participants who reported less than 6 h of sleep, we repeated our
analyses without these participants (n= 9). Our findings were replicated, with only a
Table 3. Baseline memory performance for each condition in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. Performance was calculated as the percentage of correctly recalled
word pairs at the final baseline testing round.
Experiment 1 Experiment 2
Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected
Sleep 77.56 ±1.58 77.12 ±1.96 62.74 ±2.37 64.84 ±2.64
Wake 75.85 ±1.94 75.92 ±2.14 62.65 ±2.65 64.39 ±2.76
Data are presented as means ±SEM.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.t003
Fig 3. Experiment 2 Memory retention. Perfomance at the 12-hour test in Experiment 2. Performance is indicated by
the percentage of word pairs recalled at test, with performance on the final baseline test set to 100%. Data are shown as
means ±SEM; p<.05.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.g003
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 8 / 14
significant memory benefit of sleep emerging from the ANOVA (F(1, 111) = 6.39, p= .013, η
p2
= 0.05). There were no other significant main effects or interactions (all p>.05).
As before, there was no significant correlation between self-reported sleep and task perfor-
mance in either the sleep-expected (r= 0.09, p= .633) or sleep unexpected condition (r= 0.11,
p= .558).
Alertness
SSS scores (Table 4) were comparable across conditions at the baseline test (all p>.05), how-
ever as observed in Experiment 1, at the 12-hour test participants in the sleep groups rated
themselves as less alert than those in the wake groups (F(1,120) = 5.12, p= .026, η
p2
= 0.04).
There was no main effect of Expectancy and no DelayExpectancy interaction in this analysis
(all p>.05).
The number of attentional lapses made by participants in the PVT (Table 5) were equivalent
across Delay and Expectancy conditions at both the baseline test and the 12-hour test, no main
effects or interaction effects were observed at either session (all p>.05).
Discussion
In the current study, we sought to replicate the seminal finding that sleep preferentially
strengthens memories that are deemed to be relevant for the future [24]. In Experiment 1, we
ran an online replication of Wilhelm et al. [24], using a verbal paired associates paradigm. We
observed a benefit of sleep for the retention of word pairs, but, unlike the original study, we
found no evidence that sleep-associated memory effects were influenced by knowledge of a
future test. In Experiment 2, we reduced pre-sleep memory performance to account for the
potential impact of memory strength on overnight consolidation [17,36,37,4446]. We repli-
cated the results of Experiment 1, observing an overall memory benefit of sleep, but not a selec-
tive influence of sleep on future-relevant information.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to attempt a direct replication of Wilhelm et al. [24]
using the same paired associates protocol. Although our results do not support the original
findings, they are in keeping with a growing number of studies that have also failed to observe
an amplified benefit of sleep on future-relevant information [34,35]. The finding that sleep
does not offer a selective memory benefit for future relevant information has therefore been
Table 4. Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS) scores for each condition and each session of Experiment 2.
Encoding 12-hour test
Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected
Sleep 2.58 ±0.13 2.58 ±0.12 3.48 ±0.29 3.26 ±0.23
Wake 2.77 ±0.19 2.84 ±0.14 2.84 ±0.21 2.81 ±0.23
Data are presented as means ±SEM.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.t004
Table 5. Percentage of PVT trials with attentional lapses (RT >500ms) in Experiment 2.
Encoding 12-hour test
Expected Unexpected Expected Unexpected
Sleep 9.81 ±2.22 8.00 ±2.08 22.26 ±3.66 14.51 ±3.09
Wake 11.36 ±2.41 10.26 ±2.06 15.55 ±3.53 11.72 ±2.30
Data are presented as means ±SEM.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110.t005
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 9 / 14
shown across a number of different declarative and non-declarative memory paradigms.
These findings also complement a broader literature, where selective sleep-associated memory
effects are not always replicated. For example, emotionally salient information is widely con-
sidered to be particularly sensitive to consolidation during sleep [47], but many studies have
failed to find a preferential impact of sleep on emotionally negative relative to neutral memo-
ries [6,2629,48]. To what extent, and under what conditions, sleep offers selective benefits to
memory is therefore still largely unknown.
Previous studies suggest that the memory benefits of sleep are amplified for weakly-
encoded relative to strongly-encoded information [17,36,37,45,46,49,50]. We therefore rea-
soned that baseline performance levels in Experiment 1 may have been too high to allow
knowledge of a future test to augment sleep-associated consolidation. To address this issue, we
modified our task in a second experiment to reduce performance at baseline. Mirroring Exper-
iment 1, memory retention was superior after sleep than wakefulness, but this effect was com-
parable for individuals who did and did not expect a future test. Hence, memory strength at
encoding does not appear to explain our failure to replicate the findings of Wilhelm et al. [24].
It is nevertheless important to emphasise that our study was carried out in an online envi-
ronment and not in a laboratory. The experimental control afforded by a laboratory may have
had a major impact on how sleep influenced memory consolidation in the original investiga-
tion by Wilhelm et al. [24]. Of particular importance was the physical presence of an experi-
menter, which would have presumably increased the salience of instructions pertaining to
future tests. Other factors that can be more readily measured in the laboratory include the
time between encoding and bed time, overall sleep time and sleep architecture. Although
online questionnaires can provide some insight into participant sleep practices, they do not
offer the same degree of precision as objective laboratory measures such as polysomnography.
Our findings therefore demonstrate that a selective overnight strengthening of future-relevant
information does not emerge in an online environment, although further work is necessary to
determine whether such effects replicate in the laboratory.
Beyond the experimental environment, there were also several minor procedural differ-
ences between this study and Wilhelm et al. [24], which might have influenced our findings.
Notably, we did not attempt to control the time between completion of the experimental tasks
and sleep onset. There is evidence to suggest that sleep-memory effects are amplified when
sleep closely follows learning [17,51,52], and a shorter delay between learning and sleep might
also facilitate the selective strengthening of future-relevant information. Further procedural
differences between our study and Wilhelm et al. include our use of an explicit encoding
instruction (i.e. we asked participants to imagine the words of each pair interacting whereas
Wilhelm et al did not report doing so), and our puzzle matching (interference) task, which was
shorter in duration than the video game used by Wilhelm et al. It is possible that these proce-
dural differences reduced the sensitivity of our protocol to selective memory strengthening in
sleep.
Our results are however, in keeping with an emerging body of online research investigating
sleep-associated consolidation. For example, Kroneisen & Kuepper-Tetzel [51] also observed a
benefit of overnight sleep (vs daytime wakefulness) on the retention of verbal paired associates
using a web-based task. To our knowledge, we are the first to show in an online study that the
memory effects of sleep are retained across a longer retention interval of 7 days. This effect is
in line with numerous laboratory studies that have shown long-term benefits of sleep for mem-
ory [5355] and supports the view that sleep promotes the retention of durable and long-last-
ing memory representations [13].
A general concern in studies comparing memory retention after overnight sleep and day-
time wakefulness is that participants may be more alert in the morning after sleep, as
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 10 / 14
compared to the evening after wakefulness, meaning that an apparent memory benefit of sleep
could be driven by between-condition differences in alertness. However, in both experiments
of the current study, the sleep groups rated themselves as less alert than the wake groups when
they returned to complete the test phase. This is precisely the opposite pattern of results that
would be expected if between-group differences in alertness could explain the purported mem-
ory effect of sleep. We cannot rule out an impact of other circadian factors pertaining to even-
ing and morning testing, particularly given that participants were allocated to their respective
conditions based on the time of day that they signed up for the study (although they were
unaware of the crucial sleep vs wake manipulation). Many other studies have however reported
superior retention after a daytime nap relative to a corresponding interval of wakefulness, sug-
gesting that sleep-associated memory benefits are not driven by time-of-day effects.
In sum, we were unable to replicate the seminal finding that sleep selectively strengthens
memories that are deemed relevant for the future [24]. Our findings, derived from two online
experiments, are in keeping with a number of recent studies that have also failed to observe a
targeted effect of sleep on future-relevant information [34,35]. However, given the important
differences in experimental control between our online study and the original laboratory
experiment of Wilhelm et al. [24], further work is necessary to determine the conditions under
which these selective sleep-memory effects may emerge. Nevertheless, our findings add to a
developing body of online research indicating that sleep provides robust and long-lasting ben-
efits for memory [56].
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Jennifer E. Ashton, Scott A. Cairney.
Data curation: Jennifer E. Ashton.
Formal analysis: Jennifer E. Ashton.
Funding acquisition: Scott A. Cairney.
Investigation: Jennifer E. Ashton.
Methodology: Jennifer E. Ashton, Scott A. Cairney.
Project administration: Jennifer E. Ashton.
Resources: Jennifer E. Ashton, Scott A. Cairney.
Software: Jennifer E. Ashton.
Supervision: Scott A. Cairney.
Validation: Jennifer E. Ashton.
Visualization: Jennifer E. Ashton, Scott A. Cairney.
Writing original draft: Jennifer E. Ashton, Scott A. Cairney.
Writing review & editing: Jennifer E. Ashton, Scott A. Cairney.
References
1. Rasch B, Born J. About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013; 93(2):681–766. https://doi.org/10.
1152/physrev.00032.2012 PMID: 23589831
2. Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11(2):114–26. https://
doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762 PMID: 20046194
3. Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature. 2005; 437(7063):1272–8. https://doi.org/
10.1038/nature04286 PMID: 16251952
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 11 / 14
4. Klinzing JG, Niethard N, Born J. Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat Neu-
rosci. 2019; 22(10):1598–610. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3 PMID: 31451802
5. Gais S, Lucas B, Born J. Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learn Mem. 2006 May 1; 13(3):259–
62. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.132106 PMID: 16741280
6. Ashton JE, Harrington MO, Langthorne D, Ngo HV V., Cairney SA. Sleep deprivation induces frag-
mented memory loss. Learn Mem. 2020; 27(4):130–5. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.050757.119 PMID:
32179655
7. Cairney SA, Lindsay S, Paller KA, Gaskell MG. Sleep preserves original and distorted memory traces.
Cortex. 2018; 99:39–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.005 PMID: 29145007
8. Durrant SJ, Cairney SA, Lewis PA. Cross-modal transfer of statistical information benefits from sleep.
Cortex. 2016; 78:85–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.02.011 PMID: 27017231
9. Gaskell MG, Cairney SA, Rodd JM. Contextual priming of word meanings is stabilized over sleep. Cog-
nition. 2019; 182:109–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.007 PMID: 30227332
10. Scullin MK. Sleep, memory, and aging: The link between slow-wave sleep and episodic memory
changes from younger to older adults. Psychol Aging. 2013; 28(1):105–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0028830 PMID: 22708533
11. Born J. Slow-wave sleep and the consolidation of long-term memory. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 11
(sup1):16–21. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622971003637637 PMID: 20509828
12. Cairney SA, Sobczak JM, Lindsay S, Gaskell MG. Mechanisms of memory retrieval in slow-wave sleep.
Sleep. 2017; 40(9). https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsx114 PMID: 28934526
13. Lewis PA, Durrant SJ. Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends
Cogn Sci. 2011; 15(8):343–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.004 PMID: 21764357
14. Rasch B, Born J. Maintaining memories by reactivation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007 Dec 1; 17(6):698–
703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.007 PMID: 18222688
15. Groch S, Wilhelm I, Diekelmann S, Born J. The role of REM sleep in the processing of emotional memo-
ries: Evidence from behavior and event-related potentials. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013; 99:1–9. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.10.006 PMID: 23123802
16. Payne JD, Stickgold R, Swanberg K, Kensinger EA. Sleep preferentially enhances memory for emo-
tional components of scenes. Psychol Sci. 2008 Aug; 19(8):781–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.
2008.02157.x PMID: 18816285
17. Payne JD, Chambers AM, Kensinger EA. Sleep promotes lasting changes in selective memory for emo-
tional scenes. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012; 6:108. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00108 PMID:
23181013
18. Payne JD, Kensinger EA, Wamsley EJ, Nathan Spreng R, Alger SE, Gibler K, et al. Napping and the
selective consolidation of negative aspects of scenes. Emotion. 2015; 15(2):176–86. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0038683 PMID: 25706830
19. Cunningham TJ, Chambers AM, Payne JD. Prospection and emotional memory: How expectation
affects emotional memory formation following sleep and wake. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:862. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00862 PMID: 25136328
20. Fischer S, Born J. Anticipated Reward Enhances Offline Learning During Sleep. J Exp Psychol Learn
Mem Cogn. 2009; 35(6):1586–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017256 PMID: 19857029
21. Scullin MK, McDaniel MA. Remembering to execute a goal: Sleep on it! Psychol Sci. 2010; 21(7):1028–
35. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373373 PMID: 20519489
22. Diekelmann S, Wilhelm I, Wagner U, Born J. Sleep Improves Prospective Remembering by Facilitating
Spontaneous-Associative Retrieval Processes. PLoS One. 2013; 8(10). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0077621 PMID: 24143246
23. van Dongen E V., Thielen JW, Takashima A, Barth M, Ferna
´ndez G. Sleep supports selective retention
of associative memories based on relevance for future utilization. PLoS One. 2012; 7(8). https://doi.org/
10.1371/journal.pone.0043426 PMID: 22916259
24. Wilhelm I, Diekelmann S, Molzow I, Ayoub A, Molle M, Born J. Sleep Selectively EnhancesMemory
Expected to Be of Future Relevance. J Neurosci. 2011; 31(5):1563–9. https://doi.org/10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.3575-10.2011 PMID: 21289163
25. Baran B, Pace-Schott EF, Ericson C, Spencer RMC. Processing of emotional reactivity and emotional
memory over sleep. J Neurosci. 2012 Jan 18; 32(3):1035–42. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
2532-11.2012 PMID: 22262901
26. Lewis PAA, Cairney S, Manning L, Critchley HDD. The impact of overnight consolidation upon memory
for emotional and neutral encoding contexts. Neuropsychologia. 2011 Jul 1; 49(9):2619–29.
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 12 / 14
27. Lehmann M, Seifritz E, Rasch B. Sleep benefits emotional and neutral associative memories equally.
Somnologie. 2016; 20(1):47–53.
28. Ashton JE, Cairney SA, Gaskell MG. No effect of targeted memory reactivation during slow-wave sleep
on emotional recognition memory. J Sleep Res. 2017 May 11; 27(1):129–37. https://doi.org/10.1111/
jsr.12542 PMID: 28493346
29. Ashton JE, Harrington MO, Guttesen AA
´V, Smith AK, Cairney SA. Sleep Preserves Physiological
Arousal in Emotional Memory. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-42478-2 PMID:
30979941
30. Cairney SA, Durrant SJ, Jackson R, Lewis PA. Sleep spindles provide indirect support to the consolida-
tion of emotional encoding contexts. Neuropsychologia. 2014; 63:285–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2014.09.016 PMID: 25223465
31. Stamm AW, Nguyen ND, Seicol BJ, Fagan A, Oh A, Drumm M, et al. Negative reinforcement impairs
overnight memory consolidation. Learn Mem. 2014; 21(11):591–6. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.035196.
114 PMID: 25320351
32. Baran B, Daniels D, Spencer RMC. Sleep-Dependent Consolidation of Value-Based Learning. PLoS
One. 2013; 8(10):75326. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075326 PMID: 24130703
33. Tucker MA, Tang SX, Uzoh A, Morgan A, Stickgold R. To sleep, to strive, or both: How best to optimize
memory. PLoS One. 2011; 6(7). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021737 PMID: 21799746
34. Wamsley EJ, Hamilton K, Graveline Y, Manceor S, Parr E. Test expectation enhances memory consoli-
dation across both sleep and wake. Ravel N, editor. PLoS One. 2016 Oct 19; 11(10). https://doi.org/10.
1371/journal.pone.0165141 PMID: 27760193
35. Reverberi S, Kohn N, Ferna
´ndez G. No evidence for an effect of explicit relevance instruction on consol-
idation of associative memories. Neuropsychologia. 2020; 143:107491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2020.107491 PMID: 32422141
36. Creery JD, Oudiette D, Antony JW, Paller KA. Targeted memory reactivation during sleep depends on
prior learning. Sleep. 2015 May 1; 38(5):755–63. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.4670 PMID: 25515103
37. Cairney SA, Lindsay S, Sobczak JM, Paller KA, Gaskell MG. The Benefits of Targeted Memory Reacti-
vation for Consolidation in Sleep are Contingent on Memory Accuracy and Direct Cue-Memory Associa-
tions. Sleep. 2016; https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.5772 PMID: 26856905
38. Schechtman E, Lampe A, Wilson BJ, Kwon E, Anderson MC, Paller KA. Sleep reactivation did not
boost suppression-induced forgetting. Sci Rep. 2021; 11(1).
39. Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, Phillips R, Dement WC. Quantificationof Sleepiness: A New
Approach. Psychophysiology. 1973; 10(4):431–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1973.tb00801.x
PMID: 4719486
40. Dienes Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:781. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781 PMID: 25120503
41. Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. In: Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
ences. 2008. p. 305–22.
42. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical
Computing. 2019.
43. Kassambara A. Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests. 2020.
44. Lo JC, Dijk D-JJ, Groeger JA. Comparing the effects of nocturnal sleep and daytime napping on declar-
ative memory consolidation. Mazza M, editor. PLoS One. 2014 Sep 17; 9(9):e108100. https://doi.org/
10.1371/journal.pone.0108100 PMID: 25229457
45. Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Muto V, Schenkel M, Knoblauch V, Mu¨nch M, et al. Encoding difficulty pro-
motes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. J Neurosci. 2006; 26(35):8976–
82. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2464-06.2006 PMID: 16943553
46. Drosopoulos S, Schulze C, Fischer S, Born J. Sleep’s function in the spontaneous recovery and consoli-
dation of memories. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2007; 136(2):169–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.
2.169 PMID: 17500644
47. Walker MP, van der Helm E. Overnight Therapy? The Role of Sleep in Emotional Brain Processing.
Psychol Bull. 2009; 135(5):731–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016570 PMID: 19702380
48. Baran B, Pace-Schott EF, Ericson C, Spencer RMC. Processing of emotional reactivity and emotional
memory over sleep. J Neurosci. 2012; 32(3):1035–42. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-11.
2012 PMID: 22262901
49. Denis D, Schapiro AC, Poskanzer C, Bursal V, Charon L, Morgan A, et al. The roles of item exposure
and visualization success in the consolidation of memories across wake and sleep. Learn Mem. 2020;
27(11):451–6. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.051383.120 PMID: 33060281
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 13 / 14
50. Denis D, Mylonas D, Poskanzer C, Bursal V, Payne JD, Stickgold R. Sleep Spindles Preferentially Con-
solidate Weakly Encoded Memories. J Neurosci. 2021 Mar 19; 41(18):4088–99. https://doi.org/10.
1523/JNEUROSCI.0818-20.2021 PMID: 33741722
51. Talamini LM, Nieuwenhuis ILC, Takashima A, Jensen O. Sleep directly following learning benefits con-
solidation of spatial associative memory. Learn Mem. 2008; 15(4):233–7. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.
771608 PMID: 18391183
52. Gais S, Lucas B, Born J. Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learn Mem. 2006; https://doi.org/10.
1101/lm.132106 PMID: 16741280
53. Cousins JN, Wong KF, Raghunath BL, Look C, Chee MWL. The long-term memory benefits of a day-
time nap compared with cramming. Sleep. 2019; 42(1). https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsy207 PMID:
30371902
54. Alger SE, Lau H, Fishbein W. Slow wave sleep during a daytime nap is necessary for protection from
subsequent interference and long-term retention. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2012; 98(2):188–96. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.06.003 PMID: 22732649
55. Wagner U, Hallschmid M, Rasch B, Born J. Brief Sleep After Learning Keeps Emotional Memories Alive
for Years. Biol Psychiatry. 2006; 60(7):788–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.061 PMID:
16806090
56. Kroneisen M, Kuepper-Tetzel CE. Using Day and Night–Scheduling Retrieval Practice and Sleep. Psy-
chol Learn Teach. 2021; 20(1):40–57.
PLOS ONE
Sleep and selective memory consolidation
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258110 November 4, 2021 14 / 14
... This motivated us to switch from free wordlist recall to paired-associate learning in Experiment 1B (N = 192). Replicating prior lab-based studies (e.g., Payne et al., 2012;Plihal & Born, 1997), this task revealed a clear and robust sleep benefit, giving us confidence that sleep-related memory effects can be detected both within our sample and in an online study (see also Ashton & Cairney, 2021;Mak et al., 2022 ...
... Forty cue-target pairs were taken from a previous study (Ashton & Cairney, 2021; Experiment 1). Words in each pair (e.g., trophyprize) ...
... d = 0.85. Therefore, in this assessment of episodic declarative memory, we observed a sleepassociated benefit (e.g., Ashton & Cairney, 2021;Plihal & Born, 1997;Wang et al., 2017), showing that the experimental parameters of the current study were suitable for detecting sleep-related memory effects. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The episodic context account (Gaskell et al., 2019) proposes that the act of language comprehension gives rise to an episodic discourse representation, and that this representation is prone to sleep-related memory effects. In three experiments, we tested this prediction by asking participants to read/listen to naturalistic stories before their memory was tested after a 12-hr interval, which included either daytime wakefulness or overnight sleep. To assess discourse memory, we used sentence recognition (Experiment 1; N = 386), free story recall (Experiment 2; N = 96), and cued recall (Experiments 2 and 3; N = 192). We found no evidence of sleep-related effects in sentence recognition or free recall, but cued recall (aka fill-in-the-blank) showed that the degree of time-dependent distortion, as indexed by both a subjective categorisation measure and Latent Semantic Analysis, was lower after sleep than after wake. Our experiments suggest that the effect of sleep on discourse memory may [1] be constrained by the retrieval processes (recollection vs. familiarity & associative vs. item), [2] lie on a qualitative level that is difficult to detect in an all-or-nothing scoring metric, and [3] primarily situate in the textbase level of the tripartite model of discourse processing. Overall, in line with the episodic context account, our findings highlight a specific role of declarative memory and sleep in day-to-day language comprehension, but with limitations on their impact depending on the nature of the retrieval processes.
... For instance, one of the most robust findings in the memory literature is that word pairs encoded before sleep (vs. wakefulness) are usually recalled with greater accuracy (e.g., Ashton & Cairney, 2021;Backhaus et al., 2008;Lo et al., 2014;Mak et al., 2023a;Payne et al., 2012;Plihal & Born, 1997;Potkin & Bunney, 2012). ...
... However, it has been repeatedly demonstrated that as long as appropriate measures are taken (e.g., Rodd, 2019;Curtis et al., 2022), data quality from online experiments is no different from lab-based experiments (e.g., Anwyl-Irvine et al., 2020; Barnhoorn et al., 2015;Mak & Twitchell, 2020;Mak et al., 2021b). Furthermore, two recent online studies using the same experimental design (Ashton & Cairney, 2021;Mak et al., 2023a) found clear evidence of a sleep benefit in the classic paired-associate learning paradigm, replicating well-established evidence from lab-based experiments (e.g., Lo et al., 2014;Plihal & Born, 1997). Importantly, the effect sizes for sleep from these online studies were roughly equivalent to those reported by labbased studies. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Human memory is known to be supported by sleep. However, less is known about the effect of sleep on false memory, where people remember events that never occurred. In the laboratory, false memories are often induced via the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm where participants are presented with semantically related words such as nurse, hospital, and sick (studied words). Subsequently, participants are likely to falsely remember that a related lure word such as doctor was presented. Multiple studies have examined whether these false memories are influenced by sleep, with contradictory results. A recent meta-analysis suggests that sleep may increase DRM false memory when short lists are used. We tested this in a registered report (N=488) with a 2 (Interval: Immediate vs. 12-hr Delay) x 2 (Test Time: 9AM vs. 9PM) between-participant DRM experiment, using short DRM lists (N = 8 words/list) and free recall as the memory test. We found that the Sleep and Wake participants were well-matched on their number of total responses, but those in the Sleep group produced fewer intrusions (i.e., words that were neither studied nor lure words), and when this was statistically controlled for, they showed a greater tendency to recall more critical lures as well as more studied words. Our findings support the view that sleep may facilitate gist abstraction and/or spreading activation, alongside strengthening/protecting newly encoded declarative memories.
... We used the "classic" 12-h sleep versus wake design, motivated on the basis that this design elicits sleep effects in an online setting, giving us increased confidence that the effect of sleep could be successfully studied here (Ashton and Cairney 2021;Ashton et al. 2022;Denis et al. 2022). However, this design has two limitations that should be addressed in future research. ...
Article
Sleep benefits memory consolidation. However, factors present at initial encoding may moderate this effect. Here, we examined the role that encoding strategy plays in subsequent memory consolidation during sleep. Eighty-nine participants encoded pairs of words using two different strategies. Each participant encoded half of the word pairs using an integrative visualization technique, where the two items were imagined in an integrated scene. The other half were encoded noninte-gratively, with each word pair item visualized separately. Memory was tested before and after a period of nocturnal sleep (N = 47) or daytime wake (N = 42) via cued recall tests. Immediate memory performance was significantly better for word pairs encoded using the integrative strategy compared with the nonintegrative strategy (P < 0.001). When looking at the change in recall across the delay, there was significantly less forgetting of integrated word pairs across a night of sleep compared with a day spent awake (P < 0.001), with no significant difference in the nonintegrated pairs (P = 0.19). This finding was driven by more forgetting of integrated compared with not-integrated pairs across the wake delay (P < 0.001), whereas forgetting was equivalent across the sleep delay (P = 0.26). Together, these results show that the strategy engaged in during encoding impacts both the immediate retention of memories and their subsequent consolidation across sleep and wake intervals.
... We used the "classic" 12-h sleep versus wake design, motivated on the basis that this design elicits sleep effects in an online setting, giving us increased confidence that the effect of sleep could be successfully studied here (Ashton and Cairney 2021;Ashton et al. 2022;Denis et al. 2022). However, this design has two limitations that should be addressed in future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sleep benefits memory consolidation. However, factors present at initial encoding may moderate this effect. Here, we examined the role that encoding strategy plays in subsequent memory consolidation during sleep. Eighty-nine participants encoded pairs of words using two different strategies. Each participant encoded half of the word pairs using an integrative visualization technique, where the two items were imagined in an integrated scene. The other half were encoded nonintegratively, with each word pair item visualized separately. Memory was tested before and after a period of nocturnal sleep ( N = 47) or daytime wake ( N = 42) via cued recall tests. Immediate memory performance was significantly better for word pairs encoded using the integrative strategy compared with the nonintegrative strategy ( P < 0.001). When looking at the change in recall across the delay, there was significantly less forgetting of integrated word pairs across a night of sleep compared with a day spent awake ( P < 0.001), with no significant difference in the nonintegrated pairs ( P = 0.19). This finding was driven by more forgetting of integrated compared with not-integrated pairs across the wake delay ( P < 0.001), whereas forgetting was equivalent across the sleep delay ( P = 0.26). Together, these results show that the strategy engaged in during encoding impacts both the immediate retention of memories and their subsequent consolidation across sleep and wake intervals.
... However, it has been repeatedly demonstrated that as long as appropriate measures, such as preregistration and attention checks, are in place (e.g., Curtis et al., 2022;Mak, 2021;Rodd, 2019), data quality from online experiments is no different from lab-based experiments (e.g., Anwyl-Irvine et al., 2020; Barnhoorn et al., 2015;Mak et al., 2021a;Mak & Twitchell, 2020). Furthermore, three recent online studies using the same sleep manipulation as in the current study (Ashton et al., 2021; found clear evidence of a sleep benefit in the classic paired-associate learning paradigm, replicating wellestablished evidence from lab-based studies (e.g., Lo et al., 2014;Plihal & Born, 1997;Scullin, 2013). Together, these suggest that it is possible to detect sleep-related memory effects in online experiments, as long as preregistered exclusionary criteria and attention checks are put in place, both of which were implemented in the current (and the next) experiment. ...
Article
Full-text available
Familiar words come with a wealth of associated knowledge about their variety of usage, accumulated over a lifetime. How do we track and adjust this knowledge as new instances of a word are encountered? A recent study (Cognition) found that, for homonyms (e.g., bank), sleep-associated consolidation facilitates the updating of meaning dominance. Here, we tested the generality of this finding by exposing participants to (Experiment 1; N = 125) nonhomonyms (e.g., bathtub) in sentences that biased their meanings toward a specific interpretation (e.g., bathtub-slip vs. bathtub-relax), and (Experiment 2; N = 128) word-class ambiguous words (e.g., loan) in sentences where the words were used in their dispreferred word class (e.g., "He will loan me money"). Both experiments showed that such sentential experience influenced later interpretation and usage of the words more after a night's sleep than a day awake. We interpret these results as evidence for a general role of episodic memory in language comprehension such that new episodic memories are formed every time a sentence is comprehended, and these memories contribute to lexical processing next time the word is encountered, as well as potentially to the fine-tuning of long-term lexical knowledge. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
... An overreliance on small sample sizes in many experiments is likely a major contributor to conflicting results (2), with studies being underpowered to detect true effects. While it is challenging to recruit large samples for typical laboratory experiments, utilizing online tools to examine the behavioral effect of sleep on memory represents a promising avenue to increase sample size and sample from a broader portion of society (17)(18)(19). We turned to this resource to examine, in two well-powered experiments, whether sleep enhances emotional memory in human adults in the largest studies of sleep and emotional memory to date. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research suggests that sleep benefits memory. Moreover, it is often claimed that sleep selectively benefits memory for emotionally salient information over neutral information. However, not all scientists are convinced by this relationship [e.g., J. M. Siegel. Curr. Sleep Med. Rep. , 7, 15–18 (2021)]. One criticism of the overall sleep and memory literature—like other literature—is that many studies are underpowered and lacking in generalizability [M. J. Cordi, B. Rasch. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. , 67, 1–7 (2021)], thus leaving the evidence mixed and confusing to interpret. Because large replication studies are sorely needed, we recruited over 250 participants spanning various age ranges and backgrounds in an effort to confirm sleep’s preferential emotional memory consolidation benefit using a well-established task. We found that sleep selectively benefits memory for negative emotional objects at the expense of their paired neutral backgrounds, confirming our prior work and clearly demonstrating a role for sleep in emotional memory formation. In a second experiment also using a large sample, we examined whether this effect generalized to positive emotional memory. We found that while participants demonstrated better memory for positive objects compared to their neutral backgrounds, sleep did not modulate this effect. This research provides strong support for a sleep-specific benefit on memory consolidation for specifically negative information and more broadly affirms the benefit of sleep for cognition.
... Furthermore, two recent online studies using the same sleep manipulation as in the current study (Ashton & Cairney, 2021;Mak et al., in preparation) found clear evidence of a sleep benefit in the classic paired-associate learning paradigm, replicating well-established evidence from lab-based studies (e.g., Lo et al., 2014;Plihal & Born, 1997). Together, these suggest that it is possible to detect sleep-related memory effects in online experiments, as long as pre-registered exclusionary criteria and attention checks are put in place, both of which were implemented in the current (and the next) experiment. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Familiar words come with a wealth of associated knowledge about their variety of usage, accumulated over a lifetime. How do we track and adjust this knowledge as new instances of a word are encountered? A recent study (Gaskell, Cairney & Rodd, 2019, Cognition) found that, for homonyms (e.g., bank), sleep-associated consolidation facilitates the updating of meaning dominance. Here, we tested the generality of this finding by exposing participants to (Experiment 1; N = 125) non-homonyms (e.g., bathtub) in sentences that biased their meanings towards a specific interpretation (e.g., bathtub-slip vs. bathtub-relax), and (Experiment 2; N = 128) word-class ambiguous words (e.g., loan) in sentences where the words were used in their dispreferred word class (e.g., “He will loan me money”). Both experiments showed that such sentential experience influenced later interpretation and usage of the words more after a night’s sleep than a day awake. We interpret these results in relation to an episodic context account of language comprehension in which new episodic memories are formed every time a sentence is comprehended, and these memories contribute to lexical processing next time the word is encountered and potentially the fine-tuning of long-term lexical knowledge.
Preprint
Full-text available
Sleep disturbances are associated with intrusive memories, but the neurocognitive mechanisms underpinning this relationship are poorly understood. Here, we show that an absence of sleep disrupts prefrontal inhibition of memory retrieval, and that the overnight restoration of this inhibitory mechanism is predicted by time spent in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. The functional impairments arising from sleep loss are linked to a behavioural deficit in the ability to suppress unwanted memories, and coincide with a deterioration of deliberate patterns of self-generated thought. We conclude that sleep deprivation gives rise to intrusive memories via the disruption of neural circuits governing mnemonic inhibitory control, which may rely on REM sleep.
Article
Emotion regulation (i.e. either up- or down-regulating affective responses to emotional stimuli) has been shown to modulate long-term emotional memory formation. Further, research has demonstrated that the emotional aspects of scenes are preferentially remembered relative to neutral aspects (known as the emotional memory trade-off effect). This trade-off is often enhanced when sleep follows learning, compared to an equivalent period of time spent awake. However, the interactive effects of sleep and emotion regulation on emotional memory are poorly understood. We presented 87 participants with pictures of neutral or negative objects on neutral backgrounds paired with instructions to either increase or decrease their emotional response by altering personal relevance, or to passively view the stimuli. Following a 12 h period of sleep or wakefulness, participants were tested for their memory of objects and backgrounds separately. Although we replicated the emotional memory trade-off effect, no differences in the magnitude of the trade-off effect were observed between regulation conditions. Sleep improved all aspects of memory, but it did not preferentially benefit memory for emotional components of scenes. Irrespective of a period of sleep or wake following encoding, findings suggest emotion regulation during encoding did not influence memory for emotional items at a 12-hour delay.
Article
Sleep strengthens declarative memory, but research investigating the effect of sleep on memory for information that is not explicitly studied for a test is sparse. In two experiments, we investigated the effect of sleep on gist-based and veridical representations of incidentally encoded information. Participants rated words from Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) lists in either a deep or shallow encoding task and completed a surprise memory test after either sleep or wake. In Experiment 1, words were presented in lists, in order of descending associativity with the unpresented critical lure. Memory for list words and critical lures in both encoding tasks was stronger after sleep than wake, suggesting that sleep consolidated gist-based memory. In Experiment 2, the same words were presented in a random order across the experiment to minimize gist-based processing. Sleep strengthened veridical memory for list words following deep, but not shallow, encoding and did not affect critical lures. These results suggest sleep consolidates gist and veridical representations of information after incidental encoding, and that sleep-dependent consolidation processes may depend on processes at encoding, such as overlapping context and the strength of veridical memory traces.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep has been shown to be critical for memory consolidation, with some research suggesting that certain memories are prioritized for consolidation. Initial strength of a memory appears to be an important boundary condition in determining which memories are consolidated during sleep. However, the role of consolidation-mediating oscillations, such as sleep spindles and slow oscillations, in this preferential consolidation has not been explored. Here, 54 human participants (76% female) studied pairs of words to three distinct encoding strengths, with recall being tested immediately following learning and again six hours later. Thirty-six had a two-hour nap opportunity following learning, whilst the remaining 18 remained awake throughout. Results showed that across six hours awake, weakly encoded memories deteriorated the fastest. In the nap group however, this effect was attenuated, with forgetting rates equivalent across encoding strengths. Within the nap group, consolidation of weakly encoded items was associated with fast sleep spindle density during non-rapid eye movement sleep. Moreover, sleep spindles that were coupled to slow oscillations predicted the consolidation of weak memories independently of uncoupled sleep spindles. These relationships were unique to weakly encoded items, with spindles not correlating with memory for intermediate or strong items. This suggests that sleep spindles facilitate memory consolidation, guided in part by memory strength.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTGiven the countless pieces of information we encode each day, how does the brain select which memories to commit to long-term storage? Sleep is known to aid in memory consolidation, and it appears that certain memories are prioritized to receive this benefit. Here, we found that compared to staying awake, sleep was associated with better memory for weakly encoded information. This suggests that sleep helps attenuate the forgetting of weak memory traces. Fast sleep spindles, a hallmark oscillation of non-rapid eye movement sleep, mediate consolidation processes. We extend this to show that fast spindles were uniquely associated with the consolidation of weakly encoded memories. This provides new evidence for preferential sleep-based consolidation and elucidate a physiological correlate of this benefit.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep's role in memory consolidation is widely acknowledged, but its role in weakening memories is still debated. Memory weakening is evolutionary beneficial and makes an integral contribution to cognition. We sought evidence on whether sleep-based memory reactivation can facilitate memory suppression. Participants learned pairs of associable words (e.g., DIET–CREAM) and were then exposed to hint words (e.g., DIET) and instructed to either recall (“think”) or suppress (“no-think”) the corresponding target words (e.g., CREAM). As expected, suppression impaired retention when tested immediately after a 90-min nap. To test if reactivation could selectively enhance memory suppression during sleep, we unobtrusively presented one of two sounds conveying suppression instructions during sleep, followed by hint words. Results showed that targeted memory reactivation did not enhance suppression-induced forgetting. Although not predicted, post-hoc analyses revealed that sleep cues strengthened memory, but only for suppressed pairs that were weakly encoded before sleep. The results leave open the question of whether memory suppression can be augmented during sleep, but suggest strategies for future studies manipulating memory suppression during sleep. Additionally, our findings support the notion that sleep reactivation is particularly beneficial for weakly encoded information, which may be prioritized for consolidation.
Article
Full-text available
Memory consolidation during sleep does not benefit all memories equally. Initial encoding strength appears to play a role in governing where sleep effects are seen, but it is unclear whether sleep preferentially consolidates weaker or stronger memories. We manipulated encoding strength along two dimensions-the number of item presentations, and success at visualizing each item, in a sample of 82 participants. Sleep benefited memory of successfully visualized items only. Within these, the sleep-wake difference was largest for more weakly encoded information. These results suggest that the benefit of sleep on memory is seen most clearly for items that are encoded to a lower initial strength.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep right after studying new material is more conducive to memory than a period of wakefulness. Another way to counteract forgetting is to practice retrieval: taking a test strengthens memory more effectively than restudying the material. The current work aims at investigating the interaction between sleep and testing by asking if testing adds to, neutralizes, or decreases the effect of sleep on memory? We tested this in one pilot and one experiment by manipulating the timing of the practice test as well as whether practice was followed by sleep or wakefulness when learning foreign language vocabulary. Taking a delayed practice test significantly reduces forgetting for both the sleep and the wakefulness group. An immediate practice test, in contrast, had no such effect; here we find the standard beneficial sleep effect. However, the immediate practice test leads to higher recall in the final test in comparison to a delayed practice test, but only for the sleep group. Practical recommendations imply two things: first, if students study in the evening, they should test themselves immediately after learning. Second, if students study during the day the practice test should be delayed in order to reinforce memory and reduce forgetting of the material.
Article
Full-text available
Newly encoded memories are stabilized over time through a process or a set of processes termed consolidation, which happens preferentially during sleep. However, not all memories profit equally from this offline stabilization. Previous research suggested that one factor, which determines whether a memory will benefit from sleep consolidation, is future relevance. The aim of our current study was to replicate these findings and expand them to investigate their neural underpinnings. In our experiment, 38 participants learned two sets of object-location associations. The two sets of stimuli were presented to each participant intermixed and in random order. After study, participants performed a baseline retention test and were thereafter instructed that, after a delay containing sleep, they would be tested and rewarded only on one of the two sets of stimuli. This relevance instruction was revoked, however, immediately before the test. Thus, this manipulation affected memory consolidation while having no influence on encoding and retrieval. This retention interval was monitored via actigraphy recordings. While the study session was purely behavioral, the test session was conducted in an MRI scanner, thus we collected neuroimaging data at retrieval of relevant compared with non-relevant items. Behaviorally, we found no effect of the relevance manipulation on memory retention, confidence rating, or reaction time. At a neural level, no effect of relevance on memory retrieval-related brain operations was observed. Contrary to our expectations, the relevance manipulation did not result in improved consolidation, nor in improved subsequent performance at retrieval. These findings challenge previously published results and suggest that future relevance as manipulated here may not be sufficient to produce enhanced memory consolidation.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep deprivation increases rates of forgetting in episodic memory. Yet, whether an extended lack of sleep alters the qualitative nature of forgetting is unknown. We compared forgetting of episodic memories across intervals of overnight sleep, daytime wakefulness and overnight sleep deprivation. Item-level forgetting was amplified across daytime wakefulness and overnight sleep deprivation, as compared to sleep. Importantly, however, overnight sleep deprivation led to a further deficit in associative memory that was not observed after daytime wakefulness. These findings suggest that sleep deprivation induces fragmentation among item memories and their associations, altering the qualitative nature of episodic forgetting.
Article
Full-text available
Traumatic experiences are associated with increased emotional arousal. Overnight consolidation strengthens the episodic content of emotional memories, but it is still unclear how sleep influences the associated arousal response. To investigate this question, we compared the effects of sleep and wake on psychophysiological and subjective reactivity during emotional memory retrieval. Participants provided affective ratings for negative and neutral images while heart rate deceleration (HRD) and skin conductance responses (SCRs) were monitored. Following a 12-hour delay of sleep or wakefulness, participants completed an image recognition task where HRD, SCRs and affective ratings were recorded again. HRD responses to previously-encoded (“old”) negative images were preserved after sleep but diminished after wakefulness. No between-group difference in HRD was observed for novel negative images at recognition, indicating that the effects of sleep for old images were not driven by a generalised overnight increase in visceral activity, or circadian factors. No significant effects of sleep were observed for SCRs or subjective ratings. Our data suggest that cardiac arousal experienced at the time of encoding is sensitive to plasticity-promoting processes during sleep in a similar manner to episodic aspects of emotional memory.
Article
Full-text available
Study objectives Daytime naps benefit long-term memory relative to taking a break and remaining awake. However, the use of naps as a practical way to improve learning has not been examined, in particular, how memory following a nap compares with spending the equivalent amount of time cramming. Methods Young adults learned detailed factual knowledge in sessions that flanked one-hour spent napping (n=27), taking a break (n=27), or cramming that information (n=30). Recall was examined 30-mins and one week after learning. Results When tested 30-mins after learning, cramming and napping led to significantly better memory than taking a break. After a week, napping maintained this significant advantage, but cramming did not. Conclusions These findings demonstrate the longer-term benefits of napping for retention of memoranda akin to what students encounter daily, and encourage more widespread adoption of napping in education.
Article
Full-text available
Evidence is growing for the involvement of consolidation processes in the learning and retention of language, largely based on instances of new linguistic components (e.g., new words). Here, we assessed whether consolidation effects extend to the semantic processing of highly familiar words. The experiments were based on the word-meaning priming paradigm in which a homophone is encountered in a context that biases interpretation towards the subordinate meaning. The homophone is subsequently used in a word-association test to determine whether the priming encounter facilitates the retrieval of the primed meaning. In Experiment 1 (N = 74), we tested the resilience of priming over periods of 2 and 12 h that were spent awake or asleep, and found that sleep periods were associated with stronger subsequent priming effects. In Experiment 2 (N = 55) we tested whether the sleep benefit could be explained in terms of a lack of retroactive interference by testing participants 24 h after priming. Participants who had the priming encounter in the evening showed stronger priming effects after 24 h than participants primed in the morning, suggesting that sleep makes priming resistant to interference during the following day awake. The results suggest that consolidation effects can be found even for highly familiar linguistic materials. We interpret these findings in terms of a contextual binding account in which all language perception provides a learning opportunity, with sleep and consolidation contributing to the updating of our expectations, ready for the next day.
Article
Long-term memory formation is a major function of sleep. Based on evidence from neurophysiological and behavioral studies mainly in humans and rodents, we consider the formation of long-term memory during sleep as an active systems consolidation process that is embedded in a process of global synaptic downscaling. Repeated neuronal replay of representations originating from the hippocampus during slow-wave sleep leads to a gradual transformation and integration of representations in neocorti- cal networks. We highlight three features of this process: (i) hippocampal replay that, by capturing episodic memory aspects, drives consolidation of both hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus-dependent memory; (ii) brain oscillations hall- marking slow-wave and rapid-eye movement sleep that provide mechanisms for regulating both information flow across distant brain networks and local synaptic plasticity; and (iii) qualitative transformations of memories during systems consolidation resulting in abstracted, gist-like representations.